The Neural Substrates of Visual Implicit Memory: Do the Two Hemispheres Play Different Roles?

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Neural Substrates of Visual Implicit Memory: Do the Two Hemispheres Play Different Roles?"

Transcription

1 The Neural Substrates of Visual Implicit Memory: Do the Two Hemispheres Play Different Roles? N. E. A. Kroll 1, A. P. Yonelinas 1, M. M. Kishiyama 1, K. Baynes 1, R. T. Knight 2, and M. S. Gazzaniga 3 Abstract & Identification of visually presented words is facilitated by implicit memory, or visual priming, for past visual experiences with those words. There is disagreement over the neuroanatomical substrates of this form of implicit memory. Several studies have suggested that this form of priming relies on a visual word-form system localized in the right occipital lobe, whereas other studies have indicated that both hemispheres are equally involved. The discrepancies may be related to the types of priming tasks that have been used because the former studies have relied primarily on word-stem completion tasks and the latter on tasks like word-fragment completion. The present experiments compared word-fragment and word-stem measurements of visual implicit memory in patients with right occipital lobe lesions and patients with complete callosotomies. The patients showed normal visual implicit memory on fragment completion tests, but essentially no visual priming on standard stem completion tests. However, when we used a set of word stems that had only one correct solution for each test item, as was true of the items in the fragment completion tests, the patients showed normal priming effects. The results indicate that visual implicit memory for words is not solely dependent upon the right hemisphere, rather it reflects changes in processing efficiency in bilateral visual regions involved in the initial processing of the items. However, under conditions of high lexical competition (i.e., multiple completion word stems), the lexical processes, which are dominant in the left hemisphere, overshadow the visual priming supported by the left hemisphere. & INTRODUCTION 1 University of California Davis, 2 University of California Berkeley, 3 Dartmouth College In tests of explicit memory, such as recognition and free recall, subjects are requested to actively remember previous events. In contrast, the instructions for tests of implicit memory either make no reference to previous events or even tell the subject to ignore them. For example, in a word-stem completion test, subjects are first exposed to a series of words (e.g., salmon) and are later shown word stems (e.g., sal ) and are instructed to complete the stem with the first word that comes to mind. Subjects are more likely to complete a stem with a word if it had been previously seen (i.e., they exhibit priming effects), even if they do not consciously recollect having seen that word previously. Another frequently used implicit memory task is the word-fragment completion test, which is similar to the stem completion task except that subjects are presented with fragmented words at test (e.g., e_e_an_e for the word elegance). What makes the explicit/implicit distinction particularly interesting is that patients with damage to the medial temporal lobes perform more poorly than normal subjects on tests of explicit memory, but perform normally on tests of implicit memory (e.g., Schacter, Chiu, & Ochsner, 1993). This indicates that implicit and explicit forms of memory rely on partially distinct neuroanatomical substrates. These two forms of memory also exhibit distinct functional properties. For example, changes in the perceptual format of words between study and test, such as from uppercase letters to lowercase letters or from auditory to visual presentation, greatly reduces the priming effects observed in implicit tests, such as stem completion (for reviews, see Roediger & McDermott, 1993; Schacter et al., 1993). In contrast, similar changes in perceptual format often do not affect performance on explicit tests. The sensitivity of implicit memory performance to changes in perceptual format has been taken as evidence that these tests rely on form-specific sensory memory processes or systems (Schacter, 1992; Roediger & Blaxton, 1987; Jacoby, 1983). However, significant priming effects are observed even when the study and test format changes, indicating that implicit memory tasks can also rely in part on abstract form-independent memory processes, such as phonological, lexical, or semantic processes (e.g., Curran, Schacter, & Galluccio, 1999; Rueckl & Mathew, 1999). Several studies using a divided visual field technique in healthy subjects have D 2003 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15:6, pp

2 suggested that the form-specific component of visual implicit memory is supported primarily by the right hemisphere, whereas the abstract component is supported equally by both hemispheres (e.g., Marsolek & Hudson, 1999; Marsolek, Kosslyn, & Squire, 1992). Another set of studies that support the hypothesis that structures in the right hemisphere are required for the form-specific component of visual implicit memory are based on the study of a patient with a right hemisphere occipital lobe lesion. This patient has been studied quite extensively and consistently exhibits a severe perceptual priming deficit in word-stem completion tests, despite normal performance on a variety of explicit memory tests (e.g., Vaidya, Gabrieli, Verfaellie, Fleischman, & Askari, 1998; Fleischman et al., 1995; Gabrieli, Fleischman, Keane, Reminger, & Morrell, 1995; Keane, Gabrieli, Mapstone, Johnson, & Corkin, 1995), suggesting that the right occipital lobe plays a necessary role in implicit visual priming but not explicit memory. However, other studies suggest that both hemispheres are involved in supporting visual implicit memory. For example, divided visual field studies using the word-fragment completion task (Kroll, Rocha, Yonelinas, Baynes, & Frederick, 2001) and the perceptual identification task (e.g., Burgund & Marsolek, 1997; Koivisto, 1995) have indicated that both hemispheres exhibit equivalent visual priming effects. Moreover, in a previous study ( Yonelinas et al., 2001), we evaluated the contribution of the two hemispheres to visual implicit memory by examining patients who suffered from extensive right occipital lobe lesions and patients whose left (verbal) hemisphere had been surgically separated from the right by means of a callosotomy. If the right occipital lobe is necessary for visual implicit memory, then, we reasoned, both of these types of patients should exhibit a pronounced deficit on tests of visual implicit memory. 1 We measured implicit memory using four different visual implicit tests to determine if the results generalized across a variety of test procedures (i.e., lexical decision, mirror reading, picture fragment completion, and visual word-fragment completion). In all of these priming tests, both types of patients presented with normal visual priming, suggesting that the right hemisphere was not necessary for visual priming. Why are the previous studies in disagreement about the role of the right hemisphere in visual implicit memory? One possible explanation for the discrepancies observed in the patient studies is that only certain types of occipital lobe lesions might lead to visual priming deficits. Given that the different patients have not been tested on the same test procedures it is not possible to directly rule out this possibility. However, differences in lesions could not explain the differences observed across different implicit tests in the divided visual field studies of healthy subjects. A more promising explanation is that the discrepancies arose because of the different retrieval tasks that have been used in previous studies. The studies that have indicated a dominant role for the right hemisphere have relied almost exclusively on the word-stem completion task (e.g., Marsolek & Hudson, 1999; Vaidya et al., 1998; Fleischman et al., 1995; Gabrieli et al., 1995; Keane et al., 1995; Marsolek et al., 1992), whereas the studies that have indicated no special role for the right hemisphere have used a variety of other visual implicit memory tests, such as word-fragment completion and perceptual identification (Kroll et al., 2001; Yonelinas et al., 2001; Burgund & Marsolek, 1997; Koivisto, 1995). Although the stem completion task is similar to other visual implicit tests there is evidence that it relies less on perceptual processes and more on phonological or semantic processes than do tasks like word-fragment completion (Blum & Yonelinas, 2001; Rueckl & Mathew, 1999; Bassili, Smith, & MacLeod, 1989). Thus, differences in the processing demands of the retrieval tasks might lead to differences in how the two hemispheres contribute to performance. In the current study, we examined stem and fragment completion priming in patients with extensive right occipital lobe lesions and callosotomy patients in whom the left hemisphere had been surgically separated from the right. We first report results on standard wordfragment completion (Experiment 1) and word-stem completion tasks (Experiment 2) to determine whether the previous discrepancies were due to differences in patient populations, or to differences in retrieval tasks. The results indicated that the patients exhibited normal fragment priming but were impaired on stem priming, showing that there is a critical difference in the manner in which these two tasks rely on the two hemispheres. Experiments 3 and 4 examined fragment and stem completion priming, respectively, using the same sets of words, and again showed reduced stem priming and normal fragment priming in the patients, indicating that subtle differences in materials were not responsible for the observed dissociation. Experiment 5 was designed to determine if the reduced priming in the stem task was due to the fact that stems typically have many competing solutions, whereas fragments typically have only one, by using stems that had only one correct solution. The same patients who exhibited stem completion priming deficits on multiple completion stems were now found to exhibit normal priming on the single completion stems, indicating that the role of the right hemisphere in priming is determined by the extent to which there are multiple competing items for a given retrieval cue. RESULTS The scores on the implicit memory tests are presented in Table 1, and the visual priming scores are presented in Figures 1 and 2. For each of the experiments, a subject s visual priming score was measured as the 834 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 6

3 Table 1. (continued ) Table 1. Percentage of Correct Completions for Each of the Patients Tested and Median Percentage of Correct Completions for Each of the Control Groups Tested in Each of the Conditions (New, Auditorily Primed, Visually Primed) for Each of the Experiments Patient/Group New Condition Auditorily Primed Visually Primed Experiment 1: Eight-letter word fragments Student controls Aged-matched controls Right occipital lobe patients C. B F. N J. C N. A Callosotomy patients D. R J. W V. J Experiment 2: Standard word stems Student controls Right occipital lobe patients C. B F. N J. C N. A Experiment 3: Five-letter word fragments Student controls Right occipital lobe patients C. B F. N N. A Experiment 4: Five-letter word stems with multiple solutions Student controls Aged-matched controls Right occipital lobe patients C. B F. N N. A Patient/Group New Condition Auditorily Primed Visually Primed Experiment 4: Five-letter word stems with multiple solutions Callosotomy patient J. W Experiment 5: Five-letter word stems with unique solutions Student control Aged-matched control Right occipital lobe patients C. B F. N N. A Callosotomy patient J. W difference between the percentage of correct completions of words previously seen and the percentage of correct completions of words previously heard (i.e., the heard words, which should have also primed lexical processing components, served as a baseline). This difference score was used to obtain a measure of priming that was based solely on the visual component of the stimulus. A completed fragment or stem was judged as correct if it matched the corresponding word from the original word list (i.e., the word that had been seen or heard). Not all patients were available for testing on every experiment, but if they were tested on an experiment, those results are reported. Note that most of the patients were older than the student control subjects. Although visual priming is not generally influenced by aging (e.g., Rybash, 1996), to confirm that aging did not play a critical role in the current studies, an older control group was included in Experiments 1, 4, and 5. Experiment 1 Experiment 1 is an extension of a word-fragment completion experiment that we reported previously ( Yonelinas et al., 2001). The left panel of Figure 1 presents the visual priming performance for fragment completion that we reported in that earlier experiment on 2 occipital lobe patients (F. N. and J. C.), 3 callosotomy patients (D. R., V. J., and J. W.), and the median of the 16 normal older control subjects. The priming performance of a third occipital lobe patient (C. B.) and 27 student controls added in the present experiment is Kroll et al. 835

4 Figure 1. (A) Visual priming scores for the fragment completion task of Experiment 1. (B) Visual priming scores for the stem completion task of Experiment 2. The data for the 27 student control subjects in Experiment 1 and the 16 student control subjects in Experiment 2 are presented in a box-and-whisker format, with the median shown as a horizontal line, the upper and lower quartiles shown as the upper and lower edges of the box, and the most extreme scores shown as the ends of the whiskers. The median of the 16 older control subjects is represented by a circle. The scores of patients with right occipital lobe lesions ( J. C., F. N., N. A., and C. B.) are represented by square-shaped symbols; those of the callosotomy patients ( J. W., D. R., and V. J.) are represented by diamond-shaped symbols. also included in that figure. Before we discuss the priming scores, it is appropriate to compare the baseline (priming for words heard) performance. The older normal subjects had a baseline of M =.61, SD =.19, and the students had a baseline of M =.62, SD =.11. The baseline for the five patients, however, was considerably lower, M =.42, SD =.13. However, differences in baseline did not appear to influence the priming effects on the fragment completion test. The eight older control subjects with the lowest baseline completion rates had baselines that were comparable to the patients (M =.38, SD =.10) and the priming scores for these control subjects were not appreciably different from the subjects with higher baseline performance. Note that no baseline differences were observed in the subsequent experiments. All of the right occipital lobe lesion and callosotomy patients exhibited robust priming effects on the fragment completion test (Figure 1, left panel). The patients were all within the range of the student controls, with the exception of one, C. B., who exhibited slightly greater priming than the controls. The results are clear in showing that right occipital lobe damage does not disrupt visual priming on the word-fragment completion task and that the isolated left hemisphere is capable of supporting normal visual priming on this task. The results are consistent with a previous report showing that these patients exhibited normal visual priming on perceptual identification, mirror reading and picture fragment completion tests ( Yonelinas et al., 2001). Experiment 2 Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1 except that priming was measured using a stem completion, rather than a fragment completion task. The right panel of Figure 1 presents the visual priming performance for stem priming for 4 occipital lobe patients (C. B., J. C., F. N., and N. A.) and for 16 student controls. In contrast to the results from the fragment completion test in which the patients exhibited normal priming scores, all four of the patients tested had extremely low stem completion priming scores scores matched by only the lower quartile of the control subjects. The deficit in stem completion priming is consistent with the stem completion deficits reported for a patient with right occipital lobe damage (e.g., Vaidya et al., 1998; Fleischman et al., 1995; Gabrieli et al., 1995; Keane et al., 1995) and confirms that the deficit observed in that patient is also observed in other similar patients. Based on Experiments 1 and 2, it is difficult to determine why the word-stem and word-fragment tests make different demands on the two hemispheres because the two tests differ in a number of ways. For example in most fragment completion tests, including the one used in Experiment 1, the items are low-frequency words. In 836 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 6

5 contrast, in stem completion tests, including Experiment 2, higher frequency words are used. Other item differences, such as the linguistic class of words used and word length, also differ. Thus, it is possible that differences in materials were responsible for the different outcomes in these two tasks. To address this issue, we examined fragment and stem completion priming in Experiments 3 and 4, respectively, using the same set of words in both experiments. Experiment 3 Experiment 3 tested for fragment priming with five-letter words. The leftmost panel of Figure 2 presents the visual priming performance for fragment completion for 3 occipital lobe patients (C. B., F. N., and N. A.) and 15 student controls. All three of the patients measured had priming scores that fell within the normal range. The results are consistent with those of Experiment 1 in showing that damage to the right occipital lobe does not reduce visual priming effects seen in the fragment completion test. Experiment 4 Experiment 4 tested for stem priming with the same fiveletter words used in the fragment test in Experiment 3. The center panel of Figure 2 presents the visual priming performance for stem completion for 3 occipital lobe patients (C. B., F. N., and N. A.), 1 callosotomy patient ( J. W.), the median for 5 normal older control subjects, and 15 student controls. While the median performance of the older controls is very close to that of the students, the priming scores of all four of the patients fall below that of all of the control subjects. The results are consistent with previous studies, including Experiment 2, indicating that the damage to the right occipital lobe or isolation of the left hemisphere leads to a deficit in visual priming in stem completion. Moreover, because the words were the same as those used in the fragment completion test in Experiment 3, the results indicate that the differences between fragment and stem completion priming cannot be attributed to differences in the type of words used in those tasks. One other potentially critical difference between the stem, and fragment completion tests is in the number of competing completions for each test cue. For most word fragments, there is only a single correct completion (e.g., the only English word that can complete e_e_an_e is elegance), whereas most word stems have numerous correct completions (e.g., sal can be completed with salmon, sales, salute, etc). It is possible that differences in the number of competitors may have led to the dissociation between the two tasks. To test this hypothesis, we developed a set of words that had only a single obvious five-letter completion for each word s stem. Figure 2. (A) Visual priming scores for the five-letter word fragment completion task of Experiment 3. (B) Visual priming scores for the fiveletter word-stem completion task of Experiment 4. (C) Visual priming scores for the fiveletter word unique stem completion task of Experiment 5. The data for the 15 student control subjects in each of Experiments 3 5 are presented in a box-and-whisker format. The medians of the five older control subjects who were tested in both Experiments 4 and 5 are represented by circles. The scores of patients with right occipital lobe lesions (F. N., N. A., and C. B.) are represented by square-shaped symbols; those of the callosotomy patient ( J. W.) are represented by diamond-shaped symbols. Kroll et al. 837

6 Experiment 5 Experiment 5 was similar to Experiment 4, but used word stems that could be completed with only one fiveletter word. The right panel of Figure 2 presents the visual priming performance for stem completion for 3 occipital lobe patients (C. B., F. N., and N. A.), 1 callosotomy patient ( J. W.), the median for 5 normal older control subjects, and 15 student controls. With the unique-solution stem completion task, the priming scores of all four of the patients were normal, indicating that stem completion priming is not impaired by right occipital lobe damage or by isolation of the right hemisphere as long as there are no competing completions to the word stems. DISCUSSION Previous studies have led to conflicting results regarding the importance of the right hemisphere for visual implicit memory for verbal materials. The current study indicates that the discrepancies were due to the different retrieval tasks that were used. Namely, the right hemisphere is not necessary for visual priming as measured on fragment completion or on stem completion tasks in which there is a single possible completion. However, for word stems with multiple completions, damage to the right hemisphere does disrupt visual priming. The finding that normal visual priming on multiple completion stems requires the right hemisphere is consistent with previous studies of a patient with severe right occipital lobe damage who did not exhibit visual priming on the stem completion tests (Vaidya et al., 1998; Fleischman et al., 1995; Gabrieli et al., 1995; Keane et al., 1995) and with divided visual field studies of healthy subjects that indicate that the left hemisphere does not exhibit visual priming effects as measured on the stem completion task (Marsolek et al., 1992; Marsolek & Hudson, 1999). Importantly, all of these studies utilized multiple completion word stems. The finding that fragment completion priming was not disrupted by right occipital lobe damage or by isolation of the right hemisphere is consistent with previous studies of these patients that showed that visual priming on a variety of different tests was not impaired ( Yonelinas et al., 2001) and with divided visual field studies of fragment completion in healthy subjects who exhibit comparable visual priming in the left and right hemispheres (Kroll et al., 2001). The current results are important in showing that visual implicit memory is not rigidly localized in the right hemisphere. That is, the left hemisphere can support normal visual priming when the right occipital lobe has been severely damaged and when the right hemisphere has been isolated from the left. Thus, the results do not support claims that implicit visual priming effects are based on a visual word-form system that is localized in the right hemisphere (Gabrieli, 1998; Schacter, 1994; Schacter & Tulving, 1994; Squire, 1994). Marsolek (1999) proposes that visual implicit memory depends upon multiple components. Form-specific visual priming and abstract visual priming are two of these components that he assumes operate in parallel (rather than in sequence), with the form-specific system operating most efficiently in the right hemisphere and the abstract system operating most efficiently in the left. In this way, the right hemisphere would generally produce the most robust visual priming effects, but task demands might be altered in such a way that visual and abstract priming could be observed in both hemispheres. How, though could the task demands of the different implicit memory tests have produced the pattern of results seen in the current study? In particular, why does the left hemisphere support visual priming on most priming tasks but not with multiple completion word stems? One possibility is that with multiple completion stems, the visual priming effects supported by the isolated left occipital lobe are masked by the contribution of lexical processes that are dominant in that hemisphere. That is, the left hemisphere, like the right, supports visual priming of words, in the sense that the visual networks initially involved in processing an item are primed and thus facilitate reprocessing of the item at time of test. The left hemisphere differs from the right, however, in the sense that lexical processes dominate over visual processes in the left hemisphere. Thus, in the case of multiple completion stems, the lexical system will typically be capable of producing a valid word completion. The lexical system is dominant in the left in the sense that if the lexical system produces a completion, then that completion will be selected for output. Although the left hemisphere visual system may have been primed for the studied item, its contribution will be masked or preempted by the lexical process. In contrast, with single completion word stems, it is not necessary to select between alternative responses and, thus, the contribution of the visual system should be unobscured. Other priming tasks like fragment completion are similar to single completion stems in the sense that the effect of visual (form) processes is observed because the lexical processes cannot produce a valid completion for every test item and thus the less dominant visual processes will contribute to performance. This account is also consistent with the pattern of results observed in divided visual field studies. That is, visual priming effects will be seen in the left hemisphere with single completions because both visual and lexical processes contribute to performance, whereas with multiple completion stems, visual priming effects will be masked by the dominant lexical processes in that hemisphere. In contrast, in the right hemisphere, the lexical process is less likely to be effective, resulting in 838 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 6

7 visual priming effects in that hemisphere for single and multiple completion tasks. The current results show that the standard stem completion task is somewhat unusual, compared to other visual priming tasks, in that it is disrupted by right occipital damage or when the right hemisphere is isolated. Thus, the study joins a growing body of research indicating that word-stem completion is distinct from other visual implicit memory tests. For example, as previously discussed, earlier studies indicated that stem completion is less perceptual and more sensitive to semantic variables than are the other visual priming tasks (e.g., Blum & Yonelinas, 2001; Rueckl & Mathew, 1999; Bassili et al., 1989). The current study focused on visual priming for verbal materials and indicated that it was not localized specifically to the right hemisphere. However, whether visual priming for nonverbal materials exhibit laterality effects is not yet clear. There is some evidence to suggest that the right hemisphere does play a more important role in nonverbal priming effects. For example, larger visual priming effects have been observed in the right than left hemisphere using divided visual field methods (Marsolek, 1999), and neural deactivations associated with priming of visual objects have been reported to be more pronounced over the right than left hemispheres (e.g., Henson, Shallice, Gorno-Tempini, & Dolan, 2002; Vuilleumier, Henson, Driver, & Dolan, 2002; although also see Elliott & Dolan, 1998). Further studies examining visual priming in patients with left and right occipital lobe damage will be useful in determining the role of the two hemispheres in nonverbal visual priming. An explanation of implicit visual memory that is consistent with the existing results is that visual priming is not the product of a single localized memory system that is rigidly localized in one hemisphere or the other, but that it rather reflects the general by-products of previous visual processing. Thus, when both hemispheres are involved in processing visual stimuli, as in healthy subjects, then both hemispheres will exhibit evidence of priming. In contrast, when the left hemisphere is responsible for processing visual stimuli, as with the lesion patients in the current study, then the left hemisphere will exhibit visual priming effects when these priming effects are not masked by other, more compelling left hemisphere processes. METHODS Subjects Four patients with right occipital lobe damage were tested. Three of these patients had incurred right posterior cerebral artery infarcts with right occipital and right inferior temporal occipital lesions (patients J. C., F. N., and N. A.). Patient J. C. suffered a perinatal right posterior cerebral artery infarct resulting in extensive damage to the right occipital temporal cortex. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) revealed complete destruction of the right occipital lobe with damage involving V1, V2, V3, V4, significant amounts of the fusiform and lingual gyri as well as the posterior hippocampal and parahippocampal regions. There was a thin segment of residual tissue extending through the region of extensive cystic encephalomalacia likely representing scar tissue. Patient F. N. suffered bilateral posterior cerebral artery infarcts. There was minimal damage (<1 cm) in the V2 region of the left occipital pole. No visual deficits were detected in the right visual field. Damage in the right hemisphere included all of V1 and medial portions of V2 and V3. The lesion included posterior portions of the fusiform and lingual gyri extending to the margin of the hippocampal and parahippocampal gyri. Damage did not extend to the lateral convexity. Both patients had dense left hemianopsias and neither patient had any evidence of residual form discrimination in their left visual fields (these two patients are discussed in detail in Wessinger, Fendrich, & Gazzaniga, 1997). Patient N. A. sustained an infarct of the right posterior cerebral artery. Damage included extensive loss of V1 and infarction of medial portions of V2 and V3. There was additional extensive damage to fusiform, lingual, hippocampal, and parahippocampal cortices. Patient N. A. had a dense left hemianopsia and difficulty with spatial memory. The fourth patient, C. B., suffered a closed head injury during an equestrian accident requiring surgery to remove a hematoma in the right occipital region. Her CT scan revealed extensive damage to the right occipital and parietal lobes. There was complete loss of V1, V2, V3 as well as extensive damage to the fusiform lingual, hippocampal, and parahippocampal cortices. Damage extended superiorly to include the right inferior and parietal cortices. Patient C. B. had a dense left hemianopsia. In addition, three patients with complete callosotomies (patients V. J., J. W., and D. R.) were tested. For these patients, the callosotomies were carried out in two stages as treatment for intractable epilepsy. No extracallosal damage was noted in V. J. or J. W., but D. R. s MRI revealed spared fibers in the rostrum and an area of hyperintense signal in the region of the quadrigeminal cistern. Neither D. R. nor V. J. was able to make verbal responses to stimuli presented to the left visual field (Baynes, Tramo, & Gazzaniga, 1992; Baynes, Eliassen, Lutsep, & Gazzaniga, 1998), and J. W. s ability to make verbal responses to left visual field stimuli is limited (Gazzaniga et al., 1996; Baynes, Wessinger, Fendrich, & Gazzaniga, 1995). The demographic and neuropsychological data for the lesion patients is presented in Table 2 and Figure 3. Standard test scores were obtained after trauma. The patients performed well on the IQ and memory tests with the following exceptions. D. R. and V. J. demonstrated borderline performance IQs (about 2 SD below the mean) that were significantly below the level of their Kroll et al. 839

8 Table 2. Demographic and Neuropsychological Data for the Lesion Patients Group Right Occipital Lesion Callosotomy Participant J. C. F. N. N. A. C. B. J. W. D. R. V. J. Sex female male male female male female female Birth Trauma year perinatal Verbal IQ Performance IQ Delayed Memory Index For right occipital lobe patients, the trauma year is the year of the infarct or, in the case of C. B., the year of her equestrian accident. For callosotomy patients, the trauma year is the year of the surgery. respective verbal IQs. J. C. exhibited low-average verbal and performance IQ (about 1 SD below the mean). All three callosotomy patients performed below normal on the delayed memory index. The patients were tested over a period between 1994 and 2001 during the course of other behavioral testing. Unfortunately, some patients became unavailable during this period and it was not possible to test all of the Figure 3. Computer reconstructions of magnetic resonance brain scans for each of the four patients with right occipital lesions. Red areas represent site of lesion on transverse sections. The lateral view indicates the level and orientation of each section from the most ventral section (1) to the most dorsal (12). The bottom row represents a summing over all patients. 840 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 6

9 patients on all the tasks. For those patients tested in more than one experiment, a minimum of 2 weeks separated each pair of tests, with the exception of N. A. whose tests were separated by a single week. Student controls were obtained from undergraduate psychology courses at the University of California Davis and were given class credit for their participation. Twentyseven were tested in Experiment 1, 16 in Experiment 2, and 15 in each of Experiments 3 5. Older controls recruited from the local community were selected to be similar in age to the older patients and were paid for their participation. Sixteen older controls were tested in Experiment 1 (Yonelinas et al., 2001) and five older controls (males, aged 61 64) were tested in both Experiments 4 and 5. Three of these were tested first in Experiment 4 and the other two were tested first in Experiment 5. The two tests were separated by a minimum of 2 weeks. Note that the student controls are approximately the same age as C. B., who was herself a student at the time of testing. Design and Procedure Visual presentation was via a 15-in. computer monitor. The stimuli were white words centered on a black background and using DOS text-mode font. Experiment 1 Forty 8-letter words were presented visually to the participants, and 40 other 8-letter words were read to the participants by the examiner. These were presented in interleaved blocks of 20 items, and subjects were instructed to carefully attend to each word. Then, 160 visual word fragments (e.g., _c_sso_s), 80 studied words plus 80 new words, were presented, and subjects were given 10 sec to complete each fragment with the first word that came to mind. Materials were counterbalanced across subjects to equate the difficulty level of auditory and visual word lists. Experiment 2 Fifty words (5 primacy words, 40 critical words, and 5 recency words) were presented visually to the participants, and 50 other words (5 primacy words, 40 critical words, and 5 recency words) were read to the participants by the examiner. These were presented in interleaved blocks of 25 words, and subjects were instructed to carefully attend to each word. Then, 120 visual word stems (e.g., pea ), 80 of the studied words plus 40 of the new words, were presented, and subjects had 10 sec to complete each stem with the first word that came to mind. The 120 words, graciously supplied by Dr. Larry Squire, had been ranked by frequency, number of possible words beginning with its first three letters (stem), number of pretest subjects who chose that word to complete that stem by chance, and the length of word; then assigned to three lists of 40 each so that each list had roughly the same average and distribution on each of the above measures. The sequencing of study modalities and the assignment of words to lists (i.e., visual study, auditory study, and new lists) were counterbalanced across subjects to equate the difficulty of word lists. Experiment 3 Fifty-four words were presented visually to the participants, and 54 other words were read to the participants by the examiner. The subjects judged the number of syllables in each word. Half of the subjects received 27 visual words, then 54 auditory words, and then 27 visual words. The other subjects received the opposite ordering of the modalities. After finishing the study list, subjects were shown the 162 word fragments (e.g., g_e_d or _w_s_) of the 108 studied words, plus those of 54 new words. Subjects had 10 sec to complete each fragment. Lists were counterbalanced to equate the difficulty of fragments across modalities. Pilot studies, where 20 students were just given the fragments without a preceding study period, were used to rank the fragments for difficulty and words were assigned to lists in such a way as to balance the difficulty of the lists. Experiment 4 The same procedure was used as in Experiment 3, except that now the test stimuli were word stems (e.g., gre ). The words used were the same as in Experiment 3. The average Kucera Francis written frequency value for the words used in both experiments was Experiment 5 The same procedure was used as in Experiment 4, except that now the words used were the only fiveletter words that would complete their stems. For example, the stem pau can only be completed by the word pause. In a few cases, alternatives existed but were of such low frequency that subjects were unlikely to know them. For example, the stem lig can be completed by both light and the extremely low frequency liger. The average Kucera Francis written frequency value for the words used in this experiment was Acknowledgments We are very grateful to C. B., F. N., J. W., N. A., J. C., D. R., and V. J. for their full and enthusiastic participation in this study. Research in this article was supported by NIMH MH (A. Y.), NIDCD RO1 DC (K. B.), and NINDS grant NS21135 (R. T. K.). Kroll et al. 841

10 Reprint requests should be sent to Neal Kroll, Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, USA, or via Note 1. Although the right occipital lobe is present in callosotomy patients, the resection of the corpus callosum prevents communication between the hemispheres and verbal responses are generally considered to reflect processing within the isolated left hemisphere. Thus, facilitation observed in verbal responses of callosotomy patients should reflect only left hemisphere processes. The similarity in the pattern of results across experiments for the two groups of patients supports this hypothesis. REFERENCES Bassili, J. N., Smith, M. C., & MacLeod, C. M. (1989). Presentation modality and type of processing effects on priming in auditory and visual word-stem completion. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 41A, Baynes, K., Eliassen, J. C., Lutsep, H. L., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1998). Modular organization of cognitive systems masked by interhemispheric integration. Science, 280, Baynes, K., Tramo, M. J., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1992). Reading with a limited lexicon in the right hemisphere of a callosotomy patient. Neuropsychologia, 30, Baynes, K., Wessinger, C. M., Fendrich, R., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1995). The emergence of the capacity to name left visual field stimuli in a callosotomy patient: Implications for functional plasticity. Neuropsychologia, 33, Blum, D., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2001). Transfer across modality in perceptual implicit memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, Burgund, E. D., & Marsolek, C. J. (1997). Letter-case specific priming in the right cerebral hemisphere with a form-specific perceptual identification task. Brain and Cognition, 35, Curran, T., Schachter, D. L., & Galluccio, L. (1999). Cross-modal priming and explicit memory in patients with verbal production deficits. Brain and Cognition, 39, Elliott, R., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Neural responses during preference and memory judgments for subliminally presented stimuli: A functional neuroimaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, Fleischman, D. A., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Reminger, S., Rinaldi, J., Morrell, F., & Wilson, R. (1995). Conceptual priming in perceptual identification for patients with Alzheimer s disease and a patient with right occipital lobectomy. Neuropsychology, 9, Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, Gabrieli, J. D. E., Fleischman, D. A., Keane, M. M., Reminger, S. L., & Morrell, F. (1995). Double dissociation between memory systems underlying explicit and implicit memory in the human brain. Psychological Science, 6, Gazzaniga, M. S., Eliassen, J. C., Nisenson, L., Wessinger, C. M., Fendrich, R., & Baynes, K. (1996). Collaboration between the hemispheres of a callosotomy patient: Emerging right hemisphere speech and the left hemisphere interpreter. Brain, 119, Henson, R. N. A., Shallice, T., Gorno-Tempini, M. I., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Face repetition effects in implicit and explicit memory tests as measured by fmri. Cerebral Cortex, 12, Jacoby, L. L. (1983). Perceptual enhancement: Persistent effects of experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, Keane, M. M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Mapstone, H. C., Johnson, K. A., & Corkin, S. (1995). Double dissociation of memory capacities after bilateral occipital-lobe or medial temporal lobe lesions. Brain, 118, Koivisto, M. (1995). On functional brain asymmetries in perceptual priming. Brain and Cognition, 29, Kroll, N. E. A., Rocha, D. A., Yonelinas, A. P., Baynes, K., & Frederick, C. (2001). Form-specific visual priming in the left and right hemispheres. Brain and Cognition, 47, Marsolek, C. J. (1999). Dissociable neural subsystems underlie abstract and specific object recognition. Psychological Science, 10, Marsolek, C. J., & Hudson, T. E. (1999). Task and stimulus demands influence letter-case specific priming in the right cerebral hemisphere. Laterality, 4, Marsolek, C. J., Kosslyn, S. M., & Squire, L. R. (1992). Form specific visual priming in the right cerebral hemisphere. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, Roediger, H. L., III, & Blaxton, T. A. (1987). Effects of varying modality, surface features, and retention interval on priming in word fragment completion. Memory and Cognition, 15, Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory in normal human subjects. Handbook of Neuropsychology, 8, Rueckl, J. G., & Mathew, S. (1999). Implicit memory for phonological processes in visual stem completion. Memory and Cognition, 27, Rybash, J. (1996). Memory aging research: Real life and laboratory relationships. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, Schacter, D. L. (1992). Priming and multiple memory systems: Perceptual mechanisms of implicit memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, Schacter, D. L. (1994) In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp ). Cambridge: MIT. Schacter, D. L., Chiu, C. Y. P., & Ochsner, K. N. (1993). Implicit memory A selective review. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16. Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (1994). In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 1 38). Cambridge: MIT. Squire, L. R. (1994). In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp ). Cambridge: MIT. Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Verfaellie, M., Fleischman, D., & Askari, N. (1998). Font-specific priming following global amnesia and occipital lobe damage. Neuropsychology, 12, Vuilleumier, P., Henson, R. N., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Multiple levels of visual object constancy revealed by event-related fmri of repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 5, Wessinger, C. M., Fendrich, R., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1997). Islands of residual vision in hemianopic patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, Yonelinas, A. P., Kroll, N., Baynes, K., Dobbins, I. G., Frederick, C. M., Knight, R. T., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2001). Visual implicit memory in the left hemisphere: Evidence from callosotomy and right occipital-lobe lesion patients. Psychological Science, 12, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 15, Number 6

Cross-Modal Priming and Explicit Memory in Patients with Verbal Production Deficits

Cross-Modal Priming and Explicit Memory in Patients with Verbal Production Deficits Brain and Cognition 39, 133 146 (1999) Article ID brcg.1998.1063, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Cross-Modal Priming and Explicit Memory in Patients with Verbal Production Deficits Tim

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Theories of Sensory, short-term & long-term memories Memory & brain Cellular bases

More information

Importance of Deficits

Importance of Deficits Importance of Deficits In complex systems the parts are often so integrated that they cannot be detected in normal operation Need to break the system to discover the components not just physical components

More information

Deficits in Recall Following Partial and Complete Commissurotomy

Deficits in Recall Following Partial and Complete Commissurotomy Deficits in Recall Following Partial and Complete Commissurotomy Elizabeth A. Phelps, 1 William Hirst, 2 and Michael S. Gazzaniga 3 1 Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory TENNET XI 135 Results Allocentric WM. There was no difference between lesioned and control rats; i.e., there was an equal proportion of rats from the two groups in the target quadrant (saline, 100%; lesioned,

More information

A dissociation between spatial and identity matching in callosotomy patients

A dissociation between spatial and identity matching in callosotomy patients Cognitive Neuroscience, 8±87 (999) ALTHOUGH they are structurally similar, the two hemispheres of the human brain have many functional asymmetries. Some of these, such as language and motor control, have

More information

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Henry Molaison From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Henry Gustav Molaison (February 26, 1926 December 2, 2008), known widely as H.M., was an American memory disorder patient who had a bilateral medial

More information

Summarized by. Biointelligence Laboratory, Seoul National University

Summarized by. Biointelligence Laboratory, Seoul National University Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 3 rd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2008. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and db.-t. TZhang Biointelligence

More information

Morris water maze: standard test for spatial memory in rodents

Morris water maze: standard test for spatial memory in rodents Vertebrate Models: The Hippocampus 34 Vertebrate Models: The Hippocampus 35 Vertebrate Models: The Hippocampus 36 Vertebrate Models: The Hippocampus 37 Animal Models of Learning (Vertebrates) Morris water

More information

A Comparison of Forgetting for Conscious and Automatic Memory Processes in Word Fragment Completion Tasks

A Comparison of Forgetting for Conscious and Automatic Memory Processes in Word Fragment Completion Tasks Journal of Memory and Language 45, 585 615 (2001) doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2792, available online at http://www.academicpress.com on A Comparison of Forgetting for Conscious and Automatic Memory Processes

More information

When Amnesic Patients Perform Well on Recognition Memory Tests

When Amnesic Patients Perform Well on Recognition Memory Tests Behavioral Neuroscience 1997, Vol. Ill, No. 6,1163-117 In the public domain When Amnesic Patients Perform Well on Recognition Memory Tests Jonathan M. Reed University of California, San Diego Stephan B.

More information

Memory. Psychology 3910 Guest Lecture by Steve Smith

Memory. Psychology 3910 Guest Lecture by Steve Smith Memory Psychology 3910 Guest Lecture by Steve Smith Note: Due to copyright restrictions, I had to remove the images from the Weschler Memory Scales from the slides I posted online. Wechsler Memory Scales

More information

Remember/Know Judgments Probe Degrees of Recollection

Remember/Know Judgments Probe Degrees of Recollection Remember/Know Judgments Probe Degrees of Recollection Peter E. Wais, Laura Mickes, and John T. Wixted Abstract & Remembering and knowing are states of awareness that accompany the retrieval of facts, faces,

More information

An electrophysiological analysis of modality-specific aspects of word repetition

An electrophysiological analysis of modality-specific aspects of word repetition Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 655 665. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research An electrophysiological analysis of modality-specific aspects

More information

MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0

MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0 MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0 Dawn M. McBride Methods for Measuring Conscious and Automatic Memory A Brief Review Abstract: Memory researchers have discussed the relationship between consciousness and memory

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Types and Structure of Memory Types of Memory Type of Memory Time Course Capacity Conscious Awareness Mechanism of Loss Sensory Short-Term and Working Long-Term Nondeclarative

More information

Excellent Network Courses. Department of Neurology Affiliated hospital of Jiangsu University

Excellent Network Courses. Department of Neurology Affiliated hospital of Jiangsu University Excellent Network Courses Department of Neurology Affiliated hospital of Jiangsu University Agnosia Visual Agnosia Lissauer (1890) described 2 types: a) Apperceptive Cannot see objects b) Associative Does

More information

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition 1 Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition Intro Memory Empirical Studies Interf MR Scan LTM Codes DCT Imagery & Spatial Cognition Rel Org Principles ImplEnc SpatEq Neuro Imaging Critique StruEq Prop

More information

The Effects of Unitization on Familiarity-Based Source Memory: Testing a Behavioral Prediction Derived From Neuroimaging Data

The Effects of Unitization on Familiarity-Based Source Memory: Testing a Behavioral Prediction Derived From Neuroimaging Data Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2008, Vol. 34, No. 4, 730 740 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.730

More information

A systems neuroscience approach to memory

A systems neuroscience approach to memory A systems neuroscience approach to memory Critical brain structures for declarative memory Relational memory vs. item memory Recollection vs. familiarity Recall vs. recognition What about PDs? R-K paradigm

More information

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning John Gabrieli Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Institute for Medical Engineering & Sciences McGovern Institute for Brain Sciences MIT Levels of Analysis

More information

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg The concept that different parts of the brain did different things started with Spurzheim and Gall, whose phrenology became quite

More information

On the relationship between recognition familiarity and perceptual uency: Evidence for distinct mnemonic processes

On the relationship between recognition familiarity and perceptual uency: Evidence for distinct mnemonic processes Acta Psychologica 98 (1998) 211±230 On the relationship between recognition familiarity and perceptual uency: Evidence for distinct mnemonic processes Anthony D. Wagner *, John D.E. Gabrieli Department

More information

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (6), 1067-1073 Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources MOSHE NAVEH-BENJAMIN University of Missouri,

More information

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective MILITARY PSYCHOLOGY, 21:(Suppl. 1)S108 S112, 2009 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0899-5605 print / 1532-7876 online DOI: 10.1080/08995600802554748 Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future:

More information

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005,?? 12 (?), (5),???-??? 852-857 BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval LARRY L. JACOBY, YUJIRO SHIMIZU,

More information

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION INTRODUCTION Research on the Deaf Brain is beginning to provide a new evidence base for policy and practice in relation to intervention with deaf children. This talk outlines the multi-channel nature of

More information

The contribution of recollection and familiarity to yes±no and forced-choice recognition tests in healthy subjects and amnesics

The contribution of recollection and familiarity to yes±no and forced-choice recognition tests in healthy subjects and amnesics Neuropsychologia 38 (2000) 1333±1341 www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia The contribution of recollection and familiarity to yes±no and forced-choice recognition tests in healthy subjects and amnesics

More information

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. Provided for non-commercial research and educational use. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This article was originally published in the Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference,

More information

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni VISUAL AGNOSIA -damage to the extrastriate visual regions (occipital, parietal and temporal lobes) disrupts recognition of complex visual stimuli

More information

The effects of generation on auditory implicit memory

The effects of generation on auditory implicit memory Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (6), 1157-1167 doi: 10.3758/MC.36.6.1157 The effects of generation on auditory implicit memory ILANA A T. Z. DEW AND NEIL W. MULLIGAN University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill,

More information

Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) 2100 2106 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Intact implicit verbal relational memory

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate Supplementary Results Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate behavioral experiment was conducted (n = 16) to verify (a) that retrieval-induced forgetting is observed

More information

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Author's response to reviews Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Authors: Julia Miro (juliamirollado@gmail.com) Pablo Ripollès (pablo.ripolles.vidal@gmail.com)

More information

The origins of localization

The origins of localization Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. The origins of localization The concept that different parts of the brain did different

More information

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D.

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. The origins of localization The concept that different parts of the brain did different

More information

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice Multiple Choice 1. Which structure is not part of the visual pathway in the brain? a. occipital lobe b. optic chiasm c. lateral geniculate nucleus *d. frontal lobe Answer location: Visual Pathways 2. Which

More information

Convergent Behavioral and Neuropsychological Evidence for a Distinction Between Identification and Production Forms of Repetition Priming

Convergent Behavioral and Neuropsychological Evidence for a Distinction Between Identification and Production Forms of Repetition Priming Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1999, Vol. 128, No. 4,479-498 Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-3445/99/S3.00 Convergent Behavioral and Neuropsychological

More information

DISSOCIABLE NEURAL SUBSYSTEMS UNDERLIE VISUAL WORKING MEMORY FOR ABSTRACT CATEGORIES AND SPECIFIC EXEMPLARS

DISSOCIABLE NEURAL SUBSYSTEMS UNDERLIE VISUAL WORKING MEMORY FOR ABSTRACT CATEGORIES AND SPECIFIC EXEMPLARS (in press) Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience Visual Working Memory 1 DISSOCIABLE NEURAL SUBSYSTEMS UNDERLIE VISUAL WORKING MEMORY FOR ABSTRACT CATEGORIES AND SPECIFIC EXEMPLARS Chad J.

More information

Neuroimaging studies of priming

Neuroimaging studies of priming Progress in Neurobiology 70 (2003) 53 81 Neuroimaging studies of priming R.N.A. Henson a,b, a Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, UK b Wellcome Department of

More information

Myers Psychology for AP*

Myers Psychology for AP* Myers Psychology for AP* David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2010 *AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board, which

More information

Recognition Memory for Single Items and for Associations Is Similarly Impaired Following Damage to the Hippocampal Region

Recognition Memory for Single Items and for Associations Is Similarly Impaired Following Damage to the Hippocampal Region Research Recognition Memory for Single Items and for Associations Is Similarly Impaired Following Damage to the Hippocampal Region Craig E.L. Stark, 1 Peter J. Bayley, 2 and Larry R. Squire 2,3,4 1 Departments

More information

Learning Objectives.

Learning Objectives. Emilie O Neill, class of 2016 Learning Objectives 1. Describe the types of deficits that occur with lesions in association areas including: prosopagnosia, neglect, aphasias, agnosia, apraxia 2. Discuss

More information

The frontal lobe role in memory: a review of convergent evidence and implications for the Wada memory test

The frontal lobe role in memory: a review of convergent evidence and implications for the Wada memory test Epilepsy & Behavior 3 (2002) 309 315 Review The frontal lobe role in memory: a review of convergent evidence and implications for the Wada memory test Jeffrey G. Ojemann a, * and William M. Kelley b a

More information

Level-of-Processing Effects in Word-Completion Priming: A Neuropsychological Study

Level-of-Processing Effects in Word-Completion Priming: A Neuropsychological Study Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1996, Vol. 22, No. 4,933-947 In the public domain Level-of-Processing Effects in Word-Completion Priming: A Neuropsychological Study

More information

New Conceptual Associative Learning in Amnesia: A Case Study

New Conceptual Associative Learning in Amnesia: A Case Study Journal of Memory and Language 43, 291 315 (2000) doi:10.1006/jmla.2000.2733, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on New Conceptual Associative Learning in Amnesia: A Case Study Suparna Rajaram

More information

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Neuroscience of Consciousness II 1 C83MAB: Mind and Brain Neuroscience of Consciousness II Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Consciousness State of consciousness - Being awake/alert/attentive/responsive Contents

More information

Serial model. Amnesia. Amnesia. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras. Lecture 3: HM, the medial temporal lobe, and amnesia

Serial model. Amnesia. Amnesia. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras. Lecture 3: HM, the medial temporal lobe, and amnesia Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Serial model Memory terminology based on information processing models e.g., Serial Model Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras Lecture 3: HM, the medial temporal lobe, and amnesia

More information

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy Jeff Ojemann, MD Department of Neurological Surgery University of Washington Children s Hospital & Regional

More information

Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming

Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming Brain (2000), 123, 620 640 Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming Randy L. Buckner, 1,3 Wilma Koutstaal, 2 Daniel L. Schacter 2 and Bruce

More information

Introduction. Reder, Donavos, & Erickson (2000) demonstrated that two types of perceptual effects occur for recognition memory:

Introduction. Reder, Donavos, & Erickson (2000) demonstrated that two types of perceptual effects occur for recognition memory: Introduction This work is concerned with the representation and processing of perceptual information in memory and their influence on explicit and implicit memory performance. Reder, Donavos, & Erickson

More information

Presurgical Language Mapping Kathleen B. McDermott, 1 Jason M. Watson, 1 and Jeffrey G. Ojemann 2

Presurgical Language Mapping Kathleen B. McDermott, 1 Jason M. Watson, 1 and Jeffrey G. Ojemann 2 CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Presurgical Language Mapping Kathleen B. McDermott, 1 Jason M. Watson, 1 and Jeffrey G. Ojemann 2 1 Washington University in St. Louis and 2 University of Washington

More information

Introduction to Long-Term Memory

Introduction to Long-Term Memory Introduction to Long-Term Memory Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/26/2018: Lecture 05-4 Note: This Powerpoint presentation may contain macros that I wrote to help me create

More information

Nicole M. Dudukovic, Ph.D. Department of Psychology 1227 University of Oregon Eugene, OR Phone:

Nicole M. Dudukovic, Ph.D. Department of Psychology 1227 University of Oregon Eugene, OR Phone: Nicole M. Dudukovic, Ph.D. Department of Psychology 1227 University of Oregon Eugene, OR 97403 Email: ndudukov@uoregon.edu Phone: 541-346-7225 EDUCATION 2007 Stanford University, Ph.D., Psychology (Cognitive

More information

Do all these faces look familiar? Can you name them all? Why is it difficult to recall names even though you can recognize them? More generally, why

Do all these faces look familiar? Can you name them all? Why is it difficult to recall names even though you can recognize them? More generally, why Do all these faces look familiar? Can you name them all? Why is it difficult to recall names even though you can recognize them? More generally, why do we forget things? Learning Causes Forgetting: Interference

More information

Higher Cortical Function

Higher Cortical Function Emilie O Neill, class of 2016 Higher Cortical Function Objectives Describe the association cortical areas processing sensory, motor, executive, language, and emotion/memory information (know general location

More information

Lag-Sensitive Repetition Suppression Effects in the Anterior Parahippocampal Gyrus

Lag-Sensitive Repetition Suppression Effects in the Anterior Parahippocampal Gyrus HIPPOCAMPUS 15:557 561 (2005) RAPID COMMUNICATION Lag-Sensitive Repetition Suppression Effects in the Anterior Parahippocampal Gyrus Craig J. Brozinsky,* Andrew P. Yonelinas, Neal E.A. Kroll, and Charan

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Based on: Chapter 18 and 19, Breedlove, Watson, Rosenzweig, 6e/7e. Cerebral Cortex Brain s most complex area with billions of neurons and

More information

Intact Conceptual Priming in the Absence of Declarative Memory D.A. Levy, 1,2 C.E.L. Stark, 3 and L.R. Squire 1,2,4,5

Intact Conceptual Priming in the Absence of Declarative Memory D.A. Levy, 1,2 C.E.L. Stark, 3 and L.R. Squire 1,2,4,5 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Intact Conceptual Priming in the Absence of Declarative Memory D.A. Levy, 1,2 C.E.L. Stark, 3 and L.R. Squire 1,2,4,5 1 Veterans Affairs Healthcare System, San Diego,

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu www.cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html n Learning and Memory n Human Communication n Emotion 1 What is memory? n Working Memory:

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (3), 490-494 Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness GINO CAMP, DIANE PECHER, and HENK G. SCHMIDT Erasmus University Rotterdam,

More information

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH Marie Rivenez 1,2, Chris Darwin 1, Anne Guillaume 2 1 Department of Psychology University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK 2 Département Sciences Cognitives Institut

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition

The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition Rachel J. Garoff-Eaton, Elizabeth A. Kensinger and Daniel L. Schacter Learn. Mem. 2007 14: 684-692 Access the most recent version at

More information

Memory Processes in Perceptual Decision Making

Memory Processes in Perceptual Decision Making Memory Processes in Perceptual Decision Making Manish Saggar (mishu@cs.utexas.edu), Risto Miikkulainen (risto@cs.utexas.edu), Department of Computer Science, University of Texas at Austin, TX, 78712 USA

More information

fmri (functional MRI)

fmri (functional MRI) Lesion fmri (functional MRI) Electroencephalogram (EEG) Brainstem CT (computed tomography) Scan Medulla PET (positron emission tomography) Scan Reticular Formation MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) Thalamus

More information

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and 77 5 Discussion The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and implicit learning systems. Chapter 2 described the effects of performing a working memory task

More information

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Tracey Miser (miser.4@osu.edu) Department of Psychology, 1835 Neil Avenue Columbus, OH43210 USA Vladimir Sloutsky (sloutsky.1@osu.edu)

More information

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight? How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? What happens to visual-devoted brain structure in individuals who are born deprived of sight?

More information

central sulcus parietal lobe frontal lobe occipital lobe Sylvian fissure temporal lobe

central sulcus parietal lobe frontal lobe occipital lobe Sylvian fissure temporal lobe frontal lobe central sulcus parietal lobe occipital lobe Sylvian fissure temporal lobe The lobes of the brain. Note that the crease along the top of the temporal lobe is the Sylvian fissure, which divides

More information

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m. Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Wednesday, 16 March

More information

The effect of retrieval instructions on false recognition: exploring the nature of the gist memory impairment in amnesia

The effect of retrieval instructions on false recognition: exploring the nature of the gist memory impairment in amnesia Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 2360 2368 The effect of retrieval instructions on false recognition: exploring the nature of the gist memory impairment in amnesia Mieke Verfaellie a,, Daniel L. Schacter b,

More information

HBEV: Non-Print Items

HBEV: Non-Print Items Non-Print Items Abstract: Amnesia is a neurobehavioral syndrome characterized by a selective impairment in memory in the context of preserved intelligence and other cognitive abilities. Permanent amnesia

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Experimental Design Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Overview Design of functional neuroimaging studies Categorical designs Factorial designs Parametric

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html Memory Working Memory Long-term Memory Declarative Memory Procedural Memory

More information

DO NOT CITE WITHOUT PERMISSION OF AUTHOR. Some Thoughts on the Interaction between Perception and Reflection. Julie A. Higgins & Marcia K.

DO NOT CITE WITHOUT PERMISSION OF AUTHOR. Some Thoughts on the Interaction between Perception and Reflection. Julie A. Higgins & Marcia K. Higgins, J. A., & Johnson, M. K. (in press). Some thoughts on the interaction between perception and reflection. In J.M. Wolfe & L. Robertson (Eds.), From Perception to Consciousness: Searching with Anne

More information

Disparity of Non-verbal Language Learning

Disparity of Non-verbal Language Learning Bethel Sileshi Disparity of Non-verbal Language Learning Identical Rates of Acquisition, Neurological Adaptations, Cognitive Outcomes Science 100 Capstone, University of Alberta April 7, 2014 Introduction

More information

A Basic-Systems Approach to Autobiographical Memory David C. Rubin

A Basic-Systems Approach to Autobiographical Memory David C. Rubin CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE A Basic-Systems Approach to Autobiographical Memory David C. Rubin Duke University ABSTRACT Memory for complex everyday events involving vision, hearing, smell,

More information

The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research

The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research Journal of Memory and Language 46, 441 517 (2002) doi:10.1006/jmla.2002.2864, available online at http://www.academicpress.com on The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research

More information

Psychology Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d e. 2. a b c d e 14. a b c d e

Psychology Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d e. 2. a b c d e 14. a b c d e 1 7 Psychology 3450 - Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A Name: MUN ID: Indicate your answer by crossing out the appropriate letter. Answer 20 questions only. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d

More information

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience Levels of organization Central Nervous System 1m 10 11 neurons Neural systems and neuroanatomy Systems 10cm Networks 1mm Neurons 100μm 10 8 neurons Professor Daniel Leeds

More information

Under What Conditions Is Recognition Spared Relative to Recall After Selective Hippocampal Damage in Humans?

Under What Conditions Is Recognition Spared Relative to Recall After Selective Hippocampal Damage in Humans? Under What Conditions Is Recognition Spared Relative to Recall After Selective Hippocampal Damage in Humans? J.S. Holdstock, 1 * A.R. Mayes, 1 N.Roberts, 3 E. Cezayirli, 3 C.L. Isaac, 2 R.C. O Reilly,

More information

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1428 1442 Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Alessandra Schiavetto a, Stefan Köhler a, Cheryl L. Grady a, Gordon Winocur a,c,

More information

Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Studies

Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Studies Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Studies Polina Golland 1, Bruce Fischl 2, Mona Spiridon 3, Nancy Kanwisher 3, Randy L. Buckner 4, Martha E. Shenton 5, Ron Kikinis 6, Anders Dale 2, and W. Eric

More information

Insights into the functional specificity of the human corpus callosum

Insights into the functional specificity of the human corpus callosum Brain (2000), 123, 920 926 Insights into the functional specificity of the human corpus callosum Margaret G. Funnell, Paul M. Corballis and Michael S. Gazzaniga Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Dartmouth

More information

MEMORY. Announcements. Practice Question 2. Practice Question 1 10/3/2012. Next Quiz available Oct 11

MEMORY. Announcements. Practice Question 2. Practice Question 1 10/3/2012. Next Quiz available Oct 11 Announcements Next Quiz available Oct 11 Due Oct 16 MEMORY Practice Question 1 Practice Question 2 What type of operant conditioning is Stewie using to get attention from his mom? A rercer that acquires

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) 3439 3447 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Neural correlates of contextual cueing

More information

The Neurophysiology of Memory

The Neurophysiology of Memory The Neurophysiology of Memory WENDY A. SUZUKI a,c AND HOWARD EICHENBAUM b a Center for Neural Science, New York University, 4 Washington Place, Room 809, New York, New York 10003, USA b Laboratory of Cognitive

More information

HAL author manuscript

HAL author manuscript HAL author manuscript Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior 2008;44(2):10 The neural correlates of visual mental imagery: an ongoing debate Paolo Bartolomeo Inserm Unit

More information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information The Journal of Neuroscience, 2000, Vol. 20 RC108 1of5 Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information Charan Ranganath, 1 Marcia K. Johnson, 2 and Mark

More information

Recall in Penfield Experiment

Recall in Penfield Experiment Multiple Memory Systems LTM isn t a single unitary system Different types of systems Systems mediated by different areas of the brain Penfield (1955) Recall in Penfield Experiment Recalled childhood events,

More information

Implicit memory. Schendan, HE. University of Plymouth https://pearl.plymouth.ac.uk

Implicit memory. Schendan, HE. University of Plymouth https://pearl.plymouth.ac.uk University of Plymouth PEARL https://pearl.plymouth.ac.uk 14 Medicine, Dentistry and Health Sciences 2012-03-02 Implicit memory Schendan, HE http://hdl.handle.net/10026.1/1207 Academic Press All content

More information

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition Chapter 2 The Neural Basis of Cognition Capgras Syndrome Alzheimer s patients & others delusion that significant others are robots or impersonators - paranoia Two brain systems for facial recognition -

More information

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus FRONTAL LOBE Central Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Cingulate Lateral Lateral View Medial View Motor execution and higher cognitive functions (e.g., language production, impulse inhibition, reasoning

More information