Quantity matching by an orangutan (Pongo abelii)

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Quantity matching by an orangutan (Pongo abelii)"

Transcription

1 DOI /s ORIGINAL PAPER Quantity matching by an orangutan (Pongo abelii) Jennifer Vonk Received: 11 June 2013 / Revised: 8 July 2013 / Accepted: 10 July 2013 Ó Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013 Abstract An adult male orangutan (Pongo abelii) was presented with a series of delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS) tasks in which he was to match images based on (a) the number of individual animals depicted in the photograph (from 1 to 4), (b) the number of abstract shapes presented in the stimulus (from 1 to 4), or (c) the number of dots presented in the stimulus (from 1 to 4, 4 7, or 7 10). The spatial arrangement of the dots and the background color of the stimuli varied, and the size of the dots was manipulated to control for overall ratio of foreground to background. The subject s performance was not affected by these perceptual features, but was affected by the absolute difference and ratio between number of elements in the comparison stimuli. However, the relationship between these variables and his performance was not always linear as predicted by the analog magnitude model. In addition, the subject showed a high degree of transfer to novel numerosities up to ten, indicating that orangutans are capable of estimating quantity for a greater number of items than can presumably be subtilized by humans. Keywords Orangutan Quantity Magnitude Matching-to-sample Introduction Some degree of numerical ability has been established in a wide variety of animal species (for reviews, see Brannon and Roitman 2003; Davis and Perusse 1988; Dehaene J. Vonk (&) Department of Psychology, Oakland University, 2200 N Squirrel Rd., Rochester, MI 48309, USA vonk@oakland.edu 1997). Many distantly related species are capable of relative numerousness judgments. Although primates have been tested most often (Cantlon and Brannon 2006), this list also includes a number of non-primate species such as canids (Baker et al. 2012; West and Young 2002), black bears, (Vonk and Beran 2012), elephants (Perdue et al. 2012), dolphins, (Jaakkola et al. 2005; Kilian et al. 2003), birds (Emmerton 1998; Garland et al. 2012; Pepperberg 2006), amphibians (Uller et al. 2003), and fish (Agrillo et al. 2009; Gomez-Laplaza and Gerlai 2011). From an evolutionary standpoint, it is hardly surprising that many species are capable of estimating differences between quantities as such information provides valuable information when foraging or competing for food. What is of interest from an experimental standpoint is the nature of the underlying representation of quantity that animals utilize. For instance, researchers have evaluated whether animals represent quantity in the form of magnitude judgments, which can be represented perceptually based on mass, density, area etc., or whether they are capable of representing numerosity, which can be distinguished from these perceptual features. Non-humans are capable of estimating the magnitude of different quantities of items and of selecting greater or lesser relative quantities of items, often after extensive training (Anderson et al. 2005, 2007; Beran 2001, 2007; Hauser, Carey and Hauser 2000; Jaakkola et al. 2005; Pepperberg 2006; Thomas et al. 1980; Washburn and Rumbaugh 1991; Vonk and Beran 2012; West and Young 2002). Furthermore, in many prior studies, these discriminations can be performed via a variety of mechanisms independent of true numerical representation, such as perceptual estimation of magnitude, and the use of cues such as surface area and ratio of background to foreground in two-dimensional stimuli. Procedures attempting to

2 control the influence of physical features involve presenting arbitrary stimuli such as dots on a computer screen, controlling for dot size, ratio, and density and having the subjects select the array of items that contains the greater or lesser number of items (Beran 2008; Brannon 2006; Cantlon and Brannon 2006; Vonk and Beran 2012). The results of these studies suggest that numerical estimation in non-humans may be more akin to magnitude estimation than true counting, as performance declines with increased ratio between sets, as predicted by Weber s law (Beran 2008; Brannon and Terrace 2000; Cantlon and Brannon 2006; Jordon and Brannon 2006). However, in some studies, the performance of both monkeys (Beran 2008) and black bears (Vonk and Beran 2012) remained high when amount or area was confounded with number, and even when enumerating subsets within moving arrays, hinting at numerical rather than magnitude representations. However, success under these circumstances tends to be achieved only after a large number of training trials, calling into question the extent to which numerosities are spontaneously represented in non-human species. The majority of studies on animals numerical abilities, and the only data on orangutans, focus on their abilities to make relative numerousness judgments (Anderson et al. 2007; Call 2000; Hanus and Call 2007; Shumaker et al. 2001; Uher and Call 2008). In contrast, the current study focuses on the ability of an adult male orangutan to match stimuli based on absolute numerosities of various twodimensional stimuli. Jordan and colleagues have shown that rhesus macaques can match stimuli representing the numerical value of random shapes or, cross-modally, the number of auditory sounds presented (Jordon and Brannon 2006; Jordan et al. 2008). Nieder et al. (2006) had previously shown that rhesus macaques could match numerosities, but matching paradigms have not commonly been used in other species. Here, a delayed matching-to-sample procedure was used where the sample was no longer visible at the time of test, making it necessary to maintain a representation of quantity in mind, as in Jordan et al. (2008). Orangutans have not previously been tested with regard to their ability to estimate absolute numerosities in a matching paradigm. The ability to match or label stimuli containing more than five elements on the basis of the number of elements in the stimuli might be taken as evidence for absolute numerical representation, assuming that features allowing for magnitude estimation are controlled. The greater number of items is important because items fewer than five can be subtilized (Davis and Perusse 1988; Dehaene 1992; Feigenson et al. 2002). Furthermore, most studies assessing numerosity have relied on either real food rewards as stimuli or abstract shapes such as dots. Prior studies have not tested an animal s ability to quantify the number of individuals in a photograph, and very few have used varying asymmetrical shapes in a two-dimensional stimulus array (see Thomas et al for one example). No such studies have been conducted with orangutans or other apes. These types of stimuli importantly provide natural controls for perceptual cues such as absolute size, magnitude, and density. Following these tests, the orangutan subject of the current study was presented with stimuli containing dot stimuli to provide a comparison of performance with natural and more commonly used, arbitrary, abstract stimuli. Few non-social species have been tested for the ability to estimate numerosities, leaving open the possibility that such an ability arose to track group members rather than to quantify food items (see also Vonk and Beran 2012). Orangutans, unlike many other primates previously tested in such paradigms, do not have the need to track group members. Orangutans have been underrepresented in many cognitive tasks compared to chimpanzees. In addition, the majority of the animals previously tested for their abilities to match, label, or rank numerical stimuli have undergone extensive training in the laboratory (Beran 2001, 2008; Beran et al. 2006; Brannon and Roitman 2003; Jordan et al. 2008). The current orangutan subject had participated in few experimental tasks (Vonk 2002, 2003; Vonk and MacDonald 2004), had never been trained to make any kind of numerical judgments or identity matches, and was not symbol or language trained. Thus, the current study is able to capture relatively untrained representations of quantities. Methods Subject The subject was a 22-year-old adult male Sumatran orangutan (Pongo abelii) named Molek who had participated in a number of touchscreen experiments involving the categorization of natural stimuli (Vonk 2002, 2003; Vonk and MacDonald 2004). None of the prior experiments had involved making numerousness judgements, and only one had involved the presentation of abstract stimuli (Vonk 2003). Molek had previously been trained on the matching-to-sample (MTS) task in a study where he matched items based on sameness of shape or color (Vonk 2003), taxonomic classification (Vonk, in preparation), and social relationship or physical activity (Vonk 2002). In each of the prior 6 experiments that involved matching, Molek participated in sessions. Importantly, Molek had not been trained on an identity matching task but had performed at high levels in the previous conceptual matching experiments. Molek was housed with a group of other orangutans at the Toronto zoo, Toronto, ON, Canada.

3 He participated in these experiments while housed separately off exhibit approximately three afternoons a week. Materials Within each discrimination, 24 novel color photographs or drawings were presented. There were six images within each of four number categories, and the same 24 images were used on each session. Individuals The first discrimination that was presented was the only one in which photographs of animals were used instead of drawings of abstract stimuli. A list of the stimuli appears in Appendix. Animals from various taxa appeared within each numerosity category such that sometimes the incorrect comparison might have appeared physically more similar to the sample. Molek had previously been trained to discriminate and match stimuli based on species and taxonomic categories (Vonk in preparation, Vonk and Mac- Donald 2004) and thus might be expected to make several errors in this experiment when required to attend to the number of elements within the stimulus instead. It was possible that the incorrect comparison stimulus sometimes might have depicted the same or closely related species as the animal(s) in the sample stimulus. An example of stimuli presented on one possible trial appears in Fig. 1 (top). Shapes In this discrimination, Molek was presented with stimuli that included differing numbers of abstract images which were non-symmetrical colored shapes on different colored backgrounds. The components of the stimuli were arranged into varying patterns so that all of the stimuli were perceptually distinct. Once again the number of elements within the stimuli ranged from one to four. An example of the type of stimuli presented on one possible trial appears in Fig. 1 (bottom). Notably, ratio of background to foreground, pattern of the elements, and absolute size of the elements of the stimuli could not be used to aid in discriminating stimuli of varying numerosities in the discriminations Individuals or Shapes. Uncontrolled 1 4 In this discrimination, Molek was presented with black dots on six different colored backgrounds. The background color of the stimuli was manipulated in order to determine whether the orangutan was distracted by perceptual Fig. 1 (top) An example of a possible combination of stimuli for a particular trial in the Individuals discrimination. Here, the sample appears at the top of the figure. On an actual trial, the sample stimulus appeared in the center of the monitor alone and disappeared after being selected by the subject. The two comparison stimuli, depicted at the bottom of the figure, then appeared. (bottom) An example of a possible combination of stimuli for a particular trial in the Shapes discrimination features of the stimuli unrelated to the relevant numerousness values of the stimuli. Thus, each stimulus of the same numerosity appeared on a different colored background, e.g., blue, green, pink, red, yellow, and white. The number of dots ranged from one to four, and the dots within each number value were arranged in the same pattern, except for the number three stimuli in which one of the exemplars (white background) was arranged in a different pattern from the other five exemplars. An example of stimuli presented on one possible trial appears in Fig. 2 (top). Note that here the incorrect comparison stimulus matches the background color of the sample, while the correct comparison stimulus does not. The pattern in which the three dots are displayed differs from the sample to the correct comparison.

4 stimuli with greater numerosities. However, this slight absolute difference in dot size may have provided another cue by which the orangutan may have performed the discriminations. 4 7 In this discrimination, Molek was presented with black dots on different colored backgrounds (yellow, blue, white, green, and red purple) and the number of dots ranged from four to seven. Within the sets of four and six dots, there were four different patterns. Two of the stimuli shared each of the first two arrangements, and only one stimulus was composed of each of the third and fourth arrangements. Within the five-dot set, there were three unique patterns, each shared by two of the stimuli. Within the set of seven dots, there were also four different patterns; however, three of the stimuli shared the same pattern, and the other three exemplars consisted of dots arranged into unique patterns. Once again the size of the dots was varied to control for the ratio of black dots to colored background. An example of stimuli presented on one possible trial appears in Fig. 2 (bottom). Note that here the dot pattern of the five dots in the sample is identical to the dot pattern in the correct stimulus. On other trials, the dot pattern in the sample stimulus would not match that of the correct stimulus Fig. 2 (top) An example of a possible combination of stimuli for a particular trial in the Uncontrolled 1 4 discrimination. Here the background color of the incorrect, but not the correct, comparison stimulus matches that of the sample. (bottom) An example of a possible combination of stimuli for a particular trial in the 4 7 discrimination. Here, the pattern of the dots in the sample and in the correct stimulus is the same Controlled 1 4 In this discrimination, Molek was presented with black dots on different colored backgrounds (same colors as above). The number of dots ranged from one to four, and the dots within each number value were arranged in the same pattern, except for the number three stimuli in which four of the exemplars were arranged in the same pattern and the other two exemplars (blue and white backgrounds) shared a different arrangement. The stimuli also differed from those in the previous discrimination in that the dots were made systematically smaller as the number of elements within the stimuli increased so as to control for the ratio of black dot pattern to background as well as the size of the individual elements of the stimuli. Thus, the proportion of figure to background size was manipulated in all subsequent discriminations such that the dots were smaller in Within this discrimination, the number of dots ranged from seven to ten and the arrangement of dots varied as follows. For the sets of eight, nine, and ten dots, there were three unique pattern arrangements shared by two differently colored background stimuli each. For the seven-dot set, there were four unique patterns. One pattern was shared by three of the stimuli. The other three stimuli within that set were arranged into unique patterns. Once again, the color of backgrounds varied within the stimuli (same colors as for 4 7) and the size of the dots were varied to control for the ratio of black dot to colored background. Procedure The experiment was programmed in Filemaker Pro 3 software and was run on a Macintosh 5300 PowerBook computer. The images were either digitally scanned photographs (Individuals) or were drawn in Deskpaint (All other sets). Each stimulus was approximately 3 by 4 on the 13 Apple touchscreen monitor. The sample stimulus was centered horizontally and vertically on the screen, whereas the two comparison images were separated by approximately 1.5 and were vertically aligned.

5 Testing took place in the holding area of the Toronto Zoo Indo-Malaya pavilion when the orangutan was off exhibit. The monitor was placed against the bars of the subject s holding cage and the subject reached through the mesh holes to touch the screen. The stimuli and the subject s responses could be viewed on the PowerBook placed behind the monitor. In these experiments, the experimenter sat behind the laptop, which was covered by a protective covering, connected to the touchscreen which was pushed right up against the orangutan s enclosure. The images on the touchscreen were mirror-reversed from the images on the laptop, and the experimenter always gazed directly at the midpoint of the screen. The experimenter could not see the orangutan s face or fingers, or the front of the touchscreen, when he made a response so could not react to the correctness or incorrectness of the choice until after the choice was made. The subject was tested at the same time each day and received one to four sessions per day three or four times a week. The subject was not food deprived in any way. Each session consisted of twelve trials. Therefore, each block of five sessions consisted of 60 trials. During a trial, a sample stimulus was presented in the center of the screen and stayed on screen until the subject attended to the sample and touched it; then it disappeared. The two comparison stimuli subsequently appeared on the screen after a brief delay (approximately 3 s). The subject was then required to select, by touching, only one of the two comparison stimuli. If he selected the stimulus that matched the number of elements depicted in the sample, he was given a small food reward (M and Ms or dried fruits and nuts) by hand. After selection of the comparison stimulus and reward, if required, the screen advanced to the next sample stimulus until all twelve trials were completed. There was no time-out for incorrect choices. During the twelve trials within a session, each of the four numerical values was depicted in the sample stimulus three times, with a different exemplar from the category being used for each of the three trials. Each number value was tested against each of the other three numerosities during each session, so that there would be, for instance, three trials where one element appeared in the sample. Different single element stimuli were used as the correct matches (or reinforced comparison stimuli) for these samples on all three of these trials. There were no identity matching trials. On one single element trial, for example, the non-reinforced comparison stimulus depicted two elements. On another single element trial, the nonreinforced comparison stimulus depicted three elements and, on the third trial, the non-reinforced comparison stimulus depicted four elements. Three stimuli within each numerousness value appeared twice within each session, while the other three stimuli within each numerousness value appeared only once per session. The stimuli were sorted randomly before each session. Therefore, the stimuli that appeared twice on some sessions appeared only once on other sessions. If a stimulus appeared twice within a session, it appeared as both a reinforced and a non-reinforced comparison stimulus on different trials, but not as a sample. Therefore, a stimulus that was correct once would be incorrect on its second presentation or vice versa. The stimuli that appeared as samples during a session appeared only once during that session. Side of presentation was counterbalanced so that half of the correct choices appeared on the left and half appeared on the right side of the screen. Order of presentation and pairing of stimuli were randomized. Therefore, each session involved many novel pairings of stimuli within trials. Molek completed twenty-five sessions with the first two discriminations (individuals and shapes), fifteen sessions with the dot patterns 1 4 and 4 7 and eight sessions with the 7 10 dot discrimination. The varying number of sessions reflects the attempt to obtain consistent above chance responding before moving on to novel stimuli. Molek acquired this consistent level of performance more rapidly as he progressed through the discriminations. Molek received the initial sessions with the discriminations in the order described above, although he was not required to reach any predetermined criterion on one type of discrimination before presentation of the next discrimination began. Thus, presentation of subsequent discriminations sometimes commenced before all of the sessions had been completed with the prior discrimination, but not until the first block of five sessions had been completed. Molek was sometimes presented with more than one type of discrimination on the same day in order to help keep the sessions more interesting for him. For each discrimination, binomial tests were conducted on Molek s score for each session of 12 trials. We report the first session for which Molek s performance was above chance for each discrimination. Additionally, we conducted binary logistic regressions of his scores with session number, difference, and ratio as factors. Results Individuals significantly above chance on the first session (two-tailed, n = 12, p = 0.03). Molek s percent correct across five session blocks (60 trials) appears in Fig. 3. A binary logistic regression of his score (correct versus incorrect) on each trial with the absolute numerical difference between

6 Fig. 3 Percent correct scores and standard errors for each block of five sessions (60 trials) in each of the discriminations the number of individuals in the sample and the incorrect choice (absolute difference hereafter), the ratio between the number of individuals in the sample and the incorrect choice (smaller # divided by larger #, ratio hereafter), and session as the independent variables revealed significant effects of absolute difference and ratio (odds ratios = and , p s \ and = 0.01). Note that odds ratios greater than 1.00 indicate that the likelihood of the subject being correct was greater with greater absolute numerical differences between the correct and incorrect stimuli, and greater ratios between them, consistent with Weber s law and predictions of the analog magnitude model. However, the relationship was not perfectly linear as predicted by the model. Table 1 presents Molek s percent correct (and standard errors) as a function of absolute differences between correct and incorrect comparison stimuli for each of the discriminations presented. Shapes significantly above chance on the second session (twotailed, n = 12, p = 0.04). Molek s percent correct across five session blocks (60 trials) appears in Fig. 3. A binomial logistic regression on score with absolute difference, ratio, and session as independent variables revealed significant effects of absolute difference and ratio (odds ratios = and , p s \ 0.001). Again, Molek s performance tended to improve as the absolute difference and ratio between the correct and incorrect stimuli increased (albeit not in a strictly linear fashion). Uncontrolled 1 4 above chance on the fourth session (two-tailed, n =12,p = 0.04). Molek s percent correct across five session blocks (60 trials) appears in Fig. 3. A binary logistic regression was performed on Molek s score on each trial with pattern (same or different), background color (same or different), session, absolute difference, and ratio as variables. There was a significant effect of both absolute difference and ratio on Molek s performance (odds ratios = and , p s = and 0.005, respectively), as well as an effect of session (odds ratio = 1.149, p = 0.005). Again, Molek s performance tended to improve as the absolute difference and ratio between the correct and incorrect stimuli increased (this time in a linear fashion). Performance was also higher in later sessions indicating an effect of learning with this discrimination. Controlled 1 4 significantly above chance on the third session (two-tailed, n = 12, p = 0.04). Molek s percent correct across five session blocks (60 trials) appears in Fig. 3. A binary logistic regression was performed on Molek s score on each trial with session, pattern (same or different), background color (same or different), absolute difference, and ratio as variables. There were significant effects of both absolute difference and session (odds ratios = and 1.116, respectively, both p s = 0.04). Consistent with performance on the previous discriminations, Molek s performance improved as the absolute difference and ratio between the correct and incorrect stimuli increased. 4 7 significantly above chance on the first session (two-tailed, n = 12, p\0.001) indicating that he was able to transfer to

7 Table 1 Percent correct (with standard error in parentheses) for each discrimination, as a function of the absolute numerical difference between the number of elements in the correct and incorrect comparison stimuli Absolute difference Individuals Shapes Uncontrolled (1 4) Controlled (1 4) (0.03) 0.69 (0.04) 0.78 (0.04) 0.80 (0.04) 0.79 (0.03) 0.75 (0.05) (0.04) 0.61 (0.04) 0.79 (0.05) 0.84 (0.05) 0.83 (0.04) 0.92 (0.07) (0.06) 0.96 (0.06) 0.87 (0.07) 0.98 (0.07) 0.70 (0.05) 0.65 (0.10) novel numerosities even though the number of trials on which the sample pattern matched that of the correct choice was substantially reduced. This is true because, in the previous discrimination, numerosities 1, 2, and 4 were always presented in the same pattern, and, in the current discrimination, the patterns of dots varied for all numerical stimuli. Molek s percent correct across five session blocks (60 trials) appears in Fig. 3. A binary logistic regression was performed on Molek s score on each trial with session, pattern (same or different), background color (same or different), absolute difference, and ratio as variables. There were significant effects of absolute difference, ratio, and session (odds ratios = 0.005, 0, and , respectively, all p s = 0.01). Here, the effect of session reflected a decline in performance in the last block of five sessions, rather than an effect of learning significantly above chance by the first session (two-tailed, n = 9 because three trials had to be discarded do to computer error, p = 0.04). Molek s percent correct across five session blocks (60 trials) appears in Fig. 3. A binary logistic regression was performed on Molek s score on each trial with session, pattern (same or different), background color (same or different), absolute difference, and ratio as variables. There were no significant effects (all p s [ 0.05). Discussion Molek quickly reached above chance levels of performance on all discriminations presented to him and showed a high degree of transfer to novel stimulus sets and numerosities. His performance was not influenced by whether the background color of the stimuli or the pattern of the dots in the correct stimulus matched that of the sample (in sets where pattern varied), indicating that he was not distracted or guided by irrelevant (or relevant) perceptual cues, other than the numerosity of the elements in the stimuli (see also Brannon and Terrace 2000; Jordon and Brannon 2006). Furthermore, the ratio of background to foreground in the stimuli did not appear to be a factor in his discrimination performance as this was not a reliable cue in the first two discrimination sets (Individuals and Shapes), and there was no difference in his performance when this feature was controlled or uncontrolled (see Fig. 3). Furthermore, performance did not increase when size of the individual dots could be used as a cue in some of the dot sets. This orangutan s high levels of accuracy in making these matching judgments with numerical stimuli are even more impressive in light of his previous training. He had previously been reinforced for matching stimuli according to biological relatedness in natural category experiments (Vonk in preparation, Vonk and MacDonald 2004). On some trials, making a correct choice in the discrimination involving individuals would be in conflict with that prior training, and yet Molek performed at high levels initially without extensive training. Molek demonstrated effects of learning on only one discrimination Uncontrolled 1 4, which involved the first presentation of dots as stimuli. Thus, for all other discriminations, it appears that accurate matching was not as a function of learning. During some discriminations, performance actually declined across sessions (significantly with discrimination 4 7), an effect which might possibly be explained as boredom with the stimuli. Notably, Molek s performance was perfect on trial one with this stimulus set. Language-trained chimpanzees may also be able to make absolute numerousness judgments spontaneously (Hayes and Nissen 1971; Woodruff and Premack 1981). Extensive training with the use of symbols may have allowed chimpanzee subjects to hone skills not normally called for in a more typical rearing environment. Molek s performance is notable as he was not language or symbol-trained and had experience making only natural category and relational discriminations. Rhesus macaques have been trained to match stimuli up to numerosities of twelve (Jordon and Brannon 2006). However, in the first experiment(s) by these authors, subjects were not presented with a range of distracters they merely discriminated between 2 and 8 and between 3 and 12 in training. Later, they were required to choose matches from a range of numerosities 3 7 and 4 10, but their performance was evaluated to determine whether they estimated larger quantities as being more similar to larger values and vice versa. In experiment 2, they were required

8 to make absolute matches and discrimination was at chance levels for many of the number pairs particularly those farther removed from the trained numerosities of 2, 3, 8, and 9. Sarah, a language-trained chimpanzee could match perceptually dissimilar items on the basis of proportion or numbers 1 4 (Woodruff and Premack 1981). A gray parrot, Alex, has been trained to match stimulus arrays of varying numerosities to symbols representing the cardinal value of the set. Alex can correctly label up to six items independently of mass or contour (Pepperberg 1994; Pepperberg and Gordon 2005). The orangutan, Molek s performance is in line with these findings from other species, suggesting that he can match quantities at least to ten and is likely using a magnitude estimation process. Most of the recent research conducted with nonhuman subjects has indicated a significant and linear effect of ratio and scalar distance on the animals performance (Beran and Rumbaugh 2001; Biro and Matsuzawa 2001; Cantlon and Brannon 2006; Emmerton 1998; Flombaum et al. 2004; Jaakkola et al. 2005; Jordon and Brannon 2006; Smithetal.2003). In addition, rhesus macaques demonstrate more uncertainty and make more errors when making numerosity judgements between stimuli closer to the target value (Beran et al. 2006). Consistent with data reported for other nonhuman species (in pigeons, Emmerton 1998; indolphins, Jaakkola et al in chimpanzees and rhesus macaques, Beran et al. 2006; Beran and Rumbaugh 2001; Biro and Matsuzawa 2001; Cantlon and Brannon 2006; Flombaum et al. 2004; Jordan and Brannon 2006; andin squirrel monkeys and a hamadryas baboon, Smith et al. 2003, see also Olthof et al. 1997), Molek s performance was affected by both the absolute difference and ratio between correct and incorrect stimuli, consistent with the analog magnitude model. The current results are the first reported data on an orangutan s ability to make absolute rather than relative numerical judgments. The data from the individuals and shapes conditions are most compelling because these stimuli did not contain confounds such as dot size present in the later stimuli. Orangutans have previously been shown to outperform chimpanzees in a task requiring them to inhibit the tendency to respond to larger quantities of food rewards (Shumaker et al. 2001). Perhaps orangutans have a more highly developed representation of quantity than other non-human primates tested thus far. This finding would be of interest given that orangutans are less social and would suggest that tracking group members is not critical for the evolution of numerical competence. It is also possible that this particular subject performed especially well on conceptual matching-to-sample tasks because he had not been over-trained on identity matching tasks (see also Vonk et al. 2012). Future studies comparing training methods are necessary to elucidate the role of training in establishing a mechanism for making numerousness judgments in various species. Acknowledgments This research was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) Postgraduate scholarship to the author. The cooperation and support of the staff at the Toronto Zoo was greatly appreciated. Special thanks to Bev Carter, Karyn Tunwell, Bridget Burke-Johnson, Andrea Beatson, Rick Vos, David Partington, Des Macguire, and Jackie Craig, without whose assistance these experiments would not have been possible. I also thank Suzanne MacDonald for providing opportunity and the equipment and Jon Toth for building the protective covering. Thanks to Michael J. Beran and Kelly Jaakola for insightful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. Appendix Number of elements Animal depicted 1 Tree boa Meadow vole Walrus Stork Infant rhesus macaque Butterfly fish 2 Badgers Fish Olive baboons Chacma baboon mother and infant Egret chicks Canadian geese 3 Brown Rats Human adult females Orangutans Fish Emus Chimpanzees 4 Burrowing owlets Penguins Orangutans Adult human females Ringtail Lemurs Chimpanzees References Agrillo C, Dadda M, Serena G, Bisazza A (2009) Use of number by fish. PLoS ONE 4:e4786. doi: /journal.pone Anderson US, Stoinski TS, Bloomsmith MA, Marr MJ, Smith AD, Maple TL (2005) Relative numerousness judgment and summation in young and old western lowland gorillas. J Comp Psychol 119:

9 Anderson US, Stoinski TS, Bloomsmith MA, Maple TL (2007) Relative numerousness judgment and summation in young, middle-aged, and older adult orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii and Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 121:1 11. doi: / Baker JM, Morath J, Rodzon KS, Jordan KE (2012) A shared system of representation governing quantity discrimination in canids. Frontiers Psychol 3. doi: /fpsyg Beran MJ (2001) Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115: Beran MJ (2007) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) enumerate sequentially presented sets of items using analog numerical representations. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 33:42 54 Beran MJ (2008) Monkeys (Macaca mulatta and Cebus apella) track, enumerate and compare multiple sets of moving items. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 34:63 74 Beran MJ, Rumbaugh DM (2001) Constructive enumeration by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) on a computerized task. Anim Cog 4:81 89 Beran MJ, Smith JD, Redford JS, Washburn DA (2006) Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) monitor uncertainty during numerosity judgments. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 32: Biro D, Matsuzawa T (2001) Chimpanzee numerical competence: cardinal and ordinal skills. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, New York, pp Brannon EM (2006) The representation of numerical magnitude. Curr Opinion Neurobiol 16: Brannon EM, Roitman JD (2003) Nonverbal representations of time and number in animals and human infants. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, pp doi: / ch6 Brannon EM, Terrace HS (2000) Representation of the numerosities 1 9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 26:31 49 Call J (2000) Estimating and operating on discrete quantities in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 114: Cantlon JF, Brannon EM (2006) Shared system for ordering small and large numbers in monkeys and humans. Psycho Sci 17: Davis H, Perusse R (1988) Numerical competence in animals: definitional issues, current evidence and a new research agenda. Behav Brain Sci 11: Dehaene S (1992) Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition 44:1 42 Dehaene S (1997) The number sense: how the mind creates mathematics. Oxford University Press, New York Emmerton J (1998) Numerosity differences and effects of stimulus density on pigeons discrimination performance. Anim Learn Behav 26: Feigenson L, Carey S, Hauser M (2002) The representations underlying infants choice of more: object files versus analog magnitudes. Psychol Sci 13: Flombaum JI, Junge JA, Hauser MD (2004) Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers. Cognition 97: Garland A, Low J, Burns KC (2012) Large quantity discrimination by north island robins (Petroica longipes). Anim Cog 15: doi: /s Gomez-Laplaza LM, Gerlai R (2011) Can angelfish (Pterophyllum scalare) count? Discrimination among different shoal sizes follows Weber s law. Anim Cogn 14:1 9 Hanus D, Call J (2007) Discrete quantity judgments in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus): the effect of presenting whole sets versus item-by-item. J Comp Psychol 121: doi: / Hauser MD, Carey S, Hauser LB (2000) Spontaneous number representation in semi-free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Proc R Soc Lond Biol Sci 267: Hayes KJ, Nissen CH (1971) Higher mental function of a home-raised chimpanzee. In: Schrier AM, Stollnitz F (eds) Behavior of nonhuman primates. Academic Press, New York, pp Jaakkola K, Fellner W, Erb L, Rodriguez M, Guarino E (2005) Understanding of the concept Numerically less by Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). J Comp Psychol 119: Jordan KE, MacLean EL, Brannon EM (2008) Monkeys match and tally quantities across senses. Cognition 108: doi: /j.cognition Jordon KE, Brannon EM (2006) Weber s law influences numerical representations in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Anim Cog 9: Kilian A, Yaman S, von Fersen L, Gunturkun O (2003) A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity. Learn Behav 31: Nieder A, Diester I, Tudusciuc O (2006) Temporal and spatial enumeration processes in the primate parietal cortex. Science 313: doi: /science Olthof A, Iden CM, Roberts WA (1997) Judgments of ordinality and summation of number symbols by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 23: Pepperberg IM (1994) Numerical competence in an African gray parrot (Psittacus Erithacus). J Comp Psychol 108:36 44 Pepperberg IM (2006) Ordinality and inferential abilities of a grey parrot (Psittacus Erithacus). J Comp Psychol 120: Pepperberg IM, Gordon JD (2005) Number comprehension by a grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), including a zero-like concept. J Comp Psychol 119: Perdue BM, Talbot CF, Stone AM, Beran MJ (2012) Putting the elephant back in the herd: elephant relative quantity judgments match those of other species. Anim Cog 15: doi: /s y Shumaker RW, Palkovich AM, Beck BB, Guagnano GA, Morowitz H (2001) Spontaneous use of magnitude discrimination and ordination by the orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 115: Smith BR, Piel AK, Candland DK (2003) Numerity of a socially housed hamadryas baboon (Papio hamadryas) and a socially housed squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). J Comp Psychol 117: Thomas RK, Fowlkes D, Vickery JD (1980) Conceptual numerousness judgements by squirrel monkeys. Am J Psycho 93: Uher J, Call J (2008) How the great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla) perform on the reversed reward contingency task II: transfer to new quantities, long-term retention, and the impact of quantity ratios. J Comp Psychol 122: doi: / Uller C, Jaeger R, Guidry G, Martin C (2003) Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more: rudiments of number in an amphibian. Anim Cogn 6: Vonk J (2002) Can Orangutans (Pongo abelii) and Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) acquire concepts for social relationships? Int J Comp Cog 15: Vonk J (2003) Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and Orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of first and second order relations. Anim Cog 6:77 86 Vonk J, Beran MJ (2012) Bears count too: quantity estimation and comparison in black bears, ursus americanus. Anim Behav 84: doi: /j.anbehav Vonk J, MacDonald SE (2004) Levels of abstraction in Orangutan (Pongo abelii) categorization. J Comp Psychol 118:3 13

10 Vonk J, Jett SE, Mosteller KW (2012) Concept formation in American black bears (Ursus americanus). Anim Behav 84: Washburn DA, Rumbaugh DM (1991) Ordinal judgements of numerical symbols by macaques (Macaca mulatta). Psychol Sci 2: West RE, Young RJ (2002) Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count? Anim Cog 5: Woodruff G, Premack D (1981) Primitive mathematical concepts in the chimpanzee: proportionality and numerosity. Nature 293:

Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) select Arabic numerals or visual quantities corresponding to a number of sequentially completed maze trials

Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) select Arabic numerals or visual quantities corresponding to a number of sequentially completed maze trials Learning & Behavior 7, 35 (1), 53-5 Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) select Arabic numerals or visual quantities corresponding to a number of sequentially completed maze trials EMILY H. HARRIS, DAVID A.

More information

Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of first- and second-order relations

Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of first- and second-order relations Anim Cogn (2003) 6 : 77 86 DOI 10.1007/s10071-003-0159-x ORIGINAL ARTICLE Jennifer Vonk Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of first- and second-order relations

More information

Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Counting in a Computerized Testing Paradigm

Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Counting in a Computerized Testing Paradigm Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Language Research Center Language Research Center 1998 Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Counting in a Computerized Testing Paradigm Michael

More information

The Influence of Task Specific Factors on Quantity Discrimination by Brown Capuchins (Cebus apella) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus)

The Influence of Task Specific Factors on Quantity Discrimination by Brown Capuchins (Cebus apella) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) Bucknell University Bucknell Digital Commons Honors Theses Student Theses 2016 The Influence of Task Specific Factors on Quantity Discrimination by Brown Capuchins (Cebus apella) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri

More information

Animal Arithmetic. Non-humans Represent Numbers

Animal Arithmetic. Non-humans Represent Numbers Animal Arithmetic J. F. Cantlon, Rochester University, Rochester, NC, USA E. M. Brannon, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA ã 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Non-humans Represent Numbers Early reports

More information

Levels of Abstraction in Orangutan (Pongo abelii) Categorization

Levels of Abstraction in Orangutan (Pongo abelii) Categorization Journal of Comparative Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2004, Vol. 118, No. 1, 3 13 0735-7036/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.118.1.3 Levels of Abstraction in

More information

Discrimination of small quantities by fish (redtail splitfin, Xenotoca eiseni)

Discrimination of small quantities by fish (redtail splitfin, Xenotoca eiseni) Anim Cogn (2013) 16:307 312 DOI 10.1007/s10071-012-0590-y SHORT COMMUNICATION Discrimination of small quantities by fish (redtail splitfin, Xenotoca eiseni) Gionata Stancher Valeria Anna Sovrano Davide

More information

Humans and nonhuman animals discriminate numbers in a

Humans and nonhuman animals discriminate numbers in a Semantic congruity affects numerical judgments similarly in monkeys and humans Jessica F. Cantlon* and Elizabeth M. Brannon* Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience,

More information

Representational format determines numerical competence in monkeys

Representational format determines numerical competence in monkeys Received 10 Nov 2010 Accepted 2 Mar 2011 Published 29 Mar 2011 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms1262 Representational format determines numerical competence in monkeys Vanessa Schmitt 1,2 & Julia Fischer 1,2 A range

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

Basic Math in Monkeys and College Students

Basic Math in Monkeys and College Students Basic Math in Monkeys and College Students PLoS BIOLOGY Jessica F. Cantlon *, Elizabeth M. Brannon Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham,

More information

The Role of Reference Points in Ordinal Numerical Comparisons by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta)

The Role of Reference Points in Ordinal Numerical Comparisons by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 2, 120 134 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.2.120

More information

Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans Jessica F. Cantlon and Elizabeth M. Brannon

Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans Jessica F. Cantlon and Elizabeth M. Brannon PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans Jessica F. Cantlon and Elizabeth M. Brannon Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences

More information

Relative numerosity discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence for approximate numerical representations

Relative numerosity discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence for approximate numerical representations Anim Cogn (28) 11:43 57 DOI 1.17/s171-7-89- ORIGINAL PAPER Relative numerosity discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence for approximate numerical representations Masaki Tomonaga Received:

More information

Large Number Discrimination in Newborn Fish

Large Number Discrimination in Newborn Fish Laura Piffer*, Maria Elena Miletto Petrazzini, Christian Agrillo Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy Abstract Quantitative abilities have been reported in a wide range

More information

Arithmetic in newborn chicks Rosa Rugani 1, *, Laura Fontanari 1, Eleonora Simoni 2, Lucia Regolin 2 and Giorgio Vallortigara 1

Arithmetic in newborn chicks Rosa Rugani 1, *, Laura Fontanari 1, Eleonora Simoni 2, Lucia Regolin 2 and Giorgio Vallortigara 1 Arithmetic in newborn chicks Rosa Rugani 1, *, Laura Fontanari 1, Eleonora Simoni 2, Lucia Regolin 2 and Giorgio Vallortigara 1 276, 2451 2460 doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0044 Published online 1 April 2009 1

More information

Numerical discrimination by frogs (Bombina orientalis)

Numerical discrimination by frogs (Bombina orientalis) Anim Cogn (2015) 18:219 229 DOI 10.1007/s10071-014-0791-7 ORIGINAL PAPER Numerical discrimination by frogs (Bombina orientalis) G. Stancher R. Rugani L. Regolin G. Vallortigara Received: 24 February 2014

More information

Numerical Cognition in Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta)

Numerical Cognition in Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Dissertations Department of Psychology 7-15-2008 Numerical Cognition in Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Emily Harris Marr Georgia

More information

Petra H. J. M. Vlamings, Jana Uher, and Josep Call. Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

Petra H. J. M. Vlamings, Jana Uher, and Josep Call. Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 1, 60 70 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.1.60 How

More information

Comparative Studies of Numerical Cognition in Nonhuman Primates: From Numerical Comparison to Arithmetic. Sarah Mychal Jones

Comparative Studies of Numerical Cognition in Nonhuman Primates: From Numerical Comparison to Arithmetic. Sarah Mychal Jones Comparative Studies of Numerical Cognition in Nonhuman Primates: From Numerical Comparison to Arithmetic by Sarah Mychal Jones Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Duke University Date: Approved:

More information

NATURAL CONCEPTS IN A JUVENILE GORILLA (GORILLA GORILLA GORILLA) AT THREE LEVELS OF ABSTRACTION JENNIFER VONK AND SUZANNE E.

NATURAL CONCEPTS IN A JUVENILE GORILLA (GORILLA GORILLA GORILLA) AT THREE LEVELS OF ABSTRACTION JENNIFER VONK AND SUZANNE E. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR,, NUMBER (NOVEMBER) NATURAL CONCEPTS IN A JUVENILE GORILLA (GORILLA GORILLA GORILLA) AT THREE LEVELS OF ABSTRACTION JENNIFER VONK AND SUZANNE E. MACDONALD

More information

Representation of the Numerosities 1-9 by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatto)

Representation of the Numerosities 1-9 by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatto) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Betuvkx Processes 2000, Vol. 26, No. 1,31-49 Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/QQ/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0097-7403.26.1.31

More information

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Elizabeth M. Brannon*, Courtney J. Wusthoff #, C.R. Gallistel^, and John Gibbon @ * Center for Cognitive Neuroscience

More information

Evidence for Two Numerical Systems That Are Similar in Humans and Guppies

Evidence for Two Numerical Systems That Are Similar in Humans and Guppies Evidence for Two Numerical Systems That Are Similar in Humans and Guppies Christian Agrillo 1 *, Laura Piffer 1, Angelo Bisazza 1, Brian Butterworth 2 1 Department of General Psychology, University of

More information

SPACE, TIME AND NUMBER IN THE BRAIN SEARCHING FOR THE FOUNDATIONS OF MATHEMATICAL THOUGHT

SPACE, TIME AND NUMBER IN THE BRAIN SEARCHING FOR THE FOUNDATIONS OF MATHEMATICAL THOUGHT SPACE, TIME AND NUMBER IN THE BRAIN SEARCHING FOR THE FOUNDATIONS OF MATHEMATICAL THOUGHT SPACE, TIME AND NUMBER IN THE BRAIN SEARCHING FOR THE FOUNDATIONS OF MATHEMATICAL THOUGHT AN ATTENTION AND PERFORMANCE

More information

Manuscript Version Sage holds the Copyright. Introduction. Seemingly, no two investigators agree on what intelligence means or includes, but

Manuscript Version Sage holds the Copyright. Introduction. Seemingly, no two investigators agree on what intelligence means or includes, but 1 Thomas, R.K. (2016). Intelligence, Evolution of. In H L. Miller (Ed.). Encyclopedia of Theory in Psychology (pp. 454-456). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Manuscript Version Sage holds the Copyright

More information

Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus lunulatus) solve the reverse contingency task without a modiwed procedure

Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus lunulatus) solve the reverse contingency task without a modiwed procedure Anim Cogn (2007) 10:387 396 DOI 10.1007/s10071-007-0076-5 ORIGINAL PAPER Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus lunulatus) solve the reverse contingency task without a modiwed procedure Anna Albiach-Serrano Federico

More information

Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans 1

Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans 1 Japanese Psychological Research 1997, Volume 39, No. 3, 236 252 Special Issue: Cognition and behavior of chimpanzees Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans 1 NOBUAKI

More information

Large quantity discrimination by North Island robins (Petroica longipes)

Large quantity discrimination by North Island robins (Petroica longipes) DOI 10.1007/s10071-012-0537-3 ORIGINAL PAPER Large quantity discrimination by North Island robins (Petroica longipes) Alexis Garland Jason Low Kevin C. Burns Received: 31 October 2011 / Revised: 25 June

More information

Evaluating information-seeking approaches to metacognition

Evaluating information-seeking approaches to metacognition Current Zoology 57 (4): 531 542, 2011 Evaluating information-seeking approaches to metacognition Jonathon D. CRYSTAL 1*, Allison L. FOOTE 2 1 Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University,

More information

Common Representations of Abstract Quantities Sara Cordes, Christina L. Williams, and Warren H. Meck

Common Representations of Abstract Quantities Sara Cordes, Christina L. Williams, and Warren H. Meck CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Common Representations of Abstract Quantities Sara Cordes, Christina L. Williams, and Warren H. Meck Duke University ABSTRACT Representations of abstract quantities

More information

How Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) Quantify Objects and Substances

How Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) Quantify Objects and Substances How Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) Quantify Objects and Substances Kristy vanmarle, Justine Aw, Koleen McCrink, & Laurie R. Santos Yale University Humans and nonhuman animals appear to share a capacity

More information

Lecture. Experimental Models of Introspection in the Animal

Lecture. Experimental Models of Introspection in the Animal Introspection and metacognition: Mechanisms of self-knowledge Stanislas Dehaene Chair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture Experimental Models of Introspection in the Animal Course translated from

More information

Memory for "What", "Where", and "When" Information by Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca Mulatta) and Adult Humans

Memory for What, Where, and When Information by Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca Mulatta) and Adult Humans Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Theses Department of Psychology 11-27-2007 Memory for "What", "Where", and "When" Information by Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca Mulatta)

More information

Further evidence for addition and numerical competence by a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus)

Further evidence for addition and numerical competence by a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) DOI 10.1007/s10071-012-0470-5 SHORT COMMUNICATION Further evidence for addition and numerical competence by a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) Irene M. Pepperberg Received: 19 August 2011 / Revised: 3

More information

A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity

A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity Learning & Behavior 2003, 31 (2), 133-142 A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity ANNETTE KILIAN Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany and Tiergarten Nürnberg, Nürnberg,

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

Activity counts: the effect of swimming activity on quantity discrimination in fish

Activity counts: the effect of swimming activity on quantity discrimination in fish ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 12 November 2012 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00484 Activity counts: the effect of swimming activity on quantity discrimination in fish Luis M. Gómez-Laplaza 1 * and Robert

More information

Non-visual numerical discrimination in a blind cavefish (Phreatichthys andruzzii)

Non-visual numerical discrimination in a blind cavefish (Phreatichthys andruzzii) 2014. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd (2014) 217, 1902-1909 doi:10.1242/jeb.101683 RESEARCH ARTICLE Non-visual numerical discrimination in a blind cavefish (Phreatichthys andruzzii) Angelo Bisazza

More information

Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Maintain Learning Set Despite Second-order Stimulusresponse Spatial Discontiguity

Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Maintain Learning Set Despite Second-order Stimulusresponse Spatial Discontiguity Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Language Research Center Language Research Center 27 Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Maintain Learning Set Despite Second-order Stimulusresponse

More information

Retrospective and prospective metacognitive judgments in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)

Retrospective and prospective metacognitive judgments in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) DOI 10.1007/s10071-013-0657-4 ORIGINAL PAPER Retrospective and prospective metacognitive judgments in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) Gin Morgan Nate Kornell Tamar Kornblum Herbert S. Terrace Received:

More information

. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN

. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN 1 Roger K. Thomas The University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Unites States of America rkthomas@uga.edu Synonyms. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN Pre-publication manuscript version. Published as follows: Thomas,

More information

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons by Thomas A. Daniel A thesis proposal submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Auburn University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree

More information

AUDREY E. PARRISH, Ph.D.

AUDREY E. PARRISH, Ph.D. AUDREY E. PARRISH, Ph.D. The Citadel Office: 843-953- 5328 audrey.parrish@citadel.edu Capers Hall 324- A EDUCATION 2015 Doctorate of Philosophy (Ph.D.), Psychology Cognitive Sciences Georgia State University,

More information

How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled?

How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 21, 72 (7), 1839-1853 doi:1758/app.72.7.1839 How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled? MIDORI TOKITA

More information

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Sarah J. Miles (smiles25@uwo.ca) and John Paul Minda (jpminda@uwo.ca) Department of Psychology The University of Western Ontario London, ON N6A 5C2 Abstract

More information

Cylinder Task. Author Information Juan F. Duque University of Nebraska-Lincoln Lincoln, Nebraska, USA

Cylinder Task. Author Information Juan F. Duque University of Nebraska-Lincoln Lincoln, Nebraska, USA Author Information Juan F. Duque University of Nebraska-Lincoln Lincoln, Nebraska, USA jfduque89@gmail.com Cylinder Task Jeffrey R. Stevens University of Nebraska-Lincoln Lincoln, Nebraska, USA jeffrey.r.stevens@gmail.com

More information

Rats Show Adaptive Choice in a Metacognitive Task With High Uncertainty

Rats Show Adaptive Choice in a Metacognitive Task With High Uncertainty Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2017 American Psychological Association 2017, Vol. 43, No. 1, 109 118 2329-8456/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000130 Rats Show

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Performance on a Face Discrimination Task by Orangutans Reflects a Possible Interaction between Familiarity and Novelty

Performance on a Face Discrimination Task by Orangutans Reflects a Possible Interaction between Familiarity and Novelty Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Theses Department of Psychology Summer 8-2012 Performance on a Face Discrimination Task by Orangutans Reflects a Possible Interaction

More information

Orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) Spatial Memory and Behavior in a Foraging Task

Orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) Spatial Memory and Behavior in a Foraging Task Journal of Comparative Psychology 1999, Vol. 113, No. 2, 213-217 Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7036/99/S3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii)

More information

Cognitive Evaluation of Natural Stimuli Sequencing Tasks in Rhesus Monkeys. (Macaca mulatta) Madeline Zimmerman. Briarcliff High School

Cognitive Evaluation of Natural Stimuli Sequencing Tasks in Rhesus Monkeys. (Macaca mulatta) Madeline Zimmerman. Briarcliff High School Zimmerman, Mad. 1 Cognitive Evaluation of Natural Stimuli Sequencing Tasks in Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Madeline Zimmerman Briarcliff High School 1 Zimmerman, Mad. 2 Acknowledgements First and foremost,

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Journal of Experimental Psychology: General The Interplay Between Nonsymbolic Number and Its Continuous Visual Properties Titia Gebuis and Bert Reynvoet Online First Publication, November 14, 2011. doi:

More information

How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task

How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task Anim Cogn (2006) 9: 193 199 DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0019-6 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Nicholas J. Mulcahy Josep Call How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task Received: 19 October 2005 / Revised: 20 January

More information

Relative quantity judgments in South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens)

Relative quantity judgments in South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens) Anim Cogn (2011) 14:695 706 DOI 10.1007/s10071-011-0404-7 ORIGINAL PAPER Relative quantity judgments in South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens) José Z. Abramson Victoria Hernández-Lloreda Josep Call

More information

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2009) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Child Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp Children s

More information

AUDREY E. PARRISH, Ph.D.

AUDREY E. PARRISH, Ph.D. AUDREY E. PARRISH, Ph.D. The Citadel audrey.parrish@citadel.edu Office: 843-953-5328 Capers Hall 324-A EDUCATION 2015 Doctorate of Philosophy (Ph.D.), Psychology Cognitive Sciences Georgia State University,

More information

Running Head: SENSITIVITY TO TAXNOMIC ASSOCIATIONS

Running Head: SENSITIVITY TO TAXNOMIC ASSOCIATIONS Taxonomic Association Sensitivity 1 Running Head: SENSITIVITY TO TAXNOMIC ASSOCIATIONS Sensitivity to Purely Taxonomic Associations among Young Children Wyatt D Emilia Carnegie Mellon University Taxonomic

More information

Animal Cognition. Introduction to Cognitive Science

Animal Cognition. Introduction to Cognitive Science Animal Cognition Introduction to Cognitive Science Intelligent Animals? Parrot Intelligence Crow Intelligence I Crow Intelligence II Cow Intelligence Orca Intelligence Dolphin Play Funny Animal Intelligence

More information

Topics in Animal Cognition. Oliver W. Layton

Topics in Animal Cognition. Oliver W. Layton Topics in Animal Cognition Oliver W. Layton October 9, 2014 About Me Animal Cognition Animal Cognition What is animal cognition? Critical thinking: cognition or learning? What is the representation

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Comp Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 October 12.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Comp Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 October 12. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Comp Psychol. 2007 November ; 121(4): 363 371. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.121.4.363. A Comparative Analysis of Serial Ordering in Ring-Tailed

More information

Intuitive optics: what great apes infer from mirrors and shadows

Intuitive optics: what great apes infer from mirrors and shadows Animal Cognition (2018) 21:493 512 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1184-0 ORIGINAL PAPER Intuitive optics: what great apes infer from mirrors and shadows Christoph J. Völter 1,2 Josep Call 1,2 Received:

More information

PAPER Intuitions about gravity and solidity in great apes: the tubes task

PAPER Intuitions about gravity and solidity in great apes: the tubes task Developmental Science 13:2 (2010), pp 320 330 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00881.x PAPER Intuitions about and solidity in great apes: the tubes task Trix Cacchione 1 and Josep Call 2 1. Department of

More information

AUDREY E. PARRISH, Ph.D.

AUDREY E. PARRISH, Ph.D. AUDREY E. PARRISH, Ph.D. The Citadel Office: 843-953- 5328 audrey.parrish@citadel.edu Capers Hall 324- A EDUCATION 2015 Doctorate of Philosophy (Ph.D.), Psychology Cognitive Sciences Georgia State University,

More information

NUMEROSITY DISCRIMINATION IN PRESCHOOL CHILDREN ALZIRA ALMEIDA, JOANA ARANTES, AND ARMANDO MACHADO

NUMEROSITY DISCRIMINATION IN PRESCHOOL CHILDREN ALZIRA ALMEIDA, JOANA ARANTES, AND ARMANDO MACHADO JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2007, 88, 339 354 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) NUMEROSITY DISCRIMINATION IN PRESCHOOL CHILDREN ALZIRA ALMEIDA, JOANA ARANTES, AND ARMANDO MACHADO UNIVERSITY OF MINHO,

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Long-term memory for concepts by squirrel monkeys

Long-term memory for concepts by squirrel monkeys Physiological Psychology 1984, Vol.12 (2), 97-102 Long-term memory for concepts by squirrel monkeys LEONARD E. BURDYN, JR., LINDA M. NOBLE, LORELEI E. SHREVES, and ROGER K. THOMAS University of Georgia,

More information

RESULTS Human vs Macaques

RESULTS Human vs Macaques 14 RESULTS Human vs Macaques As mentioned before, this experiment aimed to test the ability of monkeys in categorizing monkeys and humans into two different conceptual classes. The two classes were presented

More information

Andrea W. Clay a b, Mollie A. Bloomsmith a b, M. Jackson Marr b & Terry L. Maple b a Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory

Andrea W. Clay a b, Mollie A. Bloomsmith a b, M. Jackson Marr b & Terry L. Maple b a Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 08:19 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Chronometric Studies of Numerical Cognition in Five-month-old Infants. Justin N. Wood & Elizabeth S. Spelke Harvard University

Chronometric Studies of Numerical Cognition in Five-month-old Infants. Justin N. Wood & Elizabeth S. Spelke Harvard University Chronometric Studies of Numerical Cognition in Five-month-old Infants Justin N. Wood & Elizabeth S. Spelke Harvard University Address correspondence to: Justin N. Wood Laboratory for Developmental Studies

More information

What does an intermediate success rate mean? An analysis of a Piagetian liquid conservation task in the great apes

What does an intermediate success rate mean? An analysis of a Piagetian liquid conservation task in the great apes Cognition 99 (2006) 53 71 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit What does an intermediate success rate mean? An analysis of a Piagetian liquid conservation task in the great apes Chikako Suda*, Josep Call Max

More information

MICHAEL J. BERAN, PH.D.

MICHAEL J. BERAN, PH.D. Jump to: Education Honors Positions Grant Funding Associations and Service Mentoring Books Journal Publications Book Chapters and Articles Book Reviews Abstracts Invited Presentations Conference Presentations

More information

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

CANTAB Test descriptions by function CANTAB Test descriptions by function The 22 tests in the CANTAB battery may be divided into the following main types of task: screening tests visual memory tests executive function, working memory and

More information

References. Carter, D.E., & Werner, T.J. (1978). Complex learning and information processing by

References. Carter, D.E., & Werner, T.J. (1978). Complex learning and information processing by References Carter, D.E., & Werner, T.J. (1978). Complex learning and information processing by pigeons: a critical analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 565-601. Cook, R.G., Katz,

More information

Not Knowing What One Knows: A Meaningful Failure of Metacognition in Capuchin Monkeys

Not Knowing What One Knows: A Meaningful Failure of Metacognition in Capuchin Monkeys Animal Behavior and Cognition Attribution 3.0 Unported (CC BY 3.0) ABC 2018, 5(1):55-67 https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.05.01.05.2018 Not Knowing What One Knows: A Meaningful Failure of Metacognition in Capuchin

More information

Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus californianus)

Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (4), 427 435 Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) DAVID KASTAK University of California, Santa Cruz, California

More information

Piagetian Liquid Conservation in the Great Apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, and Pongo pygmaeus)

Piagetian Liquid Conservation in the Great Apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, and Pongo pygmaeus) Journal of Comparative Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 118, No. 3, 265 279 0735-7036/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.118.3.265 Piagetian Liquid Conservation

More information

Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel

Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel 1 This is an in press article to appear in Psychological Science, summer 2006 Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel Justin Halberda, Johns Hopkins University Sean F. Sires,

More information

Apes use of iconic cues in the object-choice task

Apes use of iconic cues in the object-choice task Anim Cogn (2006) 9 DOI 10.1007/s10071-005-0013-4 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Esther Herrmann Alicia P. Melis Michael Tomasello Apes use of iconic cues in the object-choice task Received: 21 December 2004 / Revised:

More information

Visual Search for Human Gaze Direction by a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)

Visual Search for Human Gaze Direction by a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Visual Search for Human Gaze Direction by a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Masaki Tomonaga*, Tomoko Imura Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi, Japan Abstract Background: Humans detect

More information

Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli

Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli Behav Res (2011) 43:981 986 DOI 10.3758/s13428-011-0097-5 Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli Titia Gebuis & Bert Reynvoet Published online: 22 April 2011 # Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2011 Abstract Studies

More information

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1996, 3 (2), 222 226 Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana and THOMAS J. PALMERI Vanderbilt University,

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2005 Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli William

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training. Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral training. a, Mazes used for behavioral training. Asterisks indicate reward location. Only some example mazes are shown (for example, right choice and not left choice maze

More information

PROBLEM-SOLVING WITH ORANGUTANS (PONGO PYGMAEUS AND PONGO ABELII) AND CHIMPANZEES (PAN TROGLODYTES):

PROBLEM-SOLVING WITH ORANGUTANS (PONGO PYGMAEUS AND PONGO ABELII) AND CHIMPANZEES (PAN TROGLODYTES): PROBLEM-SOLVING WITH ORANGUTANS (PONGO PYGMAEUS AND PONGO ABELII) AND CHIMPANZEES (PAN TROGLODYTES): USING THE IPAD TO PROVIDE NOVEL ENRICHMENT OPPORTUNITIES A Professional Paper by HELEN BOOSTROM Submitted

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

Visual Processing Speed in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) and Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta)

Visual Processing Speed in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) and Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Faculty Publications Department of Psychology 2013 Visual Processing Speed in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) and Rhesus Macaques

More information

Content Scope & Sequence

Content Scope & Sequence Content Scope & Sequence GRADE 2 scottforesman.com (800) 552-2259 Copyright Pearson Education, Inc. 0606443 1 Counting, Coins, and Combinations Counting, Coins, and Combinations (Addition, Subtraction,

More information

UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI

UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2008, 41, 411 415 NUMBER 3(FALL 2008) THE EFFECTS OF TACT AND LISTENER TRAINING ON THE EMERGENCE OF BIDIRECTIONAL INTRAVERBAL RELATIONS ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR,

More information

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1999, 72, 57 79 NUMBER 1(JULY) ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI

More information

Running Head: AUDITORY NUMBER DISCRIMINATION IN INFANTS. Infants auditory enumeration: Evidence for analog magnitudes in the small number range

Running Head: AUDITORY NUMBER DISCRIMINATION IN INFANTS. Infants auditory enumeration: Evidence for analog magnitudes in the small number range Auditory number discrimination 1 Running Head: AUDITORY NUMBER DISCRIMINATION IN INFANTS Infants auditory enumeration: Evidence for analog magnitudes in the small number range Kristy vanmarle and Karen

More information

Do Chimpanzees ave Expectations About Reward Presentation Following Correct Performance on Computerized Cognitive Testing?

Do Chimpanzees ave Expectations About Reward Presentation Following Correct Performance on Computerized Cognitive Testing? Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Language Research Center Language Research Center 2001 Do Chimpanzees ave Expectations About Reward Presentation Following Correct Performance

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Ratio Abstraction by 6-Month- Old Infants Koleen McCrink and Karen Wynn

Ratio Abstraction by 6-Month- Old Infants Koleen McCrink and Karen Wynn PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Ratio Abstraction by 6-Month- Old Infants Koleen McCrink and Karen Wynn Yale University ABSTRACT Human infants appear to be capable of the rudimentary mathematical

More information

Do Simple Probability Judgments Rely on Integer Approximation?

Do Simple Probability Judgments Rely on Integer Approximation? Do Simple Probability Judgments Rely on Integer Approximation? Shaun O Grady (shaun.ogrady@berkeley.edu) Thomas L. Griffiths (tom_griffiths@berkeley.edu) Fei Xu (fei_xu@berkeley.edu) Department of Psychology,

More information

Discrimination Reversal Learning in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella)

Discrimination Reversal Learning in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) The Psychological Record, 2008, 58, 3 14 Discrimination Reversal Learning in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) Michael J. Beran, Emily D. Klein, Theodore A. Evans, Betty Chan, Timothy M. Flemming, Emily

More information

Supporting Information: Cognitive capacities for cooking in chimpanzees Felix Warneken & Alexandra G. Rosati

Supporting Information: Cognitive capacities for cooking in chimpanzees Felix Warneken & Alexandra G. Rosati Supporting Information: Cognitive capacities for cooking in chimpanzees Felix Warneken & Alexandra G. Rosati Subject Information Name Sex Age Testing Year 1 Testing Year 2 1 2 3 4 5a 5b 5c 6a 6b 7 8 9

More information