Modelling the effects of stress on gap-acceptance decisions in a driving simulator experiment using physiological sensors

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Modelling the effects of stress on gap-acceptance decisions in a driving simulator experiment using physiological sensors"

Transcription

1 Modelling the effects of stress on gap-acceptance decisions in a driving simulator experiment using physiological sensors Evangelos Paschalidis 1, Charisma F. Choudhury 2, Stephane Hess 3 1,2,3 Choice Modelling Centre, Institute for Transport Studies, University of Leeds, University Rd, Leeds LS2 9JT 1 tsep@leeds.ac.uk, 2 C.F.Choudhury@leeds.ac.uk, 3 s.hess@its.leeds.ac.uk Abstract Driving behaviour is an inherently complex process affected by various factors ranging from network topography, traffic conditions and vehicle features to driver characteristics like age, experience, aggressiveness and emotional state. The effect of emotional state and stress has received considerable attention in the context of crash analysis and safety research where driving behaviour has been found to be affected by drivers mental state/stress, cognitive workload and distraction. However, these studies are mostly based on questionnaire surveys and self-reports which can be prone to response bias and reporting/measurement errors. On a parallel stream of research, advances in sensor technologies have made it possible to observe drivers stress through human physiological responses, e.g. heart rate, electro-dermal activity etc. However, these studies have been primarily focused on detecting stress rather than quantifying or modelling its effects on driving decisions. The present paper combines these two approaches in a single framework and investigates the gap-acceptance behaviour of drivers during an intersection crossing task with data collected using a driving simulator. The participants were subjected to stress, induced by time pressure, and their stress levels were measured using physiological indicators namely, skin conductance and heart rate. In addition to statistical analyses, discrete choice models were developed to build mathematical relationships between the accept-reject choices and the driver demographics, traffic conditions and stress levels. The results of the models indicated significant impacts of stress levels leading to increased probabilities of accepting a gap. The improvement in model fit and safety implications derived from model estimates are also discussed. The insights from the results can be used for designing appropriate intervention strategies. Keywords: gap-acceptance, driving stress, driving simulator, heart rate, skin conductance 1. Introduction Road safety continues to be an important issue with road crashes among the leading causes of death - accounting for more than 1.2 million fatalities and 50 million injuries globally each year. Driving behaviour is a factor in over 90% of crashes, with speeding as one of the major contributors (World Health Organization, 2015). Driving behaviour models, which provide mathematical representations of drivers decisions involving acceleration-deceleration, lane-changing, overtaking, etc., are increasingly being used for evaluation and prediction of road safety parameters and formulating remedial measures. Reliable driving behaviour models are also critical for accurate prediction of congestion

2 levels in microscopic traffic simulation tools (Barceló, 2015) and analyses of emissions (Ahn et al., 2002). Driving decisions are affected by various factors, including network topography, traffic conditions, and driver characteristics - which include, among others, demographics, personality traits and emotional state. Existing driving behaviour models address many of these factors, either fully or partially, where the effects of surrounding traffic conditions have received considerable attention (Ossen & Hoogendoorn, 2005; Toledo, 2007; Choudhury, 2007; Marczak et al., 2013 to name a few). However, in most cases, the models do not adequately capture the sophistication of driver behaviour and the causal mechanism behind their observed decisions. In particular, research in other realms, in the context of crash analysis and safety research, has confirmed that driving behaviour is significantly affected by drivers mental state/mood (e.g. anger) (Garrity & Demick, 2001), cognitive workload (Hoogendoorn et al., 2010), distraction (Young et al., 2003) and fatigue (Thiffault & Bergeron, 2003). Previous literature on drivers stress, has mainly focused on the investigation of the relationship between stress and aberrant behaviour and its impact on safety (Ge et al., 2014; Westerman & Haigney, 2000; Hill & Boyle, 2007). However, these studies primarily examined the effects of stress based on self-reported surveys which can be prone to response bias and reporting/measurement errors. On a parallel stream of research, recent advances in sensor technologies have however made it possible to measure drivers stress levels through human physiological responses, e.g. changes in heart rate, electro-dermal activity etc. (Healey & Picard, 2005; Ahmed et al., 2015). However, these studies have been primarily focused on detecting stress rather than quantifying or modelling its effects on driving decisions in detail. This paper aims to fill in this research gap by developing gap acceptance models with explicit consideration of the effect of stress on driving behaviour. The gap-acceptance models developed in this research are based on an extensive experimental study in the University of Leeds Driving Simulator (UoLDS) where the drivers were intentionally subjected to stressful driving conditions caused by time pressure and surrounding traffic conditions. Their choices of accepted gaps were recorded alongside physiological measurements of stress indicators (skin conductance and heart rate), self-reported stress levels, traits (e.g. risk-taking propensity) and observed characteristics (age, gender, experience). A series of gap acceptance models was developed and augmented by continuous physiological measurements. The rest of the paper is organised as follows: next section presents a review of the literature. The section after that describes the methodological approach of the study. This is followed by the experimental setting and the data analyses. The estimation results are presented next followed by concluding remarks where insights from the models are discussed. 2. Literature review 2.1. Stress and driving context Driver stress has been defined as a situation that challenges drivers abilities, reduces their perceived control or threatens their mental/physical health (Gulian et al., 1989). Driver stress may arise as a consequence of several factors including the direct demands of the

3 driving task, the environmental conditions (e.g. fog occurrence), network characteristics (e.g. surface characteristics), junction frequency, speed and flow per lane and/or potential secondary tasks (e.g. use of navigation system, texting etc.) (Hill & Boyle, 2007). Moreover, time urgency and the level of congestion have been identified as two important factors influencing drivers stress (Hennessy & Wiesenthal, 1999). There is a substantial body of literature that investigates the effects of stress on driving behaviour. Drivers under stress may be overwhelmed by negative emotions and thus are more likely to get involved in hazardous situations (Ge et al., 2014). Self-reported stress has been linked to aberrant driving behaviour, namely errors and violations (Kontogiannis, 2006). These types of impaired behaviour are related to road crashed and incidents, therefore stress is considered as an issue related to traffic safety (Westerman & Haigney, 2000; Useche et al., 2015, Qu et al., 2014). Moreover, Ge et al. (2014) found that perceived stress is linked to aggressive and risky driving behaviour. Also, Clapp et al. (2011), grouped reactions under stressful situations in three main categories which are the extremely cautious driving behaviour, aberrant behaviour and aggressive (or hostile) behaviour. The aforementioned findings provide compelling evidence regarding the effects of stress on driving, however, they are based on self-reported survey results and therefore can be prone to response bias and reporting/measurement errors. An alternative, and potentially more reliable, approach to detect drivers level of stress and study its effects, is through its implications on human physiology. Recent advances in sensor technologies and affective computing have made it possible to measure drivers stress levels through physiological responses, e.g. changes in heart rate, skin conductance, blood volume pulse etc. There are several existing studies, related to driving stress, that use this type of data to detect stress (some examples Healey & Picard, 2005; Singh & Queyam, 2013; Rigas et al., 2012). However, the aforementioned studies mostly focused on detecting stress rather than investigating its effects on observed driving behaviour. Two of the most widespread physiological indicators - also used in the present study - are heart rate and skin conductance. Heart rate represents the observed heartbeats per minute. Lower heart rate is generally linked to a relaxed state while it increases under the presence of emotional stimuli or mental effort (Katsis et al., 2011). Skin conductance is related to the sweat gland activity and it is an indicator that increases or decreases proportionally to stress effort (Katsis et al., 2011). Skin conductance is composed of two different parts, namely the skin conductance level (SCL tonic part) and skin conductance response (SCR phasic part). While SCL is slow varying and related to individual characteristics, SCRs are expressed as a sudden and fast increase of skin conductance owing to the presence of a specific stimulus and thus have been linked to acute stress. Skin conductance responses are identified if the increase in skin conductance activity exceeds specific critical values. 2.2 Gap-acceptance models Driving behaviour models primarily include car-following, lane-change and gap-acceptance (Toledo, 2007). The latter of the aforementioned concepts focuses on two different aspects; the decision of drivers to change lane and the attempt of a turning or crossing manoeuvre at an intersection. In literature, several methodological approaches

4 have been developed in order predict the intersection crossing decisions of drivers. This type of gap acceptance behaviour is of prime importance when studying issues as network capacity, delays and rοad safety (Ashton, 1971; Fitzpatrick, 1991). The majority of these methodologies is based on the critical gap concept, which is defined as the minimum time gap in the priority stream which a driver moving on the minor road is willing to accept in order to cross through the conflict zone. According to Brilon et al., (1999), there are at least different methods related to gap-acceptance decisions. Some of the most cited are namely the Raff method (Raff & Hart, 1950), the Greenshields method (Greenshields et al., 1946), the lag method (see Brilon et al., 1999), the logit method (Maze, 1981) - which is a method based on traditional choice modelling techniques (see Ben-Akiva & Lerman, 1985) - the Ashworth s method (Ashworth, 1969), and the maximum likelihood method (Miller & Pretty, 1968). The main limitations regarding some of the existing methodologies, are two assumptions regarding driving behaviour namely, consistency and homogeneity (Bottom & Ashworth, 1978; Pollatschek et al., 2002). The first of these two assumptions indicates that each driver has a specific critical gap threshold and accepts all observed gaps with a value greater than this. This type of behaviour is characterised as consistent. The assumption of consistency denotes that gap-acceptance decisions are independent with respect to each available gap, thus it ignores the within-driver variability as each driver is expected to always behave in a specific way. The assumptions of gap-acceptance methodologies raise limitations in the representation of drivers behaviour since they both ignore their sophisticated decision-making process. For instance, critical gap varies among and within drivers, in different situations, and should be treated as a random variable (Guo et al., 2014). The drawbacks imposed by these assumptions have been discussed already in some studies and several methodologies have been developed to address them. Ashworth (1969) used statistical distributions to express critical gap while Pollatschek et al. (2002) developed a gap-acceptance approach to account for individual preferences and different groups of drivers (cautious or risk-takers). Moreover, in logit methodology, a behaviour cannot be considered as inconsistent if an explanatory variable is able to capture and explain it (Amin & Maurya, 2015). Despite the advances in gap-acceptance methodological approaches, not all variables related to driving behaviour have been investigated. Some of the aspects which are not yet addressed include drivers strategies when deciding to cross an intersection or not, the motivation behind an observed inconsistent behaviour and finally the effects of individual traits and characteristics (e.g. socio-demographics, personality, attitudes, state of mind, level of stress etc.). The aim of the present study is to provide an extended gap-acceptance framework, through the development of a model that accounts for variables related to driver s individual characteristics, with explicit consideration of drivers acute stress levels, and contribute to filling in this gap of driving behaviour modelling research. 3. Methodology 3.1 The gap acceptance model The gap-acceptance approach of the current paper was formulated as a binary choice model, where each gap was considered as a different accept/reject decision. This approach is a modification of the logit method mentioned in the previous section. The model

5 assumes that a driver will accept a specific gap if the utility for this decision is higher, compared to the utility of rejecting it. For model identification purposes (see e.g. Hensher et al., 2005), the utility of rejecting a gap is given a fixed value of zero and only the utility of gap-acceptance probability is estimated, with respect to that fixed value (e.g. a positive estimated utility would imply a higher probability of gap acceptance while a negative would indicate the opposite). The gap-acceptance behaviour is then defined as: GA nt = { 1 if U nt GA > 0 0 if U GA nt 0 where n and t indicate an individual driver and a specific available gap respectively. Also, GA U nt represents the utility of the driver to accept a gap. The utility of gap-acceptance is not observed therefore it is modelled as a random variable which is a function of explanatory variables, as shown in Equation 1: U GA nt = β nt X nt + γ n Z n + aν n + ε nt (1) where X nt is a vector of gap-specific variables and Z n are individual-specific variables (e.g. socio-demographics). Moreover, ν n represents a standard normal disturbance term which is used to capture the effect of unobserved variables and ε nt is the Gumbell distributed disturbance term used in choice models. Finally, β nt, γ n and a are vectors of parameters to be estimated. Following the aforementioned assumptions, the probability of gap-acceptance is defined as: P GA nt = e (U nt GA ) 1 + e (U nt GA ) (2) If the observed choice of a driver to accept a gap is set as Y nt =1, the conditional full probability of an observed drivers decision can be expressed, as shown in Equation 3: P nt = (P GA nt ν) Y nt(1 P GA nt ν) 1 Y nt (3) The conditional probability of a sequence of T observed decisions of the same driver takes the form indicated by Equation 4: T P n ν = (P nt ν) t=1 (4) As explained above, it is assumed that νn is standard normally distributed. Thus, the unconditional joint probability of the observations of a given driver is given by Εquation 5 as:

6 + P n = (P n ν)φ( ν)dν (5) where a φ(ν) is the probability density function of the individual specific disturbance term. Given that the integral in Equation 5 is indefinite, it was approximated through simulation using 1000 Halton draws Physiological data analysis The model described in the previous section, was augmented by continuous physiological measurements. These observations were used as direct explanatory variables, in order to investigate whether the gap-acceptance model would be more behaviourally representative, when drivers stress would be included. Two different responses were considered, namely, heart rate and skin conductance. The physiological variables were initially processed and transformed before their incorporation in the model. Instead of using the raw observations, the heart rate data was normalised at individual level, applying a z-score transformation ( x μ ), where x is a heart σ rate observation, μ is the heart rate mean value across the whole urban task and σ is its standard deviation (Picard et al., 2001; Maaoui & Pruski, 2010). The skin conductance observations were processed using using the Matlab package Ledalab (Karenbach, 2005). The responses were obtained applying trough-to-peak analysis and their amplitude was considered as explanatory variable in the models. The skin conductance analysis was based on event-related response activation; each gap was considered as a different stimulus. The initiation of each gap was used as a starting point and responses were detected 1-4s after that moment. Moreover, since all variables used in the models refer to the initial moment of a gap (as defined in the next section), and given that SCRs detection works forward, the SCRs activity during the previous gap was considered as an explanatory variable of a current one. An example of SCRs analysis is illustrated in Figure 1. Following literature indications (e.g. Sano et al., 2014), a critical value equal to 0.01μS was selected as a minimum critical SCRs. Moreover, each significant amplitude (above 0.01μS) was divided by the maximum observed SCR amplitude, during the simulator experiment, to minimise the effects of individual differences (Lykken, 1972). Figure 1: Example of SCRs extraction

7 4. Data collection 4.1 Driving simulator experiment The data used in this research was based on primary data collected as part of a comprehensive driving simulator study (Next Generation Driving Behaviour Models NG-DBM) for investigating the effect of stress in different driving decisions (e.g. acceleration-deceleration, overtaking, red light violation, gap acceptance, etc.). The experiments were conducted using the University of Leeds Driving Simulator (UoLDS). The UoLDS (Figure 2) is a high fidelity, dynamic simulator, with all driver controls available and fully operational, such as steering wheel and breaking pedal, while there is also a fully operational dashboard. The vehicle is placed in a 4m diameter spherical projection dome. The dome provides fully textured 3-D graphical scene with a horizontal field of view of 250 o and 45 o vertical. The raw data output consits of observations of 60Hz frequency. It may be noted that though driving simulator data has the risk to be prone to behavioural incongruence, it offers the flexibility to fully control the surrounding traffic and driving contexts (e.g. inducing time pressure and stressful scenarios) which are crucial for this particular study. Figure 2: The University of Leeds Driving Simulator [sources: University of Leeds, University of Leeds Driving Simulator] The full data collection process involved around 90 minutes of total driving in the simulator. Participants initially had a short briefing session about the simulator and its operation followed by a practice session of approximately 15 minutes duration to get familiarised with the simulated environment and vehicle dynamics (i.e. motion system). For safety reasons, participants were accompanied by a researcher, during the practice run, in the back seat. After the practice session, participants staretd the main driving sessions, composed by two different environments, an urban and a motorway, with a short break in between. At the end of each driving session, participants answered a brief questionnaire related to the events they encountered and their perceived stress levels. During the various driving scenarios, stress was induced through a series of tasks/events (e.g. unpredictable behaviour of neighbouring vehicles, amber dilemma, slow moving leader, gap-acceptance tasks etc.) and driving under time pressure. The majority of the scenarios was examined both without and under the presence of time pressure. Before each of the two main tasks, participants were instructed that they had to complete them within 35 minutes without however being accurately informed about the remaining time; they could only see a face image, placed on the dashboard (Figure 3). The green and amber states would indicate that the driver was still on time while the red was interpreted as being late to complete the task. Participants were informed that, during the whole driving task, a

8 sophisticated algorithm was running in the background evaluating variables as current speed, speed limit, distance to the end, an average estimated delay that will be caused by the events ahead etc. and printed, as an outcome, one of the three face images, which was related to their performance. However, the state of the time pressure faces was not related to their actual performance but was pre-decided in order to induce additional pressure in specific road segments. The choice of 3 different faces to indicate time pressure, was preferred to a conventional countdown timer since it would be easier to manipulate. In order to increase the likelihood that participants would consider time pressure indications, they were instructed that a penalty would be imposed in their monetary reward, for their particpation, in case they were late at the end of a scenario (red face). However, that was never the case since both main scenarios were programmed to end during and amber time pressure state. Figure 3: Time pressure indications Drivers physiological data, across the whole experiment, was collected using the Empatica E4 wristband which is a non-intrusive device that provides information about heart rate (HR), skin conductance (SC), blood volume pulse (BVP) and temperature (TEMP). Each of the physiological indicators was collected with a different frequency, depending on the attributes of the wristband. Skin conductance and temperature have 4Hz frequency, blood volume pulse 64Hz and hear rate 1Hz. Apart from the observed driving simulator and physiological data, participants also completed a series of surveys including sociodemographic characteristics, personality scales derived from the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP) (Goldberg, 1999), the multidimensional driving style inventory (MDSI) (Taubman-Ben-Ari, 2004) and some supplementary questions related to the driving scenarios. 4.2 The gap-acceptance task In the present study, the gap-acceptance task was shown twice, as a part of the urban driving scenario. Drivers faced the first gap-acceptance task without time pressure while there was time pressure (red face) during the second one. The scenario itself consisted of two groups of vehicles. At first, six blocking vehicles were shown to participants, moving at short headway distances. These vehicles were used to force drivers to stop before the main gap acceptance task. This first group of vehicles was followed by eleven vehicles that created 10 gaps. The gaps had an increasing trend, however, shorter gaps were also introduced in between, while they were identical for both intersections and across participants. For each gap-acceptance task, the drivers could choose to accept the available gap and cross or even reject them all. However, there was not a priori knowledge regarding the number of the oncoming vehicles or the waiting time required. It should be mentioned that for purposes of simplicity it was decided to constrain the gap-acceptance scenario by

9 developing a case where cars were shown only coming from the left side of the driver. Moreover, it is worth highlighting that although there might be learning or other effects (e.g. fatigue, impatience) in drivers behaviour, time pressure was always applied at the second intersection. The main reason for this design was related to drivers physiology, since it was attempted to minimise the risk of increasing their responses at the beginning of the driving task by inducing additional stressors (e.g. time pressure), that would potentially influence and prevent them from returning to the baseline levels. Also, it would be more realistic for the participants to receive a red face indication closer to the end of the driving task, rather than during the first part. A general outline of the gap-acceptance scenario setting is illustrated in Figure 4. Moreover, the presented gap sizes are presented in Table 1. The increasing trend of gaps was chosen in order to secure that drivers would not face a large gap in the beginning of the scenario and miss information related to their willingness to accept a shorter one. Figure 4: Illustration of the intersection Table 1: The available gaps and gaps sizes Gap ID Gap size (s) Explanatory analysis 5.1 Sample analysis The sample of the current analysis consisted of 41 participants (22 male, 19 female), staff members or students at the University of Leeds, that successfully completed the urban task. The mean age of participants was approximately 34 years, with 11 years standard deviation. Almost half of the participants stated that they were driving on a daily basis. The average driving experience of participants was almost 14 years. Regarding accident involvement, 6 particiants reported involvement in minor accidents while 4 reported engagement in serious accidents. It is worth mentioning that as a serious (or major) accident was defined an incident where at least one person required medical treatment and/or there was property damage above 500. Finally, 7 participants stated a ticket penalty for speeding behaviour. The descriptive statistics of the sample are also outlined in Table Gap-acceptance task analysis Before the development of the model, participants gap-acceptance behaviour was

10 examined with respect to the effects of time pressure. In Table 3 are presented the accepted gaps of each individual, for the two cases, and their respective size (a value 999 is given if no gap was accepted). A similar illustration is also provided in Figure 5. It should be mentioned that 12 out of 41 participants did not accept any of the presented to them gaps in both cases. On the other hand, six participants accepted a gap only under the time pressure conditions while they had not done it without time pressure. The remaining 23 participants accepted a gap at both intersections. The latter group of participants always accepted the same or a gap that was shown earlier, compared to the one accepted without the external stressor. To further investigate this outcome, a paired samples t-test was applied in order to compare the significance in the difference of the accepted gaps sizes at the two intersections. The results (Table 4) indicated that the mean size of the accepted gaps was statistically significant smaller at the second intersection. As mentioned in the data collection section, this decision might be triggered by learning effects however, between the two intersections, participants faced a series of additional tasks and they did at least 15min of driving. These extra tasks, could have a diminishing influence on the learning effects, increasing the likelihood observing drivers actions driven by time pressure. Table 2: Descriptive statistics of the sample Variable Intervals Frequency % mean std. dev. min max Gender Female Male Age Driving experience Frequency of driving Minor accident involvement Major accident involvement Ticket for speeding Everyday times/week Once/ week Less often No Yes No Yes No Yes Furthermore, with reference to Table 3, under the time pressure conditions, three of the participants accepted the first gap. These drivers, did not actually behave as expected during the task (stop at the intersection and wait for a gap, or not, to cross) but they drove through the streaming of oncoming vehicles without stopping. This behaviour, might seem unrealistic or irrational for the real-life driving context however, it might also consist and indication that external stressors could increase risk-taking (even such extreme behaviour would not be observed in real life). Moreover, it is worth mentioning that the 10 th gap was never accepted although it was the largest one in terms of headway size. This behaviour may indicate anticipation effects in gap-acceptance behaviour; drivers that waited until the last available gap preferred to wait additional time and cross when the intersection was clear rather than engaging in crossing under the presence of oncoming traffic. As mentioned above, almost 1/3 of participants showed this behaviour, without being influenced by time pressure in the second task.

11 Table 3: Accepted gap(s) of each participant at the two intersections ID First intersection (without time pressure) Second intersection (under time pressure) Gap ID Gap size (s) Gap ID Gap size (s) ID First intersection (without time pressure) Second intersection (under time pressure) Gap ID Gap size (s) Gap ID Gap size (s)

12 Gap size (s) Gap ID 12 Accepted gap Driver ID No time pressure Time pressure Size of accepted gap (s) Driver ID No time pressure Time pressure Figure 5: Accepted gaps and sizes without and under time pressure

13 First intersection (no time pressure) Second intersection (under time pressure) Descriptives Table 4: Results of the paired samples t-test Paired samples t-test 95% CI for Mean SD SE Mean Mean SE t df p Difference Difference Difference Lower Upper Gap-acceptance model 6.1 Parameter estimation results and interpretation A series of gap-acceptance models was estimated based on the methodology presented in section 3.1. The initial model included only traffic related variables, while more variables were gradually added. Those additional variables were grouped in socio-demographics, time pressure and stress variables. Each new model included all the previous variables plus new ones. The aim of this approach was to compare model fit and investigate whether the addition of specific variables could actually improve the model. The results of all four models are presented in Table 5. All parameter estimates are significant at 95% level ( t-ratio >1.96). Variable Table 5: Gap-acceptance models parameter estimates Traffic related variables only model Model 1 (Base) Socio-demographics included model (Model 2) Time pressure included model (Model 3) Physiological observations included model Model 4 (Final) estimate t-ratio estimate t-ratio estimate t-ratio estimate t-ratio Constant Gap size Last gap dummy Speed (first gap) Position α acc Age dummy Frequency of driving dummy Time pressure dummy Skin conductance response Heart rate LL LL R adjusted R observations The first model included as explanatory variables the gap size (in seconds), vehicle position during waiting time, vehicle speed when arriving at the intersection area, a dummy variable indicating whether there is another gap following, or not, and the standard normal

14 disturbance term (Model 1). As expected, gap size had a positive effect on gap acceptance behaviour; drivers sensitivity in accepting a gap increases with its size. Vehicle position is a variable that captures participant s position at the intersection area (starting from a value of zero) with an increasing trend as the vehicle moves forward. If a participant was outside of the intersection area during the task (was a case for some participants during the first shown gap), the variable could also take negative values. The inclusion of this variable attempted to capture drivers behaviour to better position themselves and increase the likelihood of accepting the next available gap. This variable was considered in the model since, during the data collection, it was observed that a proportion of drivers was slowly moving the car forward during the waiting time period. As expected, the effect of this variable was positively related to the gap-acceptance probability and drivers were more likely to accept a gap, the closer to the intersection the vehicle was. Another variable of this model is vehicle speed, which was considered only for the first gap of each intersection, and ignored for all the rest. It s a case specific variable used to capture the behaviour of the three participants that during the time pressure period did not stop at the junction, as expected, but slightly modified their speed and accepted the first gap. The model was initially estimated without trying to capture this abnormal behaviour, however, when vehicle speed was included, model fit was significantly improved and thus the variable was considered. Finally, the dummy variable of the next gap has a negative effect which implies that drivers gap-acceptance decisions are not shortsighted and focused on the current gap only but there are anticipation effects and drivers further consider the next available gaps before deciding whether to cross or not. This outcome was expected, considering the relative discussion presented in the explanatory analysis section. A summary of all dummy variables used in the various models and the respective coding is presented in Table 6. Table 6: Dummy variables coding Dummy variable Value 0 1 Last gap No Yes Age Below or 45 years old Above 45 years old Driving frequency Not everyday Everyday Time pressure conditions No Yes The first gap-acceptance model was further updated and variables related to participants sociodemographic characteristics were also included (Model 2). Among the several sociodemographic variables tested, those with a significant effect were age and driving frequency. Both were used as dummy variables, since they provided better model fit with this coding, rather than having a continuous or ordinal form. The age dummy had a negative effect on gap-acceptance probability, indicating that older drivers were less likely to accept an available gap compared to younger. Moreover, all others being equal, participants who stated driving everyday were more likely to accept a gap. The effect of both sociodemographic variables used in the model is further discussed in the last section, with respect to their potential safety implications. The third gap-acceptance model (Model 3) included all the variables of Model 2 and also accounted for the time pressure conditions induced at the second gap-acceptance task. The time pressure parameter had a positive effect indicating that drivers were more likely to

15 accept a gap under the presence of time pressure. Finally, the model was estimated including physiological variables related to heart rate and skin conductance responses. The extraction and transformation/normalization of the physiological responses is described in section 3.2. Both variables had a significant a positive effect. This outcome, together with the abovementioned effect of time pressure conditions, may indicate that drivers (gap-acceptance) behaviour is not only influenced by traffic conditions but also by external stressors (time pressure in this case) or acute stress levels. In the current case, drivers stress was reflected through physiological responses during gap-acceptance choices, where a raise in these indicators also implied increase in the probability of crossing. However, if crossing behaviour, as examined in the present study, is interpreted as an action that involves risk-taking propensity, then drivers physiological responses might also consist indicators of potential aberrant or risky behaviour that could lead to a crash. This conclusion however, needs to be further examined in additional and various driving contexts Model comparison As shown in Table 6, while the gap-acceptance model was enriched with new parameters, indices of model fit as final log-likelihood (LL) or R 2 and adjusted R 2 got improved. However, the undertaking of collecting and analysing driving simulator or physiological data can be demanding, time consuming and maybe more expensive, compared to collecting e.g. real traffic video data from intersections or similar approaches. This section investigates how the gap-acceptance model got benefited from the variables collected in the simulator environment and wouldn t be available through more conventional data collection methods. All models were compared with the likelihood ratio test (e.g. Ben-Akiva and Lerman, 1985). In brief, the test can be defined as: -2LL = -2(LL R - LL U ) where L R is the LL value of the restricted model (the one with less variables) and L U is the LL of the unrestricted model (the model that includes the extra variables). This difference is asymptotically χ 2 -distributed and is compared with a critical value which depends on the degrees of freedom (difference of parameters). If log-likelihood exceeds that threshold value then the null hypothesis that both models perform equally is rejected. Table 7: Likelihood ratio tests results Models -2LL Degrees of freedom (df) χ 2 (99%,df) Null hypothesis Model 1 vs Model Rejected Model 2 vs Model Rejected Model 3 vs Model Rejected The results of the various likelihood ratio tests are presented in Table 7. In all cases, the null hypothesis was rejected at 99% level which implies that the model based on traffic related

16 variables only does not equally perform with the rest models. Moreover, the gradual addition of parameters managed to significantly improve the model enhancing the assumption that driving is a complex task and several factors can influence individual performance. Moreover, Model 3 does not equally perform with Model 4, thus the contribution of physiological variables was significant, in terms of model fit and capturing observed behaviour. These outcomes consist an indication that driving behaviour models, in general, should also focus on drivers attributes apart from traffic conditions. In the present case, socio-demographics and physiological responses provided better model fit and at the same time useful insights regarding crossing behaviour. 6.3 Sensitivity analysis The current section investigates the effect of each variable on gap-acceptance probability, based on the estimated parameters. This approach aims to compare the differences in gap-acceptance probabilities derived between Model 4, that had the best fit, and Model 1 which is a model based only on variables related to traffic conditions. For the rest of the section, the former model will be referred as the proposed model while the later as the base model. To this end, a sample average value was assumed for each of the variables - apart from one at a time - which was varying within predefined bounds, specified by the observations. Based on these values, the probabilities of gap-acceptance were estimated for each model; these probabilities, represent a person that belongs to the sample average. The fixed values of the continuous variables used were: 4.295s for gap size, 0.96m/s for speed, m for position (median value), for the normalised heart rate and for the normalised skin conductance responses. For the dummy variables, sample average values were also used (varying between zero and one): 0.18 for age, 0.46 for driving frequency, 0.45 for time pressure and 0.05 for last gap. The effect of the random disturbance term was integrated out in a similar way described in section 3.1. The results of the sensitivity analysis, for the base and proposed model, are presented in Figures 6 and 7 and refer only to traffic related variables. The rest of the proposed model s variables were not compared to the base model, since they are not included in that one and thus the calculated probabilities would remain constant and a comparison would not provide useful outcomes. However, these variables were investigated with respect to their effect on the gap-acceptance probability of the proposed model only. Figure 6: Sensitivity plots of gap size and speed on gap-acceptance probability A general outcome from Figures 6 and 7 is that all traffic variables have similar patterns in both models, however, the values for the base model are always higher. The inclusion of additional variables in the proposed model, apart from improving model fit, may also resulted in smaller probabilities and potentially assisted in avoiding overpredicted values,

17 as observed in the base model. Regarding individual parameter effects (Figure 6), the calculated probabilities for speed are higher, compared to gap size but have a similar magnitude with position (Figure 7, left). For the case of speed, the curves indicate that for drivers with speed above 8m/s, the probability for accepting the first gap approaches to one. As already mentioned in Section 6.1, this variable was used to capture drivers behaviour that did not brake at the intersection. Figure 7: Sensitivity plots of vehicle s position and next gap occurrence on gap-acceptance probability In terms of position (Figure 7, left), the probabilities trends show that the closer to the intersection a driver is positioned, during waiting time, the higher the probability to accept the available gap. When position is close to the median value however, probability levels are quite low, while the slope gets steeper as position values approach the centre of intersection (which is approximately placed at 10-11m from the beginning of the intersection area). Finally, the negative effect of the last gap dummy variable (Figure 7, right), is also reflected in the probability patterns, where it is observed that when there is no other vehicle coming, after the available gap, the probability of accepting this gap is close to zero. It is worth mentioning that this outcome is based on the observations from the current simulator experiment. Although in real life that probability could be higher, and some drivers would accept the last gap, this finding still consists an indication regarding drivers decision-making process. Figure 8: Sensitivity plots of the dummy variables used in the proposed model on gap-acceptance probability

18 In Figure 8 is depicted the effect of each of the dummy variables used in the proposed model. As explained already, since these variables were not included in the base model, their effect on gap-acceptance probabilities was not investigated across models but only for the proposed one. The results indicate that the lowest probability occurs when a driver is above 45 years (holding all the rest variables constant). On the other hand, the highest probability is observed for daily frequency of driving. Moreover, it is worth mentioning that the probabilities for < 45 years and time pressure have approximately the same values. Figure 9: Sensitivity plot of heart rate and skin conductance on gap-acceptance probability The last variables included in the proposed model were those related to drivers physiology. The effect of both variables is shown in Figure 9, after applying some scale adjustment on the x-axis to account for the different values after their normalisation. The results show that both variables have a similar pattern on gap-acceptance probability, however, skin conductance is linked with higher values. This finding may indicate that within the context of the study, skin conductance is a stronger predictor for accepting a gap. 6.4 Substitution rates At the final part of the analysis, it was attempted a comparison across all the estimated models. The comparison was based on the marginal rates of substitutions (MRS), that also assists in avoiding issues of differences in scales across models. This approach investigates the required change in a specific variable, in order to counterbalance the change in another variable and keep the total utility constant (see e.g. Kløjgaard & Hess, 2014). The MRS be briefly summarised as the calculation of βi/βj ratio, where β represent the parameter estimates of variables i and j. In most studies, MRS has been used to calculate marginal willingness to pay, using the marginal utility of price in the denominator part. For each of the four models of the present analysis, the parameter of gap size was used as the denominator and the ratios were computed using each of the rest parameters as numerators. The results are illustrated in Figure 10 where the calculated values represent the relative effect of each parameter with respect to the gap size parameter of each model.

19 Figure 10: Marginal rates of substitution It should be mentioned that since the parameter of gap size is positive, the ratios with negative parameters, as numerator, are expected negative while positive ratios are expected when the opposite holds. Thus, when interpreting the MRS values, what is important is whether the absolute ratio value is higher than unity, rather than the sign of the ratio itself. For instance, MRS >1 shows that the change in utility, from a one-unit shift from the baseline of a given variable, is greater than the one occurs from an increase in gap size for 1s. The opposite applies for MRS <1. When applying the aforementioned in e.g. the last gap dummy ratio, the outcome would be that the effect of the current gap being also the last in the sequence of gaps, is almost 8 times (traffic variables model) as negatively as the increase of 1s in the gap size in the utility of gap acceptance. For the same model, an increase in speed for 1m/s is twice as positive as an increase of 1s in gap size, in the gap-acceptance utility. In the present analysis, all absolute ratio values are larger than unity. Moreover, focusing on the variables included in all four models, all values decrease as more variables are included. An exception to this pattern is the last model, where relative values increase again. A potential explanation for this finding is that when stress variables are included, they manage to better capture the effect of some variables. However, in all cases, ratios still remain smaller, compared to the first model. Moreover, position the positive relative effect of position is on average higher, compared to speed. Also, the highest positive value from this group of variables is noticed for the random disturbance term of the first model. It should be mentioned that this variable reflects the effect of a series of unobserved variables/characteristics and follows a normal distribution. The fact that the highest value is observed for the first model, may consist an indication that the variables included in this model are not sufficient to capture observed gap-acceptance behaviour, therefore, the disturbance term has a higher relative effect, to capture the influence of omitted variables. With reference to the socio-demographic variables, all relative effects are smaller when

20 time pressure and physiological variables are included in the model. This outcome consists an indication that when the latter variables are added, the effect of socio-demographics is more representative, since these models also have better fit, and in fact their impact is not as high as initially estimated in the socio-demographics model. Finally, as already mentioned in the sensitivity analysis section, the effect of skin conductance in gap-acceptance utility is higher compared to heart rate. 7. Discussion The objective of the present paper was to mathematically model of a gap-acceptance model for intersection crossing scenarios, with and without time-pressure induced stress and compare the results. The driving simulator was used as the testbed and the level of stress was approximated through heart rate and skin conductance. The initial exploratory analysis showed that gap-acceptance can change under a different context (e.g. drivers may be willing to accept shorter gaps under time pressure). Also, under the presence of time pressure, three of the participants showed aberrant and extremely risky behaviour and crossed the intersection without actually stopping. Although this type of behaviour is rather unlikely to be observed in real life, this outcome still indicates that external stressors can potentially increase risk-taking tendency that may lead to dangerous incidents, even crashes. At the next step of the analysis, the model results indicated that gap-acceptance behaviour is not only related to traffic characteristics e.g. gap size but drivers individual characteristics may also influence this type of behaviour. For example, all else being equal, the driving frequency was found to increase the probability of accepting gaps while age had a negative effect. If the increased likelihood of accepting a gap could be a potential risk-taking indication, the effects of socio-demographics may also have safety implications. For instance, previous research (e.g. Matthews et al., 1999) used driving frequency as a measure of driving exposure and positively related it to crashes and speed violations. In the present case, participants driving on a daily basis and thus with higher exposure - were more likely to accept a gap and therefore might be considered as more risk-takers. Moreover, numerous studies have investigated the effects of age on driving and risk-taking behaviour (e.g. Jonah, 1990; Krahé & Fenske, 2002; Rhodes & Pivik, 2011; Taubman & Yehel, 2012). The results of the gap-acceptance model(s) of this study support the existing literature findings, since elder drivers also had smaller probability to accept a gap and thus engage in riskier situations. The abovementioned findings not only suggest that drivers individual attributes influence their choices and should be considered in the development of driving behaviour models (since most of the existing approaches ignore or attempt to capture them using random disturbance terms) but also that gap-acceptance behaviour may be driven by risk perception of specific groups of drivers. The time pressure variable had an expected positive effect on gap-acceptance choices. Similar research (Rendon-Velez et al., 2016) has shown that time pressure is linked to higher speed and is also reflected in auxiliary measures of safety. These findings may potentially lay time pressure, in gap-acceptance behaviour, as a variable with safety implications, in a similar way to drivers socio-demographics. Finally, in terms of stress

Modeling Anger and Aggressive Driving Behavior in a Dynamic Choice-Latent Variable Model

Modeling Anger and Aggressive Driving Behavior in a Dynamic Choice-Latent Variable Model Modeling Anger and Aggressive Driving Behavior in a Dynamic Choice-Latent Variable Model Mazen Danaf Maya Abou-Zeid Isam Kaysi American University of Beirut Workshop on Discrete Choice Models EPFL June

More information

Analysis of Glance Movements in Critical Intersection Scenarios

Analysis of Glance Movements in Critical Intersection Scenarios Analysis of Glance Movements in Critical Intersection Scenarios Marina Plavši, Klaus Bengler, Heiner Bubb Lehrstuhl für Ergonomie Technische Universität München 85747 Garching b. München, Germany ABSTRACT

More information

DRIVER BEHAVIOUR STUDIES IN THE MOTORWAY OPERATIONS PLATFORM GRANT

DRIVER BEHAVIOUR STUDIES IN THE MOTORWAY OPERATIONS PLATFORM GRANT DRIVER BEHAVIOUR STUDIES IN THE MOTORWAY OPERATIONS PLATFORM GRANT Mark Brackstone and Mike McDonald Transportation Research Group Dept. of Civil and Environmental Engineering University of Southampton

More information

Chapter 5 Car driving

Chapter 5 Car driving 5 Car driving The present thesis addresses the topic of the failure to apprehend. In the previous chapters we discussed potential underlying mechanisms for the failure to apprehend, such as a failure to

More information

DRIVER PSYCHOLOGICAL TYPES AND CAR FOLLOWING: IS THERE A CORRELATION? RESULTS OF A PILOT STUDY

DRIVER PSYCHOLOGICAL TYPES AND CAR FOLLOWING: IS THERE A CORRELATION? RESULTS OF A PILOT STUDY DRIVER PSYCHOLOGICAL TYPES AND CAR FOLLOWING: IS THERE A CORRELATION? RESULTS OF A PILOT STUDY Mark Brackstone Transportation Research Group, Dept. of Civil and Environmental Engineering, University of

More information

Issues Surrounding the Normalization and Standardisation of Skin Conductance Responses (SCRs).

Issues Surrounding the Normalization and Standardisation of Skin Conductance Responses (SCRs). Issues Surrounding the Normalization and Standardisation of Skin Conductance Responses (SCRs). Jason J. Braithwaite {Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK}

More information

Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems

Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems J.D. Lee, J.D. Hoffman, H.A. Stoner, B.D. Seppelt, and M.D. Brown Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering, University of

More information

Social forgivingness and vulnerable road users

Social forgivingness and vulnerable road users Social forgivingness and vulnerable road users Maura Houtenbos, SWOV Institute for Road Safety Research PO Box 1090, 2260 BB Leidschendam; maura.houtenbos@swov.nl Abstract Pedestrians and cyclists are

More information

I just didn t see it It s all about hazard perception. Dr. Robert B. Isler Associate Professor School of Psychology University of Waikato Hamilton

I just didn t see it It s all about hazard perception. Dr. Robert B. Isler Associate Professor School of Psychology University of Waikato Hamilton I just didn t see it It s all about hazard perception Dr. Robert B. Isler Associate Professor School of Psychology University of Waikato Hamilton Brake Professional Webinar, Tuesday 14 March 2017 New Zealand

More information

The role of memory on patients with mild cognitive impairment. Sophia Vardaki, PhD National Technical University of Athens

The role of memory on patients with mild cognitive impairment. Sophia Vardaki, PhD National Technical University of Athens The role of memory on patients with mild cognitive impairment Sophia Vardaki, PhD National Technical University of Athens Athens, 26 June 2015 Objective-Presentation Structure Objective To explore the

More information

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING George J. Andersen, Rui Ni Department of Psychology University of California Riverside Riverside, California, USA E-mail: Andersen@ucr.edu E-mail: ruini@ucr.edu

More information

Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation

Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2009 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 23rd, 12:00 AM Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation Rob Gray Arizona

More information

DRIVER S SITUATION AWARENESS DURING SUPERVISION OF AUTOMATED CONTROL Comparison between SART and SAGAT measurement techniques

DRIVER S SITUATION AWARENESS DURING SUPERVISION OF AUTOMATED CONTROL Comparison between SART and SAGAT measurement techniques DRIVER S SITUATION AWARENESS DURING SUPERVISION OF AUTOMATED CONTROL Comparison between SART and SAGAT measurement techniques Arie P. van den Beukel, Mascha C. van der Voort ABSTRACT: Systems enabling

More information

Alcohol-impaired driving in the lab: Efforts to reduce the incidence of drinking and driving

Alcohol-impaired driving in the lab: Efforts to reduce the incidence of drinking and driving Alcohol-impaired driving in the lab: Efforts to reduce the incidence of drinking and driving Nicholas A. Van Dyke, M.S. Department of Psychology An Equal Opportunity University Research Interests General

More information

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Current Biology, in press Supplemental Information: Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Grigorios Sotiropoulos, Aaron R. Seitz, and Peggy Seriès Supplemental Data Detailed

More information

Lecture Notes Module 2

Lecture Notes Module 2 Lecture Notes Module 2 Two-group Experimental Designs The goal of most research is to assess a possible causal relation between the response variable and another variable called the independent variable.

More information

ROAD SAFETY (OR LACK THEREOF) IN THE PUBLIC MIND

ROAD SAFETY (OR LACK THEREOF) IN THE PUBLIC MIND ROAD SAFETY (OR LACK THEREOF) IN THE PUBLIC MIND INVESTIGATING TOLERANCE OF UNSAFE ROAD CONDITIONS AND BEHAVIOURS AMONG DRIVER S LICENCE HOLDERS IN QUEBEC November 2011 SUMMARY There is a fairly widespread

More information

A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD

A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD Yan Yang, Bryan Reimer, Bruce Mehler & Jonathan Dobres The

More information

APPENDIX FOR "DRIVING SIMULATOR FOR PERSONS WITH IMPAIRED COGNITION", PI WILLIAM K DURFEE

APPENDIX FOR DRIVING SIMULATOR FOR PERSONS WITH IMPAIRED COGNITION, PI WILLIAM K DURFEE APPENDIX FOR "DRIVING SIMULATOR FOR PERSONS WITH IMPAIRED COGNITION", PI WILLIAM K DURFEE Contents 1. Drive Log (simulation run): Used by researcher to record events that took place during a simulator

More information

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment Title: Authors: Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment Mackenzie, A.K. Harris, J.M. Journal: ACM Digital Library, (ETRA '14 Proceedings

More information

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX 2 Web Appendix A: Panel data estimation approach As noted in the main

More information

Six Sigma Glossary Lean 6 Society

Six Sigma Glossary Lean 6 Society Six Sigma Glossary Lean 6 Society ABSCISSA ACCEPTANCE REGION ALPHA RISK ALTERNATIVE HYPOTHESIS ASSIGNABLE CAUSE ASSIGNABLE VARIATIONS The horizontal axis of a graph The region of values for which the null

More information

Identification of Tissue Independent Cancer Driver Genes

Identification of Tissue Independent Cancer Driver Genes Identification of Tissue Independent Cancer Driver Genes Alexandros Manolakos, Idoia Ochoa, Kartik Venkat Supervisor: Olivier Gevaert Abstract Identification of genomic patterns in tumors is an important

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Ebbes, P. (2004). Latent instrumental variables: a new approach to solve for endogeneity s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Ebbes, P. (2004). Latent instrumental variables: a new approach to solve for endogeneity s.n. University of Groningen Latent instrumental variables Ebbes, P. IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

Chapter 7: Descriptive Statistics

Chapter 7: Descriptive Statistics Chapter Overview Chapter 7 provides an introduction to basic strategies for describing groups statistically. Statistical concepts around normal distributions are discussed. The statistical procedures of

More information

http://www.diva-portal.org This is the published version of a paper presented at Future Active Safety Technology - Towards zero traffic accidents, FastZero2017, September 18-22, 2017, Nara, Japan. Citation

More information

Crash Risk Analysis of Distracted Driving Behavior: Influence of Secondary Task Engagement and Driver Characteristics

Crash Risk Analysis of Distracted Driving Behavior: Influence of Secondary Task Engagement and Driver Characteristics University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2017 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 27th, 12:00 AM Crash Risk Analysis of Distracted Driving Behavior: Influence of Secondary Task

More information

EVALUATING CHANGES IN THE DRIVING BEHAVIOR OF YOUNG DRIVERS A FEW YEARS AFTER LICENSURE USING IN-VEHICLE DATA RECORDERS

EVALUATING CHANGES IN THE DRIVING BEHAVIOR OF YOUNG DRIVERS A FEW YEARS AFTER LICENSURE USING IN-VEHICLE DATA RECORDERS EVALUATING CHANGES IN THE DRIVING BEHAVIOR OF YOUNG DRIVERS A FEW YEARS AFTER LICENSURE USING IN-VEHICLE DATA RECORDERS Gila Albert 1, Oren Musicant 2, Tsippy Lotan 3, Tomer Toledo 4, & Einat Grimberg

More information

MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS

MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS Inference Problems under a Special Form of Heteroskedasticity Helmut Farbmacher, Heinrich Kögel 03-2015 MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS mea Amalienstr. 33_D-80799 Munich_Phone+49 89 38602-355_Fax +49 89 38602-390_www.mea.mpisoc.mpg.de

More information

Conditional spectrum-based ground motion selection. Part II: Intensity-based assessments and evaluation of alternative target spectra

Conditional spectrum-based ground motion selection. Part II: Intensity-based assessments and evaluation of alternative target spectra EARTHQUAKE ENGINEERING & STRUCTURAL DYNAMICS Published online 9 May 203 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com)..2303 Conditional spectrum-based ground motion selection. Part II: Intensity-based

More information

Funnelling Used to describe a process of narrowing down of focus within a literature review. So, the writer begins with a broad discussion providing b

Funnelling Used to describe a process of narrowing down of focus within a literature review. So, the writer begins with a broad discussion providing b Accidental sampling A lesser-used term for convenience sampling. Action research An approach that challenges the traditional conception of the researcher as separate from the real world. It is associated

More information

Profiling Drivers Risky Behaviour Towards All Road Users

Profiling Drivers Risky Behaviour Towards All Road Users Profiling Drivers Risky Behaviour Towards All Road Users Adrian B. Ellison, Stephen Greaves & Rhonda Daniels Institute of Transport and Logistics Studies The University of Sydney ACRS Conference, 10 August

More information

Available from Deakin Research Online:

Available from Deakin Research Online: This is the published version: Richardson, Ben and Fuller Tyszkiewicz, Matthew 2014, The application of non linear multilevel models to experience sampling data, European health psychologist, vol. 16,

More information

Running head: NESTED FACTOR ANALYTIC MODEL COMPARISON 1. John M. Clark III. Pearson. Author Note

Running head: NESTED FACTOR ANALYTIC MODEL COMPARISON 1. John M. Clark III. Pearson. Author Note Running head: NESTED FACTOR ANALYTIC MODEL COMPARISON 1 Nested Factor Analytic Model Comparison as a Means to Detect Aberrant Response Patterns John M. Clark III Pearson Author Note John M. Clark III,

More information

Mantel-Haenszel Procedures for Detecting Differential Item Functioning

Mantel-Haenszel Procedures for Detecting Differential Item Functioning A Comparison of Logistic Regression and Mantel-Haenszel Procedures for Detecting Differential Item Functioning H. Jane Rogers, Teachers College, Columbia University Hariharan Swaminathan, University of

More information

COMPARISON OF SELF-REPORTED AND COMPUTER-BASED HAZARD PERCEPTION SKILLS AMONG NOVICE AND EXPERIENCED DRIVERS

COMPARISON OF SELF-REPORTED AND COMPUTER-BASED HAZARD PERCEPTION SKILLS AMONG NOVICE AND EXPERIENCED DRIVERS COMPARISON OF SELF-REPORTED AND COMPUTER-BASED HAZARD PERCEPTION SKILLS AMONG NOVICE AND EXPERIENCED DRIVERS Nebi Sümer Department of Psychology Middle East Technical University Ankara, Turkey E-mail:

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

Technical Whitepaper

Technical Whitepaper Technical Whitepaper July, 2001 Prorating Scale Scores Consequential analysis using scales from: BDI (Beck Depression Inventory) NAS (Novaco Anger Scales) STAXI (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory) PIP (Psychotic

More information

Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction

Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction Susan L. Ettner, Ph.D. Professor Division of General Internal Medicine and Health Services Research, UCLA The Problem The Problem Suppose you wish to

More information

ROAD SAFETY MONITOR. ALCOHOL-IMPAIRED DRIVING IN THE UNITED STATES Results from the 2018 TIRF USA Road Safety Monitor

ROAD SAFETY MONITOR. ALCOHOL-IMPAIRED DRIVING IN THE UNITED STATES Results from the 2018 TIRF USA Road Safety Monitor This fact sheet summarizes the national results of the 2018 USA Road Safety Monitor (USA RSM) on alcohol-impaired driving. This USA RSM is the fourth annual public opinion survey conducted by the Traffic

More information

Distracted Driving Effects on CMV Operators

Distracted Driving Effects on CMV Operators Distracted Driving Effects on CMV Operators The Research in Advanced Performance Technology and Educational Readiness (RAPTER) team Institute for Simulation and Training University of Central Florida presented

More information

Driver Distraction: Towards A Working Definition

Driver Distraction: Towards A Working Definition Driver Distraction: Towards A Working Definition International Conference on Distracted Driving Toronto, Ontario October 2-5, 2005 Leo Tasca, Ph.D. Road Safety Program Office Road User Safety Division

More information

A Brief Introduction to Bayesian Statistics

A Brief Introduction to Bayesian Statistics A Brief Introduction to Statistics David Kaplan Department of Educational Psychology Methods for Social Policy Research and, Washington, DC 2017 1 / 37 The Reverend Thomas Bayes, 1701 1761 2 / 37 Pierre-Simon

More information

Research in Progress on Distracted Driving

Research in Progress on Distracted Driving Research in Progress on Distracted Driving indicates project was funded with U.S. DOT funds either directly or through a State DOT using SPR funds. Assessment of Distraction Caused By Billboards Using

More information

Business Statistics Probability

Business Statistics Probability Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Role of Monotonous Attention in Traffic Violations, Errors, and Accidents

Role of Monotonous Attention in Traffic Violations, Errors, and Accidents University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2001 Driving Assessment Conference Aug 15th, 12:00 AM Role of Monotonous Attention in Traffic Violations, Errors, and Accidents Nebi

More information

Undiagnosed ADHD Among Unionized Drivers in Ghana: Public Health and Policy Implications

Undiagnosed ADHD Among Unionized Drivers in Ghana: Public Health and Policy Implications Undiagnosed ADHD Among Unionized Drivers in Ghana: Public Health and Policy Implications Thad Ulzen MD Professor and Chair, Dept. of Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine Associate Dean for Academic Affairs

More information

Chapter 20: Test Administration and Interpretation

Chapter 20: Test Administration and Interpretation Chapter 20: Test Administration and Interpretation Thought Questions Why should a needs analysis consider both the individual and the demands of the sport? Should test scores be shared with a team, or

More information

YSU Students. STATS 3743 Dr. Huang-Hwa Andy Chang Term Project 2 May 2002

YSU Students. STATS 3743 Dr. Huang-Hwa Andy Chang Term Project 2 May 2002 YSU Students STATS 3743 Dr. Huang-Hwa Andy Chang Term Project May 00 Anthony Koulianos, Chemical Engineer Kyle Unger, Chemical Engineer Vasilia Vamvakis, Chemical Engineer I. Executive Summary It is common

More information

Traffic Violations and Errors: The Effects of Sensation Seeking and Attention

Traffic Violations and Errors: The Effects of Sensation Seeking and Attention University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2005 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 29th, 12:00 AM Traffic Violations and Errors: The Effects of Sensation Seeking and Attention

More information

PCT 101. A Perceptual Control Theory Primer. Fred Nickols 8/27/2012

PCT 101. A Perceptual Control Theory Primer. Fred Nickols 8/27/2012 PCT 101 A Perceptual Control Theory Primer Fred Nickols 8/27/2012 This paper presents a simplified, plain language explanation of Perceptual Control Theory (PCT). PCT is a powerful and practical theory

More information

COGNITIVE AND PSYCHOMOTOR CORRELATES OF HAZARD PERCEPTION ABILITY AND RISKY DRIVING

COGNITIVE AND PSYCHOMOTOR CORRELATES OF HAZARD PERCEPTION ABILITY AND RISKY DRIVING COGNITIVE AND PSYCHOMOTOR CORRELATES OF HAZARD PERCEPTION ABILITY AND RISKY DRIVING Nebi Sümer Middle East Technical University Ankara, Turkey Email: nsumer@metu.edu.tr Summary: Deficits in specific cognitive

More information

The role of music characteristics on drivers anger regulation

The role of music characteristics on drivers anger regulation The role of music characteristics on drivers anger regulation, S., Pfister, H-R. & Höger, R. Institute of Experimental Industrial Psychology - LueneLab, Leuphana University Lüneburg Abstract This research

More information

The Wellbeing Course. Resource: Mental Skills. The Wellbeing Course was written by Professor Nick Titov and Dr Blake Dear

The Wellbeing Course. Resource: Mental Skills. The Wellbeing Course was written by Professor Nick Titov and Dr Blake Dear The Wellbeing Course Resource: Mental Skills The Wellbeing Course was written by Professor Nick Titov and Dr Blake Dear About Mental Skills This resource introduces three mental skills which people find

More information

Verbal Collision Avoidance Messages of Varying Perceived Urgency Reduce Crashes in High Risk Scenarios

Verbal Collision Avoidance Messages of Varying Perceived Urgency Reduce Crashes in High Risk Scenarios University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2005 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 28th, 12:00 AM Verbal Collision Avoidance Messages of Varying Perceived Urgency Reduce Crashes

More information

The Effectiveness of Pre-Driver Training

The Effectiveness of Pre-Driver Training 67 th ROAD SAFETY CONGRESS 4 th 6 th MARCH 2002 Safer Driving The Road to Success The Effectiveness of Pre-Driver Training Dr Bill Carcary Tayside Police NEW DRIVER PROJECT Dr. Bill Carcary Tayside Police

More information

EXAMINING TEMPORAL EFFECTS OF LIFECYCLE EVENTS ON TRANSPORT MODE CHOICE DECISIONS

EXAMINING TEMPORAL EFFECTS OF LIFECYCLE EVENTS ON TRANSPORT MODE CHOICE DECISIONS EXAMINING TEMPORAL EFFECTS OF LIFECYCLE EVENTS ON TRANSPORT MODE CHOICE DECISIONS Marloes VERHOEVEN PHD Urban Planning Group Eindhoven University of Technology P.O. Box MB Eindhoven The Netherlands Phone:

More information

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS Ronald R. Mourant, Beverly K. Jaeger, and Yingzi Lin Virtual Environments Laboratory 334 Snell Engineering Center Northeastern University Boston, MA 02115-5000 In the case

More information

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) 1 Outline Method of Adjustment Method of Limits Method of Constant Stimuli Probit Analysis 2 Method of Constant Stimuli A set of equally spaced levels of the stimulus

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR

ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR CHAPTER 6 ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR Several studies were done as part of the UTDFP that were based substantially on subjective data, reflecting travelers beliefs, attitudes, and intentions.

More information

Adjusting for mode of administration effect in surveys using mailed questionnaire and telephone interview data

Adjusting for mode of administration effect in surveys using mailed questionnaire and telephone interview data Adjusting for mode of administration effect in surveys using mailed questionnaire and telephone interview data Karl Bang Christensen National Institute of Occupational Health, Denmark Helene Feveille National

More information

On the Fast Lane to Road Rage

On the Fast Lane to Road Rage University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2003 Driving Assessment Conference Jul 23rd, 12:00 AM On the Fast Lane to Road Rage Frank A. Drews University of Utah, Salt Lake City

More information

Living with Newton's Laws

Living with Newton's Laws Task #1 - Newton s 1 st Law - This is a pain in the neck Let's suppose you are in your car, waiting at a stop light. Like any good driver, you have your seat belt buckled. (It's the law.) Suddenly, a car

More information

Title of Resource. A Class Research Project to Help Students Understand Correlation and Other Basic

Title of Resource. A Class Research Project to Help Students Understand Correlation and Other Basic A Class Research Project to Help Students Understand Correlation and Other Basic Title of Resource Research Concepts Author(s) Kathleen P. Greenberg Institution State University of New York College at

More information

A critical analysis of an applied psychology journal about the effects of driving fatigue: Driver fatigue and highway driving: A simulator study.

A critical analysis of an applied psychology journal about the effects of driving fatigue: Driver fatigue and highway driving: A simulator study. A critical analysis of an applied psychology journal about the effects of driving fatigue: Driver fatigue and highway driving: A simulator study. By Ping-Huang Ting, Jiun-Ren Hwang, Ji-Liang Doong and

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Development. summary. Sam Sample. Emotional Intelligence Profile. Wednesday 5 April 2017 General Working Population (sample size 1634) Sam Sample

Development. summary. Sam Sample. Emotional Intelligence Profile. Wednesday 5 April 2017 General Working Population (sample size 1634) Sam Sample Development summary Wednesday 5 April 2017 General Working Population (sample size 1634) Emotional Intelligence Profile 1 Contents 04 About this report 05 Introduction to Emotional Intelligence 06 Your

More information

FACTORS CONTRIBUTING TO SELF-REPORTED CELL PHONE USAGE BY YOUNGER DRIVERS IN THE PACIFIC NORTHWEST

FACTORS CONTRIBUTING TO SELF-REPORTED CELL PHONE USAGE BY YOUNGER DRIVERS IN THE PACIFIC NORTHWEST FACTORS CONTRIBUTING TO SELF-REPORTED CELL PHONE USAGE BY YOUNGER DRIVERS IN THE PACIFIC NORTHWEST Hisham Jashami, Masoud Ghodrat Abadi, and David S. Hurwitz Transportation Engineering, Oregon State University

More information

Task difficulty, risk, effort and comfort in a simulated driving task - Implications for Risk Allostasis theory

Task difficulty, risk, effort and comfort in a simulated driving task - Implications for Risk Allostasis theory Task difficulty, risk, effort and comfort in a simulated driving task - Implications for Risk Allostasis theory Ben Lewis-Evans* & Talib Rothengatter 1 Traffic and Environmental Psychology Group, Experimental

More information

Tired of Raising Awareness of Driver Fatigue? Push On. NSW Roads and Traffic Authority (RTA) 1 ; Ipsos Australia 2

Tired of Raising Awareness of Driver Fatigue? Push On. NSW Roads and Traffic Authority (RTA) 1 ; Ipsos Australia 2 Tired of Raising Awareness of Driver Fatigue? Push On. Harris, R. 1, Hawkins, A.2 & Peacock, G. 2 NSW Roads and Traffic Authority (RTA) 1 ; Ipsos Australia 2 ABSTRACT Countermeasures for non-commercial

More information

PSYCHOLOGY 300B (A01) One-sample t test. n = d = ρ 1 ρ 0 δ = d (n 1) d

PSYCHOLOGY 300B (A01) One-sample t test. n = d = ρ 1 ρ 0 δ = d (n 1) d PSYCHOLOGY 300B (A01) Assignment 3 January 4, 019 σ M = σ N z = M µ σ M d = M 1 M s p d = µ 1 µ 0 σ M = µ +σ M (z) Independent-samples t test One-sample t test n = δ δ = d n d d = µ 1 µ σ δ = d n n = δ

More information

Statistical reports Regression, 2010

Statistical reports Regression, 2010 Statistical reports Regression, 2010 Niels Richard Hansen June 10, 2010 This document gives some guidelines on how to write a report on a statistical analysis. The document is organized into sections that

More information

What is analytical sociology? And is it the future of sociology?

What is analytical sociology? And is it the future of sociology? What is analytical sociology? And is it the future of sociology? Twan Huijsmans Sociology Abstract During the last few decades a new approach in sociology has been developed, analytical sociology (AS).

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

The Danger of Incorrect Expectations In Driving: The Failure to Respond

The Danger of Incorrect Expectations In Driving: The Failure to Respond University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2007 Driving Assessment Conference Jul 11th, 12:00 AM The Danger of Incorrect Expectations In Driving: The Failure to Respond Martin

More information

Section 6: Analysing Relationships Between Variables

Section 6: Analysing Relationships Between Variables 6. 1 Analysing Relationships Between Variables Section 6: Analysing Relationships Between Variables Choosing a Technique The Crosstabs Procedure The Chi Square Test The Means Procedure The Correlations

More information

HARRISON ASSESSMENTS DEBRIEF GUIDE 1. OVERVIEW OF HARRISON ASSESSMENT

HARRISON ASSESSMENTS DEBRIEF GUIDE 1. OVERVIEW OF HARRISON ASSESSMENT HARRISON ASSESSMENTS HARRISON ASSESSMENTS DEBRIEF GUIDE 1. OVERVIEW OF HARRISON ASSESSMENT Have you put aside an hour and do you have a hard copy of your report? Get a quick take on their initial reactions

More information

RESEARCH ARTICLE. GENDER DIFFERENCES IN RISK TAKING: EVIDENCE FROM A MANAGEMENT INSTITUTE Amit Kapoor 1

RESEARCH ARTICLE. GENDER DIFFERENCES IN RISK TAKING: EVIDENCE FROM A MANAGEMENT INSTITUTE Amit Kapoor 1 GENDER DIFFERENCES IN RISK TAKING: EVIDENCE FROM A MANAGEMENT INSTITUTE Amit Kapoor 1 HOW TO CITE THIS ARTICLE: Amit Kapoor. Gender differences in Risk Taking: Evidence from a Management Institute. Journal

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

6. Unusual and Influential Data

6. Unusual and Influential Data Sociology 740 John ox Lecture Notes 6. Unusual and Influential Data Copyright 2014 by John ox Unusual and Influential Data 1 1. Introduction I Linear statistical models make strong assumptions about the

More information

Georgina Salas. Topics EDCI Intro to Research Dr. A.J. Herrera

Georgina Salas. Topics EDCI Intro to Research Dr. A.J. Herrera Homework assignment topics 51-63 Georgina Salas Topics 51-63 EDCI Intro to Research 6300.62 Dr. A.J. Herrera Topic 51 1. Which average is usually reported when the standard deviation is reported? The mean

More information

Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks

Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2007 Driving Assessment Conference Jul 11th, 12:00 AM Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks James Sayer University

More information

Module 3 - Scientific Method

Module 3 - Scientific Method Module 3 - Scientific Method Distinguishing between basic and applied research. Identifying characteristics of a hypothesis, and distinguishing its conceptual variables from operational definitions used

More information

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation The description and presentation of results involves a number of topics. These include scales of measurement, descriptive statistics used to summarize

More information

MULTIPLE LINEAR REGRESSION 24.1 INTRODUCTION AND OBJECTIVES OBJECTIVES

MULTIPLE LINEAR REGRESSION 24.1 INTRODUCTION AND OBJECTIVES OBJECTIVES 24 MULTIPLE LINEAR REGRESSION 24.1 INTRODUCTION AND OBJECTIVES In the previous chapter, simple linear regression was used when you have one independent variable and one dependent variable. This chapter

More information

Examining Distracted Drivers Underestimation of Time and Overestimation of Speed

Examining Distracted Drivers Underestimation of Time and Overestimation of Speed Examining Distracted Drivers Underestimation of Time and Overestimation of Speed Michael Knodler, PhD Associate Professor Civil and Environmental Engineering University of Massachusetts Amherst Andronikos

More information

TAILGATING IN MALAYSIA: MARITAL STATUS MATTERS

TAILGATING IN MALAYSIA: MARITAL STATUS MATTERS TAILGATING IN MALAYSIA: MARITAL STATUS MATTERS Ahmad Azad Ab Rashid Malaysian Institute of Road Safety Research a.azad@miros.gov.my ABSTRACT The study adopted a self-report measure to investigate tailgating,

More information

CHAPTER 4 RESULTS. In this chapter the results of the empirical research are reported and discussed in the following order:

CHAPTER 4 RESULTS. In this chapter the results of the empirical research are reported and discussed in the following order: 71 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS 4.1 INTRODUCTION In this chapter the results of the empirical research are reported and discussed in the following order: (1) Descriptive statistics of the sample; the extraneous variables;

More information

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task Olga Lositsky lositsky@princeton.edu Robert C. Wilson Department of Psychology University

More information

Distracted Driving. Stephanie Bonne, MD

Distracted Driving. Stephanie Bonne, MD Distracted Driving Stephanie Bonne, MD Statistics The US sends 171.3 billion text messages per month 3, 328 deaths due to distracted driving in 2012 20% between the age of 20 and 30 421,000 injuries involving

More information

RAG Rating Indicator Values

RAG Rating Indicator Values Technical Guide RAG Rating Indicator Values Introduction This document sets out Public Health England s standard approach to the use of RAG ratings for indicator values in relation to comparator or benchmark

More information

Module 28 - Estimating a Population Mean (1 of 3)

Module 28 - Estimating a Population Mean (1 of 3) Module 28 - Estimating a Population Mean (1 of 3) In "Estimating a Population Mean," we focus on how to use a sample mean to estimate a population mean. This is the type of thinking we did in Modules 7

More information

Identification of human factors criteria to assess the effects of level crossing safety measures

Identification of human factors criteria to assess the effects of level crossing safety measures VTT TECHNICAL RESEARCH CENTRE OF FINLAND LTD Identification of human factors criteria to assess the effects of level crossing safety measures The 7th Annual Scientific Seminar of the Nordic Traffic Safety

More information

CHAPTER ONE CORRELATION

CHAPTER ONE CORRELATION CHAPTER ONE CORRELATION 1.0 Introduction The first chapter focuses on the nature of statistical data of correlation. The aim of the series of exercises is to ensure the students are able to use SPSS to

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information