Measurement, Theory, and Current Issues in Metacognition: An Overview

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Measurement, Theory, and Current Issues in Metacognition: An Overview"

Transcription

1 Chapter 1 Downloaded via on October 14, 2018 at 15:08:47 (UTC). See for options on how to legitimately share published articles. Measurement, Theory, and Current Issues in Metacognition: An Overview Tyler M. Miller * Department of Psychology, South Dakota State University, Brookings, South Dakota 57007, United States * tyler.miller@sdstate.edu. People thinking about their own thinking, a phenomenon known as metacognition, has a long history. Records of metacognitive thinking date back to ancient Greece, but people thinking about their own thoughts and beliefs certainly existed before then. However, actual metacognitive research in psychology only began in the mid-nineteenth century. In this chapter, I provide a very brief account of metacognition in history and present an overview of some topics in measurement, theory, and current research from my perspective as an experimental psychologist. Topics included have psychological, neuropsychological, and educational relevance. I pay particular attention to the varieties, causes, and consequences of metacognitive bias. I also include a sample of interventions from the literature used by teachers and researchers to improve metacognitive monitoring accuracy in the classroom and in the lab. Finally, the reasons why metacognitive sophistication should be promoted are found throughout the chapter American Chemical Society

2 Suppose we try to recall a forgotten name. The state of our consciousness is peculiar. There is a gap therein; but no mere gap. It is a gap that is intensely active. A sort of wraith of the name is in it, beckoning us in a given direction, making us at moments tingle with the sense of our closeness, and then letting us sink back without the longed-for term. If wrong names are proposed to us, this singularly definite gap acts immediately so as to negate them. They do not fit into its mould. And the gap of one word does not feel like the gap of another, all empty of content as both might seem necessarily to be when described as gaps. -William James, The Principles of Psychology, 1890 (1) Introduction Metacognition is thinking about thinking, knowing about knowing, or first in the literature as cognition about cognitive phenomena (p. 906; (2)). Eighty-nine years earlier, William James was writing about metacognition in his classic twovolume book, Principles of Psychology (1). In the quote above, James referred to one of the three interrelated processes that make up metacognition monitoring. The other two are knowledge and control of cognition. The person described in the quote is in the unenviable position known as a tip of the tongue state. He is sure he knows the name but is unable to produce it. In other words, he has monitored his memory and determined the name is available just not accessible. Perhaps if he were given more he could retrieve the name. Or, perhaps the name would spontaneously appear in his consciousness with more time. When asked about something else, he may monitor his memory again for the longed for information, and confess he does not know the answer and will never know the answer no matter how much time passes. These scenarios both describe metacognitive monitoring. Another component of metacognition is control and refers to a person intentionally directing her thinking. A student may prepare for an upcoming exam by allocating her study time to the material that is perceived to be the most difficult, easiest, or somewhere in between. Finally, metacognitive knowledge is a person s belief about cognitive processes. For example, an astute student will know that preparing for an exam in a distracting environment will interfere with learning. These three metacognitive processes operate in concert at each phase of learning (i.e., encoding, storage, and retrieval). John Flavell, the American developmental psychologist, is credited with the term metacognition itself (2). In his seminal works, Flavell discussed metacognition and tracked the development of metamemory in school-aged children (3). The term metacognition is a broad term that encompasses a person s awareness of all cognitive processes. Metamemory refers to a person s awareness of their own memory. Although all types of metacognition have received attention from researchers, the focus of this overview is considered metamemory. Even before William James, metacognitive processes were top-of-mind for researchers in psychology and other figures in history. People were very likely thinking about their own mental representations since the beginning. Evidence of 2

3 metacognitive behavior even exists in non-human primates (e.g., rhesus monkeys) and suggests an evolutionary pressure for this advanced form of thinking perhaps co-occurring with the appearance of humans earliest ancestors (4). Ancient Greek orators, like Simonides (c BC), exhibited metacognitive knowledge and control by recognizing the limitations of memory and devised strategies (e.g., the method of loci) to allow them to deliver long speeches and recite poetry without notes. Today, the method of loci and other strategies to scaffold memory are called mnemonic devices. The German psychologist, Herman Ebbinghaus ( ) also exhibited metacognitive knowledge and used only nonsense syllables as stimuli in his classic memory research. He used nonsense syllables, rather than meaningful syllables, because he knew meaningful stimuli would be more memorable and thus contaminate his results (5). The entire system known as structuralism promoted by the English psychologist Edward Titchener ( ) was based on monitoring one s own thinking through analytic introspection. Through introspection, the structuralist s goal was to create a periodic table of consciousness of simple characteristics like brightness, loudness, clarity and others that could be combined to form an individual s perception (6). Today, changes in measurement, theory, and technology have significantly changed the scope of metacognitive research since the work of James, Ebbinghaus, and Titchener. Researchers in many disciplines measure a wide variety of metacognitive judgments using many research designs to answer both basic and applied questions. Early Modern Research and Theory in Metacognition Measuring metacognition, like some other psychological constructs, is not as straightforward as measuring tangible objects. In early modern research, researchers began comparing peoples metacognitive judgments against an objective standard such as a person s performance on a memory test. Joseph Hart was among the first in the modern era to evaluate a person s metacognitive state against some basis of fact. In this research, Hart asked participants to make a feeling-of-knowing (FOK) judgment after failing to retrieve a question. For example, an experimenter may first ask a participant to name the prime minister of Great Britain. If the participant cannot name the prime minister, he would then report how likely he would be to select the correct name from a list of names. Hart then compared participants FOK reports to their actual ability to select the correct name. As it happened, participants were accurate at predicting their ability to select the correct name (7). Similarly, Brown and McNeill, in what may be considered a 75-year later epilogue to James quote to start this chapter, gave definitions of rare words (e.g., What is the name of the instrument that uses the position of the sun and stars to navigate? ) to participants and asked them to produce the term [sextant] (8). For some participants, the term was unknown, but for others the definition produced a state in which they felt they knew the term and they were close to producing it but simply could not. These latter participants were experiencing a peculiar state of consciousness known as the tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) state. What was most intriguing about this state of 3

4 consciousness was that participants were often able to produce the first letter of the term, the number of syllables, or similar sounding words. Flash forward to 1990, and a complete framework of the monitoring and control judgments that could be made at each stage of learning was proposed by Thomas Nelson and Louis Narens (9). This framework, which included the encoding, storage, and retrieval stages of learning connected researchers to a large body of literature investigating numerous types of judgments. Not only were researchers investigating participants FOK and TOT states, there had been intense interest in the types of monitoring and control judgments at the time of encoding (10). For example, even before learning, a student will exert control by deciding how to study by selecting a type of processing (e.g., reading, testing, highlighting). Furthermore, they decide what to study either by starting with already learned information or information that is deemed most difficult or easiest (i.e. allocation of study time or item selection). Finally, the student decides when to quit studying. These control judgments are linked to monitoring judgments that are made at the same time and much research has investigated the relationship between the two. The relationship between monitoring and control is exemplified in a model by Nelson & Narens (9). An important assumption of the model is that people have the ability to be self-reflective about their cognitive activity. The model also splits cognitive processes into two inter-related components. The first component is the object-level and encompasses any ongoing cognitive process. For example, memory is an object-level process. The second component is the meta-level and contains a model of the object-level cognitive process. It is also assumed there is a communication scheme by which the meta-level receives information about the object-level via monitoring processes, defined as subjective reports about his or her introspections (p. 127). Once updated, the meta-level can compare the object-level state to another state (such as mastery of the material) and then communicate to the object-level processes via control processes. The goal of control communication is to modify the object-level process and could include continuing an on-going process, terminating an on-going process, or changing the process (9). The Nelson and Narens (1990) model of metamemory does not assume that monitoring processes provide a veridical account of object-level processes. In fact, there are many biases that exist in metacognitive monitoring. For example, people often believe they will remember more than they actually do, a metacognitive bias known as the better-than-average effect or simply overconfidence (11, 12). This bias and others are discussed in detail in a later section. Exactly how a person monitors, or subjectively reports his or her introspections, is still the topic of debate. One early theory was the direct access theory of metacognition (13). In the theory, a person would directly assess the associative strength of to-beremembered items. In other words, the same information that is used in memory forms the basis of metacognitive judgments (14). For example, imagine a person recognizes the person standing next to them in line at the grocery store but cannot remember her name. Even though the person cannot remember her name, he is sure he knew it at one time. His metacognitive state is based on knowing the name is stored in memory, but the memory trace for that name is not strong enough to be retrieved. In this way, the direct access view claims the memory trace and 4

5 the metamemory state are based on the same cognitive process. The direct access theory is included here because it was influential in metacognition s history. Today, though, researchers have dismissed the idea that people have direct access to their own memory traces. In contrast to the direct access theory, the indirect or inferential theory of metacognition claims people do not have direct access to a memory trace, and therefore can neither assess the strength of memory nor base a metacognitive judgment on that assessment. Rather, people assess proxies of the memory trace to make metacognitive judgments (15, 16). For example, if the person at the grocery store was asked to make a FOK judgment about the likelihood of retrieving her name, he may make the judgment based on recalling other information about the person, maybe, information about where she works, where he saw her last, the names of people she is with and other related information. If he were able to retrieve more related information, he would give a higher FOK judgment. If he could retrieve little related information, he would report a lower FOK judgment. Further, the sources of related information in the inferential theory of monitoring will vary depending on the type of metacognitive judgment (17). Research on the judgment of learning (JOL), a monitoring judgment people make at the time of encoding, began almost as early as any topic in the modern era (13). A JOL is a person s report of his or her own likelihood of recalling a studied item on a later test. The exact form of the report for a JOL can vary, but essentially a person studies an item and then immediately after or sometime later reports if they will remember it on a test. Like many other metacognitive judgments, the JOL can be dichotomous the person reports they will or will not remember it on the test or the judgment can be made on a scale of for example. Importantly, the JOL report is compared to performance on the test. Comparing a metacognitive judgment, in this case, a JOL, to performance can yield two types of accuracy relative and absolute accuracy. Relative accuracy, typically measured by calculating a gamma correlation, is a person s ability to discriminate between well-learned information from poorly-learned information within a list of to-be-remembered material. Like other correlational coefficients, 0 values indicate no relationship and extreme values indicate strong relationships. Absolute accuracy on the other hand, can be measured using a difference score by subtracting performance from one s prediction. In this way, 0 values indicate perfect metacognitive accuracy, positive values indicate overconfidence and negative values indicate underconfidence. Using both relative and absolute measures of metacognitive accuracy can lead to different conclusions (18). While the vast majority of researchers use traditional measures of metacognitive accuracy (i.e., gamma correlations and/or difference scores), there is growing interest and work in the area of improving measurement. For example, Higham and colleagues pointed out that traditional conceptualizations of metacognitive accuracy fail to account for the variety of ways participant s translate their subjective metacognitive experiences to overt scale values, a process they have named mapping 1 (19). Failing to account for mapping 1 in their view can lead to multiple problems, all of which hamper a study s validity. Newer approaches to measurement that can attenuate these validity 5

6 concerns include using receiving operating characteristics and signal detection theory concepts. In another paradigm, participants rate how difficult items would be to remember later. When compared to their ability to remember the items, these difficulty ratings predicted participants memory ability (20). Today, participants make ease-of-learning judgments (21). Classic metacognitive research and most research since indicates peoples metacognitive monitoring judgments have above-chance predictive accuracy. While there is substantial room for improvement, people know what they know and know what they do not know. That people s predictions are not perfectly accurate opens up investigation into the varieties and causes of inaccuracy as well as the consequences and potential interventions to improve metacognitive accuracy these topics are discussed in detail in a later section. Research on metacognitive control has gained a significant understanding of what students choose to study if they have a goal of study. For example, if a student needs to study for an upcoming exam, she must decide if her goal is to get an A or perhaps just to pass with a D. The goal of study then, may contribute to her reasoning about how long to study and what material to study. There are two prominent models that have been proposed to explain how people decide how long to study, the discrepancy-reduction model (22) and the region-of-proximal learning model (23). In the discrepancy-reduction model, learners have a goal, which is known as the norm-of-study. If the norm-of-study is complete mastery, then learners would continue to study items until they believe they have completely memorized the material. In other words, the goal of studying is to reduce the discrepancy between what is known and the norm-of-study (22). Another model of study time allocation, proposes that, perhaps in addition to reducing the discrepancy between what is known and the goal, learners prioritize their study time from the subjectively easiest material to the hardest material. According to this model, learners terminate study as soon as they believe they are no longer learning (23). Ideally, students study for a sufficient amount of time to perform well on an upcoming test. Yet, students often do not study for enough time to perform well on tests. There may be several reasons why students stop studying prematurely. For example, they may simply run out of time, especially if they have busy schedules or if they start studying at the last minute. Students may also stop studying prematurely because they falsely believe that they are prepared for the exam (12, 24), or because they make a reasonable guess about their preparation (25). Given metacognitive control decisions, such as study time allocation, are based on metacognitive monitoring, they can only be as good as the quality of the monitoring (9). Benefits of Accurate Monitoring and Control Metacognition refers to the understanding that a person has about his or her own cognition. It is preferable in most cases to have accurate metacognition. Certainly, in educational settings, it is preferable for students to be able to 6

7 accurately assess their own knowledge. If students know that they do not understand the course material they can decide to continue to study for the exam. Several laboratory and classroom studies demonstrate that accurate metacognition is associated with better performance (26 31). Conversely, metacognitive inaccuracies can lead to poor academic performance (26), under-preparedness for exams, and inefficient study decisions (32). Metacognitive accuracy is associated with and causally related to more efficient learning and improved performance outcomes. In one such study, participants studied paired words from different languages (e.g., ardhi - soil), reported judgments of learning for each word pair and took a cued-recall test (e.g., ardhi -?; (29)). Researchers also told participants the study would conclude when the participant could recall all 36 word pairs or after a set number of trials. Following the first study-test phase, participants were able to self-select items to re-study. Participants with the most accurate JOLs and the most effective item-selection also had the highest cued-recall test performance. When metacognitive monitoring is experimentally manipulated by asking some participants to generate keywords and self-select material to re-study, the data lead to the same conclusion more accurate monitoring leads to more effective control which, in turn, leads to better performance outcomes (30). Biases in Metacognition When differences in monitoring accuracy emerge, one immediately wonders why there are differences. Countless studies show that people show inaccurate metacognition in the direction of being overconfident in their abilities and characteristics and this overconfidence extends to a variety of domains. People are often overconfident about their dating attractiveness (33), driving ability (34, 35), performance on college course exams (36), humor recognition (12), and gun safety knowledge (37). Low performers in particular are prone to being overconfident about their abilities. It has been suggested that low performers suffer from a double curse, they do not know the material they will be tested on and they do not know that they are underprepared (12). Indeed, David Dunning likened low performers inflated self-assessments to a form of brain damage (i.e., anosognosia), and suggested that people performing poorly cannot be expected to recognize their ineptitude and that the ability to recognize the depth of their inadequacies is beyond them (38). It follows from the double-curse account that if low performers lack knowledge and awareness then, in addition to making inaccurate performance predictions, they would also be unduly confident in these predictions. However, there is some evidence that people may not be entirely unaware of their metacognitive shortcomings (26). In one study, participants made performance predictions about an upcoming test and then reported their confidence in the accuracy of the prediction. For example, a participant could have reported that she would earn an 86% on the test and either high or low confidence that the prediction was accurate. Some research groups have coined this confidence judgment a second-order judgment the performance prediction is the first-order 7

8 metacognitive judgment and confidence about the performance prediction is the second-order metacognitive judgment (39). Miller and Geraci showed that while students are sometimes overconfident, they predict that they will perform much better than they do, their confidence in the accuracy of their prediction can be low, suggesting a dissociation between the two types of judgments (26). The finding that overly high performance predictions of low performers were associated with low confidence was taken as evidence that some participants may have awareness that their judgments are inaccurate. Other possible explanations for low performers overconfidence are that they wish to look good to the experimenter (37), they are motivated to be overconfident (40), they attribute their poor performance to external factors (41), or their overconfidence is simply a statistical artifact (42). Two recent papers have provided support for the idea that students may base their predictions for upcoming exams on desired grades (43, 44). In one such series of studies, students in upper and lower level college courses reported scores they hoped to earn on an upcoming test (desired grade) and reported scores they thought they would earn (predicted grade; (43)). In another series of studies, students in upper and lower level college courses reported predictions and indicated what factors they considered when making the predictions. The factors were categorized around two major themes: 1) educational factors like exam preparation, or 2) motivational factors like desired grades (44). In both sets of studies, motivational factors explained more variance in predictions than educational factors. One clear implication is that any attempt to improve metacognitive accuracy that does not address motivational factors, like desired grades, are not likely to eliminate overconfidence. However, as some have noted, there could be adaptive reasons for people to believe they can perform better than they actually do (40, 45). Future research should assess metacognitive accuracy versus wishful thinking to determine their beneficial and/or detrimental contributions to performance and other outcomes in educational, occupational, social, and other important areas of functioning. Overconfidence is not the only bias in metacognitive monitoring judgments. Another systematic distortion is the hard-easy effect and occurs when people overestimate their learning on hard items and underestimate their learning on easy items (46). Participants can also show underconfidence with practice (UWP). When participants have multiple study opportunities and make multiple JOLs, their JOLs may underestimate the amount of learning that has taken place (47). In other words, people do not believe they have learned as much as they have. This fact, coupled with the fact that people believe their memories will remain accessible over time has been referred to as a stability bias in human memory (48). Biological Bases of Metacognition Metacognitive research using special population samples and imaging technology has made the understanding of the brain areas associated with metacognitive processes more complete. For example, younger college student 8

9 participants display more overconfidence than their older student counterparts (49). Compared to older adults, some studies show younger people display more overconfidence than older adults (50) or equivalent metacognitive resolution (51). Older adults spared metacognitive ability has even been used to improve test performance (52, 53). Older adults with dementia attributed to Alzheimer s disease show little insight into their cognitive impairment. In fact, lack of insight is an early symptom of dementia (54). Importantly, individuals with vascular dementia have relatively preserved insight. The difference in metacognitive insight between the two types of dementias may be explained by the differences in pathophysiology. Whereas significant neocortical, frontal lobe atrophy is common with dementia of the Alzheimer s type and fronto-temporal dementia, frontal-lobe atrophy is not as common in vascular dementia (55). Similarly, patients with frontal lobe lesions due to surgical removal of brain tumors or arteriovenous malformation treatment exhibited greater metacognitive inaccuracy (i.e., overconfidence) than control subjects, particularly those with left frontal lesions (56). Research using imaging technology has corroborated the important role of the pre-frontal cortex in metacognitive processing (57, 58). In one such study, participant s grey matter volume in the anterior prefrontal cortex was associated with more metacognitive awareness (59). In another imaging study, participants viewed pictures and were asked to make dichotomous predictions about their future memory performance (60). Participants reported either they would remember or would not remember the picture on a recognition memory test while fmri data were collected. The authors reached several conclusions from the data. Among them was the conclusion that medial-temporal lobe activity, long implicated in memory formation, was not associated with metacognitive processing. Additionally, ventro-medial and lateral prefrontal cortical activity was associated with greater metacognitive accuracy. Experimentally, repetitive trans-cranial magnetic stimulation (rtms) has been used to depress activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex resulting in less insight into cognitive processes (61). Training Metacognition Given the benefits of accurate metacognition but also the systematic biases that exist, there have been several attempts to improve or train metacognition in the classroom and in the laboratory. These attempts have yielded mixed results. Several interventions to improve metacognitive accuracy have used feedback. For example, studies show that providing repeated feedback to participants about the accuracy of their predictions appears to lead to modest improvements in metacognitive accuracy. In one study, participants viewed word pairs under various encoding tasks and then rated their confidence that their answers were correct on five successive days of sessions (62). Participants who received feedback after each session, showed moderate improvement in their confidence accuracy from session one to five while control condition participants did not improve. In another study, participants made retrospective confidence judgments 9

10 about the accuracy of their responses to general knowledge questions and received feedback about their accuracy (63). After 23 1-hour sessions, participants who were overconfident at the beginning of the training improved their calibration; although the majority of the improvement occurred between the first and second sessions. The authors wrote the training protocol for the study was both arduous and expensive, so much so that they completed a second experiment to determine if similar improvement would be seen with a shortened training program that was only 11 sessions long instead of 23 (pg. 166). The shortened training program did yield similar improvement. Another theme of intervention studies has been to give participants practice making predictions and practice taking memory tests. For example, when participants make one global judgment of learning, complete a practice test, and then are allowed to adjust their performance prediction, they become more accurate (64). Sometimes, these interventions improve predictions for certain participants and not others. For example, Kelemen and colleagues showed that participants predictions improved significantly after 5 sessions of making performance predictions, but it was only the high achieving students who were able to improve their metacognitive accuracy (65). This outcome when an intervention designed to benefit low achieving students has a greater benefit for high achieving students has become known as the Matthew effect (36, 65). There are other non-intervention approaches that have been used to improve metacognitive accuracy in the laboratory, too. For example, one way to improve accuracy is to increase the time between when the subject finishes studying and when the prediction is made (66). This improvement is known as the delayed- JOL effect and is thought to occur because participants are only able to use the contents of long term memory when the judgment is made (i.e., the monitoring dual memories hypothesis), which matches what the participant will use during the test. The advantage of delayed-jols over immediate JOLs has recently been confirmed through a meta-analysis involving more than 40 studies (67). Methods to improve metacognition in the classroom have used interventions that include practice (68), feedback (41), incentives (24), self-reflection (69) and combinations of these interventions. Results from these studies indicate that metacognitive monitoring ability is very resistant to intervention, and in instances when the intervention does improve metacognitive monitoring it sometimes only benefits the highest-performing students. Education and Metacognition At the same time modern metacognitive research was beginning, Ann Brown acknowledged the topic as having huge importance for educational practice (70). All varieties of study designs (i.e., correlational, cross-sectional, and longitudinal studies) have confirmed the importance of instruction using metacognitive theory beginning in very early grade-levels and continuing through secondary education to improve performance outcomes (71). On the other hand, metacognitive inaccuracy can lead to underachievement (72). Further, it has been argued that 10

11 flawed self-assessment has had the effect of higher rates of attrition from STEM fields (73). Not only should instructors be cognizant of and use instruction that fosters students metacognitive sophistication, instructors need to inform, and sometimes retrain students about effective study strategies (74). Fostering students understanding that monitoring processes are flawed is a key factor. For example, most students are not aware of the most effective study strategies, preferring less effective study strategies like re-reading or highlighting (75). At the extreme, some strategies used by students can even lead their monitoring accuracy and control to become more distorted. These strategies lead to so-called illusions of competence and can be detrimental to academic achievement (76, 77). With the publication of the current volume, enhancing learning and instruction with an understanding of metacognitive processes will continue to be an important applied research area. Some themes of this research include people s metacognitive capabilities, causes and consequences of metacognitive inaccuracy as well as benefits of accurate metacognition, interventions to improve monitoring and control, and selection of optimal study strategies. Understanding what people know about their own cognition and how they use that knowledge to guide their future behavior in order to develop more effective interventions and teaching techniques will have a significant impact on students lives in the high-stakes world of education. References 1. James, W. The Principles of Psychology; Henry Holt: New York, 1890; pp Flavell, J. H. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring. Amer. Psychol. 1979, 34, Kreutzer, M. A.; Leonard, C.; Flavell, J. H. An interview study of children s knowledge about memory. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1975, 40, Metcalfe, J. In Handbook of Metamemory and Memory; Dunlosky, J.; Bjork, R. A., Eds.; Psychology Press: New York, 2008; pp Ebbinghaus, H. Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology (translated by H. A. Ruger); Teachers College Press: New York, 1913; pp Titchener, E. B. Systematic Psychology: Prolegomena; Macmillan: New York, 1929; pp Hart, J. T. Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. J. Educ. Psychol. 1965, 56, Brown, R.; McNeill, D. The tip of the tongue phenomenon. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1966, 5, Nelson, T. O.; Narens, L. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Bower, G. H., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, 1990; Vol. 26, pp Tauber, S. K. & Dunlosky, J. In The Oxford Handbook of Metamemory; Dunlsoky, J.; Tauber, S. K., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, 2016; pp

12 11. Alicke, M. Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1985, 49, Kruger, J.; Dunning, D. Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties recognizing one s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, Arbuckle, T Y.; Cuddy, L. L. Discrimination of item strength at time of presentation. J. Exper. Psychol. 1969, 81, Schwartz, B. L. Sources of information in metamemory: Judgments of learning and feelings of knowing. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1994, 1, Koriat, A. How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychol. Rev. 1993, 100, Schwartz, B. L.; Metcalfe, J. Cue familiarity but not target retrievability enhances feeling-of-knowing judgments. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn., Mem., Cognit. 1992, 18, Hunt, R. R.; Ellis, H. C. Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology; McGraw Hill: New York, 2004; pp Schraw, G.; Kuch, F.; Gutierrez, A. P. Measure for measure: Calibrating ten commonly used calibration scores. Learn. Instr. 2013, 24, Higham, P. A.; Zawadzka, K.; Hanczakowski, M. In The Oxford Handbook of Metamemory; Dunlsoky, J.; Tauber, S. K., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, 2016; pp Underwood, B. J. Individual and group prediction of item difficulty for free learning. J. Exp. Psychol. 1966, 71, Strain, A. C.; Azevedo, R.; D Mello, S. K. Using a false biofeedback methodology to explore relationships between learners affect, metacognition, and performance. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 38, Dunlosky, J.; Hertzog, C. In Metacognition in Educational Theory and Practice; Hacker, D. J.; Dunlosky, J.; Graesser, A. C., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, 1998; pp Metcalfe, J.; Kornell, N. A region of proximal learning model of study time allocation. J. Mem. Lang. 2005, 52, Miller, T. M.; Geraci, L. Training metacognition in the classroom: How incentives and feedback influence exam predictions. Metacognit. Learn. 2411, 6, Miller, T. M.; Geraci, L. Unskilled but aware: Reinterpreting overconfidence in low-performing students. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn., Mem. Cognit. 2011, 37, Everson, H. T.; Tobias, S. The ability to estimate knowledge and performance in college: A metacognitive analysis. Instr. Sci. 1998, 26, Nelson, T. O.; Dunlosky, J.; Graf, A.; Narens, L. Utilization of metacognitive judgments in the allocation of study during multitrial learning. Psychol. Sci. 1994, 5, Nietfeld, J. L.; Cao, L.; Osborne, J. W. The effect of distributed monitoring exercises and feedback on performance, monitoring accuracy, and self-efficacy. Metacognit. Learn. 2006, 1,

13 29. Thiede, K. W. The importance of monitoring and self-regulation during multitrial learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1999, 6, Thiede, K. W.; Anderson, M. C. M.; Therriault, D. Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts. J. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 95, Rawson, K. A.; O Neil, R.; Dunlosky, J. Accurate monitoring leads to effective control and greater learning of patient education materials. J. Exp. Psychol.: Appl. 2011, 17, Bjork, R. A.; Dunlosky, J.; Kornell, N. Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, Preuss, G. S.; Alicke, M. D. Everybody loves me: Self-evaluations and metaperceptions of dating popularity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 35, Knouse, L. E.; Bagwell, C. L.; Barkley, R. A.; Murphy, K. R. Accuracy of self-evaluation in adults with ADHD: Evidence from a driving study. J. Atten. Disord. 2005, 8, Williams, A. F. Views of U.S. drivers about driving safety. J. Saf. Res. 2003, 34, Hacker, D. J.; Bol, L.; Horgan, D.; Rakow, E. A. Test prediction and performance in a classroom context. J. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 92, Ehrlinger, J.; Johnson, K.; Banner, M.; Dunning, D.; Kruger, J. Why the unskilled are unaware: Further explorations of (absent) self-insight among the incompetent. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Processes 2008, 105, Dunning, D. Self-insight: Roadblocks and Detours on the Path to Knowing Thyself; Taylor & Francis Books: New York, 2005; pp Dunlosky, J.; Serra, M. J.; Matvey, G.; Rawson, K. A. Second-order judgments about judgments of learning. J. Gen. Psychol. 2005, 132, Gramzow, R. H.; Willard, G.; Mendes, W. B. Big tales and cool heads: Academic exaggeration is related to cardiac vagal reactivity. Emotion 2008, 8, Hacker, D. J.; Bol, L.; Bahbahani, K. Explaining calibration accuracy in classroom contexts: The effects of incentives, reflection and explanatory style. Metacognit. Learn. 2008, 3, Krueger, J.; Mueller, R. A. Unskilled, unaware, or both? The better-thanaverage heuristic and statistical regression predict errors in estimates of own performance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 82, Serra, M. J.; DeMarree, K. G. Unskilled and unaware in the classroom: College students desired grades predict their biased grade predictions. Mem. Cognit. 2016, 44, Saenz, G. D.; Geraci, L.; Miller, T. M.; Tirso, R. Metacognition in the classroom: The association between students exam predictions and their desired grades. Conscious. Cognit. 2017, 51, Shin, H.; Bjorklund, D. F.; Beck, E. F. The adaptive nature of children s overestimation in a strategic memory task. Cognit. Dev. 2007, 22,

14 46. Lichtenstein, S.; Fischoff, B.; Phillips, L. D. In Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases; Kahneman, D.; Slovic, P.; Tversky, A., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, 1982; pp Koriat, A.; Sheffer, L.; Ma ayan, H. Comparing objective and subjective learning curves: Judgments of learning exhibit increased underconfidence with practice. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 2002, 131, Kornell, N.; Bjork, R. A. A stability bias in human memory: Overestimating remembering and underestimating learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 2009, 138, Grimes, P. W. The overconfident principles of economics student: An examination of a metacognitive skill. J. Econ. Educ. 2002, 33, Hertzog, C.; Sinclair, S. M.; Dunlosky, J. Age differences in the monitoring of learning: Cross-sectional evidence of spared resolution across the adult life span. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, Hertzog, C.; Kidder, D. P.; Powell-Moman, A.; Dunlosky, J. Aging and monitoring associative learning: Is monitoring accuracy spared or impaired? Psychol. Aging 2002, 17, Dunlosky, J.; Kubat-Silman, A.; Hertzog, C. Training monitoring skills improves older adults associative learning. Psychol. Aging 2003, 18, Hertzog, C.; Dunlosky, J. Metacognition in later adulthood: Spared monitoring can benefit older adults self-regulation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 20, Mendez, M. F.; Cummings, J. L. Dementia: A Clinical Approach. Butterworth Heinemann: Philadelphia, PA, 2003; pp Salat, D. H.; Kaye, J. H.; Janowsky, J. S. Selective preservation and degeneration in the prefrontal cortex in aging and Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 2001, 58, Vilkki, J.; Servo, A.; Outi, S. Word list learning and prediction of recall after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychology 1998, 12, Pannu, J. K.; Kaszniak, A. W. Metamemory experiments in neurological populations: A review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2005, 15, Schwartz, B. L.; Bacon, E. In Handbook of Metamemory and Memory; Dunlosky, J.; Bjork, R. A., Eds.; Psychology Press: New York, 2008; pp Fleming, S. M.; Weil, R. S.; Nagy, Z.; Dolan, R. J.; Rees, G. Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Sci. 2010, 329, Kao, Y. C.; Davis, E. S.; Gabrieli, J. D. E. Neural correlates of actual and predicted memory formation. Nat. Neurosci. 2005, 8, Rounis, E.; Maniscalco, B.; Rothwell, J. C.; Passingham, R. E.; Lau, H. Theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation to the prefrontal cortex impairs metacognitive visual awareness. Cognit. Neurosci. 2010, 1, Adams, P. A.; Adams, J. K. Training in confidence-judgments. Amer. J. Psychol. 1958, 71, Lichtenstein, S.; Fischoff, B. Training for calibration. Organ. Beh. Hum. Perform. 1980, 26,

15 64. Miller, T. M.; Geraci, L. Improving metacognitive accuracy: How failing to retrieve practice items reduces overconfidence. Conscious. Cognit. 2014, 29, Kelemen, W. L.; Winningham, R. G.; Weaver, C. A., III Repeated testing sessions and scholastic aptitude in college students metacognitive accuracy. Eur. J. Cognit. Psychol. 2007, 19, Nelson, T. O.; Dunlosky, J. When people s judgments of learning are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The delayed-jol effect. Psychol. Sci. 1991, 2, Rhodes, M. G.; Tauber, S. K. The influence of delaying judgments of learning on metacognitive accuracy: A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 2011, 137, Bol, L.; Hacker, D. J.; O Shea, P.; Allen, D. The influence of overt practice, achievement level, and explanatory style on calibration accuracy and performance. J. Exp. Educ. 2005, 73, Nietfeld, J. L.; Cao, L.; Osborne, J. W. Metacognitive monitoring accuracy and student performance in the postsecondary classroom. J. Exp. Educ. 2005, 74, Brown, A. L. In Advances in Child Development and Behavior; Reese H. W., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, 1975; Vol. 10, pp Kron-Sperl, V.; Schneider, W.; Hasselhorn, M. The development and effectiveness of memory strategies in kindergarten and elementary school: Findings from the Würzburg and Göttingen longitudinal studies. Cognit. Dev. 2008, 23, Dunlosky, J.; Rawson, K. A. Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self evaluations undermine students learning and retention. Learn. Instr. 2012, 22, Stinebrickner, R.; Stinebrickner, T. R. A major in science? Initial beliefs and final outcomes for college major and dropout. Rev. Econ. Stud. 2016, 81, Dunlosky, J.; Rawson, K. A.; Marsh, E. J.; Nathan, M. J.; Willingham, D. T. Improving students learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2013, 14, Karpicke, J. D.; Butler, A. C.; Roediger, H. L., III Metacognitive Strategies in student learning: Do students practice retrieval when they study on their own? Mem. 2009, 17, Benjamin, A. S.; Bjork, R. A.; Schwartz, B. L. The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 1998, 127, Rhodes, M. G.; Castel, A. D. Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: Evidence for metacognitive illusions. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 2008, 137,

Metamemory. Adam B. Blake and Alan D. Castel University of California, Los Angeles, USA

Metamemory. Adam B. Blake and Alan D. Castel University of California, Los Angeles, USA Metamemory Adam B. Blake and Alan D. Castel University of California, Los Angeles, USA Metamemory refers to the set of processes responsible for governing the knowledge, the beliefs about, and the control

More information

Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting

Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2012, 65 (11), 2231 2257 Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting Michael J. Serra and Benjamin D. England Department

More information

The effects of categorical relatedness on judgements of learning (JOLs)

The effects of categorical relatedness on judgements of learning (JOLs) MEMORY, 2006, 14 (2), 253±261 The effects of categorical relatedness on judgements of learning (JOLs) Greg Matvey and John Dunlosky The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, USA Bennett L. Schwartz

More information

Calibration research: where do we go from here?

Calibration research: where do we go from here? HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY ARTICLE published: 09 July 2012 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00229 : where do we go from here? Linda Bol 1 * and Douglas J. Hacker 2 1 Department of Educational Foundations and Leadership,

More information

University of Southampton Research Repository eprints Soton

University of Southampton Research Repository eprints Soton University of Southampton Research Repository eprints Soton Copyright and Moral Rights for this thesis are retained by the author and/or other copyright owners. A copy can be downloaded for personal non-commercial

More information

Does Retrieval Fluency Contribute to the Underconfidence-With-Practice Effect?

Does Retrieval Fluency Contribute to the Underconfidence-With-Practice Effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2005, Vol. 31, No. 6, 1258 1266 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1258

More information

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing This document contains 4 examples of writing and feedback comments from Level 2A lab reports, and 4 steps to help you apply

More information

Memory Predictions Are Influenced by Perceptual Information: Evidence for Metacognitive Illusions

Memory Predictions Are Influenced by Perceptual Information: Evidence for Metacognitive Illusions Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 8 by the American Psychological Association 8, Vol. 137, No. 4, 615 625 96-3445/8/$12. DOI: 1.137/a13684 Memory Predictions Are Influenced by Perceptual

More information

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition 1 Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition Intro Memory Empirical Studies Interf MR Scan LTM Codes DCT Imagery & Spatial Cognition Rel Org Principles ImplEnc SpatEq Neuro Imaging Critique StruEq Prop

More information

Don t Look at My Answer: Subjective Uncertainty Underlies Preschoolers Exclusion of Their Least Accurate Memories

Don t Look at My Answer: Subjective Uncertainty Underlies Preschoolers Exclusion of Their Least Accurate Memories 5473PSSXXX10.1177/09567976145473Hembacher, GhettiPreschoolers Uncertainty and Decision Making research-article014 Research Article Don t Look at My Answer: Subjective Uncertainty Underlies Preschoolers

More information

AP Psychology Scoring Components Page(s)

AP Psychology Scoring Components Page(s) Scoring Components SC1 The course provides instruction in history and approaches. 5 SC2 The course provides instruction in research methods used in psychological science, practice and ethics. 3 SC3 The

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

Nathaniel L. Foster Department of Psychology St. Mary s College of Maryland St. Mary s City, MD (240)

Nathaniel L. Foster Department of Psychology St. Mary s College of Maryland St. Mary s City, MD (240) Nathaniel L. Foster Department of Psychology St. Mary s College of Maryland St. Mary s City, MD 20686 (240) 895-4697 nlfoster@smcm.edu EDUCATION University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro,

More information

Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1. Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him. Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2

Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1. Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him. Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2 Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1 Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2 1 University of California, Berkeley 2 Cornell University Author Note

More information

Psychological. Influences on Personal Probability. Chapter 17. Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc.

Psychological. Influences on Personal Probability. Chapter 17. Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc. Psychological Chapter 17 Influences on Personal Probability Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc. 17.2 Equivalent Probabilities, Different Decisions Certainty Effect: people

More information

1. The Greek philosopher who believed that intelligence was inherited was: A) Aristotle. B) Plato. C) Descartes. D) Simonides.

1. The Greek philosopher who believed that intelligence was inherited was: A) Aristotle. B) Plato. C) Descartes. D) Simonides. 1. The Greek philosopher who believed that intelligence was inherited was: A) Aristotle. B) Plato. C) Descartes. D) Simonides. 2. To say that psychology is a science means that: A) psychologists study

More information

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Tracey Miser (miser.4@osu.edu) Department of Psychology, 1835 Neil Avenue Columbus, OH43210 USA Vladimir Sloutsky (sloutsky.1@osu.edu)

More information

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040 School of Psychology Important Degree Information: B.Sc./M.A. Honours The general requirements are 480 credits over a period of normally 4 years (and not more than 5 years) or part-time equivalent; the

More information

Calibration in tennis: The role of feedback and expertise

Calibration in tennis: The role of feedback and expertise Calibration in tennis: The role of feedback and expertise Gerard J. Fogarty (fogarty@usq.edu.au) Department of Psychology University of Southern Queensland, Toowoomba QLD 4350 Australia Anthony Ross (a_ross4@hotmail.com)

More information

1. The Greek philosopher who believed that intelligence was inherited was: A) Aristotle. B) Plato. C) Descartes. D) Simonides.

1. The Greek philosopher who believed that intelligence was inherited was: A) Aristotle. B) Plato. C) Descartes. D) Simonides. Name: Date: 1. The Greek philosopher who believed that intelligence was inherited was: A) Aristotle. B) Plato. C) Descartes. D) Simonides. 2. The fourth-century scholar who anticipated health psychology's

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Why is dispersion of memory important*

Why is dispersion of memory important* What is memory* It is a web of connections Research has shown that people who lose their memory also lose the ability to connect things to each other in their mind It is these connections that let us understand

More information

Response speeding mediates the contributions of cue familiarity and target retrievability to metamnemonic judgments

Response speeding mediates the contributions of cue familiarity and target retrievability to metamnemonic judgments Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 25,?? 12 (?), (5),???-??? 874-879 Response speeding mediates the contributions of cue familiarity and target retrievability to metamnemonic judgments AARON S. BENJAMIN

More information

Psych 136S Review Questions, Summer 2015

Psych 136S Review Questions, Summer 2015 Psych 136S Review Questions, Summer 2015 For each paper you should be able to briefly summarize the methods and results and explain why the results are important. The guided summary for the Roediger et

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

CHAPTER 3: SOCIAL PERCEPTION: UNDERSTANDING OTHER PEOPLE CHAPTER OVERVIEW

CHAPTER 3: SOCIAL PERCEPTION: UNDERSTANDING OTHER PEOPLE CHAPTER OVERVIEW CHAPTER 3: SOCIAL PERCEPTION: UNDERSTANDING OTHER PEOPLE CHAPTER OVERVIEW Chapter 3 covers three main topics: impressions others make on us, attribution processes and biases, and the impressions we make

More information

Information and cue-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states

Information and cue-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states Information and cue-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states Psycholinguistics 2 Contents: Introduction... 1 Pilot Experiment... 2 Experiment... 3 Participants... 3 Materials... 4 Design... 4 Procedure...

More information

After the Diagnosis: Rehabilitation & Support Options for Mild Dementia

After the Diagnosis: Rehabilitation & Support Options for Mild Dementia After the Diagnosis: Rehabilitation & Support Options for Mild Dementia Dr. Toni Nicholls, Clinical Neuropsychologist Peronne Village, cottage #20, Worthing, Christ Church 621-2022 Say these aloud Dog

More information

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels A peer-reviewed electronic journal. Copyright is retained by the first or sole author, who grants right of first publication to Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation. Permission is granted to distribute

More information

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES (2018-19) PSYCHOLOGY Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology OUTLINE OF THE CHAPTER (i) Scientific Methods in Psychology -observation, case study, surveys, psychological tests, experimentation

More information

Confidence and Memory Accuracy: Do we know what we think we know? Bennett Brocka. University of Iowa

Confidence and Memory Accuracy: Do we know what we think we know? Bennett Brocka. University of Iowa Running Head: CONFIDENCE AND MEMORY ACCURACY 1 Confidence and Memory Accuracy: Do we know what we think we know? Bennett Brocka University of Iowa Running Head: CONFIDENCE AND MEMORY ACCURACY 2 Abstract

More information

Importance of Deficits

Importance of Deficits Importance of Deficits In complex systems the parts are often so integrated that they cannot be detected in normal operation Need to break the system to discover the components not just physical components

More information

Retrospective bias in test performance: Providing easy items at the beginning of a test makes students believe they did better on it

Retrospective bias in test performance: Providing easy items at the beginning of a test makes students believe they did better on it Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (3), 366-376 doi:10.3758/mc.38.3.366 Retrospective bias in test performance: Providing easy items at the beginning of a test makes students believe they did better on it YANA

More information

What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of

What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of Memories AIM:Bartlett aimed to investigate the effect of culture on memory. memory ability/ability to recall and have advantageous effects if asked to do a

More information

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments Consciousness and Cognition 9, 215 219 (2000) doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0438, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

More information

Schematic knowledge changes what judgments of learning predict in a source memory task

Schematic knowledge changes what judgments of learning predict in a source memory task Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (1), 42-51 doi:10.3758/mc.37.1.42 Schematic knowledge changes what judgments of learning predict in a source memory task AGNIESZKA E. KONOPKA AND AARON S. BENJAMIN University

More information

The effect of metacomprehension judgment task on comprehension monitoring and metacognitive accuracy

The effect of metacomprehension judgment task on comprehension monitoring and metacognitive accuracy Metacognition Learning (2012) 7:113 131 DOI 10.1007/s11409-012-9087-y The effect of metacomprehension judgment task on comprehension monitoring and metacognitive accuracy Yasuhiro Ozuru & Christopher A.

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment Decisions

An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment Decisions International Review of Business and Finance ISSN 0976-5891 Volume 9, Number 1 (2017), pp. 57-61 Research India Publications http://www.ripublication.com An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment

More information

Explaining Calibration in Classroom Contexts: The Effects of Incentives, Reflection, and Attributional Style. Douglas J. Hacker. University of Utah

Explaining Calibration in Classroom Contexts: The Effects of Incentives, Reflection, and Attributional Style. Douglas J. Hacker. University of Utah Calibration in Classrooms 1 Hacker, D. J., Bol, L., Bahbahani, K. (in press). Explaining calibration in classroom contexts: The effects of incentives, reflection, and attributional style. Metacognition

More information

UNESCO EOLSS. This article deals with risk-defusing behavior. It is argued that this forms a central part in decision processes.

UNESCO EOLSS. This article deals with risk-defusing behavior. It is argued that this forms a central part in decision processes. RISK-DEFUSING BEHAVIOR Oswald Huber University of Fribourg, Switzerland Keywords: cognitive bias, control, cost of risk-defusing operators, decision making, effect of risk-defusing operators, lottery,

More information

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15 Improving memory 2 PSY 200 Greg Francis Lecture 22 Get a good night s sleep. Memory We seem to be unable to control our memories w learn things we don t want to remember w unable to learn things we want

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences 2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences Stanislas Dehaene Chair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture n 5 Bayesian Decision-Making Lecture material translated

More information

Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk.

Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk. chunking Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk. clustering Organizing items into related groups during

More information

Audio: In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2

Audio: In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2 Psychology 312: Lecture 2 Psychology as a Science Slide #1 Psychology As A Science In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2 Outline Psychology is an empirical science.

More information

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Parkland College A with Honors Projects Honors Program 2015 Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Hannah Li Parkland College Recommended Citation Li, Hannah, "Social and Pragmatic Language

More information

FAQ: Heuristics, Biases, and Alternatives

FAQ: Heuristics, Biases, and Alternatives Question 1: What is meant by the phrase biases in judgment heuristics? Response: A bias is a predisposition to think or act in a certain way based on past experience or values (Bazerman, 2006). The term

More information

The Standard Theory of Conscious Perception

The Standard Theory of Conscious Perception The Standard Theory of Conscious Perception C. D. Jennings Department of Philosophy Boston University Pacific APA 2012 Outline 1 Introduction Motivation Background 2 Setting up the Problem Working Definitions

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/24/2018: Lecture 05-2 Note: This Powerpoint presentation

More information

Reduce Tension by Making the Desired Choice Easier

Reduce Tension by Making the Desired Choice Easier Daniel Kahneman Talk at Social and Behavioral Sciences Meeting at OEOB Reduce Tension by Making the Desired Choice Easier Here is one of the best theoretical ideas that psychology has to offer developed

More information

Empirical Analysis of the Intrinsic-Extrinsic Distinction of Judgments of Learning (JOLs): Effects of Relatedness and Serial Position on JOLs

Empirical Analysis of the Intrinsic-Extrinsic Distinction of Judgments of Learning (JOLs): Effects of Relatedness and Serial Position on JOLs Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, emory, and Cognition 2001, Vol. 27, No. 5, 1180-1191 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/01/S5.00 DOI: 10.1O37//0278-7393.27.5.1180

More information

Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary. NLPcourses.com

Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary. NLPcourses.com Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary NLPcourses.com Table of Contents Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE by Richard Bandler Summary... 6 Chapter 1 Who s Driving the Bus?... 6 Chapter 2 Running Your Own

More information

Involuntary and Voluntary Memory Sequencing Phenomena

Involuntary and Voluntary Memory Sequencing Phenomena 5 Involuntary and Voluntary Memory Sequencing Phenomena An Interesting Puzzle for the Study of Autobiographical Memory Organization and Retrieval Jennifer Talarico and John H. Mace Introduction One of

More information

Purpose of Grading. Suggestions for assembling items. Suggestions for administering items

Purpose of Grading. Suggestions for assembling items. Suggestions for administering items Suggestions for assembling items Clean up errors -- proofread! Keep items free from racial, ethnic, and gender bias Include clear directions -- don t assume students know these Group items according to

More information

Psychology Stage 1 Modules / 2018

Psychology Stage 1 Modules / 2018 Psychology Stage 1 Modules - 2017 / 2018 PSYC101: Psychology An Introduction - (Semester 1) 20 credits This module is an introduction to Psychology as a science. It includes elements which expose students

More information

Executive Functions and ADHD

Executive Functions and ADHD Image by Photographer s Name (Credit in black type) or Image by Photographer s Name (Credit in white type) Executive Functions and ADHD: Theory Underlying the New Brown Executive Functions/Attention Scales

More information

Sperling conducted experiments on An experiment was conducted by Sperling in the field of visual sensory memory.

Sperling conducted experiments on An experiment was conducted by Sperling in the field of visual sensory memory. Levels of category Basic Level Category: Subordinate Category: Superordinate Category: Stages of development of Piaget 1. Sensorimotor stage 0-2 2. Preoperational stage 2-7 3. Concrete operational stage

More information

Familiarity and Retrieval Processes in Delayed Judgments of Learning

Familiarity and Retrieval Processes in Delayed Judgments of Learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2008, Vol. 34, No. 5, 1084 1097 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0012580 Familiarity

More information

Consciousness Gleitman et al. (2011), Chapter 6, Part 1

Consciousness Gleitman et al. (2011), Chapter 6, Part 1 Consciousness Gleitman et al. (2011), Chapter 6, Part 1 Mike D Zmura Department of Cognitive Sciences, UCI Psych 9A / Psy Beh 11A March 11, 2014 T. M. D'Zmura 1 Consciousness Moment-by-moment awareness

More information

Epilepsy and Neuropsychology

Epilepsy and Neuropsychology Epilepsy and Neuropsychology Dr. Sare Akdag, RPsych Neuropsychology Service, BC Children s Hospital Clinical Assistant Professor, Dept of Paediatrics, UBC November 24, 2008 BC Epilepsy Society Lecture

More information

Neurobiology and Information Processing Theory: the science behind education

Neurobiology and Information Processing Theory: the science behind education Educational Psychology Professor Moos 4 December, 2008 Neurobiology and Information Processing Theory: the science behind education If you were to ask a fifth grader why he goes to school everyday, he

More information

Advanced Placement Psychology Syllabus Rolesville High School, Fall

Advanced Placement Psychology Syllabus Rolesville High School, Fall Instructor Information Julie P. Telenko Email: jtelenko@wcpss.net Website: rhsjtelenko.weebly.com Twitter: @jtelenkorhs Advanced Placement Psychology Syllabus Rolesville High School, Fall 2016-17 Course

More information

BACK TO THE FUTURE AND FORWARD TO THE PAST: EXPLORING RETROSPECTIVE BIASES AND CULTURE

BACK TO THE FUTURE AND FORWARD TO THE PAST: EXPLORING RETROSPECTIVE BIASES AND CULTURE BACK TO THE FUTURE AND FORWARD TO THE PAST: EXPLORING RETROSPECTIVE BIASES AND CULTURE Therese A. Louie, Lucas College and Graduate School of Business, San José State University, One Washington Square,

More information

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Sarah J. Miles (smiles25@uwo.ca) and John Paul Minda (jpminda@uwo.ca) Department of Psychology The University of Western Ontario London, ON N6A 5C2 Abstract

More information

What is Psychology? chapter 1

What is Psychology? chapter 1 What is Psychology? chapter 1 Overview! The science of psychology! What psychologists do! Critical and scientific thinking! Correlational studies! The experiment! Evaluating findings What is psychology?

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Running head: THE ENACTMENT EFFECT AND JUDGMENTS OF LEARNING 1. The Enactment Effect and Judgments of Learning: Action Memory Meets Metamemory

Running head: THE ENACTMENT EFFECT AND JUDGMENTS OF LEARNING 1. The Enactment Effect and Judgments of Learning: Action Memory Meets Metamemory Running head: THE ENACTMENT EFFECT AND JUDGMENTS OF LEARNING 1 The Enactment Effect and Judgments of Learning: Action Memory Meets Metamemory Andy Jin University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Spring

More information

Chapter 11 Nonexperimental Quantitative Research Steps in Nonexperimental Research

Chapter 11 Nonexperimental Quantitative Research Steps in Nonexperimental Research Chapter 11 Nonexperimental Quantitative Research (Reminder: Don t forget to utilize the concept maps and study questions as you study this and the other chapters.) Nonexperimental research is needed because

More information

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing? Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC How are we doing? Prepared by Maura McCool, M.S. Office of Research, Evaluation and Assessment Metropolitan Community Colleges Fall 2003 1 General Education Assessment

More information

ID + MD = OD Towards a Fundamental Algorithm for Consciousness. by Thomas McGrath. June 30, Abstract

ID + MD = OD Towards a Fundamental Algorithm for Consciousness. by Thomas McGrath. June 30, Abstract ID + MD = OD Towards a Fundamental Algorithm for Consciousness by Thomas McGrath June 30, 2018 Abstract The Algorithm described in this short paper is a simplified formal representation of consciousness

More information

Metamemory: An Update of Critical Findings

Metamemory: An Update of Critical Findings Metamemory: An Update of Critical Findings Bennett L Schwartz, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States Janet Metcalfe, Columbia University, New York, NY, United States Ó 2017 Elsevier

More information

24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037)

24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037) 24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037) Psychology is introduced as an elective subject at the higher secondary stage of school education. As a discipline, psychology specializes in the study of experiences, behaviours,

More information

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information:

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: EVALUATION OF THE MODAL MODEL OF MEMORY Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: bamponsah@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

Category structure modulates interleaving and blocking advantage in inductive category acquisition

Category structure modulates interleaving and blocking advantage in inductive category acquisition Category structure modulates interleaving and blocking advantage in inductive category acquisition Paulo F. Carvalho (pcarvalh@indiana.edu) Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, 1101 E 10th St

More information

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts Main Goals of this Paper: Professor JeeLoo Liu 1. To present an account of phenomenal

More information

Psychology 205, Revelle, Fall 2014 Research Methods in Psychology Mid-Term. Name:

Psychology 205, Revelle, Fall 2014 Research Methods in Psychology Mid-Term. Name: Name: 1. (2 points) What is the primary advantage of using the median instead of the mean as a measure of central tendency? It is less affected by outliers. 2. (2 points) Why is counterbalancing important

More information

Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences

Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences Intelligence and reasoning are not one and the same Ira A. Noveck and Jérôme Prado Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences Cognitives, CNRS-Université de Lyon,

More information

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Eiichi Hoshino 1,2, Ken Mogi 2, 1 Tokyo Institute of Technology, Department of Computational Intelligence and Systems Science. 4259

More information

Exam 2 PS 306, Spring 2004

Exam 2 PS 306, Spring 2004 Exam 2 PS 306, Spring 2004 1. Briefly define the term confound. Then, using a very explicit example of practice effects (maybe even with numbers?), illustrate why conducting a repeated measures experiment

More information

Memory and learning at school

Memory and learning at school Memory and learning at school Andrea Greve and Duncan Astle MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit The brain, cognition and learning Debunking a few myths. What do you know about the brain? If you ask someone

More information

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention Chapter 6 Attention Attention William James, in 1890, wrote Everyone knows what attention is. Attention is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously

More information

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information:

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: bamponsah@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education 2014/2015 2016/2017 Session Overview

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Design of the Continuous Knowledge Self-Assessment. Thomas R. O Neill, Ph.D. Michael R. Peabody, Ph.D. Aimee R. Eden, Ph.D.

Design of the Continuous Knowledge Self-Assessment. Thomas R. O Neill, Ph.D. Michael R. Peabody, Ph.D. Aimee R. Eden, Ph.D. Design of the Continuous Knowledge Self-Assessment Thomas R. O Neill, Ph.D. Michael R. Peabody, Ph.D. Aimee R. Eden, Ph.D. Disclosure All three presenters are salaried employees of ABFM PURPOSE Purpose

More information

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about?

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about? 2 Asking and answering research questions What s it about? (Social Psychology pp. 24 54) Social psychologists strive to reach general conclusions by developing scientific theories about why people behave

More information

CHAPTER 8 EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN

CHAPTER 8 EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN CHAPTER 8 1 EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN LEARNING OBJECTIVES 2 Define confounding variable, and describe how confounding variables are related to internal validity Describe the posttest-only design and the pretestposttest

More information

Visualizing Success: Investigating the Relationship between Ability and Self-Efficacy in the Domain of Visual Processing

Visualizing Success: Investigating the Relationship between Ability and Self-Efficacy in the Domain of Visual Processing Visualizing Success: Investigating the Relationship between Ability and Self-Efficacy in the Domain of Visual Processing Jason Power, Jeffrey Buckley and Niall Seery Department of Design and Manufacturing

More information

2 Psychological Processes : An Introduction

2 Psychological Processes : An Introduction 2 Psychological Processes : An Introduction 2.1 Introduction In our everyday life we try to achieve various goals through different activities, receive information from our environment, learn about many

More information

MINDFULNESS & EDUCATION. Davis Behavioral Health

MINDFULNESS & EDUCATION. Davis Behavioral Health MINDFULNESS & EDUCATION Davis Behavioral Health WELCOME & INTRODUCTIONS ØName ØSchool & position ØWhat brings you here? ØAny previous knowledge of mindfulness, or participation in mindfulness practice?

More information

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism [Slide 1]: Welcome Welcome to the New Mexico TEAM technical assistance module on making eligibility determinations under the category of autism. This module will review the guidance of the NM TEAM section

More information

Behavioral Biases in Underwriting: Implications for Insurers

Behavioral Biases in Underwriting: Implications for Insurers Behavioral Biases in Underwriting: Implications for Insurers Jiten Voralia Swiss Re This presentation has been prepared for the Actuaries Institute 2015 ASTIN and AFIR/ERM Colloquium. The Institute Council

More information

Unit Three: Behavior and Cognition. Marshall High School Mr. Cline Psychology Unit Three AE

Unit Three: Behavior and Cognition. Marshall High School Mr. Cline Psychology Unit Three AE Unit Three: Behavior and Cognition Marshall High School Mr. Cline Psychology Unit Three AE In 1994, two American scholars published a best-selling, controversial book called The Bell Curve. * Intelligence

More information

Memory Development. Cognitive Development

Memory Development. Cognitive Development Memory Development Cognitive Development Memory as information storage Memory Why does our memory sometimes fail us? Memory Schachter s Seven Sins of Memory 1. Transience 2. Absent-Mindedness 3. Blocking

More information

AP Psychology. Course Audit

AP Psychology. Course Audit Ledyard High School 24 Gallup Hill Road Ledyard CT. 06339 AP Psychology Course Audit Daniel Mello Eric Van Frachen 1 AP Psychology Syllabus: The purpose of AP Psychology is to introduce students to the

More information

MEMORY STORAGE. There are three major kinds of storage:

MEMORY STORAGE. There are three major kinds of storage: MEMORY Jill Price was capable of remembering everything that happened last year and several years ago. Memory is the ability to store and retrieve information over time. Memories are the residue of those

More information

YOUR NEW MONEY STORY ROADMAP FOR MONEY MASTERY

YOUR NEW MONEY STORY ROADMAP FOR MONEY MASTERY YOUR NEW MONEY STORY ROADMAP FOR MONEY MASTERY A Self-Guided Online-Retreat with David Krueger MD Seminar Series Outline I. Your Money Story David Krueger MD Seminar I. YOUR MONEY STORY [Workbook: Introduction

More information