Parietal rtms distorts the mental number line: Simulating spatial neglect in healthy subjects

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Parietal rtms distorts the mental number line: Simulating spatial neglect in healthy subjects"

Transcription

1 Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) Parietal rtms distorts the mental number line: Simulating spatial neglect in healthy subjects Silke M. Göbel a,b,, Marco Calabria c, Alessandro Farnè c,d, Yves Rossetti c,d,e a Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK b Department of Psychology, University of York, York YO10 5DD, UK c Espace et Action, UMR-S 534 INSERM-UCBL, Institut National de la Santé etdelarecherchemédicale, Université Claude Bernard, 16 Avenue Lépine, Bron, France d Institut Fédératif des Neurosciences de Lyon (IFNL), Bron, France e Mouvement et Handicap, Hôpital Henry Gabrielle, Hospices Civils de Lyon et Université Claude Bernard, St Genis Laval, France Received 13 May 2005; received in revised form 14 September 2005; accepted 16 September 2005 Available online 2 November 2005 Abstract Patients with left-sided visuospatial neglect, typically after damage to the right parietal lobe, show a systematic bias towards larger numbers when asked to bisect a numerical interval. This has been taken as further evidence for a spatial representation of numbers, perhaps akin to a mental number line with smaller numbers represented to the left and larger numbers to the right. Previously, contralateral neglect-like symptoms in physical line bisection have been induced in healthy subjects with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rtms) over right posterior parietal lobe. Here we used rtms over parietal and occipital sites in healthy subjects to investigate spatial representations in a number bisection task. Subjects were asked to name the midpoint of numerical intervals without calculating. On control trials subjects behaviour was similar to performance reported in physical line bisection experiments. Subjects underestimated the midpoint of the numerical interval. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation produced representational neglect-like symptoms in number bisection when applied over right posterior parietal cortex (right PPC). Repetitive TMS over right PPC shifted the perceived midpoint of the numerical interval significantly to the right while occipital TMS had no effect on bisection performance. Our study therefore provides further evidence that subjects use spatial representations, perhaps akin to a mental number line, in basic numerical processing tasks. Furthermore, we showed that the right posterior parietal cortex is crucially involved in spatial representation of numbers Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Number bisection; Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; Posterior parietal cortex; Unilateral neglect; Mental number line 1. Introduction Healthy subjects when asked to bisect a physical line tend to exhibit a small leftward bias, i.e. they normally bisect lines slightly to the left of the midpoint (pseudoneglect) (Jewell & McCourt, 2000). In contrast, neglect patients show a rightward bias in line bisection that might be due to neglecting part or most of the left side of the line (Halligan & Marshall, 1993). There are several means to alleviate unilateral neglect or to simulate neglect in healthy subjects such as short-term adaptation Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of York, York YO10 5DD, UK. Tel.: ; fax: address: s.goebel@psychology.york.ac.uk (S.M. Göbel). to prisms (for a review see Rossetti & Rode, 2002). Fierro et al. (2000) showed that this rightward bias in line bisection could be induced in healthy subjects by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over right but not left posterior parietal cortex (PPC). Although the basic pathophysiological principles leading to spatial neglect are still an issue of lively debate (e.g. Halligan & Marshall, 1994; Karnath, 2001; Milner & Goodale, 1995), there is evidence that the most frequent anatomical correlate of visuospatial hemineglect in humans is damage to the inferior parietal lobule (Halligan, Fink, Marshall, & Vallar, 2003; Vallar & Perani, 1986). Neuroimaging and neurointerference data support the view that the right inferior parietal lobule is involved in spatial tasks such as line bisection (Bjoertomt, Cowey, & Walsh, 2002; Fink et al., 2000; Fink, Marshall, Weiss, & Zilles, 2001), visual search (Walsh, Ellison, Ashbridge, & Cowey, 1999) and /$ see front matter 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi: /j.neuropsychologia

2 S.M. Göbel et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) mental rotation (Harris & Miniussi, 2003). Some patients with unilateral neglect also show a spatial deficit for left-sided stimuli that extends to mental images (Bisiach & Luzatti, 1978; Bisiach & Vallar, 1988). Recently Zorzi, Priftis, and Umilta (2002) reported that neglect patients with right parietal lesions not only show a systematic bias when asked to bisect a line, but also when they were asked to bisect a numerical interval (see also Rossetti et al., 2004). Along with the importance of left PPC in calculation and number representation (Chochon, Cohen, van de Moortele, & Dehaene, 1999; Cipolotti & van Harskamp, 2001; Dehaene, 2000; Dehaene, Piazza, Pinel, & Cohen, 2003; Göbel, Walsh, & Rushworth, 2001; Hecaen, Angelergues, & Houllier, 1961) these findings suggest a role for the right PPC in spatial number representation, perhaps akin to a mental number line. Galton (1880) was the first to report what he called mental number forms, highly individualistic associations of numbers with a mental image of numbers represented in space, mostly along a line, a mental number line. Mental number lines have also been described more recently (Seron, Pesenti, Noel, Deloche, & Cornet, 1992) and it has been suggested that explicit automatic mental number lines could be a specific form of synaesthesia (Hubbard, Piazza, Pinnel, & Dehaene, 2005). However, support for the number line model of number representation (Restle, 1970) does not just come from the selective group of subjects with explicit mental number lines but also stems from associations between number size and spatial response codes found in subjects who do not possess explicit mental number lines. Several studies have proposed a link between number size and spatial response codes and provide support for a canonical orientation of the mental number line (i.e. smaller numbers represented to the left side of space and larger numbers to the right side of space). Subjects performing a parity judgement task, for example, are faster to respond with their left hand to smaller numbers and with their right hand to larger numbers (the spatial numerical response code or SNARC effect, Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux, 1993). Similar effects of task-irrelevant number size on spatial responses have been reported, for example for pointing (Fischer, 2003) oreyemovement responses (Bächtold, Baumüller, & Brugger, 1998; Fischer, Warlop, Hill, & Fias, 2004) to the left or the right side of space, bisecting digits strings (Fischer, 2001) and orienting spatial attention (Fischer, Castel, Dodd, & Pratt, 2003). Disturbances in number processing and calculation have been commonly reported after damage to the inferior parietal lobes (Cipolotti & van Harskamp, 2001; Dehaene, 2000; Hecaen et al., 1961). Acalculia, difficulty in calculation, is mostly associated with lesions in the left hemisphere (Grafman, Passafiume, Faglioni, & Boller, 1982). However, recent imaging studies suggest that areas in left and right intraparietal sulcus (IPS) are active during number processing (Chochon et al., 1999; Eger, Sterzer, Russ, Giraud, & Kleinschmidt, 2003; Pinel, Piazza, Le Bihan, & Dehaene, 2004; Rickard et al., 2000). Dehaene et al. (2003), in the latest version of the triple-code model of number processing, suggest that a non-verbal representation of numerical quantity perhaps analogous to a number line is present in the horizontal segment of the intraparietal sulcus of both hemispheres. Thus, hemispheric dominance for basic number processing is less clear than for spatial cognition. Few acalculic patients with left parietal lesions have been reported who were impaired in basic number processing tasks such as number comparison or number bisection (Cipolotti & van Harskamp, 2001; Lemer, Dehaene, Spelke, & Cohen, 2003). However, most neglect patients with right parietal lesions tested on number bisection show an impairment on these tasks (Rossetti et al., 2004; Vuilleumier & Rafal, 1999; Zorzi et al., 2002). Interestingly, one of the few acalculic patients in the literature who failed at number bisection (patient MAR, Dehaene & Cohen, 1997) had a lesion in the right parietal lobe, but was left-handed. The aim of the present study was to explore the role of both posterior parietal cortices in spatial number representation by using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. In particular we aimed to test the prediction that the right posterior parietal cortex in healthy subjects to be involved in spatial representation of numbers as assessed by number bisection. 2. Methods 2.1. Subjects Twenty-eight healthy volunteers, aged years, with English as their mother tongue were tested (12 female and 16 male, all right-handed). All subjects were checked for TMS exclusion criteria and gave their informed written consent before participating. The procedure was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Oxford (OxREC number C02.092). Subjects were over 18 years of age, either held or were studying for a university degree, had normal or corrected to normal visual acuity and were free of any neurological condition. They were reimbursed for travel expenses and time taken to participate in the study Stimuli and apparatus Subjects were presented aurally with 60 different pairs of three-digit numbers in ascending order from to Each pair of three-digit numbers was in the same hundreds. Numerical distance between the numbers of each pair was either 16, 25, 36, 49 or 64. Each pair of numbers was recorded as one single sound file before the experiment and was spoken by a male British voice. An example for one such single sound file is (see Fig. 1). The recorded numbers were then manipulated with the help of a sound programme (CoolEditPro) so that the entire presentation of each sound file took 3500 ms with 500 ms separating the first three-digit number (e.g. 758) from the second three-digit number (e.g. 783). For generating the magnetic pulses we used a Magstim Super Rapid stimulator (The Magstim Company Limited, Whitland, UK) with a 70 mm figure of eight coil. Magnetic stimulus intensity was between 55 and 70% of the maximum stimulator output. The intensity of the magnetic stimulation was set separately for each individual at 10% above their active motor threshold. Motor threshold was defined as minimum percentage of the stimulator output that evoked a visually detectable twitch in the contralateral hand when contracted (Pridmore et al., 1998). Frequency, intensity and duration of rtms were in accordance with the safety guidelines suggested by Wassermann (1998) Procedure Subjects sat in front of two loudspeakers and were blindfolded for the duration of the experiment. Subjects had to estimate and name the number that lay in the middle of the interval described by the two numbers. Instructions provided to the subject emphasised that they should perform the task without calculating and answer as quickly as possible. Their verbal response and reaction times (RT) were recorded. RT was calculated with respect to the subjects response onset.

3 862 S.M. Göbel et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) Fig. 1. In each trial subjects heard a male voice naming two three-digit numbers. These two three-digit numbers were separated by a silence lasting 500 ms. The total duration of the utterance of the two three-digit numbers was 3500 ms. Subjects were asked to name the numerical midpoint of the interval described by the two three-digit numbers without calculating. The intertrial interval lasted 3000 ms. Subjects responded towards the end of the intertrial interval. On 50% of the trials immediately after the end of the utterance rtms was applied at 5 Hz for 1000 ms over parietal or occipital stimulation sites. Each block consisted of 120 trials, preceded by an initial training block of five non-tms trials. The order of the sound files was pseudorandom within each block. The intertrial interval was 3 s. On 60 trials in each experimental block rtms was applied over the subject s scalp at 5 Hz. The rtms stimulation started immediately after the end of the sound file and lasted for 1000 ms (see Fig. 1). These TMS parameters were chosen, because pilot studies showed that subjects responses in our task occur mainly between 2000 and 3000 ms after the end of the sound file. Previously we used a slightly different rtms protocol (500 ms of 10 Hz rtms), but this was in number comparison tasks in which subjects gave responses within 500 ms of stimulus presentation (Göbel et al., 2001). For the current experiment we decided to use 1000 ms of rtms in order to have a longer time window of possible rtms interference. We lowered the frequency of rtms to 5 Hz to be able to have a sufficient number of TMS trials while being in line with safety regulations. The rtms trials occurred pseudorandomly with the constraint that at the end of a block each sound file had been presented twice, once with TMS and once without TMS. Fourteen subjects (six female, eight male; aged years, mean age 22 years) completed two experimental blocks (experiment 1). In one block they received stimulation over the left angular gyrus/adjacent posterior part of the intraparietal sulcus (pipl + S), in the other block TMS was applied over their right pipl + S. The order of stimulated hemispheres was counter-balanced across subjects. The remaining 14 subjects (six female, eight male; aged years, mean age 27 years) were stimulated over the occipital cortex (experiment 2; OCC). A central occipital cortex site was chosen as control site for several reasons. First, we decided to stimulate over the occipital cortex because Bjoertomt et al. (2002) have successfully used the occipital cortex as control site before in a line bisection task. Secondly, we only stimulated centrally and not over left and right occipital cortex, because there is no evidence that the lateralised noise of the discharge of the TMS coils may result in measurable attentional biases in bisection tasks (Bjoertomt et al., 2002; Fierro et al., 2000) Localisation of TMS sites High-resolution T1-weighted anatomical scans from a 3 T Varian INOVA MRI system (1.5 mm thick axial slices with 1 mm 1 mm in plane resolution) were obtained of the subjects brains. The pipl + S site was first established for each subject separately for each hemisphere by using a visual search task (for details see Ashbridge, Walsh, & Cowey, 1997; Göbel et al., 2001). The number bisection task was then carried out while rtms was delivered at the site that showed disruptive effects for the visual search task (see Fig. 2A, B, D and E). The location of the pipl + S site was confirmed by MRI scan using a method of the frameless stereotaxy. A Polaris (Northern Digital, Ontario, Canada) infra-red tracking device was used to measure the position of the subject s head and Brainsight software (Rogue Research, Montreal, Canada) was used to co-register the subject s head with the subject s MRI scan. The coil was placed between the posterior superior temporal sulcus and the intraparietal sulcus, and thus over the same brain regions as previously stimulated (see Ashbridge et al., 1997; Göbel et al., 2001; Rushworth, Ellison, & Walsh, 2001). The occipital site (OCC) was established for each subject by using the electrode system (AEEGS, 1991). Transcranial magnetic stimulation was applied at the site that was 10% of the inion nasion distance (about cm) above inion. The location of the occipital site was then confirmed by frameless stereotaxy (see Fig. 2C and F) Data analysis Bisection errors were calculated by subtracting the arithmetical midpoint from the subjective midpoint that was given by the subjects. Negative bisection errors therefore mean that subjects perceived the midpoint to be smaller than the arithmetical midpoint and positive bisection errors result when subjects perceived the midpoint to be larger than the arithmetical midpoint. Median bisection errors and reaction times were analysed separately, because there was no significant correlation between reaction times and bisection errors (r = 0.13, p = 0.43). First, we analysed only the data from trials without TMS. Two separate repeated-measures one-way ANOVAs were carried out on reaction times/bisection errors with interval size (16, 25, 36, 49, and 64) as a withinsubject factor and with session (left IPL + S, right IPL + S, OCC) as a betweensubject factor. There were no significant differences between the groups (for RT: F(8, 74) = 1.62, p = 0.13; for bisection error: F(8, 74) = 0.48, p = 0.86). To analyse the effect of TMS, we ran separate analyses for experiments 1 and 2. As the TMS effect in general did not vary for the different numerical intervals, we did not include interval size as a factor in these analyses. The effect of parietal TMS was analysed by two separate repeated-measures two-way ANOVAs on reaction times/bisection errors with trial type (control trials, trials with TMS) and hemisphere (left parietal, right parietal) as within-subject factors and order (left parietal first, right parietal first) as a between-subject factor. We followed up

4 S.M. Göbel et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) Fig. 2. TMS sites over right posterior IPS + L (A and D), left posterior IPS + L (B and E) and occipital lobe (C and F). The top row of images show axial views of an average MRI scan derived from several subjects after transformation to a standard template. Circles indicate the points where TMS was directed. Mean co-ordinates are indicated by crosshairs (right posterior IPS + L: x = 38, y = 65, z = 48; left posterior IPS + L: x = 34, y = 62, z = 50, occipital lobe: x = 1, y = 98, z = 18). The bottom row of images shows approximately axial views of a single subject s MRI scan. The MRI scan was co-registered with visible landmarks on the subject s head so that the position of the TMS coil could be measured with respect to the subject s brain during the experiment. Crosses indicate the position of the TMS coil at which rtms was administered. these analyses by separate one-way ANOVAs on reaction times/bisection errors with trial type (control trials, trials with TMS) as a within-subject factor. The effect of occipital TMS was assessed by two separate one-way ANOVAs on reaction times/bisection errors with trial type (control trials, trials with TMS) as within-subject factor. 3. Results 3.1. Behavioural results Subjects took on average 2737 ms before they responded. The larger the numerical interval the longer were their reaction times (F(4, 36) = 5.33, p = 0.002, see Fig. 3A). On average subjects underestimated the midpoint of the numerical interval by half a digit ( 0.49). However, the size of the numerical interval affected the bisection error significantly (F(4, 36) = 3.26, p = 0.02, see Fig. 3B). While subjects underestimated the midpoint for intervals 16, 25, 36 and 49, subjects overestimated the midpoint of largest interval (64) by 0.54 digits Parietal TMS Interestingly, parietal TMS did not affect RTs (F(1, 13) = 2.07, p = 0.18; see Fig. 4). However, parietal stimulation significantly shifted the perceived midpoint (F(1, 13) = 5.94, p = 0.03; see Fig. 5). When subjects were stimulated over right parietal cortex the negativity of the average bisection error decreased significantly (from 0.52 to 0.13; F(1, 13) = 4.52, p = 0.05). Transcranial magnetic stimulation over left parietal cortex also shifted the bisection error (from 0.48 to 0.25), but this effect was statistically not significant (F(1, 13) = 2.28, p = 0.16). All effects and interactions of order were nonsignificant Occipital TMS When TMS was applied over the occipital cortex reaction times decreased significantly from 2970 to 2814 ms (F(1,

5 864 S.M. Göbel et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) Fig. 3. (A) Reaction times (RTs) on control trials: subjects took longer to respond the larger the numerical interval. (B) Number bisection errors on control trials: subjects underestimated the midpoint for smaller numerical intervals. 4. Discussion Fig. 4. rtms effect on reaction times: reaction times on controls trials (in black) were not significantly different from reaction times for TMS trials (in grey) when rtms was applied over the parietal sites. TMS, however, significantly decreased reaction times when applied over the occipital site. 13) = 5.28, p = 0.04, see Fig. 4). In contrast, there was no significant effect of TMS on the bisection error (F(1, 13) = 0.53, p = 0.48; see Fig. 5) when TMS was applied over the occipital cortex. Our study provides further evidence that the posterior parietal lobe is involved in spatial number representation. We showed a significant shift of bisection errors towards the right side of space for magnetic stimulation when TMS was applied over the right posterior parietal cortex. This study presents converging evidence to number bisection studies on patients (Rossetti et al., 2004; Zorzi et al., 2002) showing that patients with right parietal damage and neglect misjudge the midpoint of a numerical interval and to findings from TMS studies of line bisection (Bjoertomt et al., 2002; Fierro, Brighina, Piazza, Oliveri, & Bisiach, 2001). Fierro et al. (2001) showed that stimulating normal subjects over right posterior parietal lobule leads to a shift in bisection errors towards the right for line bisection. These findings suggest that transcranial magnetic stimulation over the right parietal cortex is altering a representation of numbers that is likely to follow a spatial format. As it has been reported that TMS applied to this area is capable of inducing a right-sided bias for physical line bisection (Bjoertomt et al., 2002; Fierro et al., 2001) it is probably the left side of that spatial representation of numbers Fig. 5. rtms effect on number bisection errors: on control trials (in grey) subjects underestimated the midpoint of the numerical interval. On trials with repetitive TMS (in black), subjects showed a significant shift in midpoint estimation towards the right for parietal rtms (experiment 1). Occipital rtms (experiment 2), however, had no effect on number bisection errors.

6 S.M. Göbel et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) that is also altered by TMS in the present study. In our study we also found behavioural evidence that subjects performed number bisection analogous to line bisection, with smaller numbers to the left and larger numbers towards the right side of a mental number line. On average subjects underestimated the midpoint of the numerical interval. This is in line with results from an earlier study of number bisection (Calabria, Michel, Jacquin- Courtois, Göbel, & Rossetti, 2004) and is similar to findings in physical line bisection. Although this has remained a matter of debate for years, it seems reasonable to admit that when healthy subjects are tested on line bisection tasks they tend to show a slight but significant tendency to overestimate the length of the left side of the line relative to its right side, i.e. they put the midpoint of the physical line to the left of the correct midpoint (Jewell & McCourt, 2000). This has been termed pseudoneglect. Furthermore, Werth and Poppel (1988) showed that in imagined lines the direction of pseudoneglect in healthy subjects depends on the length of the line with leftward errors for short lines and rightward errors for long lines (it is however unclear whether there is a reliable effect of line length in physical line bisection in normals, see Halligan & Marshall, 1988; McCourt & Jewell, 1999). In our experiment it did not only take subjects longer to respond for larger numerical intervals (as reported before, e.g. Nuerk, Geppert, van Herten, & Willmes, 2002), but the bisection error changed significantly with the size of the numerical interval. On average subjects showed a bias towards the left side for shorter numerical lines and a bias towards the right side for the longest interval which is analogous to Werth and Poppel s findings. Hence, our behavioural data suggest that subjects solved the number bisection task in a manner analogous to line bisection and employed a spatial number representation. Alternatively both biases may be a result of a disturbance of a common magnitude processor or mechanism (Walsh, 2003). In addition, the fact that this tendency is found for every interval as in another study (Calabria et al., 2004) suggests that number bisection may be more sensitive than line bisection (see also Rossetti et al., 2004). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation had a significant effect on bisection errors when applied over the right posterior parietal site, but not when applied over the occipital site, thus showing an involvement of the posterior parietal lobe in estimating the midpoint of a numerical interval. The posterior parietal cortex has been shown to be involved in various cognitive tasks (Simon, Mangin, Cohen, Le Bihan, & Dehaene, 2002; Walsh, 2003) including number processing (Chochon et al., 1999; Dehaene, Spelke, Pinel, Stanescu, & Tsivkin, 1999; Gruber, Indefrey, Steinmetz, & Kleinschmidt, 2001) and physical line bisection (Bjoertomt et al., 2002; Fierro et al., 2001; Fink et al., 2000, 2001). Basic number processing tasks such as number comparison activate the intraparietal sulcus bilaterally (Eger et al., 2003; Fias, Lammertyn, Reynvoet, Dupont, & Orban, 2003; Pinel et al., 2004). When the posterior parietal lobe is damaged, subjects are impaired on various cognitive tasks including calculation (Cipolotti & van Harskamp, 2001; Grafman et al., 1982) and spatial cognition (Driver & Mattingley, 1998; Halligan & Marshall, 1993; Halligan et al., 2003). Neuroimaging studies, however, also suggest an involvement of the posterior parietal cortex in stimulus-response processing (Bunge, Hazeltine, Scanlon, Rosen, & Gabrieli, 2002; Rushworth, Johansen-Berg, Göbel, & Devlin, 2003; Sawamura, Shima, & Tanji, 2002) and verbal short-term memory (Paulesu, Frith, & Frackowiak, 1993; Price, 1998). The significant shift in bisection errors on trials with TMS over the right parietal site in our study, however, is unlikely to be due to impairments in stimulus-response processing or verbal short-term memory. Neither impairments in stimulus-response processing nor in verbal short-term memory predict a directional shift in number bisection errors. In both cases, however, an increase in reaction times would be predicted, but repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the parietal sites in our study had no significant effect on reaction times. The TMS effect was specific to a spatial measure, the bisection error. Transcranial magnetic stimulation over the occipital site, however, significantly decreased reaction times. This is a common finding for magnetic stimulation over task-irrelevant sites (Walsh & Pascual-Leone, 2003) and is probably due to the loud click of the coil when the magnetic pulse is discharged and to the concurrent tactile stimulation of the skull (intersensory facilitation, Hershenon, 1962). In our study we found a significant shift of bisection errors towards the right side of space for magnetic stimulation when TMS was applied over the right posterior parietal cortex. However, it could be argued that in order to solve the task, subjects had to estimate the midpoint of the numerical interval and that magnetic stimulation impaired estimation rather than a spatial number representation. The debate about whether there are separate brain systems for estimation and exact calculation is by no means decided (Cipolotti & van Harskamp, 2001; Dehaene et al., 2003; Lemer et al., 2003), and indeed estimation and approximation have been related to activity in the right posterior parietal cortex and IPS (Dehaene et al., 1999; Stanescu-Cosson et al., 2000). Furthermore, patients with left hemisphere damage (Dehaene & Cohen, 1991) have been reported who, although inefficient and inaccurate at calculation, could still approximate answers. Therefore, one could argue that right parietal TMS in our study interfered with the subjects ability to estimate a numerical answer, and not with a spatial number representation. Vuilleumier, Ortigue, and Brugger (2004) reported that neglect patients when asked to judge whether a single number was smaller or larger than 5 were selectively slower to respond to 4 and when asked to compare numbers to 7 they were selectively slower to respond to 6. This result cannot be explained by an impairment in estimation. Furthermore, this finding is specific to patients with neglect, thus strongly arguing for a spatial representation of numbers in the right parietal lobe. More evidence for an underlying spatial deficit causing shifts in number bisection stems from prism adaptation studies. Rossetti et al. (1998) showed that neglect patients improve their performance in several neuropsychological tests after prism adaptation. Further studies have confirmed and extended this results ranging from line bisection (e.g. Farné, Rossetti, Toniolo, & Ladavas, 2002) to mental imagery (Rode, Rossetti, & Biosson, 2001). Mental number bisection can also be improved following prism adaptation. Neglect patients in contrast to controls

7 866 S.M. Göbel et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) systematically overestimate the midpoint of the numerical interval, but this symptom improved markedly after prism exposure (Rossetti et al., 2004). Fewer studies have investigated cognitive effects of prism adaptation in healthy individuals (Berberovic & Mattingley, 2003; Colent, Pisella, & Rossetti, 2000; Girardi, McIntosh, Michel, Vallar, & Rossetti, 2004; Michel et al., 2003a; Michel, Rossetti, Rode, & Tilikete, 2003b). Subjects performance on line bisection tasks after exposure to left-deviating prisms was very similar to neglect patients, and the same has recently been shown for number bisection. Calabria et al. (2004) showed that exposure to left-deviating prisms in normal subjects leads to a significant shift in number bisection towards the right side of space. We therefore suggest that magnetic stimulation over right posterior parietal cortex in our experiment led to an impairment of the left side of a spatial number representation, thus leading to a systematic shift in midpoint estimation towards the right side. We did not expect TMS over left parietal cortex to have an effect on number bisection. However, in our data there is a trend for a TMS effect over left parietal cortex. Although spatial neglect has been mainly reported after right hemisphere lesions, there have been studies showing that left posterior parietal impairments can result in visuospatial impairments, typically in the right side of space (Eglin, Robertson, & Knight, 1991). In a previous study we showed that rtms over left posterior PPC impaired performance in a number comparison task especially for numbers greater than (and in spatial terms on the right of) a reference number (Göbel et al., 2001). Therefore one might have predicted that TMS over the left posterior parietal cortex shifted the midpoint estimation towards the left. But this is not what we found. Although the effect on its own is non-significant there is a surprising trend for a midpoint shift to the right, like for the right posterior parietal cortex. Interestingly, the same general tendency has been reported in studies using prism adaptation. Prism adaptation to the right is significantly efficient in patients while left-shifting prisms most often produce non-significant effects. Furthermore, the reverse is true for healthy subjects in which significant effects are found with left-shifting prisms and non-significant trends with right-shifting prisms (Colent et al., 2000). We suggest that this finding might be explained in a framework described by Berberovic and Mattingley (2003) in which a hemispheric asymmetry in the processing of spatial errors for peripersonal and extrapersonal space is proposed. They tested healthy subjects before and after prism adaptation in visual midpoint judgements. As in previous studies, there was a significant rightward shift in visual midpoint judgements after adaptation to left-deviating prisms in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. But they also found a significant rightward shift after adaptation to right-deviating prisms, which was specific for extrapersonal space. This finding is analogous to our TMS effect over left posterior parietal cortex on number bisection. Much more research certainly is needed at this early stage, but we would like to speculate that the directional TMS trend over left posterior parietal cortex might be an indication that most people represent numbers in extrapersonal space. This hypothesis can be tested, for example by assessing number bisection errors before and after prism adaptation to right-deviating prisms. Assuming that Berberovic and Mattingley (2003) framework applies and mental number lines are represented in extrapersonal space, we predict that adaptation to right-deviating prisms in normal subjects will also result in a shift of the perceived midpoint towards the right. In conclusion, our study provides evidence that subjects use spatial representations, perhaps akin to a mental number line with smaller numbers to the left side of space and larger numbers to the right side of space, in basic numerical processing tasks such as number bisection. Furthermore, we showed that the right posterior parietal cortex is crucially involved in spatial representation of numbers. Acknowledgements This work was supported by the Leverhulme Trust and the Medical Research Council. SG was a Junior Research Fellow at Jesus College, Oxford. We also thank Peter Hobden for his assistance with the MRI scans. References AEEGS. (1991). American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 8(2), Ashbridge, E., Walsh, V., & Cowey, A. (1997). Temporal aspects of visual search studied by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 35(8), Bächtold, D., Baumüller, M., & Brugger, P. (1998). Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space. Neuropsychologia, 36(8), Berberovic, N., & Mattingley, J. B. (2003). Effects of prismatic adaptation on judgements of spatial extent in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 41, Bisiach, E., & Luzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex, 14, Bisiach, E., & Vallar, G. (1988). Hemineglect in humans. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology: vol. 1 (pp ). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Bjoertomt, O., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2002). Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain, 125(9), Bunge, S. A., Hazeltine, E., Scanlon, M. D., Rosen, A. C., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Dissociable contributions to prefrontal and parietal cortices to response selection. NeuroImage, 17, Calabria, M., Michel, C., Jacquin-Courtois, S., Göbel, S. M., & Rossetti, Y. (2004). Prism adaptation in healthy subjects distorts the mental number line. Poster presentation at the First Congress of the European Neuropsychological Societies, Modena, Italy. Chochon, F., Cohen, L., van de Moortele, P. F., & Dehaene, S. (1999). Differential contributions of the left and right inferior parietal lobules to number processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(6), Cipolotti, L., & van Harskamp, N. (2001). Disturbances of number processing and calculation. In R. S. Berndt (Ed.), Language and aphasia: vol. 3 (2nd ed., pp ). Elsevier Science. Colent, C., Pisella, L., & Rossetti, Y. (2000). Cognitive bias induced by visuo-motor adaptation to prisms: A simulation of unilateral neglect in normal individuals? Neuroreport, 11(9), Dehaene, S. (2000). Cerebral bases of number processing and calculation. In M. S. Gazzanigga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3),

8 S.M. Göbel et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1991). Two mental calculation systems: A case study of severe acalculia with preserved approximation. Neuropsychologia, 29(11), Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1997). Cerebral pathways for calculation: Double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex, 33(2), Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of mathematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science, 284(5416), Driver, J., & Mattingley, J. B. (1998). Parietal neglect and visual awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 1(1), Eger, E., Sterzer, P., Russ, M. O., Giraud, A. L., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2003). A supramodal number representation in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron, 37(4), Eglin, M., Robertson, L. C., & Knight, R. T. (1991). Cortical substrates supporting visual search in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 1(3), Farné, A., Rossetti, Y., Toniolo, S., & Ladavas, E. (2002). Ameliorating neglect with prism adaptation: Visuo-manual and visuo-verbal measures. Neuropsychologia, 40(7), Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., & Orban, G. A. (2003). Parietal representation of symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), Fierro, B., Brighina, F., Oliveri, M., Piazza, A., La-Bua, V., Buffa, D., et al. (2000). Contralateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rtms in healthy subjects. Neuroreport, 11(7), Fierro, B., Brighina, F., Piazza, A., Oliveri, M., & Bisiach, E. (2001). Timing of right parietal and frontal cortex activity in visuo-spatial perception: A TMS study in normal individuals. Neuroreport, 12(11), Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Shah, N. J., Weiss, P. H., Halligan, P. W., Grosse-Ruyken, M., et al. (2000). Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fmri. Neurology, 54(6), Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Weiss, P. H., & Zilles, K. (2001). The neural basis of vertical and horizontal line bisection judgments: An fmri study of normal volunteers. NeuroImage, 14, Fischer, M. H. (2001). Number processing induces spatial performance biases. Neurology, 57(5), Fischer, M. H. (2003). Spatial representation in number processing evidence from a pointing task. Visual Cognition, 10(4), Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), Galton, F. (1980). Visualised numerals. Nature, 21, Girardi, M., McIntosh, R. D., Michel, C., Vallar, G., & Rossetti, Y. (2004). Sensorimotor effects on central space representation: Prism adaptation influences haptic and visual representations in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 42(11), Göbel, S., Walsh, V., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2001). The mental number line and the human angular gyrus. NeuroImage, 14(6), Grafman, J., Passafiume, D., Faglioni, P., & Boller, F. (1982). Calculation disturbances in adults with focal hemispheric damage. Cortex, 18(1), Gruber, O., Indefrey, P., Steinmetz, H., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2001). Dissociating neural correlates of cognitive components in mental calculation. Cerebral Cortex, 11, Halligan, P. W., Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., & Vallar, G. (2003). Spatial cognition: Evidence from visual neglect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1988). How long is a piece of string? A study of line bisection in a case of visual neglect. Cortex, 24(2), Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1993). The bisection of horizontal and radial lines: A case study of normal controls and ten patients with left visuospatial neglect. International Journal of Neuroscience, 70(3/4), Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1994). Completion in visuo-spatial neglect: A case study. Cortex, 30(4), Harris, I. M., & Miniussi, C. (2003). Parietal lobe contribution to mental rotation demonstrated with rtms. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), Hecaen, H., Angelergues, R., & Houllier, S. (1961). Les variétés cliniques des acalculias au cours des lesions rétrolandiques: Approche statistique du problème. Revue Neurologique (Paris), 102, Hershenon, M. (1962). Reaction time as measure of intersensory facilitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, Jewell, G., & McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: A review and metaanalysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), Karnath, H. O. (2001). New insights into the functions of the superior temporal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(8), Lemer, C., Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., & Cohen, L. (2003). Approximate quantities and exact number words: Dissociable systems. Neuropsychologia, 41(14), McCourt, M. E., & Jewell, G. (1999). Visuospatial attention in line bisection: Stimulus modulation of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 37(7), Michel, C., Pisella, L., Halligan, P. W., Luaute, J., Rode, G., Boisson, D., et al. (2003). Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: Implications for theory. Neuropsychologia, 41(1), Michel, C., Rossetti, Z., Rode, G., & Tilikete, C. (2003). After-effects of visuo-manual adaptation to prisms on body posture in normal subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 148, Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: OUP. Nuerk, H.-C., Geppert, B. E., van Herten, M., & Willmes, K. (2002). On the impact of different number representations in the number bisection task. Cortex, 38, Paulesu, E., Frith, C. D., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1993). The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature, 362(6418), Pinel, P., Piazza, M., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron, 41(6), Price, C. J. (1998). The functional anatomy of word comprehension and production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(8), Pridmore, S., Fernandes Filho, J. A., Nahas, Z., Liberatos, C., & George, M. S. (1998). Motor threshold in transcranial magnetic stimulation: A comparison of a neurophysiological method and a visualization of movement method. Journal of Electroconvulsive Therapy, 14(1), Restle, F. (1970). Speed of adding and comparing numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83(2), Rickard, T. C., Romero, S. G., Basso, G., Wharton, C., Flitman, S., & Grafman, J. (2000). The calculating brain: An fmri study. Neuropsychologia, 38, Rode, G., Rossetti, Y., & Boisson, D. (2001). Prism adaptation improves representational neglect. Neuropsychologia, 39(11), Rossetti, Y., Jacquin-Courtois, S., Rode, G., Ota, H., Michel, C., & Boisson, D. (2004). Does action make the link between number and space representation? Visuo-manual adaptation improves number bisection in unilateral neglect. Psychological Science, 15(6), Rossetti, Y., & Rode, G. (2002). In H.-O. Karnath (Ed.), Reducing spatial neglect by visual and other sensory manipulation: Noncognitive (physiological) routes to the rehabilitation of a cognitive disorder (pp ). London: Oxford University Press. Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., Pisella, L., Farne, A., Li, L., Boisson, D., et al. (1998). Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature, 395(6698), Rushworth, M. F. S., Ellison, A., & Walsh, V. (2001). Complementary localization and lateralization of orienting and motor attention. Nature Neuroscience, 4(6),

9 868 S.M. Göbel et al. / Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) Rushworth, M. F. S., Johansen-Berg, H., Göbel, S. M., & Devlin, J. T. (2003). The left parietal and premotor cortices: Motor attention and selection. NeuroImage, 20, Sawamura, H., Shima, K., & Tanji, J. (2002). Numerical representation for action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. Nature, 415(6874), Seron, X., Pesenti, M., Noel, M.-P., Deloche, G., & Cornet, J.-A. (1992). Images of numbers: Or When 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue. Cognition, 44(1/2), Simon, O., Mangin, J. F., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Topographical layout of hand, eye, calculation, and language-related areas in the human parietal lobe. Neuron, 33(3), Stanescu-Cosson, R., Pinel, P., van de Moortele, P. F., Le Bihan, D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2000). Understanding dissociations in dyscalculia: A brain imaging study of the impact of number size on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate calculation. Brain, 123(11), Vallar, G., & Perani, D. (1986). The anatomy of unilateral neglect after righthemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/ct-scan correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia, 24(5), Vuilleumier, P., Ortigue, S., & Brugger, P. (2004). The number space and neglect. Cortex, 40(2), Vuilleumier, P., & Rafal, R. (1999). Both means more than two : Localizing and counting in patients with visuospatial neglect. Nature Neuroscience, 2(9), Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(11), Walsh, V., Ellison, A., Ashbridge, E., & Cowey, A. (1999). The role of the parietal cortex in visual attention Hemispheric asymmetries and the effects of learning: A magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia, 37(2), Walsh, V., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2003). Transcranial magnetic stimulation A neurochronometrics of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Wassermann, E. M. (1996). Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: Report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology: Evoked Potentials, 108(1), Werth, R., & Poppel, E. (1988). Compression and lateral shift of mental coordinate systems in a line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 26(5), Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umilta, C. (2002). Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417(6885),

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

NeuroImage 44 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 44 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 44 (2009) 563 568 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg The role of the angular gyrus in the modulation of visuospatial attention

More information

Visuospatial priming of the mental number line

Visuospatial priming of the mental number line Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Cognition 106 (2008) 770 779 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Visuospatial priming of the mental number line Ivilin Stoianov *, Peter Kramer, Carlo Umiltà, Marco

More information

Time Perception in Spatial Neglect: A Distorted Representation?

Time Perception in Spatial Neglect: A Distorted Representation? Neuropsychology 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 25, No. 2, 193 200 0894-4105/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021304 Time Perception in Spatial Neglect: A Distorted Representation? Marco Calabria

More information

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Session 4 Virtual Lesion Approach I Alexandra Reichenbach MPI for Biological Cybernetics Tübingen, Germany Today s Schedule Virtual Lesion Approach : Study Design Rationale

More information

Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory

Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 25 39 Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory Carine Michel a, Laure Pisella a, Peter W. Halligan b, Jacques Luauté a, Gilles

More information

Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits following TMS

Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits following TMS 501 Journal of Neuropsychology (2008), 2, 501 508 q 2008 The British Psychological Society The British Psychological Society www.bpsjournals.co.uk Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits

More information

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Sensory to Motor Transformation Sensory information (visual,

More information

Unilateral Spatial Neglect USN

Unilateral Spatial Neglect USN 86 214 28 2 Behavioural Inattention Test Catherine Bergego Scale 28 2 214 223 2008 Key Words unilateral spatial neglect language disturbance mechanism priming rehabilitation Unilateral Spatial Neglect

More information

The effect of orientation on number word processing

The effect of orientation on number word processing Acta Psychologica 124 (2007) 370 381 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The effect of orientation on number word processing Roi Cohen Kadosh *, Avishai Henik, Orly Rubinsten Department of Behavioral Sciences,

More information

Facilitating visuospatial attention for the contralateral hemifield by repetitive TMS on the posterior parietal cortex

Facilitating visuospatial attention for the contralateral hemifield by repetitive TMS on the posterior parietal cortex Neuroscience Letters 382 (2005) 280 285 Facilitating visuospatial attention for the contralateral hemifield by repetitive TMS on the posterior parietal cortex Yun-Hee Kim a,b,, Soo-Jung Min b,1, Myoung-Hwan

More information

Look, no hands: A perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention

Look, no hands: A perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention Nicholls, Loftus & Gevers 1 Look, no hands: A perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention Michael E.R. Nicholls* Andrea M. Loftus* Wim Gevers # *Department of Psychology University

More information

ABSTRACT THE NUMBER SPACE AND NEGLECT

ABSTRACT THE NUMBER SPACE AND NEGLECT ABSTRACT THE NUMBER SPACE AND NEGLECT Patrik Vuilleumier 1, Stéphanie Ortigue 2 and Peter Brugger 3 ( 1 Laboratory of Behavioural Neurology and Imaging of Cognition, Geneva; 2 Functional Brain Mapping

More information

The differential involvement of inferior parietal lobule in number comparison: a rtms study

The differential involvement of inferior parietal lobule in number comparison: a rtms study Neuropsychologia 42 (2004) 1902 1909 The differential involvement of inferior parietal lobule in number comparison: a rtms study Marco Sandrini a,b,, Paolo M. Rossini a,c, Carlo Miniussi a a IRCCS S. Giovanni

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

Objective correlates of an unusual subjective experience: A single-case study of number form synaesthesia

Objective correlates of an unusual subjective experience: A single-case study of number form synaesthesia COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2006, 23 (8), 1162 1173 Objective correlates of an unusual subjective experience: A single-case study of number form synaesthesia M. Piazza and P. Pinel INSERM, CEA, Cognitive

More information

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B87 Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized Wim Gevers*,

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

HEMIEXTINCTION INDUCED BY TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION OVER THE RIGHT TEMPORO-PARIETAL JUNCTION

HEMIEXTINCTION INDUCED BY TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION OVER THE RIGHT TEMPORO-PARIETAL JUNCTION Neuroscience 142 (2006) 119 123 HEMIEXTINCTION INDUCED BY TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION OVER THE RIGHT TEMPORO-PARIETAL JUNCTION I. G. MEISTER,* M. WIENEMANN, D. BUELTE, C. GRÜNEWALD, R. SPARING, N.

More information

Event-related TMS over the right posterior parietal cortex induces ipsilateral visuospatial

Event-related TMS over the right posterior parietal cortex induces ipsilateral visuospatial COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE NEUROREPORT Event-related TMS over the right posterior parietal cortex induces ipsilateral visuospatial interference Gilles Pourtois, 1,2,CA Yves Vandermeeren, 2 Etienne Olivier

More information

Judgments involving symbolic numbers systematically activate

Judgments involving symbolic numbers systematically activate Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: A functional MRI study Fulvia Castelli, Daniel E. Glaser, and Brian Butterworth Division of the Humanities and Social Sciences, California

More information

An exploration of the role of the superior temporal gyrus in visual search and spatial perception using TMS

An exploration of the role of the superior temporal gyrus in visual search and spatial perception using TMS DOI: 10.1093/brain/awh244 Brain (2004), 127, 2307 2315 An exploration of the role of the superior temporal gyrus in visual search and spatial perception using TMS Amanda Ellison, Igor Schindler, Lara L.

More information

Sensorimotor effects on central space representation: prism adaptation influences haptic and visual representations in normal subjects

Sensorimotor effects on central space representation: prism adaptation influences haptic and visual representations in normal subjects Neuropsychologia 42 (2004) 1477 1487 Sensorimotor effects on central space representation: prism adaptation influences haptic and visual representations in normal subjects M. Girardi a,b, R. D. McIntosh

More information

Explicit versus Implicit Processing of Representational Space in Neglect: Dissociations in Accessing the Mental Number Line

Explicit versus Implicit Processing of Representational Space in Neglect: Dissociations in Accessing the Mental Number Line Explicit versus Implicit Processing of Representational Space in Neglect: Dissociations in Accessing the Mental Number Line Konstantinos Priftis 1,2, Marco Zorzi 1, Francesca Meneghello 2, Roberto Marenzi

More information

Effect of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation(TMS) on Visual Search Task

Effect of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation(TMS) on Visual Search Task INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF APPLIED BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING VOL.2, NO.2 2009 1 Effect of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation(TMS) on Visual Search Task K. Iramina, Guest member ABSTRACT Transcranial magnetic

More information

Durham Research Online

Durham Research Online Durham Research Online Deposited in DRO: 08 February 2010 Version of attached file: Accepted Version Peer-review status of attached file: Peer-reviewed Citation for published item: Ellison, A. and Schindler,

More information

Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients

Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients NeuroImage 14, S98 S102 (2001) doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0815, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients Anna Berti,*,1 Nicola Smania, and Alan Allport

More information

Shared Spatial Representations for Numbers and Space: The Reversal of the SNARC and the Simon Effects

Shared Spatial Representations for Numbers and Space: The Reversal of the SNARC and the Simon Effects Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 5, 1197 1207 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.5.1197

More information

UNILATERAL-SPATIAL INATTENTION

UNILATERAL-SPATIAL INATTENTION UNILATERAL-SPATIAL INATTENTION \ ICBO CONGRESS POMONA. CA APRIL 8, 2010 ROBERT B. SANET, O.D., F.C.O.V.D. San Diego Center for Vision Care 7898 Broadway Lemon Grove CA 91945 rsanet@cs.com UNILATERAL SPATIAL

More information

Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments

Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2010, 17 (2), 219-223 doi:10.3758/pbr.17.2.219 Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments ROLF REBER University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway PASCAL WURTZ

More information

Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements. Influence Spatial-Numerical Associations

Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements. Influence Spatial-Numerical Associations Running head: CODING STRATEGIES IN NUMBER SPACE Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements Influence Spatial-Numerical Associations Oliver Lindemann 1, 2, Juan M. Abolafia 1,3, Jay Pratt 4

More information

Abstract. stimuli (see SNARC effect) have identified an attentional bias induced by numeric

Abstract. stimuli (see SNARC effect) have identified an attentional bias induced by numeric Abstract Previous studies investigating interactions between spatial attention and numeric stimuli (see SNARC effect) have identified an attentional bias induced by numeric stimuli in accordance with a

More information

Carlo Semenza (University of Padova) Simple Calculation In The Brain: Evidence From Direct Cortical Electro-Stimulation

Carlo Semenza (University of Padova) Simple Calculation In The Brain: Evidence From Direct Cortical Electro-Stimulation Carlo Semenza (University of Padova) Simple Calculation In The Brain: Evidence From Direct Cortical Electro-Stimulation New Approaches To The Neural Basis of Mathematical Cognition. Symposium 9 How do

More information

University of Padova, Padova, Italy b University College London, London, UK PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

University of Padova, Padova, Italy b University College London, London, UK PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Agrillo, Christian] On: 15 March 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 919880202] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

This article was published in an Elsevier journal. The attached copy is furnished to the author for non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the author s institution, sharing

More information

Research Article Long-Term Efficacy of Prism Adaptation on Spatial Neglect: Preliminary Results on Different Spatial Components

Research Article Long-Term Efficacy of Prism Adaptation on Spatial Neglect: Preliminary Results on Different Spatial Components The Scientific World Journal Volume 2012, Article ID 618528, 8 pages doi:10.1100/2012/618528 The cientificworldjournal Research Article Long-Term Efficacy of Prism Adaptation on Spatial Neglect: Preliminary

More information

Representational pseudoneglect in line bisection

Representational pseudoneglect in line bisection Psychon Bull Rev (2012) 19:879 883 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0285-z BRIEF REPORT Representational pseudoneglect in line bisection Stephen Darling & Robert H. Logie & Sergio Della Sala Published online: 13

More information

Neuroanatomical Approaches to the Study of Mathematical Ability and Disability Tony J. Simon and Susan M. Rivera

Neuroanatomical Approaches to the Study of Mathematical Ability and Disability Tony J. Simon and Susan M. Rivera Chapter 13 Neuroanatomical Approaches to the Study of Mathematical Ability and Disability Tony J. Simon and Susan M. Rivera + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + Our goal

More information

Gender differences in brain activation patterns during mental rotation and number related cognitive tasks

Gender differences in brain activation patterns during mental rotation and number related cognitive tasks Psychology Science, Volume 47, 2005 (1), p. 112-131 Gender differences in brain activation patterns during mental rotation and number related cognitive tasks KARIN KUCIAN 1,THOMAS LOENNEKER 1,THOMAS DIETRICH

More information

Dissociated long lasting improvements of straight-ahead pointing and line bisection tasks in two hemineglect patients

Dissociated long lasting improvements of straight-ahead pointing and line bisection tasks in two hemineglect patients Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 327 334 www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Dissociated long lasting improvements of straight-ahead pointing and line bisection tasks in two hemineglect patients Laure Pisella

More information

The Predictive Nature of Pseudoneglect for Visual Neglect: Evidence from Parietal Theta Burst Stimulation

The Predictive Nature of Pseudoneglect for Visual Neglect: Evidence from Parietal Theta Burst Stimulation The Predictive Nature of Pseudoneglect for Visual Neglect: Evidence from Parietal Theta Burst Stimulation Alice Varnava 1,2 *, Martynas Dervinis 2, Christopher D. Chambers 2 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab This report details my summer research project for the REU Theoretical and Computational Neuroscience program as

More information

rtms over the intraparietal sulcus disrupts numerosity processing

rtms over the intraparietal sulcus disrupts numerosity processing Exp Brain Res (2007) 179:631 642 DOI 10.1007/s00221-006-0820-0 RESEARCH ARTICLE rtms over the intraparietal sulcus disrupts numerosity processing Marinella Cappelletti Hilary Barth Felipe Fregni Elizabeth

More information

Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Lisa Koski, PhD, Clin Psy TMS Neurorehabilitation Lab Royal Victoria Hospital 2009-12-14 BIC Seminar, MNI Overview History, basic principles, instrumentation

More information

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Edinburgh Research Explorer Edinburgh Research Explorer The automatic pilot of the hand is unbalanced by visual neglect Citation for published version: McIntosh, RD, Rossit, S, Malhotra, P, Harvey, M & Butler, SH 2010, 'The automatic

More information

The importance of response type to the relationship between temporal order and numerical magnitude

The importance of response type to the relationship between temporal order and numerical magnitude Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:1604 1613 DOI 10.3758/s13414-011-0114-x The importance of response type to the relationship between temporal order and numerical magnitude Michael E. R. Nicholls & Megan

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuroreport. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 5.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuroreport. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 5. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Neuroreport. 2011 October 5; 22(14): 700 705. doi:10.1097/wnr.0b013e32834a3e20. Effects of prism adaptation on motor-intentional spatial

More information

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Submitted to Current Biology Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Anna Plodowski, Rachel Swainson, Georgina M. Jackson, Chris Rorden and Stephen R. Jackson School of Psychology

More information

Contribution of frontal cortex to the spatial representation of number

Contribution of frontal cortex to the spatial representation of number ARTICLE IN PRESS available at www.sciencedirect.com journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Research report Contribution of frontal cortex to the spatial representation of number Elena Rusconi

More information

Number magnitude to finger mapping is disembodied and topological

Number magnitude to finger mapping is disembodied and topological Exp Brain Res () 9:9 DOI.7/s---x RESEARCH ARTICLE Number magnitude to finger mapping is disembodied and topological Myrthe A. Plaisier Jeroen B. J. Smeets Received: 7 August / Accepted: 7 January / Published

More information

The effect of musical expertise on the representation of space

The effect of musical expertise on the representation of space The effect of musical expertise on the representation of space The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation Published

More information

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp 1288-1290 Improved executive functioning following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation [Brief Communications] Moser,

More information

SPECIAL INTEREST GROUP MEETING

SPECIAL INTEREST GROUP MEETING SPECIAL INTEREST GROUP MEETING Cognitive Neurorehabilitation S. Clarke & G. Rode Service de Médecine Physique et Réadaptation, Hôpital Henry Gabrielle. Hospices Civils de Lyon, gilles.rode@chu-lyon.fr

More information

Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Groningen, The Netherlands

Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Groningen, The Netherlands This article was downloaded by:[radboud University Nijmegan] On: 9 January 2008 Access Details: [subscription number 732592891] Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers

Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Cognition 106 (2008) 1441 1450 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers Yi-hui

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

From SNARC to SQUARC: Universal Mental Quantity Line?

From SNARC to SQUARC: Universal Mental Quantity Line? From SNARC to SQUARC: Universal Mental Quantity Line? Atanas Kirjakovski 1 & Narisuke Utsuki 1 1 Graduate School of Intercultural Studies, Kobe University, Kobe, Japan Correspondence: Atanas Kirjakovski,

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Making the blindsighted see

Making the blindsighted see Neuropsychologia 45 (2007) 3346 3350 Note Making the blindsighted see Juha Silvanto a,, Alan Cowey b, Nilli Lavie a, Vincent Walsh a a Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychology,

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Priming the Mental Time Line Maria Grazia Di Bono, Marco Casarotti, Konstantinos Priftis, Lucia Gava, Carlo Umiltà, and Marco Zorzi

More information

THE SPATIAL REPRESENTATION OF NON-SYMBOLIC NUMERICAL QUANTITIES

THE SPATIAL REPRESENTATION OF NON-SYMBOLIC NUMERICAL QUANTITIES THE SPATIAL REPRESENTATION OF NON-SYMBOLIC NUMERICAL QUANTITIES Riccardo Luccio, Antonia Fumarola, Giorgia Tamburini, & Tiziano Agostini Department of Life Sciences, University of Trieste Abstract. Dehaene

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository

City, University of London Institutional Repository City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Cappelletti, M., Lee, H. L., Freeman, E. D. & Price, C. J. (2010). The role of right and left parietal lobes in the conceptual

More information

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight? How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? What happens to visual-devoted brain structure in individuals who are born deprived of sight?

More information

Left hand movements and right hemisphere activation in unilateral spatial neglect: a test of the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis

Left hand movements and right hemisphere activation in unilateral spatial neglect: a test of the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1350 1355 Left hand movements and right hemisphere activation in unilateral spatial neglect: a test of the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis Guido Gainotti a,, Roberta Perri

More information

PRISM ADAPTATION IMPROVES CHRONIC VISUAL AND HAPTIC NEGLECT: A SINGLE CASE STUDY

PRISM ADAPTATION IMPROVES CHRONIC VISUAL AND HAPTIC NEGLECT: A SINGLE CASE STUDY PRISM ADAPTATION IMPROVES CHRONIC VISUAL AND HAPTIC NEGLECT: A SINGLE CASE STUDY Robert D. McIntosh 1, Yves Rossetti 2 and A. David Milner 1 ( 1 Department of Psychology, University of Durham, UK; 2 Espace

More information

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Selective Attention Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form,

More information

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists Visual Imagery Study of Imagery Banned by behaviorists Possible subject of study in cognitive psychology Cognitive psychology is distinguished from the earlier behaviorism by its claim that there are internal

More information

The spatial representation of numbers: evidence from neglect and pseudoneglect

The spatial representation of numbers: evidence from neglect and pseudoneglect Exp Brain Res (2009) 192:561 569 DOI 10.1007/s00221-008-1623-2 REVIEW The spatial representation of numbers: evidence from neglect and pseudoneglect Carlo Umiltà Konstantinos Priftis Marco Zorzi Received:

More information

How is Number Associated with Space? The Role of Working Memory

How is Number Associated with Space? The Role of Working Memory C H A P T E R 10 How is Number Associated with Space? The Role of Working Memory Wim Fias,* Jean-Philippe van Dijck* and Wim Gevers * Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Belgium Unité

More information

Improving neglect by TMS

Improving neglect by TMS Behavioural Neurology 17 (2006) 169 176 169 IOS Press Improving neglect by TMS Brigida Fierro a, Filippo Brighina a, and Edoardo Bisiach b a Dipartimento di Neurologia, Oftalmologia, Otorinolaringoiatria

More information

When Numbers Get Heavy: Is the Mental Number Line Exclusively Numerical?

When Numbers Get Heavy: Is the Mental Number Line Exclusively Numerical? When Numbers Get Heavy: Is the Mental Number Line Exclusively Numerical? Kevin J. Holmes 1 *, Stella F. Lourenco 2 1 Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley, California, United States

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

Two eyes make a pair: facial organization and perceptual learning reduce visual extinction

Two eyes make a pair: facial organization and perceptual learning reduce visual extinction Neuropsychologia 39 (21) 1144 1149 www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Two eyes make a pair: facial organization and perceptual learning reduce visual extinction Patrik Vuilleumier a, *, Noam Sagiv

More information

REHABILITATION OF UNILATERAL spatial neglect. Rehabilitation of Unilateral Spatial Neglect: New Insights From Magnetic Resonance Perfusion Imaging

REHABILITATION OF UNILATERAL spatial neglect. Rehabilitation of Unilateral Spatial Neglect: New Insights From Magnetic Resonance Perfusion Imaging S43 REVIEW ARTICLE Rehabilitation of Unilateral Spatial Neglect: New Insights From Magnetic Resonance Perfusion Imaging Argye E. Hillis, MD ABSTRACT. Hillis AE. Rehabilitation of unilateral spatial neglect:

More information

Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments

Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments Neuron, Vol. 41, 1 20, March 25, 2004, Copyright 2004 by Cell Press Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments Philippe Pinel,* Manuela

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso Control of visuo-spatial attention Emiliano Macaluso CB demo Attention Limited processing resources Overwhelming sensory input cannot be fully processed => SELECTIVE PROCESSING Selection via spatial orienting

More information

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above.

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above. Section: Chapter 14: Multiple Choice 1. Alex the parrot: pp.529-530 shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all

More information

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. Cortical Organization Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. 2. Secondary cortex: located immediately adjacent to primary cortical areas,

More information

Commentary to Dijkerman & de Haan: Somatosensory processes subserving perception

Commentary to Dijkerman & de Haan: Somatosensory processes subserving perception Commentary to Dijkerman & de Haan: Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action. Appeared on Behavioral and Brain Sciences (BBS), 30, 2, 221-222. Body image and body schema: The shared representation

More information

Dominant Limb Motor Impersistence Associated with Anterior Callosal Disconnection

Dominant Limb Motor Impersistence Associated with Anterior Callosal Disconnection Dominant Limb Motor Impersistence Associated with Anterior Callosal Disconnection S. W. Seo, MD, 1 K. Jung, MS, 2 H. You, PhD 2, E. J. Kim, MD, PhD 1, B. H. Lee, MA, 1 D. L. Na, MD 1 1 Department of Neurology,

More information

On the Structure and Neural Correlates of the Numerical Magnitude Representation and its Influence in the Assessment of Verbal Working Memory

On the Structure and Neural Correlates of the Numerical Magnitude Representation and its Influence in the Assessment of Verbal Working Memory On the Structure and Neural Correlates of the Numerical Magnitude Representation and its Influence in the Assessment of Verbal Working Memory Von der Philosophischen Fakultät der Rheinisch-Westfälischen

More information

LOST IN SPACE THE FATE OF MEMORY REPRESENTATIONS FOR NON- NEGLECTED STIMULI

LOST IN SPACE THE FATE OF MEMORY REPRESENTATIONS FOR NON- NEGLECTED STIMULI LOST IN SPACE THE FATE OF MEMORY REPRESENTATIONS FOR NON- NEGLECTED STIMULI Susanne Ferber 1 and James Danckert 2 1 Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition 1 Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition Intro Memory Empirical Studies Interf MR Scan LTM Codes DCT Imagery & Spatial Cognition Rel Org Principles ImplEnc SpatEq Neuro Imaging Critique StruEq Prop

More information

Title: Non-Spatial Neglect for the Mental Number Line. Authors: Jean-Philippe van Dijck, Wim Gevers, Christophe Lafosse, Fabrizio Doricchi, Wim Fias

Title: Non-Spatial Neglect for the Mental Number Line. Authors: Jean-Philippe van Dijck, Wim Gevers, Christophe Lafosse, Fabrizio Doricchi, Wim Fias Title: Non-Spatial Neglect for the Mental Number Line. Authors: Jean-Philippe van Dijck, Wim Gevers, Christophe Lafosse, Fabrizio Doricchi, Wim Fias PII: S00-()00- DOI: doi:0.0/j.neuropsychologia..0.00

More information

AUTHOR S QUERY SHEET UNCORRECTED PROOF

AUTHOR S QUERY SHEET UNCORRECTED PROOF AUTHOR S QUERY SHEET Author(s): MICHEL et al. PCNP 203232 Article title: Article no: Dear Author Some questions have arisen during the preparation of your manuscript for typesetting. Please consider each

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [University of California Los Angeles] On: 29 September 2008 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 794341886] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered

More information

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest Experimental Design Kate Watkins Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford With thanks to: Heidi Johansen-Berg Joe Devlin Outline Choices for experimental paradigm Subtraction / hierarchical

More information

Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fmri

Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fmri Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 1238 1248 Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fmri Roi Cohen Kadosh a, Avishai Henik a,, Orly Rubinsten a, Harald Mohr b, Halit Dori

More information

Getting a Grip on Numbers: Numerical Magnitude Priming in Object Grasping

Getting a Grip on Numbers: Numerical Magnitude Priming in Object Grasping Running head: GETTING A GRIP ON NUMBERS Getting a Grip on Numbers: Numerical Magnitude Priming in Object Grasping Oliver Lindemann 1, 2, Juan M. Abolafia 1, Giovanna Girardi 3 & Harold Bekkering 1 1 Radboud

More information

Unilateral spatial inattention (USI),

Unilateral spatial inattention (USI), PRISM ADAPTATION IN THE REHABILITATION OF PATIENTS WITH UNILATERAL SPATIAL INATTENTION Maura E. Massucci, OD Gibsonia, PA Abstract Unilateral spatial inattention (USI), also known as neglect, can occur

More information

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg The concept that different parts of the brain did different things started with Spurzheim and Gall, whose phrenology became quite

More information

Seeing through the tongue: cross-modal plasticity in the congenitally blind

Seeing through the tongue: cross-modal plasticity in the congenitally blind International Congress Series 1270 (2004) 79 84 Seeing through the tongue: cross-modal plasticity in the congenitally blind Ron Kupers a, *, Maurice Ptito b www.ics-elsevier.com a Center for Functionally

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Supplemental Information. Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain. Disrupts Melody Processing

Supplemental Information. Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain. Disrupts Melody Processing Current Biology, Volume 27 Supplemental Information Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain Disrupts Melody Processing Frank E. Garcea, Benjamin L. Chernoff, Bram Diamond, Wesley Lewis, Maxwell

More information

Pain is more than an unpleasant feeling associated with a somatosensory sensation.

Pain is more than an unpleasant feeling associated with a somatosensory sensation. Where is my pain? Pain is more than an unpleasant feeling associated with a somatosensory sensation. It is an important signal that prompts identification, localization, and reaction to a potential physical

More information

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION INTRODUCTION Research on the Deaf Brain is beginning to provide a new evidence base for policy and practice in relation to intervention with deaf children. This talk outlines the multi-channel nature of

More information

Can brain stimulation help with relearning movement after stroke?

Can brain stimulation help with relearning movement after stroke? stroke.org.uk Final report summary Can brain stimulation help with relearning movement after stroke? The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning after stroke PROJECT CODE: TSA

More information