u. S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "u. S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences"

Transcription

1 -, , The Psychological Record, 2003, 53, MUTUAL SELECTION AND MEMBERSHIP IN OPEN-ENDED CLASSES: VARIANT-TO-BASE AND BASE-TO-VARIANT TESTING LANNY FIELDS, PRIYA MATNEJA, and ANTONIOS VARELAS Queens Col/ege and the Graduate School of the City University of New York JAMES BELANICH u. S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences Two open-ended classes were formed when students were exposed to a forced choice primary generalization test, conducted in a matching-to-sample context. The classes were at opposite ends of a fill-based continuum. The endpoints of the continuum were called base stimuli and the intermediate stimuli were called variants. Class members were identified with tests conducted in the traditional variant-to-base format and a new base-to-variant format. The ranges of variants that functioned as members of the same nominal class differed with test format. In addition, the direction of that difference reversed for the classes at each end of the continuum. Followup tests showed that adjacent variants in a class were discriminable from each other. Thus, forced choice primary generalization tests induced classes of similar but discriminable stimuli. The fact that the same nominal class has two different widths raises a fundamental question about the definition of class membership. That issue is discussed along with recommendations for answering the question. Behavior that is controlled by categories or classes of stimuli is of great adaptive utility because it enables an individual to respond effectively to the new stimuli that are inevitably encountered in real world settings (Bruner, Goodnow, & Austin, 1965; Medin & Smith, 1984; Rosch & Mervis, 1975; Smith, 1989). Some categories or stimulus classes are said to be open-ended because they contain an infinite number of exemplars that resemble each other (Herrnstein, 1990). Open-ended classes have also been called dimensional, perceptual (Fields & Reeve, 2000), feature-based (Stromer & Mackay, 1997), similarity-based (Wasserman, Keidinger, & Bhatt, 1988), fuzzy (Rosch & Mervis, 1975), illdefined (Homa & Chambliss, 1985; Homa & Little, 1985), and This research was conducted with support from Contract DASW01-96-K-0009 from the U. S. Army Research Institute, and by PSC-CUNY Research Awards 68547, 69567, and We thank Xiqiang Zhu for his assistance in the development of the software used to produce the fill patterns, conduct the experiment, and analyze the data reported herein. Reprints can be obtained from Lanny Fields, Department of Psychology, Queens College/CUNY, Kissena Boulevard, Flushing, NY

2 288 FIELDS ET AL. probabilistic (Medin & Smith, 1984). Because open-ended classes are ubiquitous, it is important to understand the properties that characterize class members. The present experiment investigated an unexplored property of open-ended classes: mutual selectivity. An open-ended class consists of contiguous stimuli along some continuum, all of which occasion the same response that has been trained to one or a few of the stimuli in that range (Cook, Wright, & Krendrick, 1990; Fields & Reeve, 2001; Goldiamond, 1966; Hull, 1920; Keller & Schoenfeld, 1950; Lea, 1984; Reeve & Fields, 2001; Wasserman et ai., 1988; Wright, Cook, Rivera, Sands, & Delius, 1988). This definition implies that all of the stimuli in a class should be substitutable or interchangeable with each other (Goldiamond, 1962; Sidman, 1994). In tests conducted in a conditional discrimination format, interchangeability would be demonstrated by the selection of any class member used as a comparison stimulus in the presence of any member of the same class presented as a sample. That is, all of the stimuli in a class should result in the mutual selection of each other. Mutual selection, then, might be viewed as a defining property of stimuli that are members of open-ended classes. Indeed, mutual selection of physically disparate stimuli is the basis for demonstrating the formation of another type of category called an equivalence class (Fields & Verhave, 1987; Sidman, 1994; Sidman & Tailby, 1982). Of necessity, the evaluation of mutual selectivity requires the presentation of tests conducted in variant-to-base and base-to-variant formats (Reeve & Fields, 2001). These can be illustrated by considering straight lines that vary in length, with the shortest and longest lines serving as the base stimuli for the classes of short and long lines, respectively. When tests are conducted in the variant-to-base format, intermediate length lines called variants are presented as sample stimuli along with the longest and shortest lines (base stimuli) as comparisons. With these tests, the class of long lines would include all variants that occasioned the selection of the long line comparison with the same high likelihood. When the base-to-variant tests are conducted, either the longest or shortest lines (the base stimuli) are presented as samples with the variants as comparisons. In these tests, the class of long lines would include all variants selected with the same high likelihood in the presence of the long line presented as a sample (Fields & Reeve, 2000). According to mutual selection, the variants that Bllre members of an open-ended class would be those that occasion the selection of the base stimulus during the variant-to-base tests SIIJ'U:ll are also selected in the presence of the same base stimulus during the base-to-variant tests. In addition, the variants that slire ll1loft class members would include those variants that were selected in the presence of a given base stimulus in base-to-variant tests but did not occasion the selection of the same base stimulus in variant-to-base tests, or vice versa. Notwithstanding the plausibility of assuming that the stimuli in an open-ended class should have the functional property of mutual selection, data obtained from many studies of stimulus generalization and psychophysics suggest otherwise.

3 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 289 With respect to generalization, generalization among similar stimuli is influenced by variables such as the frequency (Thomas & Vogt, 1983; Thomas, Windell, Williams, & White, 1985), range (Croll, 1972; Hansen, Tomie, Thomas, & Thomas, 1974; Thomas & Bistey, 1964; Thomas, Strub, & Dickson, 1974), number (Thomas & Bistey, 1964), separation (Thomas, Mood, Morrison, & Wiertelak, 1991), assymetry (Thomas, 1993), frame or reference (Thomas, Lusky, & Morrison, 1992), and stimulus labeling (Galizio & Baron, 1976, 1979) in generalization tests. Because generalization tests are used to assess class width, and test format is a parameter of generalization testing, test format, then, might influence the width of the same nominal open-ended stimulus class. With regard to thresholds, many studies have shown that the quantitative value of a difference threshold is influenced by the psychophysical method used for its determination (Gescheider, 1997; Woodworth, 1938; Woodworth & Schlosberg, 1954). A difference threshold is the rnallri'lowes~ range of stimuli that occasion the same response. An openended class is the bl/'ocllo1es~ range of stimuli that occasion the same response. Thus, the determination of difference thresholds and open-ended class width both involve the measurement of stimulus ranges that occasion common responses. Because procedural variables influence difference thresholds, procedural variables such as test format might influence the range of stimuli that function as members of the same nominal open-ended class. To summarize, the generalization and threshold experiments suggest that the ranges of stimuli that function as members of the same nominal class might vary with test format. If so, not all of the stimuli in the same nominal class would result in the mutual selection of each other. Mutual selection, then, would not be a functional property of the stimuli in open-ended classes. To date, only variant-to-base tests have been used to identify stimuli that are members of open-ended classes (Fields & Reeve, 2001; Wasserman et al, 1988; Wright et al, 1988; ). To our knowledge, base-to-variant tests have never been used to identify the variants that function as members of openended classes. Thus, no data are available to determine whether all of the stimuli in an open-ended class actually occasion the mutual selection of each other. In the present experiment, open-ended classes were formed by students with prior exposure to forced choice primary generalization test trials that were conducted in the absence of reinforcement (Reeve & Fields, 2001). This new method for the establishment of open-ended classes differs from traditional induction procedures that involve differential reinforcement of responding in the presence of multiple exemplars (Homa & Little, 1985; Honig & Stewart, 1988; Jitsumori, 1996; Malott & Siddall, 1972; Markle, 1990; Rosch & Mervis, 1975; Tiemann & Markle, 1990; Wasserman et al, 1988; Wright et al, 1988). After the induction of open-ended classes, we measured the range of stimuli that functioned as members of the same nominal class using generalization tests conducted in the traditional variantto-base format and a newly developed base-to-variant format. In a second experiment we measured the discriminability of the stimuli along the domain to determine whether the class consisted of stimuli that were discriminable from each other.

4 290 FIELDS ET AL. Experiment 1 Method Subjects Three undergraduate students at Queens College participated in the study. They were recruited from advanced psychology classes and were not familiar with the research area. Students received partial course credit upon completion of the experiment but credit did not depend upon performance. All participants were required to read and sign an informed consent statement prior to participation in the experiment. The entire experiment lasted about 3 hours. Each experimental session lasted approximately 1.5 hr. Apparatus Hardware. The experiment was conducted with IBM-compatible personal computers that displayed all stimuli on 15-in. SVGA color monitors. Responses consisted of touching specific keys on a standard keyboard. The experiment was controlled by customized software developed to conduct stimulus control experiments. Stimuli. During Phase 2, horizontal lines of varying length were used to train subjects to use a neither comparison. The lines were produced with a string of ASCII character Alternate 196, each of which will be called a unit. Each unit was 3 mm wide by 5 mm high on the monitor. The lines varied in length from 1 to 24 units. The neither comparison and its use are described below. During Phases 3-5, 19 stimuli were presented to the subjects. Each was a 2- x 2-in. borderless square that contained a different percentage of dark pixels on a white background. The pixels were large enough that the squares did not appear as different shades of gray. A stimulus dimension was produced by varying the percentage of darkened pixels in the square. The percentage of dark pixels defined each square's "fill" value and ranged from 23% (FiIl23) to 77% (Fill77) in 3% increments. For each fill value, the pattern of pixels was randomly generated preexperimentally and remained fixed throughout the experiment. Fill23 and Fill77 defined the endpoints of the fill dimension. Fill50 was the physical midpoint of the dimension, while Fill35 and Fill62 were two intermediate values on either side of the midpoint. The remaining 12 stimuli are not illustrated. Prior research has shown that the adjacent stimuli along the fill continuum are discriminable from each other (Reeve & Fields, 2001). Procedure Trial format and responses within a trial. All trials used a matching-tosample format. A trial began when "Press ENTER" appeared on the screen. Pressing the enter key cleared the screen and displayed a sample stimulus at the top center of the monitor. Pressing the space bar

5 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 291 displayed two comparison stimuli at the bottom left and right corners while the sample remained on the screen. During trials in which the third comparison was programmed, the words "If NEITHER press 4" appeared between the two other comparisons. During a trial, the left or right comparison was selected by pressing the number 1 or 2 key, respectively. Pressing the number 4 key selected the neither comparison, when available. A comparison selection cleared the screen and concurrently displayed a feedback message centered on the screen. When informative feedback was scheduled, the messages "RIGHT" or "WRONG" appeared, depending on the accuracy of the comparison selection. The message remained on the screen until the R (for "RIGHT") or W (for "WRONG") key was pressed. During some training and all test trials, noninformative feedback was scheduled following a comparison selection. This consisted of a dashed line surrounding the letter E (- - E - -) that signaled the end of a trial. The dashed line remained on screen until the student pressed the "E" key which was used as an observing response (Dinsmoor, 1985) to the non informative feedback. After an appropriate observing response was made, the screen was cleared and the next trial began (Fields, Landon Jimenez, Buffington, & Adams, 1995). Trial block structure and feedback contingencies. Each phase of training and testing was conducted with blocks of trials. Within each block in all experimental phases, the trials were presented in a random order without replacement. At the start of training, a block was presented repeatedly with informative feedback after each comparison selection until all trials within the block occasioned 100% correct responding. Thereafter, the percentage of trials that occasioned informative feedback was reduced to 75%, 25%, and finally to 0% over successive blocks as long as 100% accuracy within a block was maintained. During feedback reduction, the trials that were followed by informative feedback were randomly determined. If 100% correct responding was not achieved within three blocks at a given feedback level during training, the student was returned to the previous feedback level for that particular block. Phase 1: Instructions and keyboard familiarization. Prior to the experiment, students were presented with the following instructions on the screen: Thank you for volunteering to participate. PLEASE DO NOT TOUCH ANY KEYS ON THE KEYBOARD YET! You will be presented with many trials. Each trial contains three or four CUES that are shapes, symbols, or common words. YOUR TASK IS TO DISCOVER HOW TO RESPOND CORRECTLY TO THE CUES BY PRESSING CERTAIN KEYS ON THE COMPUTER'S KEYBOARD. Initially, INSTRUCTIONS will tell you how to respond to the cues, and LABELS will help you identify the cues on the screen. The labels and instructions will slowly disappear. The experiment is conducted in phases. When each phase ends, the computer will sometimes tell you how you did. If you want to take a break at any time, call the experimenter. Thank you for your cooperation! Press the space bar to continue.

6 292 FIELDS ET AL. After pressing the space bar, students learned to emit the appropriate keyboard responses to complete a trial. To accomplish this, 16 trials, each containing three English words such as KING, QUEEN, and CAMEL, were presented. The semantic relatedness between the sample word (e.g., KING) and one of the comparisons (e.g., QUEEN) was used to prompt the selection of the correct comparison. Informative feedback followed each comparison selection (refer to Fields, Reeve, Adams, Brown, & Verhave, 1997, for further details). Correct responding to the stimuli in a trial during Phase 1 was also facilitated by instructional prompts (e.g., "Make your choice by pressing 1 or 2") that were deleted in a serial manner across trials (refer to Fields et ai., 1997, or Fields, Adams, Verhave, & Newman, 1990, for further details). Phase 1 ended once the stimuli were presented without prompts and performance exceeded 85% accuracy (14/16 correct trials) during a single block. For the remainder of the experiment, if a nonexperimentally defined key was pressed during a trial, the instruction used to prompt the appropriate key press during Keyboard Familiarization (Phase 1) reappeared on the screen for three subsequent trials. Phase 2: Neither-comparison training. The goal of the experiment was to measure the range of variants that function as members of higherand lower-value fill-based classes. It was considered likely, however, that some intermediate value fill-variants would not be functioning as a member of either the high- or the low-fill class. Thus, it was necessary to provide a subject with a response (selection of the neither comparison) that could be used to indicate that a particular fill variant was not functioning as a member of either the high- or low-fill-based class. This response has also been called a default option (Innis, Lane, Miller, & Critchfield, 1998; Lane, Clow, Innis, & Critchfield, 1998). The use of the neither comparison was trained with stimuli that were horizontal lines which varied from 1 to 24 units in length. Because each unit was 3 mm wide, the lines varied from 3 to 72 mm in length in 3-mm steps. All trials included three comparison stimuli: a 1- and a 24-unit line along with a neither option. The presentation of the 1- to 6-unit lines as samples occasioned reinforcement for the selection of the 1-unit comparison. The presentation of the 19- to 24-unit lines occasioned reinforcement for the selection of the 24-unit comparison. The presentation of the 11- to 14-unit lines occasioned reinforcement for the selection of the neither comparison. The remaining line lengths were used in testing only and never occasioned reinforcement. All training and testing consisted of blocks of trials. All trials in a block produced differential feedback until responding in a block reached 100% accuracy. Thereafter, feedback was reduced as previously described. Once performance was maintained in the absence of feedback, generalization tests were conducted using all line lengths. Some tests were conducted in the variant-to-base format in which all of the line lengths were presented as samples. Each trial contained the same three comparisons used for training. Other tests were conducted in the base-to-variant

7 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 293 format. In some trials, the i-unit line was presented as the sample along with a variant, the neither option, and the 24-unit line as the comparisons. On the remaining trials, the 24-unit line was presented as the sample. The comparisons consisted of a variant, the neither option, and the i-unit line. In all of the base-to-variant tests, the variant changed from trial to trial in a random order. Phase 3: Identity conditional discrimination training. Next, students received identity conditional discrimination training with fill-based stimuli. FilI23 and Fill77 were randomly alternated as samples. The two comparisons were Fill23 and Fill77. Each comparison appeared equally often on the left and right of the screen. A correct response was the selection of the comparison that was identical to the sample. When 100% informative feedback was scheduled, each block contained 32 trials. When 75, 25, or 0% feedback was scheduled, each block contained 16 trials. Feedback was reduced as previously described. Phase 3 was completed when a student made no errors during a block with 0% informative feedback. Phase 4: Class formation through two-choice generalization testing. Once the identity conditional discriminations were established, two broad fill-based stimulus classes were induced by conducting a two-choice generalization test in a variant-to-base format (Reeve & Fields, 2001). Within a test block, each of the 17 fill values was presented as a sample twice for a total of 34 trials. Fill23 and Fill77 were the two comparisons. For each sample, a given comparison appeared once on the left and once on the right in random order. All comparison selections were followed by noninformative feedback. Students received 38 two-choice generalization trials in each of 20 test blocks. Thus, each variant was presented 40 times, a value that maximized the range of variants that came to function as members of perceptual classes (Reeve & Fields, 2001). The trial format, block structure, response contingencies, and responses within a trial were the same as those used in Phase 2. Phase 5: Measurement of class width with three-choice generalization tests. As mentioned in the introduction, the widths of functionally independent classes can be determined using generalization tests that contain stimuli from from one of two classes and a neither comparison (Belanich, 2002; Reeve & Fields, 2001). That procedure was used in the current experiment to measure the widths of fill-based classes at each end of the fill continuum. These generalization tests were conducted using the same fill stimuli with three comparison selections available on all trials. Half of the trials were conducted in the variant-tobase format and the other in the base-to-variant format, each of which will be described below. The stimuli used in all trials are listed in Table 1. In the variant-to-base tests, the variants of the fill stimuli were presented as samples with the Fi1l23, Fill77, and the neither option presented as comparisons. For each fill variant presented as a sample, we measured the likelihood of selecting each of the three comparisons. On all trials, subjects could also select the neither comparison. Thus, the

8 294 FIELDS ET AL. Table 1 Symbolic Representation of Stimulus Triads in Phase 5 Type Sa Co+ Co- #Trials Variant-to-Base A1(23) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(26) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(29) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(32) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(35) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(38) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(41) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(44) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(47) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(50) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(53) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(56) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(59) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(62) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(65) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(68) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(71) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A(74) A1(23) A2(77) 2 A2(77) A1(23) A2(77) 2 Base-to-Variant A1(23) A1(23) A2(77) 2 Low Fill A1(23) A(26) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(29) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(32) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(35) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(38) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(41) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(44) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(47) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(50) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(53) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(56) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(59) A2(77) 2 A1(23) A(62) A2(77) 2 Base-to-Variant A2(77) A2(77) A2(23) 2 High Fill A2(77) A(74) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(68) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(65) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(62) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(59) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(56) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(53) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(50) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(47) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(44) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(41) A2(23) 2 A2(77) A(38) A2(23) 2 Note. In each trial block, Co+s appeared equally often on the left and right. The numerals in parentheses indicate the value of the fill pattern used for the indicated stimulus.

9 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 295 inclusion of the neither comparison allowed for a functional separation of the range of low- and high-fill stimuli that occasioned the selection of the Fill23 or Fi1l77. This test was conducted in a block of 38 trials. Each variant was presented in 2 trials in the block. Variant values were presented in randomized order without replacement. In all base-to-variant tests, one of the endpoint stimuli on the fill dimension was presented as a sample, the other endpoint stimulus was presented as the nominal negative comparison, and all trials contained the neither comparison. The variants of the fill patterns were presented as the other comparison. Half of the trials contained FiII23 as the sample with Fill77 and the neither option as constant comparisons, and fill values 23 to 68 as the other comparisons. The other trials contained Fill?? as the sample with Fill23 and the neither option as constant comparisons, and fill values 77 to 32 as the other comparisons. Pilot work suggested that the ranges of variants that functioned as class members could be identified with the use of the aforementioned ranges of variants. The inclusion of the neither comparison on all trials permitted the functional separation of the ranges of fill variants that were selected exclusively in the presence of the Fill23 and Fill?? samples. All of these tests were conducted in a block of 56 trials. Each variant in a block was presented in 2 trials. Variant values were presented in randomized order without replacement Definition of class membership and order of presenting test blocks. The variant-to-base and base-to-variant test blocks were presented four times each, in simple alternation. Each variant was presented eight times in each test format, allowing for a 12% variation in selection probability. In the variant-to-base tests, the variants that occasioned the selection of a given comparison on at least 88% of the trials were functioning as potential members of the same class as the selected comparison. In the base-to-variant tests, the variants that were selected in the presence of a given sample on at least 88% of the trials were functioning as members of the same class as that sample. Results and Discussion Two-choice variant-to-base generalization tests. Figure 1 contains the results of the two-choice generalization tests conducted with the fill variants in the variant-to-base format. During this test, a broad range of contiguous lower fill variants almost always occasioned the selection of the FiII23 comparison. In addition, a different range of contiguous higher fill variants almost always occasioned the selection of the Fill77 comparison. These ranges of stimuli will be called the "plateaus" of the gradients. The likelihood of selecting either comparison decreased rapidly and systematically for the intermediate range of fill variants that were between the plateaus. For Subjects 1692 and 1?0?, similar ranges of variants occasioned the selection of the FiII23 and Fill77 comparisons. For Subject 1732, however, a much larger range of variants occasioned the selection of the Fill23 rather than the Fill77 comparison. Finally, the variants that defined the plateaus for Subjects

10 I I 296 FIELDS ET AL. Variant to Base 100 z Q u w w U) ~ w 40 U It: w Do TF1692SR ' Z Q... u w w U) ~ 40 w U It: 20 w Do. 0 TF1707WF 23 2' z Q... u ~ w U) z 40 w U It: w Do TF1732FP 2'3 : '3 59 FILL VARIANTS Figure 1. Results of the 2-choice generalization tests conducted in Experiment 1, prior to the formation of equivalence classes. Likelihood of selecting Fill23 and Fill77 plotted as functions of the value of the fill stimuli presented as samples. Data for each subject are illustrated on separate graphs.

11 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH and 1707 occasioned somewhat higher levels of comparison selection than they did for Subject The variants that occasioned the selection of the low-fill comparison could have been functioning as members of a low-fill class, the variants that occasioned the selection of the high-fill comparison could have been functioning as members of a high-fill class. That conclusion, however, is questionable because of the availability of only two comparisons. In a two-choice generalization test, the selection of one comparison is the arithmetic complement of the selection of the other comparison. Thus, if we assume that the low-fill stimuli were functioning as members of the low-fill class, the other fill variants might have occasioned the selection of the other comparison as a default and might not be functioning as a class. Further, because only two comparisons were available, the selection of one comparison in the presence of intermediate fill variants might have been driven by the responses available rather than by the class functionality of the stimuli. Thus, particular variants might not have been functioning as members of a class even though they were occasioning responses that would appear to index class membership. For these reasons, the data obtained in the two-choice generaization tests could not be used to determine the range of fill variants that were functioning as VARIANT TO BASE TF1692SR TF1707WF TF1732FP ':J ~':~f j.. eo eo I:J ~4Q '".., 5J 59 " it 17 2'! 8 3ii.. t 47 &'! Sa ' , & a: eo w E eo ~~~ 23 8!! ~I:L ~.. 20 i!!rs_3s"ti.,h.tn5;r-j Io1B..,."~'" Fill VARIANTS ::-'L o s'! it I:~ 8. ' a SO 4& FILL VARIANTS I ~~ 23 :8 S! ~ t n 11 n loo~ ~ a 35 4' B FILL VARIANTS Figure 2. The results of the 3-choice generalization tests conducted in the variant-to-base format during Experiment 1, prior to the formation of equivalence classes. The top functions illustrate the likelihood of selecting the longest line comparison (FiIl77), the middle function illustrates the likelihood of selecting the neither comparison, and the bottom function illustrates the likelihood of selecting the longest line comparison (FiIl23), all in the presence of each line length presented as a sample. Data for a given subject are indicated on the same column.

12 298 FIELDS ET AL. members of low-fill or high-fill classes (Fields et ai., 1997; Reeve & Fields, 2001; Wasserman et ai., 1988). Three-choice variant-to-base tests. The results of the 3-choice variant-to-base generalization tests are shown in Figure 2. The variants closest to Fill23 almost always occasioned the selection of the Fill23 comparison. As the value of the variants became larger, the likelihood of selecting the Fill23 comparison declined and was accompanied by a complementary increase in the selection of the neither comparison. These variants, however, rarely occasioned the selection of the Fill?? comparison. Finally, Subjects 1692 and 1707 used the neither comparison more frequently than did Subject A similar pattern of responding was observed in the presence of the lower values of the fill variants. The variants that were most similar to Fill?? almost always occasioned the selection of the Fill?? comparison. As the value of a variant became lower, the likelihood of selecting the Fill?? comparison declined and was accompanied by a complementary increase in the selection of the neither comparison. These variants, however, rarely occasioned the selection of the Fill23 comparison. The intermediate values TF1692SR ='OO~ Ii eo i eo > = 40 ~ 20 o 23 8 sa 4{ on 4! &'a e looj "" 80-5 " 60 l 40 Q. 20 o i3 2'0 33.,., ~ 5.1B 71 f1 BASE TO VARIANT - Sample: A1 (FILL-23) TF1707WF looj 00 eo o TF1732FP 100 ;:: eo ~ eo ~ 4Q Q. 20 o --A..- i3 2' " 53 s'o IB 71 n FILL VARtANTS 100 eo o "A_A.JI._. 238 a '! gob 71 "h FILL VARIANTS 100 eo o -I\./\ ! s'3 s'd FILL VARIANTS Figure 3. The results of the 3-choice generalization tests conducted in the base-to-variant format. In these tests, Fill23 was the sample. The values of every other variant presented as comparisons are listed on the abscissas of each graph. The top graphs illustrate the likelihood of selecting the fill variants indicated on the abscissa in the presence of the Fill23 sample. The middle graphs illustrate the likelihood of selecting the neither comparison in the presence of the Fill23 sample and the variant indicated on the abscissa. The bottom graphs illustrate the likelihood of selecting the Fill77 comparison in the context of the Fill23 sample and each variant value presented as a comparison as indicated on the abscissa. Each column contains data for a different subject.

13 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 299 of the fill variants typically occasioned the selection of the neither comparison more than either of the fill comparisons for Subjects 1692 and This occurred to a lesser degree for Subject Stimuli along a continuum are functioning as members of a class if they all occasion the selection of the same comparison stimulus with a high and equivalent likelihood. The outcomes of the generalization tests conducted in the variant-to-base format showed the formation of two classes: a low-fill class consisting of fill-based variants that occasioned the selection of the lowest fill value on the dimension and a high-fill class consisting of another nonoverlapping range of fill variants that occasioned the selection of the highest fill value. Finally, the complementary relation between the selection of one comparison and the selection of the neither comparison showed that two functionally independent stimulus classes were formed, one at each end of the fill-based continuum. Three-choice base-to-variant tests. Figure 3 illustrates the outcome of the three-choice base-to-variant tests conducted with Fill23 as the sample. The variants most similar to Fill23 were almost always selected in the presence of the Fill23 sample. Variants of greater value were selected with decreasing likelihood and were accompanied by a complementary increase in the selection of the neither comparison. The Fill77 comparison was rarely selected in the presence of Fill23 and the variants used as comparisons. Similar results were obtained for all subjects. For Subjects 1692 or 1707, the higher value fill comparisons were rarely selected in the presence of the Fill23 sample. For Subject 1732, however, the higher value fill comparisons were selected with somewhat greater likelihood in the presence of the Fill23 sample. Figure 4 illustrates the results of the base-to-variant tests that used the Fill77 stimulus as a sample. The variants most similar to Fill77 were almost always selected in the presence of the FiII77 sample stimulus. Variants of lesser value were selected with decreasing likelihood and were accompanied by a complementary increase in the selection of the neither comparison. The Fill23 comparison was rarely selected in the presence of Fill77 and the variants used as comparisons. Similar results were obtained for all subjects. One notable difference can be seen across subjects, however. For Subjects 1692 and 1707, the lower value fill comparisons were rarely selected in the presence of the Fill77 sample. For Subject 1732, however, the lower value fill comparisons were selected with somewhat greater likelihood in the presence of the Fill77 sample. Stimuli along a continuum function as members of a class if they are selected with high and equivalent likelihoods in the presence of a given stimulus. The outcomes of the generalization tests conducted in the baseto-variant format, which were presented in Figures 3 and 4, showed the formation of two classes: a low-fill class consisting of fill-based variants that were selected in the presence of the lowest fill value and a high-fill class consisting of another nonoverlapping range of fill variants that were selected in the presence of the highest fill value. Finally, the complementary relation between the selection of the variant comparisons and the selection of the

14 300 FIELDS ET AL. BASE TO VARIANT - Sample= A2(FILL-77) TF1692SR TF170TWF TF1732FP _ 100 / F c f ~ > '0.0.0 i! cr d :81!'s SS t L 23 S 53 4' :n Q 59 tis 11 n 23 a!! Sa 59 B a is ;; 80 L ~ ~ Z.0 '0 '0 cr M eo "I '0 '0 '0 ~ \..... ~ is a n ' 41 &3 go FILL VARIANTS FILL VARIANTS FILL VARIANTS Figure 4. The results of the 3-choice generalization tests conducted in the base-to-variant format. In these tests, Fill77 was the sample. The values of every other variant presented as comparisons are listed on the abscissas of each graph. The top graphs illustrate the likelihood of selecting the fill variants indicated on the abscissa in the presence of the Fill77 sample. The middle graphs illustrate the likelihood of selecting the neither comparison in the presence of the Fill?? sample and the variant indicated on the abscissa. The bottom graphs illustrate the likelihood of selecting the Fill23 comparison in the context of the Fill77 sample and each variant value presented as a comparison as indicated on the abscissa. Each column contains data for a different subject. neither comparison showed that two functionally independent stimulus classes were formed, one at each end of the fill-based continuum. Test format effects on class width. Figure 5 depicts the generalization gradients obtained in both test formats for the high- and low-fill classes for each subject. Class width was defined by the variant-to-base test as the range of stimuli that occasioned the selection of the endpoint stimuli. Class width was also defined by the base-to-variant test as the range of variants that were selected in the presence of a given endpoint stimulus. These class widths are reflected by the plateaus of the relevant gradients and are also listed quantitatively in Table 2. A comparison of these gradients show how test format influenced the widths of the classes at each end of the fill-based continuum. When the low-fill stimuli were considered, the range of variants that occasioned the selection of the Fill23 comparison during the variant-tobase test was larger than the range of variants that were selected in the presence of the Fill23 sample during the base-to-variant test. That is, the width of the low-fill class as measured by the variant-to-base test was

15 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 301 LOW FILL CLASS VB vs BV TF1692SR ~ ~ 80 ~ % ~ : 0: 0 50 ::: 40 0 ~ ~ 30 ~ 30 I~BV I --VB HIGH FILL CLASS ~ ~ TF1707WF ~ % Q 70 % 60 0: 0 50 ~ 40 ~ 30 0: ::: 40 ~ TF1732FP ~ 80 ~ 80 % 70 1/ 70 ~ 60 x 60 0: 0: ::: 40 ~ 40 ~ 30 ~ FILL VARIANTS FILL VARIANTS Figure 5. A comparison of the variant-to-base and base-to-variant 3-choice generalization gradients. The graphs on the left and right axes are for the low- and high-fill classes, respectively. Each row is for a different subject. On each graph, the gradients defined by the dark triangles illustrate the results of variant-to-base tests and show the likelihood of selecting an endpoint fill comparison in the presence of each variant. For these functions, the abscissa values are the fill variants presented as samples. The gradients defined by the light triangles illustrate the results of base-to-variant tests and show the likelihood of selecting a variant in the presence of an endpoint fill stimulus presented as a sample. For these functions, the abscissa values are the fill variants presented as comparisons. greater than class width as measured by the base-to-variant test. In contrast, when the high-fill stimuli were considered, the range of variants that occasioned the selection of the Fill77 comparison during the variantto-base test was smaller than the range of variants that were selected in the presence of the Fill77 sample during the base-to-variant test. That is, the width of the high-fill class as measured by the variant-to-base test

16 302 FIELDS ET AL. Table 2 Class Width Measured in Fill Units Presented as a Function of Class, Test Format, and Subject Class width by Class width by Difference in test format test format class width Low Fill High Fill (B-V) - (V-B) Subject B-V* V-B# B-V V-B Low Fill High Fill Average * During base-to-variant tests, class width was equal to the absolute difference in the base value for a class and the variant most distant from that base value that was selected on at least 88% of test trials. # During variant-to-base tests, class width was equal to the absolute difference in the base value for a class and the variant most distant from that base value that occasioned the selection of the base stimulus on at least 88% of test trials. was smaller than class width measured with the base-to-variant test. A 2 x 2 ANOVA confirmed the significance of the interaction between test format and class location on class width, F(3, 8) = 45.02, P = Tukey-Kramer post hoc paired comparison tests showed that the difference in class width measured in each test fomat was significant for the low-fill class (p <.01) and high-fill class (p <.01). Finally, the absolute magnitude of the differences in class width measured in the two test formats were not significantly different for the two classes (p =.57). The extent to which the differences in class width can be attributed to test format can be evaluated with a correlational analysis that uses adjusted values of,e. This analysis showed that test format accounted for 93% of the difference in the range of variants that functioned as members of the low-fill class, and 99% of the difference in the range of variants that functioned as members of the high-fill class. A correlational analysis also showed that the absolute value of the variants in each class accounted for 96% of the difference in the directionality effect of the test format. To summarize, test format influenced the widths of two open-ended classes, and the differences in class width obtained with each test format reversed for the classes at each end of the fill dimension. Possible determinants of the test format effect on class width. Test format appears to be a nonsensory determinant of the stimuli that function as members of an open-ended class. It is also possible that the observed differences in class width might have been driven by a confound that differentiated the trials presented in each format. One such variable is the negative comparison used in each test format. The variantto-base tests contained the same two comparisons on all trials. In contrast, the base-to-variant tests contained variants as one comparison and a constant negative comparison. In the base-to-variant tests, then, the selection of the variable comparison might have been driven by responding away from the constant negative comparison (Carrigan & Sidman, 1992; Johnson & Sidman, 1993; Strikeleather & Sidman, 1990;

17 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 303 Stromer, 1986; Stromer & Osborne, 1982; Tomonaga, 1993). According to this argument, the difference in class width observed in the current experiment would reflect the effect of a methodological confound rather than a true difference in class width produced by test format. Two points, however, raise questions regarding the plausibility of this hypothesis. First, if the argument is to be accepted, the constancy of the negative comparison should have had the same effect on the direction of the test format effect regardless of the absolute values of the stimuli in a class. In the current experiment, the differences in class width reversed for the high-fill and low-fill classes. Therefore, the constant negative comparison hypothesis does not account for at least 50% of the findings reported in this experiment. Second, when a specific variant is used in both test formats, once all of the stimuli on a trial are present, the trials in both formats COU1l~BlDU1I ~ihs sams ftihll'ss sftdmul~d, albeit in different positions during a trial. Thus, if membership in Class 1 is being evaluated, a variant-to-base trial would have X' presented as the sample with X1 and X2 as comparisons, while the companion base-to-variant trial would have X1 presented as the sample with X' and X2 as comparisons. If it is assumed that a subject scans all of the stimuli on a trial many times before selecting a comparison, it does not seem plausible that the nominal negative comparison can be considered as a constant in the base-to-variant trial any more than in a variant-to-base trial. These considerations suggest that the presence or absence of a constant negative comparison does not provide a plausible account for the differences in class width measured with the different test formats. The difference in the effect of test format on class width would appear to be governed by some as yet unspecified parameters that characterize the stimulus displays presented under each format. Some possible parameters might be the order of introducing the stimuli in a trial, and the relative positions of sample and comparison stimuli in a trial. Experiment 2 At least two demonstrations are needed to conclude that the stimuli arrayed along a continuum are functioning as members of a class (Fields & Reeve, 2001; Lea, 1984; Reeve & Fields, 2001; Wasserman et ai., 1988). First, many variants must occasion a given response (such as comparison selection) even though that response has been established in the presence of only one or a few stimuli in the dimension (Keller & Schoenfeld, 1950; Wright et ai., 1988). The importance of the first requirement can be appreciated by considering that appropriate and novel stimulus relations will control responding even though those relations had not been directly trained (Bruner et ai., 1956; Fields & Reeve, 2001; Hull, 1920; Keller & Schoenfeld, 1950). Thus, with minimal training, a subject can respond appropriately to new stimuli. Such performances are of major adaptive value for an organism's survival.

18 304 FIELDS ET AL. Second, those stimuli must be discriminable from each other (Honig & Stewart, 1988; Wasserman et ai., 1988). The importance of the second requirement can be understood by considering the implications of not satisfying that requirement. Specifically, stimuli can be measurably different from each other but are not discriminable from each other (can not or do not occasion differential responding). When that occurs, the stimuli that differ physically, would be functioning as one, from a psychological point of view (Bhatt, Wasserman, Reynolds, & Knauss, 1988; Cook et ai., 1990; Fields et ai., 1997; Wasserman et ai., 1988). If the stimuli in a putative class were not discriminable from each other, it would be trivial to say that they were functioning as members of a conceptual class or category. Thus, the generalization of responding among such stimuli would not be of adaptive significance. A demonstration of discriminability among the stimuli in a putative class, then, is needed to conclude that the stimuli in a set are actually functioning as members of a class (Bruner et ai., 1956; Cook et ai., 1990; Fields et ai., 1997; Lea, 1984; Wasserman et ai., 1988). In Experiment 1, many contiguous variants occasioned the selection of the same comparison stimuli. Thus, those stimuli might have been functioning as members of open-ended classes. Experiment 1, however, did not provide measures of the discriminability of adjacent stimuli along the fill dimension. Experiment 2 determined whether many of adjacent fill patterns in each putative class were discriminable from each other. Method Subjects and Apparatus The subjects were the same as those who participated in Experiment 1. Experiment 2 was conducted immediately after Experiment 1. The apparatus, trial contingencies, and fill stimuli were the same as those used in Experiment 1. Procedure Discriminability of adjacent fill patterns was assessed in a procedure that involved five phases. In Phase 1, Fill values 23, 26, 29, 32, as well as 65,68,71,74, and 77 were used as samples along with Fi1l23, Fill77, and neither as comparisons. In Phase 2, Fill values 26, 29, 32, 38, as well as 62, 65, 68, 71, and 74 were used as samples along with Fi1l26, Fi1l74, and neither as comparisons. In Phase 3, Fill values 32, 35, 38, as well as 59, 62, 65 and 68 were used as samples along with Fi1l32, Fi1l68, and neither as comparisons. In Phase 4, Fill values 38, 41, 44, as well as 56, 59, and 62 were used as samples along with Fi1l38, Fi1l62, and neither as comparisons. In Phase 5, Fill values 44,47,50,53, and 56 were used as samples along with Fi1l44, Fi1l56, and neither as comparisons. In each phase, selection of a given fill comparison was reinforced only if it was identical to the sample. Selection of the neither comparison was reinforced only when the sample presented was not identical to

19 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 305 either of the comparison fill values. These contingencies maximized the discrimination between the intermediate fill values and the fill values at the endpoints of the dimension (Blough & Blough, 1977; Hamilton & Coleman, 1933; Reeve & Fields, 2001; Wright, 1972; Wright & Cumming, 1971). In each phase, the training block was repeated with 100% informative feedback until 100% correct responding was obtained. Feedback was then reduced as indicated in trial block structure and feedback contingencies described in Experiment 1. No informative feedback was presented for the last four blocks of each phase. Results and Discussion In Phase 1, there was substantial intersubject variation both in the accuracy of comparison selection during the first training block (30, 60, and 90%) and the number of blocks required to reach the mastery criterion (2, 3, and 25). Two subjects learned the conditional discriminations with relatively little training. For them, the reinforcement contingencies in Experiment 2 probably sharpened preexisting discriminations between adjacent fill patterns. The third subject (1707) required 25 blocks to master the conditional discriminations. In most blocks, the neither comparison was selected on all trials. In a few other blocks, the neither comparison was selected on most trials, with random selection of the fill-based comparisons on the remaining trials. Suddenly, correct comparison selections were occasioned by all of the trials on the training block. By the end of Phase 1, then, all subjects were responding differentially to adjacent stimuli along the fill dimension. Thus, both endpoint stimuli were fully discriminable from their adjacent fill variants. In Phases 2-5, comparison selection was 100% correct during the first training block for all subjects. These performances indicate that the endpoint variants were immediately and completely discriminated from the adjacent variants. Thus, the adjacent fill patterns must have been fully discriminable from each other IPlIrDIOIr ~IO the initiation of training in each of these phases. This pattern of responding is all the more interesting when one recognizes that at the end of each prior phase, those same stimuli occasioned the selection of the same comparison: the neither comparison, and also occasioned differential feedback for these comparison selections. The occurrence of correct responding from the inception of training implies that two stimulus control topographies had been acquired during Phase 1. First, subjects learned to discriminate the specific adjacent stimuli used in Phase 1. Second, subjects learned that endpoint and adjacent stimuli occasioned differential comparison selection, regardless of stimulus value (Sidman, Wynne, Maguire, & Barnes, 1989). The last four blocks run in Phases 1-5 of Experiment 2 were conducted under extinction conditions. Thus, the data collected under these conditions showed the maintenance of discriminability among stimuli when no feedback was provided for comparison selection. Table 3

20 306 FIELDS ET AL. Table 3 Generalization Test Data From Last Four Blocks of Each Phase in Experiment 2 Ends Phase Low High Comp Low NC High Low NC High Low NC High Low NC High Low NC High Sample Variants o Note. The last four blocks were conducted in the absence of differential feedback. Relative frequency of selection of the low- and high-fill comparison or the neither comparison (NC) in the presence of the sample variants is indicated at the top of the table. Data were averaged across subjects. shows the performances occasioned by each fill value used as a sample during the last four generalization test blocks presented at the end of each phase of Experiment 2. Because all subjects responded in essentially the same manner, the data in Table 2 represented performances averaged across subjects. In each phase of Experiment 2, subjects almost always selected a given endpoint comparison in the presence of the same stimulus presented as a sample. They rarely, if ever, selected the neither comparison or the fillbased comparison that was the other endpoint stimulus. In addition, all of the intermediate fill values used as samples almost always occasioned the selection of the neither comparison. These data show the maintenance of discriminability between adjacent fill values in the absence of reinforcement. Two criteria must be satisfied to conclude that stimuli are functioning as members of an open-ended class: common responding occasioned by many stimuli arrayed contiguously along some dimension, and discriminability among those stimuli. The results of Experiment 1 satisfied the first criterion by showing that the stimuli in a set occasioned the same response (conditional comparison selection). The results of Experiment 2 satisfied the second criterion by showing that many of the stimuli in a potential class were discriminable from each other. The performances observed in the initial blocks of each phase of Experiment 2 support the view that many of the fill variants that had been

21 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 307 used in Experiment 1 were discriminable from each other during Experiment 1. Thus, it seems plausible to conclude that discriminable variants were functioning as members of open-ended classes in Experiment 1 (Bourne, Dominowski, & Loftus, 1979; Cook et ai., 1990; Lea, 1984). General Discussion Procedures that induce open-ended classes. Typically, open-ended classes are established by use of multiple exemplar training with differential reinforcement (Cook et ai., 1990; Malott & Siddall, 1972; Markle, 1990; Wasserman et ai., 1988; Wright et ai., 1988). In a recent study, open-ended classes were established without the use of reinforcement (Reeve & Fields, 2001). Instead, Reeve induced classes by exposure to a forced choice generalization test conducted with no reinforcement. The same procedure was used to induce open-ended classes in the current experiment, thereby demonstrating the reliability of the effect reported by Reeve. Because a unique procedure was used to establish open-ended classes and class width was influenced by test format, it is possible that the test format effect might be limited to classes that are formed by forced choice generalization testing. Further research will be needed to determine whether the differential effects of test format would also be seen with classes induced by multiple exemplar training with reinforcement. Measurement of class width with different test formats. In the current experiment, the same nominal class had two different widths. Which class width would more accurately predict the variants that would occasion a response trained to one class member? One possibility is that the transfer would be predicted best by the broadest range of stimuli that function as a class. In this case, if class width had been measured with a test conducted in only one format and that format yielded the smaller class width, one would underestimate the range of stimuli to which the result would transfer. The other possibility is that transfer would be predicted by the narrowest range of stimuli that function as a class. In this case, if class width was measured with a test conducted in only one format and that format yielded the larger class width, one would overestimate the range of stimuli to which a response would transfer. In both cases, then, the predictive accuracy of function transfer would be compromised by the measurement of class width using tests conducted in only one format. According to this analysis, the predictive accuracy of function transfer should be optimized by the measurement of class width using tests conducted in both test formats. This analysis also has implications for using mutual selection as a basis for defining class membership. According to the notion of mutual selection, the width of an open-ended class would include the range of variants that occasioned the selection of base stimuli during variant-tobase tests and were also selected during base-to-variant tests. The utility of defining class width in terms of mutual selection, however, will depend

22 308 FIELDS ET AL. on the outcomes of function-transfer tests. If the range of variants to which a function transfers is predicted most accurately by the format that produces the narrower class width, mutual selection would be a desirable means of defining class width. If class width defined by mutual selection provides an accurate prediction of transfer for many different stimulus and response functions acquired by one class member, that width, then, might specify the "core" width of an open-ended class. Additional research, then, will clarify the utility of using mutual selection as a definitional property of stimuli in open-ended classes. References BELANICH, J. (2001). Generalized equivalence classes as response transfer networks. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Graduate Center of The City University of New York, New York. BHATT, R. S., WASSERMAN, E. A., REYNOLDS, W. F., Jr., & KNAUSS, K. S. (1988). Conceptual behavior in pigeons: Categorization of both familiar and novel examples from four classes of natural and artificiial stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 3, BLOUGH, D., & BLOUGH, P. (1977). Animal psychophysics. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp ). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. BOURNE, L. E., DOMINOWSKI, R. L., & LOFTUS, E. F. (1979). Cognitive Processes. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. BRUNER, J. S., GOODNOW, J. J., & AUSTIN, G. A. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley. CARRIGAN, P., & SIDMAN, M. (1992). Conditional discrimination and equivalence relations: A theoretical analysis of control by negative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, COOK, R. G., WRIGHT, A. A., & KENDRICK, D. F. (1990). Visual categorization by pigeons. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Herrnstein, S. Kosslyn, & D. Mumford (Eds.), Models of behavior: Behavioral approaches to pattern recognition and concept formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. CROLL, W. L. (1972). Generalization as a function of the range of test stimuli: The role of discriminability and response bias. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 13, DINSMOOR, J. A. (1985). The role of obsening and attention in establishing stimulus control. Joumal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, FIELDS, L., ADAMS, B. J., VERHAVE, T., & NEWMAN, S. (1990). The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, FIELDS, L., LANDON-JIMENEZ, D. V., BUFFINGTON, D. M., & ADAMS, B. J. (1995). Maintained nodal distance effects after equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, FIELDS, L., & REEVE, K. F. (2000). Synthesizing equivalence classes and natural categories from perceptual and relational classes. In J. C. Leslie & D. Blackman (Eds.), Issues in experimental and applied analyses of human behavior (pp ). Reno, NV: Context Press.

23 TEST FORMAT AND CLASS WIDTH 309 FIELDS, L., & REEVE, K. F. (2001). A methodological integration of generalized equivalence classes, natural categories, and cross modal perception. The Psychological Record, 51, FIELDS, L., REEVE, K. F., ADAMS, B. J., BROWN, J. L., & VERHAVE, T. (1997). Predicting the extension of equivalence classes from primary generalization gradients: The merger of equivalence classes and perceptual classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68, FIELDS, L., & VERHAVE, T. (1987). The structure of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, GALIZIO, M., & BARON, A. (1976). Label training and auditory generalization. Learning and Motivation, 7, GALIZIO, M., & BARON, A. (1979). Human postdiscrimination gradients: The effects of three-stimulus discrimination training. Animal Learning and Behavior, 7, GESCHEIDER, G. (1997). Psychophysics: The Fundamentals (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. GOLDIAMOND, I. (1962). Perception. In A. J. Bachrach (Ed.), Experimental foundations of clinical psychology (pp ). New York: Basic Books. GOLDIAMOND, I. (1966). Perception, language, and conceptualization rules. In B. Klienmuntz (Ed.), Problem solving (pp ). New York: Wiley. HAMILTON, W. F., & COLEMAN, T. B. (1933). Trichromatic vision in the pigeon as illustrated by the spectral hue discrimination curve. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 15, HANSEN, G., TOMIE, A., THOMAS, D. R., & THOMAS, D. H. (1974). Effect of test stimulus range on stimulus generalization in human subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, HERRNSTEIN, R. J. (1990). Levels of stimulus control: A functional approach. Cognition, 37, HOMA, D., & CHAMBLISS, D. (1975). The relative contributions of common and distinctive information on the abstraction from ill-defined categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, HOMA, D., & LITTLE, J. (1985). The abstraction and long-term retention of ill-defined categories by children. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, HONIG, W. K., & STEWART, K. E. (1988). Pigeons can discriminate locations presented in pictures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, HULL, C. L. (1920). Quantitative aspects of the evolution of concepts. Psychological Monographs, (Whole 123). INNIS, A., LANE, S. D., MILLER, E. R., & CRITCHFIELD, T. S. (1998). Stimulus equivalence: Effects of a default-response option on emergence of untrained stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 70, JITSUMORI, M. (1996). A prototype effect and categorization of artificial polymorphous stimuli in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, JOHNSON, C., & SIDMAN, M. (1993). Conditional discrimination and equivalence relations: Control by negative stimuli. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, KELLER, F. S., & SCHOENFELD, w. N. (1950). Principles of psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

24 310 FIELDS ET AL. LANE, S. D., CLOW, J. K., INNIS, A., & CRITCHFIELD, T. S. (1998). Generalization of cross-modal stimulus equivalence classes: Operant processes as components in human category formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 70, LEA, S. E. G. (1984). In what sense do pigeons learn concepts? In H. L. ROitblatt, T. G. Bever, & H. S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. MALOn, R., & SIDDALL, J. W. (1972). Acquisition of the people concept in the pigeon. Psychological Reports, 31,3-13. MARKLE, S. M. (1990). Designs for instructional designers. Champaign, IL: Stipes Publishing Company. MEDIN, D. L., & SMITH, E. E. (1984). Concepts and concept formation. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 35, REEVE, K. F., & FIELDS, L. (2001). Perceptual classes established with forcedchoice primary generalization tests and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76, ROSCH, E. H., & MERVIS, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, SIDMAN, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative, Inc. SIDMAN, M., & TAILBY, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, SIDMAN, M., WYNNE, C. K., MAGUIRE, R. w., & BARNES, T. (1989). Functional classes and equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, SMITH, E. A. (1989). Concepts and induction. In Michael I. Posner (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. STRIKELEATHER, G., & SIDMAN, M. (1990). An instance of spurious equivalence relations. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, STROMER, R. (1986). Control by exclusion in arbitrary matching-to-sample. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, STROMER, R., & MACKAY, H. A. (1997). Naming and the formation of stimulus classes. In T. R. Zentall & P. M. Smeets (Eds.), Stimulus class formation in humans and animals. Amsterdam, NL: North-Holland. STROMER, R., & OSBORNE, J. G. (1982). Control of adolescents' arbitrary matching-to-sample by positive and negative stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, THOMAS, D. R. (1993). A model for adaptation-level effects on stimulus generalization. Psychological Review, 100, THOMAS, D. R., & BISTEY, G. (1964). Stimulus generalization as a function of the number and range of generalization test stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, THOMAS, D. R., & JONES, C. G. (1962). Stimulus generalization as a function of the frame of reference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, THOMAS, D. R., LUSKY, M., & MORRISON, S. (1992). A comparison of generalization functions and frame of reference effects in different training paradigms. Perception and Psychophysics, 51, THOMAS, D. R., MOOD, K., MORRISON, S., & WIERTELAK, E. (1991). Peak shift revisited: A test of alternative interpretations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17,

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

ENHANCING EQUIVALENCE CLASS FORMATION BY PRETRAINING OF OTHER EQUIVALENCE CLASSES

ENHANCING EQUIVALENCE CLASS FORMATION BY PRETRAINING OF OTHER EQUIVALENCE CLASSES The Psychological Record, 1997, 47, 69-96 ENHANCING EQUIVALENCE CLASS FORMATION BY PRETRAINING OF OTHER EQUIVALENCE CLASSES DAWN M. BUFFINGTON 1,3, LANNY FIELDS 2, 3, and BARBARA J. ADAMS 1, 3 1 Queens

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES Journal of Experimental Vol. 51, No. 4, 1956 Psychology CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES KENNETH H. KURTZ AND CARL I. HOVLAND Under conditions where several concepts are learned concurrently

More information

The Analysis of Equivalence Classes

The Analysis of Equivalence Classes ARI Research Note 97-15 The Analysis of Equivalence Classes Lanny Fields Queens College of City University of New York Research and Advanced Concepts Office Michael Drillings, Chief June 1997 &'ak; i-

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? Keyed. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (1), 1 25 Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? J.H. Wearden, A. Parry, and L. Stamp University of

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

Carroll Collected. John Carroll University. Abdulrazaq Imam John Carroll University,

Carroll Collected. John Carroll University. Abdulrazaq Imam John Carroll University, John Carroll University Carroll Collected Psychology Psychology 2006 Experimental control of nodality via equal presentations of conditional discriminations in different equivalence protocols under speed

More information

CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION AND STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE: EFFECTS OF SUPPRESSING DERIVED SYMMETRICAL RESPONSES ON THE EMERGENCE OF TRANSITIVITY

CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION AND STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE: EFFECTS OF SUPPRESSING DERIVED SYMMETRICAL RESPONSES ON THE EMERGENCE OF TRANSITIVITY CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION AND STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE: EFFECTS OF SUPPRESSING DERIVED SYMMETRICAL RESPONSES ON THE EMERGENCE OF TRANSITIVITY Aaron A. Jones, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER

More information

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented 1 Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample A Thesis Presented by Kimberly M. Walter The Department of Counseling and Applied

More information

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS The Psychological Record, 2000, 50, 429-442 LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS LUIS A. PEREZ-GONZALEZ, JOSEPH E. SPRADLIN, and KATHRYN J. SAUNDERS University of Oviedo, Spain

More information

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee The Psychological Record, 1997,47,145-166 THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served

More information

Same/different discrimination learning with trial-unique stimuli

Same/different discrimination learning with trial-unique stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (3), 644-650 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.3.644 Same/different discrimination learning with trial-unique stimuli Daniel I. Brooks and Edward A. Wasserman University of Iowa,

More information

INTERACTIONS OF CONTINGENCIES, INSTRUCTIONAL ACCURACY, AND INSTRUCTIONAL HISTORY IN CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION

INTERACTIONS OF CONTINGENCIES, INSTRUCTIONAL ACCURACY, AND INSTRUCTIONAL HISTORY IN CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION The Psychological Record, 2005, 55, 633-646 INTERACTIONS OF CONTINGENCIES, INSTRUCTIONAL ACCURACY, AND INSTRUCTIONAL HISTORY IN CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION HECTOR MARTINEZ and RICARDO TAMAYO University

More information

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity Perception & Psychophysics 1980,28 (3), 249-253 Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity STEPHEN HANDEL and JAMES W. RHODES University oftennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37916 Subjects

More information

Effects of CARO on Stimulus Equivalence: A Systematic Replication

Effects of CARO on Stimulus Equivalence: A Systematic Replication John Carroll University Carroll Collected Psychology Psychology Effects of CARO on Stimulus Equivalence: A Systematic Replication Abdulrazaq Imam John Carroll University, aimam@jcu.edu J. V. Blanche Follow

More information

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 122 2001 Participating Faculty Professor James Dickson (dickson@stolaf.edu) Professor Dana Gross (grossd@stolaf.edu)

More information

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez The Psychological Record, 28, 58, 57 528 Some Effects of Magnitude of Reinforcement on Persistence of Responding Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman The University of Minnesota Angel Jimenez The University

More information

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1 Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen gert.haubensak@t-online.de Abstract Two experiments are reported in which the participants judged

More information

Stimulus Control And Generalization Of Point- Loss Punishment With Humans

Stimulus Control And Generalization Of Point- Loss Punishment With Humans See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/245606 Stimulus Control And Generalization Of Point- Loss Punishment With Humans Article July

More information

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge Kimery R. Levering (kleveri1@binghamton.edu) Kenneth J. Kurtz (kkurtz@binghamton.edu) Department of Psychology, Binghamton

More information

DOES STIMULUS COMPLEXITY AFFECT ACQUISITION OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS AND THE EMERGENCE OF DERIVED RELATIONS? Tiffani L. Martin, B.A., B.S.

DOES STIMULUS COMPLEXITY AFFECT ACQUISITION OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS AND THE EMERGENCE OF DERIVED RELATIONS? Tiffani L. Martin, B.A., B.S. DOES STIMULUS COMPLEXITY AFFECT ACQUISITION OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS AND THE EMERGENCE OF DERIVED RELATIONS? Tiffani L. Martin, B.A., B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects Journal of Eye Movement Research 2(5):3, 1-6 Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects Candido V. B. B. Pessôa Edson M. Huziwara Peter

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Discrimination blocking: Acquisition versus performance deficits in human contingency learning

Discrimination blocking: Acquisition versus performance deficits in human contingency learning Learning & Behavior 7, 35 (3), 149-162 Discrimination blocking: Acquisition versus performance deficits in human contingency learning Leyre Castro and Edward A. Wasserman University of Iowa, Iowa City,

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept Matthew D. Zeigenfuse (mzeigenf@msu.edu) Department of Psychology, Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48823 USA

More information

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1999, 72, 57 79 NUMBER 1(JULY) ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI

More information

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 J'OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 446-452 (1963) Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 A visual configuration, or pattern, has qualities over and above those which can be specified

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Erik Arntzen, Terje Grondahl, and Christoffer Eilifsen. Akershus University College

Erik Arntzen, Terje Grondahl, and Christoffer Eilifsen. Akershus University College The Psychological Record, 2010, 60, 437 462 THE EFFECTS OF DIFFERENT TRAINING STRUCTURES IN THE ESTABLISHMENT OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS AND SUBSEQUENT PERFORMANCE ON TESTS FOR STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE

More information

UNIVERSITY OF NEW MEXICO

UNIVERSITY OF NEW MEXICO JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2007, 88, 179 197 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) TRANSFORMATION OF THE DISCRIMINATIVE AND ELICITING FUNCTIONS OF GENERALIZED RELATIONAL STIMULI MICHAEL J. DOUGHER,

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Transfer of Dimensional Associability in Human Contingency Learning

Transfer of Dimensional Associability in Human Contingency Learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 42, No. 1, 15 31 2329-8456/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000082 Transfer of

More information

. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN

. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN 1 Roger K. Thomas The University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Unites States of America rkthomas@uga.edu Synonyms. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN Pre-publication manuscript version. Published as follows: Thomas,

More information

Bryan Roche. Simon Dymond

Bryan Roche. Simon Dymond The Psychological Record, 2008, 58, 71 90 A Transformation Of Functions In Accordance With The Nonarbitrary Relational Properties Of Sexual Stimuli Bryan Roche National University of Ireland, Maynooth

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Dimensional interaction in distance judgment

Dimensional interaction in distance judgment Perception, 2015, volume 44, pages 490 510 doi:10.1068/p7723 Dimensional interaction in distance judgment Stephen Dopkins, Darin Hoyer Psychology Department, The George Washington University, 2125 G Street,

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING George J. Andersen, Rui Ni Department of Psychology University of California Riverside Riverside, California, USA E-mail: Andersen@ucr.edu E-mail: ruini@ucr.edu

More information

EFFECTS OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION TRAINING ON SYMMETRY AND SEMANTIC PRIMING. Caleb D. Hudgins, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

EFFECTS OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION TRAINING ON SYMMETRY AND SEMANTIC PRIMING. Caleb D. Hudgins, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of EFFECTS OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION TRAINING ON SYMMETRY AND SEMANTIC PRIMING Caleb D. Hudgins, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS August 2011 APPROVED:

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 396 409 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.396

More information

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism A Thesis Presented by Hannorah O. Thurman The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment of the requirements

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning

Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning Paulo F. Carvalho (pcarvalh@indiana.edu) Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University 1101 East Tenth Street Bloomington,

More information

Statistical Methods and Reasoning for the Clinical Sciences

Statistical Methods and Reasoning for the Clinical Sciences Statistical Methods and Reasoning for the Clinical Sciences Evidence-Based Practice Eiki B. Satake, PhD Contents Preface Introduction to Evidence-Based Statistics: Philosophical Foundation and Preliminaries

More information

THE EFFECTS OF RESPONSE TOPOGRAPHY ON FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE CLASS FORMATION

THE EFFECTS OF RESPONSE TOPOGRAPHY ON FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE CLASS FORMATION The Psychological Record, 2001, 51, 89-110 THE EFFECTS OF RESPONSE TOPOGRAPHY ON FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE CLASS FORMATION ROBERT BONES, MICHAEL KEENAN, DARREN ASKIN, LOUISE ADAMS, DAVID TAYLOR, and ORLA

More information

Concurrent Identity Training is not Necessary for Associative Symmetry in Successive Matching

Concurrent Identity Training is not Necessary for Associative Symmetry in Successive Matching Purdue University Purdue e-pubs Department of Psychological Sciences Faculty Publications Department of Psychological Sciences 1-2014 Concurrent Identity Training is not Necessary for Associative Symmetry

More information

REACTION TIMES AND THE EMERGENCE OF CLASS CONSISTENT RESPONDING: A CASE FOR PRECURRENT RESPONDING?

REACTION TIMES AND THE EMERGENCE OF CLASS CONSISTENT RESPONDING: A CASE FOR PRECURRENT RESPONDING? The Psychological Record 2000 50 305-337 REACTON TMES AND THE EMERGENCE OF CLASS CONSSTENT RESPONDNG: A CASE FOR PRECURRENT RESPONDNG? PER HOLTH and ERK ARNTZEN University of Oslo Norway Three experiments

More information

ESTABLISHING FRACTION-DECIMAL EQUIVALENCE USING A RESPONDENT-TYPE TRAINING PROCEDURE.

ESTABLISHING FRACTION-DECIMAL EQUIVALENCE USING A RESPONDENT-TYPE TRAINING PROCEDURE. The Psychological Record, 2001, 51, 151-165 ESTABLISHING FRACTION-DECIMAL EQUIVALENCE USING A RESPONDENT-TYPE TRAINING PROCEDURE. GERALDINE LEADER University of Ulster at Jordanstown DERMOT BARNES-HOLMES

More information

Analysis of the Role of Associative Inhibition in Perceptual Learning by Means of the Same Different Task

Analysis of the Role of Associative Inhibition in Perceptual Learning by Means of the Same Different Task Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2008, Vol. 34, No. 4, 475 485 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.34.4.475

More information

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons Entire Set of Printable Figures For Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons Cook In one experiment, Cerella (1980) trained pigeons to discriminate intact drawings of Charlie Brown from normal drawings

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

The Influence of Perceptual Difficulty on Family Resemblance Sorting

The Influence of Perceptual Difficulty on Family Resemblance Sorting The Influence of Perceptual Difficulty on Family Resemblance Sorting Fraser Milton (f.n.milton@exeter.ac.uk) School of Psychology, University of Exeter, United Kingdom A.J. Wills (a.j.wills@exeter.ac.uk)

More information

PROBABILITY OF EQUIVALENCE FORMATION: FAMILIAR STIMULI AND TRAINING SEQUENCE. ERIK ARNTZEN Akershus University College

PROBABILITY OF EQUIVALENCE FORMATION: FAMILIAR STIMULI AND TRAINING SEQUENCE. ERIK ARNTZEN Akershus University College The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 275-291 PROBABILITY OF EQUIVALENCE FORMATION: FAMILIAR STIMULI AND TRAINING SEQUENCE ERIK ARNTZEN Akershus University College The present study was conducted to show

More information

Single-Subject Withdrawal Designs in Delayed Matching-to-Sample Procedures

Single-Subject Withdrawal Designs in Delayed Matching-to-Sample Procedures EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2011, 12, 157-172 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2011) 157 Single-Subject Withdrawal Designs in Delayed Matching-to-Sample Procedures Akershus University College In most studies

More information

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 2, 434 445 2009 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014529 Influence of Visual Stimulus

More information

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli.

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli. Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli. Harry A. Mackay, Brooks Thompson, & William J. McIlvane

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

INCREASING THE PROBABILITY OF STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE WITH ADULTS WITH MILD MENTAL RETARDATION

INCREASING THE PROBABILITY OF STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE WITH ADULTS WITH MILD MENTAL RETARDATION The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 423-435 INCREASING THE PROBABILITY OF STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE WITH ADULTS WITH MILD MENTAL RETARDATION RICHARD R. SAUNDERS and JULIE E. MCENTEE University of Kansas In

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form Supervisor: Date first assessed: Individual being Supervised: Certification being sought: Instructions: Please mark each item with either a 0,1,2 or 3 based on rating scale Rating Scale: 0 - cannot identify

More information

EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK

EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Stanislaus In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented 1 Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present A Thesis Presented by Lindsey M. Ryan The Department of Counseling and Applied

More information

The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016

The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016 The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016 Jose Martinez-Diaz, Ph.D., BCBA-D FIT School of Behavior Analysis and ABA Technologies, Inc.

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Analyzing Distances in a Frontal Plane. Stephen Dopkins. The George Washington University. Jesse Sargent. Francis Marion University

Analyzing Distances in a Frontal Plane. Stephen Dopkins. The George Washington University. Jesse Sargent. Francis Marion University Analyzing Distances in a Frontal Plane Stephen Dopkins The George Washington University Jesse Sargent Francis Marion University Please address correspondence to: Stephen Dopkins Psychology Department 2125

More information

Masaki Ishizuka, Tetsumi Moriyama. Tokiwa University, Mito, Japan

Masaki Ishizuka, Tetsumi Moriyama. Tokiwa University, Mito, Japan Psychology Research, September 2018, Vol. 8, No. 9, 411-434 doi:10.17265/2159-5542/2018.09.001 D DAVID PUBLISHING Symmetry Formation by Undergraduates in Successive Matching: A Re-evaluation of Urcuioli

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

Location and Salience of Unique Features in Human Perceptual Learning

Location and Salience of Unique Features in Human Perceptual Learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2012, Vol. 38, No. 4, 407 418 2012 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0029733 Location and Salience of Unique

More information

The Color of Similarity

The Color of Similarity The Color of Similarity Brooke O. Breaux (bfo1493@louisiana.edu) Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, LA 70504 USA Michele I. Feist (feist@louisiana.edu) Institute

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism A Thesis Presented by Melissa A. Ezold The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment

More information

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment

More information

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1996, 3 (2), 222 226 Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana and THOMAS J. PALMERI Vanderbilt University,

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

DERIVED CONDITIONAL POSITION DISCRIMINATION IN INDIVIDUALS WITHOUT NAMI NG SKILLS

DERIVED CONDITIONAL POSITION DISCRIMINATION IN INDIVIDUALS WITHOUT NAMI NG SKILLS The Psychological Record, 2006, 56, 287-301 DERIVED CONDITIONAL POSITION DISCRIMINATION IN INDIVIDUALS WITHOUT NAMI NG SKILLS KATHRYN J. SAUNDERS, JENNIFER O'DONNELL, DEAN C. WILLIAMS, and JOSEPH E. SPRADLIN

More information

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory Alejandro Lleras, 1 Ronald A. Rensink, 2 and James T. Enns

More information

Abstract-Concept Learning Carryover Effects From the Initial Training Set in Pigeons (Columba livia)

Abstract-Concept Learning Carryover Effects From the Initial Training Set in Pigeons (Columba livia) Journal of Comparative Psychology 29 American Psychological Association 29, Vol. 123, No. 1, 79 89 735-36/9/$12. DOI: 1.137/a13126 Abstract-Concept Learning Carryover Effects From the Initial Training

More information

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments Aniket Kittur (nkittur@ucla.edu) Keith J. Holyoak (holyoak@lifesci.ucla.edu) Department of Psychology, University

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

Conceptual Behavior in Pigeons: Categorization of Both Familiar and Novel Examples From Four Classes of Natural and Artificial Stimuli

Conceptual Behavior in Pigeons: Categorization of Both Familiar and Novel Examples From Four Classes of Natural and Artificial Stimuli Journal of xperimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1988, Vol. 14, No. 3, 219-234 Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/8S/$00.75 Conceptual Behavior in Pigeons:

More information

Decline of the McCollough effect by orientation-specific post-adaptation exposure to achromatic gratings

Decline of the McCollough effect by orientation-specific post-adaptation exposure to achromatic gratings *Manuscript Click here to view linked References Decline of the McCollough effect by orientation-specific post-adaptation exposure to achromatic gratings J. Bulthé, H. Op de Beeck Laboratory of Biological

More information

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2008, 41, 125 130 NUMBER 1(SPRING 2008) A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER LOUISIANA STATE UNIVERSITY

More information

CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS. DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton

CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS. DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton The Psychological Record, 2001, 51, 549-560 CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton College students repeatedly chose between

More information

The effect of time pressure and the spatial integration of the stimulus dimensions on overall similarity categorization.

The effect of time pressure and the spatial integration of the stimulus dimensions on overall similarity categorization. The effect of time pressure and the spatial integration of the stimulus dimensions on overall similarity categorization. Fraser Milton (f.n.milton@ex.ac.uk) Lotta Viika (lv222@ex.ac.uk) Holly Henderson

More information