The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh: Quantitative Genetic Variation in Females that Mate Late Relative to Sexual Maturation

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh: Quantitative Genetic Variation in Females that Mate Late Relative to Sexual Maturation"

Transcription

1 vol. 169, no. 3 the american naturalist march 2007 The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh: Quantitative Genetic Variation in Females that Mate Late Relative to Sexual Maturation Patricia J. Moore, 1,* W. Edwin Harris, 2, and Allen J. Moore 1, 1. School of Biosciences, University of Exeter, Cornwall Campus, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9EZ, United Kingdom; 2. Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PT, United Kingdom Submitted June 12, 2006; Accepted September 26, 2006; Electronically published January 22, 2007 abstract: In many species, females abandon mate choice to ensure that eggs are fertilized before they are lost. But why do females not just maintain oocytes longer if there is a benefit to mate choice? We conducted a quantitative genetic study in the cockroach Nauphoeta cinerea to test whether genetic constraints prevent the evolution of oocyte maintenance or selection against oocyte loss is weak when females mate late relative to sexual maturity. We found standing genetic variation within the population and no evidence for genetic constraints. Levels of genetic variation are of the magnitude found for life-history traits in general, suggesting that this trait has been exposed to selection. We unexpectedly found two categories of females: those that delay reproduction and those that reproduce at a normal time when mating late, which could indicate alternative strategies. However, frequency-dependent selection does not maintain this variation as females that delay always reproduce less well. Given these findings, we suggest that there may be advantages to egg degradation. The evolution of maintenance of fertilizable oocytes over time would then be constrained by the need to maintain the mechanism by which females control the distribution of resources between current and future reproductive events. Keywords: delayed reproduction, genetic constraints, life history, Nauphoeta cinerea, oocyte maintenance, threshold character. A core assumption of life-history theory is that reproduction is costly (Roff 1992; Stearns 1992). Organisms face * Corresponding author; p.j.moore@exeter.ac.uk. ed.harris@manchester.ac.uk. a.j.moore@exeter.ac.uk. Am. Nat Vol. 169, pp by The University of Chicago /2007/ $ All rights reserved. trade-offs in which different physiological processes compete for limited resources (Gustafsson et al. 1994), and the cost of reproduction is partly determined by the trade-off in resource allocation between gametes and somatic cells. Some resource allocation decisions are developmental, with allocation to germ or soma occurring before an individual reaches adulthood (Jervis et al. 2005). There also can be phenotypic plasticity in adult allocation, and individuals can respond to changes in environmental conditions (Kaitala et al. 1997). Females can delay reproduction and oocyte maturation under suboptimal conditions and wait for more favorable conditions (e.g., Byers et al. 2005). Nonetheless, given that producing oocytes comes at the expense of other physiological functions, once oocytes have been produced, females need to use them rather than risk losing that investment. In addition to decisions regarding allocation of resources, reproduction requires coordination between reproductive physiology and social conditions. Both highquality oocytes and the sperm to fertilize them are required for reproduction. Thus, females need to coordinate sexual maturation and investment in oocytes with availability of acceptable mates (Tsitrone et al. 2003a, 2003b). In species where females mate nonrandomly, females may be required to delay mating if high-quality mates are not encountered when females reach sexual maturity (Andersson 1994). Exercising female mate choice poses a potential cost, however, because eggs ready for fertilization might deteriorate while the females are searching for acceptable males (Jennions and Petrie 1997). If there is a cost to a mistiming of sexual maturation and mating and timing of maturation is canalized, females are predicted to respond to the risk of lost fertility by reducing their choosiness (Moore and Moore 2001). In unmated females with mature reproductive systems (or mature eggs), behavioral responses to avoid a delay in availability of sperm are observed. Thus, females have been shown to become less choosy when gravid in midwife toads (Lea et al. 2000) and with age in crickets, guppies, and cockroaches (Gray 1999; Kodric-

2 312 The American Naturalist Brown and Nicoletto 2001; Moore and Moore 2001). When faced with the loss of expensive oocytes, any male may be better than no male at all. Oocytes, once produced, degenerate with time (Fissore et al. 2002). Thus, females are under time pressure to fertilize oocytes once they are mature. But why must oocytes be lost over time? Why are females unable to maintain mature oocytes? Mature sperm can be stored for long periods of time in both the male and female reproductive tract (Birkhead and Møller 1993; Simmons 2001), so it is reasonable to suppose that similar storage functions could evolve for oocytes. If mate choice is adaptive, why abandon this behavioral strategy rather than evolving a mechanism to preserve eggs longer? We investigated the question as to why mature eggs are not maintained in the cockroach Nauphoeta cinerea. This hemimetabolous insect is ovoviviparous; although no nutrients are passed to developing offspring, females brood eggs internally and give live birth (Roth and Willis 1954). Thus, there is a relatively large maternal investment in N. cinerea. In contrast, males make only genetic contributions to their offspring (Moore and Moore 1988; Moore 1994). This asymmetry in parental investment is expected to result in sexual selection (Trivers 1972), and adaptive female mate choice has been documented in N. cinerea (reviewed in Moore 1990; Moore and Pizzari 2005). However, females become less choosy as fertility degrades (Moore and Moore 2001). In insects, reproductive behavior is controlled through interactions between the brain, the corpora allata, and the ovaries and is mediated by juvenile hormone (Roth 1964a, 1964b; Buschor et al. 1984). In N. cinerea there is a period of sexual maturation following eclosion to the adult stage. Newly emerged and young adult females are not receptive to male courtship and will not mate, and only low levels of juvenile hormone are released leading to gradual oocyte maturation and yolk deposition. Once sexually mature, females respond to male courtship and will copulate. Reproduction in N. cinerea consists of cycles of oocyte maturation, pregnancy, and parturition, a cycle that normally lasts about 45 days (Moore 1994). Females that mate past the time when they first become sexually receptive have reduced fertility (Moore and Moore 2001). Both the number of clutches and the size of clutches are reduced. The reduction in the number of offspring produced when females mate late relative to sexual maturation is due to a reduction in the number of oocytes fertilized and not to embryo death in utero (Moore and Harris 2003). We have shown that the loss of fertility as females delay mating is correlated to a loss of oocytes via apoptosis (Moore and Sharma 2005). However, apoptosis not only removes mature follicles ready for fertilization but also removes immature oocytes that will be utilized in future reproductive cycles. Thus, the time at which a female mates, relative to sexual maturation, has long-term consequences for lifetime reproductive success. During the course of our experiments documenting the cost of mating late relative to sexual maturation (Moore and Moore 2001), we observed substantial variation in the reproductive potential of females that mate late. Some females are able to cope well with a delay in mating relative to sexual maturity, while others perform very poorly. Given this variation in the ability of females to utilize oocytes over time, we investigated the potential for oocytes to be maintained over time. Two main explanations for a lack of evolution are genetic constraints and a lack of selection. A genetic constraint on evolution will arise even in the presence of selection if oocytes are not maintained, because there is no additive genetic variance underlying the observed differences in fertility following sexual maturation (no evolvability). There also may be genetic constraints because of trade-offs arising from pleiotropy or linkage disequilibrium among life-history traits that constrain the evolutionary response to maintaining oocytes (Merilä and Sheldon 1999). This would be reflected in a pattern of strong genetic correlations or covariances (Arnold 1992). Alternatively, genetic variation may be hidden to selection if females never experience the environment where loss of oocytes is expressed, and loss of oocytes is genetically robust (i.e., genetic variation has relatively small phenotypic effects) under typical environmental conditions (de Visser et al. 2003; Hermisson and Wagner 2004). This would be indicated by higher than normal genetic variation under the unusual environment (Hoffmann and Merilä 1999; de Visser et al. 2003; Hermisson and Wagner 2004). We therefore sought to examine how these alternatives might affect the N. cinerea system by undertaking a quantitative genetic study designed to examine the levels of genetic variation in fertility-related traits in females that mate late relative to sexual maturation. In the course of the experiment, however, we uncovered unexpected but obvious differences among females. While the design of the experiment, mating females at 18 days after adult eclosion, is the same as in our other studies in this area (Moore and Moore 2001; Moore and Harris 2003; Moore and Sharma 2005), the large sample sizes required by the half-sib breeding design allowed us to detect a previously undocumented pattern in female response to this treatment. It became obvious that there was a bimodal distribution of the time to parturition of the first clutch of offspring, suggesting two categories of females. We have never seen this pattern in females that mate at sexual maturity, where time to parturition of the first clutch is unimodal and normally distributed. This presented a third possibility: for many genetically determined dichotomous (threshold) traits, a

3 The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh 313 trade-off of fitness associated with different phenotypes maintains variation (Roff 1996; Hazel et al. 2004). Thus, following our analysis of genetic variation, we looked for differences in components of fitness between females with early or delayed parturition of the first clutch as well as the genetic basis for the expression of this threshold trait. Methods Animal Husbandry Animals used to generate our quantitative genetic breeding design were collected from eight outbred, mixed-sex, massrearing colonies housed in standard rearing conditions with a 12L : 12D photoperiod at 27 C and ad lib. access to rodent chow and water. These colonies contained several thousand individuals, and there was extensive enforced gene flow among the colonies. The colonies have been in culture for more than 50 years. This represents approximately 150 generations, so the colonies were likely to be at genetic equilibrium for our experiment. These cockroaches breed continuously throughout the year and have no period of reproductive diapause. Late-instar nymphs were separated by sex and housed under standard rearing conditions in 30 # 18 # 12-cm plastic boxes. Newly emerged virgin adults were collected daily as tenerals from the single-sex colonies for use as sires and dams in a halfsib breeding design. Sexual maturity (and therefore receptivity to mating) at 27 C is not reached until 4 to 7 days of age for females (Roth 1964a). Males begin to be able to produce spermataphores when as young as 2 days of age and are fully competent at 6 7 days of age (Roth 1964a) which parallels the development of their attractiveness to females and pheromone development (Moore et al. 1995). Breeding Design We used a standard paternal half-sib breeding design (dams nested within sires; Lynch and Walsh 1998) to estimate additive and nonadditive genetic plus common environment contributions to variation in reproduction in females where mating is delayed relative to sexual maturation. Fifty virgin adult males (9 12 days after adult eclosion) were mated to four unrelated virgin adult females (6 9 days after adult eclosion) sequentially over 3 days. Immediately after mating, the females were placed in individual 11 # 11 # 3-cm plastic boxes, provided with ad lib. rodent chow and water, and housed in incubators in standard rearing conditions. Females were checked daily for parturition of their first clutch. Following birth of the clutch, offspring were counted. To control for density effects, 20 offspring from each clutch were placed into a 17 # 12 # 5-cm plastic box, provided with ad lib. rodent chow and water, and housed under standard rearing conditions until adults began to emerge. Clutches were checked daily for newly emerged adults. Four adult females were collected from each clutch for analysis of fecundity when forced to mate late relative to development of sexual maturity. At 18 days after adult eclosion, each adult daughter was mated to a 10-day-old virgin male, isolated from the mass colonies as above. Following mating, we placed these females into 11 # 11 # 3-cm plastic boxes with ad lib. food and water. All females were housed in individual boxes in a walk-in incubator at 27 C. We checked females daily for the presence of newborn offspring. For each clutch of offspring produced, we recorded the date and number of offspring born. We removed all of the offspring, and the female was returned to her individual box until birth of the next clutch, when this procedure was repeated. Observations continued until the death of the female. Life-History Traits We analyzed two life-history traits, longevity and fecundity, in the daughters produced from our breeding design. Because we measured all of the clutches produced by females, we also had information on the embryonic development time of the offspring of these daughters, providing a measure of offspring quality for different females. We did not measure female body size as a variable because in previous experiments we never found an influence of body size on fecundity (R. F. Preziosi, P. J. Moore, and A. J. Moore, unpublished data). Fecundity. We examined the fecundity of females when mating is delayed relative to sexual maturation by analyzing the number of offspring produced in each of the first three clutches as a series of separate traits (first clutch, second clutch, etc.). We also scored the total numbers of clutches a female produced over her lifetime and the total number of offspring produced in all clutches combined. Life span. We measured the total life span of females that delayed mating relative to sexual maturation from adult eclosion to death. Offspring development. The development time of the first clutch was measured as the number of days from mating to parturition of the first clutch. The development time of subsequent clutches was measured as the number of days between parturition of each clutch. Statistical Analyses Data from all characters we measured were normally distributed. It was not possible to transform time to first parturition to normality (see Results ). While a few fe-

4 314 The American Naturalist males in the study produced as many as nine clutches over their lifetime, clutch size is reported only for clutches 1 3 (for genetic analyses) and 1 6 (for phenotypic analyses of fitness differences) because the number of females producing clutches declined rapidly, precluding meaningful analyses. The total number of offspring produced, however, includes those from all clutches. Analysis of genetic variation. To analyze genetic variation in fecundity-related traits, we fitted a standard linear mixed-effects ANOVA using restricted maximum likelihood (REML) with dam nested in sire to estimate sire and dam variance and covariance components using the varcomp function in S-PLUS, version 6.2 (Pinheiro and Bates 2000). Statistical significance of additive and dam variances was determined by F-tests with Satterthwaitecorrected degrees of freedom (Lynch and Walsh 1998). We calculated narrow-sense and broad-sense heritabilities from the sire and dam variances estimated by REML. We used weighted family means to estimate the standard error of heritability estimates (Lynch and Walsh 1998). We calculated coefficients of variation for the traits we measured to provide standardized estimates of phenotypic, additive genetic, and residual variation to facilitate direct comparison of traits with different scales of measure (Houle 1992). We estimated additive genetic correlations among all characters using covariances from a REML analysis. Standard error for genetic correlations was calculated in S-PLUS using the jackknife method applied to pseudovalues (Potvin and Roff 1993; Roff and Preziosi 1994; Roff 2006). Analysis of fitness trade-offs. Initial examination of the distribution of development times of the first clutch of offspring led us to divide the females into two populations based on the number of days between mating and parturition of the first clutch (see Results ; fig. 1). In Nauphoeta cinerea, over 90% of females mated concurrently to development of sexual maturity produce their first clutch within 34 to 55 days after mating (Roth 1964b; Moore 1994; P. Moore, unpublished data). This includes a 7-day period of oocyte maturation and a day period of embryonic development. After the first clutch is produced, subsequent clutches are born about 50 days apart (Roth 1964b). Therefore, in this study, normal reproductive timing was defined as between 40 and 60 days postmating, and delayed reproductive timing was defined as parturition of the first clutch 60 days or more postmating. We used ANOVA or t-tests (Sokal and Rolf 1995) to examine differences in both developmental timing and fecundity traits between categories of females (normal or delayed developmental timing). The strength of pairwise relationships between variables was measured using Pearson product-moment correlations. The hypothesis that Figure 1: Frequency distribution of development of first clutch of offspring for females that mate late relative to sexual maturation. Females fell into one of two categories, based on the development time of their first clutch of offspring. One group of females produced a first clutch with normal timing, within 60 days of mating (hatched bars). The other group of females produced a first clutch with delayed timing, taking more than 60 days to parturition (solid bars). there was no difference between the survival curves for the two groups of females was tested using the log rank test. All means are reported with standard errors. Analyses were performed with JMP Professional, version 5.0.1a. Heritability of the threshold trait. In order to analyze whether there was an inherited component to the membership in the group of females that had normal timing of parturition of the first clutch of offspring or the group that had delayed timing, we analyzed the heritability of liability for delayed production of offspring. This method assumes that there is an underlying continuous trait, influenced by multiple additive genetic effects, and that, although there is a normal distribution of the underlying character, individuals that are above or below a threshold express a different trait. Each female was categorized as either normal (0) or delayed (1), based on the developmental timing of her first clutch of offspring (see below). We then used the method of Reich et al. (1972) to measure the heritability and associated standard error for the threshold of producing a delayed clutch, assuming that all affected (delayed) individuals were probands and applying the formula from Hill and Smith (Elston et al. 1977). Calculations of heritability on the underlying scale (Roff 1997), using REML ANOVA analysis on the half-sib data, with normal and delayed scored as 0 or 1 and the mean proportion of delayed reproduction (p) associated with sire classes, are also presented.

5 The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh 315 Table 1: ANOVA tables of nested half-sib/full-sib analyses for each trait Trait/source df MS F P Total no. offspring: Sire 41 5, Dam within sire 102 3, Error 322 2, No. clutches: Sire Dam within sire Error No. offspring, first clutch: Sire Dam within sire !.001 Error No. offspring, second clutch: Sire Dam within sire Error No. offspring, third clutch: Sire Dam within sire Error Life span: Sire 41 16, Dam within sire , Error ,432.8 Note: Significance was estimated using Satterthwaite-corrected degrees of freedom (Lynch and Walsh 1998). Results Genetic Variation We examined the patterns of genetic variation for fecundity traits in females that were prevented from mating until 18 days after adult eclosion. There was significant additive genetic (sire) variance for total number of offspring, number of clutches, number of offspring in the second clutch, and number of offspring in the third clutch (table 1). There were significant dam variances for total number of offspring, number of offspring in the first clutch, and number of offspring in the third clutch (table 1). The coefficients of additive genetic variation for all but life span suggest that there is scope for an evolutionary response to selection in fecundity-related traits (table 2). The narrow-sense heritability for the number of offspring in the first clutch was low, whereas other clutch sizes showed higher heritabilities (table 2). This reflected a higher phenotypic variance rather than lower additive genetic variance in the first clutch. The coefficients of additive genetic variance were of similar magnitude for the number of offspring in all three clutches. There was no measurable heritability for life span, Table 2: Description of phenotypic and genetic variation in life-history traits of females that mate late relative to sexual maturation Trait Mean V P V A V G h 2 (SE) H 2 (SE) CV A CV R Total no. offspring , (.15).20 (.14) No. clutches over a lifetime (.15).20 (.14) No. offspring, first clutch (.15).25 (.12) No. offspring, second clutch (.12).09 (.13) No. offspring, third clutch (.16).25 (.16) Life span (days) , , (.12).11 (.13) Note: All traits were analyzed using 42 sires, 145 dams, and 502 offspring. Additive genetic variance (V A ) was calculated as 4 # sire variance; genetic variance (V G ) was calculated as 2 # sire dam variance (Lynch and Walsh 1998). Significance of additive and dam variances is provided in table 1. V P is phenotypic variance; h 2 is the narrow-sense heritability; H 2 is the broad-sense heritability; CV A is the additive genetic coefficient of variation; CV R is the residual coefficient of variation (both CV A and CV R were calculated according to Houle 1992).

6 316 The American Naturalist Table 3: Phenotypic and additive genetic correlations for life-history traits of females that mate late relative to sexual maturation Total no. offspring No. clutches First clutch No. offspring Second clutch Third clutch Normal or delayed reproduction Life span Total no. offspring.88 (!.001).54 (!.001).60 (!.001).69 (!.001).29 (!.001).28 (!.001) No. clutches over a lifetime.93 (.02).35 (!.001).30 (!.001).31 (!.001).17 (!.001).51 (!.001) No. offspring, first clutch.16 (.53).17 (.37).29 (!.001).16 (!.001).05 (.337).07 (.151) No. offspring, second clutch.28 (.84).43 (.46).31 (.22).55 (!.001).24 (!.001).07 (.195) No. offspring, third clutch.93 (.03).67 (1.07).14 (.10).11 (.75).40 (!.001).09 (.105) Normal or delayed reproduction 1.26 (.10).86 (.13).18 (.79).29 (.19) 1.01 (.38).09 (.090) Note: Phenotypic correlations are presented on the upper diagonal, with significance in parentheses. Genetic correlations are presented on the lower diagonal, with SE in parentheses. Each r G value is based on restricted maximum likelihood analysis, with SE obtained by jackknife. No genetic correlations could be calculated between fecundity traits and life span, given the lack of genetic variance for life span. and the coefficient of additive genetic variance for life span was relatively low (table 1); we therefore did not consider the genetic aspects of life span further as a potential constraint on evolution of life histories in females that mate relatively late. Broad-sense and narrow-sense heritabilities were similar for all traits (table 2), indicating that there were few common environment or nonadditive effects. There were no obvious trade-offs among fitness components. Phenotypic correlations show expected relationships between fecundity-related traits (table 3). There were significant positive phenotypic correlations between all of these traits except life span and individual clutch sizes, which were not significantly different from zero. Very high correlations reflected the ovoviviparous reproduction of Nauphoeta cinerea females and the necessary relationship between number of clutches and overall fecundity. The phenotypic correlations between total number of offspring and number of offspring in each clutch also were moderately high. However, there was a weaker (although still strong) relationship between the numbers of offspring in each clutch. There were also significant phenotypic correlations between numbers of offspring in individual clutches and total number of clutches produced over a life span. There was no evidence of any genetic constraints due to high negative genetic correlations among fecundity traits (table 3). Two pairs of traits were virtually genetically identical: number of clutches over a lifetime and total number of offspring, and number of offspring in the third clutch and total number of offspring. There was no overwhelming or obvious pattern of covariation. There was a mix of positive and negative genetic correlations, although the positive correlations were all higher than the negative correlations. While there was a positive genetic correlation between the number of offspring in the first and second clutch, most genetic correlations with the size of the first clutch were negative and small. Trade-Offs among Alternative Responses to Mating Late Relative to Sexual Maturation Mating late relative to development of sexual maturity had an obvious effect on schedule of reproduction for some females, with a bimodal distribution for timing of parturition of their first clutch of offspring (fig. 1). Among the daughters mated at 18 days after adult eclosion, 50.7% produced a first clutch within 60 days from mating and thus had normal reproductive timing, based on our previous experience mating females at 7 days after adult eclosion. The mean time to parturition of the first clutch within this category was days. Parturition occurred between 33 and 60 days after mating. Among the offspring mated at 18 days after adult eclosion, 42.1% had a delayed first reproductive event, with the parturition of the first clutch more than 60 days after mating. The mean development time of this category was days, with the range between 61 and 154 days. This effect was seen only in the first clutch, as development times of offspring born after the first reproductive event were not significantly different between females with normal timing of the first reproductive event and females with delayed timing of the first reproductive event (table 4). The category into which a female fell had significant fitness consequences, but there were no trade-offs between the groups. Females that produced a first clutch with normal timing had more offspring over their lifetime than females that had a delayed first parturition (fig. 2A). Females with normal first parturition had an average of

7 The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh 317 Table 4: Mean development time ( 1 SE) of offspring in females with normal or delayed production of the first clutch with test of differences between means Clutch no., timing of first clutch Mean days to parturition Sample size t statistic from t-test Probability Normal Delayed Normal Delayed Normal Delayed Normal Delayed Normal Delayed Note: Development time of clutches is measured as the number of days between parturition of the clutches offspring over a lifetime, whereas females with delayed first parturition had an average of offspring ( F p 36.01, df p 1, 405, P!.0001). There was no difference in the number of offspring in the first clutch between females with normal first parturition and those with delayed first parturition (table 5). Instead, differences in lifetime reproductive success reflect decreased numbers of offspring in subsequent clutches (fig. 2B; table 5) and number of clutches produced. The pattern of fertility is similar in the two categories, with a rise in offspring numbers between the first and second clutch and then a reduction in clutch size over time. However, the increase in clutch size between the first and second clutch is smaller in the females that had a delayed first clutch compared with females with normal timing of the first clutch, and clutch size drops off more rapidly between the second and third clutch. Females with normal timing of first parturition also had more clutches over their lifetime (F p 11.89, df p 1, 405, P p.001). The mean number of clutches for females with normal reproductive timing of the first clutch was , and for females with delayed reproductive timing, the mean number of clutches was In females that had normal timing of first parturition, there was no significant correlation between the number of offspring in the first clutch and the number of clutches produced over a lifetime ( r p 0.03, P p.657, N p 236). However, in females that had a delayed first parturition, there was a significant positive correlation between the number of offspring in the first clutch and the number of clutches produced over a lifetime ( r p 0.28, P p.0001, N p 196). There was no trade-off between decreased reproduction and survival between the two categories, as decreased reproductive output did not result in longer life span in females that delayed production of their first clutch (fig. 3). There was no significant difference in the survival curves between females that had normal timing of first parturition and those that had delayed first parturition 2 ( x p 1.529, P p.216). There was no difference in survivorship between the two classes of females, even when we censored the data at 300 or 400 days (data not shown). Females that had a normal first parturition lived an average of days, and females that had a delayed first par- turition lived an average of days. Given the length of time between clutches, this difference would not translate into more offspring. Moreover, the pattern of correlation between life span and clutch size was the same for both categories. We found negative correlations between the average numbers of offspring per clutch and life span for both females with normal first parturition ( r p 0.28, P!.001, N p 226) and those with delayed first parturition ( r p 0.20, P p.006, N p 192). Is Membership Random in the Two Categories of Reproductive Timing? Given the fitness costs of delaying the production of the first clutch, why do all females not produce offspring normally? As the distribution of timing of first parturition suggests two discrete categories of females, we analyzed our timing of first parturition as a threshold trait. We found a clear genetic influence on category membership. The heritability for the threshold for delayed production

8 318 The American Naturalist Discussion Figure 2: Mean number of offspring ( SE) born to females that produce a first clutch within normal timing, and females that delay production of a first clutch. A, Females that produced a first clutch with normal developmental time (dashed line) accumulated a greater mean number of offspring over their lifetime compared with females that had delayed parturition of their first clutch (solid line). B, While there was no difference in numbers of offspring in the first clutch between females with normal parturition (hatched bars) and delayed parturition (solid bars) of the first clutch, there were significantly fewer offspring in the subsequent clutches of offspring in the females that had delayed parturition. Bars represent standard errors. of a first clutch within the first reproductive cycle was 0.80 ( SE p 0.22). Heritability on the underlying scale was 0.72 (0.16). Genetic correlations between category of reproduction and overall reproduction supports the phenotypic analyses (table 3). Total number of offspring and number of clutches have a high (approximately 1) negative genetic correlation with category (normal, scored 0, or delayed, scored 1). However, the genetic correlations between reproduction in first or second clutches and category are much smaller. Thus, the genes that influence membership in a category are the same or linked with total fecundity and number of clutches, as they must be, but less strongly linked to genes influencing fecundity in individual clutches. There is no evidence that females that delay mating are genetically predisposed to producing larger clutches; in fact, the opposite appears to be true. Females are expected to have evolved adaptations that allow them to minimize the costs that might arise when, for instance, they have fertilizable eggs that will degenerate over time but no sperm with which to fertilize them. Evolution requires both selection and the presence of heritable variation in the selected trait (Lynch and Walsh 1998). In this study, we examined the evolutionary potential of maintaining fertility after sexual maturation by examining the pattern of genetic variation underlying fecundity of females that mate late relative to sexual maturation in Nauphoeta cinerea. We found that fecundity has the potential to evolve because the necessary standing genetic variation exists within the population. There were no genetic trade-offs among the life-history traits we measured. However, there was also no evidence that fecundity following mating late relative to sexual maturation has been protected from selection, because the pattern of additive genetic variation is similar to other life-history traits (Mousseau and Roff 1987; Merilä and Sheldon 1999). Unexpectedly, we found that females had one of two separate responses to mating late relative to sexual reproduction ability, and membership in these categories had a strong genetic component. Again, however, there were no tradeoffs in fitness between these categories, providing necessary frequency-dependent advantages to maintain a threshold trait. Thus, we need to ask why females have not evolved the ability to maintain fertility over time. Genetic Variation in the Ability to Cope with Mating Late Relative to Sexual Maturation One potential explanation to the question of why the ability to maintain high-quality oocytes following maturation has not evolved are genetic constraints arising from the lack of genetic variation in the control of the onset of apoptosis following maturation or constraints imposed by genetic correlations between fitness traits. Our results allow us to reject this hypothesis. There was a significant contribution of additive genetic variance to both the number of clutches produced and the number of total offspring. Indeed, the genetic correlation between these two traits indicates that, genetically, they are essentially the same trait. In addition, negative genetic correlations between size of the first clutch and other fitness traits were of small size. Genetic correlations between type of reproduction (delayed or normal) and individual clutch size suggest no trade-off between clutch size and delaying reproduction. Thus, there is additive genetic variance in fertility and no obvious genetic trade-offs when females are mated at 18 days after adult eclosion. Thus, fertility following mating late has the genetic potential to evolve.

9 The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh 319 Table 5: Mean number of offspring ( 1 SE) born to females with normal or delayed production of the first clutch with test of differences between means Clutch no., timing of first clutch Mean no. offspring Sample size t statistic from t-test Probability Normal Delayed !.001 Normal Delayed !.001 Normal Delayed !.001 Normal Delayed !.001 Normal Delayed !.002 Normal Delayed Our study measures only a small number of life-history traits, and therefore it is possible that there are genetic constraints we did not measure. For instance, we did not measure body size in our offspring because body size has never been a significant variable in our previous studies. However, the patterns we see are similar to those in other studies of the genetics of life history (Mousseau and Roff 1987; Merilä and Sheldon 1999, 2000). Empirically, there are fewer genetic constraints on life-history evolution than might be predicted by theory (Sheldon et al. 2003; Charmantier et al. 2006). Our study fits well within this general finding. Given the severe reduction in lifetime reproductive success in females that coped poorly with mating late, we might expect strong selection to eliminate variation in response to postponed mating from the population. One possibility is that females cannot maintain mature oocytes because although the genetic variation available for adaptation is available in the population, it is protected from selection (Merilä and Sheldon 1999). Under rare unfavorable conditions, selection on alleles resulting in decreased fitness will be much less effective (Hoffmann and Merilä 1999). This could occur for N. cinerea because the variation in fecundity that we measured would be expressed only in rare social environments where males are infrequently encountered, because the behavioral adaptations lead to females accepting less preferable mates in order to acquire sperm. We cannot measure selection directly, but we can look for signatures of selection in our genetic analysis. If it were the case that fecundity following mating late has not experienced selection, we would expect to see higher levels of genetic variation as would be anticipated for protected traits. Under the hypothesis that selection can only occur under rare conditions, fecundity variation should be genetically robust, and we should see increased genetic variation under the novel environment (de Visser Figure 3: Survival curves for females that produced a first clutch with normal reproductive timing (gray line, N p 225) and females that had delayed parturition of their first clutch (black line, N p 170). There was no significant difference between the two curves.

10 320 The American Naturalist et al. 2003; Hermisson and Wagner 2004). The pattern of genetic variation we see, however, allows us to reject the lack of selection hypothesis. The levels and pattern of genetic variation we see are similar to those seen for fecundity of females mated when sexually mature (e.g., lifetime 2 reproduction h p 0.19, number of clutches p 0.20, average clutch size p 0.11; Moore 1994) as well as of the level expected for life-history traits in general (Mousseau and Roff 1997; Merilä and Sheldon 1999). The relatively low (but nonzero) levels of additive genetic variation we found in fecundity suggest that the fitness cost of mating late relative to sexual maturity has resulted in selection. Fitness Performance following Mating Late Relative to Sexual Maturation The lifetime reproductive success of females that mate late relative to sexual maturation fell into two categories. One category of females produced a first clutch within a normal reproductive cycle and had a reproductive output similar to females that mate soon after reaching sexual maturity. The second category, however, had a delay in parturition of their first clutch. A bimodal response might indicate one group of females entering reproductive diapause. However, N. cinerea reproduce continually throughout the year and no diapause has ever been observed by us or reported in this species. The lifetime reproductive success of females in this second category was reduced both through missing a reproductive cycle and by having fewer offspring in the subsequent cycles. Thus, there was significant phenotypic variation among females in their ability to cope with the mistiming of mating relative to sexual maturation. There was significant heritable variation in the category that was expressed by a female. However, there is no evidence of fitness trade-offs maintaining alternative conditional strategies. The females who reproduced normally reproduced best, while females with delayed reproduction had severely compromised reproductive success. Females that start out with poor fecundity are unable to recover in successive reproductive cycles. Additionally, females that delayed reproduction did not accrue other types of benefits, such as longer life span. Thus, there is no evidence of frequency-dependent advantages that can lead to adaptive categorical traits (Roff 1996; Hazel et al. 2004). Might There Be a Cost of Maintaining Mature Oocytes? We have not discovered why oocytes are not maintained, but we can suggest a hypothesis. It may be that females have not evolved the ability to store mature oocytes simply because it is better that they do not. Females that have mature oocytes ready to fertilize under conditions where opportunities for reproduction are limited, such as when sperm are not available, face two choices. They could either maintain their oocytes while they wait for a better reproductive opportunity or resorb the nutrients invested in the current oocytes, recycling them into future reproduction. Interestingly, a similar bimodal response was seen in the seed beetle Callosobruchus maculatus when females were deprived of seeds on which to lay their eggs. One group of females dumped their eggs early while the other group retained their eggs until death (Messina and Slade 1999). Variation in oviposition rate in the absence of seeds is in part genetically based (Messina and Fry 2003). Thus, this may be a general response to a poor reproductive environment. The trade-off between current and future reproduction will depend on the ability of an individual to redirect resources invested in this reproductive event into future reproductive efforts (Tsitrone et al. 2003a, 2003b; Jervis et al. 2005). Animals have evolved a mechanism, oosorption, for recycling nutrients that are not oviposited, and we propose that this is a means by which resources are reallocated into survival when opportunities for reproduction are limited (Bell and Bohm 1975; Papaj 2000). Consider the category of females in our study that performed poorly and had reduced lifetime reproductive success when they mated late relative to sexual maturation. This might have reflected a more efficient or rapid switch to oosorption, which was highly costly in terms of lifetime fecundity. However, our experiment was conducted on females under good nutritional conditions. Perhaps females with loci leading to more efficient oosorption would be better able to cope with poor nutritional conditions or changes in food availability. Thus, the hidden genetic variation that was detrimental to fitness under these conditions would provide a benefit in another environmental context. We are exploring this hypothesis. Conclusion Given the ability of sperm to be stored in the male and female reproductive tracts, often for extreme lengths of time (Birkhead and Møller 1993; Simmons 2001), it is curious that oocytes seem so perishable. Even in species where females need to delay offspring development relative to mating, it is through delayed implantation of the embryo or delayed ovulation rather than maintenance of unfertilized, mature eggs (Weir and Rowlands 1973; Kaitala et al. 1997; Thom et al. 2004). The inability to store oocytes therefore requires an evolutionary explanation. We find no genetic constraint of the evolution of improved oocyte storage and no evidence of a lack of selection associated with the loss of current and future eggs. Females vary in how they respond to the mistiming of mating relative to

11 The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh 321 sexual maturation, falling into two genetically defined groups. There are no fitness trade-offs to belonging to one or the other group; those who delay reproduction always produce fewer offspring. This suggests that there must be some benefit to oosorption we have not measured. It is likely that this is dependent on nutritional environment. We therefore propose that although the genetic variance necessary for extending oocyte shelf life exists within the population and that there should be strong selection on increasing fecundity under certain social conditions, the evolution of this trait is likely to be constrained by complex trade-offs between resource allocation to current and future reproduction in response to changing nutritional environments. Acknowledgments We thank T. Montrose for expert technical assistance and R. Ward for help with setting up the experiment. E. Barrett, M. Edvardsson, J. Hunt, and T. Tregenza provided helpful comments. We are especially grateful for the outstandingly comprehensive and insightful reviews of F. Messina, D. Roff, and an anonymous reviewer. D. Roff provided considerable advice on statistical analyses. This work has been supported by grants from the Natural Environment Research Council and the Leverhulme Trust to P.J.M. and A.J.M. P.J.M. conceived the research idea; P.J.M., A.J.M., and W.E.H. designed the experiments; P.J.M. and W.E.H. collected the data; P.J.M., A.J.M., and W.E.H. conducted statistical analyses; P.J.M. and A.J.M. wrote the article. Literature Cited Andersson, M Sexual selection. Princeton University Press. Princeton, NJ. Arnold, S. J Constraints on phenotypic evolution. American Naturalist 140(suppl.):S85 S107. Bell, W. J., and M. K. Bohm Oösorption in insects. Biological Review 50: Birkhead, T. R., and A. P. Møller Sexual selection and the temporal separation of reproductive events: sperm storage data from reptiles, birds, and mammals. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 50: Buschor, J., P. Beyeler, and B. Lanzrein Factors responsible for the initiation of a second oocyte maturation cycle in the ovoviviparous cockroach Nauphoeta cinerea. Journal of Insect Physiology 30: Byers, J. A., P. A. Wiseman, L. Jones, and T. J. Roffe A large cost of female mate sampling in pronghorn. American Naturalist 166: Charmantier, A., C. Perrias, R. H. McCleery, and B. C. Sheldon Evolutionary response to selection on clutch size in a longterm study of the mute swan. American Naturalist 167: de Visser, J. A. G. M., J. Hermisson, G. P. Wagner, L. A. Meyers, H. Bagheri-Chaichian, J. L. Blanchard, L. Chao, et al Perspective: evolution and the detection of genetic robustness. Evolution 57: Elston, R. C., W. G. Hill, and C. Smith Query: estimating heritability of a dichotomous trait. Biometrics 33: Fissore, R. A., M. Kurokawa, J. Knott, M. Zhang, and J. Smyth Mechanisms underlying ooctye activation and postovulatory ageing. Reproduction 124: Gray, D. A Intrinsic factors affecting female mate choice in house crickets: time cost, female age, nutritional condition, body size and size-relative reproductive investment. Journal of Insect Behavior 12: Gustafsson, L., D. Nordling, M. S. Andersson, B. C. Sheldon, and A. Qvarnström Infectious diseases, reproductive effort and the cost of reproduction in birds. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 346: Hazel, W., R. Smock, and C. M. Lively The ecological genetics of conditional strategies. American Naturalist 163: Hermisson, J., and G. P. Wagner The population genetic theory of hidden variation and genetic robustness. Genetics 168: Hoffmann, A. A., and J. Merilä Heritable variation and evolution under favourable and unfavourable conditions. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 14: Houle, D Comparing evolvability and variability of quantitative traits. Genetics 130: Jennions, M. D., and M. Petrie Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences. Biological Reviews 72: Jervis, M. A., C. L. Boggs, and P. N. Ferns Egg maturation strategy and its associated trade-offs: a synthesis focusing on Lepidoptera. Ecological Entomology 30: Kaitala, V., T. Mappes, and H. Ylonen Delayed female reproduction in equilibrium and chaotic populations. Evolutionary Ecology 11: Kodric-Brown, A., and P. F. Nicoletto Age and experience affect female choice in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). American Naturalist 157: Lea, J., T. Halliday, and M. Dyson Reproductive stage and history affects the phonotactic preferences of female midwife toads, Alytes muetensis. Animal Behaviour 60: Lynch, M., and J. B. Walsh Genetics and analysis of quantitative traits. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA. Merilä, J., and B. C. Sheldon Genetic architecture of fitness and nonfitness traits: empirical patterns and development of ideas. Heredity 83: Lifetime reproductive success and heritability in nature. American Naturalist 155: Messina, F. J., and J. D. Fry Environment-dependent reversal of a life history trade-off in the seed beetle Callosobruchus maculatus. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16: Messina, F. J., and A. F. Slade Expression of a life-history trade-off in a seed beetle depends on environmental context. Physiological Entomology 24: Moore, A. J Sexual selection and the genetics of pheromonally mediated social behavior in Nauphoeta cinerea (Dictyoptera: Blaberidae). Entomologica Generalis 15: Genetic evidence for the good genes process of sexual selection. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 35: Moore, A. J., and P. J. Moore Female strategy during mate choice: threshold assessment. Evolution 42: Moore, A. J., and T. Pizzari Quantitative genetic models of

1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts)

1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts) Zoology 357 - Evolutionary Ecology - First Exam 1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts) b. Name two non-selectionist

More information

THE EVOLUTION OF INDICATOR TRAITS FOR PARENTAL QUALITY: THE ROLE OF MATERNAL AND PATERNAL EFFECTS

THE EVOLUTION OF INDICATOR TRAITS FOR PARENTAL QUALITY: THE ROLE OF MATERNAL AND PATERNAL EFFECTS Vol. 150, No. 5 The American Naturalist November 1997 THE EVOLUTION OF INDICATOR TRAITS FOR PARENTAL QUALITY: THE ROLE OF MATERNAL AND PATERNAL EFFECTS Jason B. Wolf, 1, *, Allen J. Moore, 2 and Edmund

More information

r = intrinsic rate of natural increase = the instantaneous rate of change in population size (per individual). If r > 0, then increasing

r = intrinsic rate of natural increase = the instantaneous rate of change in population size (per individual). If r > 0, then increasing Zoology 357 - Evolutionary Ecology - First Exam 1. (6 points) List three types of natural selection and show how the population changes over time (graph the initial phenotype frequency distribution and

More information

Variation in sperm size within and between ejaculates. in a cockroach. W. E. HARRIS*, A. J. MOORE and P. J. MOORE

Variation in sperm size within and between ejaculates. in a cockroach. W. E. HARRIS*, A. J. MOORE and P. J. MOORE Functional Ecology 2007 Variation in sperm size within and between ejaculates Blackwell Publishing Ltd in a cockroach W. E. HARRIS*, A. J. MOORE and P. J. MOORE *Faculty of Life Sciences, The University

More information

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 1 Outline causes sexual selection vs. natural selection sexual selection during courtship male-male

More information

Genetic variation in paternal investment in a seed beetle

Genetic variation in paternal investment in a seed beetle ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1998, 56, 953 961 Article No. ar980853 Genetic variation in paternal investment in a seed beetle UDO M. SAVALLI & CHARLES W. FOX The Louis Calder Center and Department of Biological Sciences,

More information

November 4, 2009 Bioe 109 Fall 2009 Lecture 17 The evolution of mating systems. The evolution of sex ratio

November 4, 2009 Bioe 109 Fall 2009 Lecture 17 The evolution of mating systems. The evolution of sex ratio November 4, 2009 Bioe 109 Fall 2009 Lecture 17 The evolution of mating systems The evolution of sex ratio - let us define sex ratio as the proportion of males to females. - in discussing the evolution

More information

Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety

Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 277 283 Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety Mark W. Blows, 1 * David Berrigan 2,3 and George W. Gilchrist 3 1 Department of Zoology,

More information

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences Males and females have the same genes. Why do the sexes often look and act so differently? Why is the male often insanely ornamented? (Or simply insane?)

More information

Plasticity in reproductive effort of male dung flies. (Scatophaga stercoraria) as a response to larval density. P. STOCKLEY and N. J.

Plasticity in reproductive effort of male dung flies. (Scatophaga stercoraria) as a response to larval density. P. STOCKLEY and N. J. Functional Ecology 2001 Plasticity in reproductive effort of male dung flies Blackwell Science, Ltd (Scatophaga stercoraria) as a response to larval density P. STOCKLEY and N. J. SEAL Population Biology

More information

Lecture Outline. Darwin s Theory of Natural Selection. Modern Theory of Natural Selection. Changes in frequencies of alleles

Lecture Outline. Darwin s Theory of Natural Selection. Modern Theory of Natural Selection. Changes in frequencies of alleles 1. Basics of Natural Selection Lecture Outline 2. How to test for the key components of natural selection a. Variation b. Heritability c. Can the trait respond to selection? d. What are the selective forces?

More information

Sexual selection. 1) Sexual dimorphism. 2) Variation in mating success. 3) Sexual selection. 4) Female choice based on male ornaments

Sexual selection. 1) Sexual dimorphism. 2) Variation in mating success. 3) Sexual selection. 4) Female choice based on male ornaments Sexual selection 1) Sexual dimorphism 2) Variation in mating success 3) Sexual selection 4) Female choice based on male ornaments 5) The evolution of female preference 1) Sexual dimorphism http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/file:descent_of_man_-_figure_16.jpg

More information

Adaptation and Optimality Theory

Adaptation and Optimality Theory Adaptation and Optimality Theory Prisoner s Dilemma game: An optimality model What is the optimal strategy when you are playing with different people of unknown reputation? Defect: We call this the evolutionarily

More information

Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection

Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection Why study mating systems? What are the ecological conditions under which different mating systems arise? Implications for evolution mating is where genes are passed

More information

Some observations. Some traits are difficult to view as adaptations, because they appear to provide a disadvantage to the organism

Some observations. Some traits are difficult to view as adaptations, because they appear to provide a disadvantage to the organism Some traits are difficult to view as adaptations, because they appear to provide a disadvantage to the organism Darwin asked: Can natural selection explain these differences? Structural traits: cumbersome

More information

Report. Paternal Indirect Genetic Effects on Offspring Viability and the Benefits of Polyandry

Report. Paternal Indirect Genetic Effects on Offspring Viability and the Benefits of Polyandry Current Biology 17, 32 36, January 9, 2007 ª2007 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.054 Paternal Indirect Genetic Effects on Offspring Viability and the Benefits of Polyandry Report

More information

The evolutionary genetics of sexual size dimorphism in the cricket Allonemobius socius

The evolutionary genetics of sexual size dimorphism in the cricket Allonemobius socius (2007) 99, 218 223 & 2007 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0018-067X/07 $30.00 ORIGINAL ARTICLE www.nature.com/hdy The evolutionary genetics of sexual size dimorphism in the cricket Allonemobius

More information

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise...

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise... 1. Female choice of males that have exaggerated characters has been explained by female choice of males that have better genes. Explain how female choice of higher quality males might lead to exaggerated

More information

Female mating preferences are widely recognized as being

Female mating preferences are widely recognized as being Quantitative genetic correlation between trait and preference supports a sexually selected sperm process Leigh W. Simmons* and Janne S. Kotiaho *Centre for Evolutionary Biology, School of Animal Biology

More information

Cancer develops after somatic mutations overcome the multiple

Cancer develops after somatic mutations overcome the multiple Genetic variation in cancer predisposition: Mutational decay of a robust genetic control network Steven A. Frank* Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697-2525

More information

Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology

Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology A. In this section we will look the nearly universal conflict that will eventually arise in any species where there is some form of parental

More information

EVOLUTIONARY ECOLOGY OF PROGENY SIZE

EVOLUTIONARY ECOLOGY OF PROGENY SIZE Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2000. 45:341 369 Copyright 2000 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved. EVOLUTIONARY ECOLOGY OF PROGENY SIZE IN ARTHROPODS Charles W. Fox and Mary Ellen Czesak Department of Entomology,

More information

Gustatory adaptation affects sexual maturation in male German cockroaches, Blattella germanica

Gustatory adaptation affects sexual maturation in male German cockroaches, Blattella germanica Physiological Entomology (2016) 41, 19 23 DOI: 10.1111/phen.12122 Gustatory adaptation affects sexual maturation in male German cockroaches, Blattella germanica KIM JENSEN 1,2, COBY SCHAL 1,2 and JULES

More information

Types of Mating Systems

Types of Mating Systems The Human Mating System Note: this is the stereotypical EP view (e.g., Buss) Types of Mating Systems Random Monogamy Polygyny: variance in male reproductive success > variance in female reproductive success

More information

Cost of reproduction in Callosobruchus maculatus: effects of mating on male longevity and the effect of male mating status on female longevity

Cost of reproduction in Callosobruchus maculatus: effects of mating on male longevity and the effect of male mating status on female longevity Journal of Insect Physiology 51 (25) 122 1226 www.elsevier.com/locate/jinsphys Cost of reproduction in Callosobruchus maculatus: effects of mating on male longevity and the effect of male mating status

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line in evolution is reproductive success reproductive success:

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons behind the lifestyles of our non-human primate relatives

More information

The role of maternal and paternal effects in the evolution of parental quality by sexual selection

The role of maternal and paternal effects in the evolution of parental quality by sexual selection The role of maternal and paternal effects in the evolution of parental quality by sexual selection J. B. WOLF,* E. D. BRODIE III* & A. J. MOORE *Department of Biology, Jordan Hall, Indiana University,

More information

Parental age effects on the female fitness traits in Drosophila melanogaster

Parental age effects on the female fitness traits in Drosophila melanogaster Egypt. Acad. J. Biolog. Sci., 5(2): 191-196 (2012) A. Entomology Email: egyptianacademic@yahoo.com ISSN: 1687 8809 Received: 21/ 9 /2012 www.eajbs.eg.net Parental age effects on the female fitness traits

More information

Life-Span Development Thirteenth Edition

Life-Span Development Thirteenth Edition Life-Span Development Thirteenth Edition Natural Selection and Adaptive Behavior Natural Selection: an evolutionary process by which those individuals of a species that are best adapted are the ones that

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons behind the lifestyles of our non-human primate relatives

More information

Biology 352, Spring 2018 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade)

Biology 352, Spring 2018 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade) Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade) 1-1) Match the following terms used in behavioral ecology with the appropriate topics of behavioral study listed to the right (you may apply more than one topic

More information

Lectures 7 & 8 Wednesday, October 12, 2011 & Friday, October 14, 2011

Lectures 7 & 8 Wednesday, October 12, 2011 & Friday, October 14, 2011 Lectures 7 & 8 Wednesday, October 12, 2011 & Friday, October 14, 2011 Recombination Diploid organisms: The first step in sexual reproduction is the production of gametes, each of which has half the chromosomes

More information

MATERNAL EFFECTS ON OFFSPRING DEPEND ON FEMALE MATING PATTERN AND OFFSPRING ENVIRONMENT IN YELLOW DUNG FLIES

MATERNAL EFFECTS ON OFFSPRING DEPEND ON FEMALE MATING PATTERN AND OFFSPRING ENVIRONMENT IN YELLOW DUNG FLIES Evolution, 57(2), 2003, pp. 297 304 MATERNAL EFFECTS ON OFFSPRING DEPEND ON FEMALE MATING PATTERN AND OFFSPRING ENVIRONMENT IN YELLOW DUNG FLIES TOM TREGENZA, 1,2 NINA WEDELL, 1 DAVID J. HOSKEN, 3 AND

More information

The Inheritance of Complex Traits

The Inheritance of Complex Traits The Inheritance of Complex Traits Differences Among Siblings Is due to both Genetic and Environmental Factors VIDEO: Designer Babies Traits Controlled by Two or More Genes Many phenotypes are influenced

More information

Reproduction. Asexual vs Sexual

Reproduction. Asexual vs Sexual Reproduction Asexual vs Sexual Why is Reproduction Important? The means by which an organism produces offspring Biologically and evolutionarily speaking, reproduction is what has made the continuation

More information

Genetic and nutritional effects on male traits and reproductive performance in Tribolium flour beetles

Genetic and nutritional effects on male traits and reproductive performance in Tribolium flour beetles doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02408.x Genetic and nutritional effects on male traits and reproductive performance in Tribolium flour beetles S. M. LEWIS, N. TIGREROS, T. FEDINA 1 &Q.L.MING 2 Department

More information

Animal Reproduction Chapter 46. Fission. Budding. Parthenogenesis. Fragmentation 11/27/2017

Animal Reproduction Chapter 46. Fission. Budding. Parthenogenesis. Fragmentation 11/27/2017 Animal Reproduction Chapter 46 Both asexual and sexual reproduction occur in the animal kingdom Sexual reproduction is the creation of an offspring by fusion of a male gamete (sperm) and female gamete

More information

Evolution of Mating Systems. Chapter 8

Evolution of Mating Systems. Chapter 8 Evolution of Mating Systems Chapter 8 Mating Systems-Chapter 8 1 Monogamy 2 Polyandry 3 Polygyny And the many combinations within! Why should a male be monogamous? 1 extension of guarding, little chance

More information

Sex ratio bias in the dung beetle Onthophagus taurus: adaptive allocation or sex-specific offspring mortality?

Sex ratio bias in the dung beetle Onthophagus taurus: adaptive allocation or sex-specific offspring mortality? Evol Ecol (2011) 25:363 372 DOI 10.1007/s10682-010-9423-0 RESEARCH ARTICLE Sex ratio bias in the dung beetle Onthophagus taurus: adaptive allocation or sex-specific offspring mortality? Clarissa M. House

More information

Eutherian Mammals Hamadryas Baboon

Eutherian Mammals Hamadryas Baboon ANIMAL 1 REPRODUCTION Eutherian Mammals Hamadryas Baboon Hamadryas Baboons form social hierarchies that involve the formation of troops, bands, clans and harems. A harem is the smallest unit and usually

More information

3/26/ Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy. 1. Fecundity

3/26/ Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy. 1. Fecundity Sexual Dimorphism 1. Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy A. sexual reproduction involving the fusion of two dissimilar gametes;individuals producing the

More information

Lekking and the Lek Paradox

Lekking and the Lek Paradox Lekking and the Lek Paradox Mating Systems 5. Lekking: One sex (usually males) provides only genes to their mate. No direct benefits are passed to the mate. 6. Cooperative: Some individuals forgo reproduction

More information

Time allowed: 2 hours Answer ALL questions in Section A, ALL PARTS of the question in Section B and ONE question from Section C.

Time allowed: 2 hours Answer ALL questions in Section A, ALL PARTS of the question in Section B and ONE question from Section C. UNIVERSITY OF EAST ANGLIA School of Biological Sciences Main Series UG Examination 2014-2015 BEHAVIOURAL ECOLOGY BIO-5010B Time allowed: 2 hours Answer ALL questions in Section A, ALL PARTS of the question

More information

So what is a species?

So what is a species? So what is a species? Evolutionary Forces New Groups Biological species concept defined by Ernst Mayr population whose members can interbreed & produce viable, fertile offspring reproductively compatible

More information

Report. Evolutionary Response to Sexual Selection in Male Genital Morphology

Report. Evolutionary Response to Sexual Selection in Male Genital Morphology Current Biology 19, 1442 1446, September 15, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.056 Evolutionary Response to Sexual Selection in Male Genital Morphology Report Leigh

More information

11.1 Genetic Variation Within Population. KEY CONCEPT A population shares a common gene pool.

11.1 Genetic Variation Within Population. KEY CONCEPT A population shares a common gene pool. KEY CONCEPT A population shares a common gene pool. Genetic variation in a population increases the chance that some individuals will survive. Genetic variation leads to phenotypic variation. Phenotypic

More information

Dynamics of sex ratio and female unmatedness under haplodiploidy

Dynamics of sex ratio and female unmatedness under haplodiploidy Dynamics of sex ratio and female unmatedness under haplodiploidy Andy Gardner School of Biology, University of St Andrews, Dyers Brae, St Andrews KY6 9TH, U.K. Keywords Arrhenotoky, ecology, evolution,

More information

Multivariate Post-copulatory Selection and Quantitative Genetics of Drosophila simulans Sex Comb

Multivariate Post-copulatory Selection and Quantitative Genetics of Drosophila simulans Sex Comb Multivariate Post-copulatory Selection and Quantitative Genetics of Drosophila simulans Sex Comb Submitted by Manar S. Maraqa to the University of Exeter as a thesis for the degree of Master by Research

More information

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30)

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) FILM SESSIONS Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) 1. Talking to Strangers (60 min.) 1 This film surveys some of the general communication signals used by animals from diverse taxa,

More information

TECH EXTENSION. How low can we go? Finding a practical path to produce more pigs from fewer and better boars.

TECH EXTENSION. How low can we go? Finding a practical path to produce more pigs from fewer and better boars. TECH EXTENSION How low can we go? Finding a practical path to produce more pigs from fewer and better boars www.fastgenetics.com Artificial insemination (AI) has become the standard breeding method for

More information

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection Intrasexual Natural Selection Sexual Selection Survival Mating Success Parental Care Intrasexual

More information

Genetic Variation in Maternal Investment Patterns in Platyfish, Xiphophorus maculatus. ALEXANDRA L. BASOLO and WILLIAM E. WAGNER, JR.

Genetic Variation in Maternal Investment Patterns in Platyfish, Xiphophorus maculatus. ALEXANDRA L. BASOLO and WILLIAM E. WAGNER, JR. ZEBRAFISH Volume 3, Number 3, 2006 Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. Genetic Variation in Maternal Investment Patterns in Platyfish, Xiphophorus maculatus ALEXANDRA L. BASOLO and WILLIAM E. WAGNER, JR. ABSTRACT Maternal

More information

Chapter 46 ~ Animal Reproduction

Chapter 46 ~ Animal Reproduction Chapter 46 ~ Animal Reproduction Overview Asexual (one parent) fission (parent separation) budding (corals) fragmentation & regeneration (inverts) parthenogenesis Sexual (fusion of haploid gametes) gametes

More information

The effects of copulation duration in the bruchid beetle Callosobruchus maculatus

The effects of copulation duration in the bruchid beetle Callosobruchus maculatus Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arj045 Advance Access publication 8 February 2006 The effects of copulation duration in the bruchid beetle Callosobruchus maculatus Martin Edvardsson a and David Canal

More information

PATERNAL CARE: DIRECT AND INDIRECT GENETIC EFFECTS OF FATHERS ON OFFSPRING PERFORMANCE

PATERNAL CARE: DIRECT AND INDIRECT GENETIC EFFECTS OF FATHERS ON OFFSPRING PERFORMANCE ORIGINAL ARTICLE doi:0./j.558-5646.202.0699.x PATERNAL CARE: DIRECT AND INDIRECT GENETIC EFFECTS OF FATHERS ON OFFSPRING PERFORMANCE Megan L. Head,,2 Lisa K. Berry, Nick J. Royle, and Allen J. Moore,3

More information

Fertilization. Bởi: OpenStaxCollege

Fertilization. Bởi: OpenStaxCollege Fertilization Bởi: OpenStaxCollege Sexual reproduction starts with the combination of a sperm and an egg in a process called fertilization. This can occur either inside (internal fertilization) or outside

More information

Ch. 23 The Evolution of Populations

Ch. 23 The Evolution of Populations Ch. 23 The Evolution of Populations 1 Essential question: Do populations evolve? 2 Mutation and Sexual reproduction produce genetic variation that makes evolution possible What is the smallest unit of

More information

The Operational Sex Ratio, the Potential Reproductive Rate, and the Opportunity for Sexual Selection Stephen M. Shuster

The Operational Sex Ratio, the Potential Reproductive Rate, and the Opportunity for Sexual Selection Stephen M. Shuster The Operational Sex Ratio, the Potential Reproductive Rate, and the Opportunity for Sexual Selection Stephen M. Shuster Stephen M. Shuster Northern Arizona University Objectives. Definitions of OSR/PRR.

More information

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli Lecture 19: Animal Behavior I. Background A. Animal behavior reflects and arises from biological properties 1. Exhibited behavior defends on the physiological systems and processes unique to a given organism

More information

Chapter 21.2 Mechanisms of Evolutionary Change

Chapter 21.2 Mechanisms of Evolutionary Change Beak depth of Beak depth Colonie High AP Biology Chapter 21.2 Mechanisms of Evolutionary Change Populations Evolve! Natural selection acts on individuals differential survival survival of the fittest differential

More information

Overview of Animal Breeding

Overview of Animal Breeding Overview of Animal Breeding 1 Required Information Successful animal breeding requires 1. the collection and storage of data on individually identified animals; 2. complete pedigree information about the

More information

MECHANISMS AND PATTERNS OF EVOLUTION

MECHANISMS AND PATTERNS OF EVOLUTION MECHANISMS AND PATTERNS OF EVOLUTION Evolution What is it again? Evolution is the change in allele frequencies of a population over generations Mechanisms of Evolution what can make evolution happen? 1.

More information

MBG* Animal Breeding Methods Fall Final Exam

MBG* Animal Breeding Methods Fall Final Exam MBG*4030 - Animal Breeding Methods Fall 2007 - Final Exam 1 Problem Questions Mick Dundee used his financial resources to purchase the Now That s A Croc crocodile farm that had been operating for a number

More information

Title. Evolution of maternal investment strategies for the Strawberry Poison-Dart Frog, Dendrobates pumilio, based on environmental risk factors

Title. Evolution of maternal investment strategies for the Strawberry Poison-Dart Frog, Dendrobates pumilio, based on environmental risk factors Title Evolution of maternal investment strategies for the Strawberry Poison-Dart Frog, Dendrobates pumilio, based on environmental risk factors Author Richard Pashkow: rpashkow@mail.usf.edu Abstract The

More information

Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Classification

Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Classification Introduction 1 Introduction 2 Individuals rarely mate at random for a number of reasons: Dispersal may be limited Individuals may or may not be able to self Individuals may reproduce asexually Individuals

More information

Chromosome Theory & Sex-Linked Transmission

Chromosome Theory & Sex-Linked Transmission Chromosome Theory & Sex-Linked Transmission (CHAPTER 3 & 5- Brooker Text) Feb 14, 2006 BIO 184 Dr. Tom Peavy - Maturation of the sperm in the epididymis - Mammalian sperm become capable of fertilizing

More information

Sexual Reproduction. For most diploid eukaryotes, sexual reproduction is the only mechanism resulting in new members of a species.

Sexual Reproduction. For most diploid eukaryotes, sexual reproduction is the only mechanism resulting in new members of a species. Sex Determination Sexual Reproduction For most diploid eukaryotes, sexual reproduction is the only mechanism resulting in new members of a species. Meiosis in the sexual organs of parents produces haploid

More information

Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs. Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs

Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs. Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs Research by Gerhardt et al. (1996). Males call to attract females. Males call to attract females Ribbet! Figure 11.27 Tiny and super cute!

More information

Chapter 22 The Reproductive System (I)

Chapter 22 The Reproductive System (I) Chapter 22 The Reproductive System (I) An Overview of Reproductive Physiology o The Male Reproductive System o The Female Reproductive System 22.1 Reproductive System Overview Reproductive system = all

More information

BSc and MSc Degree Examinations

BSc and MSc Degree Examinations Examination Candidate Number: Desk Number: Department : BIOLOGY BSc and MSc Degree Examinations 2018-9 Title of Exam: Evolutionary Ecology Time Allowed: 2 hours Marking Scheme: Total marks available for

More information

Roadmap. Inbreeding How inbred is a population? What are the consequences of inbreeding?

Roadmap. Inbreeding How inbred is a population? What are the consequences of inbreeding? 1 Roadmap Quantitative traits What kinds of variation can selection work on? How much will a population respond to selection? Heritability How can response be restored? Inbreeding How inbred is a population?

More information

SEX-RATIO CONFLICT BETWEEN QUEENS AND WORKERS IN EUSOCIAL HYMENOPTERA: MECHANISMS, COSTS, AND THE EVOLUTION OF SPLIT COLONY SEX RATIOS

SEX-RATIO CONFLICT BETWEEN QUEENS AND WORKERS IN EUSOCIAL HYMENOPTERA: MECHANISMS, COSTS, AND THE EVOLUTION OF SPLIT COLONY SEX RATIOS Evolution, 59(12), 2005, pp. 2626 2638 SEX-RATIO CONFLICT BETWEEN QUEENS AND WORKERS IN EUSOCIAL HYMENOPTERA: MECHANISMS, COSTS, AND THE EVOLUTION OF SPLIT COLONY SEX RATIOS KEN R. HELMS, 1,2,3 MAX REUTER,

More information

Biology of Breeding: Considerations for maximizing genetic diversity of breeding groups

Biology of Breeding: Considerations for maximizing genetic diversity of breeding groups Dillon Damuth 03/01/2015 Biology of Breeding: Considerations for maximizing genetic diversity of breeding groups When a person joins the hobby of reptile keeping and make the decision to breed animals

More information

Within-clutch variation in offspring sex determined by differences in sire body size: cryptic mate choice in the wild

Within-clutch variation in offspring sex determined by differences in sire body size: cryptic mate choice in the wild doi: 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00665.x SHORT COMMUNICATION Within-clutch variation in offspring sex determined by differences in sire body size: cryptic mate choice in the wild RYAN CALSBEEK* & BARRY SINERVO

More information

THE EFFECT OF POLYGYNY ON THE SEX RATIO OF MICE (MUS MUSCULUS)

THE EFFECT OF POLYGYNY ON THE SEX RATIO OF MICE (MUS MUSCULUS) 235 THE EFFECT OF POLYGYNY ON THE SEX RATIO OF MICE (MUS MUSCULUS) BY WILLIAM H. GATES. (Department of Zoology and Entomology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, Louisiana.) (Received igth September

More information

EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY BIOS EXAM #2 FALL 2017

EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY BIOS EXAM #2 FALL 2017 EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY BIOS 30305 EXAM #2 FALL 2017 There are 3 parts to this exam. Use your time efficiently and be sure to put your name on the top of each page. Part I. True (T) or False (F) (2 points

More information

Intersexual Competition

Intersexual Competition (in press, October 2012). In P. Whelan & A. Bolin (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Human Sexuality. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Blackwell. Intersexual Competition Valerie G. Starratt, Nova Southeastern University, valerie.starratt@nova.edu

More information

The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction

The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction I. Introduction to Behavioral Ecology Behavioral ecologists test hypotheses about the adaptive function of a behavior Behavioral Ecology Behavioral ecologists

More information

L567: From last time:

L567: From last time: L567: From last time: Fisher: in Fisher s original presentation, he considered female preference for male traits that were initially favored by natural selection. This could lead to a covariance between

More information

Estimating genetic variation within families

Estimating genetic variation within families Estimating genetic variation within families Peter M. Visscher Queensland Institute of Medical Research Brisbane, Australia peter.visscher@qimr.edu.au 1 Overview Estimation of genetic parameters Variation

More information

Sexual selection. Intrasexual selection mating success determined by within-sex interactions e.g., male-male combat

Sexual selection. Intrasexual selection mating success determined by within-sex interactions e.g., male-male combat Sexual dimorphism Sexual selection Is sexual selection different from natural selection? Darwin saw them as distinct - only sexual selection could produce traits that compromise survival The basic principle

More information

Animal Behavior. Thur March 24: Animal Behavior: outline. Questions: What, How, and Why. Behavior is shaped partly by inheritance

Animal Behavior. Thur March 24: Animal Behavior: outline. Questions: What, How, and Why. Behavior is shaped partly by inheritance Thur March 24: Animal Behavior Animal Behavior: outline Animal Behavior Questions Behavior is partly shaped by Inheritance Behavior is partly shaped by Learning Hormones and Behavior The Genetics of Behavior

More information

Mating systems and parental investment. Mating systems. Resource distribution. Polygyny. Pattern of matings in a population. Antithesis = promiscuity

Mating systems and parental investment. Mating systems. Resource distribution. Polygyny. Pattern of matings in a population. Antithesis = promiscuity 1 Mating systems and parental investment Mating systems Pattern of matings in a population green anole Antithesis = promiscuity Polygyny Scramble: no attempts to defend females, resources horseshoe crabs

More information

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H

More information

B. male gametes that may be carried by the wind

B. male gametes that may be carried by the wind 1. Which characteristic of sexual reproduction has specifically favored the survival of animals that live on land? A. fusion of gametes in the outside environment B. male gametes that may be carried by

More information

(A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above

(A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above CLEP Biology - Problem Drill 24: Behavior No. 1 of 10 1. Habituation is a type of. (A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above Having a certain drive

More information

Female mating preferences for colourful males in a population of guppies subject to high predation

Female mating preferences for colourful males in a population of guppies subject to high predation Journal of Fish Biology (2004) 65, 1154 1159 doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2004.00502.x, available online at http://www.blackwell-synergy.com Female mating preferences for colourful males in a population of

More information

Rival Male Relatedness Does Not Affect Ejaculate Allocation as Predicted by Sperm Competition Theory

Rival Male Relatedness Does Not Affect Ejaculate Allocation as Predicted by Sperm Competition Theory Rival Male Relatedness Does Not Affect Ejaculate Allocation as Predicted by Sperm Competition Theory Melissa L. Thomas*, Leigh W. Simmons Centre for Evolutionary Biology, School of Animal Biology, The

More information

SEX RATIO MANIPULATION BY THE PARASITOID WASP MUSCIDIFURAX RAPTOR IN RESPONSE TO HOST SIZE

SEX RATIO MANIPULATION BY THE PARASITOID WASP MUSCIDIFURAX RAPTOR IN RESPONSE TO HOST SIZE This is an electronic version of an article published as Seidl, S.E. and B.H. King. 1993. Sex ratio response to host size in the parasitoid wasp Muscidifurax raptor. Evolution 47:1876-1882. SEX RATIO MANIPULATION

More information

Introduction At times when pollen is scarce in the natural environment

Introduction At times when pollen is scarce in the natural environment Artificial Bee Diets Taryn Major Linda Eaton Kathy Haskard Philip Vlaskovsky Rob Manning (Department of Agriculture and Food) Introduction At times when pollen is scarce in the natural environment Bees

More information

Reinvestigating good genes benefits of mate choice in Drosophila simulans

Reinvestigating good genes benefits of mate choice in Drosophila simulans bs_bs_banner 295..306 Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2012, 106, 295 306. With 5 figures Reinvestigating good genes benefits of mate choice in Drosophila simulans MANMOHAN D. SHARMA, ROBERT

More information

Authors Knowing something about the authors can illuminate the topic of the paper. Where does Robert Brooks work, and what does he usually research?

Authors Knowing something about the authors can illuminate the topic of the paper. Where does Robert Brooks work, and what does he usually research? Dissecting a scientific paper about evolutionary biology II Now that you ve had a guided tour of one scientific paper, it s time to attack another one with a little less assistance. The sections below

More information

THE EVOLUTION OF POPULATIONS

THE EVOLUTION OF POPULATIONS THE EVOLUTION OF POPULATIONS HOW DOES A POPULATION OF PENGUINS EVOLVE? Every year, king penguins return to breed in the same colony in which they are born. These colonies help penguins to guard, protect

More information

3.1 Meiosis

3.1 Meiosis 3.1 Meiosis Chromosome Number Individuals of the same species have the same number of chromosomes. Meiosis is used to half the number of chromosomes passed to the offspring. What is meiosis Meiosis happens

More information

How Populations Evolve

How Populations Evolve Chapter 16: pp. 283-298 BIOLOGY 10th Edition How Populations Evolve 10% of population Copyright The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. natural disaster kills five

More information

Kin recognition plays multiple roles across the animal kingdom. From an

Kin recognition plays multiple roles across the animal kingdom. From an Sample paper critique #1 Kin recognition plays multiple roles across the animal kingdom. From an evolutionary point of view, one might expect kin recognition to be particularly important in the recognition

More information

PopGen4: Assortative mating

PopGen4: Assortative mating opgen4: Assortative mating Introduction Although random mating is the most important system of mating in many natural populations, non-random mating can also be an important mating system in some populations.

More information

Understanding Reversals in the Relative Strength of Sexual Selection on Males and Females: A Role for Sperm Competition?

Understanding Reversals in the Relative Strength of Sexual Selection on Males and Females: A Role for Sperm Competition? vol. 159, no. 6 the american naturalist june 2002 Understanding Reversals in the Relative Strength of Sexual Selection on Males and Females: A Role for Sperm Competition? Patrick D. Lorch * Biology Group,

More information

Bio 1M: Evolutionary processes

Bio 1M: Evolutionary processes Bio 1M: Evolutionary processes Evolution by natural selection Is something missing from the story I told last chapter? Heritable variation in traits Selection (i.e., differential reproductive success)

More information