Short-term homeostasis of REM sleep assessed in an intermittent REM sleep deprivation protocol in the rat

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Short-term homeostasis of REM sleep assessed in an intermittent REM sleep deprivation protocol in the rat"

Transcription

1 J. Sleep Res. (2002) 11, Short-term homeostasis of REM sleep assessed in an intermittent REM sleep deprivation protocol in the rat ADRIÁN OCAMPO-GARCÉS 1 and ENNIO A. VIVALDI 2 1 Departamento de Neurología y Neurocirugı a, Hospital Clínico Jose Joaquín Aguirre, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile and 2 Programa de Fisiología y Biofísica, Instituto de Ciencias Biomédicas, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile Accepted in revised form 28 November 2001; received 10 January 2001 SUMMARY An intermittent rapid eye movement (REM) sleep deprivation protocol was applied to determine whether an increase in REM sleep propensity occurs throughout an interval without REM sleep comparable with the spontaneous sleep cycle of the rat. Seven chronically implanted rats under a 12 : 12 light dark schedule were subjected to an intermittent REM sleep deprivation protocol that started at hour 6 after lights-on and lasted for 3 h. It consisted of six instances of a 10-min REM sleep permission window alternating with a 20-min REM sleep deprivation window. REM sleep increased throughout the protocol, so that total REM sleep in the two REM sleep permission windows of the third hour became comparable with that expected in the corresponding baseline hour. Attempted REM sleep transitions were already increased in the second deprivation window. Attempted transitions to REM sleep were more frequent in the second than in the first half of any 20-min deprivation window. From one deprivation window to the next, transitions to REM sleep changed in correspondence to the amount of REM sleep in the permission window in-between. Our results suggest that: (i) REM sleep pressure increases throughout a time segment similar in duration to a spontaneous interval without REM sleep; (ii) it diminishes during REM sleep occurrence; and (iii) that drop is proportional to the intervening amount of REM sleep. These results are consistent with a homeostatic REM sleep regulatory mechanism that operates in the time scale of spontaneous sleep cycle. KEYWORDS rapid eye movement sleep, REM sleep propensity, short-term REM sleep homeostasis, sleep cycle, sleep deprivation, ultradian rhythms INTRODUCTION Studies in rats, cats and humans support the existence of a short-term homeostatic process operating within sleep cycles. That process is reflected by the fact that the duration of an interval without rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is proportional to the duration of the preceding REM sleep episode (Barbato and Wehr 1998; Benington and Heller 1994a; Ocampo et al. 1990; Ursin 1970; Vivaldi et al. 1994a). This suggests the presence of an hourglass type mechanism where REM sleep propensity accumulates in the course of an interval Correspondence: Dr Ennio A. Vivaldi, Programa de Fisiología y Biofísica, Instituto de Ciencias Biomédicas, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Independencia 1027, Casilla 70005, Correo 7, Santiago, Chile. Tel.: ; fax: ; evivaldi@machi.med.uchile.cl without REM sleep and dissipates during the next REM sleep episode. Accordingly, if a REM sleep episode is longer, it dissipates more REM sleep propensity and will take longer to buildup again enough pressure to trigger the next episode. In undisturbed sleep, the duration of a REM sleep episode is not correlated with the duration of the previous interval without REM sleep, suggesting that in spontaneous cycling a REM sleep episode is terminated by factors other than homeostatic (Barbato and Wehr 1998; Benington and Heller 1994a; Ocampo et al. 1990; Ursin 1970; Vivaldi et al. 1994a). The duration of spontaneous sleep cycles in the rat averages about 12 min in the rest-predominant lights-on phase (Trachsel et al. 1991; Vivaldi et al. 1994a). The number of attempts at transitions from non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep to REM sleep increases in the absence of REM sleep episodes and may be taken as an index Ó 2002 European Sleep Research Society 81

2 82 A. Ocampo-Garce s and E. A. Vivaldi of the REM sleep propensity (Benington and Heller 1994a; Endo et al. 1997; Siegel and Gordon 1965). The increase in the number of REM sleep attempts is affected by the duration of the preceding REM sleep episode, where longer episodes are followed by intervals with less attempts (Vivaldi et al. 1998). In selective REM sleep deprivation protocols the number of interventions required for maintaining the deprivation increases steadily during at least the two first hours of the protocol (Benington et al. 1994; Endo et al. 1997; Ocampo-Garce s et al. 2000). The REM sleep homeostasis and selective REM sleep deprivation have been studied through deprivation protocols in the hours or days scale (Rechtschaffen et al. 1999). The proposition that a short-term homeostatic mechanism is one of the factors that regulate REM sleep occurrence, allows to approach the issue of selective REM sleep deprivation within a time scale in the order of magnitude of the spontaneous REM sleep NREM sleep cycling in the rat. In the present study, we have designed an intermittent selective REM sleep deprivation protocol. Deprivation was performed at a time within the 12 : 12 light dark schedule when the REM sleep expression is highest and slow wave activity in NREM sleep has already declined (Franken et al. 1991a,b; Trachsel et al. 1988; Vivaldi et al. 1994b). Through this protocol we intended to demonstrate that propensity increases throughout an interval without REM sleep that is within the temporal scale of the rat ultradian sleep cycle; and that there is a relationship between REM sleep propensity as assessed by attempts at transitions and the expression of the REM sleep state. METHODS Data acquisition Seven Sprague Dawley rats weighting g were implanted under deep chlornembutal 3 ml kg )1 i.p. anesthesia with four epidural and two neck muscle stainless steel electrodes. Cortical electrodes allowed the detection of delta and sigma activities. Two midline epidural electrodes allowed theta activity detection. Rats were housed in a cm cage, placed within a sound-isolated cube, under a 12 : 12 light dark schedule, ambient temperature of C, with water and food ad libitum. The data acquisition program sampled four channels looking for a corresponding relevant element: single delta waves (1 4 Hz), trains of three consecutive sigma waves (11 16 Hz), trains of eight theta waves (4 8 Hz), and muscle spikes or movement artifacts. The software quantified the amount of detected elements in each successive 15-s epoch and built a four column table with that data. An off-line state scoring algorithm assigned each epoch to W, NREM sleep or REM sleep. (Roncagliolo and Vivaldi 1991; Vivaldi et al. 1994a,b). The algorithm favored the scoring of REM sleep over NREM sleep, so that an epoch could be assigned to REM sleep even if less than half of it actually belonged to that state. On the other hand, when the state-by-epoch arrays were processed, two consecutive REM sleep epochs were required to establish a REM sleep episode. Isolated single epochs were considered missed REM sleep transitions. Besides automated scoring, the 3-h experimental and baseline sessions were recorded on paper and visually analyzed by two independent scorers. Selective REM sleep deprivation procedure and deprivation protocols Ten days after surgery, rats were adapted for at least 2 days to the recording environment. Two baseline recordings were then obtained in two consecutive days, to be followed by an experimental recording. Hours are denominated according to the light dark schedule, 0 being the first hour after lights-on. When we refer to a given hour we mean the 1-h interval that starts at that time, e.g. hour 6 refers to the interval from 6:00 to 7:00. Recordings started at hour 3 and continued up to hour 11. Figure 1 illustrates the 3-h REM sleep intermittent deprivation protocol (IRD) that comprised hours 6 8. The experimental protocol consisted in the alternation, for six successive times, of 10 min without interventions (REM sleep permission window), and 20 min of selective REM sleep deprivation (REM sleep deprivation window). We use the term bin to refer to each of these six successive sequences of one permission and one deprivation window (Fig. 1). Each rat was subjected up to two times to the protocol. At least 1 day was left between trials. A total of 11 protocols were successfully run. During deprivation, interventions to interrupt REM sleep were prompted by the appearance of the first unequivocal REM sleep signs (burst of sleep spindles, decreasing amplitude of cortical EEG, sustained midline theta trains, and lowering muscle tonus). The 3 5 s of REM sleep the rats could obtain Figure 1. Diagram of the intermittent rapid eye movement (REM) sleep deprivation protocol (IRD). Each pair of a 10-min permission window and its following 20-min selective REM sleep deprivation window constitutes a half-hour bin numbered from 1 to 6.

3 Intermittent REM sleep deprivation in the rat 83 before REM sleep was interrupted were not included in the analysis. The intervention to interrupt REM sleep was the gentle movement of the cage that was standing over a foam cushion, by means of a manually operated mechanism connected to a lever outside the isolation box. Interventions were documented on polygraphic paper. In less than 20% of cases REM sleep ended spontaneously before the intervention, an event designated as a missed REM sleep transition. Rapid eye movement sleep transitions were counted as such whether they were spontaneously or externally interrupted. They had to be separated by at least 15 s from one another to be counted as distinct events. Any ongoing REM sleep episode occurring at the time in which a deprivation window had to start, was immediately interrupted. Data analysis and statistics The values of several variables were calculated, as stated below, for hours or for bins or for halves of deprivation windows. All rats had two baselines that are averaged to obtain a single baseline value. Four rats had two experimental protocols and three rats had one experimental protocol. In the former case, results are treated similarly to baselines, i.e. the two experimental protocols are averaged to obtain a single experimental value. In the latter case the single value is used. The hourly amount of REM sleep and NREM sleep throughout the recording hours (3 11 after lights-on) were compared by ANOVAs with the factors hour (nine levels), protocol (two levels: baseline and experimental) and the interaction of hour and protocol (hour protocol). Posthoc analysis within protocols were performed using the Tukey multiple range test. As in the experimental protocol IRD took place during hours 6 8, the time interval analyzed corresponded to the 3 h occurring before, during and after IRD. In the 20-min deprivation windows within IRD the following variables were analyzed: (a) amount of NREM sleep, (b) number of REM sleep transitions and (c) REM sleep transition index. The REM sleep transition index was quantified as the number of transitions per 10 min of sleep. In the 10-min permission windows within IRD the following variables were analyzed: (a) amount of NREM sleep, (b) amount of REM sleep, (c) fraction of total sleep time (TST) occupied by REM sleep (REM/TST), (d) number of REM sleep episodes, and (e) duration of REM sleep episodes. Transition index and REM/ TST were calculated only if TST amounted to at least 10% of the time window. In order to compare the amount of NREM sleep and of REM sleep transitions between baseline and deprivation windows, the actual baseline values in the 30-min period were multiplied by 2/3. Analogously, to compare amount of NREM sleep, the amount of REM sleep and number of REM sleep episodes between baseline and permission windows, the actual baseline values were multiplied by 1/3. In the cases of number and duration of REM sleep episodes, data were aggregated in two-bin groups. All variables were subjected to ANOVAs with either factor bin (six levels) or two bin aggregate (three levels), factor protocol (two levels: baseline and experimental) and the interaction of bin or two bin aggregate and protocol (bin protocol). Post-hoc analysis within protocols were performed using the Tukey multiple range test. When the ANOVA showed a significant effect for factor protocol or interaction, paired t-tests were performed between corresponding baseline and experimental values. The REM sleep transition index was calculated for the 10-min halves of the deprivation windows. To be included in the analysis both halves had to contain at least 1 min of NREM sleep. An ANOVA was made with factor bin (six levels), half (two levels) and interaction of bin and half (bin half). The average transition index for the first and second half of the deprivation windows in each rat was also calculated and a comparison was made by a paired t-test. A relationship between amount of REM sleep in a permission window and change in REM sleep propensity was sought by regressing the difference in transition index between two consecutive deprivation windows on the amount of REM sleep in the intervening permission window. Ordinary linear and robust regression (Hamilton 1998) methods were used. RESULTS REM and NREM sleep before, during and after the intermittent deprivation protocol The time courses of REM and NREM sleep in hours 3 11 are summarized in Fig. 2 with a 1-h resolution. IRD occurs in hours 6 8 in the experimental protocol. The amount of REM sleep during IRD is the sum of their corresponding two 10-min REM sleep permission windows. The two-way ANOVA for response variable REM sleep content showed significant effects for factors hour (F ¼ 2.91, P ¼ ) and hour protocol (F ¼ 3.47, P ¼ ), but not for factor protocol (F ¼ 0.17, P ¼ ). The average hourly amount of REM sleep was 6.3 min for baseline and 6.1 min for the experimental protocol. Post-hoc analysis within the experimental condition showed that hour 9 (i.e. the first hour after IRD) differed significantly from all others. Hour 6 (i.e. the first hour of IRD) differed significantly from all others except 7 and 3 (with the latter the significance reached P < 0.08). The only other significant pairwise comparison was between hours 7 and 10. In the baseline condition no pairwise comparison reached significance. The paired t-tests showed significant differences for hour 6(P< 0.02) and 9 (P < 0.001), and in hour 7 it resulted in P ¼ The two-way ANOVA for response variable NREM sleep content showed a significant effect only for factor hour (F ¼ 3.79, P ¼ ), not for factors protocol nor hour protocol. The average hourly amount of NREM sleep was 29.1 min for baseline and 28.4 min for the experimental protocol. This significance of the factor hour

4 84 A. Ocampo-Garce s and E. A. Vivaldi a experimental protocol (IRD), a 36% decrease (P < 0.05 in a paired t-test). In hours 9 11 REM sleep average was 17.7 min during baseline and 25.4 min during the experimental protocol, a 44% increase (P < 0.005). For NREM sleep in hours 6 8 the values were, respectively, 98.1 and 90.1, an 8% decrease (P > 0.05). In hours 9 11 the values were, respectively, 72.6 and 74.1, a 2% increase (P > 0.05). NREM sleep, REM sleep, REM sleep transitions and transition index throughout the IRD protocol b Figure 2. Hourly mean values (±SEM) of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (a) and non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep (b) before, during (horizontal hatched bar in the abscissa) and after the IRD protocol (filled symbols), and throughout the corresponding 9 h of baseline (open symbols). The amount of REM sleep during IRD (hours 6 8 in the experimental protocol) is the sum of the two 10-min REM sleep permission windows corresponding to that hour. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests showed significant effects for factors hour and hour protocol in the case of variable REM sleep and in factor hour in the case of NREM sleep. When a significant ANOVA for factor protocol or hour protocol occurred, paired t-tests between baseline and experimental values were performed (results are indicated by * ¼ 0.05 and ** ¼ 0.01). See text for a summary of statistical analysis. for NREM sleep is mainly as a result of lesser amount observed in the last 3 h in both protocols. The amount of REM sleep and the amount of NREM sleep were added within 3-h groups (hours 3 5, 6 8 and 9 11) for baseline and experimental conditions. The ANOVA for REM sleep was not significant for factor 3-h nor factor protocol, but it was significant for the interaction factor (P < 0.01). The ANOVA for NREM sleep was significant for factor 3-h (P < 0.001) and it was not significant for factor protocol nor the interaction. The amount of REM sleep in groups 6 8 was 20.3 min during baseline and 13.0 min during the Figure 3 displays the time course of six variables through the bins that constitute the IRD. Figure 3a confirms that NREM sleep is not affected throughout the six REM sleep deprivation windows of IRD, the ANOVA not being significant for factor protocol (F ¼ 1.33, P ¼ ). Figure 3(b,c) shows the temporal courses of REM sleep transitions and transition index. Factor protocol differs significantly in both variables (F ¼ 47.35, P < in the former and F ¼ 45.83, P < in the latter). Pairwise post-hoc analysis for both variables show that in IRD bin 1 differs significantly from bins 2 6 and that there are no significant differences among these last five bins. This indicates that both variables increase from the first to the second deprivation window and afterwards they stay at that higher than baseline level without undergoing further increase. The paired t-tests showed significant differences in bins 2, 4, 5 and 6 both for variables REM sleep transitions and transition index. Figure 3(d f) shows that throughout the permission windows of the experimental protocol the amount of NREM sleep tends to decrease while the amount of REM sleep, and hence REM/TST follows an opposite course. ANOVAs with model bin, protocol and bin protocol are highly significant for all three variables. Factor protocol is significant for NREM sleep (F ¼ 19.44, P < ), REM sleep (F ¼ 17.26, P ¼ ) and REM/TST (F ¼ 51.27, P < ). For variable REM/TST factors bin (F ¼ 6.25, P ¼ ) and bin protocol (F ¼ 4.45, P ¼ ) are also significant. For this same variable REM/TST, pairwise comparisons are significant for bin 1 vs. 5 and 6, for bin 2 vs. 4, 5 and 6, for bin 3 vs. 6 and for bin 4 vs. 6. The paired t-tests showed significant differences in bins 1 and 6 for variable NREM sleep; in bin 6 for variable REM sleep; and in bins 1, 5 and 6 for variable REM/TST. Putting all variables in context, the increase in REM sleep transitions and transition index in the deprivation window is the first significant change to be assessed, already evident at bin 2, followed by the change in REM/TST in the permission window. Number and duration of REM sleep episodes throughout the IRD protocol Figure 4a shows the incidence of REM sleep episodes throughout the IRD protocol. Results are aggregated in two-bin groups, and the number of episodes is standardized to a 20-min base to compare experimental and baseline results. The graphs

5 Intermittent REM sleep deprivation in the rat 85 a d b e c f Figure 3. Time course of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep (a and d), number of REM sleep transitions (b), transition index (c), rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (e) and REM/total sleep time (TST) (f), during IRD protocol (filled symbols), and corresponding baseline values (open symbols). IRD values in (a) (c) were obtained during the 20-min REM sleep deprivation windows. IRD values in (d) (f) were obtained during the 10-min REM sleep permission windows. As explained in the Methods section, corresponding 30-min baseline values are multiplied by 2/3 in (a) and (b) and by 1/3 in (d) and (e) to allow for comparison with the respective IRD window. The number of observations in each bin that qualified for analysis is shown over the abscissa. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests showed significant effects for factor protocol in all cases except the NREM sleep in deprivation window shown in panel (a). When a significant ANOVA for factor protocol or hour protocol occurred, paired t-tests between baseline and experimental values were performed (results are indicated by * ¼ 0.05 and ** ¼ 0.01). See text for a summary of statistical analysis. suggest that the increase in the amount of REM sleep observed in Fig. 3e is caused by an increase in the duration rather than in the number of episodes. For the latter variable the ANOVA is not significant, whereas for the duration of episodes factors bin group (F ¼ 9.99, P ¼ ) and protocol (F ¼ 13.61, P ¼ ) are both significant. Pairwise comparisons are significant for bin groups 1, 2 vs. both 3, 4 and 5, 6. The paired t-tests showed significant differences in 2-bin groups and for variable REM episode duration. REM sleep transitions within deprivation windows The time course of attempted transitions to REM sleep within deprivation windows was evaluated by comparing the transition index in the two halves. A requirement of at least 1.0 min of NREM sleep in both 10-min halves was established. Figure 5 shows that in all bins a higher transition index is observed in the second half of the deprivation window. The ANOVA was significant for factors half (F ¼ 17.19,

6 86 A. Ocampo-Garce s and E. A. Vivaldi a b Figure 4. Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep episode number (a) and REM sleep episode duration (b) in groups of two REM sleep permission windows (mean ± SEM) throughout IRD (filled symbols) and corresponding baseline hours (open symbols). As explained in the Methods section, in (a) the corresponding 1-h baseline values are multiplied by 1/3 to allow for comparison with the respective IRD data. The number of observations in each bin that qualified for analysis are shown over the abscissa. ANOVA tests showed significant effects for factor protocol in the case of variable episode duration. When a significant ANOVA for factor protocol or hour protocol occurred, paired t-tests between baseline and experimental values were performed (results are indicated by * ¼ 0.05 and ** ¼ 0.01). See text for a summary of statistical analysis. P ¼ ) and bin (F ¼ 2.75, P ¼ ). The average transition indexes of the first and second halves within each animal were also calculated and subjected to a paired t-test that was also significant (n ¼ 7, t ¼ 4.261, P ¼ ). REM sleep transition index in consecutive deprivation windows as a function of REM sleep in the intervening permission window Figure 6 shows the change in REM sleep transition index between two consecutive REM sleep deprivation windows as a function of the REM sleep present in the intervening REM sleep permission window. A negative slope was obtained with ordinary linear regression (n ¼ 42, slope ¼ )0.16, R 2 ¼ 0.15, P < 0.01) and with robust regression (n ¼ 42, slope ¼ )0.18, P < 0.001). The analysis was repeated leaving aside the observations with coordinates (0.75, 13.3), (3.75, 9.57) and (9.00, )3.33). The ordinary linear regression remained significant (n ¼ 39, P ¼ 0.011). Figure 5. Transitions indexes (mean ± SEM) in the first (upward triangles) and second (downward triangles) halves of each 20-min deprivation window are shown. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests were significant for factors half and bin. An average value within each rat for each half window was obtained, their mean values (±SEM) being represented by filled circles at the right of the graph. These average values were subjected to a paired t-test that resulted significant. The number of pairs of observations in each bin that qualified for analysis is shown over the abscissa. See text for a summary of statistical analysis. DISCUSSION The present report explored whether short-term REM sleep homeostasis in the rat can be evidenced by means of selective deprivation of REM sleep in a protocol performed within the order of magnitude of the spontaneous sleep cycle in the rat. The protocol lasted 3 h and consisted of six alternations of a 10-min REM sleep permission and a 20-min REM sleep deprivation window. The main goal of this study was to test two predictions that would derive from the existence of a short-term homeostatic mechanism: (1) that REM sleep pressure increases throughout a time interval comparable with the length of the interval without REM sleep in the spontaneous sleep cycle of the rat, and (2) that during a REM sleep episode a significant reduction of the REM sleep pressure occurs. During deprivation, the homeostatic pressure to REM sleep was operationally assessed as the transition rate, or attempts to enter REM sleep relative to the amount of NREM sleep. Selectivity, effectiveness and rebound of REM sleep deprivation The assessment of REM sleep requires a definition of a threshold duration that will distinguish an episode from an aborted attempt to enter the state. These criteria are relevant for variables such as the total amount of REM sleep, the average length of REM sleep cycles and the mean duration of episodes (Trachsel et al. 1991). As explained in Methods, we

7 Intermittent REM sleep deprivation in the rat 87 deprivation windows would add up to less than 10% of the average amount of REM sleep in permission windows. We did not include this REM sleep expression in our analysis and graphs. To be truly selective, a REM sleep deprivation procedure should not affect NREM sleep. In our study, when baseline and experimental values during IRD are compared, REM sleep is reduced from 20.3 to 13 min and NREM sleep from 98.1 to 90.1 min. Although these changes are similar in absolute terms, they constitute a 36% decrease in the case of REM sleep but only an 8% decrease in the case of NREM sleep, pairwise comparisons being significant only in the former case. Furthermore, we can anecdotally indicate that in two of the seven rats the absolute value of NREM sleep is higher in IRD than in baseline. Effect of REM sleep absence on REM sleep pressure Figure 6. Scatter plot of changes in transition rate between two consecutive REM sleep deprivation windows (following window minus preceding window) as a function of the REM sleep cumulated in the intervening REM sleep permission window. Regression analysis was significant with ordinary linear regression (slope ¼ )0.16, P < 0.01), with robust regression (slope ¼ )0.18, P < 0.001). It remained significant if the observations with coordinates (0.75, 13.3), (3.75, 9.57) and (9.00, )3.33) were not considered. See text for a summary of statistical analysis. detected missed REM sleep transitions and scored states using an epoch length of 15 s. The intermittent deprivation protocol was always started at the same phase of the light dark schedule, 6 h after lights-on. This phase was chosen because it is the time when REM sleep is attaining its highest expression (Franken et al. 1991a; Trachsel et al. 1988; Vivaldi et al. 1994a) and NREM sleep homeostatic pressure is already yielding (Franken et al. 1991b; Trachsel et al. 1988). The expression of REM sleep is much more strongly linked to the circadian oscillator than that of NREM sleep (Dijk and Czeisler 1995). Al least some aspects of REM sleep homeostasis appear to be under a circadian modulation. After a 2-h selective REM sleep deprivation in Wistar rats at two different phases within the light period, only the REM sleep deprivation performed late in the light period induced a homeostatic rebound (Benington et al. 1994). A study based on 24-h REM sleep deprivation ending at different circadian phases concludes that the attempts to enter REM sleep during deprivation, but not the amount of compensatory REM sleep expression, varies according to circadian phase (Wurts and Edgar 2000). Ideally, a REM sleep deprivation protocol should be complete and selective. Concerning the completeness of the deprivation, each time the animals attempted a REM sleep episode in a deprivation window they were not allowed to show signs of REM sleep for more than 3 s, usually theta activity without full cortical desynchronization. According to this estimation, the number of interrupted transitions in The ultradian sleep cycle is operationally defined as a REM sleep episode and the adjacent interval without REM sleep. The interval can be occupied exclusively by NREM sleep, as it is usually the case for humans, or by variable proportions of NREM sleep and wakefulness, as it occurs in the fragmented sleep of most rodents (Zepelin 1994). Failed attempts to enter REM sleep can be detected within the interval. The probability of occurrence of those aborted episodes increases throughout the interval (Benington and Heller 1994a), a fact that can be likened to the increase of attempts to enter REM sleep as the REM sleep deprivation protocol progresses. This analogy suggests that in baseline conditions during a normal interval REM sleep propensity augments as implied by short-term homeostasis. This implication is supported by our results. In fact, the length of our 20-min deprivation window is not far from the scale of spontaneous intervals, and when its 10-min halves are compared, attempts to enter REM sleep are much more frequent in the second half. Effect of REM sleep occurrence on REM sleep pressure The existence of a REM sleep homeostatic mechanism assumes that REM sleep pressure builds up during REM sleep deprivation and dissipates itself by the actual occurrence of REM sleep. The relationship between the magnitude of the debt acquired during deprivation and the compensatory rebound (Rechtschaffen et al. 1999) would indicate that REM sleep rebound ceases once the accumulated pressure has been dissipated (Parmeggiani et al. 1980). At the core of the short-term REM sleep homeostasis hypothesis is the concept that during a REM sleep episode REM sleep pressure is dissipated so that the longer the episode, the less REM sleep pressure remains and the longer the interval to the next REM sleep episode. Observations in the cat (Ursin 1970), rat (Benington and Heller 1994a; Vivaldi et al. 1994b) and human (Barbato and Wehr 1998) are consistent with this hypothesis. Schuller et al. (1999) have communicated that diminution of REM sleep propensity after a 30 or 60 min recovery following

8 88 A. Ocampo-Garce s and E. A. Vivaldi a 2-h selective REM sleep deprivation is determined by the time spent in REM sleep during the recovery interval. Our results indicate that the REM sleep transition index tends to diminish or increase from one deprivation window to the next according to the amount of REM sleep present in the permission window in-between. Effect of selective REM sleep deprivation on the duration of REM sleep episodes The increase in REM sleep during permission windows is explained by an increase in episode duration. In baseline conditions short-term homeostasis is not evidenced by a regulation of REM sleep episode length by the duration of its preceding interval (Barbato and Wehr 1998; Benington and Heller 1994a; Ursin 1970; Vivaldi et al. 1994a). In our intermittent REM sleep deprivation protocol we observed that REM sleep transition rate is already increased in the first hour of the protocol while REM/TST and REM sleep episode durations become significantly augmented at the second hour. These two results suggest that REM sleep episode duration responds homeostatically to the preceding interval without REM sleep only if the latter is long enough for sufficient pressure to accumulate. Organization of the sleep cycle: short-term homeostasis vs. ultradian oscillator Comparative studies of adult mammals indicate that the duration of the sleep cycle is related to constitutive variables such as body weight, metabolic rate and brain size (Zepelin 1994). This fact suggests that the ultradian sleep cycle is linked to metabolic and energetic requirements (Jouvet 1994; Parmeggiani 1990) that must somehow limit the duration of intervals without REM sleep. The periodic signal that prompts the occurrence of a REM sleep episode might be the output of an ultradian pacemaker as originally proposed by Kleitmann (for review, see Kleitman 1982). Rapid eye movement sleep alternation might also result from a REM sleep oscillator system that emerges from the interaction of REM-on and REM-off neurons (Hobson et al. 1975; McCarley and Massaquoi 1986). The issue of an oscillatory vs. an homeostatic control of REM sleep expression has been considered within the context of a discussion on whether the function of REM sleep is related to waking or to NREM sleep (Benington and Heller 1994b). As discussed elsewhere (Vivaldi et al. 1994a), a short-term homeostatic regulation is easier to reconcile with explanations of REM sleep periodic occurrence that assign to REM sleep itself a role in its cyclicity, either because the REM sleep generator may reciprocally interact with other state generators or because there is some functional property specific to REM sleep that may be translated into a feedback loop. The basic tenant of short-term homeostasis states that there must be some mechanism by which REM sleep pressure builds up when REM sleep is absent propitiating a new REM sleep episode, and by which REM sleep pressure dissipates when REM sleep occurs allowing thus for a new interval without the state. The rebound that follows REM sleep deprivation speaks eloquently in favor of a homeostatic mechanism that must be taken into account by any model aimed at explaining REM sleep periodicity. In conclusion, the results of this intermittent REM sleep deprivation protocol indicate that, within a time scale close to that of spontaneous sleep cycles, first, REM sleep pressure increases throughout a time segment not much longer in duration than that of a spontaneously occurring interval without REM sleep; secondly, REM sleep pressure diminishes during the REM sleep episode; and, thirdly, that decrease is related to the amount of REM sleep expressed. These results support the postulate that a short-term REM sleep homeostatic mechanism plays a role in the ultradian organization of the sleep cycle. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank Carolina Llanos and Alberto Rodríguez for their help in this study and Rodrigo Villegas for his advice in the statistical analysis. This work was supported by grants FONDECYT , and Dr Adrián Ocampo-Garce s was supported by a fellowship from the National Council of Science and Technology (CONICYT Chile). REFERENCES Barbato, G. and Wehr, T. A. Homeostatic regulation of REM sleep in humans during extended sleep. Sleep, 1998, 21: Benington, J. H. and Heller, H. C. Does the function of REM sleep concern non-rem sleep or waking? Prog. Neurobiol., 1994b, 44: Benington, J. H. and Heller, H. C. REM-sleep timing is controlled homeostatically by accumulation of REM-sleep propensity in non- REM sleep. Am. J. Physiol., 1994a, 266: R1992 R2000. Benington, J. H., Woudenberg, M. and Heller, H. C. REM-sleep propensity accumulates during 2-h REM-sleep deprivation in the rest period in rats. Neurosc. Lett., 1994, 180: Dijk, D. J. and Czeisler, C. Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans. J. Neurosci., 1995, 15: Endo, T., Schwierin, B., Borbély, A. A. and Tobler, I. Selective and total sleep deprivation: effect on the sleep EEG in the rat. Psychiatry Res., 1997, 66: Franken, P., Dijk, D. J., Tobler, I. and Borbély, A. A. Sleep deprivation in rats: effects on EEG power spectra, vigilance states, and cortical temperature. Am. J. Physiol., 1991a, 261: R198 R208. Franken, P., Tobler, I. and Borbély, A. A. Sleep homeostasis in the rat. simulation of the time course of EEG slow-wave activity. Neurosci. Lett., 1991b, 130: Hamilton, L. C. Statistics with Stata 5. Duxbury Press, Pacific Grove, USA, Hobson, J. A., McCarley, R. W. and Wyzinski, P. W. Sleep cycle oscillation: reciprocal discharge by two brainstem neuronal groups. Science, 1975, 189: Jouvet, M. Paradoxical sleep mechanisms. Sleep, 1994, 17 (Suppl. 1): S77 S83.

9 Intermittent REM sleep deprivation in the rat 89 Kleitman, N. Basic rest-activity cycle 22 years later. Sleep, 1982, 5: McCarley, R. W. and Massaquoi, S. G. A limit cycle mathematical model of the REM sleep oscillator system. Am. J. Physiol., 1986, 251: R1011 R1029. Ocampo, A., Wyneken, U. and Vivaldi, E. A. Active sleep and homeostatic regulation of sleep wake cycle. Arch. Biol. Med. Exp., 1990, 23 (Suppl. 2): R218. Ocampo-Garcés, A., Molina, E., Rodríguez, A. and Vivaldi, E. A. Homeostasis of REM sleep after total and selective sleep deprivation in the rat. J. Neurophysiol., 2000, 84: Parmeggiani, P. L., Cianci, L., Calasso, M., Zamboni, G. and Perez, E. Quantitative analysis of short term deprivation and recovery of desynchronized sleep in cats. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 1980, 50: Parmeggiani, P. L. Homeostatic function of the hypotalamus and control of the wake-sleep cycle. In: M. Mancia (Ed) The Diencephalon and Sleep. Raven Press, New York, 1990: Rechtschaffen, A., Bergmann, B. M., Gilliland, M. A. and Bauer, K. Effect of method, duration, and sleep stage on rebounds from sleep deprivation in the rat. Sleep, 1999, 22: Roncagliolo, M. and Vivaldi, E. A. Time course of rat sleep variables assessed by a microcomputer-generated data base. Brain Res. Bull., 1991, 27: Schuller, J., Borbély, A. A. and Tobler, I. REM sleep regulation in the rat. Sleep Res. Online, 1999, 2 (Suppl. 1): 556. Siegel, J. and Gordon, T. P. Paradoxical sleep: deprivation in the cat. Science, 1965, 148: Trachsel, L., Tobler, I. and Borbély, A. A. Electroencephalogram analysis of non-rapid eye movement sleep in rats. Am. J. Physiol., 1988, 255: R27 R37. Trachsel, L., Tobler, I., Achermann, P. and Borbély, A. A. Sleep continuity and the REM-non REM cycle in the rat under baseline conditions and after sleep deprivation. Physiol. Behav., 1991, 49: Ursin, R. Sleep stage relation within the sleep cycles in the cat. Brain Res., 1970, 11: Vivaldi, E. A., Ocampo, A., Roncagliolo, M., Wyneken, U. and Zapata, A. M. Short-term homeostasis of active sleep and the architecture of sleep in the rat. J. Neurophysiol., 1994a, 72: Vivaldi, E. A., Wyneken, U., Roncagliolo, M., Ocampo, A. and Zapata, A. M. Measures of location and dispersion of sleep state distributions within the circular frame of a 12: 12 light: dark schedule in the rat. Sleep, 1994b, 17: Vivaldi, E. A., Ocampo, A. and Zapata, A. M. Distribution of missed REM transitions throughout the intervals between REM episodes in the rat. J. Sleep Res., 1998, 7 (Suppl. 2): 297. Wurts, S. W. and Edgar, D. M. Circadian and homeostatic control of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep: promotion of REM tendency by the suprachiasmatic nucleus. J. Neurosci., 2000, 20: Zepelin, H. Mammalian sleep. In: M. Kryger, T. Roth and W. C. Dement (Eds) Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. W.B. Saunders Company, Philadelphia, 1994,

SLEEP, SLEEP DEPRIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE

SLEEP, SLEEP DEPRIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 30722 Sleep 5.qxd 6/27/2003 2:16 PM Page 567 SLEEP, SLEEP DEPRIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE Sleep Regulation in the Djungarian Hamster: Comparison of the Dynamics Leading to the Slow-Wave Activity Increase

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects ORIGINAL ARTICLES Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects Maria Josè Esposito, Ms.Sc.,Vincenzo Natale, M.D., Ph.D., Miranda Occhionero, M.D., Ph.D., and PierCarla Cicogna, Ph.D.

More information

Circadian variation of EEG power spectra in NREM and REM sleep in humans: Dissociation from body temperature

Circadian variation of EEG power spectra in NREM and REM sleep in humans: Dissociation from body temperature J. Sleep Res. (1999) 8, 189 195 Circadian variation of EEG power spectra in NREM and REM sleep in humans: Dissociation from body temperature DERK-JAN DIJK Circadian, Neuroendocrine and Sleep Disorders

More information

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association Sleep in patients with a number of sleep disorders and

More information

The Time Course of the Probability of Transition Into and Out of REM Sleep

The Time Course of the Probability of Transition Into and Out of REM Sleep REM Sleep Transitions The Time Course of the Probability of Transition Into and Out of REM Sleep Alejandro Bassi, PhD 1 ; Ennio A. Vivaldi, MD 2 ; Adrián Ocampo-Garcés, MD, PhD 2 1 Department of Computer

More information

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle Sleep 10(1):62-68, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components

More information

Correlations using the NREM-REM sleep cycle frequency support distinct regulation mechanisms for REM and NREM sleep

Correlations using the NREM-REM sleep cycle frequency support distinct regulation mechanisms for REM and NREM sleep J Appl Physiol 93: 141 146, 2002. First published February 22, 2002; 10.1152/japplphysiol.00917.2001. Correlations using the NREM-REM sleep cycle frequency support distinct regulation mechanisms for REM

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Hippocampal recordings. a. (top) Post-operative MRI (left, depicting a depth electrode implanted along the longitudinal hippocampal axis) and co-registered preoperative MRI (right)

More information

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events Summary of Updates in Version 2.1 July 1, 2014 The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) is committed to ensuring that The AASM Manual

More information

Nov versus Fam. Fam 1 versus. Fam 2. Supplementary figure 1

Nov versus Fam. Fam 1 versus. Fam 2. Supplementary figure 1 a Environment map similarity score (mean r ).5..3.2.1 Fam 1 versus Fam 2 Nov versus Fam b Environment cofiring similarity score (mean r ).7.6.5..3.2.1 Nov versus Fam Fam 1 versus Fam 2 First half versus

More information

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm Sleep, 2(3):299-307 1980 Raven Press, New York The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm L. C. Johnson Naval Health Research Center, San Diego, California Summary: Two studies, using data from fragmented

More information

Modelling the relation of body temperature and sleep: importance of the circadian rhythm in skin temperature

Modelling the relation of body temperature and sleep: importance of the circadian rhythm in skin temperature Modelling the relation of body temperature and sleep: importance of the circadian rhythm in skin temperature EUS J.W. VAN SOMEREN NETHERLANDS INSTITUTE FOR BRAIN RESEARCH, AMSTERDAM A close relation between

More information

Piecewise smooth maps for the circadian modulation of sleep-wake dynamics

Piecewise smooth maps for the circadian modulation of sleep-wake dynamics Piecewise smooth maps for the circadian modulation of sleep-wake dynamics Victoria Booth, Depts of Mathematics and Anesthesiology University of Michigan Cecilia Diniz Behn, Dept of Applied Mathematics

More information

The Transcription Factor DBP Affects Circadian Sleep Consolidation and Rhythmic EEG Activity

The Transcription Factor DBP Affects Circadian Sleep Consolidation and Rhythmic EEG Activity The Journal of Neuroscience, January 15, 2000, 20(2):617 625 The Transcription Factor DBP Affects Circadian Sleep Consolidation and Rhythmic EEG Activity Paul Franken, 1 Luis Lopez-Molina, 2 Lysiane Marcacci,

More information

Slow oscillations in human non-rapid eye movement sleep electroencephalogram: effects of increased sleep pressure

Slow oscillations in human non-rapid eye movement sleep electroencephalogram: effects of increased sleep pressure J. Sleep Res. () 9, 8 37 Slow oscillations in human EEG doi:./j.365-869.9.775.x Slow oscillations in human non-rapid eye movement sleep electroencephalogram: effects of increased sleep pressure ALESSIA

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training. Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral training. a, Mazes used for behavioral training. Asterisks indicate reward location. Only some example mazes are shown (for example, right choice and not left choice maze

More information

LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART

LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART POLYSOMNOGRAM, SLEEP STAGE SCORING, INTERPRETATION Sleep Computing Committee, Japanese Society of Sleep Research LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART POLYSOMNOGRAM, SLEEP STAGE SCORING,

More information

Deletion of the Mammalian Circadian Clock Gene BMAL1/Mop3 Alters Baseline Sleep Architecture and the Response to Sleep Deprivation

Deletion of the Mammalian Circadian Clock Gene BMAL1/Mop3 Alters Baseline Sleep Architecture and the Response to Sleep Deprivation CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS Deletion of the Mammalian Circadian Clock Gene BMAL1/Mop3 Alters Baseline Sleep Architecture and the Response to Sleep Deprivation Aaron Laposky, PhD 1 ; Amy Easton, PhD 1 ; Christine

More information

Sleep Homeostasis and Models of Sleep Regulation

Sleep Homeostasis and Models of Sleep Regulation JOURNAL Borbély, Achermann OF BIOLOGICAL / SLEEP RHYTHMS HOMEOSTASIS / December 1999 Sleep Homeostasis and Models of Sleep Regulation Alexander A. Borbély 1 and Peter Achermann Institute of Pharmacology

More information

CLASSIFICATION OF SLEEP STAGES IN INFANTS: A NEURO FUZZY APPROACH

CLASSIFICATION OF SLEEP STAGES IN INFANTS: A NEURO FUZZY APPROACH CLASSIFICATION OF SLEEP STAGES IN INFANTS: A NEURO FUZZY APPROACH J. E. Heiss, C. M. Held, P. A. Estévez, C. A. Perez, C. A. Holzmann, J. P. Pérez Department of Electrical Engineering, Universidad de Chile,

More information

A Modified Method for Scoring Slow Wave Sleep of Older Subjects

A Modified Method for Scoring Slow Wave Sleep of Older Subjects Sleep, 5(2):195-199 1982 Raven Press, New York A Modified Method for Scoring Slow Wave Sleep of Older Subjects Wilse B. Webb and Lewis M. Dreblow Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville,

More information

Fast synaptic inhibition in the mammalian central nervous

Fast synaptic inhibition in the mammalian central nervous Diazepam-induced changes in sleep: Role of the 1 GABA A receptor subtype Irene Tobler*, Caroline Kopp, Tom Deboer, and Uwe Rudolph Institute of Pharmacology and Toxicology, University of Zürich, Winterthurerstrasse

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

Protocol for Rat Sleep EEG

Protocol for Rat Sleep EEG Protocol for Rat Sleep EEG Subjects Male Spraue Dawley rats weihin 250-300 rams at the time of surery are used. Food and water are available ad libitum throuhout the experiment. Rats are roup housed prior

More information

states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY

states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY introduction all of us are aware of the many different states of brain activity, including sleep, wakefulness, extreme

More information

SLEEP STAGING AND AROUSAL. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program)

SLEEP STAGING AND AROUSAL. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program) SLEEP STAGING AND AROUSAL Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program) Scoring of Sleep Stages in Adults A. Stages of Sleep Stage W Stage

More information

Homeostatic Regulation of REM Sleep in Humans During Extended Sleep

Homeostatic Regulation of REM Sleep in Humans During Extended Sleep Homeostatic Regulation of REM Sleep in Humans During Extended Sleep Giuseppe Barbato and Thomas A. Wehr Clinical Psychobiology Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda Md Summary: Benington

More information

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection 1 Exclusion criteria and outlier detection 1 2 Supplementary Fig. 1 31 subjects complied with the inclusion criteria as tested during the familiarization session. The upper part of the figure (ovals) indicates

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi: 10.1038/nature06310 SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION www.nature.com/nature 1 www.nature.com/nature 2 www.nature.com/nature 3 Supplementary Figure S1 Spontaneous duration of wake, SWS and REM sleep (expressed

More information

Physiology of Normal Sleep: From Young to Old

Physiology of Normal Sleep: From Young to Old Physiology of Normal Sleep: From Young to Old V. Mohan Kumar Sree Chitra Tirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology, Thiruvananthapuram 1 What is sleep? As per behavioral criteria: Reduced motor

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

Supplementary Figure 1

Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Miniature microdrive, spike sorting and sleep stage detection. a, A movable recording probe with 8-tetrodes (32-channels). It weighs ~1g. b, A mouse implanted with 8 tetrodes in

More information

Distribution of REM Latencies and Other Sleep Phenomena in Depression as Explained by a Single Ultradian Rhythm Disturbance

Distribution of REM Latencies and Other Sleep Phenomena in Depression as Explained by a Single Ultradian Rhythm Disturbance Sleep, 7(2):126-136 1984 Raven Press, New York Distribution of REM Latencies and Other Sleep Phenomena in Depression as Explained by a Single Ultradian Rhythm Disturbance 0. G. M. Beersma, S. Daan, and

More information

Characterization of Sleep Spindles

Characterization of Sleep Spindles Characterization of Sleep Spindles Simon Freedman Illinois Institute of Technology and W.M. Keck Center for Neurophysics, UCLA (Dated: September 5, 2011) Local Field Potential (LFP) measurements from sleep

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may have as many as 200,000

More information

The Forty-Eight Hour Day

The Forty-Eight Hour Day Sleep, 1(2):191197 1978 Raven Press, New York The FortyEight Hour Day Wilse B. Webb Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida Summary: Four normal young adult male subjects

More information

Webinar Q&A Report Noninvasive, Automated Measurement of Sleep, Wake and Breathing in Rodents

Webinar Q&A Report Noninvasive, Automated Measurement of Sleep, Wake and Breathing in Rodents Webinar Q&A Report Noninvasive, Automated Measurement of Sleep, Wake and Breathing in Rodents Q: How does the PiezoSleep system distinguish between inactivity (quiet wake) vs. sleep? B. O Hara: The piezo

More information

Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes

Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY Copyright 1989 by The Society for Psychophysiological Research, Inc. Vol. 26, No. 5 Printed in U.S.A. Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes

More information

Methodology: Scoring and Computerized Methods

Methodology: Scoring and Computerized Methods Sleep, 17(1):28-36 1994 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Methodology: Scoring and Computerized Methods Scoring Transitions to REM Sleep in Rats Based on the EEG Phenomena

More information

Circadian rhythm and Sleep. Radwan Banimustafa MD

Circadian rhythm and Sleep. Radwan Banimustafa MD Circadian rhythm and Sleep Radwan Banimustafa MD Homeostasis Maintenance of equilibrium by active regulation of internal states: Cardiovascular function (blood pressure, heart rate) Body temperature Food

More information

Sleep stages. Awake Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3 Stage 4 Rapid eye movement sleep (REM) Slow wave sleep (NREM)

Sleep stages. Awake Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3 Stage 4 Rapid eye movement sleep (REM) Slow wave sleep (NREM) Sleep stages Awake Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3 Stage 4 Rapid eye movement sleep (REM) Slow wave sleep (NREM) EEG waves EEG Electrode Placement Classifying EEG brain waves Frequency: the number of oscillations/waves

More information

Quantitative measurements of sleepiness

Quantitative measurements of sleepiness Quantitative measurements of sleepiness Väsymyksen kvantitatiiviset mittausmenetelmät Pia Forsman, PhD Department of Physics University of Helsinki Week LECTURE, Pia, D104 Tue, 12:15-14:00 3 13.1 Safety,

More information

from sleep to attention lecture 4 April 9, 2012 control of sleep/wake state production II

from sleep to attention lecture 4 April 9, 2012 control of sleep/wake state production II from sleep to attention lecture 4 April 9, 2012 control of sleep/wake state production II "From the moment of my birth, the angels of anxiety, worry, and death stood at my side, followed me out when I

More information

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4.

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4. Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4. Jude F. Mitchell, Kristy A. Sundberg, and John H. Reynolds Systems Neurobiology Lab, The Salk Institute,

More information

Normal sleep mechanisms & why do we sleep?

Normal sleep mechanisms & why do we sleep? 4 rd Congress of the European Academy of Neurology Lisbon, Portugal, June 16-19, 2018 Teaching Course 18 Basics of sleep medicine - Level 1 Normal sleep mechanisms & why do we sleep? Rolf Fronczek Leiden,

More information

Genetic variation in EEG activity during sleep in inbred mice

Genetic variation in EEG activity during sleep in inbred mice Genetic variation in EEG activity during sleep in inbred mice PAUL FRANKEN, ALAIN MALAFOSSE, AND MEHDI TAFTI Biochemistry and Neurophysiology Unit, Department of Psychiatry, University of Geneva, CH-1225

More information

Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms. Reading: BCP Chapter 19

Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms. Reading: BCP Chapter 19 Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms Reading: BCP Chapter 19 Brain Rhythms and Sleep Earth has a rhythmic environment. For example, day and night cycle back and forth, tides ebb and flow and temperature varies

More information

EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122

EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122 EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122 Zoltán Lelkes Electroencenphalography Hans Berger pen time amplifier electrodes 1 The waves of the EEG gamma > 30 Hz beta: 13-30 Hz Mental activity:

More information

Interindividual differences in the dynamics of the homeostatic process are trait-like and distinct for sleep versus wakefulness

Interindividual differences in the dynamics of the homeostatic process are trait-like and distinct for sleep versus wakefulness J Sleep Res. (2017) 26, 171 178 Dynamics of sleep-wake homeostasis Interindividual differences in the dynamics of the homeostatic process are trait-like and distinct for sleep versus wakefulness THOMAS

More information

Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms

Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms Keywords: 8853 Journal of Physiology (1999), 516.2, pp. 611 627 611 Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms

More information

Correlation Dimension versus Fractal Exponent During Sleep Onset

Correlation Dimension versus Fractal Exponent During Sleep Onset Correlation Dimension versus Fractal Exponent During Sleep Onset K. Šušmáková Institute of Measurement Science, Slovak Academy of Sciences Dúbravská cesta 9, 84 19 Bratislava, Slovak Republic E-mail: umersusm@savba.sk

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Large-scale calcium imaging in vivo.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Large-scale calcium imaging in vivo. Supplementary Figure 1 Large-scale calcium imaging in vivo. (a) Schematic illustration of the in vivo camera imaging set-up for large-scale calcium imaging. (b) High-magnification two-photon image from

More information

Grouping of Spindle Activity during Slow Oscillations in Human Non-Rapid Eye Movement Sleep

Grouping of Spindle Activity during Slow Oscillations in Human Non-Rapid Eye Movement Sleep The Journal of Neuroscience, December 15, 2002, 22(24):10941 10947 Grouping of Spindle Activity during Slow Oscillations in Human Non-Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Matthias Mölle, Lisa Marshall, Steffen Gais,

More information

Temporal patterning of neural synchrony in the basal ganglia in Parkinson s disease

Temporal patterning of neural synchrony in the basal ganglia in Parkinson s disease Temporal patterning of neural synchrony in the basal ganglia in Parkinson s disease Shivakeshavan Ratnadurai-Giridharan 1, S. Elizabeth Zauber 2, Robert M. Worth 1,3, Thomas Witt 3, Sungwoo Ahn 1,5, Leonid

More information

EEG Electrode Placement

EEG Electrode Placement EEG Electrode Placement Classifying EEG brain waves Frequency: the number of oscillations/waves per second, measured in Hertz (Hz) reflects the firing rate of neurons alpha, beta, theta, delta Amplitude:

More information

ON-LINE REPOSITORY MATERIAL DIFFERENCES IN SLEEP-INDUCED HYPOXIA BETWEEN A/J AND DBA/2J MOUSE STRAINS

ON-LINE REPOSITORY MATERIAL DIFFERENCES IN SLEEP-INDUCED HYPOXIA BETWEEN A/J AND DBA/2J MOUSE STRAINS 37 ON-LINE REPOSITORY MATERIAL DIFFERENCES IN SLEEP-INDUCED HYPOXIA BETWEEN A/J AND DBA/2J MOUSE STRAINS Arnon E. Rubin, Vsevolod Y. Polotsky, Alex Balbir, Jerry A. Krishnan, Alan R. Schwartz, Philip L.

More information

H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study

H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study Sleep 12(4):374-378, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1989 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Short Communication H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study

More information

Precisely Synchronized Oscillatory Firing Patterns Require Electroencephalographic Activation

Precisely Synchronized Oscillatory Firing Patterns Require Electroencephalographic Activation The Journal of Neuroscience, May 15, 1999, 19(10):3992 4010 Precisely Synchronized Oscillatory Firing Patterns Require Electroencephalographic Activation Suzana Herculano-Houzel, Matthias H. J. Munk, Sergio

More information

Sleep. No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness,

Sleep. No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness, Sleep Neil B. Kavey, MD Columbia Presbyterian Medical Center No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness, It is an active physiologic

More information

Sleep. No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness,

Sleep. No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness, Sleep Neil B. Kavey, MD Columbia Presbyterian Medical Center No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness, It is an active physiologic

More information

States of Consciousness

States of Consciousness States of Consciousness Sleep, Dreams, and Body Rhythms Introduction Consciousness Awareness of oneself and one s environment Body Rhythms Biological Rhythms Periodic physiological fluctuations Can affect

More information

The Disappearing Slow Wave Activity of Hibernators

The Disappearing Slow Wave Activity of Hibernators Sleep Research Online 1(2): 96-11, 1998 http://www.sro.org/1998/larkin/96/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 196-214X 1998 WebSciences The Disappearing Slow Wave Activity of Hibernators Jennie E.

More information

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA 1 Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA CONTENTS I Personal and clinical data II Conclusion. III Recommendations for therapy IV Report. 1. Procedures of EEG recording and analysis 2. Search for

More information

The winter recurrence of depressive episodes in patients

The winter recurrence of depressive episodes in patients EEG and Subjective Sleepiness during Extended Wakefulness in Seasonal Affective Disorder: Circadian and Homeostatic Influences Christian Cajochen, Daniel P. Brunner, Kurt Kräuchi, Peter Graw, and Anna

More information

HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS. Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann Acacia Aguirre Martin Moore-Ede

HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS. Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann Acacia Aguirre Martin Moore-Ede Proceedings of the 23 Winter Simulation Conference S. Chick, P. J. Sánchez, D. Ferrin, and D. J. Morrice, eds. HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann

More information

Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements

Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements Y. Isomura et al. 1 Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements Yoshikazu Isomura, Rie Harukuni, Takashi Takekawa, Hidenori Aizawa & Tomoki Fukai

More information

The connection between sleep spindles and epilepsy in a spatially extended neural field model

The connection between sleep spindles and epilepsy in a spatially extended neural field model The connection between sleep spindles and epilepsy in a spatially extended neural field model 1 2 3 Carolina M. S. Lidstrom Undergraduate in Bioengineering UCSD clidstro@ucsd.edu 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

More information

Sleep Stages Solution v0.1

Sleep Stages Solution v0.1 Sleep Stages Solution v0.1 June 2016 Table of contents Key terms... 2 Introduction... 2 Test Protocol... 3 Inputs and Outputs... 4 Validation and Accuracy... 4 Example datasets... 6 Limitations of the

More information

Assessing sleepiness in the rat: a multiple sleep latencies test compared to polysomnographic measures of sleepiness

Assessing sleepiness in the rat: a multiple sleep latencies test compared to polysomnographic measures of sleepiness Assessing sleepiness in the rat: a multiple sleep latencies test compared to polysomnograp... Page 1 of 14 Journal List > NIHPA Author Manuscripts J Sleep Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009

More information

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night?

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night? Sleep Research Online 2(1): 15-19, 1999 http://www.sro.org/1999/ferrara/15/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 1999 WebSciences Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound:

More information

Contents Sleep stage scoring Sleep regulation Sleep stage scoring in infants Sleep spindles 11

Contents Sleep stage scoring Sleep regulation Sleep stage scoring in infants Sleep spindles 11 Contents. Summary. Introduction 5.. Sleep stage scoring 6.. Sleep regulation 6.. Sleep stage scoring in infants 8.. Sleep spindles... Definition of sleep spindles... Premature spindles... Sleep spindles

More information

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Yingxue Wang, Sandro Romani, Brian Lustig, Anthony Leonardo, Eva Pastalkova Supplementary Materials Supplementary Modeling

More information

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT 1 Courtesy of Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.

More information

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms People typically sleep about 8 hours per day, and spend 16 hours awake. Most people sleep over 175,000 hours in their lifetime. The vast amount of time spent sleeping

More information

SLEEP. -in 1953, first demonstration that brain was active during sleep. 4. Stages 3& 4: Delta Waves, large slow waves; deep sleep

SLEEP. -in 1953, first demonstration that brain was active during sleep. 4. Stages 3& 4: Delta Waves, large slow waves; deep sleep SLEEP DEF: altered state, between waking and unconsciousness, defined by specific patterns of brain activity. I. How much sleep do I need? 1. Long vs. Short Sleepers -across developmental stages -individual

More information

Artificial organisms that sleep

Artificial organisms that sleep Artificial organisms that sleep Marco Mirolli 1,2, Domenico Parisi 1 1 Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, National Research Council Viale Marx 15, 137, Rome, Italy parisi@ip.rm.cnr.it 2

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Trial structure for go/no-go behavior

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Trial structure for go/no-go behavior Supplementary Figure 1 Trial structure for go/no-go behavior a, Overall timeline of experiments. Day 1: A1 mapping, injection of AAV1-SYN-GCAMP6s, cranial window and headpost implantation. Water restriction

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA 1 CONTENTS I Personal and clinical data II Conclusion. III Recommendations for therapy IV Report. 1. Procedures of EEG recording and analysis 2. Search for paroxysms 3. Eyes Open background EEG rhythms

More information

Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness

Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness Sleep, 18(10):890-894 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness

More information

Human sleep spindle characteristics after sleep deprivation

Human sleep spindle characteristics after sleep deprivation Clinical Neurophysiology 114 (2003) 2258 2267 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Human sleep spindle characteristics after sleep deprivation Vera Knoblauch a, Wim L.J. Martens b, Anna Wirz-Justice a, Christian

More information

Atlas, rules, and recording techniques for the scoring of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep

Atlas, rules, and recording techniques for the scoring of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep Sleep Medicine 3 (2002) 187 199 Consensus Report Atlas, rules, and recording techniques for the scoring of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep Mario Giovanni Terzano a, *, Liborio Parrino a,

More information

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL 116 CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL 6.1 INTRODUCTION Electrical impulses generated by nerve firings in the brain pass through the head and represent the electroencephalogram (EEG). Electrical

More information

Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data

Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation

More information

Sleep and Dreaming. Sleep Deprivation Trivia

Sleep and Dreaming. Sleep Deprivation Trivia Sleep and Dreaming Sleep Deprivation Trivia Peter Tripp stayed awake for 201 hours in 1959. Guinness Book of Records record is 18 days, 21 hours, 40 minutes. Sleep deprivation implicated in Three Mile

More information

University of Groningen. Models of human sleep regulation Beersma, Domien G.M. Published in: Sleep Medicine Reviews DOI: /S (98)

University of Groningen. Models of human sleep regulation Beersma, Domien G.M. Published in: Sleep Medicine Reviews DOI: /S (98) University of Groningen Models of human sleep regulation Beersma, Domien G.M. Published in: Sleep Medicine Reviews DOI: 10.1016/S1087-0792(98)90052-1 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's

More information

Effects of light exposure and sleep displacement on dim light melatonin onset Gordijn, Margaretha; Beersma, DGM; Korte, HJ; Van den Hoofdakker, RH

Effects of light exposure and sleep displacement on dim light melatonin onset Gordijn, Margaretha; Beersma, DGM; Korte, HJ; Van den Hoofdakker, RH University of Groningen Effects of light exposure and sleep displacement on dim light melatonin onset Gordijn, Margaretha; Beersma, DGM; Korte, HJ; Van den Hoofdakker, RH Published in: Journal of Sleep

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Reinforcement altered Training phase RTs.

Supplementary Figure 1. Reinforcement altered Training phase RTs. Supplementary Figure 1. Reinforcement altered Training phase RTs. To investigate if there were any effects of reinforcement on Simon task performance, the slope of RTs within each block was quantified

More information

Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services

Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services www.lakeshoresupport.com Humans have biological rhythms, known as Circadian Rhythms (CR) CR refers to cyclical

More information

Chronotype and Sex Effects on Sleep Architecture and Quantitative Sleep EEG in Healthy Young Adults

Chronotype and Sex Effects on Sleep Architecture and Quantitative Sleep EEG in Healthy Young Adults CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS Chronotype and Sex Effects on Sleep Architecture and Quantitative Sleep EEG in Healthy Young Adults Valérie Mongrain, MSc 1,2 ; Julie Carrier, PhD 1-3 ; Marie Dumont, PhD 1,2,4 1 Chronobiology

More information

Neurophysiology & EEG

Neurophysiology & EEG Neurophysiology & EEG PG4 Core Curriculum Ian A. Cook, M.D. Associate Director, Laboratory of Brain, Behavior, & Pharmacology UCLA Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences Semel Institute for

More information

MODULE 08: SLEEP, DREAMS, AND BODY RHYTHMS CONSCIOUSNESS

MODULE 08: SLEEP, DREAMS, AND BODY RHYTHMS CONSCIOUSNESS MODULE 08: SLEEP, DREAMS, AND BODY RHYTHMS CONSCIOUSNESS CONSCIOUSNESS Awareness of yourself and your environment. CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS Biological rhythms (for example, of temperature and wakefulness) that

More information

THE EFFECT OF WHOLE-BODY VIBRATION ON HUMAN PERFORMANCE AND PHYSIOLOGICAL FUNCTIONS

THE EFFECT OF WHOLE-BODY VIBRATION ON HUMAN PERFORMANCE AND PHYSIOLOGICAL FUNCTIONS Industrial Health, 1977, 15, 13. THE EFFECT OF WHOLE-BODY VIBRATION ON HUMAN PERFORMANCE AND PHYSIOLOGICAL FUNCTIONS PART 1. EXPERIMENTAL STUDY OF THE EFFECT OF VERTICAL VIBRATION ON HUMAN SLEEP Kazuhide

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing

More information

Sleep Patterns of Pubertal Males

Sleep Patterns of Pubertal Males Pediat. Res. 6: 6-68 (97) Adolescence puberty sleep sleep, stages of Sleep Patterns of Pubertal Males ROBERT L. WILLIAMS 7, ISMET KARACAN, CAROLYN J. HURSGH, AND CLARENCE E. DAVIS Departments of Psychiatry

More information

Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals

Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals Nissl stained coronal or horizontal sections were made from kainate injected (right) and saline injected (left) animals at different

More information

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep Sleep, 6(4):319-325 1983 Raven Press, New York Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep T. Roehrs, F. Zorick, J. Sicklesteel, R. Wittig, and T. Roth Sleep Disorders and Research

More information

Behavioral generalization

Behavioral generalization Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral generalization. a. Behavioral generalization curves in four Individual sessions. Shown is the conditioned response (CR, mean ± SEM), as a function of absolute (main) or

More information

Modeling Human Sleep Patterns

Modeling Human Sleep Patterns Modeling Human Sleep Patterns Rebecca Gleit Advisor: Professor Victoria Booth ABSTRACT Human sleep is composed of three main sleep stages, which exhibit fairly regular cycling throughout the night. With

More information

Sleep Spindle Detection Based on Complex Demodulation Jia-bin LI, Bei WANG* and Yu ZHANG

Sleep Spindle Detection Based on Complex Demodulation Jia-bin LI, Bei WANG* and Yu ZHANG International Conference on Biomedical and Biological Engineering (BBE 216) Sleep Spindle Detection Based on Complex Demodulation Jia-bin LI, Bei WANG* and Yu ZHANG Department of Automation, School of

More information