Moderating effects of need for cognition on attractiveness stereotyping. Samantha Hansen. Algoma University College

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Moderating effects of need for cognition on attractiveness stereotyping. Samantha Hansen. Algoma University College"

Transcription

1 Moderating 1 Running head: NEED FOR COGNITION AND ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPING SP PSYC HAN 97/98 RESERVE Moderating effects of need for cognition on attractiveness stereotyping Samantha Hansen Algoma University College SP PSYC HAN 97/98 RESERVE

2 Moderating 2 Abstract The following paper is a review of the literature regarding both attractiveness stereotyping and the personality construct, need for cognition. First identified by Dion, Bersheid and Walster (1972), the attractiveness stereotype has led to copious investigation into the factors underlying the stereotype. Recent attention has focused on individual difference variables that might moderate its effects. The findings of Dion and Dion (1987) suggest that a person's belief in a just world moderates the attractiveness bias via status cues. In this review, a second such variable, need for cognition, that has been found to relate to routes of persuasion (e.g., peripheral), is proposed as a moderating variable in attractiveness stereotyping.

3 Moderating 3 Moderating Effects of Need for Cognition on Attractiveness Stereotyping During the past 25 years, much research has been generated concerning the attractiveness stereotype; this refers to the tendency to attribute more positive characteristics to physically attractive, compared to unattractive, individuals. Pervasive and widespread, this bias toward perceiving attractive people as more socially desirable than unattractive people, has a tremendous impact on interpersonal relations in all facets of life. While individuals appear to differ on the extent to which they implement this bias. Recently, researchers have undertaken to identify those individual difference variables which might serve to moderate this bias in social perception. The present paper reviews those variables thus far identified as moderators and proposes that the personality construct "need for cognition" may be one such variable that influences the degree to which people bias their social perceptions in favour of attractive persons. Attractiveness Stereotyping In their landmark study appropriately labelled, "What is Beautiful is Good", Dion, Bersheid and Walster (1972) first demonstrated the existence of the attractiveness stereotype. Participants were asked to rate photographs of men and women previously determined to be either attractive, unattractive or of average attractiveness, on 27 different personality traits. They were also asked to estimate the target person's likelihood of life success and happiness. Results

4 Moderating 4 indicated that a strong bias in favour of attractive people existed; physically attractive persons were generally assumed to possess more socially desirable personalities than those less physically appealing. Specifically, attractive targets were perceived to be more poised, interesting, sociable, sexy, sensitive, kind, outgoing, and strong than unattractive targets. In addition, they were also expected to attain greater life success and happiness than their unattractive counterparts. Several subsequent studies have confirmed the existence of the attractiveness bias (e.g., Dion & Dion, 1987; Adams & Huston, 1975). This bias appears to be pervasive, existing across the lifespan, regardless of target age. The bias has been identified when infants are judging adults (Langlois & Roggman, 1990), adults are judging children (Dion, 1972), and adults are judging other adults (Adams & Huston, 1975). In short, those who are physically attractive are perceived more favourably than those who are unattractive. In a meta-analysis of the research on attractiveness stereotyping, Feingold (1992) concluded that physically attractive men and women were perceived as more sociable, dominant, sexually warm, mentally healthy and socially skilled, but not as possessing greater character (e.g., honesty, genuineness, morality, etc.) than physically unattractive individuals. Feingold's meta-analysis of physical attractiveness indicated no significant relationship existed between attractiveness and basic personality traits such as sociability,

5 Moderating 5 dominance and mental health, however, a modest correlation existed between self-ratings of attractiveness and personality traits when these latter traits were related to social behaviour. Those who considered themselves to be attractive tended to experience less loneliness, social anxiety and self-consciousness than those who saw themselves as unattractive. The concerns stimulated by Feingold's (1992) assertion that the pervasiveness of such a stereotype has far-reaching implications on personal perception, social success, academic achievement and life satisfaction are valid because relative attractiveness remains fairly stable over time (Johnson & Pittenger, 1984) and there exists a general agreement on who is considered attractive (e.g., women with "babyfaceness") (Zebrowitz, Olson, &Hoffman, 1993). For such reasons, researchers have placed high importance on determining those factors that might foster the attractiveness stereotype. In his review of the literature, Feingold (1992) concluded that the attractiveness stereotype develops as the result of three causes. First, physically attractive people are over-represented in the entertainment media. The characters played by attractive actors are most often shown to have superior social skills and to be sexually uninhibited. These repeated exposures lead viewers to generalize such attributes to attractive persons. Feingold also attributes the stereotype to peoples' tendency toward the fundamental attribution error; that is, the tendency to make dispositional attributions for the situationally-determined behaviors of others. Although this bias is applied

6 Moderating 6 equally to attractive and unattractive people, Feingold contends that it maintains a role in the fostering of the attractiveness bias. The third manner in which the stereotype is fostered is through temporal extension. Feingold asserts that people tend to assume that the particular traits displayed by others in a social situation are enduring, when in fact, they are often due to the momentary situation. For reasons such as these, it is widely accepted that the attraction bias exists. Moderating Variables Recently, attention has been directed at identifying individual differences that might moderate the effects of attractiveness stereotyping. Feingold (1992) found that sex, the most frequently studied subject variable, does not moderate the effect of attractiveness on trait attribution. Male and female participants are equally likely to use attractiveness as a cue in making personality inferences about others. However, an earlier study by Dermer and Thiel (1975) did report a moderating effect by a different subject variable. Photographs of extremely unattractive, extremely attractive, and moderately attractive women were presented to subjects who themselves had previously been rated as either unattractive, attractive or of average attractiveness. The subjects were then required to rate the photographs using Dion et al.'s (1972) Social Desirability Index consisting of 27 personality traits. The results indicated that the subject's own level of attractiveness affected their use of the stereotype; attractive subjects were more likely to display the attractiveness stereotype (i.e., use

7 Moderating 7 attractiveness as an evaluative cue) than unattractive subjects. A 1987 study by Dion and Dion suggested that personality traits may also serve as moderators of the attractiveness bias. Dion and Dion sought to determine whether an individual's "belief in a just world" might moderate the stereotype. The developer of this construct, Lerner (1990, as cited in Dion & Dion, 1987), defined it as a person's "naive faith of a close connection between one's character and one's fate"; in other words, the belief that, "people get what they deserve and deserve what they get". Participants were asked to complete the Just World Scale and to rate the personality traits of either attractive or unattractive male and female targets on a social desirability scale similar to that employed by Dion et al. (1972). The results indicated that participants who believed in a just world perceived the personalities of attractive male targets as more socially desirable than unattractive male targets. In addition, believers in a just world perceived the attractive male targets as more socially desirable than did nonbelievers. Similar results were not found in the female target conditions. Dion and Dion (1987) used the theory of status characteristics and expectation states to explain the effect. They suggested that in addition to attractiveness, subjects also considered other status cues, in this case, the sex of the target, when making personality inferences. It is this aggregate of cues that affects an individual's judgements. Thus, (in our society) maleness and attractiveness elicit more positive attributions than femaleness and attractiveness. Dion and Dion concluded that, in making personality inferences, a person's belief in a just

8 Moderating 8 world moderates the effects of the attractiveness bias; that is, the degree to which an individual applies the stereotype is dependent on his or her belief in a just world. The tendency to attribute positive characteristics to attractive people and thereby perceive them to be more socially desirable than unattractive people remains insufficiently understood. According to Feingold, it seems that people form expectations about attractive people, perhaps based in part, on the portrayal of attractive individuals in the entertainment media. Unaware that these expectations influence their behavior toward attractive people (e.g., by treating them exceptionally well), observers may misattribute the behavior of attractive people to dispositional sources when, in fact, the behavior is more likely the result of situational cues; namely the differential treatment that attractive people receive from the observer. As a result of this (mis)attributional process, the attractiveness stereotype can lead to attractive persons acquiring some of the characteristics mistakenly attributed to them, a form of self-fulfilling prophecy. As suggested by Feingold (1992), if attractive people are repeatedly treated as outgoing and sociable by others, there is a possibility that they will begin to see themselves in this light and begin displaying such qualities in their daily interactions. Although intriguing, this explanation of the stereotype is incomplete since studies such as that of Dion and Dion (1987) have shown that not all people succumb to the attractiveness bias in all circumstances. These investigators

9 Moderating 9 found that a person's belief in a just world moderates the effects of the bias. In his meta-analysis Feingold (1992) suggested that future research should be directed at identifying additional subject variables that might moderate the effects of attractiveness stereotyping. One possible individual difference variable is "need for cognition." Need for Cognition "Need for cognition" refers to an individual's tendency to "engage in and enjoy effortful cognitive activities". First coined by Cacioppo and Petty (1982), this construct has been the focus of much research on individual differences in personality attributes, information acquisition, and information processing (Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein and Jarvis, 1996). Cacioppo and Petty (1982) observed that, regardless of the situation, some individuals seem to be "cognitive misers" while others appear to be "chronic cognizers." Cognitive misers were hypothesized to be individuals who possessed low intrinsic motivation to engage in effortful cognitive endeavours, whereas chronic cognizers were thought to have high intrinsic motivation for such endeavours. In the initial factor-analytic study on this individual difference variable, Cacioppo and Petty (1982) used a known-groups method to compare the responses of university faculty members and assembly-line workers on 45 statements with strong face validity of need for cognition (e.g., "I only think as hard as I have to"). Participants were asked to indicate the degree to which each item characterized them on a nine-point Likert-type scale. Cacioppo and

10 Moderating 10 Petty found that much of the variance was accounted for by a single factor, which they termed "need for cognition". This personality construct was thought to fall along a bipolar continuum. Individuals with a high need for cognition (NFC) were hypothesized as having a tendency to seek out, think about and reflect on information in order to attain an understanding of the world around them. Conversely, people with a low NFC were thought to have a tendency not to exert much effort in making sense of their surroundings, but rather to rely on more heuristically-oriented (i.e., voting for the tallest or most attractive candidate) cognitive processes to reach the same end. In a second study, Cacioppo and Petty (1982) used a more homogenous group of university undergraduates and the same results were found. In both cases, they successfully demonstrated that neither test anxiety nor response set (i.e., social desirability) affected responses on the NFC scale. A short form of the scale (Cacioppo, Petty & Kao, 1984) consisting of 18 items was found to be highly correlated with the original 34-item scale. Many independent researchers have since confirmed the reliability and validity of both scales (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 1996; Sadowski & Gulgoz, 1992b; Cacioppo, Petty & Morris, 1983). The scales psychometric properties and its usefulness as a predictive measure have stimulated an immense amount of research on the need for cognition. Many individual differences have been associated with NFC. In their review of the literature, Cacioppo et al. (1996) reported that high NFC was found to be associated with openness to ideas, introspectiveness (Berzonsky &

11 Moderating 1 1 Sullivan, 1992), private self-consciousness, and high self-esteem (Mueller & Grove, 1991). A high NFC was shown to be negatively related to neuroticism (Dornic, Ekehammar & Laaksonen, 1991), external locus of control (Fletcher, Danilovics, Fernandez, Peterson & Reeder, 1986), and social anxiety (Petty & Jarvis, 1996). In other words, high NFC persons tend to be emotionally stable, socially comfortable and have an internal locus of control. Cacioppo and Petty's (1982) initial assertion was that individuals with a high NFC tend to enjoy effortful cognitive endeavours. In studies of self-reported affective reactions to cognitive tasks, it was found that individuals high, compared to low, in NFC experienced more positive affective reactions to cognitive tasks, especially to challenging cognitive tasks (Condra, 1992). For example, Cacioppo and Petty (1982) found that when participants performed a complex number-circling task, those in the high NFC group reported more enjoyment than those in the low NFC group; however, when the number-circling task was quite simple, those in the low NFC group reported the highest enjoyment. Thompson, Chaiken and Hazlewood (1993) found that the drive to complete challenging tasks in participants with high compared to low NFC was significantly reduced when an external reward for completing the task was offered; this finding suggested that individuals who are high in NFC have an internal motivation to seek out and engage in challenging cognitive endeavours.. Presumably, satisfaction of this need results in a positive emotional reward which, in turn, reinforces the need for cognition.

12 Moderating 12 The validity of the construct of need for cognition has been supported by research examining the frequency and depth of effortful thought amongst those hypothesized to differ in this construct. For example, after being exposed to a cognitive task, such as an impression formation exercise (concerning the merits of a new product), those with a high NFC generated more task-relevant thoughts than those with a low NFC (Lassiter, Briggs & Slaw, 1991, as cited in Cacioppo et al., 1996). Relatedly, on recall of previously presented material, high NFC participants recalled a significantly greater number of items than low NFC participants (Cacioppo, Petty, and Morris, 1983). Consistent with the latter finding, individuals high in NFC have been shown to have a larger breadth of general knowledge than those low in NFC (Cacioppo et al., 1986). Furthermore, studies that measured performance on a variety of cognitive tasks found that people high in NFC tended to outperform their low NFC counterparts (Dornic et al., 1991, as cited in Cacioppo et al., 1996; Sadowski & Gulogoz, 1992a). Such studies support the assertion that individuals with a high NFC spend a great deal of time and exert much mental effort in thinking about the world around them. In addition to studying the differential amounts of information acquisition in people with varying levels of NFC, researchers have also examined how people, high and low in NFC, differ in terms of their responsiveness to argument quality. For example, Cacioppo et al. (1983) presented a set of either strong or weak arguments (for the institution of senior comprehensive exams) to participants varying in NFC who initially held the same position on the issue.

13 Moderating 13 Results indicated that the quality of the argument had a greater impact on message evaluation and source impressions of participants high, rather than low, in NFC. A later study undertaken by Axsom, Yates and Chaiken (1987) demonstrated that individuals high in NFC tended to rely specifically on the quality or logic of the argument, whereas their counterparts relied on heuristic cues. Many studies have since acknowledged this finding that people low in NFC rely on cues such as audience response, number of arguments presented, and the expertness and attractiveness of the message source (Cacioppo et al., 1996). In short, individuals with low NFC seem unwilling to put forth any more cognitive effort than is necessary, whereas individuals with high NFC seem to be driven to thoroughly evaluate available information empirically and rationally when forming judgements and making decisions. Need for Cognition and Attractiveness Stereotyping Throughout the literature on NFC, many references are made to Petty and Cacioppo's (1986) Elaboration Likelihood Model (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 1996). According to this model, there are two distinct routes to persuasion on which individuals reliably differ; these are central and peripheral routes. Those who use the central route tend to evaluate the quality of the argument to form an opinion, whereas those who use the peripheral route use heuristic cues such as previous knowledge, number of arguments and the attractiveness of the message source. These same tendencies have been reported for individuals high and low in NFC respectively (Cacioppo et al., 1996; Cacioppo & Petty,

14 Moderating ). Cacioppo et al. (1996) reported that peripheral route users tend to have weak attitudes that are highly susceptible to counter arguments and are not very predictive of behaviour. In contrast, the central route users' attitudes tend to be relatively strong, and more predictive of behaviour. Similar findings have been made concerning attitude persistence and resistance to change in individuals varying in NFC (Cacioppo & Petty, 1984; Cacioppo et al., 1986; Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992). In summary, individuals who score high, rather than low, on the NFC scale have a strong intrinsic motivation to engage in and enjoy effortful cognitive activities. In addition to having been correlated with several other personality traits, variations in the level of NFC have also been shown to moderate information acquisition and processing. Those with a high NFC tend to be constantly thinking and critically evaluating the world around them, whereas those with a low NFC tend to rely on simple rules of thumb to make sense of their world. Individuals with a high level of NFC tend to generate more thoughts, have greater knowledge bases and perform better on complex cognitive tasks than individuals with a lower level of NFC. Because people with a high NFC are more apt to expend cognitive effort, they also have a greater tendency than people with a low NFC to correct biases in their judgements (Petty & Jarvis, 1996). Such differences, coupled with the finding that those high in NFC tend to use central route processing most often, whereas those with a low NFC are more likely to use peripheral processing, suggest that this construct may serve to

15 Moderating 15 moderate the attractiveness stereotype. No research has yet examined the possibility that need for cognition represents an individual difference variable that moderates attractiveness stereotyping.

16 Moderating 16 References Adams, G. R. and Huston, T. L. (1975). Social perception of middle-aged persons varying in physical attractiveness. Developmental Psychology, 11, Axsom, D., Yates, S. M., and Chaiken, S. (1987). Audience response as a heuristic cue in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, Cacioppo, T. J., and Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., and Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: the life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E. and Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Kao, C. F., and Rodriguez, R. (1986). Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., and Morris, K. J. (1983). Effects of need for cognition on message evaluation, recall and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,

17 Moderating 17 Condra, M. (1992). The link between need for cognition and political interest, involvement and media usage. Psychology, 29, Dermer, M. and Thiel, D. L. (1975). When beauty may fail Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, Dion, K., Bersheid, E., and Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, Dion, K. L. and Dion, K. K. (1987). Belief in a just world and physical attractiveness stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, Feingold, A. (1992). Good-looking people are not what we think. Psychological Bulletin, 11, Fletcher, F. J. 0., Danilovics, P., Fernandez, G., Peterson, D., and Reeder, G. D. (1986). Attributional complexity: an individual difference measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, Haugtvedt, C. P., and Petty, R. E. (1992). Personality and persuasion: need for cognition moderates the persistence and resistence of attitude changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, Johnson, D. F. and Pittenger, J. B. (1984). Attribution, the attractiveness stereotype, and the elderly. Developmental Psychology, 20, Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., and Riesser-Danner, L. A. (1990). Infants' differential social responses to attractive and unattractive faces. Developmental Psychology, 26,

18 Moderating 18 Petty, R. E. and Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The Elaboration Likelihood Model of persuasion. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, Sadowski, C. J. and Gulgoz, S. (1992a). Association of need for cognition and course performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74, 498. Sadowski, C. J. and Gulgoz, S. (1992b). Internal consistency and test retest reliability of the Need for Cognition Scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74, 610. Thompson, E. P., Chaiken, S., and Hazlewood, J. D. (1993). Need for cognition and desire for control as moderators of extrinsic reward effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, Zebrowitz, L. A., Olson, K., and Hoffman, K. (1993). Stability of babyfaceness and attractiveness across the life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64,

19 Running head: NEED FOR COGNITION AND ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPING Moderating Moderating effects of need for cognition on attractiveness stereotyping Samantha Hansen Algoma University College

20 Moderating 2 Abstract The present study investigated the moderating effects of the need for cognition (NFC) on the attractiveness bias. Based on previous research suggesting that people low in NFC are more strongly influenced by peripheral cues (e.g., attractiveness), it was expected that such individuals in the present study would exhibit stronger attractiveness biases than individuals high in NFC. Male and female participants, high and low in NFC, rated four photographs that varied in attractiveness and sex: (a) an attractive male, (b) an attractive female, (c) an unattractive male, (d) an unattractive female. Each participant rated a single photo on 17 personality traits using a 7-point scale. The results indicated that both high and low NFC participants evidenced an attractiveness bias, however, that of the low NFC participants was found to be significantly stronger. This pattern of findings suggests that previous assumptions about the pervasiveness of the bias need to be modified to include the moderating effects of the individual difference variable, need for cognition.

21 Moderating 3 Moderating Effects of Need for Cognition on Attractiveness Stereotyping During the past 25 years, much research has been generated concerning the attractiveness stereotype; this refers to the tendency to attribute more positive characteristics to physically attractive, compared to unattractive, individuals. Pervasive and widespread, this bias toward perceiving attractive people as more socially desirable than unattractive people has a tremendous impact on interpersonal relations in all facets of life. However, individuals appear to differ on the extent to which they implement this bias. Recently, researchers have undertaken to identify those individual difference variables which might serve to moderate this bias in social perception. The present study sought to determine whether the personality construct, need for cognition (NFC), also moderates the effects of the attractiveness bias. In their landmark study appropriately labelled, "What is Beautiful is Good", Dion, Bersheid and Walster (1972) first demonstrated the existence of the attractiveness stereotype. Participants were asked to rate photographs of men and women previously determined to be either attractive, unattractive or of average attractiveness, on 27 different personality traits. They were also asked to estimate the target person's likelihood of life success and happiness. Results indicated that a strong bias in favor of attractive people existed; physically attractive persons were generally assumed to possess more socially desirable personalities than those less physically appealing. Specifically, attractive

22 Moderating 4 targets were perceived to be more poised, interesting, sociable, sexy, sensitive, kind, outgoing, and strong than unattractive targets. In addition, they were also expected to attain greater life success and happiness than their unattractive counterparts. Several subsequent studies have confirmed the existence of the attractiveness bias (e.g., Adams & Huston, 1975; Dion & Dion, 1987). The stereotype appears to be pervasive, existing across the lifespan, regardless of target age. Specifically, the bias has been identified when infants are judging adults (Langlois & Roggman, 1990), adults are judging children (Dion et al., 1972), and adults are judging other adults (Adams & Huston, 1975). In a meta-analysis of the research on attractiveness stereotyping, Feingold (1992) concluded that physically attractive male and female adults were perceived as more sociable, dominant, sexually warm, mentally healthy and socially skilled, but not as possessing greater character (e.g., honesty, genuineness, morality, etc.) than physically unattractive individuals. Feingold's (1992) meta-analysis of physical attractiveness indicated that no significant relationship existed between judges' ratings of attractiveness and target's selfratings of traits such as sociability, dominance and mental health. On the other hand, a modest correlation existed between self-ratings of attractiveness and self-ratings of personality traits related to social behavior. Those who considered themselves to be attractive tended to report less loneliness, social anxiety and self-consciousness than those who judged themselves to be

23 Moderating 5 unattractive. Being that the stereotype is so pervasive, attention has been directed at determining individual differences that might moderate the effects of the attractiveness bias. Individual Differences in Attractiveness Stereotyping To date, few studies have examined the moderating effects of individual difference variables on the attractiveness stereotype. Feingold (1992) found that sex, the most frequently studied subject variable, did not moderate the effect of attractiveness on trait attributions. Male and female participants were equally likely to use attractiveness as a cue in making personality inferences about others. Similarly, Adams & Huston (1975) reported that the age of the evaluator does not moderate the attractiveness bias. However, an earlier study by Dermer and Thiel (1975) reported that the participants' own level of attractiveness served as a moderator of the stereotype. Specifically, attractive participants were more likely to display the attractiveness stereotype (i.e., use attractiveness as an evaluative cue) than unattractive participants. In addition to sex and attractiveness, one's "belief in a just world" has also been found to moderate the effects of the attractiveness stereotype; however, this effect appears to be restricted to male targets (Dion & Dion, 1987). Dion and Dion have suggested that believers in a just world employ status cues in their social perceptions; since being male and attractive are qualities that are perceived (in our society) to be of higher status than being female and attractive, only in the male target conditions was "belief in a just world" a moderator of the attractiveness bias.

24 Moderating 6 The aforementioned findings can be summarized as follows: the use of attractiveness as a cue in making trait attributions to others has not been shown to be moderated by either the age or sex of the observer; however, a moderating effect has been shown for (a) the observer's own level of attractiveness and (b) for the observer's belief in a just world. Feingold (1992) suggested that future research should be directed at identifying additional individual difference variables that might moderate the effects of attractiveness stereotyping. One possible individual difference variable is "need for cognition." Need for Cognition First coined by Cacioppo and Petty (1982), "need for cognition" (NEC) refers to an individual's tendency to engage in and enjoy effortful cognitive activities. Cacioppo and Petty observed that, regardless of the situation, some individuals seem to be "cognitive misers" while others appear to be "chronic cognizers." In the initial factor-analytic study on this individual difference variable, Cacioppo and Petty (1982) used a known-groups method to compare the responses of university faculty members and assembly-line workers on 45 statements with strong face validity of need for cognition (e.g., "I only think as hard as I have to"). Participants were asked to indicate the degree to which each item characterized them on a nine-point Likert-type scale. Cacioppo and Petty found that much of the variance was accounted for by a single factor, which they termed "need for cognition". Numerous investigations have since elaborated on the differences

25 Moderating 7 between individuals varying in the NFC. Studies investigating information acquisition and retention have found that individuals with a higher NFC demonstrate both greater frequency and depth of cognitive processing (Cacioppo, Petty, Kao & Rodriguez, 1986; Cacioppo, Petty & Morris, 1983; Sadowski & Gulogoz, 1992a). In addition to having a greater tendency to seek out, think about and reflect on information than those with a lower NFC, individuals with a higher NFC have been found to experience more positive affective reactions to challenging cognitive tasks (Cacioppo & Petty, 1992; Condra, 1992; Thompson, Chaiken & Hazlewood, 1993). Hence, Cacioppo and Petty's supposition that individuals varying in the NFC differ in the degree to which they enjoy and engage in effortful cognitive processing has been confirmed in various studies. Researchers have also examined how people, high and low in the NFC, differ in terms of their responsiveness to argument quality and their persistence and resistance to attitude change. Cumulative evidence seems to suggests that individuals with a high NFC rely specifically on quality and logic, whereas those with a low NFC rely only on heuristic cues (Le., audience response, number of arguments presented, expertness and attractiveness of message source) when evaluating an argument (Axsom, Yates & Chaiken, 1987; Cacioppo et al., 1996; Cacioppo et al., 1983). These varied sources of information have been labelled "central" and "peripheral" cues within the context of the Elaboration Likelihood Model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). Also consistent with this model, the attitudes

26 Moderating 8 of individuals with a high NFC tend to be relatively strong and highly predictive of behavior; in contrast, the attitudes of those with a low NFC tend to be highly susceptible to counter arguments and are not highly predictive of behavior (Cacioppo et al., 1986; Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992). Furthermore, because individuals with high levels of NFC are more likely to expend cognitive effort, they also have a greater tendency to correct biases in their judgements, compared to those with low NFC levels (Cacioppo et al., 1996). In light of the evidence favoring the use of heuristic cues by those with low NFC, it is reasonable to suggest that such persons would have a strong tendency to judge target persons based on attractiveness cues. To date, no research has examined any moderating effects of need for cognition on the implementation of the attractiveness bias. Present Study In the present study it was hypothesized that need for cognition would moderate the effects of the attractiveness stereotype. Specifically, it was predicted that those with a low need for cognition would attribute more socially desirable characteristics to attractive, compared to unattractive, targets than those with a high need for cognition. Such an effect was expected since attractiveness stereotyping entails an evaluation based solely on the level of attractiveness; individuals low in NFC have a tendency to rely on such peripheral (i.e., heuristic) cues, whereas those with a high NFC are more likely to use central processing, a route that does not contain attractiveness cues as

27 Moderating 9 the basis of judgements. Method Participants One hundred and sixty students, recruited from Algoma University College and Sault College, participated in this investigation. The sample consisted of 80 men (age 19-45) and 80 women (age18-56). In each of these groups there were equal numbers of high and low NFC participants. Those students who were recruited from a classroom received course credit for their participation. Materials The Need for Cognition Scale (Cacioppo et al., 1984) consisted of 18 statements for which the participants were asked to indicate the extent to which each item was characteristic of them. The possible responses were presented in a 5-point Likert-type format ranging from "extremely uncharacteristic" to "extremely characteristic", with "uncertain" as a neutral midpoint. The responses were scored with values ranging from 1 to 5 and keyed so that higher scores indicated a higher NFC. As reported by Cacioppo et al. (1996), the majority of factor analytic studies on the Need for Cognition Scale (NCS) have found that a single factor parsimoniously accounts for the variability in the responses. The responses to

28 Moderating 10 the 18-item NCS are internally consistent (Cronbach's alpha =.90) and reliable at retest (r =.88; Sadowski & Gulgoz, 1992b). The reliability of the scale was found to be similar for men and women (e.g., Cronbach alpha =.86 for men and women; Sadowski, 1993). Such findings coupled with those that NFC is not significantly related to response biases such as test anxiety (r =.02, ns; Cacioppo & Petty, 1992) or social desirability (r =.14, ns; Cacioppo & Petty, 1982), suggest that the NFC is both a valid and reliable construct. The Just World Scale (Rubin & Peplau, 1975) consisted of twenty statements for which participants were asked to indicate their degree of agreement or disagreement using a 5-point Likert-type response format. The possible responses ranged from "strongly agree" to "strongly disagree", with "neither agree nor disagree" serving as the neutral midpoint. Nine of the items were reverse scored. Higher scores indicated higher Just World Beliefs. A brief questionnaire was administered which asked participants to provide their first name and telephone number in order to schedule a future meeting. In addition, they were asked to indicate their sex, age and own level of attractiveness (rated on a 7-point scale). The stimulus materials included a photograph of an attractive male, an unattractive male, an attractive female, and an unattractive female, as well as a social desirability index comprised of the sum of the 17 trait ratings. Initially, 10 photographs of attractive women and 10 photographs of attractive men were selected from a number of specialty products and magazines. Care was taken in

29 Moderating 11 only choosing faces that did not appear familiar. After being digitally scanned, the photographs were standardized for both pose and background. All photos were cropped at the shoulder to minimize clothing cues. Each set (male and female) was presented in a random sequence to an independent panel of judges (male and female undergraduates ranging from 18 to 50 years of age). The panel members were asked to rate the level of physical attractiveness of each target person on a 7-point scale. Panel members were also asked whether they recognized the target person. None of the targets were reported as familiar. The photograph rated highest in each set was chosen for use in the experiment. Similar to the process used by Perlini, Bertolissi and Lind (in press), the attractive female and attractive male target photographs were digitally modified using Corel Photopaint and Kai's Power Goo to create similar, but unattractive, targets. This was achieved by thinning the lips, slightly increasing the distance between the eyes, pinching the eyes to appear smaller and enlarging the tip of the nose. All other facial features were held constant (e.g., hair length, style and color, skin texture, jaw line etc.). As a manipulation check, the unattractive versions of the male and female targets were then embedded in a random sequence of photographs and presented to a panel of new judges. These individuals did in fact perceive the photos as being unattractive. The Social Desirability Index was comprised of the same 17 bipolar traits used in Dion et al.'s (1972) study. As reported by Dion et al., the internal consistency of these trait ratings is quite high (coefficient alpha =.903). Each

30 Moderating 12 bipolar trait was rated on a 7-point scale, with higher ratings indicating greater social desirability. The traits were as follows: poised-awkward, strong-weak, interesting-boring, assertive-submissive, sociable-unsociable, independentdependent, warm-cold, desirable as a friend-undesirable as a friend, genuineartificial, kind-cruel, exciting-dull, sophisticated-unsophisticated, sexually warmsexually cold, sincere-insincere, sensitive-insensitive, well mannered-ill mannered, polite-impolite. The bipolar, 7-point rating of attractive-unattractive was embedded amongst the trait ratings and served as a check on the attractiveness manipulation. The positive pole was on the right for half of the items and on the left for the other half in order to counter acquiescence response set. Procedure The prospective participants were approached and asked if they would consent to participate in a study on social perception. It was explained that they would first complete two scales and a brief questionnaire and that during a second phase, which would be later scheduled, they would be asked to make a number of responses to a target photograph. A package containing the untitled Need for Cognition and Just World Scales and a brief questionnaire was then distributed to the consenting participants. As recommended by Cacioppo et al. (1996), the participants were instructed that there were no correct answers, their honesty in responding to the questions was important, and their responses were

31 Moderating 13 confidential. The scores on the NFC scale were normally distributed (M = 64, SD = 11) with a median of 64, which is consistent with the findings of Haugtvedt and Petty (1992). A median split was performed to determine which individuals would be considered high and low in the NFC. The participants were matched on NFC and sex (i.e., high NFC males, high NFC females, low NFC males and low NFC females). Collapsed across NFC, the means remained consistent between men and women (M = 64.10, M = 63.44, respectively). Participants in each of the four groups were then randomly assigned to one of four target conditions (i.e., attractive male photo, attractive female photo, unattractive male photo, unattractive female photo). As is illustrated in Table 1, the design of the study was a 2 (Sex of Participant: male/female) X 2 (Sex of Target: male/female) X 2 (Need for Cognition: high/low) X 2 (Attractiveness of Target: attractive/unattractive) between groups design. Insert Table 1 about here The second phase was administered in small groups. Upon arrival, each participant was seated in a cubicle and asked to refrain from commenting on the stimulus materials. They were given a package containing a single photograph and the Social Desirability Index and instructed to rate the target person on each

32 Moderating 14 bipolar trait using the 7-point scale provided. Upon completion, the participants were given a detailed written debriefing wherein the true purpose of the study was explained. Hypotheses. Based on research concerning need for cognition and attractiveness stereotyping, a main effect was only predicted for attractiveness of target. More germane to this study were the predicted interactions. It was anticipated that the main effect for attractiveness would be qualified by a higher order interaction; that is, NFC was expected to moderate the tendency to attribute more positive traits to attractive, compared to unattractive, targets. More specifically, the attractiveness bias amongst the low NFC participants was predicted to be significantly larger than that of the high NFC participants. Relatedly, within the attractive target conditions, the low NFC participants were predicted to ascribe more socially desirable attributes to the physically attractive targets than were the high NFC participants. In contrast, amongst physically unattractive targets the difference in attributions of social desirability between the high and low NFC participants was predicted to be considerably smaller since the target lacked the heuristic cue (namely, attractiveness) that the low NFC subjects would ordinarily rely upon. Based on Feingold's (1992) comprehensive review of the literature, the sex of the participant was not expected to have an interaction with target attractiveness. The Cacioppo and Petty (1996) review strongly demonstrates

33 Moderating 15 that there are no sex differences in NFC; thus, NFC was not predicted to interact with sex of judge. However, target sex has been found to interact with target attractiveness and the individual difference variable, just world belief (Dion & Dion, 1987). As discussed earlier, belief in a just world moderated the attractiveness bias for male targets only. This finding was interpreted by Dion and Dion as suggesting that both sex and physical attractiveness are status cues that believers in a just world are sensitive to. It was unclear whether participants who differ in NFC would be similarly sensitive to sex of target when exhibiting their attractiveness bias. Consequently, no specific predictions were made about the interaction amongst NFC, target attractiveness and target sex. Results The attractiveness manipulation check and the dependent measures were analyzed in a 2 X 2 X 2 X 2 factorial ANOVA design with Participant NFC (High/Low), Participant Sex (Male/Female), Photo Attractiveness (Attractive/Unattractive), and Photo Sex (Male/Female) as independent variables. Check on the Attractiveness Manipulation Analysis of the attractiveness variable yielded a highly significant main effect for Photo Attractiveness, F (1,144) = , p <.001. As was expected, the attractive photos were in fact perceived by the observers to be more

34 Moderating 16 physically attractive than the unattractive photos (Ms = 6.09 vs. 4.20, respectively). Analysis also indicated main effects for Participant Sex [F (1,144) = 11.80, Q. <.001] and Photo Sex [F (1,144) = 8.12, g <.005]; however, each of these main effects were qualified by higher-order interactions. The main effect for Photo Sex was qualified by a two-way interaction; namely, Participant NFC X Photo Sex, F (1,144) = 3.34, a <.07. This interaction was further qualified by a higher-order 3-way interaction of Participant Sex X Participant NFC X Photo Sex, F (1,144) = 4.49, <.03. Further analyses were performed on this interaction. Simple main effect analyses performed on each level of the NFC factor indicated that for high NFC participants, there was a main effect for Participant Sex, [F (1,76) = 7.22, a <.009] and a main effect for Photo Sex, [F (1,76) = 6.40, a <.01]. Females rated the target photos as more attractive (M = 5.48, SD = 1.52 ) than the males rated the same photos (M = 4.63, SD = 1.41). In addition, the female photos were rated higher in attractiveness (M = 5.45, SD = 1.45) than the male photos OA = 4.65, SD = 1.49). For low NFC participants, there were no main effects nor an interaction. In short, it appeared that Participant Sex and Photo Sex only effected the attractiveness ratings of high NFC participants.

35 Moderating 17 Social Desirability Index Analysis of the social desirability index was computed using a 2 (Participant Sex: Male/Female) X 2 (Participant NFC: High/Low) X 2 (Photo Attractiveness: Attractive/Unattractive) X 2 (Photo Sex: Male/Female) ANOVA. Main effects were found for: (1) Participant Sex, F (1,144) = 15.50, p <.001, [Ms = vs , for men and women respectively], (2) Participant NFC, F (1,144) = 11.13, p <.001, (3) Photo Sex, F (1,144) = 4.56, p <.03 and (4) Photo Attractiveness, F (1,144) = 40.41, p <.001. However, main effects of Participant NFC, Photo Sex and Target Attractiveness were qualified by the following higher-order interactions: (1) Participant NFC X Photo Sex, F (1,144) = 3.22, p <.075 and (2) Participant NFC X Photo Attractiveness, F (1,144) = 6.46, p <.01. Simple main effect analyses were performed on each of these interactions as follows. Participant NFC X Photo Sex. Simple main effect analyses conducted at both levels of the Participant NFC factor indicated that high NFC judges rated male and female photos as similar in social desirability (Ms = vs ); however, low NFC judges rated male targets as more socially desirable that female targets, F (1,78) = 4.64, p <.03; Ms = vs , respectively. Hence, only amongst the low NFC participants was sex of photo used as a cue in judging the social desirability of targets. When Photo Sex was held constant, and the effects of NFC on social desirability ratings was analyzed, the results indicated that for female targets

36 Moderating 18 there was no difference in attributions of social desirability of low NFCs compared to high NFCs, (Ms = vs , respectively), F <1. However, within the male photo conditions, it was found that individuals low in NFC rated the targets as more socially desirable than did individuals high in NFC, F (1,78) = 9.54, p <.003, (Ms = vs , respectively). The means and standard deviations underlying these effects are presented in Table 2. Insert Table 2 about here Participant NFC X Photo Attractiveness. Simple effect analyses at each level of the Participant NFC factor indicated that, as expected, both low [F (1,78) = 31.55, p <.001] and high NFC [F (1,78) = 7.40, j <.008] participants evidenced an attractiveness bias, that is, both ascribed more positive traits to the attractive, as compared to the unattractive, photos. The means and standard deviations underlying these effects are presented in Table 3. Insert Table 3 about here Simple main effect analyses at each level of the Attractiveness factor indicated that high and low NFC participants rated the photos of the unattractive target as equivalent in social desirability (Ms = vs ), F (1,78) =.31, p >.10; however, amongst attractive target photos, low NFC participants rated

37 Moderating 19 such photos as significantly higher in social desirability than high NFC participants rated such photos (Ms = vs ), F (1,78) = 14.38, <.001. This latter finding confirms the hypothesis that low NFC participants construe attractiveness as a stronger cue in their attributions of social desirability than high NFC participants. Correlations. In order to identify those variables predictive of social desirability ascriptions, Pearson Product Moment correlations amongst 5 variables were conducted; (1) Participant NFC, (2) Participant Just World Belief, (3) Selfratings of Attractiveness, (4) Target Ratings of Attractiveness, (5) Target Ratings of Social Desirability. In a preliminary analysis, the data for all participants was analyzed. Given the sample size (N=160), correlations exceeding r = were deemed statistically significant in this analysis. Refer to Table 4 for these correlations. Insert Table 4 about here The only significant predictor of social desirability ratings was Target Ratings of Attractiveness, r = Given that NFC predicted both Target Ratings of Attractiveness and Target Ratings of Social Desirability in the earlier ANOVAs, the aforementioned correlation was further examined for participants

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution 1 Robert D. Mather University of Central Oklahoma Charles M. Mather University of Science

More information

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies Emma ter Mors 1,2, Mieneke Weenig 1, Naomi Ellemers 1, Dancker Daamen 1 1 Leiden University,

More information

33 Multiple choice questions

33 Multiple choice questions NAME 33 Multiple choice questions 1. central route a. Aspect of persuasion, emotional appeals are quite powerful. Negative appeals play to the audience's b. Aspect of persuasion, whether or not someone

More information

Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives. Chapter 3 Outline -Attitude Change

Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives. Chapter 3 Outline -Attitude Change Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives 1) An understanding of how both internal mental processes and external influences lead to attitude change 2) An understanding of when and how behavior which is inconsistent

More information

What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract

What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract The present research examined whether physically attractive Facebook

More information

The Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Perceptions of Mental Illness

The Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Perceptions of Mental Illness Proceedings of The National Conference On Undergraduate Research (NCUR) 204 University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY April 3-5, 204 The Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Perceptions of Mental Illness

More information

PSYC2600 Lecture One Attitudes

PSYC2600 Lecture One Attitudes PSYC2600 Lecture One Attitudes 16/07/14 7:01 AM Motivation (Lei, 2010) Intrinsic motivation have an internal motivation, you are interested. Extrinsic motivation have no specific interest, but are motivated

More information

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration Dr. Chan-Chien Chiu, Associate Professor of Taiwan Hospitality & Tourism College, Taiwan ABSTRACT This

More information

Physical Appearance and Trait Judgements. By Alex Stevens, Hope Childree, Laura Perry, Amy Sapp, and Emily Lunsford

Physical Appearance and Trait Judgements. By Alex Stevens, Hope Childree, Laura Perry, Amy Sapp, and Emily Lunsford Physical Appearance and Trait Judgements By Alex Stevens, Hope Childree, Laura Perry, Amy Sapp, and Emily Lunsford Previous Research The idea that physical appearance is associated with the attribution

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz MULTIPLE CHOICE: 1. We usually adapt our behavior to the demands of the social situation, and in ambiguous situations: A) We take our cues from the behavior of others. B) We will do the same thing that

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

Need for Cognition: Does It Influence Professional Judgment?

Need for Cognition: Does It Influence Professional Judgment? Review of Integrative Business and Economics Research, Vol. 6, no. 1, pp.240-248, January 2017 240 Need for Cognition: Does It Influence Professional Judgment? Wimpie Yustino Setiawan Universitas Atma

More information

Chapter 13. Social Psychology

Chapter 13. Social Psychology Social Psychology Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? Social Psychology Interpreting the Behavior of Others Behaving in the Presence of Others Establishing Relations With Others Social

More information

individual differences strong situation interactional psychology locus of control personality general self-efficacy trait theory self-esteem

individual differences strong situation interactional psychology locus of control personality general self-efficacy trait theory self-esteem individual differences strong situation interactional psychology locus of control personality general self-efficacy trait theory self-esteem integrative approach self-monitoring A situation that overwhelms

More information

Matching Versus Mismatching Attitude Functions: Implications for Scrutiny of Persuasive Messages

Matching Versus Mismatching Attitude Functions: Implications for Scrutiny of Persuasive Messages Matching Versus Mismatching Attitude Functions: Implications for Scrutiny of Persuasive Messages Richard E. Petty and Duane T. Wegener Abstract: Two studies were conducted to examine the hypothesis that

More information

PSYC1001 NOTES. Science and Statistics

PSYC1001 NOTES. Science and Statistics PSYC1001 NOTES TOPICS PAGES Science and Statistics ---------------------------------------------------------------- 2-11 Personality Psychology -------------------------------------------------------------

More information

Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1

Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1 Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1 The Effect of High Attractiveness on Perceived Intelligence Katlyn Hogue, Cara Mulhall, and Kara Stewart Hanover College PSY 344:

More information

Methodology Introduction of the study Statement of Problem Objective Hypothesis Method

Methodology Introduction of the study Statement of Problem Objective Hypothesis Method 3.1. Introduction of the study 3.2. Statement of Problem 3.3. Objective 3.4. Hypothesis 3.5. Method 3.5.1. Procedure Sample A.5.2. Variable A.5.3. Research Design A.5.4. Operational Definition Of The Terms

More information

Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE)

Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE) Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE) Eachus, P and Cassidy, SF Title Authors Type URL Published Date 2004 Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE) Eachus, P and Cassidy,

More information

RUNNING HEAD: RAPID EVALUATION OF BIG FIVE 1

RUNNING HEAD: RAPID EVALUATION OF BIG FIVE 1 RUNNING HEAD: RAPID EVALUATION OF BIG FIVE 1 The Accuracy of Rapid Evaluation of Big Five Personality Traits Melissa Poole, Nick Bliznoff, and Jackie Martin Hanover College RAPID EVALUATION OF BIG FIVE

More information

Craft Personality Questionnaire

Craft Personality Questionnaire Craft Personality Questionnaire Evidence of Reliability and Validity 888-298-6227 TalentLens.com Copyright 2008 Pearson Education, Inc., or its affiliates. All rights reserved. Copyright 2008 by Pearson

More information

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM participant client HFM 03/13/201 This report was generated by the HFMtalentindex Online Assessment system. The data in this report are based on the answers given by the participant on one or more psychological

More information

Personality, Perception, & Attribution

Personality, Perception, & Attribution Personality, Perception, & Attribution Variables Influencing Individual Behavior Big Five Personality Traits Personality Characteristics in Organizations Locus of Control Internal I control what happens

More information

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information on Consumer Elaboration and Brand Attitude

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information on Consumer Elaboration and Brand Attitude The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information on Consumer Elaboration and Brand Attitude Chan-Chien Chiu, Associate Professor, Department of Food and Beverage Management, Taiwan Hospitality & Tourism

More information

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory The Myers Briggs Type Inventory Charles C. Healy Professor of Education, UCLA In press with Kapes, J.T. et. al. (2001) A counselor s guide to Career Assessment Instruments. (4th Ed.) Alexandria, VA: National

More information

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE CHAPTER FIFTEEN Motivation INTRODUCTION Many of us have unrealized abilities. Some of us could run marathons, others could write novels, and still others could get straight A s in management classes. But

More information

Stability and Change of Adolescent. Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study. Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill. Division of Psychology

Stability and Change of Adolescent. Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study. Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill. Division of Psychology Stability and Change of Adolescent Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill Division of Psychology The Australian National University Paper presented to the

More information

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials for Wisneski and Skitka Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction Table of Contents 2 Pilot Studies 2 High Awareness

More information

Discovering Diversity Profile Group Report

Discovering Diversity Profile Group Report Discovering Diversity Profile Group Report Sample Report (5 People) Friday, June 18, 2010 This report is provided by: Intesi! Resources 14230 N. 20th Way Phoenix, AZ 85022 Phone: 602-482-6422 Toll Free:

More information

Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality. Copyright Allyn & Bacon (2009)

Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality. Copyright Allyn & Bacon (2009) Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality Roots in Gestalt Psychology Human beings seek meaning in their environments We organize the sensations we receive into meaningful perceptions Complex stimuli

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

Trait Perceptions of Dynamic and Static Faces as a Function of Facial. Maturity and Facial Expression

Trait Perceptions of Dynamic and Static Faces as a Function of Facial. Maturity and Facial Expression Trait Perceptions of Dynamic and Static Faces as a Function of Facial Maturity and Facial Expression Master s Thesis Presented to The Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences Brandeis University

More information

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not?

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not? The Effects of Voice Pitch on Attractiveness 1 The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not? Lead Author Katie Leaderbrand Co-Researchers Josh Dekam, and Ashley

More information

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students Mia Bonner Millersville University Abstract Previous research (Elion, Slaney, Wang and French, 2012) found

More information

Attribution of Traits to Self and Others: Situationality vs. Uncertainty

Attribution of Traits to Self and Others: Situationality vs. Uncertainty Attribution of Traits to Self and Others: Situationality vs. Uncertainty JOOP VAN DER PLIGT and J. RICHARD EISER University of Exeter Although there is considerable support for the hypothesis that people

More information

Survey Research. We can learn a lot simply by asking people what we want to know... THE PREVALENCE OF SURVEYS IN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH

Survey Research. We can learn a lot simply by asking people what we want to know... THE PREVALENCE OF SURVEYS IN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH Survey Research From surveys we can learn how large groups of people think and act. To trust generalizations made on the basis of surveys, however, the sample must be representative, the response rate

More information

Non-smokers rights movement begins Per capita cigarette consumption First medical reports linking smoking and cancer.

Non-smokers rights movement begins Per capita cigarette consumption First medical reports linking smoking and cancer. 5 Non-smokers rights movement begins Per capita cigarette consumption 4 3 2 1 First medical reports linking smoking and cancer End of WW II Great Depression 19 191 192 193 194 195 Years US Surgeon General

More information

The notion that people sometimes base judgments on. Ease of Retrieval Effects in Persuasion: A Self-Validation Analysis

The notion that people sometimes base judgments on. Ease of Retrieval Effects in Persuasion: A Self-Validation Analysis 10.1177/014616702237651 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN Tormala et al. / EASE OF RETRIEVAL IN PERSUASION Ease of Retrieval Effects in Persuasion: A Self-Validation Analysis Zakary L. Tormala

More information

Reactions of College Students to the Sexuality of Older People

Reactions of College Students to the Sexuality of Older People Reactions of College Students to the Sexuality of Older People Emily Waterman a The reactions of 153 college students to incidents of sexuality were measured using a series of scenarios. Age of target

More information

Belief in a Just World and Physical Attractiveness Stereotyping

Belief in a Just World and Physical Attractiveness Stereotyping Journal of Pm'~Mlity and Social Psychology Copyright 1987 by the American l~ychological A.~u~ciafion, Inc. 1987, Vol. 52, No. 4, 775-780 0022-3514/87/$00.75 Belief in a Just World and Physical Attractiveness

More information

Lesson 12. Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior

Lesson 12. Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior Lesson 12 Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior Learning Objectives 1. Identify the focus and goals of individual behavior within organizations. 2. Explain the role that attitudes play in job

More information

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect Abstract This article reports the effects of hedonic versus utilitarian consumption goals on consumers choices between

More information

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Copyright 2015 McGraw-Hill Education. All rights reserved. No reproduction or distribution without the prior written consent of McGraw-Hill Education. Learning

More information

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1983, Vol. 51, No. 5, 721-725 Copyright 1983 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation

More information

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment Marshall University Marshall Digital Scholar Management Faculty Research Management, Marketing and MIS Fall 11-14-2009 Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment Wai Kwan

More information

Sample Report. Sample Report Report. Fa c i l i tat or s (05/13) 180

Sample Report. Sample Report Report. Fa c i l i tat or s (05/13) 180 Sample Report Report Sample Report Fa c i l i tat or s R E P O R T F A C I L I T A T O R S R E P O RT (05/13) 180 PREPARE/ENRICH Customized Version Facilitator: David H. Olson Date Completed: 09/26/11

More information

Assignment 4: True or Quasi-Experiment

Assignment 4: True or Quasi-Experiment Assignment 4: True or Quasi-Experiment Objectives: After completing this assignment, you will be able to Evaluate when you must use an experiment to answer a research question Develop statistical hypotheses

More information

The Criminal Face Effect: Physical Attractiveness and Character Integrity as Determinants of Perceived Criminality

The Criminal Face Effect: Physical Attractiveness and Character Integrity as Determinants of Perceived Criminality The Criminal Face Effect: Physical Attractiveness and Character Integrity as Determinants of Perceived Criminality by, Department of Psychology, University of Warwick ABSTRACT A study was conducted which

More information

MEN AND BODY DISSATISFACTION 1

MEN AND BODY DISSATISFACTION 1 MEN AND BODY DISSATISFACTION 1 Men and their Body Dissatisfaction: The Effects of Media Grooming Madison Chaney, Claudia Della Polla, Kirstie Gamboa, Alexa Rosas Texas Tech University MEN AND BODY DISSATISFACTION

More information

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, 785-789. Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 EXAMINATION OF THE PASSIVE FACIAL FEEDBACK HYPOTHESIS USING AN IMPLICIT MEASURE: WITH A FURROWED BROW, NEUTRAL OBJECTS

More information

IDEA Technical Report No. 20. Updated Technical Manual for the IDEA Feedback System for Administrators. Stephen L. Benton Dan Li

IDEA Technical Report No. 20. Updated Technical Manual for the IDEA Feedback System for Administrators. Stephen L. Benton Dan Li IDEA Technical Report No. 20 Updated Technical Manual for the IDEA Feedback System for Administrators Stephen L. Benton Dan Li July 2018 2 Table of Contents Introduction... 5 Sample Description... 6 Response

More information

SOCI 323 Social Psychology

SOCI 323 Social Psychology SOCI 323 Session 9 SOCIAL PERCEPTION Lecturer: Dr. Peace Mamle Tetteh, Department of Sociology Contact Information: ptetteh@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education 2014/2015

More information

Attitude Measurement

Attitude Measurement Business Research Methods 9e Zikmund Babin Carr Griffin Attitude Measurement 14 Chapter 14 Attitude Measurement 2013 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be scanned, copied or duplicated, or

More information

Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale

Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale Overview In an informal poll of 150 educated research professionals attending the 2009 Sawtooth Conference, 100% of those who voted raised their hands

More information

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu Person Perception Forming Impressions of Others Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu Person Perception person perception: how we come to know about others temporary states, emotions, intentions and desires impression

More information

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2014, 42(7), 1105-1116 Society for Personality Research http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2014.42.7.1105 ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN

More information

Do Clothing Style and Color Affect Our Perceptions of Others?

Do Clothing Style and Color Affect Our Perceptions of Others? Arcadia University ScholarWorks@Arcadia Faculty Curated Undergraduate Works Undergraduate Research Winter 1-15-2018 Do Clothing Style and Color Affect Our Perceptions of Others? Ariel M. Kershner Arcadia

More information

AU TQF 2 Doctoral Degree. Course Description

AU TQF 2 Doctoral Degree. Course Description Course Description 1. Foundation Courses CP 5000 General Psychology Non-credit Basic psychological concepts and to introduce students to the scientific study of behavior. Learning and Behavior, Altered

More information

LOCUS OF CONTROL AND REACTION TO ILLNESS: A STUDY OF PATIENTS WITH CHRONIC RENAL FAILURE

LOCUS OF CONTROL AND REACTION TO ILLNESS: A STUDY OF PATIENTS WITH CHRONIC RENAL FAILURE Med. J. Malaysia VoI. 39 No. 4 December 1984 LOCUS OF CONTROL AND REACTION TO ILLNESS: A STUDY OF PATIENTS WITH CHRONIC RENAL FAILURE BOEY KAM WENG SUMMARY The relationship between locus of control and

More information

Likert Scaling: A how to do it guide As quoted from

Likert Scaling: A how to do it guide As quoted from Likert Scaling: A how to do it guide As quoted from www.drweedman.com/likert.doc Likert scaling is a process which relies heavily on computer processing of results and as a consequence is my favorite method

More information

Computers in Human Behavior

Computers in Human Behavior Computers in Human Behavior 24 (2008) 2965 2971 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Computers in Human Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh Social facilitation and human

More information

Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis'

Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis' Japanese Psychological Rescarch 1994, Vol.36, No. 1. 41-48 Special lssue: Decision Making Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis' KAZUHISA

More information

Exploring the associations between Social Media Addiction, Media Pressure, Self-esteem and Fear of Negative Appearance Evaluation among young adults.

Exploring the associations between Social Media Addiction, Media Pressure, Self-esteem and Fear of Negative Appearance Evaluation among young adults. Exploring the associations between Social Media Addiction, Media Pressure, and Fear of Negative Appearance Evaluation among Harshitha.S.* & Ayesha Arif Zinna** *Research Scholar, Department of Applied

More information

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES (2018-19) PSYCHOLOGY Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology OUTLINE OF THE CHAPTER (i) Scientific Methods in Psychology -observation, case study, surveys, psychological tests, experimentation

More information

Can Leaders Step Outside of the Gender Box? An Examination of Leadership and Gender Role Stereotypes

Can Leaders Step Outside of the Gender Box? An Examination of Leadership and Gender Role Stereotypes Butler University Digital Commons @ Butler University Scholarship and Professional Work - Business Lacy School of Business 2008 Can Leaders Step Outside of the Gender Box? An Examination of Leadership

More information

MHR 405-Chapter 2. Motivation: The forces within a person that affect his or her direction, intensity and persistence of voluntary behaviour

MHR 405-Chapter 2. Motivation: The forces within a person that affect his or her direction, intensity and persistence of voluntary behaviour MHR 405-Chapter 2 Motivation: The forces within a person that affect his or her direction, intensity and persistence of voluntary behaviour Figure 2.1 Ability: The natural aptitudes and learned capabilities

More information

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE Previous chapter namely Review of the Literature was concerned with the review of the research studies conducted in the field of teacher education, with special reference

More information

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1060 1066 Brief Report Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies

More information

Discovering Diversity Profile Individual Report

Discovering Diversity Profile Individual Report Individual Report Respondent Name Monday, November 13, 2006 This report is provided by: Integro Leadership Institute 1380 Wilmington Pike, Suite 113B West Chester PA 19382 Toll Free (866) 468-3476 Phone

More information

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT Study Focus Empathy s impact on: O Human Interactions O Consequences of those interactions O Empathy Development Study Purpose to examine the empathetic

More information

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION 92 THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION 1. Motivation is a highly dynamic construct that is constantly changing in reaction to life experiences. 2. Needs and goals are constantly growing and changing. 3. As individuals

More information

THE INTEGRITY PROFILING SYSTEM

THE INTEGRITY PROFILING SYSTEM THE INTEGRITY PROFILING SYSTEM The Integrity Profiling System which Soft Skills has produced is the result of a world first research into leadership and integrity in Australia. Where we established what

More information

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH The following document provides background information on the research and development of the Emergenetics Profile instrument. Emergenetics Defined 1. Emergenetics

More information

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing? Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC How are we doing? Prepared by Maura McCool, M.S. Office of Research, Evaluation and Assessment Metropolitan Community Colleges Fall 2003 1 General Education Assessment

More information

Multiple Act criterion:

Multiple Act criterion: Common Features of Trait Theories Generality and Stability of Traits: Trait theorists all use consistencies in an individual s behavior and explain why persons respond in different ways to the same stimulus

More information

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81.

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81. MSP RESEARCH NOTE B5PQ Reliability and Validity This research note describes the reliability and validity of the B5PQ. Evidence for the reliability and validity of is presented against some of the key

More information

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology Preface p. xv Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology p. 3 Sociology, Psychology, or Both? p.

More information

Multiple Levels of Motivation for News Viewing: Theory of Planned Behavior versus. Elaboration Likelihood Model Predictions. Rebecca M. L.

Multiple Levels of Motivation for News Viewing: Theory of Planned Behavior versus. Elaboration Likelihood Model Predictions. Rebecca M. L. Levels of Motivation 1 Running Head: LEVELS OF MOTIVATION Multiple Levels of Motivation for News Viewing: Theory of Planned Behavior versus Elaboration Likelihood Model Predictions Rebecca M. L. Curnalia

More information

Results and Discussion Section

Results and Discussion Section Results and Discussion Section Your second paper consists of a title page, results section and discussion section, references, and table and/or figure that will focus on the results of data collected in

More information

CHAPTER 2 FOUNDATIONS OF INDIVIDUAL BEHAVIOR

CHAPTER 2 FOUNDATIONS OF INDIVIDUAL BEHAVIOR CHAPTER 2 FOUNDATIONS OF INDIVIDUAL BEHAVIOR "Intelligence is but one characteristic that people bring with them when they join an organization. In this chapter, we look at how biographical characteristics

More information

Running Head: NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 1

Running Head: NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 1 Running Head: NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 1 Coherence and Fidelity in Fisher s Narrative Paradigm Amy Kuhlman Wheaton College 13 November 2013 NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 2 Abstract This paper

More information

Factors that affect interpersonal attraction:

Factors that affect interpersonal attraction: Introduction: You are a member of a social world on a planet containing about 7 billion people. Our behaviors differ in different settings (like if we are in the college, market, or at home). Human behavior

More information

CHAPTER 11: Personality, attitudes and motivation

CHAPTER 11: Personality, attitudes and motivation QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS CHAPTER 11: Personality, attitudes and motivation Practice questions - text book page 147-148 1) Which one of the following is not included in the definition of personality psychology?

More information

Interpersonal Compatibility and S ocial Loafing: An Introduction to a S tudy

Interpersonal Compatibility and S ocial Loafing: An Introduction to a S tudy Interpersonal Compatibility and S ocial Loafing: An Introduction to a S tudy Social loafing has been defined as the tendency of individuals to exert less effort when working collectively than coactively

More information

Development. summary. Sam Sample. Emotional Intelligence Profile. Wednesday 5 April 2017 General Working Population (sample size 1634) Sam Sample

Development. summary. Sam Sample. Emotional Intelligence Profile. Wednesday 5 April 2017 General Working Population (sample size 1634) Sam Sample Development summary Wednesday 5 April 2017 General Working Population (sample size 1634) Emotional Intelligence Profile 1 Contents 04 About this report 05 Introduction to Emotional Intelligence 06 Your

More information

Attitude I. Attitude A. A positive or negative evaluation of a concept B. Attitudes tend to be based on 1)...values 2)...beliefs 3)...

Attitude I. Attitude A. A positive or negative evaluation of a concept B. Attitudes tend to be based on 1)...values 2)...beliefs 3)... Attitude I. Attitude A. A positive or negative evaluation of a concept B. Attitudes tend to be based on 1)...values 2)...beliefs 3)...affect, how things make us feels 4)...past experience II. Values A.

More information

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 6.1 Research Design Research is an organized, systematic, data based, critical, objective, scientific inquiry or investigation into a specific problem, undertaken with the

More information

The effect of positive mood on intention to use computerized decision aids

The effect of positive mood on intention to use computerized decision aids Worcester Polytechnic Institute DigitalCommons@WPI User Exprience and Decision Making Research Laboratory Publications User Experience and Decision Making Research Laboratory 1-1-2008 The effect of positive

More information

The Influence of Spokesperson Trustworthiness on Message Elaboration, Attitude Strength, and Advertising Effectiveness

The Influence of Spokesperson Trustworthiness on Message Elaboration, Attitude Strength, and Advertising Effectiveness PRIESTER ENDORSER AND TRUSTWORTHINESS PETTY JOURNAL OF CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY, 13(4), 408 421 Copyright 2003, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Influence of Spokesperson Trustworthiness on Message Elaboration,

More information

Raymond C. Hawkins II. Fielding Graduate University; The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, USA

Raymond C. Hawkins II. Fielding Graduate University; The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, USA Psychology Research, December 2016, Vol. 6, No. 12, 748-754 doi:10.17265/2159-5542/2016.12.005 D DAVID PUBLISHING Weight Bias Internalization: Semantic Differential Measurement and Treatment Implications

More information

AND ITS VARIOUS DEVICES. Attitude is such an abstract, complex mental set. up that its measurement has remained controversial.

AND ITS VARIOUS DEVICES. Attitude is such an abstract, complex mental set. up that its measurement has remained controversial. CHAPTER III attitude measurement AND ITS VARIOUS DEVICES Attitude is such an abstract, complex mental set up that its measurement has remained controversial. Psychologists studied attitudes of individuals

More information

BODY IMAGE AND FIRST IMPRESSIONS 1

BODY IMAGE AND FIRST IMPRESSIONS 1 BODY IMAGE AND FIRST IMPRESSIONS 1 BODY IMAGE AND FIRST IMPRESSIONS 2 Abstract Research has been done separately in the past on the following three topics: Attraction biases, first impressions, and body

More information

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance 1 Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Erica Behrens Winona State University Abstract This study examines the effects of self-consciousness

More information

PSYC 221 Introduction to General Psychology

PSYC 221 Introduction to General Psychology PSYC 221 Introduction to General Psychology Session 13 Summary Lecturer: Dr. Joana Salifu Yendork, Psychology Department Contact Information: jyendork@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing

More information

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation Running head: SELF-EFFICACY, FEEDBACK PERCEPTION AND INTRINSIC MOTIVATION The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation Group 16 Lindy Wijsman

More information

attitude the theory that we explain someone's behavior by crediting the situation or the person's disposition attribution theory

attitude the theory that we explain someone's behavior by crediting the situation or the person's disposition attribution theory attitude feelings, often influenced by our beliefs, that predispose us to respond in a particular way to objects, people, and events attribution theory the theory that we explain someone's behavior by

More information

ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition. Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation

ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition. Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation 1 Learning Objectives Based on Chapter 2 of the ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th ed., this session describes

More information

SAMPLE. Coaching Report for. Sally Sample. This Sample Report provided by:

SAMPLE. Coaching Report for. Sally Sample. This Sample Report provided by: SAMPLE Coaching Report for This Sample Report provided by: Profiles International, Inc. Profiles Office Park 5205 Lake Shore Drive Waco, Texas, USA 76710 254.751.1644 Introduction The coaching report for

More information