Brain-Mind States: I. Longitudinal Field Study of Sleep/Wake Factors Influencing Mentation Report Length

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Brain-Mind States: I. Longitudinal Field Study of Sleep/Wake Factors Influencing Mentation Report Length"

Transcription

1 MENTATION IN WAKE AND SLEEP Brain-Mind States: I. Longitudinal Field Study of Sleep/Wake Factors Influencing Mentation Report Length Robert Stickgold PhD, April Malia BA, Roar Fosse DSCi, Ruth Propper PhD, and J. Allan Hobson MD Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School Study objectives: To collect and analyze reports of mental activity across sleep/wake states. Design: Mentation reports were collected in a longitudinal design by combining our Nightcap sleep monitor with daytime experience sampling techniques. Reports were collected over 14 days and nights from active and quiet wake, after instrumental awakenings at sleep onset, and after both spontaneous and instrumental awakenings from REM and NREM sleep. Setting: All reports were collected in the normal home, work and school environments of the subjects. Participants: Subjects included 8 male and 8 female undergraduate students (19 26 years of age). Interventions: N/A Measurements and Results: A total of 1,748 reports, averaging 109 per subject, were collected from active wake across the day (n=894), from quiet wake in the pre-sleep onset period (n=58), from sleep onset (n=280), and from later REM (n=269) and nonrem (n=247) awakenings. Median report lengths varied more than 2-fold, in the order REM > active wake > quiet wake > NREM sleep onset. The extended protocol allowed many novel comparisons between conditions. In addition, while spontaneous REM reports were longer than those from forced awakenings, the difference was explained by the time within the REM period at which the awakenings occurred. Finally, intersubject differences in REM report lengths were correlated with similar differences in waking report lengths. Conclusions: The use of the Nightcap sleep monitoring system along with waking experience sampling permits a more complete sampling and analysis of mental activity across the sleep/wake cycle than has been previously possible. Key words: Sleep; dreams; mentation; sleep onset; Nightcap; experience sampling; REM; nonrem INTRODUCTION SYSTEMATIC EFFORTS TO CHARACTERIZE DREAMING AND TO CORRELATE THIS MENTAL STATE with its underlying brain physiology began in earnest with the discovery of REM sleep by Aserinsky and Kleitman in and the apparent correlation of dreaming with REM by Dement and Kleitman in ,3 This led many to conclude the physiological state of REM and the psychological state of dreaming were identical, a conclusion that was soon disproved by the finding that significant amounts of mentation, much of it distinctly dreamlike, is reported after NREM awakening. 4 Some scientists have interpreted this result to indicate that mental activity of a dreamlike nature is continuous across all states, varying only in intensity with the rises and falls of brain activation level 5,6 while others continue to argue for the qualitative differences between REM and NREM mentation. 7-9 Now, more than 50 years after the discovery of the link between REM sleep and dreaming, we are left without even agreement on a definition of dreaming, let alone when it occurs. Both methodological and ideological disputes over how to define dreaming and the appropriate measures of mental activity to Accepted for publication December 2000 Address correspondence to: Dr. Robert Stickgold, Laboratory of Neurophysiology, Massachusetts Mental Health Center, 74 Fenwood Road Boston, MA 02115; Fax: ; rstickgold@hms.harvard.edu study have left the field in disarray. What has become clear is that mental activity can occur in all stages of the sleep/wake cycle, but that the characteristics of this mentation vary dramatically across the cycle. To study these variations in mental activity across the sleep/wake cycle, and ultimately to explain this variation in terms of the robust shifts in the underlying cognitive, physiological, and molecular mechanisms of the brain, it would be desirable to begin with a large collection of mentation reports collected longitudinally, under naturalistic conditions, across the entire sleep/wake cycle. Because of the costs in time and personnel involved in sleep laboratory studies of dreaming, no such study has ever been conducted. Until now, most investigations have looked at only one or two states, and many have focused on a single dream characteristic. In order to obtain longitudinal data under naturalistic conditions and across a wide range of conditions, a method of collecting mentation reports in the field is required. The availability of the Nightcap sleep monitor 10,11 now makes such studies feasible. The Nightcap has been shown to reliably distinguish REM from NREM sleep 11,12 and to permit the rapid and efficient collection of large numbers of sleep mentation reports in the home from instrumental awakenings at sleep onset 13 and from spontaneous REM and NREM awakenings across the night. 14 In a study of the efficacy of the Nightcap, Ajilore et al. (1995) studied 10 subjects both in the sleep laboratory and in the home. Studies in the laboratory combined standard polysomnography with Nightcap recordings. Overall, 87% of one-minute epochs were scored the same by the two methods. Agreements for indi- SLEEP, Vol. 24, No. 2,

2 vidual subjects varied from 81% to 93%. In contrast, inter-rater reliability for the manually scored polysomnograph data was 95%, a value only 8% higher than that of the Nightcap. Mean REM latency was 111 minutes based on polysomnographic records and 107 minutes based on Nightcap data. Similar results were reported by Yun et al. 12 We now report the collection of 1,867 mentation reports from 16 subjects, over 14 days and nights as the subjects moved through their normal work, home, and school environments. 15 These reports, collected by experience sampling across the day and with the Nightcap sleep monitor at night in subjects homes, provide the opportunity to compare reports collected from waking, sleep onset, NREM and REM sleep, and within sleep both from forced awakenings at predetermined points in the sleep cycle and from spontaneous awakenings across the night. The fully transcribed text contains 200,700 words describing mental activity and has now been analyzed for the prevalence and intensity of 30 mentation features. In this first paper, we characterize the data set and then present an analysis of variations in report lengths across sleep/wake states, awakening conditions, circadian phases, times in stage, gender, and individual reporting styles. These comparisons confirm the robustness of physiological-state-dependent variations in mentation, provide new insights into the sources of differences seen between dream studies, and offer an explanation of the difference in length seen between spontaneous and laboratory reports. They also demonstrate that the impact of individual reporting styles on the lengths of reports is correlated across sleep/wake states. METHODS Sixteen undergraduate students (19 26 years of age, 8 male, 8 female) provided informed consent and were paid for participation in the study. Subjects began with a preliminary seven-day training protocol during which they were asked to produce a spontaneous mentation report each morning when they woke up and were also paged once each day for a daytime mentation report. Following completion of the preliminary protocol, subjects carried a pager during the day and wore the Nightcap, a home-based sleep monitoring system, 11 at night for 14 additional days. The 14 days could be consecutive or broken into two or three blocks as individual schedules demanded. Subjects provided dictated mentation reports four times each day when paged, as well as when they awoke from sleep during the 14 nights. In some cases, subjects continued to provide waking reports between blocks, resulting in more than 14 days of waking reports. Reports from the preliminary protocol were not included in the database. Report collection protocol: To obtain daytime mentation reports, subjects were paged four times each day using standard pagers in tone mode. These reports were categorized as active wake (aw) reports. For each day, subjects defined their available time during the day. This was divided into four equal time divisions and subjects were paged at a random time within each quartile of their day. Subjects were given the following instructions: When you are beeped, think back and try to remember what was going on in your mind in the time prior to your being beeped (i.e., anywhere up to fifteen minutes before the beep). Where were you? Who else was there? What were you doing? What were you seeing, thinking, and feeling? What was happening around you? To obtain nocturnal mentation reports, subjects wore the Nightcap sleep-monitoring system, 10,11 connected to a Macintosh computer. 13 The Nightcap monitors head movements with a multi-polar cylindrical mercury switch on the forehead and eyelid movements with a piezoelectric film adhered to the upper eyelid. Sleep/wake states were distinguished for one-minute epochs based on activity in both channels (wake), in neither channel (NREM), or in the eyelid channel only (REM). The algorithm has been described in detail elsewhere. 11 Analyses were performed by the Nightcap, and instrumental awakenings made based on protocol requirements. On four nights, no instrumental awakenings were performed; on two nights instrumental awakenings were made during the sleep onset period, and on the remaining eight nights, instrumental awakenings were made Week One 4X - 4X SO 4X R 4X N 4X - 4X N 4X R d1 d2 d3 d4 d5 d6 d7 Week Two 4X - 4X SO 4X N 4X R 4X - 4X R 4X N d8 d9 d10 d11 d12 d13 d14 d1-14 protocol day wake sleep 4X - SO 4 daytime reports no forced awakenings sleep onset awakenings R N REM then NREM awakenings NREM then REM awakenings Figure 1 Report collection protocol. Reports were collected four times each day during waking as well as following all awakenings from sleep at night. On nights 1, 5, 8, and 12, no forced awakenings were made. On all other nights, forced awakenings were made as indicated. TRC = total recall count; aw = active wake; qw = quiet wake; SO = sleep onset; NREM = nonrem sleep; REM = rapid eye movement sleep SLEEP, Vol. 24, No. 2,

3 from both REM and NREM sleep. The order for awakenings across the 14 days is shown in Figure 1. On sleep onset nights, subjects were repeatedly prompted for reports during the first hour after retiring for the night. Quiet wake reports were collected four and eight minutes after the Nightcap was turned on if the subject had not yet fallen asleep. Additional reports were collected each night after multiple awakenings, 15, 45, 75, 120, and 300 seconds into Nightcap-identified sleep, in a pseudorandom order. 13 Reports were collected continuously for the first hour of the night, after which subjects were allowed to sleep undisturbed through the remainder of the night. Reports collected from the sleep onset period but while the subject was awake were categorized as quiet wake (qw) reports. All others were classified as sleep onset (SO) reports. On REM first nights, subjects were awakened 10 minutes into their second REM period and then 15 minutes into the fourth NREM period of the night. On NREM first nights, they were awakened 15 minutes into their third NREM period and then 10 minutes into their third REM period. The use of the Nightcap to monitor sleep and to awaken subjects has been previously described for each of the awakening protocols: 1) no awakenings; 14 2) sleep onset awakenings; 13 and 3 & 4) awakenings in balanced order from both REM and NREM. 16 Regardless of protocol, reports were collected after all instrumental, spontaneous, and morning awakenings. Subject were given the same instructions for all conditions: When you awaken, think back and try to remember what was going on in your mind in the time prior to waking. Where did you think you were? Who else was there (i.e., in your dream)? What were you doing? What were you seeing, thinking, and feeling? What was happening around you? If no mentation was recalled, subjects were instructed to report this fact. In addition to reports collected in response to computer-directed awakenings, subjects were also instructed to report any mental activity they could remember from the period preceding every spontaneous awakening on the 14 nights. By definition, these represented all awakenings not initiated by the computer and thus included truly spontaneous awakenings as well as those triggered by environmental stimuli, such as roommates making noise or alarm clocks in the morning. Sleep recording: Sleep was monitored with the Nightcap sleep monitoring system Use of this compact recording system can be easily taught to subjects, who run the entire protocol without technical assistance. It s use with college students, 16 clinical patients, 17 and on-board the Mir space station 18 has been previously described. Report lengths: All reports were collected by dictation into hand-held microcassette recorders (Sony, Model M-770V), which automatically recorded the time and date of each report on the tape. Tapes were transcribed verbatim and subsequently edited for Total Recall Count, or TRC, 19 removing all extraverbal utterances (e.g., sighs, coughs, yawns), nonwords (um s and er s), repeated words (e.g., I was thinking about, thinking about ), and secondary elaborations (e.g., descriptions of related waking events). TRCs were obtained using the word-count function of Microsoft Word v5.1. TRC reports were put into a random order and provided to the judges, who were blind to the stage and subject from which each report originated. Statistical analyses: Statistical analyses were performed using StatView v4.5 and SuperANOVA v1.11. Because of the number of reports collected from each subject, as well as the number collected in each major report category, analyses could be carried out in a repeated measures design, with within-subject comparisons as well. Further breakdowns permitted analyses within each of the five major report categories, as described below. Descriptive statistic: All mean report times in Tables 1 and 2 were averaged across all reports in a given category. Similarly, all report frequencies are the ratios of the number of all reports with content to the number of reports in a given category. Median and mean TRCs were calculated for all reports in a given category. In general, median values are presented for clarity, and did not differ substantially from log-transformed values (see below). ANOVA s: All ANOVA s were calculated as repeated measures ANOVA s, with the dependent variable being the mean log- Table 1 Distribution of reports into major categories. The distribution of reports among the five major reporting conditions are given. Mean time of report - average clock time of reports in each condition, with nocturnal times prior to midnight treated as continuous with the subsequent AM period; time into Nightcap recording average time between turning on of Nightcap and initiation of report in minutes; Time into N/REM period average time between computer identified start of a REM or NREM period and the subsequent initiation of reports within that period. TOTAL ACTIVE WAKE QUIET WAKE SLEEP ONSET NREM REM Mean time of report - 5:02 PM 2:04 AM 2:27 AM 6:47 AM 6:59 AM Time into Nightcap recording (min) Time into N/REM period (min) # of reports # of reports with content (90%) (99%) (98%) (87%) (67%) (85%) # reports per subject - mean min max SLEEP, Vol. 24, No. 2,

4 transformed TRC for all reports within condition and subject. The log-transform was calculated as log 10 (TRC+1) 19. Correlations: Pearson correlation coefficients and significance tests were performed on log-transformed TRCs, either on a per report basis (REM and NREM reports; circadian and sleep time effects) or on per subject means (individual differences). RESULTS Distribution of Reports Each of the 16 subjects produced, on average, 117 reports, for a total of 1,867 reports, of which 1,748 (94%, mean=109 per subject) were validated for time and wake/sleep stage. Approximately half of these reports (51%) were obtained from the daytime paging protocol (mean=56 reports per subject), 20% from the sleep onset period (quiet wake and sleep onset; mean=22 per subject), and the remaining 29% from later REM and NREM awakenings (mean=32 per subject). These were almost evenly split between sleep stages (52% REM, 48% NREM). The specific breakdown of reports along with their mean report times is shown in Table 1. The frequencies of recall across sleep states 87%, 85%, and 67% for sleep onset, REM, and NREM are similar to the values obtained by Foulkes and Schmidt, 5 whose recall rates in sleep laboratory studies were 93%, 83% and 67%. Wake reports: Active wake reports were obtained four times a day by paging subjects at pseudorandom times. A total of 881 reports with content were collected. The distribution of reports among the four time periods, as well as mean report lengths, are given in Table 2 (wake). In most cases (90%) subjects produced at least three reports on a given day. The fourth scheduled report was collected on only 73% of the days. Report numbers permit both within and between subject comparisons across the four daily reports. Sleep onset reports: All told, 338 reports were obtained from the sleep onset period, including 58 from periods of quiet wake (qw) and the remainder after periods of of sleep (SO). The distribution of reports among these six conditions is given in Table 2 (sleep onset). Approximately equal numbers of reports (54 58) were obtained from each of the six conditions. Report numbers permitted comparisons across the six conditions, but did not permit within subject comparisons. REM and NREM reports: All told, 516 reports were obtained after the initial sleep onset period, either through computer-initiated forced awakenings at specific times in the REM/NREM cycle or following spontaneous awakenings. The distribution of reports among the four conditions (2 [spontaneous, forced] x 2 [REM, NREM]) is shown in Table 2 (REM/NREM). Approximately equal numbers of REM (n = 269) and NREM (n=247) reports were collected, but with almost twice as many spontaneous reports (n=336) as forced reports (n=180) in each state (REM: 175 vs. 94; NREM: 161 vs. 86). Report numbers permitted comparisons across the four conditions, as well as within subject for total REM and NREM reports and for spontaneous reports, but not for forced awakening reports. Report Length as a Function of State The length of reports was measured both by the total number of words in the reports (word count) and the number of distinct words used to describe the mental content from the period requested (total recall count, TRC). 19 On average, TRCs excluded 21% of the words in reports as redundancies, nonwords, and descriptions of material other than prior mental content. 19 The percent and absolute number of words removed per report varied by stage (repeated measures ANOVA s: for percent reduction, df = 4, F = 18.0, p < ; for word reduction, df=4, F=13.7, p<0.0001). For percentages, NREM report were reduced by Table 2 Distribution of reports into subcategories of major conditions. Columns are as described in Table 1; active wake: 1st through 4th reports are sequential reports on each reporting date; sleep onset: conditions are as described in methods; REM/NREM: REM and NREM conditions are divided into reports collected following Nightcap-calculated awakenings (forced) and reports collected following spontaneous awakening (spont.) Clock Time NC Time N/REM Time REPORTS REPORTS PER SUBJECT (total) w/content w/content (mean) (min) (max) ACTIVE WAKE 1st rpt 1:13 PM % nd rpt 4:08 PM % rd rpt 7:00 PM % th rpt 8:59 PM % SLEEP ONSET quiet wake 1:48 AM 13' % " 2:22 AM 33' % " 2:22 AM 35' % " 2:30 AM 34' % ' 2:12 AM 36' % ' 2:35 AM 37' % REM/NREM spont. REM 7:24 AM 354' 20.7' % spont. NREM 7:10 AM 333' 25.2' % forced REM 5:44 AM 241' 11.8' % forced NREM 5:46 AM 254' 18.9' % SLEEP, Vol. 24, No. 2,

5 35%, while the other four stages were reduced by only 17% 23%. For absolute lengths, qw and SO were reduced by words, while W, REM, and NREM were reduced by words per report. Even larger differences were seen across subjects whose average word reduction ranged from 14% 34% (33 55 words). Statistical analyses were carried out on TRCs, which are thought to more accurately reflect prior mental content. 14,19 TRC varied by major reporting state (Fig. 2; repeated measures ANOVA of log(trc+1), five states x 16 subjects, df=4, F=27.2, p<0.0001). Reports were significantly longer in REM (median=94 words) than quiet wake (median=55 words), sleep onset (median=44 words), and NREM (median=45 words) (contrast analyses, p< and F>26 for qw, SO, and NREM), and active wake reports (median=78 words) were significantly longer than quiet wake (F=11.8, p=0.001), sleep onset (F=43.8, p<0.0001), and NREM (F=37.6, p<0.0001). Quiet wake reports were also longer than sleep onset (F=10.1, p=0.002) and NREM reports (F=7.3, p=0.009). The only comparisons that failed to reach significance were those between NREM and sleep onset (F 100 df=5, F=1.7, p=0.16), although the mean decrease of 20% between waking reports and five min reports was nearly identical to the 21% decrease seen in a larger study of 865 sleep onset reports 13 and TRC showed a similar overall pattern of decreasing length with increasing sleep time. REM and NREM reports: REM and NREM reports were obtained either through forced awakenings at specific times in the REM/NREM cycle or when subjects awakened spontaneously. On average, reports from REM awakenings were twice as long as those from NREM (Fig.3) (median TRC = 94 vs. 45; repeated measures ANOVA of log(trc+1): 2 (sleep stage; REM and NREM) X 2 (awakening protocol; forced and spontaneous), n=16, main effect of sleep stage: df=1, F=48.7, p<0.0001). In contrast, the median length of spontaneous reports was only 27% greater than of forced awakening reports (median TRC=79 vs. 62; mean TRC=135 vs. 97), and the difference failed to reach significance (df=1, F=0.63, p=.44). Interestingly, an interaction between the awakening protocol and sleep stage was significant (df=1, F=7.4, p=0.02), with spontaneous REM reports longer aw qw SO NREM REM State Figure 2 TRC by major reporting state. Values are medians for all reports collected in each of the major reporting states. Error bars = s.e.m s. = 0.23, p = 0.63) and between active wake and REM (F = 3.0, p = 0.09). Thus, while SO reports have often been noted for their similarities to REM reports, their lengths were in fact even shorter than NREM. Wake reports: Wake reports were obtained four times a day by paging subjects at pseudorandom times. Median TRCs for the four times ranged from 74.0 to 79.5 words and did not differ significantly from one another (repeated measures ANOVA of log(trc+1), df=3, F=0.05, p=0.98). Thus, the length of reports obtained from subjects in the wake state did not depend on the time of day at which they were collected. Sleep onset reports: Sleep onset reports obtained from the pre-sleep quiet wake period and after periods of 15, 45, 75, 120, and 300 seconds of sleep varied in median length. Reports collected prior to sleep onset were significantly longer than those collected after sleep onset (paired t-test for log(trc+1), df=14, t=3.1, p=.008). When all six conditions were compared, the differences did not reach significance (repeated measures ANOVA, 0 REM NREM REM NREM Spontaneous Forced Figure 3 Median TRC as a function of REM/NREM stage and awakening protocol. Median TRCs and s.e.m s are shown for all reports collected from sleep reports obtained outside of the sleep onset protocol. Forced reports are those collected following computer-initiated awakenings. Spontaneous reports are reports collected from all other awakenings. than forced REM reports (df=15, t=2.66, p=0.02), but forced REM and NREM reports not significantly different in length (df=13, t=1.0, p=0.31). Because forced awakenings occurred at constant times within the REM/NREM cycle (10 minutes into REM or 15 minutes into NREM), no correlation could be measured between report length and time in REM or NREM. For spontaneous awakenings, however, a wide sample of times within stage was obtained. For these reports, TRC increased significantly with time in REM (log(trc+1), r=0.23, df=141, F=8.0, p=.005). While the median TRC increased by 59% from the first to third quarter hour of the REM period (Fig. 4A), the mean TRC increased by 134% (246 vs. 105 words). This difference between median and mean ratios reflects the presence of exceptionally long outliers in spontaneous reports from the third quarter-hour of the REM periods. In contrast, there was no significant change in TRC with time in SLEEP, Vol. 24, No. 2,

6 Time in stage (min) Figure 4 Median TRC as a function of elapsed time in REM and NREM for spontaneous awakenings. Spontaneous REM and NREM reports were classified by the time from start of the REM (filled circles) or NREM (open circles) period at which they were collected. No value for NREM, min. is given because only two reports were obtained. Error bars = s.e.m s. NREM (r=.07, df=96, F=0.49, p=.48) and the median TRC was actually greatest in the first quarter-hour of NREM (Fig. 4B). These results are similar to those reported previously. 2,3,14,20,21 One possible confound in these results would be if reports collected min into REM periods were generally collected later in the night than those collected 0 15 min into REM periods. But no such correlations were found. Across the four elapsed time in stage intervals for REM in Figure 4, mean time in bed differed by less than half an hour (range= min; ANOVA: df=3, F=0.41, p=0.74). Even less variation was found for NREM reports (range= min; ANOVA: df=2, F=0.10, p=0.96). Differences between forced and spontaneous report lengths: In our data set, the median TRC for spontaneous REM reports was 35% greater than for forced REM reports, and the mean difference was 48%. These values are important for the ongoing discussion of possible differences between home (spontaneous) reports and sleep laboratory (forced) reports. 22,23 Several explanations have been offered for these differences, including differences in: 1) setting; 2) subject populations; 3) number of nights of report collection; 4) time lag between awakening and reporting; 5) detrimental effects of forced awakening on recall and; (6) biased sampling in spontaneous collection protocols. Although our protocol eliminates the first four of these differences, the last two options remain. But our data suggest yet another explanation. Since spontaneous reports were, on average collected 20.7 minutes into the REM period, while forced reports were collected 11.8 minutes into the period (Table 2), and since spontaneous reports collected minutes into the REM period had a median TRC 56% greater than for reports collected in the first 15 minutes (Fig. 4A), the 35% difference seen between spontaneous and forced REM reports might simply reflect the time in REM at which reports were collected. SLEEP, Vol. 24, No. 2, To test this hypothesis, we analyzed 85 spontaneous REM reports from the first 20 minutes of REM periods. Reports were collected, on average, 11.8±0.5 (s.e.m.) min into the periods, the same as the 11.8 minute average seen for all forced reports. For these spontaneous reports, the median TRC was only 91 words, or only a statistically insignificant 14% more than forced REM reports (unpaired t-test of log(trc+1), df=166, t=1.12, p=0.26). Thus, the difference in the lengths of spontaneous and forced REM reports can be largely explained simply by the difference in time into REM at which the reports were collected. It is worth noting that this explanation would predict the observed similarity in lengths of forced and spontaneous NREM reports based on the absence of variations in TRC with elapsed time in NREM. Circadian and sleep time effects: In addition to the analyses presented above, it is also important to ask how report length varies as a function of the time of night at which the report was collected and as a function of the time since retiring to bed. Wake reports: As noted above, there was no significant variation in the length of reports obtained from wake subjects during the day as a function of time of day. Across the four reporting periods, median TRCs ranged only from 74.0 to 79.5 words. Quiet wake and sleep onset: Quiet wake and sleep onset reports were collected for a period of 60 minutes after subjects retired for the night. While report length overall showed a trend toward shorter reports as the hour progressed (r=0.11, df=299, F=3.63, p=0.06), this resulted entirely from the contribution of quiet wake reports at the start of the hour. When reports collected from sleeping subjects (i.e., after seconds of sleep) were analyzed, no correlation with time within the hour was seen (r=0.02, df=242, F=0.11, p=0.74). Similarly, quiet wake reports showed only a trend toward shorter lengths across the hour (r=0.20, df=55, F=2.24, p=0.14), and explained only 4% of the variance. In contrast, significant variations were seen based on clock time. At later times of night, quiet wake and sleep onset reports became progressively shorter (r=0.27, df=299, F=4.82, p<0.0001), decreasing from a linear regression best fit value for TRC of 68 words at 11 PM to only 30 words at 5 AM. Similar results were seen when quiet wake reports and sleep onset reports are analyzed separately, with wake-state reports decreasing in length from 91 words to 30 words and sleep-state reports from 61 words to 30 words. Thus, the later a subject went to bed, the shorter the sleep onset reports became. REM and NREM reports: Spontaneous REM and NREM reports showed mixed dependencies on clock time. Thus a linear regression for REM report length showed trends toward significant increases in TRC with time of night (r=0.11, df=149, p=0.16), varying from 97 words at 2 AM to 198 words at 10 AM, while a similar regression for NREM reports showed no trend (r=0.03, df=110, p=0.72), with a best fit varying only from 80 to 92 words over the same time period. In contrast, when report length was regressed against the elapsed time since subjects went to bed, NREM reports showed a significant correlation (r=0.22, df=110, p<0.02), increasing from 45 words two hours after retiring to 126 words after 9 hours. REM reports showed a similar pattern, increasing from 103 to 211 words in length between two and nine hours after retiring

7 (r=0.13, df=149, p=0.11). Gender differences: For four of the five main report states, women s reports were longer than men s, with the difference being greatest for NREM reports (65%) and smallest for sleep onset reports (-4%). However, a 2 (Gender; Men [n = 8] and Women [ n = 8]) X 4 (Stage; Wake, Sleep Onset, REM, and NREM) repeated measures ANOVA revealed no main effects or interactions of Gender on TRC (main effect of gender: df=1, F=2.15, p=0.16; interaction gender x report state: df=3, F=1.17, p=0.33). Individual differences: It has been known that the length of dream reports obtained from subjects shows large individual differences. But it has remained unclear whether these represent specific differences in dream recall or simply state-independent differences in reporting style. Because of the longitudinal design of the current study, we could address this question directly. If individual differences in dream report lengths reflected reporting style rather than varying intensities of dreaming, we would expect correlations between the lengths of a subjects waking and REM reports, or even between REM and SO reports, and that is what we found. The mean lengths of sleep onset reports for individual subjects was highly correlated with the mean lengths of their REM reports (r=0.74, df=14, p=0.001). The contingency table (Table 3), shows the correlation coefficients and p-values for all pairwise comparisons of active wake, sleep onset, NREM and REM report lengths. Quiet wake was not included because of the small number of reports per subject TRC qw NREM Figure 5 Distribution of qw and NREM report lengths. The percent of reports with TRCs within 25-word bins. NREM reports occur more frequently than qw reports in stippled regions, but at lower rates in the middle, striped region. 40% 30% Table 3 Contingency table for log transformed TRCs by report state. Pearson correlation coefficients and p-values (in parentheses) are given for all pairwise comparisons of mean TRC by subject (n=16) and reporting state. SO REM NREM aw (.11) (.13) (.14) SO (.001) (.046) REM.70 (.002) 20% 10% 0% Time in night (min) Figure 6 Frequency of long NREM reports across the night. The percent of NREM reports collected during each 90-minute interval of the sleep with TRC 125 is plotted for that interval. (mean=3.6) from this state. All three comparisons between sleep stages were significant, and active wake showed trends towards significant correlations with all three sleep stages (p<0.15 for each). Thus report length showed positive correlations at or near significance for all comparisons between the four states, aw, SO, NEM, and REM, again favoring the reporting style hypothesis. DISCUSSION To our knowledge, this is the first longitudinal study of waking, sleep onset, REM, and NREM mentation. Over 1800 reports were obtained from 16 subjects across wake and sleep states during 14 days, and with an average of 109 wake-sleep state validated reports per subject, individual differences could be analyzed. The collection of this large data set under naturalistic conditions was made possible by a combination of two techniques, portable sleep monitoring using the Nightcap and experience sampling. The Nightcap has been shown to reliably distinguish REM from NREM sleep 11,12 and to be effective in the collection of sleep mentation reports from instrumental awakenings at sleep onset 13 and from spontaneous REM and NREM awakenings across the night. 14 The effectiveness of experience sampling has been extensively documented by Csikszentmihalyi. 24 Report lengths: In this first paper we have focused on an analysis of the length of reports, quantified as the total recall count, or TRC. 19 Median lengths of reports from the five states active wake (aw), quiet wake (qw), sleep onset (SO), REM, and NREM varied in the order: REM aw > qw > NREM SO. But the use of a single number to describe report lengths is problematic. While the median TRC for NREM reports is 18% shorter than for qw reports, the mean NREM TRC is 26% longer. This difference reflects the dramatically different distributions of SLEEP, Vol. 24, No. 2,

8 TRCs in the two conditions (Fig.5). NREM is characterized by a relatively large number of very long reports (15% over 150 words, compared to none for qw), and very short reports (23% under 25 words, compared to 12% for qw. The breadth of the NREM TRC distribution results in part from the time of night effect described above. This finding of longer reports later in the sleep period is supported by those of others. 25 Indeed, the longest reports (TRC > 125) show a dramatic time of night effect (Fig. 6). Implications of report length findings: Our findings contradict the activation-only model of sleep mentation which proposes that all mental states exist on a single continuum, controlled by the level of activation of the brain, with a secondary determinant being the presence or absence of external sensory stimuli. But while overall brain activation, as indicated by EEG activity, is lower during NREM than either in quiet wake or at sleep onset, the mean TRC for NREM reports is 51% greater than at sleep onset and 26% larger than in quiet wake. Thus, report length is not positively correlated with level of brain activation across these sleep/wake states. Indeed, for many dream features, our results indicate that NREM mentation lies between sleep onset and REM, with sleep onset mentation closer to wake than to REM mentation. 30 It is only in terms of report frequency that sleep onset mentation shows a similarity to REM. This again argues against an activation-only model of dream construction. REM - NREM differences: Our finding that REM reports are longer than those from NREM is consistent with numerous prior studies. 5,14,19,21,31-33 But despite the robustness of this general finding, both absolute report lengths and the relative differences between the two states vary considerably between studies. Thus, Stickgold et al. 14 and Antrobus 19 found similar ratios of REM to NREM TRCs (4.9 and 4.1, respectively) while the TRC s reported by Stickgold et al. 14 were more than five times those of Antrobus. 19 In contrast, the REM/NREM TRC ratio in the current study is only 2.1. These differences point out the necessity of using longitudinal studies when comparing different sleep/wake states. The source of these inter-study differences has been a matter of some dispute. We have shown here that dream reports collected from REM sleep in the home following spontaneous awakenings show no significant difference in length from those obtained following forced awakenings if and only if reports are collected at similar times within the REM period. These findings are consistent with previous results, 2,3,14 which showed a positive correlation between time into a REM period and report length. We suggest that differences in length previously reported between home and laboratory reports are simply due to differences in the time in REM from which the reports were collected. Wake-state reports: Very little work has been done comparing sleep mentation with waking mentation. Wollman and Antrobus 34 reported that subjects exposed to moderate sensory deprivation produced significantly longer mentation reports than when awakened from REM sleep. In contrast, Williams et al. 35 found that reports of waking fantasies were significantly shorter than spontaneous dream reports collected from the same subjects. In the current study, reports from active wake (median TRC=78) and quiet wake (median TRC=55) were both significantly shorter than those from REM (median TRC=94). Individual differences: We have shown that subjects who tend to produce longer REM dream reports also produce longer SLEEP, Vol. 24, No. 2, reports in all other sleep/wake states. While median REM TRCs for the 16 subjects ranged from words, over half of this variability could be predicted from the length of the subjects sleep onset reports. Thus this variation is not specifically due to enhanced REM mentation. While it is impossible to distinguish whether these individual differences reflect differences in the intensity of their experiences or simply the detail of their descriptions, we can conclude that they do not reflect inter-subject differences in the regulation of the basic sleep/wake mechanism. CONCLUSIONS The study of dream reports provides important clues to how mental activity is altered by changes in brain state. The current study permits us to draw two critical methodological conclusions. First, studies from different laboratories, and even from a single laboratory, can show enormous differences in the length of mentation reports collected under similar conditions. Attempts to compare mentation data from different states collected in different studies is thus likely to be unreliable, even for such simple measures as report frequency and report length. It is critical, therefore, that such studies be conducted in longitudinal design, so that sufficient numbers of reports can be collected from each subject to permit repeated measures analyses of state-dependent variations in mentation. The ability to perform such studies in the home instead of the sleep laboratory opens this area of research to a much wider range of scientists than has hitherto been possible. It now appears that a critical source of the disparity in report lengths for dreams collected in the home and laboratory condition may result not from the sleep environment or the awakening protocol, but simply from how long subjects remain in REM before awakening and reporting. This conclusion suggests that longer and more narratively complex dreams can be collected in the sleep laboratory simply by shifting report times to later in the night as well as later in the REM period, and serves to integrate earlier studies of the effects on report length of the time of night 25 and time in the REM period. 2,3 Finally, the variation in report lengths across sleep/wake states raises questions about activation-only models of human mentation. Such models predict that NREM reports will be shorter than those from sleep onset, since levels of EEG activation are lower in NREM. Instead, the opposite order was observed, with NREM reports significantly longer than those from sleep onset, a mismatch that extends across a range of dream features not reported here. 15,30 The consistent finding of such discrepancies provides strong evidence that brain parameters other than overall brain activation and the presence or absence of sensory input are required to explain the variable nature of mentation across sleep/wake states. By being able to collect large databases of mentation reports in a longitudinal design across multiple sleep/wake states, it is possible to re-address several important questions concerning the natural variations in and physiological underpinnings of mental activity. For example, it would be of interest to probe the similarities and differences between waking and REM mentation more deeply, looking both at intersubject variability and at variations within subjects across distinct wake-state conditions. It would also be valuable to look at links between wake-state and

9 sleep-state emotions, correlating, for example, individual differences in wake-state emotions with similar differences in REM and NREM emotions. The methodology is equally well suited for a more microscopic examination of how daytime emotions affect subsequent dream emotions and, conversely, how dream emotions affect subsequent waking emotions. While all of these questions have been addressed in the past, the ability to collect reports in a longitudinal, multi-state design opens the possibility of more comprehensive, and hopefully more conclusive, investigations of these issues. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Laurie Scott for invaluable technical assistance in collecting the mentation reports, and Elise Franko and Drake Bennett for aid in scoring the reports. This research was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (MH 48,832 and NS 26,985) and the MacArthur Foundation s Mind- Body Network. REFERENCES 1. Aserinsky E, Kleitman N. Regularly occurring periods of ocular motility and concomitant phenomena during sleep. Science 1953;118: Dement W, Kleitman N. The relation of eye movements during sleep to dream activity: An objective method for the study of dreaming. Journal of Experimental Psychology 1957a;53: Dement WC, Kleitman N. Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements, body motility, and dreaming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1957b;9: Foulkes D. Dream reports from different stages of sleep. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 1962;65: Foulkes D, Schmidt M. Temporal sequence and unit composition in dream reports from different stages of sleep. Sleep 1983;6: Antrobus JS. The neurocognition of sleep mentation: rapid eye movements, visual imagery, and dreaming. In R. Bootzin, J. K. D., Schacter. (eds). Sleep and Cognition, Washington DC: American Psychological Association, 1987; pp Nielsen TA. Mentation during sleep: The NREM/REM distinction. In Lydic, R., & Baghdoyan, H. A. (eds). Handbook of behavioral state control: Cellular and molecular mechanisms, Boca Raton: CRC Press, 1999; Hobson JA, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R. Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2000;23: (in press). 9. Nielsen TA Mentation in REM and NREM sleep: A review and possible reconciliation of two models. Behavior and Brain Sciences: 2000 in press. 10. Mamelak AN, Hobson, JA Nightcap: A Home-Based Sleep Monitoring System. Sleep 1989;12: Ajilore OA, Stickgold R, Rittenhouse C, Hobson JA. Nightcap: Laboratory and home-based evaluation of a portable sleep monitor. Psychophysiology 1995;32: Yun B, Obermeyer W H, Benca RM. Nightcap assessment of home and laboratory sleep. Sleep Research 1997;26: Rowley J, Stickgold R, Hobson JA. Eye movement and mental activity at sleep onset. Consciousness and Cognition 1998;7: Stickgold R, Pace-Schott E, Hobson JA. A new paradigm for dream research: Mentation reports following spontaneous arousal from REM and NREM sleep recorded in a home setting. Consciousness and Cognition 1994;3: Stickgold R, Scott L, Malia A, Maher E, Bennett D, Hobson JA. Longitudinal collection of mentation reports over wake-sleep states. SLEEP, Vol. 24, No. 2, Sleep 1998;21(Supplement): Stickgold R, Scott L, Rittenhouse C, Hobson JA. Sleep induced changes in associative memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1999;11: Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R, Matheson JK, Hobson JA. The Nightcap can distinguish patients with severe sleep disordered breathing (sdb) from patients with less severe sdb and controls. Submitted for publication, Sleep Research Stickgold R, Hobson JA. REM sleep and sleep efficiency are reduced during space flight. Sleep 1999;22: S Antrobus JS. REM and NREM sleep reports: comparison of word frequencies by cognitive classes. Psychophysiology 1993;20: Wolpert EA, Trosman, H. Studies in psychophysiology of dreams. I. Experimental evocation of sequential dream episodes. American Association Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 1958;79: Goodenough DR, Lewis HB, Shapiro A, Sleser I. Some correlates of dream reporting following laboratory awakenings. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases 1965;140: Goodenough DR, Lewis HB, SA, Jaret, L, IS. Dream reporting following abrupt and gradual awakenings from different types of sleep. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1965;2: Domhoff GW, Schneider A. Much ado about very little: The small effect sizes when home and laboratory collected dreams are compared. Dreaming 1999;9: Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R. Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease 1987;175: Antrobus JS, Kondo T, Reinsel R. Dreaming in the late morning: Summation of REM and diurnal cortical activation. Consciousness and Cognition 1995;4: Vogel G. An alternative view of the neurobiology of dreaming. Journal of Psychiatry 1978;135: Antrobus JS. Dreaming: cortical activation and perceptual thresholds. Journal of Mind and Behavior 1986;7: Foulkes D. Dreaming and REM sleep. Journal of Sleep Research 1993;2: Moffitt A. Dreaming: Functions and Meanings. Impuls 1995;(3): Fosse R, Stickgold R, Hobson JA. Reciprocal fluctuations in thoughts and hallucinations across sleep and wake states. Psychol Sci. 2000:12: Foulkes D, Rechtschaffen A. Presleep determinants of dream content: effects of two films. Perceptual and Motor Skills 1964;19: Waterman AD, Elton M, Kenemans, JL. Methodological issues affecting the collection of dreams. Journal of Sleep Research 1993;2: Casagrande M, Violani C, Lucidi F, Buttinelli E, Bertini, M Variations in sleep mentation as a function of time of night. International Journal of Neuroscience 1996;85: Wollman MC, Antrobus, JS. Sleeping and waking thought: Effects of external stimulation. Sleep 1986;9: Williams J, Merritt J, Rittenhouse C, Hobson JA. Bizarreness in dreams and fantasies: Implications for the activation-synthesis hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition 1992;1:

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm Sleep, 2(3):299-307 1980 Raven Press, New York The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm L. C. Johnson Naval Health Research Center, San Diego, California Summary: Two studies, using data from fragmented

More information

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle Sleep 10(1):62-68, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components

More information

Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states

Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states 1 Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states J. Allan Hobson, Edward F. Pace-Schott, and Robert Stickgold Laboratory of Neurophysiology, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects ORIGINAL ARTICLES Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects Maria Josè Esposito, Ms.Sc.,Vincenzo Natale, M.D., Ph.D., Miranda Occhionero, M.D., Ph.D., and PierCarla Cicogna, Ph.D.

More information

Understanding One s Dreams

Understanding One s Dreams One s Dreams 1 Understanding One s Dreams Agnes Mukantagara Psychology 1010 Professor: Mike Brammer November 19, 2014 One s Dreams 2 Dreaming is a human experience that psychologists have been studying

More information

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN DREAM RECALL 1

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN DREAM RECALL 1 Journal»l Abnormal Psychology 1966, Vol. 71, No. 1, 52-59 INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN DREAM RECALL 1 HELEN B. LEWIS, DONALD R. GOODENOUGH, ARTHUR SHAPIRO, AND IRVING SLESER 2 State University of New York,

More information

NORAH Sleep Study External Comment Mathias Basner, MD, PhD, MSc

NORAH Sleep Study External Comment Mathias Basner, MD, PhD, MSc NORAH Sleep Study External Comment Mathias Basner, MD, PhD, MSc University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine Page 1 > Mathias Basner Disclaimer The University of Pennsylvania and the German Aerospace

More information

Dreams and Dreaming. What are they about? Where do they come from? What are their functions? How can people use them?

Dreams and Dreaming. What are they about? Where do they come from? What are their functions? How can people use them? Dreams and Dreaming Humans have been curious about dreams throughout recorded history and, we can speculate, well before then. Typical questions include: What are they about? Where do they come from? What

More information

Modules 7. Consciousness and Attention. sleep/hypnosis 1

Modules 7. Consciousness and Attention. sleep/hypnosis 1 Modules 7 Consciousness and Attention sleep/hypnosis 1 Consciousness Our awareness of ourselves and our environments. sleep/hypnosis 2 Dual Processing Our perceptual neural pathways have two routes. The

More information

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle Sleep Sleep is described as a state of unconsciousness or partial consciousness from which a person can be roused by stimulation Period of rest and recovery People spend about a third of their lives sleeping

More information

UNROMANCING THE DREAM...

UNROMANCING THE DREAM... UNROMANCING THE DREAM... Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. W. (1977). The brain as a dream-state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1335-1348.

More information

How did you sleep last night? Were you in a deep sleep or light sleep? How many times did you wake up? What were you doing right before you went to

How did you sleep last night? Were you in a deep sleep or light sleep? How many times did you wake up? What were you doing right before you went to How did you sleep last night? Were you in a deep sleep or light sleep? How many times did you wake up? What were you doing right before you went to bed? Finish presentations Homework for the weekend Interactive

More information

Sleep Disorders. Sleep. Circadian Rhythms

Sleep Disorders. Sleep. Circadian Rhythms Sleep Disorders Sleep The Sleep Wakefulness Cycle: Circadian Rhythms Internally generated patterns of bodily functions that vary over a ~24-hour period Function even in the absence of normal cues 2 Circadian

More information

The Influence of Sleep on Memory

The Influence of Sleep on Memory The Huron University College Journal of Learning and Motivation Volume 48 Issue 1 Article 10 2010 The Influence of Sleep on Memory Andrew Soave Follow this and additional works at: https://ir.lib.uwo.ca/hucjlm

More information

The Concurrence of Fine Muscle Activity and Rapid Eye Movements During Sleep

The Concurrence of Fine Muscle Activity and Rapid Eye Movements During Sleep The Concurrence of Fine Muscle Activity and Rapid Eye Movements During Sleep BILL J. BALDRIDGE, B.A., ROY M. WHITMAN, M.D., and MILTON KRAMER, M.D. Body movements were detected by means of highly sensitive

More information

Sleep Stages Solution v0.1

Sleep Stages Solution v0.1 Sleep Stages Solution v0.1 June 2016 Table of contents Key terms... 2 Introduction... 2 Test Protocol... 3 Inputs and Outputs... 4 Validation and Accuracy... 4 Example datasets... 6 Limitations of the

More information

Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes

Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY Copyright 1989 by The Society for Psychophysiological Research, Inc. Vol. 26, No. 5 Printed in U.S.A. Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes

More information

SOMNAMBULISM: CLINICAL ASPECTS AND PATHOPHYSIOLOGICAL HYPOTHESES. Zadra, A., Desautels, A., Petit, D., Montplaisir, J. (2013) The Lancet Neurology

SOMNAMBULISM: CLINICAL ASPECTS AND PATHOPHYSIOLOGICAL HYPOTHESES. Zadra, A., Desautels, A., Petit, D., Montplaisir, J. (2013) The Lancet Neurology 11.11.2015 SOMNAMBULISM: CLINICAL ASPECTS AND PATHOPHYSIOLOGICAL HYPOTHESES Zadra, A., Desautels, A., Petit, D., Montplaisir, J. (2013) The Lancet Neurology Marion Charmillot Sleep, Cognition and Health

More information

Nightcap Measurement of Sleep Quality in Self-Described Good and Poor Sleepers

Nightcap Measurement of Sleep Quality in Self-Described Good and Poor Sleepers Sleep, 17(8):688-692 1994 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep Quality, Behavioral Scales and Sleep Scoring Nightcap Measurement of Sleep Quality in Self-Described Good

More information

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MENTAL ACTIVITY AT SLEEP ONSET 1

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MENTAL ACTIVITY AT SLEEP ONSET 1 Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1966, Vol. 71, No. 4, 280-286 INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MENTAL ACTIVITY AT SLEEP ONSET 1 DAVID FOULKES, PAUL S. SPEAR, AND JOHN D. SYMONDS University of Wyoming Reports of

More information

Non-REM Lucid Dreaming. Joe Dane Pain Management Center University of Virginia Medical School

Non-REM Lucid Dreaming. Joe Dane Pain Management Center University of Virginia Medical School Non-REM Lucid Dreaming Joe Dane Pain Management Center University of Virginia Medical School As with Pierre and others, I m not going to try to present a lot of the details of our study, nor defend what

More information

Participant ID: If you had no responsibilities, what time would your body tell you to go to sleep and wake up?

Participant ID: If you had no responsibilities, what time would your body tell you to go to sleep and wake up? What does your sleep look like on a typical week? Total Sleep Time: Bedtime:, Sleep onset latency:, Number of Awakenings:, Wake time after sleep onset:, Rise time:, Out of bed:, Naps:? Notes: Is your sleep

More information

What is Consciousness?

What is Consciousness? What is Consciousness? Module 6 Consciousness and the Two-Track Mind: Dual processing, Sleep and Dreams Class Objectives What is consciousness? What are the stages of sleep? How does sleep deprivation

More information

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep Sleep, 6(4):319-325 1983 Raven Press, New York Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep T. Roehrs, F. Zorick, J. Sicklesteel, R. Wittig, and T. Roth Sleep Disorders and Research

More information

SLEEP. -in 1953, first demonstration that brain was active during sleep. 4. Stages 3& 4: Delta Waves, large slow waves; deep sleep

SLEEP. -in 1953, first demonstration that brain was active during sleep. 4. Stages 3& 4: Delta Waves, large slow waves; deep sleep SLEEP DEF: altered state, between waking and unconsciousness, defined by specific patterns of brain activity. I. How much sleep do I need? 1. Long vs. Short Sleepers -across developmental stages -individual

More information

LEARNING DURING SLEEP: AN INDIRECT TEST OF THE ERASURE-THEORY OF DREAMING

LEARNING DURING SLEEP: AN INDIRECT TEST OF THE ERASURE-THEORY OF DREAMING LEARNING DURING SLEEP: AN INDIRECT TEST OF THE ERASURE-THEORY OF DREAMING DICK BIERMAN AND OSCAR WINTER University of Amsterdam Summary. In this study the hypothesis, put forward elsewhere, that dreams

More information

YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep

YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep Sleep is essential to our well-being. When humans fail to get good sleep over a period of time, numerous problems can occur. CAN T SLEEP!!

More information

Webinar Q&A Report Noninvasive, Automated Measurement of Sleep, Wake and Breathing in Rodents

Webinar Q&A Report Noninvasive, Automated Measurement of Sleep, Wake and Breathing in Rodents Webinar Q&A Report Noninvasive, Automated Measurement of Sleep, Wake and Breathing in Rodents Q: How does the PiezoSleep system distinguish between inactivity (quiet wake) vs. sleep? B. O Hara: The piezo

More information

Lucid Dreaming: Physiological Correlates of Consciousness during REM Sleep

Lucid Dreaming: Physiological Correlates of Consciousness during REM Sleep 1986 The Institute of Mind and Behavior, Inc. 25 1 [1211 The Journal of Mind and Behavior Spring and Summer 1986, Volume 7, Numbers 2 and 3 Pages 251 [121J-258 [128] ISSN 0271-0137 ISBN 0-930195-02-7 Lucid

More information

Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services

Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services www.lakeshoresupport.com Humans have biological rhythms, known as Circadian Rhythms (CR) CR refers to cyclical

More information

The Mind in REM Sleep: Reports of Emotional Experience

The Mind in REM Sleep: Reports of Emotional Experience MENTAL ACTIVITY IN SLEEP The Mind in REM Sleep: Reports of Emotional Experience Roar Fosse DSci, 1,2 Robert Stickgold PhD, 2 and J. Allan Hobson MD 2 1 Institute of Psychology, University in Oslo, Oslo;

More information

Assessing the Dream-Lag Effect for REM and NREM Stage 2 Dreams

Assessing the Dream-Lag Effect for REM and NREM Stage 2 Dreams Assessing the Dream-Lag Effect for REM and NREM Stage 2 Dreams Mark Blagrove 1 *, Nathalie C. Fouquet 1, Josephine A. Henley-Einion 1, Edward F. Pace-Schott 2,3, Anna C. Davies 1, Jennifer L. Neuschaffer

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Factors Affecting the Continuity Between Waking and Dreaming: Emotional Intensity and Emotional Tone of the Waking-Life Event

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Factors Affecting the Continuity Between Waking and Dreaming: Emotional Intensity and Emotional Tone of the Waking-Life Event ORIGINAL ARTICLE Factors Affecting the Continuity Between Waking and Dreaming: Emotional Intensity and Emotional Tone of the Waking-Life Event Michael Schredl, Ph.D. Many researchers are advocating the

More information

Physiological Mechanisms of Lucid Dreaming. Stephen LaBerge Sleep Research Center Stanford University

Physiological Mechanisms of Lucid Dreaming. Stephen LaBerge Sleep Research Center Stanford University Physiological Mechanisms of Lucid Dreaming Stephen LaBerge Sleep Research Center Stanford University For those of you here who aren t familiar with the general approach we have been using in our research

More information

Most people need to sleep about 8 hours each night. This is especially true for college students, since the deep sleep that occurs early in the night

Most people need to sleep about 8 hours each night. This is especially true for college students, since the deep sleep that occurs early in the night Most people need to sleep about 8 hours each night. This is especially true for college students, since the deep sleep that occurs early in the night and the dream sleep that occurs later in the night

More information

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology Biological Psychology Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology Consciousness Consciousness is your awareness of how and why you react to your surroundings. During this lesson, you may realize

More information

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology INSOMNIAS INSOMNIAS General criteria for insomnia A. Repeated difficulty with sleep initiation, duration, consolidation or quality. B. Adequate sleep opportunity, persistent sleep difficulty and associated

More information

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include:

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS Introduction Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: Intrinsic sleep disorders (e.g. narcolepsy, obstructive sleep apnoea/hypopnea

More information

Sleep problems 4/10/2014. Normal sleep (lots of variability at all ages) 2 phases of sleep. Quantity. Quality REM. Non-REM.

Sleep problems 4/10/2014. Normal sleep (lots of variability at all ages) 2 phases of sleep. Quantity. Quality REM. Non-REM. Sleep problems Normal sleep (lots of variability at all ages) Quantity Newborns: 16-20 hrs/day 1-yr olds: 12 hrs/day 6-12 yr olds: 10-11 hrs/day Quality Newborns: distributed between day and night 3-months:

More information

states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY

states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY introduction all of us are aware of the many different states of brain activity, including sleep, wakefulness, extreme

More information

The Mood-regulating Function of Sleep

The Mood-regulating Function of Sleep KRAMER/ROTH Mood-regulating Function 563 an increase in the proportion of I ;;?: 2 sec. In Parkinson's disease, the proportions of the three types of intervals were decreased. References 40 DE LEE, C.:

More information

SSRI Treatment suppresses dream recall frequency but increases subjective dream intensity in normal subjects

SSRI Treatment suppresses dream recall frequency but increases subjective dream intensity in normal subjects J. Sleep Res. (2001) 10, 129±142 SSRI Treatment suppresses dream recall frequency but increases subjective dream intensity in normal subjects EDWARD F. PACE-SCHOTT 1, TAMARA GERSH 2, ROSALIA SILVESTRI

More information

Name: Period: Reading Guide Chapter 3: Consciousness & the Two-Track Mind

Name: Period: Reading Guide Chapter 3: Consciousness & the Two-Track Mind Name: Period: Reading Guide Chapter 3: Consciousness & the Two-Track Mind 1. When did the concept of consciousness begin to reemerge in the field of psychology? Explain why. Include all three explanations

More information

Frontal Electroencephalogram Alpha Asymmetry During Sleep: Stability and Its Relation to Affective Style

Frontal Electroencephalogram Alpha Asymmetry During Sleep: Stability and Its Relation to Affective Style Emotion Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 3, No. 4, 401 407 1528-3542/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.4.401 Frontal Electroencephalogram Alpha Asymmetry During

More information

EXAM REVISION. Theories and Issues. Psychology Exam Review

EXAM REVISION. Theories and Issues. Psychology Exam Review EXAM REVISION Theories and Issues The nature of psychology Psychology is the scientific study of behaviour and the mind The approach to psychology is systematic and therefore more accurate than everyday

More information

SLEEP STUDY. Nighttime. 1. How many hours of sleep are you now getting in a typical night?

SLEEP STUDY. Nighttime. 1. How many hours of sleep are you now getting in a typical night? SLEEP STUDY Patient Name: Date of Birth: Date of Study: This questionnaire involves a broad range of sleep and sleep-related behaviors. Your answers enable us to develop a clearer picture of your sleep/wake

More information

Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data

Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation

More information

Dreaming in the Late Morning: Summation of REM and Diurnal Cortical Activation AND GEORGE FEIN

Dreaming in the Late Morning: Summation of REM and Diurnal Cortical Activation AND GEORGE FEIN CONSCIOUSNESS AND COGNITION 4,275-299 (1995) Dreaming in the Late Morning: Summation of REM and Diurnal Cortical Activation JOHN ANTROBUS,' TOSHIAKI KONDo? AND RUTH REINSEL3 The City College of The City

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Motor Area Activation During Dreamed Hand Clenching: A Pilot Study on EEG Alpha Band

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Motor Area Activation During Dreamed Hand Clenching: A Pilot Study on EEG Alpha Band ORIGINAL ARTICLES Motor Area Activation During Dreamed Hand Clenching: A Pilot Study on EEG Alpha Band Daniel Erlacher, M.A., Michael Schredl, Ph.D., Stephen LaBerge, Ph.D. In a single participant physiological

More information

Consciousness. Encounters. Alien Abductions 1/8/2012. Chapter Six. Approximately 20% of college students believe in extraterrestrials (aliens)

Consciousness. Encounters. Alien Abductions 1/8/2012. Chapter Six. Approximately 20% of college students believe in extraterrestrials (aliens) Chapter Six Consciousness Encounters Approximately 20% of college students believe in extraterrestrials (aliens) Almost 1 in 10 claim to have experienced or met an alien Are people being visited and abducted

More information

Sleep is that golden chain that ties health and our bodies together. Thomas Dekker, English dramatist ( ).

Sleep is that golden chain that ties health and our bodies together. Thomas Dekker, English dramatist ( ). Sleep Sleep is that golden chain that ties health and our bodies together. Thomas Dekker, English dramatist (1572-1632). Without adequate sleep people become irritable, have lowered resistance to illness,

More information

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs?

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs? Sleep, 6(1): 10-\5 1983 Raven Press, New York What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs? Peter Rauri and Elaine Olmstead Dartmouth Medical School, Hanover, New Hampshire, U,S,A, Summary: Subjective

More information

Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior 2e. Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst

Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior 2e. Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior 2e Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst Cognitive Domain Consciousness Chapter Module 24 Sleep, Dreams, and Body Rhythms Module 24: Sleep,

More information

Overview: First, here are the rules:

Overview: First, here are the rules: Concept: Students reports of their dream life vary from some saying they never dream to others describing vivid dreams on a frequent basis. You can more closely evaluate your dream life by completing this

More information

Dream Sources, Associative Mechanisms, and Temporal Dimension

Dream Sources, Associative Mechanisms, and Temporal Dimension Sleep 10(1):78-83, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Dream Sources, Associative Mechanisms, and Temporal Dimension Corrado Cavallero Department of Psychology, University

More information

I. What Is Consciousness? Definition Awareness of things inside you and outside you. 3 Meanings of Consciousness

I. What Is Consciousness? Definition Awareness of things inside you and outside you. 3 Meanings of Consciousness I. What Is? Definition Awareness of things inside you and outside you I. What Is? is a construct What is a construct? Something that we can t directly see or measure Examples: self-esteem intelligence

More information

Reading 6: TO SLEEP, NO DOUBT TO DREAM...

Reading 6: TO SLEEP, NO DOUBT TO DREAM... 42 Chapter II Perception and Consciousness FIGURE 5-4 Babies can distinguish between a possible (a) and impossible (b) object at 4 months old. impossible object longer, as if to say, "I can see something's

More information

SLEEP THEORIES. Sleep Protects: Sleeping in the darkness when predators loomed

SLEEP THEORIES. Sleep Protects: Sleeping in the darkness when predators loomed SLEEP THEORIES Sleep Protects: Sleeping in the darkness when predators loomed about kept our ancestors out of harm s way. Sleep Recuperates: Sleep helps restore and repair brain tissue. Sleep Helps Remembering:

More information

Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People

Sleep and Body Temperature in Morning and Evening People Sleep. 8(4):311-318 1985 Raven Press. New York Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People Jean Foret, *Nathalie Touron, *Odile Benoit, and *Ginette Bouard Laboratoire de Physiologie Neurosensorielle

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

Nocturnal road traffic noise and sleep: Day-by-day variability assessed by actigraphy and sleep logs during a one week sampling. Preliminary findings

Nocturnal road traffic noise and sleep: Day-by-day variability assessed by actigraphy and sleep logs during a one week sampling. Preliminary findings Nocturnal road traffic noise and sleep: Day-by-day variability assessed by actigraphy and sleep logs during a one week sampling. Preliminary findings Sandra Pirrera¹*, Elke De Valck¹, Raymond Cluydts¹

More information

The Stability and Variability of Dreaming

The Stability and Variability of Dreaming Sleep, 1(3):319-325 1979 Raven Press, New York The Stability and Variability of Dreaming Milton Kramer and Thomas Roth Dream-Sleep Laboratory, Veterans Administration Hospital. Cincinnati. Ohio Summary:

More information

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value Sleep 10(6):590-599, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1987 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value Brian Levine, Timothy Roehrs, Edward Stepanski, Frank

More information

Waiting Time Distributions of Actigraphy Measured Sleep

Waiting Time Distributions of Actigraphy Measured Sleep The Open Sleep Journal, 2008, 1, 1-5 1 Waiting Time Distributions of Actigraphy Measured Sleep J.E. Slaven*,1, A. Mnatsakanova 1, S. Li 1,2, J.M. Violanti 3, C.M. Burchfiel 1, B.J. Vila 4 and M.E. Andrew

More information

Running head: Exploring the Cognitive Effects of Dreaming

Running head: Exploring the Cognitive Effects of Dreaming Running head: Exploring the Cognitive Effects of Dreaming Undergraduate Research/Creative Work Fellowship: Exploring the Cognitive Effects of Dreaming Jordan Eastridge Indiana University Southeast Statement

More information

Why Do We Sleep At Night?

Why Do We Sleep At Night? 4 Why Do We Sleep At Night? Now that you know what actually happens across the sleep period, let s look at what determines when we sleep. There are two biological determiners of sleep, sleep pressure and

More information

Dreaming of white bears: The return of the suppressed at sleep onset

Dreaming of white bears: The return of the suppressed at sleep onset Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Consciousness and Cognition 17 (2008) 714 724 Consciousness and Cognition www.elsevier.com/locate/concog Dreaming of white bears: The return of the suppressed

More information

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan Sleep 10(4):393-397, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Short Report Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan C. Guilleminault, S. Mondini,

More information

Week 4 Psychology. Theory of mind is an individual s understanding that they and others think, feel, perceive, and have private experiences.

Week 4 Psychology. Theory of mind is an individual s understanding that they and others think, feel, perceive, and have private experiences. Week 4 Psychology Before we explore the concept in detail, let us understand Theory of Mind as well as what Consciousness and Stream of Consciousness are. Theory of mind is an individual s understanding

More information

Sleep and Dreams. Sleep and Dreams. Brain Waves and Sleep Stages Typical Nightly Sleep Stages. Chapter 7 States of Consciousness

Sleep and Dreams. Sleep and Dreams. Brain Waves and Sleep Stages Typical Nightly Sleep Stages. Chapter 7 States of Consciousness Chapter 7 States of Consciousness States of Consciousness Consciousness our awareness of ourselves and our environments Fantasy Prone Personality imagines and recalls experiences with lifelike vividness

More information

States of Consciousness

States of Consciousness States of Consciousness Sleep, Dreams, and Body Rhythms Introduction Consciousness Awareness of oneself and one s environment Body Rhythms Biological Rhythms Periodic physiological fluctuations Can affect

More information

The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration

The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration Sara C. Mednick 1, Ken Nakayama 1, Jose L. Cantero 2, Mercedes Atienza 2, Alicia A. Levin 2, Neha Pathak 2 and Robert Stickgold 2 1 Department

More information

ADHD and Sleep. Dr. Jessica Agnew-Blais MRC Postdoctoral Fellow SDGP Centre Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience

ADHD and Sleep. Dr. Jessica Agnew-Blais MRC Postdoctoral Fellow SDGP Centre Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience ADHD and Sleep Dr. Jessica Agnew-Blais MRC Postdoctoral Fellow SDGP Centre Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience Who am I? Who I am: ADHD researcher Parent Who I am not: Clinician Sleep expert

More information

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection 1 Exclusion criteria and outlier detection 1 2 Supplementary Fig. 1 31 subjects complied with the inclusion criteria as tested during the familiarization session. The upper part of the figure (ovals) indicates

More information

No Rest For the Weary: Some Common Sleep Disorders

No Rest For the Weary: Some Common Sleep Disorders No Rest For the Weary: Some Common Sleep Disorders Student Activity 3G Activity Introduction: It seems Mom does know best ; sleep has been proven to be essential to our health and well-being. In order

More information

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms People typically sleep about 8 hours per day, and spend 16 hours awake. Most people sleep over 175,000 hours in their lifetime. The vast amount of time spent sleeping

More information

Marieb, E.N. (2004), Human Anatomy & Physiology, Pearson Education, Inc, San Francisco, CA, USA, p459

Marieb, E.N. (2004), Human Anatomy & Physiology, Pearson Education, Inc, San Francisco, CA, USA, p459 Neuroscience Tutorial: Sleep and Dreams Discuss the following diagram with the students: Marieb, E.N. (2004), Human Anatomy & Physiology, Pearson Education, Inc, San Francisco, CA, USA, p459 1 Purves,

More information

Do You Get Enough Sleep?

Do You Get Enough Sleep? LP 3A sleep deprivation 1 Do You Get Enough Sleep? Many college students do not get enough sleep. In a survey of more than 200,000 first year students, more than 80% say that stayed up all night at least

More information

Module 22: Fact or Falsehood?

Module 22: Fact or Falsehood? Module 22: Fact or Falsehood? Concept: Before teaching a module or unit, students may have preconceptions about the material. Preconceptions may be false, which can hinder students from learning the material

More information

Activation-synthesis hypothesis. compulsive drug craving and use, despite adverse consequences. Addition. Amphetamines. Barbiturates.

Activation-synthesis hypothesis. compulsive drug craving and use, despite adverse consequences. Addition. Amphetamines. Barbiturates. Activation-synthesis hypothesis Suggests that in the brain engages in a lot of neural activity that is random. Dreams make sense of this activity. Addition compulsive drug craving and use, despite adverse

More information

EEG Electrode Placement

EEG Electrode Placement EEG Electrode Placement Classifying EEG brain waves Frequency: the number of oscillations/waves per second, measured in Hertz (Hz) reflects the firing rate of neurons alpha, beta, theta, delta Amplitude:

More information

March 14. Table of Contents: 91. March 14 & Unit 5 Graphic Organizer part Dream Journal Assignment

March 14. Table of Contents: 91. March 14 & Unit 5 Graphic Organizer part Dream Journal Assignment March 14 Agenda: 1. Graphic Organizer Part 1 for Unit 5 2. Sleep episode Table of Contents: 91. March 14 & 15 92. Unit 5 Graphic Organizer part 1 93. Dream Journal Assignment Homework: 1. Test on Friday!

More information

Chapter Five. Sleep McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved.

Chapter Five. Sleep McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. Chapter Five Sleep 2011 McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. Endocrine System Made up of ductless glands that produce hormones Hormones control various body functions/processes Hormones are

More information

Sleep, Memory, and Learning: What We All Need to Know. What We Need to Know

Sleep, Memory, and Learning: What We All Need to Know. What We Need to Know Sleep, Memory, and Learning: What We All Need to Know Robert Stickgold Harvard Medical School Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center Boston, MA Dr. Stickgold receives research funding from and consults to

More information

The Forty-Eight Hour Day

The Forty-Eight Hour Day Sleep, 1(2):191197 1978 Raven Press, New York The FortyEight Hour Day Wilse B. Webb Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida Summary: Four normal young adult male subjects

More information

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE Shih-Bin Yeh 1 and Carlos Hugh Schenck 2,3 1 Department of Neurology

More information

Psychology - Mr. Duez Unit 3 - Part I Consciousness 3 - Dreams

Psychology - Mr. Duez Unit 3 - Part I Consciousness 3 - Dreams Psychology - Mr. Duez Unit 3 - Part I Consciousness 3 - Dreams Dreams A sequence of images, emotions, and thoughts passing through a sleeping person s mind. Manifest Content: the remembered storyline of

More information

ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ

ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ USADA can grant a Therapeutic Use Exemption (TUE) in compliance with the World Anti- Doping Agency International Standard for TUEs. The TUE application process

More information

Sleep Management in Parkinson s

Sleep Management in Parkinson s Sleep Management in Parkinson s Booklet 1 Introduction An introduction to Sleep Management in Parkinson s Sleep disturbances are commonly experienced by those with Parkinson s, and by the relatives and

More information

Napping and its Effect on Short Term Memory 1

Napping and its Effect on Short Term Memory 1 Poster Presented at the April (2011) 39th Annual Western Pennsylvania Undergraduate Psychology Conference. New Wilmington, PA. Napping and its Effect on Short Term Memory 1 Abbie Leverich Robert Morris

More information

SINGLE-CASE RESEARCH. Relevant History. Relevant History 1/9/2018

SINGLE-CASE RESEARCH. Relevant History. Relevant History 1/9/2018 SINGLE-CASE RESEARCH And Small N Designs Relevant History In last half of nineteenth century, researchers more often looked at individual behavior (idiographic approach) Founders of psychological research

More information

S U P P O R T I N G S L E E P I N A S D V I C T O R I A K N O W L A N D U N I V E R S I T Y O F Y O R K

S U P P O R T I N G S L E E P I N A S D V I C T O R I A K N O W L A N D U N I V E R S I T Y O F Y O R K S U P P O R T I N G S L E E P I N A S D V I C T O R I A K N O W L A N D U N I V E R S I T Y O F Y O R K WHAT IS SLEEP FOR? If sleep doesn t serve an absolutely vital function, it is the greatest mistake

More information

[From NIGHTLIGHT 4(2), Spring 1992, Copyright, The Lucidity Institute]

[From NIGHTLIGHT 4(2), Spring 1992, Copyright, The Lucidity Institute] 1001 Nights Exploring Lucid Dreaming [From NIGHTLIGHT 4(2), Spring 1992, Copyright, The Lucidity Institute] Note: References below are to the issues of NightLight (NL) in which the experiment (X) and the

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may have as many as 200,000

More information

Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming. Elaine M. Hull

Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming. Elaine M. Hull Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming Elaine M. Hull Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping Animals generate 24 hour cycles of wakefulness and sleep. Some animals generate endogenous circannual rhythms (yearly

More information

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep Sleep. 19(7):570-575 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep Mats Gillberg, Garan Kecklund, John Axelsson and

More information

Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Pediatrics Temple University Philadelphia, PA

Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Pediatrics Temple University Philadelphia, PA 37 th Annual Meeting Society of Behavioral Medicine The Impact of School Start Time Change on Adolescents Sleep, Health, Safety, and School Functioning Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology,

More information

Sleep Management

Sleep Management www.working-minds.org.uk Sleep Management Working Minds UK: Dovey Wilday Consultancy Contact: 07941 196379 SLEEP MANAGEMENT Sleep problems occur frequently in people suffering from depression/anxiety.

More information

MODULE 08: SLEEP, DREAMS, AND BODY RHYTHMS CONSCIOUSNESS

MODULE 08: SLEEP, DREAMS, AND BODY RHYTHMS CONSCIOUSNESS MODULE 08: SLEEP, DREAMS, AND BODY RHYTHMS CONSCIOUSNESS CONSCIOUSNESS Awareness of yourself and your environment. CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS Biological rhythms (for example, of temperature and wakefulness) that

More information