PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Special Section

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Special Section"

Transcription

1 Special Section CLASSICAL DELAY EYEBLINK CONDITIONING IN 4- AND 5-MONTH-OLD HUMAN INFANTS Dragana Ivkovich, 1,2 Kimberly L. Collins, 1 Carol O. Eckerman, 1 Norman A. Krasnegor, 3 and Mark E. Stanton 2 1 Duke University, 2 U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, and 3 National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Abstract Simple delay classical eyeblink conditioning, using a tone conditioned stimulus (CS) and airpuff unconditioned stimulus (US), was studied in cross-sectional samples of 4- and 5-month-old healthy, full-term infants. Infants received two identical training sessions, 1 week apart. At both ages, infants experiencing paired tones and airpuffs demonstrated successful conditioning over two sessions, relative to control subjects who had unpaired training. Conditioning was not evident, however, during the first session. Two additional groups of 5- month-olds received varied experiences during Session 1, either unpaired presentations of the CS and US or no stimulus exposure, followed by paired conditioning during Session 2. Results from these groups suggest that the higher level of conditioning observed following two sessions of paired conditioning was not the result of familiarity with the testing environment or the stimuli involved but, rather, the result of retention of associative learning not expressed during the first conditioning session. Address correspondence to Dragana Ivkovich, Department of Psychology: Experimental, Duke University, Durham, NC ; dragana@acpub.duke.edu. Classical eyeblink conditioning has become a successful paradigm for studying cognitive and neural processes underlying learning and memory in several species, including humans. The majority of human studies have involved young or aged adult populations (e.g., Kimble & Pennypacker, 1963; Solomon & Pendlebury, 1988; Spence, 1953; Woodruff-Pak & Thompson, 1988). There have been relatively few studies of human infants and young children despite their potential for yielding fundamentally important information about the early development of cognitive and neural processes underlying associative learning, and despite the potential for eyeblink conditioning to become an important tool for assessing neurocognitive development in infants at high risk for neurobehavioral disorders. A few researchers have reported successful eyeblink conditioning in human infants and children (e.g., Fitzgerald & Brackbill, 1976; Little, Lipsitt, & Rovee-Collier, 1984; Ohlrich & Ross, 1968), but little parametric work has been conducted, and none of the work traces the development of eyeblink conditioning phenomena over a substantial span of human infancy. Recently, we have been developing procedures for studying eyeblink conditioning in 4- and 5-month-old human infants. Eventually, we expect that these procedures can be adapted for use over a broader age range, be used with infants at high risk for central nervous system damage, and enable complementary studies of eyeblink conditioning in human infants and developing rat pups (Stanton & Freeman, 1994). A first study (Ivkovich, Collins, Eckerman, Krasnegor, & Stanton, 1997) yielded only weak evidence of conditioning in infants given one session of paired training at either 4 or 5 months of age. A small subset of the infants receiving their first session at 4 months of age returned for a second paired training session 1 month later, however, and showed increased levels of conditioning that appeared to be the result of some aspect of their earlier training experience rather than developmental change. In the present study, we sought to replicate the finding of robust delay conditioning over two training sessions, but with training sessions only a week apart; establish whether the improved conditioning during the second session was the result of the retention of associative learning during the first session; and relate these findings to similar data obtained for rat pups (Stanton, Fox, & Carter, 1998). METHOD Subjects and Design Both 4- and 5-month-old infants (± 10 days) were recruited using local county birth records, and infants of both ages were randomly assigned to either a paired or an unpaired training group. Each group experienced two identical sessions of paired or unpaired training 6 to 8 days apart. Other 5-month-olds were assigned to one of two exposure-control groups. One of the control groups received unpaired presentations of the conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US) during the first session (CS/US-exposure group). The other was exposed only to the training situation, without any CS or US presentations, during the first session (context-exposure group). Both these groups then received paired training 6 to 8 days later. Data are presented here for the 57 infants who completed a criterion number of trials during both sessions (at least 30 paired tone-airpuff trials during paired sessions or 30 tone-alone trials during unpaired sessions). 1 Procedures Each infant sat on a parent s lap facing a platform about 75 cm away. A standardized visual display of brightly colored objects was presented on the platform to attract and maintain the infant s attention. The parent held the infant in a way that gently restrained large motor movements and maintained roughly the same orientation throughout the session. A specially crafted soft band was secured, using Velcro, 1. An additional 54 infants (twenty 4-month-olds and thirty-four 5-montholds) were excluded from analyses because of failure to achieve the criterion number of trials (n = 25), rescheduling conflicts (n = 26), or technical difficulties (n = 3). Chi-square analyses on subjects with an insufficient number of trials revealed no evidence of selective attrition based on gender (χ 2 [1, N = 82] = 0.39), age (χ 2 [1, N = 82] = 0.50), or conditioning procedures (for Session 1: paired vs. unpaired, χ 2 [1, N = 59] = 2.96; paired vs. CS/US-exposure, χ 2 [1, N = 27] = 0.30; paired vs. context-exposure, χ 2 [1, N = 24] = 0.00). 4 Copyright 1999 American Psychological Society VOL. 10, NO. 1, JANUARY 1999

2 D. Ivkovich et al. across the infant s forehead and around the head to support a flexible plastic tube that delivered the airpuff US (approximately 1 /20 psi) to the subject s right eye. The airpuff was generated by an audio speaker that acted as a bellows device and ejected a momentary pulse of air. Two small 7-ohm speakers, 18 in. to either side of the infant s head, delivered the tone CS (1 khz, 80 db). Background instrumental music was played at low volume so that the pure-tone CS remained distinct. Two cameras, placed about 1 m to the front and right of the infant, yielded video records of the infant s head and of a signal box with a trial counter and lights indicating when the tone and airpuff stimuli were on. Electromyographic (EMG) records of eyeblinks were collected using three gel-pad electrodes positioned at the corner of the right eye, on the right temple, and at the back of the neck. A custom-built eyeblink conditioning system (U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Health Effects Research Laboratory, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina) controlled presentation of the stimuli and amplified and integrated EMG records for subsequent analysis. Paired training sessions A 750-ms tone was presented so that the last 100 ms overlapped and co-terminated with a gentle puff of air to the right eye. The intertrial interval varied from 8 to 16 s (average = 12 s). Every 6th trial in the 10- trial blocks, as well as Trials 1 and 2 at the start of the session, was an airpuff-alone trial to test for somatosensory responsiveness. Every 10th trial was a tone-alone trial to test for conditioned responding. The maximum number of trials presented was 50 (about 15 to 20 min), but the session was terminated earlier if the infant became overly fussy. Unpaired training sessions During unpaired training sessions, there was no associative contingency between the tone and airpuff. Subjects experienced, at most, the same 43 tones and 45 puffs as used for paired training, but the stimuli were presented explicitly unpaired, 4 to 8 s apart (average = 6 s) in a manner that matched the paired condition for stimulus density. The criteria for terminating a session were the same as those for paired sessions. Context-exposure sessions The context-exposure group received a training session that was identical to a paired training session except that no tone or airpuff stimuli were delivered. Response measures The primary measure of conditioning was the percentage of conditioned responses (CRs). For analysis purposes, trials were reblocked into sets of 6 trials, all of one trial type, for five successive blocks (total of 30 trials). For paired training sessions, the percentage of CRs was obtained for blocks of 6 paired trials; for unpaired sessions, the percentage of CRs was obtained for blocks of 6 tone-alone trials, with trials sampled to correspond in sequence with the tones on paired trials during paired sessions. Two independent observers coded the video records frame by frame for the occurrence and timing of a blink of the right eye during each trial. CRs were defined as blinks that started within 0 to 22 frames (367 ms) prior to onset of the airpuff. Unconditioned responses (URs) were blinks that started within 1 to 15 frames (250 ms) after onset of the airpuff. Trials in which the subject s right eye was out of view were excluded (< 10%). The percentage of agreement between observers was 98%. Because obtaining the EMG recordings was more problematic than coding the videotapes, the primary data presented here relied on video. EMG recordings were sometimes sensitive to facial and head movements and showed large intersubject variability in signal-to-noise ratio, but were usable for about 83% of the sessions. For these sessions, UR amplitudes on individual trials, confirmed by video analysis and excluding trials with a CR, were averaged across sessions within conditioning group. No reliable differences in UR amplitude that could produce any of the differences in conditioning reported here were obtained (see Table 1). RESULTS Conditioning at 4 and 5 Months of Age Results were analyzed separately at each age using between-within analyses of variance (ANOVA) with a significance level of.05. During the first training session, there was no increase in the percentage of CRs across blocks at either age and no differences between the paired and unpaired groups in the percentage of CRs (see Fig. 1). However, at both ages, substantial increases and group differences in the percentage of CRs emerged during the second session. A Group Session Block ANOVA at each age revealed main effects of group (F[1, 19] = 9.05, p <.01, for 4-month-olds; F[1, 19] = 9.23, p <.01, for 5-month-olds) and session (F[1, 19] = 17.09, p <.001, for 4- month-olds; F[1, 19] = 35.0, p <.001, for 5-month-olds). There were also interactions of group and block (F[4, 76] = 5.4, p <.001, for 4- month-olds; F[4, 76] = 3.45, p <.01, for 5-month-olds) and group and session (F[1, 19] = 10.0, p <.01, for 4-month-olds; F[1, 19] = 30.4, p <.01, for 5-month-olds). Figure 1 suggests that the percentage of CRs increased with age during Session 2, reaching around 80% for 5-month-olds, as compared with 60% for 4-month-olds. However, ANOVAs examining age differences for the paired groups either across sessions or across the first two blocks of Session 2 did not yield statistically significant effects involving age. Table 1. Mean amplitude of unconditioned responses as a function of age, group, and session Age (months) Session 1 Session 2 Paired training ± 0.8 (8) 3.8 ± 0.6 (7) ± 0.5 (10) 4.8 ± 0.5 (10) Unpaired training ± 0.8 (7) 4.6 ± 0.6 (8) ± 0.6 (8) 4.2 ± 0.8 (10) Conditioned stimulus/unconditioned stimulus exposure ± 0.7 (4) 5.8 ± 0.8 (6) Context exposure 5 Not applicable 5.6 ± 0.9 (7) Note. The numbers in parentheses show the numbers of subjects with usable electromyographic records. VOL. 10, NO. 1, JANUARY

3 Eyeblink Conditioning in Human Infants Fig 2. Mean (±SE) percentage of conditioned responses (CRs) as a function of trial block and session, for three groups of 5-month-olds. These groups underwent different experiences during Session 1, but all received paired training during Session 2. See the text for details. Fig. 1. Mean (±SE) percentage of conditioned responses (CRs) as a function of trial block and training session, for cross-sectional samples of 4- and 5-month-old infants in the paired and unpaired groups. Data are presented for six-trial blocks of paired trials (paired groups) or corresponding tone-alone trials (unpaired groups). Conditioning at 5 Months as a Function of Type of Prior Experience The 5-month paired group was compared with the two additional groups of 5-month-olds who received different experiences during their first session (see Fig. 2). For Session 1, a 3 (group) 5 (block) ANOVA did not yield any significant differences between groups. However, a similar ANOVA for Session 2 revealed significant main effects of group (F[2, 22] = 6.06, p <.01) and block (F[4, 88] = 10.90, p <.001), but no Group Block interaction. The paired group, which was undergoing its second session of paired training, showed a significantly higher percentage of CRs relative to the two groups that were experiencing paired training for the first time. Newman-Keuls tests revealed a significant difference between the paired group and each of the other groups (CS/US-exposure: p =.03; context-exposure: p =.01), which did not differ between themselves. It appears a significant Group Block interaction did not occur because the paired group started at around 50% CRs in the first block of the second session and maintained a higher percentage of CRs across the session, whereas the other groups started at around 10% CRs. Planned comparisons on this first block yielded a significant difference between the paired group and each of the other groups (CS/US-exposure: F[1, 22] = 7.55, p =.01; context-exposure: F[1, 22] = 8.83, p <.01). Also, a planned comparison of the last block of the first session and the first block of the second session showed a significant increase in percentage of CRs for the paired group only, F(1, 22) = 12.12, p <.01. DISCUSSION We have demonstrated robust delay eyeblink conditioning in 4- and 5-month-old human infants given two sessions of paired training. The levels of conditioning obtained were higher than those previously reported for younger human infants given similar amounts of training 6 VOL. 10, NO. 1, JANUARY 1999

4 D. Ivkovich et al. (Lintz, Fitzgerald, & Brackbill, 1967; Little et al., 1984; Naito & Lipsitt, 1969) but lower than those commonly observed in human adults, who reach conditioning levels of 60% to 80% CRs in less than 50 trials (Solomon & Pendlebury, 1988). It is hazardous to infer age differences in conditioning from comparisons across these studies because of the many procedural differences that distinguish them and the absence of detailed parametric data. For example, Little et al. (1984) found no delay conditioning in 10- to 30-day-old infants with the 500- ms interstimulus interval (ISI) that is optimal for adults (Kimble, 1947), but moderate levels of conditioning with an ISI of 1,500 ms. Nevertheless, the pattern of findings that emerges across studies is not inconsistent with the suggestion that delay eyeblink conditioning in human subjects develops gradually but substantially during the first 5 months of life. Substantial postnatal development of simple delay eyeblink conditioning already has been well established for rat pups (Freeman, Spencer, Skelton, & Stanton, 1993; Stanton et al., 1998; Stanton & Freeman, 1994; Stanton, Freeman, & Skelton, 1992). Human infants tested across two sessions in the present study were able to retain conditioning from one session to the next. Little et al. (1984) also observed retention of eyeblink conditioning over a 10-day period in human infants, starting around 20 days of age. However, retention in that study was not evident at the outset of retention testing but rather took the form of savings during subsequent training. In contrast, retention in the present study appeared as an increase in the percentage of CRs from the outset of retention testing for the groups that received paired training. This raises the possibility that long-term retention of eyeblink conditioning also improves during the first 5 months of life. The present findings also established that, even when there is little expression of conditioning during a first training session, there is a carryover effect on later conditioning that is not owing to nonassociative factors such as exposure to the context or the conditioning stimuli per se. A similar result has been observed in developing rats (Stanton et al., 1998). Three groups of rat pups received either paired, unpaired, or no training at 17 days of age; then, at 20 days of age, all groups received paired training. Little evidence of eyeblink conditioning was observed among the 17-day-old rats. However, paired training at this age produced substantial savings relative to the other two groups during the subsequent training at 20 days of age. These findings suggest that the distinction between acquisition and expression of learning may be important in developmental studies of Pavlovian conditioning, and they underscore the value of the three-group design (paired, CS/US-exposure, context-exposure) we employed for addressing this issue. It also is noteworthy that in the present study, prior unpaired training did not impair subsequent paired conditioning in 5-month-old infants. During Session 2, infants in the CS/US-exposure group did not perform worse than the infants in the context-exposure group, who were experiencing the CS and US for the first time. In adult subjects, preexposure to unpaired presentations of the CS and US impairs subsequent performance (relative to a naive group) during paired training, a phenomenon attributed to conditioned inhibition (Rescorla, 1967) or learned irrelevance (Mackintosh, 1974). There was no evidence for such an impairment in the present study or in a similar study involving infant rodents (Stanton et al., 1998). Infant mammals may not be able to learn that the CS and US are negatively correlated during unpaired training in the way that adults seem to do. According to one theoretical account of such learning (Rescorla, 1972), infants may not show this effect because of differences in attentional, contextual, or configural processing that may reflect immaturity of temporal cortical structures (Allen & Gluck, 1997; Nicolle, Barry, Veronesi, & Stanton, 1989; Rudy, 1992). In summary, these findings have established that the delay eyeblink conditioning procedures used are effective for 4- and 5-month-old human infants. These procedures can now be used to examine in more detail the normative ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning phenomena. Further, these findings have revealed phenomena (e.g., unexpressed learning and the absence of impairment following unpaired CS-US exposure) similar to those seen in the developing rat pup, illustrating the potential of complementary studies of eyeblink conditioning in human infants and animal models of early mammalian development. Acknowledgments We would like to thank Elizabeth Frederick, Aileen Haggerty, Jennie Lee, Betty Tao, Carrie Westlake, and Minna Wiley for their assistance with this work and Barbara Goldman for sharing with us her experience in infant eyeblink conditioning. Support for this research was provided in part by U.S. Environmental Protection Agency Cooperative Agreement No. CR and Social and Behavioral Sciences Research Grant No. 12-FY from the March of Dimes Foundation to C.O. Eckerman and by National Institute of Mental Health Postdoctoral Fellowship No. MH to D. Ivkovich. This manuscript has been reviewed by the National Health and Environmental Effects Research Laboratory, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, and approved for publication. Mention of trade names or commercial products does not constitute endorsement or recommendation for use. REFERENCES Allen, M.T., & Gluck, M.A. (1997). Learned irrelevance pre-exposure inhibits acquisition of the classically conditioned rabbit eyeblink response. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 23, 785. Fitzgerald, H.E., & Brackbill, Y. (1976). Classical conditioning in infancy: Development and constraints. Psychological Bulletin, 83, Freeman, J.H., Jr., Spencer, C.O., Skelton, R.W., & Stanton, M.E. (1993). Ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning in the rat: Effects of US intensity and interstimulus interval on delay conditioning. Psychobiology, 21, Ivkovich, D., Collins, K., Eckerman, C.O., Krasnegor, N.A., & Stanton, M.E. (1997). Comparative developmental studies of eyeblink conditioning in infant rats and humans. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 23, 782. Kimble, G.A. (1947). Conditioning as a function of the time between conditioned and unconditioned stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, Kimble, G.A., & Pennypacker, H.S. (1963). Eyelid conditioning in young and aged subjects. The Journal of General Psychology, 103, Lintz, L.M., Fitzgerald, H.E., & Brackbill, Y. (1967). Conditioning the eyeblink response to sound in infants. Psychonomic Science, 7, Little, A.H., Lipsitt, L.P., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1984). Classical conditioning and retention of the infant s eyelid response: Effects of age and interstimulus interval. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, Mackintosh, N.J. (1974). The psychology of animal learning. New York: Academic Press. Naito, T., & Lipsitt, L.P. (1969). Two attempts to condition eyelid responses in human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, Nicolle, M.M., Barry, C.C., Veronesi, B., & Stanton, M.E. (1989). Fornix transections disrupt the ontogeny of latent inhibition in the rat. Psychobiology, 17, Ohlrich, E.S., & Ross, L.E. (1968). Acquisition and differential conditioning of the eyelid response in normal and retarded children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 6, Rescorla, R.A. (1967). Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review, 74, Rescorla, R.A. (1972). Informational variables in Pavlovian conditioning. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 6, pp. 1 46). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Rudy, J.W. (1992). Development of learning: From elemental to configural associative networks. In C. Rovee-Collier & L.P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 7, pp ). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Solomon, P.R., & Pendlebury, W.W. (1988). A model systems approach to age-related memory disorders. Neurotoxicology, 9, Spence, K.W. (1953). Learning and performance in eyelid conditioning as a function of the intensity of the UCS. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, VOL. 10, NO. 1, JANUARY

5 Eyeblink Conditioning in Human Infants Stanton, M.E., Fox, G.D., & Carter, C.S. (1998). Ontogeny of the conditioned eyeblink response in rats: Acquisition or expression? Neuropharmacology, 37, Stanton, M.E., & Freeman, J.H., Jr. (1994). Eyeblink conditioning in the infant rat: An animal model of learning in developmental neurotoxicology. Environmental Health Perspectives, 102(Suppl. 2), Stanton, M.E., Freeman, J.H., Jr., & Skelton, R.W. (1992). Eyeblink conditioning in the developing rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, Woodruff-Pak, D.S., & Thompson, R.F. (1988). Cerebellar correlates of classical conditioning across the life span. In P.B. Baltes, D.M. Featherman, & R.M. Lerner (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 1 37). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 8 VOL. 10, NO. 1, JANUARY 1999

Effects of Early Hippocampal Lesions on Trace, Delay, and Long-Delay Eyeblink Conditioning in Developing Rats

Effects of Early Hippocampal Lesions on Trace, Delay, and Long-Delay Eyeblink Conditioning in Developing Rats Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 76, 426 446 (2001) doi:10.1006/nlme.2001.4027, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Effects of Early Hippocampal Lesions on Trace, Delay, and Long-Delay

More information

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction Instructor: Paul Wolfgang Faculty sponsor: Yuan Shi, Ph.D. Andrey Mavrichev CIS 4339 Project in Computer Science May 7, 2009 Research work was completed in collaboration with Michael Tobia, Kevin L. Brown,

More information

Parallel Acquisition of Awareness and Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning

Parallel Acquisition of Awareness and Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Research Parallel Acquisition of Awareness and Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Joseph R. Manns, 1 Robert E. Clark, 2 and Larry R. Squire 3,4,5 1 Department of Psychology, University of California,

More information

Eyeblink Conditioning: A Non-invasive Biomarker for Neurodevelopmental Disorders

Eyeblink Conditioning: A Non-invasive Biomarker for Neurodevelopmental Disorders J Autism Dev Disord (2015) 45:376 394 DOI 10.1007/s10803-013-1905-9 ORIGINAL PAPER Eyeblink Conditioning: A Non-invasive Biomarker for Neurodevelopmental Disorders Bethany C. Reeb-Sutherland Nathan A.

More information

Classical Conditioning

Classical Conditioning What is classical conditioning? Classical Conditioning Learning & Memory Arlo Clark-Foos Learning to associate previously neutral stimuli with the subsequent events. Howard Eichenbaum s Thanksgiving Pavlov

More information

Acquisition of Differential Delay Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Is Independent of Awareness

Acquisition of Differential Delay Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Is Independent of Awareness Behavioral Neuroscience In the public domain 2005, Vol. 119, No. 1, 78 86 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.78 Acquisition of Differential Delay Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Is Independent of Awareness Christine

More information

Physiology & Behavior

Physiology & Behavior Physiology & Behavior 96 (2009) 399 411 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Physiology & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/phb Associative and non-associative blinking in classically

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title A Continuum of Learning and Memory Research Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9q3031wx Journal International Journal of Comparative Psychology,

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly Auditory Temporal Processes 1 Ben-Artzi, E., Babkoff, H., Fostick, L. (2011). Auditory temporal processes in the elderly. Audiology Research, 1, 21-23 ***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully

More information

Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online

Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online The use of an unpleasant sound as the unconditional stimulus in aversive Pavlovian conditioning experiments that involve children and adolescent participants Author Neumann, David, Waters, Allison, Westbury

More information

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1 The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Lecture 5.1 David S. Touretzky Based on notes by Lisa M. Saksida November, 2015 Outline Classical and instrumental conditioning The Rescorla

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ISSN: 1747-0218 (Print) 1747-0226 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20 Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive

More information

Classical conditioning, awareness, and brain systems

Classical conditioning, awareness, and brain systems 524 Review Vol.6 No.12 December 2 Classical conditioning, awareness, and brain systems Robert E. Clark, Joseph R. Manns and Larry R. Squire Memory is composed of several different abilities that are supported

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Awareness in Classical Differential Eyeblink Conditioning in Young and Aging Humans

Awareness in Classical Differential Eyeblink Conditioning in Young and Aging Humans Behavioral Neuroscience 2001, Vol. 115, No. 4, 747-757 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/01/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0735-7044.115.4.747 Awareness in Classical Differential

More information

Eyeblink cross-habituation between tactile and acoustic systems in humans

Eyeblink cross-habituation between tactile and acoustic systems in humans Psychobiology 1997,250), 66-70 Eyeblink cross-habituation between tactile and acoustic systems in humans WILLIAM A DYCUS and ALICE SCHADE POWERS St. John's University, Jamaica, New York To study cross-habituation

More information

The Cost of Learning: Interference Effects on Early Learning and Memory

The Cost of Learning: Interference Effects on Early Learning and Memory The Cost of Learning: Interference Effects on Early Learning and Memory Kevin P. Darby (darby.60@osu.edu) Department of Psychology, 1835 Neil Ave. Columbus, OH 43215 USA Vladimir M. Sloutsky (sloutsky.1@osu.edu)

More information

Parallel acquisition of awareness and differential delay eyeblink conditioning

Parallel acquisition of awareness and differential delay eyeblink conditioning Research Parallel acquisition of awareness and differential delay eyeblink conditioning Gabrielle Weidemann 1,3 and Cassandra Antees 2 1 School of Social Sciences and Psychology, University of Western

More information

Effects of Temporal Lobe Amnesia, Aging and Awareness on Human Eyeblink Conditioning John Disterhoft

Effects of Temporal Lobe Amnesia, Aging and Awareness on Human Eyeblink Conditioning John Disterhoft Eyeblink Conditioning Across Species Effects of Temporal Lobe Amnesia, Aging and Awareness on Human Eyeblink Conditioning John Disterhoft Department of Physiology Institute for Neuroscience Northwestern

More information

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Jennifer A. Kaminski (kaminski.16@osu.edu) Center for Cognitive Science, Ohio State University 10A

More information

Effects of Attention on Infants' Preference for Briefly Exposed Visual Stimuli in the Paired-Comparison Recognition-Memory Paradigm

Effects of Attention on Infants' Preference for Briefly Exposed Visual Stimuli in the Paired-Comparison Recognition-Memory Paradigm Developmental Psychology 1997, Vol. 33, No. 1,22-31 Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0012-1649/97/$3.00 Effects of Attention on Infants' Preference for Briefly Exposed Visual

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Learn Behav (2011) 39:79 86 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0022-x Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Gabriel Rodríguez & Gumersinda Alonso Published

More information

Cerebellar Substrates for Error Correction in Motor Conditioning

Cerebellar Substrates for Error Correction in Motor Conditioning Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 76, 314 341 (2001) doi:10.1006/nlme.2001.4031, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Cerebellar Substrates for Error Correction in Motor Conditioning Mark

More information

Learning. Learning. Habituation. Sensitization. Habituation and Sensitization

Learning. Learning. Habituation. Sensitization. Habituation and Sensitization Learning Learning Adaptive change in behavior that results from past experiences Nonassociative Learning Habituation Sensitization Classical Conditioning Instrumental Learning/Operant Conditioning Ability

More information

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (3), 230-245 doi:10.3758/lb.37.3.230 Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning JUSTIN A. HARRIS, SABA A GHARA EI, AND CLINTON A. MOORE

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS The Psychological Record, 2000, 50, 429-442 LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS LUIS A. PEREZ-GONZALEZ, JOSEPH E. SPRADLIN, and KATHRYN J. SAUNDERS University of Oviedo, Spain

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

A Simulation of Sutton and Barto s Temporal Difference Conditioning Model

A Simulation of Sutton and Barto s Temporal Difference Conditioning Model A Simulation of Sutton and Barto s Temporal Difference Conditioning Model Nick Schmansk Department of Cognitive and Neural Sstems Boston Universit Ma, Abstract A simulation of the Sutton and Barto [] model

More information

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form Supervisor: Date first assessed: Individual being Supervised: Certification being sought: Instructions: Please mark each item with either a 0,1,2 or 3 based on rating scale Rating Scale: 0 - cannot identify

More information

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1992. 20 (3). 233-239 The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats JAMES S. MILLER and JOYCE A.

More information

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov ( )

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov ( ) Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (1849-1936) Chapter 7 1 Ivan Petrovich Pavlov 1. Born in Ryazan, Russia on Sep 14, 1849. 2. Studied the digestive system and won the Nobel Prize in 1904 for physiology and medicine.

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) Classical vs. Instrumental The modern view is that these two types of learning involve similar

More information

Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex

Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex The Journal of Neuroscience, January 15, 2001, 21(2):682 690 Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex Tatsuya Ohyama and Michael D. Mauk Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, University

More information

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1996, 3 (2), 222 226 Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana and THOMAS J. PALMERI Vanderbilt University,

More information

CHAPTER 7 LEARNING. Jake Miller, Ocean Lakes High School

CHAPTER 7 LEARNING. Jake Miller, Ocean Lakes High School CHAPTER 7 LEARNING Jake Miller, Ocean Lakes High School Learning: Defined Learning: Relatively permanent change in [observable] behavior due to experience NOT temporary changes due to disease, injury,

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Nat Neurosci. 2006 August ; 9(8): 1004 1006. Maternal presence serves as a switch between learning fear and attraction in infancy

More information

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Chapter Title Attention and Pavlovian Conditioning Copyright Year 2011 Copyright Holder Springer Science+Business Media, LLC Corresponding Author

More information

Associative Learning

Associative Learning LEARNING Relatively permanent change in behavior Acquired from experience or observation Learning Associative Learning Observational Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Associative Learning

More information

A neurocomputational model of classical conditioning phenomena: A putative role for the hippocampal region in associative learning

A neurocomputational model of classical conditioning phenomena: A putative role for the hippocampal region in associative learning available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report A neurocomputational model of classical conditioning phenomena: A putative role for the hippocampal region in associative

More information

Associative learning

Associative learning Introduction to Learning Associative learning Event-event learning (Pavlovian/classical conditioning) Behavior-event learning (instrumental/ operant conditioning) Both are well-developed experimentally

More information

Comparable Measures of Cognitive Function in Human Infants and Laboratory Animals to Identify Environmental Health Risks to Children

Comparable Measures of Cognitive Function in Human Infants and Laboratory Animals to Identify Environmental Health Risks to Children Children s Health Review Comparable Measures of Cognitive Function in Human Infants and Laboratory Animals to Identify Environmental Health Risks to Children Carolyn Sharbaugh, 1 Susan Marie Viet, 1 Alexa

More information

DISCRIMINATION OF SERIAL AUDITORY PATTERNS IN RATS - roles of configural and elemental associations

DISCRIMINATION OF SERIAL AUDITORY PATTERNS IN RATS - roles of configural and elemental associations DISCRIMINATION OF SERIAL AUDITORY PATTERNS IN RATS - roles of configural and elemental associations Tiina Nurmi Master s thesis Department of Psychology University of Jyväskylä January 2011 CONTENTS INTRODUCTION

More information

Human experiment - Training Procedure - Matching of stimuli between rats and humans - Data analysis

Human experiment - Training Procedure - Matching of stimuli between rats and humans - Data analysis 1 More complex brains are not always better: Rats outperform humans in implicit categorybased generalization by implementing a similarity-based strategy Ben Vermaercke 1*, Elsy Cop 1, Sam Willems 1, Rudi

More information

Blocking in eyelid conditioning: Effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertrial stimulus

Blocking in eyelid conditioning: Effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertrial stimulus Animal Learning& Behavior 1979, 7 (4), 452-456 Blocking in eyelid conditioning: Effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertrial stimulus ROBERT T. MALESKE and PETER W. FREY Northwestern

More information

Cerebellar Cortical Degeneration Disrupts Discrimination Learning But Not Delay or Trace Classical Eyeblink Conditioning

Cerebellar Cortical Degeneration Disrupts Discrimination Learning But Not Delay or Trace Classical Eyeblink Conditioning Neuropsychology 2000. Vol. 14, No. 4, 537-550 Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0894-4105/00/$5.00 DOI: I0.1037//0894-4105.14.4.537 Cerebellar Cortical Degeneration Disrupts

More information

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce Learning Chapter 7 Design a Brain The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce What characteristics are needed to behave adaptively and reproduce successfully? Consider: nearly

More information

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 1 The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning Anila D Mello Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 2 Abstract Learning and changing

More information

Impaired Trace Eyeblink Conditioning in Bilateral, Medial-Temporal Lobe Amnesia

Impaired Trace Eyeblink Conditioning in Bilateral, Medial-Temporal Lobe Amnesia Behavioral Neuroscience 1997, Vol. Ill, No. 5,873-882 In the public domain Impaired Trace Eyeblink Conditioning in Bilateral, Medial-Temporal Lobe Amnesia Regina McGlinchey-Berroth Boston University School

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Chapter Six. Learning. Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning

Chapter Six. Learning. Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Chapter Six Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Part One: Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning is conditioning by association, such as a pet getting excited

More information

Following the seminal proposal of Hebb1 and many others, acquired learning

Following the seminal proposal of Hebb1 and many others, acquired learning Associative Learning and Long-Term Potentiation in Rodents: José M. Delgado-, MD, PhD Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) and Learning-Dependent Changes in Synaptic Strength Following the seminal proposal of

More information

Attention and prepulse inhibition: the effects of task-relevant, irrelevant, and no-task conditions

Attention and prepulse inhibition: the effects of task-relevant, irrelevant, and no-task conditions International Journal of Psychophysiology 56 (2005) 121 128 www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho Attention and prepulse inhibition: the effects of task-relevant, irrelevant, and no-task conditions Gary L.

More information

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience Dopamine and Learning Michael Dorris Associate Professor of Physiology & Neuroscience Studies dorrism@biomed.queensu.ca http://brain.phgy.queensu.ca/dorrislab/ NSCI 324 Systems

More information

Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - -

Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - - BRAIN PLASTICITY Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - - Recently, it was found that new neurons and glial cells are born in specific brain regions - reorganization. Brain plasticity occurs

More information

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Automatic detection - 1 Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1986, 24 (6), 431-434 SIU L. CHOW The University of Wollongong,

More information

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's JOURNAt, OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1968, 1 1, 1-8 NUMBER I (JANUARY) A UTO-SHAPING OF THE PIGEON'S KEY-PECK' PAUL L. BROWN AND HERBERT M. JENKINS MCMASTER UNIVERSITY Reliable acquistion

More information

BRAIN PLASTICITY. Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - -

BRAIN PLASTICITY. Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - - BRAIN PLASTICITY Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - - Recently, it was found that new neurons and glial cells are born in specific brain regions - reorganization. Brain plasticity occurs

More information

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces N. F. Troje* and D. Kersten *Max-Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik, Spemannstr. 38, 72076 Tübingen, Germany Department of Psychology, University

More information

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning Chapter 6: Learning 1 Learning 1. In perception we studied that we are responsive to stimuli in the external world. Although some of these stimulus-response associations are innate many are learnt. 2.

More information

Chapter 3. Outline for this lecture

Chapter 3. Outline for this lecture Chapter 3 Elicited Behaviors and Classical Conditioning Outline for this lecture Elicited Behaviors Reflexes Fixed Action Patterns Opponent Process Theory of Emotion Classical Conditioning Characteristics

More information

Parameter Invariability in the TD Model. with Complete Serial Components. Jordan Marks. Middlesex House. 24 May 1999

Parameter Invariability in the TD Model. with Complete Serial Components. Jordan Marks. Middlesex House. 24 May 1999 Parameter Invariability in the TD Model with Complete Serial Components Jordan Marks Middlesex House 24 May 1999 This work was supported in part by NIMH grant MH57893, John W. Moore, PI 1999 Jordan S.

More information

DCNV RESEARCH TOOL: INVESTIGATION OF ANTICIPATORY BRAIN POTENTIALS

DCNV RESEARCH TOOL: INVESTIGATION OF ANTICIPATORY BRAIN POTENTIALS DCNV RESEARCH TOOL: INVESTIGATION OF ANTICIPATORY BRAIN POTENTIALS Roman Golubovski, Dipl-Eng J.Sandanski 116-3/24 91000 Skopje, Macedonia Phone: + 389 91 165 367, Fax: + 389 91 165 304 email: roman.golubovski@iname.com

More information

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Hippocampal Pyramidal Neuron of Mice and Rats Figure 1 Open Field Apparatus Open Field Behavior

More information

Latent inhibition in a navigation task: Evidence for the use of associative processes in spatial memory. José Prados* Universitat de Barcelona

Latent inhibition in a navigation task: Evidence for the use of associative processes in spatial memory. José Prados* Universitat de Barcelona Psicológica (1999) 20, 151-162. ARTICULOS Latent inhibition in a navigation task: Evidence for the use of associative processes in spatial memory José Prados* Universitat de Barcelona Rats were trained

More information

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1989, Vol. 15, No. 3, 232-241 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-740389$00.75 Contextual Effects in Conditioning,

More information

Lateral Inhibition Explains Savings in Conditioning and Extinction

Lateral Inhibition Explains Savings in Conditioning and Extinction Lateral Inhibition Explains Savings in Conditioning and Extinction Ashish Gupta & David C. Noelle ({ashish.gupta, david.noelle}@vanderbilt.edu) Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science

More information

In review. Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm

In review. Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm Tracy L. Greer 1*, Lucien T. Thompson 2* 1 Department of Psychiatry, UT Southwestern Medical Center,

More information

Conditioned Eyeblink Response Consists of Two Distinct Components

Conditioned Eyeblink Response Consists of Two Distinct Components Conditioned Eyeblink Response Consists of Two Distinct Components MAGNUS IVARSSON AND PÄR SVENSSON Department of Physiological Sciences, Section for Neuroscience, Lund University, S-223 62 Lund, Sweden

More information

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(2),199-203 Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning PETER C. HOLLAND and DESWELL T. FORBES University ofpittsburgh, Pittsburgh,

More information

Study Plan: Session 1

Study Plan: Session 1 Study Plan: Session 1 6. Practice learning the vocabulary. Use the electronic flashcards from the Classical The Development of Classical : The Basic Principles of Classical Conditioned Emotional Reponses:

More information

To be diagnosed with panic disorder, a person must experience

To be diagnosed with panic disorder, a person must experience Article Reduction of Trace but Not Delay Eyeblink Conditioning in Panic Disorder Christian Grillon, Ph.D. Shmuel Lissek, Ph.D. Dana McDowell, B.A. Jessica Levenson, B.A. Daniel S. Pine, M.D. Objective:

More information

Unit 6 Learning.

Unit 6 Learning. Unit 6 Learning https://www.apstudynotes.org/psychology/outlines/chapter-6-learning/ 1. Overview 1. Learning 1. A long lasting change in behavior resulting from experience 2. Classical Conditioning 1.

More information

A Cardiac Measure of Cerebral Asymmetries in Infant Auditory Perception

A Cardiac Measure of Cerebral Asymmetries in Infant Auditory Perception Developmental Psychology 19n, Vol. 13, No. 1,54-59 A Cardiac Measure of Cerebral Asymmetries in Infant Auditory Perception BRADLEY B, GLANVILLE North Dakota State University CATHERINE T. BEST Michigan

More information

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (2), 213-219 Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning THOMAS HIGGINS and ROBERT A. RESCORLA University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments Aniket Kittur (nkittur@ucla.edu) Keith J. Holyoak (holyoak@lifesci.ucla.edu) Department of Psychology, University

More information

COMPUTATIONAL MODELS OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING: A COMPARATIVE STUDY

COMPUTATIONAL MODELS OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING: A COMPARATIVE STUDY COMPUTATIONAL MODELS OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING: A COMPARATIVE STUDY Christian Balkenius Jan Morén christian.balkenius@fil.lu.se jan.moren@fil.lu.se Lund University Cognitive Science Kungshuset, Lundagård

More information

Conditioning By Darren Burke & Caroline Tomiczek The word conditioning has two different meanings, only one of which is uncontroversial.

Conditioning By Darren Burke & Caroline Tomiczek The word conditioning has two different meanings, only one of which is uncontroversial. Conditioning By Darren Burke & Caroline Tomiczek The word conditioning has two different meanings, only one of which is uncontroversial. In common usage, even in the psychological literature, the word

More information

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting Animal Learning & Behavior 1989, 17 (3), 269-279 Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting PETER C. HOLLAND Duke University, Durham, North Carolina In three experiments, transfer effects

More information

Partial reinforcement effects on learning and extinction of place preferences in the water maze

Partial reinforcement effects on learning and extinction of place preferences in the water maze Learning & Behavior 2008, 36 (4), 311-318 doi: 10.3758/LB.36.4.311 Partial reinforcement effects on learning and extinction of place preferences in the water maze José Prados University of Leicester, Leicester,

More information

Rescorla-Wagner (1972) Theory of Classical Conditioning

Rescorla-Wagner (1972) Theory of Classical Conditioning Rescorla-Wagner (1972) Theory of Classical Conditioning HISTORY Ever since Pavlov, it was assumed that any CS followed contiguously by any US would result in conditioning. Not true: Contingency Not true:

More information

Associative Learning

Associative Learning Learning Learning Targets 1. Distinguish general differences between principles of classical conditioning, operant conditioning, and observational learning (e.g., contingencies). 2. Describe basic classical

More information

DEVELOPMENTS. Inside This Issue Page 1: Infant Learning Project information THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT DALLAS

DEVELOPMENTS. Inside This Issue Page 1: Infant Learning Project information THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT DALLAS THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT DALLAS DEVELOPMENTS Infant Learning Project Fall 2018 Inside This Issue Page 1: Infant Learning Project information Page 2: Article Review focusing on the cross cultural comparison

More information

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Chapter 6/9: Learning Chapter 6/9: Learning Learning A relatively durable change in behavior or knowledge that is due to experience. The acquisition of knowledge, skills, and behavior through reinforcement, modeling and natural

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D.

DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D. DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D. Psychology Department American University Washington, DC 20016 Phone: 202-885-1725 Email: kearns@american.edu EDUCATION 2005 American University Washington, DC Ph.D., Psychology

More information

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 28, 200 215 (1997) ARTICLE NO. LM960958 Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure MICHELLE SYMONDS AND GEOFFREY HALL University

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 4 (Cont.) Indirect Conditioning Applications of Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 4 (Cont.) Indirect Conditioning Applications of Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 (Cont.) Indirect Conditioning Applications of Conditioning Extinction Extinction a method for eliminating a conditioned response. Extinction paradigm: Present the

More information