Variation, but no covariance, in female preference functions and male song in a natural population of Drosophila montana

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Variation, but no covariance, in female preference functions and male song in a natural population of Drosophila montana"

Transcription

1 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2005, 70, doi: /j.anbehav Variation, but no covariance, in female preference functions and male song in a natural population of Drosophila montana MICHAEL G. RITCHIE*, MARI SAARIKETTU &ANNELIHOIKKALA *Environmental & Evolutionary Biology, University of St Andrews ydepartment of Biology, University of Oulu (Received 6 September 2004; initial acceptance 18 October 2004; final acceptance 23 January 2005; published online 29 August 2005; MS. number: 8265) Studies of the variance and covariance between female mating preferences and sexually selected male traits in natural populations are rare. In D. montana, male courtship song is an important target of sexual selection. We analysed the variance in components of song and preferences among F 1 families from a natural population. All song traits varied substantially, with among-family variance components ranging from 30 to 65%. The greatest variation was in carrier frequency, which is the most important predictor of mating success. This is compatible with the trait capturing mutational and other components of genetic variance in condition because of condition-dependent expression. There was also variation for some components of preference variation, with significant variation among sisters within families, and among families. Females varied in overall responsiveness, but not in the slope of the linear female preference function for male song carrier frequency. Such variation might be expected to generate assortative mating, with more choosy females mating with higher quality males, but there was no covariance across families between female responsiveness and male carrier frequency. Substantial variation in the level of responsiveness might allow low-quality males to achieve some mating success and counteract the buildup of a strong genetic covariance between preferences and traits. Ó 2005 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Studies of variation in courtship signals and preferences are very important to many areas of evolutionary biology, because the variance and covariance between these will determine the extent and nature of sexual selection and may influence population divergence and speciation (Lande 1982; Ryan & Rand 1993; Andersson 1994; Bakker & Pomiankowski 1995). In particular, few studies have accurately quantified the variance in female preferences within populations, or examined the components or sources of any such variation (Brooks & Endler 2001). Significant variability in preferences has been detected in some systems (Jennions & Petrie 1997; Hedrick & Weber 1998), but the nature and genetic basis of such variability is not well explored (Gray & Cade 1999; Brooks & Endler 2001). Female preferences are complex with different Correspondence: M. G. Ritchie, Environmental & Evolutionary Biology, Dyers Brae House, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife KY16 9TS, U.K. ( mgr@st-and.ac.uk). M. Saarikettu is at the Department of Biology, University of Oulu, PO Box 3000, Oulu, Finland. A. Hoikkala is now at the Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Jyväskylä, PO Box 35, Jyväskylä, Finland. components of preference contributing to variation in the outcome of mate choice (Wagner et al. 1995; Reinhold et al. 2002) and may be particularly difficult to analyse if females vary because of complex, multitrait preferences (Blows et al. 2003). Females may vary in responsiveness (how likely they are to mate overall), discrimination (how choosy they are among males) and in the nature of the female preference function, the relation between a trait and the probability of mating, which may have linear, quadratic or correlational terms, or unorthodox shapes (Ritchie 1996; Blows et al. 2003). Highly influential models of Fisher s runaway process have identified the extent of genetic covariance in traits and preferences as a key determinant of whether this process could lead to runaway evolution and exaggeration of these behaviours (Lande 1981; Arnold 1983). As a result, many studies have attempted either to detect indirectly or to quantify directly such a genetic covariance, and several have identified significant covariance in a range of organisms (Bakker 1993; Houde 1994; Wilkinson & Reillo 1994; Blows 1999; Gray & Cade 1999). However, covariances have not always been detected (Gilburn & Day 1994; Day & Gilburn 1997). Genetic covariance may be found if there is an intrinsic genetic association between /05/$30.00/0 Ó 2005 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

2 850 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 70, 4 genes caused by pleiotropy or tight genetic linkage, although there is little direct evidence for the very strong pleiotropy predicted by the genetic coupling hypothesis (Butlin & Ritchie 1989; Boake 1991). Nevertheless, if there is substantial genetic variation for traits and preferences, assortative mating would be expected to generate covariance within populations because of gametic phase linkage disequilibrium. Some studies have indicated that covariances may be unstable in the laboratory as might be expected if assortative mating generates and maintains such disequilibria (Houde 1994; Pomiankowski & Sheridan 1994; Blows 1999; Gray & Cade 1999). Although much of the research in this area has taken place in the context of Fisher s runaway process, other forms of sexual selection such as good genes processes would also generate covariance. However, it has increasingly come to be realized that male traits and female preferences may often be out of step during evolutionary change (Ryan & Rand 1995). Species have been described where females show preferences for heterospecific males (e.g. Basolo 1995), and this can come about either because males of their species have lost a preferred trait despite the female preference, or because females have arbitrary preferences which males of their own species have not yet exploited (Shaw 1995). Sexual conflict in the outcome of mating generates selection on males to overcome female reluctance to mate, which gives rise to a dynamic interaction between traits and preferences (Gavrilets 2000; Gavrilets et al. 2001) during which covariances might be expected to change rapidly and unpredictably (there is one intriguing case of a negative covariance, Morris et al. 1996). Acoustic communication systems offer a good opportunity to analyse variation in male traits, because these are relatively easily identified and measured, and in preferences, because playback of synthetic signals allows quantification of variation in the components of female preferences. In Drosophila montana, male song is an obligatory component of mating success (Liimatainen et al. 1992); females show a receptive gesture to males whose song they like by spreading their wings (Vuoristo et al. 1996), and they will do this to synthetic song in the laboratory (Ritchie et al. 1998). This allows researchers to carry out repeated playback experiments to females, and hence to analyse their consistency and variation. Previously we have analysed the shape of female preference functions for acoustic signals in inbred laboratory strains of D. montana. Females prefer males with high-frequency song, which has probably evolved under sexual selection as an indicator of male quality, because the offspring of males with high-frequency song have higher viability (Hoikkala et al. 1998). Previous field studies have implicated song frequency as a predictor of mating success (Aspi & Hoikkala 1995). In this study, we quantified variation in preference functions, male songs and their covariance in a natural population. METHODS Drosophila montana flies were collected in the summer of 2000 from Oulu, Finland and 27 lines were established from females mated in the wild. All the individuals studied here are F 1 progeny of these females. The flies were maintained in malt medium bottles in continuous light at 19 C. Males and females were sexed when they were 2 days old and kept in separate malt medium vials. The flies were used when they were sexually mature (21 28 days old). Males used in the playback experiments had their wings removed under carbon dioxide anaesthesia (to silence them) 1 day before the experiments. For song recording, males were confined in chambers made of a petri dish (diameter 55 mm, height 13 mm) with sexually mature virgin females. Floors of the chambers were covered with moistened filter paper and ceilings were made of nylon net. Male songs were recorded with a JVC condenser microphone and Sony TC-FX33 cassette recorder at 20 G 1 C. To analyse the song parameters we used the Signal Sound Analysis System (Engineering Design, Berkeley, CA, U.S.A.). Songs were digitized at a sampling rate of 2.5 khz after sound above 500 Hz was filtered. We measured the length of pulse trains and the number of pulses in these trains from oscillograms of the recorded songs. Interpulse interval, pulse length and the number of cycles were obtained from the fourth pulse in every pulse train. Carrier frequency was measured from Fourier spectra of the pulse trains. We measured three pulse trains for each of three males per family and used the mean per male in the analysis. The synthesized songs were made with the Signal Sound Analysis System (Engineering Design). Three songs were produced with carrier frequencies of 200, 260 and 320 Hz, while other parameters were kept constant (a pulse train consisted of nine pulses with a total intertrain interval of 1400 ms, interpulse interval of 31 ms and pulse length of 20 ms). These parameters were typical of the study population (Suvanto et al. 2000) and the preference function is known to be linear over this range (Ritchie et al. 2001) The songs were played via a subwoofer (Boston Acoustics, Inc., Peabody, MA, U.S.A. Micromedia PC Sound System) with a sound pressure level of 110 db (measured with a Tecpel sound level meter DSL-330). For playback, females were individually placed in round chambers (diameter 25 mm, height 10 mm) with a wingless male from another isofemale strain (Ritchie et al. 1998). The chambers had plastic walls and both floors and ceilings were made of acoustically transparent nylon net. They were held approximately 2 cm above the subwoofer. We observed each female for 10 min during playback and scored the incidence of wing spreading. The playback experiments were conducted between 0800 and 1200 hours at 20 G 1 C. Three females from each strain were studied and these triplets were observed simultaneously. For each triplet only one song was played on any one day. Playback sequence was randomized. All songs were used twice so every triplet was observed six times. In a control experiment we observed 50 D. montana fly pairs in silence and saw no wing lifting. All male song traits conformed to normality and their variation was analysed with a one-way ANOVA (males within families). Preference data were binomial and were analysed with a generalized linear model with a logit link

3 RITCHIE ET AL.: SONG PREFERENCE COVARIANCE IN FLIES 851 function. Terms are described in the Results. For statistical analyses, we used either Minitab12 or Genstat6 (Genstat Committee 1993). In an experiment such as this, variance among families can be regarded as a measure of broadsense heritability. Nonadditive sources of variance, maternal and common rearing effects will inflate this, so it is not a reliable measure of narrow-sense (additive genetic) heritability (Falconer & Mackay 1996). It is safe to consider the variance components as a general measure of genetic variability, but not a specific component of genetic architecture. RESULTS Table 1 shows the components of variance for male song traits among families. Families differed in all traits, including song carrier frequency. Variance components among families ranged from 30 to 65%. Table 2 summarizes the variation in female preferences. Terms used in the model were song carrier frequency (as a linear covariate), family, females (nested within family), family! song interaction and the three-way interaction. The frequency term tests whether females discriminate between the three songs, the family term tests whether families differ in overall responsiveness, and the female term whether sisters within families differ in responsiveness. The family! song interaction term tests whether families differ in preference function (i.e. the slope of the relation between song and preference) and the three-way interaction term whether the sisters vary in preference function. The first three terms were significant, the latter two not (and the model did not converge satisfactorily when we attempted to fit the latter two). Overall, higher song frequencies were preferred. Families differed in the level of overall responsiveness, with some families responding more readily to all song types. There were also significant differences in responsiveness between sisters within families. However, the slopes of the preference functions, at the level of families or sisters, did not differ significantly. Hence, in this population preference functions differed significantly in height, but not slope. Figure 1 shows the fitted preference functions (from a binomial model including only the significant terms). There were few females of high responsiveness. One would expect that variation in female responsiveness would generate assortative mating with the least responsive females mating with the better males (in this species characterized by those with higher song frequency). Figure 2 shows the covariance between family mean carrier frequency and responsiveness, estimated from a correlation analysis between family means (the different natures of the statistical models used to analyse male and female data precluded a more formal ANCOVA approach to estimating the genetic covariance). There was no suggestion of a significant covariance between these two traits (Pearson correlation: r 25 Z 0.148, P Z 0.47). Female preference variation was unrelated to any song trait. DISCUSSION The amount of genetic variability in sexually selected traits, and the covariance between selected traits and preferences, remains a topic of considerable interest in evolutionary biology. In particular, few studies have quantified variance in female preferences and their covariance with male traits (Bakker & Pomiankowski 1995; Jennions & Petrie 1997; Brooks & Endler 2001). Here, we have characterized the variability in courtship song and components of female preferences in a natural population of D. montana. All song traits showed considerable variation among families. The most important trait for mating success, song frequency, showed the greatest evidence of genetic variation with 65% of the variance being among families (average of the other traits Z 39%). Pomiankowski & Møller (1995) found that, in general, targets of sexual selection have a higher additive genetic variance than other traits, perhaps because nonlinear preferences would favour modifier genes. Preference functions for song frequency in D. montana do have a quadratic component, but this is stabilizing and the preference function is effectively linear over the normal range of male song variation (Ritchie et al. 2001). A more likely explanation for higher variance in the target of sexual selection here is condition dependence of the trait. Carrier frequency in D. montana is condition dependent because it correlates with overwinter survival (Hoikkala & Isoherranen 1997) and offspring quality (Hoikkala et al. 1998). Such traits are expected to capture genetic variance, as mutational input to their heritability will be greater than for other traits not influenced by genes affecting condition (Rowe & Houle 1996; Kotiaho et al. 2001). Female preference variation was also detected in this population, but not in all components of preferences. Overall, females preferred song of higher carrier frequency, as expected. There was substantial variability both among families and among females within families in the responsiveness, that is, the probability of accepting song of a given carrier frequency. However, the shape of the preference function (the extent of discrimination against males with low carrier frequency) did not vary. Hence, Table 1. Mean square (and variance component, as a percentage) for song trait variation among families from a one-way ANOVA Component Pulse number Pulse train length Pulse length Cycle number Interpulse interval Carrier frequency Families (NZ27) 1.87 (35) (51) 11.0 (30) 0.56 (31) 21.2 (48) (65) Males (NZ3) 0.71 (65) (49) 4.8 (70) 0.24 (69) 5.6 (52) (35) P 0.001! !0.001!0.001 The variance components among families are an indication of the broad-sense heritabilities of the traits.

4 852 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 70, 4 Table 2. Analysis of deviance for female preferences Component df Mean deviance Deviance ratio P Song frequency !0.001 Family !0.001 Females within families Song*family Song*family*female Residual Preference sexual selection will constantly favour males with high carrier frequency, but some females will overall be choosier than others. We would still expect this to generate a positive covariance because choosy females are likely to mate only with high-quality males, but we did not detect a correlation between preference and song. Our design looked for this rather indirectly and may not have detected a small correlation. The family mean approach is relatively crude, for example multiple mating will mean each family may consist of half-sibs. However, the approach has detected such a covariance in other systems (e.g. Bakker 1993). Less choosy females would have a greater probability of accepting a low-quality male, so selection generating a covariance would be less strong than in situations where, for example, there was substantial variation in the slopes of preference functions. Effectively, all females here preferred similar (high song frequency) males, but the presence of more responsive females will ensure that low-quality males achieve some mating success, and counteract the build-up of a positive genetic covariance. In Lande s (1981) model, the choosiness of the females was important under some models of female preference. However, this influenced the line of Preference Song frequency (Hz) Figure 1. Fitted preference functions. In this model, one line is fitted per family, but several are overlapping Song frequency (Hz) Figure 2. The covariance between song carrier frequency and female preference among families. equilibrium more than the establishment or size of the covariance. Why such females should persist in a population is an interesting question. Sexual selection would be expected to act against them, at least in stable populations. At low density, such females may have an advantage if mating opportunities are rare (Kaneshiro & Boake 1987). These females may also obtain other advantages if there is a viability cost associated with choosiness. Brooks & Endler (2001) found very similar results with a study of preference variation in a natural guppy, Poecilia reticulata, population. Females varied in responsiveness but to a much lesser extent in discrimination. Only responsiveness showed heritable variation (and artificial selection on mate choice was unsuccessful, Hall et al. 2004; but see Houde 1994). The guppies showed complex variation in discrimination among females, with females differing in which trait they assessed as well as responsiveness. In D. montana, the communication system seems more simple, with song carrier frequency being consistently found to be the most important trait for mating success. This could mean that selection can more effectively diminish genetic variation in important components of preference variation than in systems such as the guppies. In addition, perhaps female preferences are more likely to go to fixation than male traits because they are less condition dependent, which would lead to a decline in covariance. An interesting contrast with these results is provided by Gryllus integer in which females of one population had stabilizing preferences for the number of pulses in a chirp and showed significant variance in peak preference, and there was a significant covariance between male song and peak preference in an F 1 analysis. However, in this population females did not vary in choosiness (defined as the width of the stabilizing function; Gray & Cade 1999). Other populations of this species have different patterns of variation in preferences (Hedrick & Weber 1998), so the functions must vary genetically at some level. Ecological factors may counteract the build-up of strong covariance between preferences and traits, despite variance in both traits. In seaweed flies, Coelopa frigida, positive covariance is related to ecological stability. It is

5 RITCHIE ET AL.: SONG PREFERENCE COVARIANCE IN FLIES 853 thought that stable resources (seaweed in low tidal populations) allow large population sizes to be stable, allowing the build-up of covariance. In less stable (tidal) conditions good genes mate choice predominates, which in this species leads to dissassortative mating for much of the genome. Hence, the strength of a genetic covariance between preference and trait depends on the tidal range to which the population is exposed (Day & Gilburn 1997). Nichols & Butlin (1989) showed how genetic drift in modest population sizes can counteract the build-up of covariance between traits and preferences by a Fisher process. Also, sexual selection may be inconsistent across generations. In a neighbouring population of D. montana, the strength of sexual selection on song and morphology varied between years (Aspi & Hoikkala 1995). Genotype by environment interactions, affecting the reliability of traits and or preferences, also have the potential to disturb a simple build-up of covariance between the traits and preferences. Jia & Greenfield (1997) found that attractive signals of wax moths, Achroia grisella, correlated with fitness only under some environmental conditions. In conclusion, we found significant variation in some but not all components of mate choice variation in a natural population of D. montana, but not an expected genetic covariance between preferences and male traits. The fact that all females preferred males with high values of the trait, and varied only in overall responsiveness, will probably have mitigated against a build-up of a genetic covariance between trait and preference. Hence, this study reiterates the need to distinguish different components of preference variation to understand the nature and strength of sexual selection in natural populations (Brooks & Endler 2001). Acknowledgments This work was supported by a research grant from the Academy of Finland to A.H. (project 50591) and Evolutionary Ecology Graduate School to M.S. Natasha LeBas, other members of the Behaviour, Speciation & Genetics group at St Andrews and the anonymous referees provided helpful comments on the manuscript. References Andersson, M Sexual Selection. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Arnold, S. J Sexual selection: the interface of theory and empiricism. In: Mate Choice (Ed. by P. Bateson), pp Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Aspi, J. & Hoikkala, A Male mating success and survival in the field with respect to size and courtship song characters in Drosophila littoralis and D. montana (Diptera: Drosophilidae). Journal of Insect Behavior, 8, Bakker, T. C. M Positive genetic correlation between female preference and preferred male ornament in sticklebacks. Nature, 363, Bakker, T. C. M. & Pomiankowski, A The genetic basis of female mate preferences. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 8, Basolo, A. L Phylogenetic evidence for the role of a preexisting bias in sexual selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 259, Blows, M. W Evolution of the genetic covariance between male and female components of mate recognition: an experimental test. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 266, Blows, M. W., Brooks, R. & Kraft, P. G Exploring complex fitness surfaces: multiple ornamentation and polymorphism in male guppies. Evolution, 57, Boake, C. R. B Coevolution of senders and receivers of sexual signals: genetic coupling and genetic coevolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 6, Brooks, R. & Endler, J. A Female guppies agree to differ: phenotypic and genetic variation in mate-choice behavior and the consequences for sexual selection. Evolution, 55, Butlin, R. K. & Ritchie, M. G Genetic coupling in mate recognition systems: what is the evidence? Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 37, Day, T. H. & Gilburn, A. S Sexual selection in seaweed flies. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 26, Falconer, D. S. & Mackay, T. F. C Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. Harton: Longman. Gavrilets, S Rapid evolution of reproductive barriers driven by sexual selection. Nature, 403, Gavrilets, S., Arnqvist, G. & Friberg, U The evolution of female mate choice by sexual conflict. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 268, Genstat Committee Genstat 5 Release 3 Reference Manual. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Gilburn, A. S. & Day, T. H Evolution of female choice in seaweed flies: Fisherian and good genes mechanisms operate in different populations. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 255, Gray, D. A. & Cade, W. H Quantitative genetics of sexual selection in the field cricket Gryllus integer. Evolution, 53, Hall, M., Lindholm, A. & Brooks, R Direct selection on male attractiveness and female preference fails to produce a response. BioMed Central Evolutionary Biology, 4, 1. Hedrick, A. & Weber, T Variance in female responses to the fine structure of male song in the field cricket, Gryllus integer. Behavioral Ecology, 9, Hoikkala, A. & Isoherranen, E Variation and repeatability of courtship song characters among wild-caught and laboratoryreared Drosophila montana and D. littoralis males (Diptera: Drosophilidae). Journal of Insect Behavior, 10, Hoikkala, A., Aspi, J. & Suvanto, L Male courtship song frequency as an indicator of male genetic quality in an insect species, Drosophila montana. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 265, Houde, A. E Effect of artificial selection on male colour patterns on mating preference of female guppies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 256, Jennions, M. D. & Petrie, M Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences. Biological Reviews, 72, Jia, F.-Y. & Greenfield, M. D When are good genes good? Variable outcomes of female choice in wax moths. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 264, Kaneshiro, K. Y. & Boake, C. R. B Sexual selection and speciation: issues raised by Hawaiian Drosophila. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 2,

6 854 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 70, 4 Kotiaho, J. S., Simmons, L. W. & Tomkins, J. L Towards a resolution of the lek paradox. Nature, 410, Lande, R Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 78, Lande, R Rapid origin of sexual isolation and character divergence in a cline. Evolution, 36, Liimatainen, J., Hoikkala, A., Aspi, J. & Welbergen, P Courtship in Drosophila montana: the effects of male auditory signals on the behaviour of flies. Animal Behaviour, 43, Morris, M. R., Wagner, W. E. & Ryan, M. J A negative correlation between trait and mate preference in Xiphophorus pygmaeus. Animal Behaviour, 52, Nichols, R. A. & Butlin, R. K Does runaway sexual selection work in finite populations? Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 2, Pomiankowski, A. & Møller, A. P A resolution of the lek paradox. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 260, Pomiankowski, A. & Sheridan, L Linked sexiness and choosiness. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 9, Reinhold, K., Reinhold, K. & Jacoby, K. J Dissecting the repeatability of female choice in the grasshopper Chorthippus biguttulus. Animal Behaviour, 64, Ritchie, M. G The shape of female mating preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 93, Ritchie, M. G., Townhill, R. M. & Hoikkala, A Female preference for fly song: playbacks confirm correlational evidence of the targets of sexual selection. Animal Behaviour, 56, Ritchie, M. G., Saarikettu, M., Livingstone, S. & Hoikkala, A Characterisation of female preference functions for a sexually selected acoustic signal in D. montana, and a test of the temperature coupling hypothesis. Evolution, 55, Rowe, L. & Houle, D The lek paradox and the capture of genetic variance by condition dependent traits. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 263, Ryan, M. J. & Rand, A. S Species recognition and sexual selection as a unitary problem in animal communication. Evolution, 47, Ryan, M. J. & Rand, A. S Female responses to ancestral advertisement calls in Túngara frogs. Science, 269, Shaw, K Phylogenetic tests of the sensory exploitation model of sexual selection. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 10, Suvanto, L., Liimatainen, J. O., Tregenza, T. & Hoikkala, A Courtship signals and mate choice of the flies of inbred Drosophila montana strains. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 13, Vuoristo, M., Isoherranen, E. & Hoikkala, A Female wing spreading as acceptance signal in the Drosophila virilis group of species. Journal of Insect Behavior, 9, Wagner, W. E. J., Murray, A. M. & Cade, W. H Phenotypic variation in the mating preferences of female crickets, Gryllus integer. Animal Behaviour, 49, Wilkinson, G. S. & Reillo, P. R Female choice response to artificial selection on an exaggerated male trait in a stalk-eyed fly. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 255, 1 6.

Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage:

Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage: Animal Behaviour 78 (2009) 1357 1363 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav Sexual selection for genetic quality: disentangling the

More information

Test of indirect models of selection in the Trinidad guppy

Test of indirect models of selection in the Trinidad guppy Heredity 73 (1994) 291 297 Genetical Society of Great Britain Received 18 January 1994 Test of indirect models of selection in the Trinidad guppy FELIX BREDEN* & KELLY HORNADAYt Department of Biological

More information

CHARACTERIZATION OF FEMALE PREFERENCE FUNCTIONS FOR DROSOPHILA MONTANA COURTSHIP SONG AND A TEST OF THE TEMPERATURE COUPLING HYPOTHESIS

CHARACTERIZATION OF FEMALE PREFERENCE FUNCTIONS FOR DROSOPHILA MONTANA COURTSHIP SONG AND A TEST OF THE TEMPERATURE COUPLING HYPOTHESIS Evolution, 55(4), 2001, pp. 721 727 CHARACTERIZATION OF FEMALE PREFERENCE FUNCTIONS FOR DROSOPHILA MONTANA COURTSHIP SONG AND A TEST OF THE TEMPERATURE COUPLING HYPOTHESIS MICHAEL G. RITCHIE, 1,2 MARI

More information

Evolutionary mismatch of mating preferences and male colour patterns in guppies

Evolutionary mismatch of mating preferences and male colour patterns in guppies Anim. Behav., 1997, 53, 343 351 Evolutionary mismatch of mating preferences and male colour patterns in guppies ANNE E. HOUDE & MELISSA A. HANKES Department of Biology, Lake Forest College (Received 31

More information

Complex Inheritance of Male Courtship Song Characters in Drosophila virilis

Complex Inheritance of Male Courtship Song Characters in Drosophila virilis Behavior Genetics, Vol. 33, No. 1, January 2003 ( 2003) Complex Inheritance of Male Courtship Song Characters in Drosophila virilis Susanna Huttunen 1,2 and Jouni Aspi 1 Received 26 Feb. 2002 Final 3 May

More information

Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety

Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 277 283 Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety Mark W. Blows, 1 * David Berrigan 2,3 and George W. Gilchrist 3 1 Department of Zoology,

More information

The causes of variation in the presence of genetic covariance between sexual traits and preferences

The causes of variation in the presence of genetic covariance between sexual traits and preferences Biol. Rev. (2015), pp. 000 000. 1 doi: 10.1111/brv.12182 The causes of variation in the presence of genetic covariance between sexual traits and preferences Kasey D. Fowler-Finn 1, and Rafael L. Rodríguez

More information

What is sex? What about asexual reproduction? What is sexual reproduction?

What is sex? What about asexual reproduction? What is sexual reproduction? BIOL2007 EVOLUTION OF SEX AND SEXUAL SELECTION Kanchon Dasmahapatra EVOLUTIONARY QUESTIONS ABOUT SEX Today: A) The evolution of sex 1) What is the advantage of sex? B) Ev. of sexual dimorphism - sexual

More information

The Relative Importance of Different Direct Benefits in the Mate Choices of a Field Cricket

The Relative Importance of Different Direct Benefits in the Mate Choices of a Field Cricket University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications in the Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 3-2007 The Relative Importance of Different

More information

Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs. Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs

Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs. Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs Research by Gerhardt et al. (1996). Males call to attract females. Males call to attract females Ribbet! Figure 11.27 Tiny and super cute!

More information

Size-dependent mate preference in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni

Size-dependent mate preference in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2001, 61, 589 595 doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1613, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Size-dependent mate preference in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni ANDREW HINGLE,

More information

Mate choice for indirect genetic benefits: scrutiny of the current paradigm

Mate choice for indirect genetic benefits: scrutiny of the current paradigm Functional Ecology 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd ESSARY REVIEW Mate choice for indirect genetic benefits: scrutiny of the current paradigm J. S. KOTIAHO* and M. PUURTINEN *Department of Biological and

More information

Dissecting the complex genetic basis of mate choice

Dissecting the complex genetic basis of mate choice Dissecting the complex genetic basis of mate choice Stephen F. Chenoweth and Mark W. Blows!"#$%&'$()(*+,(-,.,$/'(&.&01#/#(23(4&$,('+2/',(/#(3%&5-+$(6/$+(7/33/'50$/,#8(9&0,#(:%275',( '24:0,;(#/-.&0#(&.7(7/#:0&1#($+&$('&.('2.#/#$(23(&('24"/.&$/2.(23(&'25#$/'

More information

The sexual selection continuum

The sexual selection continuum Received 3 January 00 Accepted 18 March 00 Published online 11 June 00 The sexual selection continuum Hanna Kokko 1*, Robert Brooks, John M. McNamara 3 and Alasdair I. Houston 4 1 Division of Environmental

More information

Male Mating Success and Survival in the Field with Respect to Size and Courtship Song Characters in

Male Mating Success and Survival in the Field with Respect to Size and Courtship Song Characters in Journal of Insect Behavior, Vol. 8, No. 1, 1995 Male Mating Success and Survival in the Field with Respect to Size and Courtship Song Characters in Drosophila littoralis and D. montana (Diptera: Drosophilidae)

More information

EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY BIOS EXAM #2 FALL 2017

EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY BIOS EXAM #2 FALL 2017 EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY BIOS 30305 EXAM #2 FALL 2017 There are 3 parts to this exam. Use your time efficiently and be sure to put your name on the top of each page. Part I. True (T) or False (F) (2 points

More information

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 1 Outline causes sexual selection vs. natural selection sexual selection during courtship male-male

More information

ELIMINATION OF MUTANT TYPES IN SELECTION EXPERIMENT BETWEEN WILD TYPE AND MUTANT EYE COLOUR IN DROSOPHILA ANANASSAE

ELIMINATION OF MUTANT TYPES IN SELECTION EXPERIMENT BETWEEN WILD TYPE AND MUTANT EYE COLOUR IN DROSOPHILA ANANASSAE 73 Journal of Scientific Research Banaras Hindu University, Varanasi Vol. 56, 2012 : 73-79 ISSN : 0447-9483 ELIMINATION OF MUTANT TYPES IN SELECTION EXPERIMENT BETWEEN WILD TYPE AND MUTANT EYE COLOUR IN

More information

Afirst step in evaluating theoretical models concerned with

Afirst step in evaluating theoretical models concerned with Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 6: 663 669 Female preference functions based on call duration in the gray tree frog (Hyla versicolor) H. Carl Gerhardt, Steven D. Tanner, Candice M. Corrigan, and Hilary

More information

PopGen4: Assortative mating

PopGen4: Assortative mating opgen4: Assortative mating Introduction Although random mating is the most important system of mating in many natural populations, non-random mating can also be an important mating system in some populations.

More information

COMPARATIVE BEHAVIOURAL ANALYSIS OF MATING BETWEEN YELLOW AND WILD TYPE DROSOPHILA OF MELANOGASTER SPECIES GROUP

COMPARATIVE BEHAVIOURAL ANALYSIS OF MATING BETWEEN YELLOW AND WILD TYPE DROSOPHILA OF MELANOGASTER SPECIES GROUP Journal of Scientific Research Vol. 58, 2014 : 45-50 Banaras Hindu University, Varanasi ISSN : 0447-9483 COMPARATIVE BEHAVIOURAL ANALYSIS OF MATING BETWEEN YELLOW AND WILD TYPE DROSOPHILA OF MELANOGASTER

More information

Magic cues versus magic preferences in speciation

Magic cues versus magic preferences in speciation Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2012, 14: 779 785 Magic cues versus magic preferences in speciation Martine E. Maan 1,2,3 and Ole Seehausen 2,3 1 Centre of Behaviour and Neurosciences, Behavioural Biology,

More information

Males and females evolve riskier traits in populations with eavesdropping parasitoids

Males and females evolve riskier traits in populations with eavesdropping parasitoids University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications in the Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 10-2018 Males and females evolve riskier

More information

SEXUAL TRAITS ARE SENSITIVE TO GENETIC STRESS AND PREDICT EXTINCTION RISK IN THE STALK-EYED FLY, DIASEMOPSIS MEIGENII

SEXUAL TRAITS ARE SENSITIVE TO GENETIC STRESS AND PREDICT EXTINCTION RISK IN THE STALK-EYED FLY, DIASEMOPSIS MEIGENII ORIGINAL ARTICLE doi:10.1111/evo.12135 SEXUAL TRAITS ARE SENSITIVE TO GENETIC STRESS AND PREDICT EXTINCTION RISK IN THE STALK-EYED FLY, DIASEMOPSIS MEIGENII Lawrence Bellamy, 1 Nadine Chapman, 1 Kevin

More information

Artificial sexual selection alters allometry in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni (Diptera: Diopsidae)

Artificial sexual selection alters allometry in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni (Diptera: Diopsidae) Genet. Res., Camb. (993), 62, pp. 23-222 With text-figures Copyright 993 Cambridge University Press 23 Artificial sexual selection alters allometry in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni (Diptera:

More information

Sexual Selection. Male and female. Lukas Schärer. Evolutionary Biology Zoological Institute University of Basel

Sexual Selection. Male and female. Lukas Schärer. Evolutionary Biology Zoological Institute University of Basel Sexual Selection Lukas Schärer! Evolutionary Biology Zoological Institute University of Basel 1 8.11.2017 Advanced-level Evolutionary Biology Male and female 2 Summary: Sexual Selection defining sexual

More information

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail.

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Author(s): Veltsos, Paris; Wicker-Thomas, Claude; Butlin, Roger; Hoikkala,

More information

Sexual selection. 1) Sexual dimorphism. 2) Variation in mating success. 3) Sexual selection. 4) Female choice based on male ornaments

Sexual selection. 1) Sexual dimorphism. 2) Variation in mating success. 3) Sexual selection. 4) Female choice based on male ornaments Sexual selection 1) Sexual dimorphism 2) Variation in mating success 3) Sexual selection 4) Female choice based on male ornaments 5) The evolution of female preference 1) Sexual dimorphism http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/file:descent_of_man_-_figure_16.jpg

More information

Mate choice for offspring performance: major bene ts or minor costs?

Mate choice for offspring performance: major bene ts or minor costs? Mate choice for offspring performance: major bene ts or minor costs? Rauno V. Alatalo *, Janne Kotiaho, Johanna Mappes and Silja Parri Department of Biological and Environmental Science, University of

More information

Quantitative genetic models of sexual selection

Quantitative genetic models of sexual selection Review TRENDS in Ecology and Evolution Vol.19 No.5 May 2004 Quantitative genetic models of sexual selection Louise S. Mead and Stevan J. Arnold Department of Zoology, 3029 Cordley Hall, Oregon State University,

More information

Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Classification

Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Classification Introduction 1 Introduction 2 Individuals rarely mate at random for a number of reasons: Dispersal may be limited Individuals may or may not be able to self Individuals may reproduce asexually Individuals

More information

Signal Costs and Constraints

Signal Costs and Constraints Signal Costs and Constraints Costs to senders of signaling Costs to receivers Constraints on senders and receivers Transmission constraints Reading: Ch. 17 Peer evaluation of group projects Please evaluate

More information

Causes and consequences of variation in female mate choice and its relation to sexual conflict in Drosophila melanogaster

Causes and consequences of variation in female mate choice and its relation to sexual conflict in Drosophila melanogaster Wilfrid Laurier University Scholars Commons @ Laurier Theses and Dissertations (Comprehensive) 2016 Causes and consequences of variation in female mate choice and its relation to sexual conflict in Drosophila

More information

The ultrasonic mating signal of the male lesser wax moth

The ultrasonic mating signal of the male lesser wax moth Physiological Entomology (1994) 19, 367-372 The ultrasonic mating signal of the male lesser wax moth W. ANDY SNEDDEN, COLIN R. TOSH* and MICHAEL G. RITCHIE School of Biological and Medical Sciences, University

More information

Meiotic drive and evolution of female choice

Meiotic drive and evolution of female choice Meiotic drive and evolution of female choice K. Reinhold *, L. Engqvist, B. Misof and J. Kurtz Institut fïr Evolutionsbiologie und O «kologie der UniversitÌt Bonn, An der Immenburg 1, D-53121 Bonn, Germany

More information

A test of plasticity in female mating preference in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata)

A test of plasticity in female mating preference in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) A test of plasticity in female mating preference in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) Alberto Corral López Degree project in biology, Master of science (2 years), 2011 Examensarbete i biologi 30 hp till

More information

MATERIALS AND METHODS

MATERIALS AND METHODS 48 M. J. RYAN amount used to produce the signal. However, because of the difficulty in determining energy expenditures associated with acoustic signalling, this comparison has only been made with insects

More information

REPORT Fisherian and ``good genes'' benefits of mate choice: how (not) to distinguish between them

REPORT Fisherian and ``good genes'' benefits of mate choice: how (not) to distinguish between them Ecology Letters, (2001) 4 : 322±326 REPORT Fisherian and ``good genes'' benefits of mate choice: how (not) to distinguish between them Hanna Kokko Division of Environmental and Evolutionary Biology, Institute

More information

Sexual selection may be defined as fitness differences that

Sexual selection may be defined as fitness differences that Behavioral Ecology Vol. 13 No. 6: 791 799 Sexual selection and condition dependence of courtship display in three species of horned dung beetles Janne S. Kotiaho Department of Zoology, University of Western

More information

Condition-dependent sexual selection can accelerate adaptation

Condition-dependent sexual selection can accelerate adaptation Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2003, 5: 867 881 Condition-dependent sexual selection can accelerate adaptation Patrick D. Lorch,* Stephen Proulx, Locke Rowe and Troy Day Department of Zoology, University

More information

Lecture Outline. Darwin s Theory of Natural Selection. Modern Theory of Natural Selection. Changes in frequencies of alleles

Lecture Outline. Darwin s Theory of Natural Selection. Modern Theory of Natural Selection. Changes in frequencies of alleles 1. Basics of Natural Selection Lecture Outline 2. How to test for the key components of natural selection a. Variation b. Heritability c. Can the trait respond to selection? d. What are the selective forces?

More information

Evoked Vocal Responses Change with Experience in Male Physalaemus pustulosus

Evoked Vocal Responses Change with Experience in Male Physalaemus pustulosus Evoked Vocal Responses with Experience in Male Physalaemus pustulosus Author(s): Beth Dawson and Michael J. Ryan Source: Copeia, 2012(4):678-682. 2012. Published By: The American Society of Ichthyologists

More information

Sex Comb Variation in Four Drosophila Species from North India

Sex Comb Variation in Four Drosophila Species from North India International Journal of Biotechnology and Bioengineering Research. ISSN 2231-1238, Volume 4, Number 4 (2013), pp. 329-334 Research India Publications http://www.ripublication.com/ ijbbr.htm Sex Comb Variation

More information

Does Mating with Ginger Root Oil-Exposed Males Confer Fitness Benefits to Female Mediterranean Fruit Flies, Ceratitis capitata (Diptera: Tephritidae)?

Does Mating with Ginger Root Oil-Exposed Males Confer Fitness Benefits to Female Mediterranean Fruit Flies, Ceratitis capitata (Diptera: Tephritidae)? FITNESS PROC. HAWAIIAN OF FEMALE ENTOMOL. MEDFLIES SOC. (2005) 37:65 71 65 Does Mating with Ginger Root Oil-Exposed Males Confer Fitness Benefits to Female Mediterranean Fruit Flies, Ceratitis capitata

More information

Female mating preferences are widely recognized as being

Female mating preferences are widely recognized as being Quantitative genetic correlation between trait and preference supports a sexually selected sperm process Leigh W. Simmons* and Janne S. Kotiaho *Centre for Evolutionary Biology, School of Animal Biology

More information

Attractiveness of sand hoods built by courting male fiddler crabs, Uca musica: test of a sensory trap hypothesis

Attractiveness of sand hoods built by courting male fiddler crabs, Uca musica: test of a sensory trap hypothesis ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2003, 65, 000 000 doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2196 Attractiveness of sand hoods built by courting male fiddler crabs, Uca musica: test of a sensory trap hypothesis JOHN H. CHRISTY, JULIA K.

More information

MULTIPLE GENETIC LINKAGES BETWEEN FEMALE PREFERENCE AND MALE SIGNAL IN RAPIDLY SPECIATING HAWAIIAN CRICKETS

MULTIPLE GENETIC LINKAGES BETWEEN FEMALE PREFERENCE AND MALE SIGNAL IN RAPIDLY SPECIATING HAWAIIAN CRICKETS ORIGINAL ARTICLE doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01007.x MULTIPLE GENETIC LINKAGES BETWEEN FEMALE PREFERENCE AND MALE SIGNAL IN RAPIDLY SPECIATING HAWAIIAN CRICKETS Chris Wiley 1,2 and Kerry L. Shaw 1 1 Department

More information

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30)

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) FILM SESSIONS Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) 1. Talking to Strangers (60 min.) 1 This film surveys some of the general communication signals used by animals from diverse taxa,

More information

Lekking and the Lek Paradox

Lekking and the Lek Paradox Lekking and the Lek Paradox Mating Systems 5. Lekking: One sex (usually males) provides only genes to their mate. No direct benefits are passed to the mate. 6. Cooperative: Some individuals forgo reproduction

More information

Multivariate Post-copulatory Selection and Quantitative Genetics of Drosophila simulans Sex Comb

Multivariate Post-copulatory Selection and Quantitative Genetics of Drosophila simulans Sex Comb Multivariate Post-copulatory Selection and Quantitative Genetics of Drosophila simulans Sex Comb Submitted by Manar S. Maraqa to the University of Exeter as a thesis for the degree of Master by Research

More information

3/26/ Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy. 1. Fecundity

3/26/ Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy. 1. Fecundity Sexual Dimorphism 1. Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy A. sexual reproduction involving the fusion of two dissimilar gametes;individuals producing the

More information

Runaway ornament diversity caused by Fisherian sexual selection (limit cycle speciation sexual traits mate choice)

Runaway ornament diversity caused by Fisherian sexual selection (limit cycle speciation sexual traits mate choice) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 95, pp. 5106 5111, April 1998 Evolution Runaway ornament diversity caused by Fisherian sexual selection (limit cycle speciation sexual traits mate choice) ANDREW POMIANKOWSKI*

More information

THE EVOLUTION OF INDICATOR TRAITS FOR PARENTAL QUALITY: THE ROLE OF MATERNAL AND PATERNAL EFFECTS

THE EVOLUTION OF INDICATOR TRAITS FOR PARENTAL QUALITY: THE ROLE OF MATERNAL AND PATERNAL EFFECTS Vol. 150, No. 5 The American Naturalist November 1997 THE EVOLUTION OF INDICATOR TRAITS FOR PARENTAL QUALITY: THE ROLE OF MATERNAL AND PATERNAL EFFECTS Jason B. Wolf, 1, *, Allen J. Moore, 2 and Edmund

More information

The role of maternal and paternal effects in the evolution of parental quality by sexual selection

The role of maternal and paternal effects in the evolution of parental quality by sexual selection The role of maternal and paternal effects in the evolution of parental quality by sexual selection J. B. WOLF,* E. D. BRODIE III* & A. J. MOORE *Department of Biology, Jordan Hall, Indiana University,

More information

Evolution of asymmetry in sexual isolation: a criticism of a test case

Evolution of asymmetry in sexual isolation: a criticism of a test case Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2004, 6: 1099 1106 Evolution of asymmetry in sexual isolation: a criticism of a test case Emilio Rolán-Alvarez* Departamento de Bioquímica, Genética e Inmunología, Facultad

More information

April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice. Female choice

April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice. Female choice April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice Female choice In general, females are expected to be choosier than males because they have more to lose by making bad mating decisions Costs and Benefits: Possible

More information

Quantitative Genetics

Quantitative Genetics Instructor: Dr. Martha B Reiskind AEC 550: Conservation Genetics Spring 2017 We will talk more about about D and R 2 and here s some additional information. Lewontin (1964) proposed standardizing D to

More information

Adaptation and Optimality Theory

Adaptation and Optimality Theory Adaptation and Optimality Theory Prisoner s Dilemma game: An optimality model What is the optimal strategy when you are playing with different people of unknown reputation? Defect: We call this the evolutionarily

More information

Evolution of frequency-dependent mate choice: keeping up with fashion trends

Evolution of frequency-dependent mate choice: keeping up with fashion trends 1 Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Evolution of frequency-dependent mate choice: keeping up with fashion trends Hanna Kokko 1, Michael D. Jennions 2, Anne Houde 3 1 Laboratory of Ecological and Evolutionary Dynamics,

More information

Female mating preferences for colourful males in a population of guppies subject to high predation

Female mating preferences for colourful males in a population of guppies subject to high predation Journal of Fish Biology (2004) 65, 1154 1159 doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2004.00502.x, available online at http://www.blackwell-synergy.com Female mating preferences for colourful males in a population of

More information

So what is a species?

So what is a species? So what is a species? Evolutionary Forces New Groups Biological species concept defined by Ernst Mayr population whose members can interbreed & produce viable, fertile offspring reproductively compatible

More information

ADVANCES IN THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR, VOL. 36. Preexisting Male Traits are Important in the Evolution of Elaborated Male Sexual Display

ADVANCES IN THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR, VOL. 36. Preexisting Male Traits are Important in the Evolution of Elaborated Male Sexual Display Comp. by: Ssanjay Date:12/8/06 Time:10:42:22 Stage:First Proof File Path:// ADVANCES IN THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR, VOL. 36 AU:2 Preexisting Male Traits are Important in the Evolution of Elaborated Male Sexual

More information

Ch. 23 The Evolution of Populations

Ch. 23 The Evolution of Populations Ch. 23 The Evolution of Populations 1 Essential question: Do populations evolve? 2 Mutation and Sexual reproduction produce genetic variation that makes evolution possible What is the smallest unit of

More information

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail.

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Author(s): Pekkala, Nina; Knott, Emily; Kotiaho, Janne Sakari; Nissinen,

More information

Female Preferences Are Not Altered by Early Acoustic Experience in the Neotropical Frog Physalaemus pustulosus

Female Preferences Are Not Altered by Early Acoustic Experience in the Neotropical Frog Physalaemus pustulosus Female Preferences Are Not Altered by Early Acoustic Experience in the Neotropical Frog Physalaemus pustulosus Author(s): Beth Dawson and Michael J. Ryan Source: Journal of Herpetology, 46(4):535-538.

More information

The quantitative genetics of sexually selected traits, preferred traits and preference: a review and analysis of the data

The quantitative genetics of sexually selected traits, preferred traits and preference: a review and analysis of the data doi: 10.1111/jeb.12483 REVIEW The quantitative genetics of sexually selected traits, preferred traits and preference: a review and analysis of the data A. Y. PROKUDA & D. A. ROFF Department of Biology,

More information

A test of quantitative genetic theory using Drosophila effects of inbreeding and rate of inbreeding on heritabilities and variance components #

A test of quantitative genetic theory using Drosophila effects of inbreeding and rate of inbreeding on heritabilities and variance components # Theatre Presentation in the Commision on Animal Genetics G2.7, EAAP 2005 Uppsala A test of quantitative genetic theory using Drosophila effects of inbreeding and rate of inbreeding on heritabilities and

More information

S18-1 Rapid evolutionary divergence of environment-dependent sexual traits in speciation: a paradox?

S18-1 Rapid evolutionary divergence of environment-dependent sexual traits in speciation: a paradox? 52(Supplement): 315 319, 2006 S18-1 Rapid evolutionary divergence of environment-dependent sexual traits in speciation: a paradox? Alexander V. BADYAEV 1, Emilie C. SNELL-ROOD Dept. of Ecology and Evolutionary

More information

Unifying and Testing Models of Sexual Selection

Unifying and Testing Models of Sexual Selection Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2006. 37:43 66 First published online as a Review in Advance on July 21, 2006 The Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics is online at http://ecolsys.annualreviews.org

More information

Good resources. Why are females choosy? Good resources. Resources from males can tip the scales of relative investment

Good resources. Why are females choosy? Good resources. Resources from males can tip the scales of relative investment Why are females choosy? Good resources direct benefits Good genes indirect benefits Sexy son hypothesis (Fisher) Handicap hypothesis (Zahavi) Good genes for sons, daughters Good resources courtship feeding

More information

Good resources. Why are females choosy? Good resources. Good resources direct benefits Good genes indirect benefits

Good resources. Why are females choosy? Good resources. Good resources direct benefits Good genes indirect benefits Why are females choosy? Good resources direct benefits Good genes indirect benefits Sexy son hypothesis (Fisher) Handicap hypothesis (Zahavi) Good genes for sons, daughters courtship feeding Good resources

More information

THE QUANTITATIVE GENETICS OF MATE CHOICE EVOLUTION: THEORY AND EMPIRICISM. A Dissertation NICHOLAS LAYNE RATTERMAN

THE QUANTITATIVE GENETICS OF MATE CHOICE EVOLUTION: THEORY AND EMPIRICISM. A Dissertation NICHOLAS LAYNE RATTERMAN THE QUANTITATIVE GENETICS OF MATE CHOICE EVOLUTION: THEORY AND EMPIRICISM A Dissertation by NICHOLAS LAYNE RATTERMAN Submitted to the Office of Graduate Studies of Texas A&M University in partial fulfillment

More information

MECHANISMS AND PATTERNS OF EVOLUTION

MECHANISMS AND PATTERNS OF EVOLUTION MECHANISMS AND PATTERNS OF EVOLUTION Evolution What is it again? Evolution is the change in allele frequencies of a population over generations Mechanisms of Evolution what can make evolution happen? 1.

More information

Mechanisms of Evolution. Macroevolution. Speciation. MICROEVOLUTION - A change in the frequency of alleles. Review population genetics Ch. 23.

Mechanisms of Evolution. Macroevolution. Speciation. MICROEVOLUTION - A change in the frequency of alleles. Review population genetics Ch. 23. Mechanisms of Evolution Macroevolution Speciation MICROEVOLUTION - A change in the frequency of alleles. Review population genetics Ch. 23. MACROEVOLUTION - Speciation (or emergence of higher taxonomic

More information

Reinvestigating good genes benefits of mate choice in Drosophila simulans

Reinvestigating good genes benefits of mate choice in Drosophila simulans bs_bs_banner 295..306 Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2012, 106, 295 306. With 5 figures Reinvestigating good genes benefits of mate choice in Drosophila simulans MANMOHAN D. SHARMA, ROBERT

More information

Study guide Lectures 19 (April 4th), 20 (April 11th), and 21 (April 13th).

Study guide Lectures 19 (April 4th), 20 (April 11th), and 21 (April 13th). Study guide Lectures 19 (April 4th), 20 (April 11th), and 21 (April 13th). Lecture 19 1. Define silent substitution? Synonymous substitution? Non-synonymous substitution? Replacement substitution? 2. How

More information

INBREEDING AND ADVERTISEMENT CALLING IN THE CRICKET TELEOGRYLLUS COMMODUS: LABORATORY AND FIELD EXPERIMENTS

INBREEDING AND ADVERTISEMENT CALLING IN THE CRICKET TELEOGRYLLUS COMMODUS: LABORATORY AND FIELD EXPERIMENTS ORIGINAL ARTICLE doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01053.x INBREEDING AND ADVERTISEMENT CALLING IN THE CRICKET TELEOGRYLLUS COMMODUS: LABORATORY AND FIELD EXPERIMENTS Jean M. Drayton, 1,2 Richard N. C. Milner,

More information

Genetic variation in paternal investment in a seed beetle

Genetic variation in paternal investment in a seed beetle ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1998, 56, 953 961 Article No. ar980853 Genetic variation in paternal investment in a seed beetle UDO M. SAVALLI & CHARLES W. FOX The Louis Calder Center and Department of Biological Sciences,

More information

Sex-linked Correlated Responses in Female Reproductive Traits to Selection on Male Eye Span in Stalk-eyed Flies 1

Sex-linked Correlated Responses in Female Reproductive Traits to Selection on Male Eye Span in Stalk-eyed Flies 1 INTEGR. COMP. BIOL., 45:500 510 (2005) Sex-linked Correlated Responses in Female Reproductive Traits to Selection on Male Eye Span in Stalk-eyed Flies 1 GERALD S. WILKINSON, 2 EMILY G. AMITIN, AND PHILIP

More information

Escalation, Retreat, and Female Indifference as Alternative Outcomes of Sexually Antagonistic Coevolution

Escalation, Retreat, and Female Indifference as Alternative Outcomes of Sexually Antagonistic Coevolution vol. 165, supplement the american naturalist may 2005 Escalation, Retreat, and Female Indifference as Alternative Outcomes of Sexually Antagonistic Coevolution Locke Rowe, 1,* Erin Cameron, 2, and Troy

More information

The Origin of Species. Mom, Dad There s something you need to know I m a MAMMAL!

The Origin of Species. Mom, Dad There s something you need to know I m a MAMMAL! The Origin of Species Mom, Dad There s something you need to know I m a MAMMAL! 2007-2008 So what is a species?!! Biological species concept "! defined by Ernst Mayr "! population whose members can interbreed

More information

The genetic control of pheromone production. and response in the pine engraver beetle Ips. pini BARBARA J. HAGER* & STEPHEN A.

The genetic control of pheromone production. and response in the pine engraver beetle Ips. pini BARBARA J. HAGER* & STEPHEN A. Heredity 77 (1996) 100-107 Received 25 October 1995 The genetic control of pheromone production and response in the pine engraver beetle Ips pini BARBARA J. HAGER* & STEPHEN A. TEALE State University of

More information

ethology Broad Selectivity for Courtship Song in the Cricket Gryllus bimaculatus Ethology Lev S. Shestakov & Varvara Yu. Vedenina

ethology Broad Selectivity for Courtship Song in the Cricket Gryllus bimaculatus Ethology Lev S. Shestakov & Varvara Yu. Vedenina international journal of behavioural biology ethology Ethology Broad Selectivity for Courtship Song in the Cricket Gryllus bimaculatus Lev S. Shestakov & Varvara Yu. Vedenina Institute for Information

More information

Evolution of genetic variation for conditiondependent

Evolution of genetic variation for conditiondependent Evolution of genetic variation for conditiondependent traits in stalk-eyed ies Gerald S. Wilkinson and Mark Taper 2 Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 242, USA 2 Department

More information

The Origin of Species

The Origin of Species The Origin of Species Macroevolution: the origin of new taxonomic groups Speciation: the origin of new species 1- Anagenesis (phyletic evolution): accumulation of heritable changes 2- Cladogenesis (branching

More information

Report. Evolutionary Response to Sexual Selection in Male Genital Morphology

Report. Evolutionary Response to Sexual Selection in Male Genital Morphology Current Biology 19, 1442 1446, September 15, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.056 Evolutionary Response to Sexual Selection in Male Genital Morphology Report Leigh

More information

Evolution of female multiple mating: A quantitative model of the sexually selected sperm hypothesis

Evolution of female multiple mating: A quantitative model of the sexually selected sperm hypothesis ORIGINAL ARTICLE doi:10.1111/evo.12550 Evolution of female multiple mating: A quantitative model of the sexually selected sperm hypothesis Greta Bocedi 1,2 and Jane M. Reid 1 1 Institute of Biological

More information

Condition dependence of a multicomponent sexual signal in the field cricket Gryllus campestris

Condition dependence of a multicomponent sexual signal in the field cricket Gryllus campestris ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 23, 65, 721 727 doi:1.16/anbe.23.283 Condition dependence of a multicomponent sexual signal in the field cricket Gryllus campestris HANNES SCHEUBER, ALAIN JACOT & MARTIN W. G. BRINKHOF

More information

Genetic analysis of sex-chromosome arrangement in Drosophila americana; a laboratory exercise for undergraduate or advanced placement students.

Genetic analysis of sex-chromosome arrangement in Drosophila americana; a laboratory exercise for undergraduate or advanced placement students. Bryant F. McAllister Department of Biology University of Texas at Arlington Arlington, TX 76019 bryantm@uta.edu DIS Vol. 8 (December 2001) 227-23 Genetic analysis of sex-chromosome arrangement in Drosophila

More information

Firefly Courtship: Behavioral and Morphological Predictors of Male Mating Success in Photinus greeni

Firefly Courtship: Behavioral and Morphological Predictors of Male Mating Success in Photinus greeni Ethology Firefly Courtship: Behavioral and Morphological Predictors of Male Mating Success in Photinus greeni Kristian Demary*, Constantinos I. Michaelidis & Sara M. Lewis* * Department of Biology, Tufts

More information

Female house crickets, Acheta domesticus, prefer the chirps of large males

Female house crickets, Acheta domesticus, prefer the chirps of large males Anim. Behav., 1997, 54, 1553 1562 Female house crickets, Acheta domesticus, prefer the chirps of large males DAVID A. GRAY Department of Biology, The University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, U.S.A.

More information

Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of evolutionary processes leading to speciation (91605)

Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of evolutionary processes leading to speciation (91605) NCEA Level 3 Biology (91605) 2013 page 1 of 6 Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of evolutionary processes leading to speciation (91605) Assessment Criteria Evidence Achievement

More information

1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts)

1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts) Zoology 357 - Evolutionary Ecology - First Exam 1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts) b. Name two non-selectionist

More information

13.17 Natural selection cannot fashion perfect organisms

13.17 Natural selection cannot fashion perfect organisms 13.17 Natural selection cannot fashion perfect organisms 1. Selection can only act on existing variation Natural selection cannot conjure up new beneficial alleles 2. Evolution is limited by historical

More information

Experimental transplant of guppies. Predator: Pike-cichlid; preys mainly on large guppies

Experimental transplant of guppies. Predator: Pike-cichlid; preys mainly on large guppies Hypothesis: That killfish predation will lead to changes in size among the prey population. EXPERIMENT Reznick and Endler transplanted guppies from pike-cichlid pools to killifish pools and measured the

More information

Parental age effects on the female fitness traits in Drosophila melanogaster

Parental age effects on the female fitness traits in Drosophila melanogaster Egypt. Acad. J. Biolog. Sci., 5(2): 191-196 (2012) A. Entomology Email: egyptianacademic@yahoo.com ISSN: 1687 8809 Received: 21/ 9 /2012 www.eajbs.eg.net Parental age effects on the female fitness traits

More information

Attraction to Orange: Sexiness, Not Gluttony

Attraction to Orange: Sexiness, Not Gluttony Current Issue Previous Issues Science Express Science Products My Science > Jayasooriya et al., pp. 847-848 About the Journal Home > Science Magazine > 3 May 2002 Science 3 May 2002: Vol. 296. no. 5569,

More information

L567: From last time:

L567: From last time: L567: From last time: Fisher: in Fisher s original presentation, he considered female preference for male traits that were initially favored by natural selection. This could lead to a covariance between

More information

The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh: Quantitative Genetic Variation in Females that Mate Late Relative to Sexual Maturation

The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh: Quantitative Genetic Variation in Females that Mate Late Relative to Sexual Maturation vol. 169, no. 3 the american naturalist march 2007 The Cost of Keeping Eggs Fresh: Quantitative Genetic Variation in Females that Mate Late Relative to Sexual Maturation Patricia J. Moore, 1,* W. Edwin

More information