A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE UNA MIRADA MÁS PROFUNDA A LA TASA DE REFORZAMIENTO Y EL RESURGIMIENTO

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE UNA MIRADA MÁS PROFUNDA A LA TASA DE REFORZAMIENTO Y EL RESURGIMIENTO"

Transcription

1 REVISTA MEXICANA DE ANÁLISIS DE LA CONDUCTA MEXICAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 215 VOL. 41, NÚMERO 2 (SEPTIEMBRE) NUMBER 2 (September) A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE UNA MIRADA MÁS PROFUNDA A LA TASA DE REFORZAMIENTO Y EL RESURGIMIENTO SHUN FUJIMAKI 1, 2, KENNON A. LATTAL 3, AND TAKAYUKI SAKAGAMI 1 1 KEIO UNIVERSITY 2 JAPAN SOCIETY FOR THE PROMOTION OF SCIENCE 3 WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY Abstract Resurgence is the recurrence of a previously reinforced and then extinguished response when a more recently reinforced response is extinguished. The purpose of the present experiments was to examine the relation between alternative reinforcement rates and resurgence with pigeons. Each experiment consisted of a three-phase procedure and each phase of three experiments was a two-component multiple schedule, except for Experiments 2a and 2b, which employed single schedules. In each experiment, the target response was reinforced according to a variable interval (VI) schedule in the Acquisition phase. In the Elimination phase, the target response was eliminated using either extinction or a differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO) schedule and the alternative response was reinforced according to a VI schedule. The rate of alternative reinforcement differed between components, but the number of reinforcers per hour (and thus the reinforcement ratio) was 6 and 18 in the Lean and Rich Correspondence: Shun Fujimaki, Department of Psychology, Keio University, , Minato-ku, Tokyo , Japan. fujimaki126@gmail.com. This research was supported by grant from Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (14J914) to the first author and a Global Professorship granted to the second author by Keio University. The authors thank Azusa Matsunaga for her help in running the experiments.

2 A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE 117 components, respectively. In the Resurgence phase, all reinforcers were withheld and resurgence was compared between components. Across all experiments, results were inconsistent. In some instances there was greater resurgence in the Rich component, but the opposite was observed in others. These results do not offer evidence supporting the prediction of behavioral momentum and related models of resurgence. Keywords: resurgence, extinction, alternative reinforcement, behavioral momentum, multiple schedule, pigeons Resumen El resurgimiento es la recurrencia de una respuesta previamente reforzada y luego extinguida cuando una respuesta más recientemente reforzada es extinguida. El propósito de los presentes experimentos fue examinar la relación entre las tasas de reforzamiento alternativo y el resurgimiento en palomas. Cada experimento consistió de un procedimiento de tres fases y cada fase de tres experimentos fue un programa múltiple de dos componentes, excepto para los Experimentos 2a y 2b, en los que se emplearon programas simples. En cada experimento, se reforzó la respuesta blanco conforme a un programa de intervalo variable (IV) en la fase de Adquisición. En la fase de Eliminación, la respuesta blanco se eliminó usando ya sea extinción o un programa de reforzamiento diferencial de otras conductas (RDO) y la respuesta alternativa se reforzó conforme a un programa IV. La tasa de reforzamiento alternativo difirió entre componentes, pero el número de reforzadores por hora (y por lo tanto la proporción de reforzamiento) fue de 6 y 18 en los componentes pobres y densos, respectivamente. En la fase de Resurgimiento, todos los reforzadores fueron descontinuados y se comparó el resurgimiento entre componentes. A través de todos los experimentos, los resultados fueron inconsistentes. En algunas instancias hubo un mayor resurgimiento en el componente denso, pero se observó lo contrario en otras. Estos resultados no ofrecen evidencia que apoye la predicción del momento conductual y los modelos de resurgimiento relacionados. Palabras clave: resurgimiento, extinción, reforzamiento alternativo, momento conductual, programa múltiple, palomas Resurgence is the recurrence of a previously reinforced and subsequently extinguished response when a more recently reinforced response is extinguished (Epstein, 1983, 1985). In a typical laboratory study, resurgence is examined by using a threephase procedure (e.g., Carey, 1951; Leitenberg, Rawson, & Bath, 197; Lieving & Lattal, 23). In the Acquisition phase, a target response is reinforced. In the Elimination phase, the target response is eliminated and an alternative response is reinforced. In the Resurgence phase, the alternative response also is eliminated. An

3 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al. 118 increase in the rate or probability of the target response in the third phase relative to that observed in the Elimination phase defines resurgence. Variables that affect resurgence may be associated with each of the three phases. With respect to the effects of variables in the Elimination phase on resurgence, conflicting results have been reported. For example, Leitenberg, Rawson, and Mulick (1975) reported that higher alternative reinforcement rates produced greater resurgence. In their study, key pecking of pigeons was reinforced on a variable interval (VI) 12 s schedule in the Acquisition phase. In the subsequent Elimination phase, an alternative lever-press response was reinforced according to a VI 3 s schedule in one (Rich) group and a VI 24 s schedule in another (Lean) group. The Rich group showed more rapid extinction of the target response in the Elimination phase and greater resurgence of the target response in the Resurgence phase. Leitenberg et al. s (1975) findings suggest that the reinforcement rate in the Elimination phase is an important variable that should be accounted for by models of resurgence. For example, Shahan and Sweeney (211) updated the model proposed by Podlesnik and Shahan (21; see also Podlesnik & Shahan, 29) to account for rapid extinction and greater resurgence under higher rates of alternative reinforcement. The model identifies two roles for alternative reinforcement during the Elimination phase. One is that alternative reinforcement itself has a disruptive effect on the target response. This assumption makes it possible to explain more rapid extinction of the target response under higher rates of alternative reinforcement. The other one is that alternative reinforcement contributes to the overall strength of the target response. This second assumption plays a critical role in explaining greater resurgence under higher rates of alternative reinforcement. Podlesnik and Shahan (29) showed that the Pavlovian stimulus reinforcer relation, widely considered to be the mechanism of resistance to change, also could determine the magnitude of resurgence, at least in relative terms (i.e., where the proportion of baseline response rate is used as the measure of resurgence). In their study, target responses in both the Rich and Lean components were reinforced on VI 12 s schedules in the Acquisition phase. In addition, response independent reinforcers delivered according to a variable time (VT) 2 s schedule were added in the Rich component (i.e., conjoint VI 12 s VT 2 s schedules were in effect in the Rich component). In the Resurgence phase, resurgence in the Rich component was greater than that in the Lean component, despite the lower response rates in that component during the Acquisition phase. They concluded that the Pavlovian stimulus reinforcer relation determined the magnitude of resurgence on a relative scale (see also Podlesnik & Shahan, 21). Note that an important aspect of the Pavlovian stimulus reinforcer relation is that the source of reinforcement does not matter. In other words, all reinforcers obtained in that context contribute to the response strength. It follows from this behavioral momentum perspective that the rate of alternative reinforcement in the Elimination phase affects the strength of target response and hence resurgence.

4 A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE 119 Other models for resurgence also assign a role to the rate of alternative reinforcement in resurgence, but with assumptions other than those of behavioral momentum theory (e.g., the response prevention hypothesis; Leitenberg et al. 1975; Rawson, Leitenberg, Mulick, & Lefebvre, 1977; see also Cleland, Foster, & Temple, 2). Several more recent studies, however, have reported results inconsistent with those of Leitenberg et al. (1975) and with the prediction of these models. Winterbauer and Bouton (21), for example, failed to show differential resurgence when a random-interval (RI) 1 s and a RI 3 s reinforcement schedules were in effect in the their Rich and Lean groups, respectively, during the Elimination phase. Cançado and Lattal (213) also examined the same issue by using within subject comparisons and did not find differential resurgence when several different values of a differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO) schedule were in effect in the Elimination phase. Taken together, models for resurgence incorporate the effects of the alternative reinforcement and predict greater resurgence under higher alternative reinforcement rates, but the aforementioned two recent studies have reported results at odds with these predictions. Cançado and Lattal s experiments used DRO schedules rather than the VI schedules used by others to arrange differential rates of reinforcement of the alternative response, which may contribute to their results in comparison to Leitenberg et al. and Shahan and colleagues (29, 211). Winterbauer and Bouton (21) used a group design with relatively few training sessions in comparison to the other experiments cited above. The purpose of the present experiments was to further examine the effect of alternative reinforcement on resurgence in a two component multiple schedule in which differential alternative reinforcement rates and/or different reinforcement schedule were in effect in the Elimination phase. Experiment 1 In this experiment resurgence was compared in a two component multiple schedule in which either higher or lower alternative reinforcement rates were in effect for eliminating the target response in the Elimination phase. The different reinforcement rates were arranged in Experiments 1a and 1b by using fixed or variable DRO schedules, respectively, in a systematic replication of Cançado and Lattal (213). Method Subjects. Four pigeons (Columba livia) were maintained at about 8% of their free feeding weights. They were housed individually with a 12:12 h light/dark cycle (lights on 8: a.m.) and had free access to water and grit in the home cage. All subjects had previous experiences with various experimental procedures.

5 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al. 12 Apparatus. Four operant chambers, 32 cm long, 25 cm wide, and 33 cm high were used. Each chamber had three response keys on the front wall 26 cm above the grid floor. Each key was 3 cm in diameter and placed 6 cm apart from each other (center to center) and could be transilluminated with lights of different colors. A minimum force of approximately.15 N was required to operate the keys. Reinforcement was 3 s access to mixed grains delivered by a food hopper located below the center key. During reinforcement, the hopper was illuminated with white light. A house light on the rear wall provided general illumination. Each chamber was housed in a soundattenuating box with a ventilation fan. White noise presented in the box masked extraneous noise. Event scheduling and data recording were controlled by a computer using Visual Basic 25 Express Edition software. Procedure. Pigeons initially were trained to key peck on a VI schedule. During this training, one of the three keys left, center, or right was white and the location of the color was randomly assigned for successive reinforcements. Each session lasted for 3 min. The mean VI values were gradually increased from 5 to 3 s across the five sessions. Each interval was sampled without replacement from 12 intervals generated using the Fleshler and Hoffman (1962) progression. Following this training, pigeons were exposed to the following procedures. In both Experiments 1a and 1b, a two-component multiple schedule arranged on the center key was in effect across all phases. Both components were 18 s in duration and separated by a 6 s intercomponent interval (ICI), during which a blackout was in effect. Each of the two components strictly alternated and occurred five times during a session. Daily sessions occurred 7 days a week at approximately the same time each day. Each pigeon first was exposed to Experiment 1a and then to Experiment 1b. The schedule and the number of sessions in each phase are shown in Table 1. In the Acquisition phase, a VI 3-s schedule was in effect in both components for a minimum of 2 sessions in Experiment 1a and 15 sessions in Experiment 1b. This phase was terminated when the following stability criterion was met: The mean response rates in each component from the final six sessions were divided into two blocks consisting of the three sessions. When each of the two sub means of each component differed from the overall mean by less than 1%, the next phase was implemented. In the Elimination phase, a DRO 2 s and 6 s schedules were in effect in what will be labeled hereafter the Rich and Lean components, respectively. Thus, reinforcers were delivered only when a response had not been emitted for a period of time and each response restarted the DRO timer in that component. The difference between Experiments 1a and 1b in this phase was whether the DRO value was constant or varied: In Experiment 1a, each DRO value was sampled without replacement from 12 intervals (Fleshler & Hoffman, 1962) so that each interreinforcer interval (IRI) was varied within a session (hereafter called VDRO). In contrast, constant DRO values

6 A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE 121 Table 1 The schedules of target and alternative responses in each phase of all Experiments. Rich Component Lean Component Phase Target Alternative Target Alternative Experiment 1a Acquisition VI 3-s - VI 3-s - Elimination EXT VDRO 2-s EXT VDRO 6-s Resurgence EXT EXT EXT EXT Experiment 1b Acquisition VI 3-s - VI 3-s - Elimination EXT FDRO 2-s EXT FDRO 6-s Resurgence EXT EXT EXT EXT Experiment 2a Acquisition VI 3-s - VI 3-s - Elimination EXT VI 2-s EXT VI 6-s Resurgence EXT EXT EXT EXT Experiment 2b Acquisition VI 3-s Elimination EXT VI 2-s - - Resurgence EXT EXT - - Experiment 2c Acquisition - - VI 3-s - Elimination - - EXT VI 6-s Resurgence - - EXT EXT Experiment 3 Acquisition VI 3-s - VI 3-s - Elimination VDRO 3-s VI 6-s EXT VI 6-s Resurgence EXT EXT EXT EXT

7 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al Experiment 1a Elim. Resurgence Experiment 1b Experiment 1a Experiment 1b Elim. Resurgence 6 Elim. Res. Elim. Resurgence 4 B14 4 C11 Rich Lean Total Responses B21 4 H Sessions Figure 1. Total number of target responses over the last 5 sessions of the Elimination phase and the all sessions of the Resurgence phase in Experiment 1a and 1b. Dashed and solid vertical lines in each graph separate the Elimination and Resurgence phases, and the Experiments 1a and 1b, respectively..5 Experiment 1a Experiment 1b 2 Experiment 1a Experiment 1b Proportion of Acquisition Rates Rich Lean Log Proportion of Elimination Rates B14 B21 C11 H13. Sessions -2 Sessions Figure 2. Proportion of the Acquisition response rates (left panel) and log proportion of the Elimination (right panel) response rates during the Resurgence phase. Solid vertical lines in each graph separate the Experiments 1a and 1b. Proportion of the Acquisition rates was calculated by dividing the response rates in each session of the Resurgence phase by mean response rates during the last 6 sessions in the Acquisition phase. Log proportion of the Elimination rates was the logarithm of values calculated by dividing the response rates in each session of the Resurgence phase by mean response during the last 3 sessions in the Elimination phase. Each point above the horizontal dashed line in the right panel represents the resurgence.

8 A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE 123 were used in Experiment 1b (hereafter called FDRO). In both DRO schedules, any response during a given IRI reset the IRI to the value of the IRI in effect. In Experiments 1a and 1b, the minimum number of sessions in this phase was 1 sessions and were terminated when the following two stability criteria were met: The target response rates decreased below 1% of baseline rates for 3 consecutive sessions in both components. The Resurgence phase was the same in both Experiments 1a and 1b such that all reinforcers were withheld. That is, extinction was in effect in both components. This phase lasted for at least 1 sessions and was terminated when both the target and alternative response rates decreased below 1% of baseline rates (i.e., Acquisition phase response rates of the target response and Elimination phase response rates of the alternative response) for 3 consecutive sessions in both components. Results and Discussion Figure 1 shows the total number of target responses in the Rich and Lean component during the last 5 sessions of the Elimination phase and all Resurgence phase sessions. Figure 2 shows these data as proportion and log proportion of the target response rates during the stable sessions of the immediately preceding Training (left panel) or Elimination (right panel) phase, respectively. The patterns of resurgence between components were different for each pigeon in both Experiments 1a and 1b. In Experiment 1a, Pigeons B21 and C11 showed somewhat greater resurgence in the Rich component. For Pigeon B14, the larger amount of resurgence occurred in the Lean component of the second session. Although there seemed to be little difference in resurgence between components as shown in Figure 1 and the left panel of Figure 2, target responding in the Lean component more frequently reappeared across sessions than did responding in the Rich component, relative to response rates in the preceding Elimination phase (see the right panel of Figure 2). For Pigeon H13, there was no systematic difference in resurgence between the Rich and Lean components. In Experiment 1b, greater resurgence occurred in Pigeons B21 and H13 than in Experiment 1a. For Pigeon H13, there was little difference in resurgence between components. For Pigeon B21, resurgence was greater in the Rich component in the fourth session, but the opposite results was found in the sixth and eighth sessions. It may be notable that the rate of target responding in the Acquisition phase of B21 was higher in the Lean component (see Table 2). Some experiments have reported that higher rates of target responding in the Acquisition phase produced greater resurgence (da Silva, Maxwell, & Lattal, 28; Winterbauer, Lucke, & Bouton, 213, Experiment 1). Thus, it was possible that the higher rate of target response in the Lean component during the Acquisition phase, rather than alternative reinforcement rates, contributed

9 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al. A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE Table 2 Mean response rates and reinforcement rates (standard deviations in parenthesis) over the last 6 sessions in the Rich and Lean components of the Acquisition and Elimination phase and the total number of sessions across Experiments Responses per Minute Reinforcers per Minute Num. of Sessions Acquisition Phase Elimination Phase Acquisition Phase Elimination Phase Experiments Pigeons Rich Lean Rich Lean Rich Lean Rich Lean Acq. Elim. Res. a Exp. 1a B (13.) 91.5 (5.99) (.1) 1.99 (.9) 2.2 (.13).86 (.22) B (11.34) (1.9) (.6) 1.91 (.12) 1.33 (.31).43 (.21) C (17.21) 52.7 (15.76) (.27) 1.73 (.41) 2.17 (.41).76 (.7) H (7.64) (7.13) (.11) 1.96 (.9) 2.56 (.).84 (.26) a Exp. 1b B (7.68) 81.7 (8.74) (.17) 1.9 (.12) 2.45 (.21).86 (.8) B (9.88) (15.88) (.6) 1.82 (.13) 2.81 (.9).95 (.4) C (6.32) (6.3) (.7) 1.97 (.17) 2.64 (.22).95 (.4) H (2.9) (8.22) (.11) 1.91 (.14) 2.93 (.5).91 (.13) Exp. 2a A (9.1) 6.28 (12.24) (4.82) (3.28) 1.94 (.8) 1.94 (.15) 2.97 (.1).98 (.14) A2 48. (7.62) (7.98) (7.27) 42.4 (8.13) 1.79 (.19) 1.89 (.22) 2.86 (.9) 1. (.11) A (17.67) 68.8 (8.72) (5.38) (7.15) 1.99 (.8) 1.98 (.9) 2.86 (.12) 1. (.8) A (2.8) (5.53) (5.26) (5.14) 1.86 (.8) 1.94 (.16) 2.87 (.9).93 (.8) Exp. 2b A (6.42) (9.4) (.9) (.1) A (3.49) (2.36) (.5) (.4) A (4.98) (6.43) (.4) (.8) A (5.95) (3.83) (.6) (.5) Exp. 2c A (5.9) (9.75) (.7) (.6) A (3.67) (6.53) (.6) (.6) A (3.2) (12.5) (.5) (.6) A (4.49) (5.11) (.9) (.3) bexp. 3 A (1.57) (2.78) (3.14) (7.8) 1.84 (.4) 1.87 (.11) 2.6 (.55).91 (.9) B (6.76) (8.58) (1.83) 29.9 (8.51) 1.63 (.33) 1.64 (.35) 2.53 (.13).76 (.19) C (12.89) 87.6 (7.35) (16.48) (16.22) 1.94 (.13) 1.9 (.12) 2.83 (.15) 1.9 (.1) D (5.5) 76.4 (15.1) (12.51) 8.53 (14.23) 1.93 (.12) 1.87 (.16) 2.67 (.15) 1.5 (.8) bexp. 3 replication A (7.27) (6.62) (3.73) 56.1 (4.5) 1.89 (.5) 1.91 (.12) 2.66 (.36).94 (.8) D (14.38) 8.63 (12.3) (9.1) (6.28) 1.94 (.11) 1.87 (.1) 2.63 (.15).98 (.) Note. Acq. = Acquisition phase; Elim. = Elimination phase; Res. = Resurgence phase. a Mean reinforcement rates in the Elimination phase are the means of the last 3 sessions because the scope of the stability criterion in this phase is limited to this range. b Mean reinforcement rates in the Rich component of the Elimination phase are calculated by dividing all reinforcers obtained from both keys by the total component duration.

10 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al. 126 to the amount of resurgence. For Pigeons B14 and C11, resurgence occurred in neither component. There was no obvious difference in resurgence between Experiments 1a and 1b suggesting that whether the DRO value in the Elimination phase was variable (VDRO) or fixed (FDRO) did not affect the amount of target responses, a finding consistent with the results reported by Doughty, da Silva, and Lattal (27). Taken together, although there were some instances of greater resurgence in the Rich component, there were other instances where resurgence was greater in the Lean component or where there were no differences in resurgence between the Rich and Lean components. It also is notable that there was little to no resurgence on several occasions. One possible reason for this result was the use of a DRO schedule for eliminating the target response. Although Doughty et al. (27) suggested that more resurgence might occur when a DRO schedule is used, the results of other studies suggest this may not be the case (e.g., Cançado & Lattal, 213; Mulick, Leitenberg, & Rawson, 1976). In addition, Pacitti and Smith (1977) suggested the possibility that the topography of alternative responding in the Elimination phase also may affect the amount of resurgence (cf. Doughty et al., 27). These procedural differences between the present experiment and prior ones investigating reinforcement rate and resurgence may have contributed to the general absence of a systematic effect of these two variables. The next two experiments (2 and 3) therefore used VI reinforcement of key pecking in the Alternative reinforcement phase of the experiment, rather than DRO, to further examine the relation between alternative reinforcement rates and the amount of resurgence. Experiment 2 In Experiment 2, resurgence was compared when the alternative response in each component was the same topography as the target response, but was reinforced on different-valued VI schedules. Method Subjects and Apparatus. Four pigeons (Columba livia), different from those used in Experiment 1, were maintained at about 8% of their free-feeding weights. They were housed individually with a 12:12 h light/dark cycle (lights on 8: a.m.) and had free access to water and grit in the home cage. All had previous experience with various experimental procedures. The apparatus was the same as in Experiment 1. Procedure. After pretraining, the pigeons were exposed to the following three phases. Daily sessions consisted of a two component multiple schedule across all phases. The details of the multiple schedule in Experiment 2a and the stability criteria

11 A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE 127 for changing between phases were as described for Experiment 1. Each phase terminated when the minimum number of sessions was conducted and the stability criteria were met. The schedules and the number of sessions in each phase are shown in Table 1. Experiment 2a Experiment 2a consisted of a two component multiple schedule across all phases. In the Acquisition phase, target responses to either the left or right side key were reinforced on a VI 3 s schedule in both components. This phase lasted for a minimum of 3 sessions. In the Elimination phase, the target responses to either side key were extinguished in both components, while alternative responses to the center key were reinforced on VI 2 s and VI 6 s schedules in the Rich and Lean components, respectively. A 3 s changeover delay (COD) was in effect between responses on the key that was operative in the previous Acquisition phase and reinforced responses on the key in effect during the Elimination phase. This phase lasted for a minimum of 15 sessions and was terminated when both target and alternative responses met the stability criteria. In the Resurgence phase, all reinforcers were withheld in both components. This latter phase lasted for at least 1 sessions. Experiments 2b and 2c In contrast to the multiple schedule used in Experiment 2a, Experiments 2b and 2c consisted of a single schedule of reinforcement in each of the three phases of the experiment. This was done in an attempt to determine whether schedule interactions between components might have contributed to the results of Experiments 1 and 2a. The schedules in Experiments 2b and 2c are shown in Table 1 and corresponded to those in effect in the Rich and Lean components, respectively, of Experiment 2a. Each session started after a 3 s blackout and ended after 3 min. In both conditions, the minimum number of sessions in the Acquisition phase was 15. Other aspects of each phase were as described for Experiment 1a. The order of Experiments 2b and 2c differed across pigeons: A1 and A3 were exposed to Experiment 2c at first and then 2b; for A2 and A4, the order was reversed. Results and Discussion Figure 3 shows the number of target responses in the Rich and Lean component during the last 5 sessions of the Elimination phase and all sessions of the Resurgence phase in Experiment 2. Figure 4 shows the Figure 3 data for each session in the resur-

12 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al Experiment 2a Experiment 2b Experiment 2c Elim. Resurgence Elim. Resurgence Elim. Resurgence 2 A1 Total Responses A2 A3 4 2 Rich Lean A4 Sessions Figure 3. Total number of target responses over the last 5 sessions of the Elimination phase and the all sessions of the Resurgence phase in Experiments 2. Dashed and solid vertical lines in each graph separate the Elimination and Resurgence phases, and the Experiments 2a, 2b, and 2c, respectively. gence conditions as a proportion of the mean response rate during the last six Acquisition phase sessions (left graphs) or the last three Elimination phase sessions (right graphs). As with Experiment 1, the patterns of resurgence as indexed by any of the measures shown in these figures were different for each pigeon. In Experiment 2a, Pigeon A3 showed greater absolute and relative resurgence (by either index) in the Rich component. As shown in the right panel of Figure 4, A2 showed resurgence during many sessions of the Resurgence phase, but there was little difference in the magnitude of resurgence between components when measured in either absolute or relative terms. The other pigeons showed little to no resurgence and no systematic difference in resurgence between components. For Pigeon A3, it should be noted that there was a big difference in the rates of target responses between the Rich and Lean component of the Acquisition phase, while reinforcement rates were almost equal (see Table 2). As noted above, da Silva et al. (28) showed that resurgence was greater when the rates of target responding were higher, at least in absolute terms (see also Winterbauer et al., 213, Experiment 1). Thus, it is unclear how the higher target response rates during the Acquisition phase and the higher alternative reinforcement rates during the Elimination phase each contributed to the greater resurgence in Pigeon A3.

13 A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE 129 In Experiments 2b and 2c, differential resurgence did not occur in Pigeons A1 and A3. For Pigeon A2, the amount of resurgence measured in either absolute or relative terms as noted above was larger in Experiment 2b than 2c. For Pigeon A4, differential resurgence did not occur in both absolute and relative terms. However, the increase in target responses from the stable sessions during the Elimination phase was somewhat greater in Experiment 2b (the Rich component), as shown in the right panel of Figure 4. In sum, differential resurgence as a function of the different rates of alternative reinforcement in the Elimination phase did not occur systematically in Experiment 2. The exceptions were A3 in Experiment 2a and A2 in Experiments 2b and 2c: These pigeons in the noted conditions showed greater resurgence when the rates of alternative reinforcement were higher. In Experiment 2, the effect of alternative reinforcement rates on resurgence was examined by manipulating the VI value between the Rich and Lean components..2 Exp. 2a Exp. 2b Exp. 2c 2 Exp. 2a Exp. 2b Exp. 2c Proportion of Acquisition Rates Rich Lean Log Proportion of Elimination Rates A1 A2 A3 A4. Sessions -2 Sessions Figure 4. Proportion of the Acquisition (left panel) and the Elimination (right panel) response rates during the Resurgence phase. Solid vertical lines in each graph separate the Experiments 2a, 2b, and 2c. Proportion of the Acquisition rates was calculated by dividing the response rates in each session of the Resurgence phase by mean response rates during the last 6 sessions in the Acquisition phase. Log proportion of the Elimination rates was the logarithm of values calculated by dividing the response rates in each session of the Resurgence phase by mean response during the last 3 sessions in the Elimination phase. Each point above the horizontal dashed line in the right panel represents the resurgence.

14 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al. 13 However, the higher reinforcement rate typically also produced higher response rates, so that the two variables are confounded, thereby obscuring the contributions of either variable to differential resurgence. In fact, both the rates of alternative responding and reinforcement differed between components in the Elimination phase in almost all instances of Experiment 2 (see Table 2). Thus, the higher rate of alternative response, not only the alternative reinforcement rate, was another variable that might contribute to the differential resurgence found between Experiments 2b and 2c shown by Pigeon A2 and in Experiment 2a shown by A3. Other studies concerning the effect of alternative reinforcement rates also have not separated these two variables (e.g., Leitenberg et al., 1975; Winterbauer & Bouton, 21). Experiment 3 addressed this problem to further examine the effect of alternative reinforcement rates on resurgence. Experiment 3 In Experiment 3, resurgence was compared when alternative responses were reinforced on VI 6 s schedules in both components of a multiple schedule, but additional reinforcers also were delivered independently of the alternative response according to a DRO schedule in the Rich component. Method Subjects and Apparatus. Four pigeons (Columba livia), different from those used in any of the preceding experiments, were maintained at about 8% of their free feeding weights. They were housed individually with a 12:12 h light/dark cycle (lights on 8: a.m.) and had free access to water and grit in the home cage. All subjects had previous experiences with various experimental procedures. The apparatus was the same as in Experiment 1. Procedure. After pretraining, the pigeons were exposed to the following three phases. Daily sessions consisted of a two component multiple schedule across all phases. The details of the multiple schedule and the stability criteria were as described for Experiment 1a. Each phase terminated when the minimum number of sessions was conducted and the stability criteria were met. Note that the response keys used in Experiment 3 differed for each pigeon: For Pigeons A11, C23, and D11, the center and left keys served as target and alternative responses, respectively, in the Rich and Lean components. For Pigeon B1, the left and right keys served as target responses in the Rich and Lean components, respectively, and the center key served as alternative responses in both components. The schedule and the number of sessions in each phase are shown in Table 1.

15 A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE Elimination Resurgence 6 Elim. Resurgence Elim. Resurgence 4 2 B1 Rich Lean 4 2 A11 Total Responses 9 6 C D Sessions Figure 5. Total number of target responses over the last 5 sessions of the Elimination phase and the all sessions of the Resurgence phase in Experiment 3. Dashed vertical lines in each graph separate the Elimination and Resurgence phases. Solid vertical lines in the two right graphs separate the first and second exposure to the procedure of Experiment 3. Note the different y-axis scale. In the Acquisition phase, target responses were reinforced on a VI 3 s schedule in both components. This phase lasted for a minimum of 2 sessions. In the Elimination phase, alternative responses were reinforced on a VI 6 s schedule in both components, and target responses to the center key were eliminated by a VDRO 3 s schedule in the Rich component and by extinction in the Lean component. This phase lasted for a minimum of 15 sessions. In the Resurgence phase, all reinforcers were withheld for at least 1 sessions. The sequence of phases was repeated for A11 and D11. Results and Discussion Figure 5 shows the number of target responses in the Rich and Lean component during the last 5 sessions of the Elimination phase and all sessions of the Resurgence phase in Experiment 3. Figure 6 shows the Figure 5 data for each session in the resurgence conditions as a proportion of the mean response rate during the last 6 Acquisition phase sessions (left graphs) or during the last 3 Elimination phase sessions (right graphs). As in Experiments 1 and 2, the patterns of resurgence as indexed by any of the measures shown in these figures were different for each pigeon. For Pigeons C23 and, especially, the second exposure of D11, differences in resurgence favored the Rich component. Pigeon B1, however, showed greater resurgence in the Lean component in both absolute and relative terms. Resurgence for Pigeon D11,

16 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al. 132 Proportion of Acquisition Rates Log Proportion of Elimination Rates First Exposure Second Exposure First Exposure Second Exposure Rich Lean B1 C23 A11 D11. Sessions -3 Sessions Figure 6. Proportion of the Acquisition (left panel) and the Elimination (right panel) response rates during the Resurgence phase. Solid vertical lines in the lower two graphs separate the first and second exposure to Experiment 3. Proportion of the Acquisition rates was calculated by dividing the response rates in each session of the Resurgence phase by mean response rates during the last 6 sessions in the Acquisition phase. Log proportion of the Elimination rates was the logarithm of values calculated by dividing the response rates in each session of the Resurgence phase by mean response during the last 3 sessions in the Elimination phase. Each point above the horizontal dashed line in the right panel represents the resurgence. however, was not different across the Rich and Lean components during the first resurgence test. Pigeon A11 did not show differential resurgence across either the first or second exposures. Both the response and reinforcement rates of target responding were almost equal between components, so that either the alternative response or reinforcement rates could be the determinant of the differential resurgence. For each pigeon, reinforcement rates in the two components during the Elimination phase were systematically different, although the reinforcement rates in the Rich component were relatively smaller than those programmed. Response independent reinforcers, or reinforcers delivered independently of the alternative response according to a DRO schedule were expected to reduce alternative response rates in the Rich component. The mean alternative response rates, however, were higher in the Rich than in the Lean component for 4 out of 6 instances (see Table 2). It was possible that the relatively small number of sessions in the Elimination phase might have contributed to the failure of this expected effect to occur. In fact, some of the data supported this possibility. Pigeon B1, for example, experienced 28 sessions in the Elimination phase and showed no difference in the alternative response rates.

17 A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE 133 Taken together, in some conditions with some pigeons, there was greater resurgence in the Rich component, while in others there was little difference in resurgence between the two components. However, the procedures of the present study again did not equate alternative response rates between components, so that it remains an open question as to whether the higher rates of alternative responses or reinforcers are responsible for the amount of resurgence. General Discussion The present experiments examined the relation between the rates of alternative reinforcement and resurgence. In Experiment 1, DRO schedules were in effect in both the Rich and Lean components during the Elimination phase. Alternative reinforcement rates in two components of this phase were consistently different although obtained rates were relatively smaller than those programmed in both components. In some conditions with some pigeons, there was greater resurgence in the Rich component while in others showed the opposite results. Thus, there was no systematic relation between alternative reinforcement rates and resurgence. In addition, a systematic difference in resurgence was not found between Experiments 1a and 1b. This latter result replicates the finding of Doughty et al. (27) that variable (VDRO) and fixed (FDRO) DROs have similar effects on the resurgence of target responses. In Experiment 2, unlike Experiment 1, the alternative response in each component was the same topography as the target response, but was reinforced according to differentvalued VI schedules. As with Experiment 1, differential resurgence was not observed as a function of the different reinforcement rates arranged in the Elimination phase (cf. Cançado & Lattal, 213; Winterbauer & Bouton, 21). In Experiment 3, alternative responses were reinforced on VI 6 s in both the Rich and Lean components in the Elimination phase. In the Rich component, additional reinforcers were delivered independently of the alternative response according to a DRO schedule. As noted above, Shahan and Sweeney s (211) model predicts that all reinforcers obtained in that component during the Elimination phase affects the magnitude of resurgence (see also Podlesnik & Shahan, 29, 21). Thus, it was predicted based on that model that greater resurgence would be found in the Rich component, where the additional response-independent reinforcers delivered. There was, however, greater resurgence in the Rich component in only two out of six instances, at least in relative terms. Of most importance was the findings that more resurgence did not occur systematically under higher rates of reinforcement in the Elimination phase across all of the present experiments: Although some pigeons showed greater resurgence in the Rich component, others showed the opposite results or little to no resurgence in either components. Thus, results of the present experiment do not offer systematic evidence supporting the prediction of behavioral momentum and other models for resurgence.

18 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al. 134 Although the effects of differential reinforcement rates in the Acquisition and Elimination phases on resurgence were mixed, there were some instances where the predicted relation held. Those instances of greater resurgence in the Rich component across the three present experiments seem to be consistent with the findings of Leitenberg et al. (1975) and the prediction of some models for resurgence (e.g., Leitenberg et al. 1975; Shahan & Sweeney, 211; see also Cleland et al., 2). However, another potential contributing variable to the observed resurgence should be considered before concluding that only alternative reinforcement rates account for these results. In most instances in which the aforementioned relation was observed between resurgence and reinforcement rates in the Elimination phase, response rates in that phase also frequently were higher than in the corresponding lower reinforcement rate component. For example, although A2 showed greater resurgence in Experiment 2b than 2c, both alternative reinforcement and response rates were also higher in Experiment 2b. Thus, the contributions of alternative response and reinforcement rates in the Elimination phase to resurgence remains unclear. Experiment 3 addressed this problem in such a way that response-independent reinforcers delivered in the Rich component were arranged specifically to prevent an increase in response rate in that component. It has been reported that response independent reinforcers decrease the response rate (e.g., Rachlin & Baum, 1972), while also increasing response strength (e.g., Nevin, Tota, Torquato, & Shull, 199). Hence, it was expected that additional reinforcers by a DRO schedule would decrease the rate of responding while increasing the alternative reinforcement rate in the Rich component. However, the attempt to equate alternative response rates between components again failed (see Table 2; four out of six instances showed higher response rate in the Rich component), perhaps partly because of the small number of sessions in the Elimination phase. Thus, as with previous studies (e.g., Leitenberg et al., 1975; Winterbauer & Bouton, 21), it still remains unclear whether the higher rates of alternative responses or reinforcers are responsible for the amount of resurgence. This point should be examined more precisely in future research. Note that a DRO schedule was in effect in both components during the Elimination phase of Experiment 1 and no particular alternative response rate was recorded. The alternative response rate therefore does not become the issue as long as a DRO schedule is used for eliminating the target response in the Elimination phase and alternative responses with particular topography are not recorded. However, it should also be noted that increasing reinforcement rate by adding the DRO schedule still may increase some unmeasured dimension of behavior much as if it were changing response rate. Procedural differences between resurgence experiments continue to be a potential source of different outcomes of the sorts discussed in these experiments. As noted above, inconsistent results have been reported with respect to the effects of variables in the

19 A FURTHER LOOK AT REINFORCEMENT RATE AND RESURGENCE 135 Elimination phase on resurgence. Some studies reported that higher alternative reinforcement rates produced greater resurgence (e.g., Leitenberg et al., 1975) or the topography of alternative response also affected the amount of resurgence (e.g., Doughty et al., 27; Pacitti & Smith, 1977). Other studies, however, reported opposite findings (e.g., Cançado & Lattal, 213; Mulick et al., 1976; Winterbauer & Bouton, 21). Several variables may contribute to these discrepancies. For example, they might stem from differences between within and between subject assessments of resurgence in that different numbers of sessions may be involved in each phase in these two types of assessments (cf, e.g., Leitenberg et al., 1975; Winterbauer & Bouton, 21, to Cancado & Lattal, 213). The measure of resurgence also may contribute to the interpretation of the results. For example, the presence of resurgence may depend on whether it is measured in absolute (e.g., response rates) or relative terms (e.g., proportion of baseline) (da Silva et al., 28; see also Podlesnik & Shahan, 29, for a discussion of this issue), or whether mean data across the subjects or individual data for each subject are used. Using stringent stability criteria seems necessary in assessing resurgence, because the effects usually are small and transient. For example, in Experiment 1a, although Pigeon B14 seemed to show the same amount of resurgence as shown in Figure 1 and the left panel of Figure 2, the recovery of the target response from the immediately preceding Elimination phase was greater in the Lean component (see the right panel of Figure 2). This difference might have been reconciled if the target responses in both component had more completely extinguished under a more strict stability criterion than that used in the Elimination phase of the present study. Thus, the conflicting results relating to the variables in the Elimination phase has these important implications for study on resurgence itself. This is the important and inevitable problem left for future research in exploring the common determinant of resurgence. References Cançado, C. R. X., & Lattal, K. A. (213). Response elimination, reinforcement rate and resurgence of operant behavior. Behavioural Processes, 1, Carey, J. P. (1951). Reinstatement of previously learned responses under conditions of extinction: A study of regression [Abstract]. American Psychologist, 6, 284. Cleland, B. S., Foster, T. M., & Temple, W. (2). Resurgence: The role of extinction. Behavioural Processes, 52, da Silva, S. P., Maxwell, M. E., & Lattal, K. A. (28). Concurrent resurgence and behavioral history. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, Doughty, A. H., da Silva, S. P., & Lattal, K. A. (27). Differential resurgence and response elimination. Behavioural Processes, 75,

20 SHUN FUJIMAKI et al. 136 Epstein, R. (1983). Resurgence of previously reinforced behavior during extinction. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 3, Epstein, R. (1985). Extinction-induced resurgence: Preliminary investigations and possible applications. The Psychological Record, 35, Fleshler, M., & Hoffman, H. S. (1962). A progression for generating variable-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, Leitenberg, H., Rawson, R. A., & Bath, K. (197). Reinforcement of competing behavior during extinction. Science, 169, Leitenberg, H., Rawson, R. A., & Mulick, J. A. (1975). Extinction and reinforcement of alternative behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, Lieving, G. A., & Lattal, K. A. (23). Recency, repeatability, and reinforcer retrenchment: An experimental analysis of resurgence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8, Mulick, J. A., Leitenberg, H., & Rawson, R. A. (1976). Alternative response training, differential reinforcement of other behavior, and extinction in squirrel monkeys (saimiri sciureus). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, Nevin, J. A., Tota, M. E., Torquato, R. D., & Shull, R. L. (199). Alternative reinforcement increases resistance to change: Pavlovian or operant contingencies? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, Pacitti, W. A., & Smith, N. F. (1977). A direct comparison of four methods for eliminating a response. Learning and Motivation, 8, Podlesnik, C. A., & Shahan, T. A. (29). Behavioral momentum and relapse of extinguished operant responding. Learning & Behavior, 37, Podlesnik, C. A., & Shahan, T. A. (21). Extinction, relapse, and behavioral momentum. Behavioural Processes, 84, Rachlin, H., & Baum, W. M. (1972). Effects of alternative reinforcement: Does the source matter? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, Rawson, R. A., Leitenberg, H., Mulick, J. A., & Lefebvre, M. F. (1977). Recovery of extinction responding in rats following discontinuation of reinforcement of alternative behavior: A test of two explanations. Animal Learning & Behavior, 5, Shahan, T. A., & Sweeney, M. M. (211). A model of resurgence based on behavioral momentum theory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, Winterbauer, N. E., & Bouton, M. E. (21). Mechanisms of resurgence of an extinguished instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 36, Winterbauer, N. E., Lucke, S., & Bouton, M. E. (213). Some factors modulating the strength of resurgence after extinction of an instrumental behavior. Learning and Motivation, 44, 6 71.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Learn Behav. 2009 November ; 37(4): 357 364. doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.357. Behavioral Momentum and Relapse of Extinguished Operant Responding

More information

Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta ISSN: Sociedad Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta.

Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta ISSN: Sociedad Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta ISSN: 0185-4534 editora@rmac-mx.org Sociedad Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta México LATTAL, KENNON A.; SMITH, JULIE M. BEHAVIORAL CONTRAST WHEN RESPONSES

More information

RESURGENCE OF CHICKS KEY-PECK RESPONDING WITH AN IMPRINTED STIMULUS OR FOOD AS REINFORCER

RESURGENCE OF CHICKS KEY-PECK RESPONDING WITH AN IMPRINTED STIMULUS OR FOOD AS REINFORCER REVISTA MEXICANA DE ANÁLISIS DE LA CONDUCTA MEXICAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2015 VOL. 41, 44-62 NÚMERO 2 (SEPTIEMBRE) NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) RESURGENCE OF CHICKS KEY-PECK RESPONDING WITH AN IMPRINTED

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

34, effects of the CoNtiguity BetweeN the extinction and the reinforcement CoMpoNeNts in observing-response procedures1

34, effects of the CoNtiguity BetweeN the extinction and the reinforcement CoMpoNeNts in observing-response procedures1 Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta 28 Número 2 (DIC) Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis 34, 333-347 Number 2 (DEC) Effects of the contiguity between the extinction and the reinforcement s in

More information

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez The Psychological Record, 28, 58, 57 528 Some Effects of Magnitude of Reinforcement on Persistence of Responding Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman The University of Minnesota Angel Jimenez The University

More information

TRANSLATIONAL RESEARCH ON THE RELAPSE OF OPERANT BEHAVIOR INVESTIGACIÓN DE TRADUCCIÓN SOBRE LA RECAÍDA DE LA CONDUCTA OPERANTE

TRANSLATIONAL RESEARCH ON THE RELAPSE OF OPERANT BEHAVIOR INVESTIGACIÓN DE TRADUCCIÓN SOBRE LA RECAÍDA DE LA CONDUCTA OPERANTE REVISTA MEXICANA DE ANÁLISIS DE LA CONDUCTA MEXICAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2015 VOL. 41, 226-251 NÚMERO 2 (SEPTIEMBRE) NUMBER 2 (September) TRANSLATIONAL RESEARCH ON THE RELAPSE OF OPERANT BEHAVIOR

More information

RESURGENCE OF LEVER PRESSING DURING A DRO CHALLENGE. Jennifer L. Hudnall, B.A.

RESURGENCE OF LEVER PRESSING DURING A DRO CHALLENGE. Jennifer L. Hudnall, B.A. RESURGENCE OF LEVER PRESSING DURING A DRO CHALLENGE By Jennifer L. Hudnall, B.A. Submitted to the graduate degree program Applied Behavioral Science and the Graduate Faculty of the University of Kansas

More information

The Effects of Repeated Resurgence Conditions on the Magnitude of Resurgence

The Effects of Repeated Resurgence Conditions on the Magnitude of Resurgence Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 8-2012 The Effects of Repeated Resurgence Conditions on the Magnitude of Resurgence Ryan Nathaniel Redner Western Michigan

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 8-2012 Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Meredith

More information

CONTEXT CHANGE EXPLAINS RESURGENCE AFTER THE EXTINCTION OF OPERANT BEHAVIOR

CONTEXT CHANGE EXPLAINS RESURGENCE AFTER THE EXTINCTION OF OPERANT BEHAVIOR REVISTA MEXICANA DE ANÁLISIS DE LA CONDUCTA MEXICAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2015 NÚMERO 2 (SEPTIEMBRE) VOL. 41, 187-210 NUMBER 2 (September) CONTEXT CHANGE EXPLAINS RESURGENCE AFTER THE EXTINCTION

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 79-91 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 79 Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats Rogelio Escobar National Autonomous University of Mexico

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Chapter 12 Behavioral Momentum Theory: Understanding Persistence and Improving Treatment

Chapter 12 Behavioral Momentum Theory: Understanding Persistence and Improving Treatment Chapter 12 Behavioral Momentum Theory: Understanding Persistence and Improving Treatment Christopher A. Podlesnik and Iser G. DeLeon 12.1 Introduction Translational research in behavior analysis aims both

More information

doi: /jeab.213 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2016, 106, NUMBER 1 (JULY) 1 UNIVERSITY OF NEW HAMPSHIRE

doi: /jeab.213 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2016, 106, NUMBER 1 (JULY) 1 UNIVERSITY OF NEW HAMPSHIRE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 216, 16, 34 57 NUMBER 1 (JULY) EFFECTS OF SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED ALTERNATIVE REINFORCEMENT ON PERSISTENCE AND RELAPSE IN CHILDREN AND PIGEONS JOHN A. NEVIN

More information

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF OTHER BEHAVIOR AND RESPONSE SUPPRESSION:

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF OTHER BEHAVIOR AND RESPONSE SUPPRESSION: psychological Record, 1993, 43, 271-288 DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF OTHER BEHAVIOR AND RESPONSE SUPPRESSION: THE EFFECTS OF THE RESPONSE-REINFORCEMENT INTERVAL THOMAS S. RIEG, NELSON F. SMITH, and STUART

More information

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1998, 70, 165 183 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

BEHAVIORAL MOMENTUM THEORY: EQUATIONS AND APPLICATIONS JOHN A. NEVIN TIMOTHY A. SHAHAN

BEHAVIORAL MOMENTUM THEORY: EQUATIONS AND APPLICATIONS JOHN A. NEVIN TIMOTHY A. SHAHAN JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2011, 44, 877 895 NUMBER 4(WINTER 2011) BEHAVIORAL MOMENTUM THEORY: EQUATIONS AND APPLICATIONS JOHN A. NEVIN UNIVERSITY OF NEW HAMPSHIRE AND TIMOTHY A. SHAHAN UTAH

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

Schedules of Reinforcement

Schedules of Reinforcement Schedules of Reinforcement MACE, PRATT, ZANGRILLO & STEEGE (2011) FISHER, PIAZZA & ROANE CH 4 Rules that describe how will be reinforced are 1. Every response gets SR+ ( ) vs where each response gets 0

More information

Examining the Effects of Reinforcement Context on Relapse of Observing

Examining the Effects of Reinforcement Context on Relapse of Observing Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2011 Examining the Effects of Reinforcement Context on Relapse of Observing Eric A. Thrailkill Utah State

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement Learning & Behavior 2005, 33 (3), 309-323 Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement GLENN S. BROWN and K. GEOFFREY WHITE University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand In two experiments, pigeons responding

More information

Psicothema ISSN: Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos del Principado de Asturias. España

Psicothema ISSN: Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos del Principado de Asturias. España Psicothema ISSN: 0214-99 psicothema@cop.es Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos del Principado de Asturias España Polín, Eduardo; Pérez, Vicente The effect of varied reinforcement on acquisition and extinction

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 3-2016 Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Andrew R. Craig Utah State University

More information

Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers

Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 1-2012 Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers Corina

More information

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Behavioural Processes 66 (2004) 213 233 Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Raymond C. Pitts, Stacy

More information

Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers

Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2008 Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers Christopher Aaron Podlesnik

More information

GENERALIZED IDENTITY MATCHING IN THE PIGEON: ROLE OF EXTENDED OBSERVING- AND CHOICE-RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS

GENERALIZED IDENTITY MATCHING IN THE PIGEON: ROLE OF EXTENDED OBSERVING- AND CHOICE-RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS REVISTA MEXICANA DE ANÁLISIS DE LA CONDUCTA MEXICAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 215 NÚMERO 1 (JUNIO) VOL. 41, 53-67 NUMBER 1 (JUNE) GENERALIZED IDENTITY MATCHING IN THE PIGEON: ROLE OF EXTENDED OBSERVING-

More information

The Persistence-Strengthening Effects of DRA: An Illustration of Bidirectional Translational Research

The Persistence-Strengthening Effects of DRA: An Illustration of Bidirectional Translational Research The Behavior Analyst 2009, 32, 000 000 No. 2 (Fall) The Persistence-Strengthening Effects of DRA: An Illustration of Bidirectional Translational Research F. Charles Mace University of Southern Maine Jennifer

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 89 (2012) 212 218 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes j o ur nal homep age : www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Providing a reinforcement history

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

Interaction between the variety of negative stimuli and the type of go/no-go procedure

Interaction between the variety of negative stimuli and the type of go/no-go procedure , 378-384 http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.34.2.307401 Copyright 2018: Editum. Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Murcia. Murcia (Spain) ISSN print edition: 0212-9728. ISSN web edition (http://revistas.um.es/analesps):

More information

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE By ANTHONY L. DEFULIO A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE

More information

Examination of Behavioral Momentum with Staff as Contextual Variables in Applied Settings with Children with Autism

Examination of Behavioral Momentum with Staff as Contextual Variables in Applied Settings with Children with Autism Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2010 Examination of Behavioral Momentum with Staff as Contextual Variables in Applied Settings with Children

More information

Red de Revistas Científicas de América Latina y el Caribe, España y Portugal. Universidad Autónoma del Estado de México.

Red de Revistas Científicas de América Latina y el Caribe, España y Portugal. Universidad Autónoma del Estado de México. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta Sociedad Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta jburgos@ cucba.udg.mx ISSN (Versión impresa): 0185-4534 MÉXICO 2007 Adam H. Doughty / Shannon S. Doughty / Jennifer

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 261-268 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) CONCURRENT SCHEULES OF PRIMARY AN CONITIONE REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' ONAL W. ZIMMERMAN CARLETON UNIVERSITY Rats responded

More information

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 264-270 Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99163 Five pigeons

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules

Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules Animal Learning & Behavior 1977, 5 (1),11-114 Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pul/man, Washington

More information

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2009, Vol. 35, No. 3, 293 311 2009 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013926 Resistance to Change Within

More information

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 217, 17, 123 135 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 1

More information

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2015, 103, 269 287 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

Effects of Differential Rates of Alternative Reinforcement on Resurgence of Human Behavior: A Translational Model of Relapse in the Anxiety Disorders

Effects of Differential Rates of Alternative Reinforcement on Resurgence of Human Behavior: A Translational Model of Relapse in the Anxiety Disorders Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2015 Effects of Differential Rates of Alternative Reinforcement on Resurgence of Human Behavior: A Translational

More information

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee The Psychological Record, 1997,47,145-166 THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

The University of Iowa

The University of Iowa The Psychological Record, 213, 63, 3 2 An Evaluation of Resurgence During Functional Communication Training David P. Wacker, Jay W. Harding, Theresa A. Morgan, Wendy K. Berg, Kelly M. Schieltz, John F.

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Extension of Behavioral Momentum Theory to Conditions with Changing Reinforcer Rates

Extension of Behavioral Momentum Theory to Conditions with Changing Reinforcer Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2017 Extension of Behavioral Momentum Theory to Conditions with Changing Reinforcer Rates Andrew R. Craig

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1987, 47, 57-72 CHOICE BETWEEN RELIABLE AND UNRELIABLE OUTCOMES: MIXED PERCENTAGE-REINFORCEMENT IN CONCURRENT CHAINS MARCIA L. SPETCH AND ROGER DUNN DALHOUSIE

More information

Overview. Simple Schedules of Reinforcement. Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement. Combined Schedules of Reinforcement BEHP 1016

Overview. Simple Schedules of Reinforcement. Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement. Combined Schedules of Reinforcement BEHP 1016 BEHP 1016 Why People Often Make Bad Choices and What to Do About It: Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement F. Charles Mace, Ph.D., BCBA-D University of Southern Maine with Jose Martinez-Diaz,

More information

Predictors of Persistence and Resurgence: Evaluation of a Behavioral Momentum-Based Approach

Predictors of Persistence and Resurgence: Evaluation of a Behavioral Momentum-Based Approach Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-1-2014 Predictors of Persistence and Resurgence: Evaluation of a Behavioral Momentum-Based Approach Mary

More information

Red de Revistas Científicas de América Latina y el Caribe, España y Portugal. Universidad Autónoma del Estado de México.

Red de Revistas Científicas de América Latina y el Caribe, España y Portugal. Universidad Autónoma del Estado de México. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta Sociedad Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta jburgos@ cucba.udg.mx ISSN (Versión impresa): 085-4534 MÉXICO 2007 Chad M. Galuska / Michael Perone RESISTANCE TO

More information

REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB

REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2012, 98, 273 282 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB UNIVERSITY

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

WHERE WOULD ABA BE WITHOUT EAB? AN EXAMPLE OF TRANSLATIONAL RESEARCH ON RECURRENCE OF OPERANT BEHAVIOR AND TREATMENT RELAPSE

WHERE WOULD ABA BE WITHOUT EAB? AN EXAMPLE OF TRANSLATIONAL RESEARCH ON RECURRENCE OF OPERANT BEHAVIOR AND TREATMENT RELAPSE REVISTA MEXICANA DE ANÁLISIS DE LA CONDUCTA MEXICAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2015 VOL. 41, 269-288 NÚMERO 2 (SEPTIEMBRE) NUMBER 2 (September) WHERE WOULD ABA BE WITHOUT EAB? AN EXAMPLE OF TRANSLATIONAL

More information

AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS. Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS. Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December 2011 APPROVED:

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

RESPONSE-CLASS HIERARCHIES AND RESURGENCE OF SEVERE PROBLEM BEHAVIOR

RESPONSE-CLASS HIERARCHIES AND RESURGENCE OF SEVERE PROBLEM BEHAVIOR The Psychological Record, 24, 54, 621-634 RESPONSE-CLASS HIERARCHIES AND RESURGENCE OF SEVERE PROBLEM BEHAVIOR GREGORY A. L1EVING, LOUIS P. HAGOPIAN, ETHAN S. LONG, and JULIA O'CONNOR The Kennedy Krieger

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 42, No. 4, 401 414 2329-8456/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000112 Contextual

More information

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1 Japanese Psychological Research 1998, Volume 40, No. 2, 104 110 Short Report Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response

More information

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL'

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1973, 2, 393-43 NUMBER 3 (NOVEMBER) PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' M. C. DAVISON AND W. TEMPLE UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND AND

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT AND RESPONSE STRENGTH TIMOTHY A. SHAHAN

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT AND RESPONSE STRENGTH TIMOTHY A. SHAHAN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2010, 93, 269 289 NUMBER 2(MARCH) CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT AND RESPONSE STRENGTH TIMOTHY A. SHAHAN UTAH STATE UNIVERSITY Stimuli associated with primary

More information

Variability as an Operant?

Variability as an Operant? The Behavior Analyst 2012, 35, 243 248 No. 2 (Fall) Variability as an Operant? Per Holth Oslo and Akershus University College Correspondence concerning this commentary should be addressed to Per Holth,

More information

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Learning and Motivation 39 (2008) 71 94 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Luís Oliveira,

More information

Preference and resistance to change in concurrent variable-interval schedules

Preference and resistance to change in concurrent variable-interval schedules Animal Learning & Behavior 2002, 30 (1), 34-42 Preference and resistance to change in concurrent variable-interval schedules MATTHEW C. BELL Santa Clara University, Santa Clara, California and BEN A. WILLIAMS

More information

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Illinois Wesleyan University Digital Commons @ IWU Honors Projects Psychology 2000 Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Sara J. Estle '00 Illinois Wesleyan University Recommended Citation

More information

Variability Is an Operant

Variability Is an Operant Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Assooation, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/85/$00.75 1985, Vol. II, No. 3, 429~152 Variability Is an Operant Suzanne

More information

An analysis of reinstatement of appropriate behavior

An analysis of reinstatement of appropriate behavior University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Theses and Dissertations Summer 2017 An analysis of reinstatement of appropriate behavior Stephen Edward Ryan University of Iowa Copyright 2017 Stephen Edward Ryan

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task

Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task Analysis of Gambling Behavior Volume 11 Article 2 2017 Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task Nathaniel C. Rice 1. United States Army Medical Research Institute of Chemical Defense,

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2012, 97, 85 100 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

More information

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, 215 231 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS

More information

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, & JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR1 1982, 38, 71-85 NUMBER I (JULY) MEMORY FOR RECENT BEHAVIOR IN THE PIGEON STEPHEN P. KRAMER CORRECTIONS DIVISION, DRAPER, UTAH Variations of the symbolic

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

IMITATION OF TARGET SEQUENCES IN PIGEONS. A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Stanislaus

IMITATION OF TARGET SEQUENCES IN PIGEONS. A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Stanislaus IMITATION OF TARGET SEQUENCES IN PIGEONS A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Stanislaus In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science in Psychology

More information

Red de Revistas Científicas de América Latina y el Caribe, España y Portugal. Universidad Autónoma del Estado de México.

Red de Revistas Científicas de América Latina y el Caribe, España y Portugal. Universidad Autónoma del Estado de México. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta Sociedad Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta jburgos@ cucba.udg.mx ISSN (Versión impresa): 0185-4534 MÉXICO 2007 Anton D. Navarro / Edmund Fantino THE ROLE OF

More information

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION By JIN HO YOON A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY

More information

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 009, 9, 367 377 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) WITHIN-SUBJECT REVERSIBILITY OF DISCRIMINATIVE FUNCTION IN THE COMPOSITE-STIMULUS CONTROL OF BEHAVIOR STANLEY J. WEISS,

More information

ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS. MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison

ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS. MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 603-620 ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison SHELLY TERRY-CAIN

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information