Self-related neural response to tailored smoking-cessation messages. predicts quitting

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Self-related neural response to tailored smoking-cessation messages. predicts quitting"

Transcription

1 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 1 Supporting Online Material Self-related neural response to tailored smoking-cessation messages predicts quitting Hannah Faye Chua 1, S. Shaun Ho 2, Agnes J. Jasinska 3, Thad A. Polk 2,4, Robert C. Welsh 5, Israel Liberzon 2, Victor J. Strecher 1 1 Health Behavior and Health Education, 2 Psychiatry, 3 Neuroscience Program, 4 Psychology and 5 Radiology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI. SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS Participants Ninety-one right-handed smokers (51.6% males) participated. Participants were between years old (mean age 37.5 ± 11.5 years). Eligibility criteria included having smoked a cigarette within the last 7 days, having smoked a minimum of 10 cigarettes on average per day and at least 100 cigarettes in a lifetime, and having an interest in quitting within the next 30 days. Participants were not enrolled in other smoking cessation programs and were not taking pharmacological treatment for smoking cessation during study enrollment 1. Participants smoked on average ± 5.8 cigarettes a day for an average of ± years. Most of the participants had previously attempted to quit (84.6%). Participants were motivated to quit (M = 8.99 [SD = 1.23] on a 10-point scale) and were also confident about quitting (M = 8.13 [SD = 1.78] on a 10-point scale). Education level was distributed across different levels (20% completed high school, 36.3% completed some college, 23.1% completed a non-4-year college degree, 12.1% completed a 4-year college degree, and 7.7% completed a graduate degree). Across the participants, 73.6% reported being employed or self-employed and 23.1% reported attending school. Top reasons for quitting smoking reported by the participants were personal health reasons, financial costs of cigarettes, and desire to have more control of their lives. Participants had no prior history of head injury or major psychiatric (schizophrenia/bipolar) or neurological illness. Participants did not receive any diagnosis of mental health condition (including depression and other substance abuse) and did not use illicit drugs during the past year. All participants were native English speakers, had normal hearing, and had good visual acuity. Messages We created statements for each of the following 3 message types: Tailored, Untailored, and Neutral messages. The messages had comparable reading levels (approximately 6 th grade). The number of words, syllables, and characters across the statements was also controlled. We based the Tailored and Untailored messages on cognitive-behavioral methods of smoking cessation and relapse prevention 1. Tailored statements include self-relevant personalization and feedback about the smoker. It involves the degree to which there are

2 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 2 recognizable features of the individual in the message (e.g., You have been smoking for 12 years. ), as well as references to the individual s state characteristics (e.g., levels of stress and coping) which were generated based on participants responses. We created Untailored statements based on information available from the Guide to Quitting Smoking section of the American Cancer Society website and other similar resources, which are generic smokingcessation messages. The topics for each Tailored and Untailored statement were constrained to make them comparable. Neutral statements served as the control condition and were facts used mostly from a different study (e.g., The weight of a carat is equal to one fifth of a gram ) 2. All participants received the same Untailored and Neutral statements, but Tailored statements varied depending on the participants survey responses. For the Tailored messages, a bank of statements was created based on the protocol used for a web-based tailored smoking-cessation program 1. By the term tailoring, we refer to a process consisting of: (a) an assessment of individual characteristics relevant to smoking cessation; (b) algorithms that use the assessment data to generate intervention messages relevant to the specific needs of the user; and (c) a protocol that delivers these messages to the smoker. For this study, we collected data from the participants via questionnaire, and the algorithm produced appropriate statements for each message type. Participants received 50 statements of each of the 3 message types. See samples in Supplementary Table 1. Supplementary Table 1. Sample statements from each message type presented as blocks Tailored messages A concern you have is being tempted to smoke when around other smokers. Something else that you feel will tempt you after you quit is because of a craving. You are worried that when angry or frustrated, you may light up. You have a lot of stress in your life right now, but you do feel you can manage it. When you feel stressed, you tend to seek out friends for support. You feel like your sister will help you stay on track once you quit. No one close to you is going to quit with you. You want to quit because you are tired of spending your money on cigarettes. You feel your health somewhat limits you, including even taking the stairs. You have tried to quit smoking before. Untailored messages Some people are tempted to smoke to control their weight or hunger. Smokers also light up when they need to concentrate. Certain moods or feelings, places, and things you do can make you want to smoke. We all face times when life can be stressful and we are not sure how to handle it all. There are many skills that can help smokers deal with stress. Someone you trust can provide support to you once you quit. Many people quit with another person so they can support each other. Many smokers quit because they are tired of spending money on cigarettes. Smokers are admitted to the hospital more often than nonsmokers. Most people need to try more than once to quit smoking for good. Neutral messages Oil was formed from the remains of animals and plants that lived millions of years ago. Sighted in the Pacific Ocean, the world's tallest sea wave was 112 feet. Wind is simple air in motion. It is caused by the uneven heating of the earth's surface by the sun.

3 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 3 The true color of ocean water is neither blue nor white. We can measure the age of a fish by using a magnifying glass to count the rings on its scales. Social bees live in complex societies of 10,000-50,000 members. The African elephant has larger ears than the Indian elephant. The killer whale is the largest member of the dolphin family. Unlike other turtles, sea turtles cannot retract their legs and heads into their shells. fmri Tasks Participants completed the Messages Task and the Self-Appraisal Task during fmri. Messages Task. We presented three types of messages: Tailored, Untailored, and Neutral. Each statement was visually presented on the screen and simultaneously delivered in audio 3. There were 5 runs, 2 blocks of each message type per run, 5 messages per block, for the total of 150 messages presented. Each block lasted 24 seconds. We presented the Message blocks in pseudorandom order within and across participants. Fixations lasting between 4-10 seconds (averaging 7s) occurred after each message block. Each run also started and ended with a 10- second fixation. Self-Appraisal Task. The paradigm was adopted from Schmitz and Johnson 4 and Schmitz, Rowley, Kawahara & Johnson 5. There were two task conditions: self-referential and word valence judgment conditions. Adjectives were presented and participants had to make a selfreferential or word valence judgment, with yes or no responses. Example adjectives used were shy, happy, and analytical. In the self-referential judgment condition, participants had to decide whether an adjective described them or not. In the valence judgment condition, participants had to decide whether the adjective was of positive valence or not. Adjectives were presented for 3 seconds plus a 1 second inter-stimulus interval. Each block contained 6 adjectives of 4 seconds each for a total of 24s. Each run included 5 blocks of each condition in alternating cycles. There were two runs. The order of presentation of adjectives was counterbalanced across participants. Each run started and ended with a 10s fixation. The task lasted 8.67 minutes. Procedure Participants completed a phone-based eligibility screening. We invited those who were eligible to participate in the study. Participants completed 3 sessions, plus a follow-up call. Baseline Session (Session 1). Participants completed the MRI safety screening form and gave written consent. They also completed a standard baseline assessment about their smoking history and psychosocial, health, and demographic characteristics relevant to smoking cessation. We used the responses to create the tailored messages. fmri Session (Session 2). Participants completed the Messages Task and Self-Appraisal Task during fmri. After the scan, we gave participants a surprise memory task to assess their attention to the Messages Task in the scanner. Sixty statements were presented to the participants. Thirty were shown earlier in the scanner and thirty were new. There were an equal number of statements from each message type in both the old and new statements. In the task, we told the participants that they would be presented a series of statements, some of which they were shown earlier during the scan. Their task was to indicate whether the statement was old or new, by

4 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 4 pressing specific response keys. After, we gave each participant one nicotine patch to be worn on the morning of their intervention session. Intervention Session (Session 3). Their third visit, scheduled within one week from their fmri scan, was the day the participants received the intervention and were also instructed to quit smoking. Participants were administered a complete web-based tailored smoking-cessation program developed by the University of Michigan Center for Health Communications Research 1. Participants spent minutes going through the intervention materials. Self-relevant personalization and feedback about the smoker (similar to the messages presented in the scanner), as well as additional tailored motivational and instructional messages were provided in the intervention. We did not collect any additional behavioral ratings to compare message perception during scanner session and intervention session. We provided participants with a 10-week supply of nicotine patch (6-weeks of 21 mg, 2- weeks of 14-mg, and 2-weeks of 7-mg patches) as recommended by the manufacturers to people who smoke at least 10 cigarettes a day. Participants were debriefed, compensated, and received printed smoking-cessation materials, including a copy of their tailored smoking-cessation program as well as a booklet developed by the National Cancer Institute (i.e., Clearing the Air: Quit Smoking Today). Four-month Follow-up. Four months after the intervention session, trained research staff conducted the follow-up interviews with participants. The interview focused on questions regarding smoking status and smoking-cessation activities 1. The primary outcome measure was the standard 7-day abstinence (7-day point prevalence abstinence). This metric has been demonstrated to have an extremely high correlation with 24-hour point prevalence, and 30-day prolonged abstinence. These three measures provide the same information when used as outcome measures in smoking-cessation studies 6. We also compensated the participants for their participation in the follow-up interview. fmri Acquisition Scanning occurred on a 3T GE Signa Excite 2 scanner (Milwaukee, Wisconsin), beginning with a structural acquisition (T1-overlay) (repetition time [TR] = 250 ms, echo time [TE] = 7 ms, flip angle [FA] = 75 o, field of view [FOV] = 220 mm, 43 oblique axial slices, 256 x 256, slice thickness 3.0 mm). A T2*-weighted, spiral-in acquisition sequence 7 was used during the actual task (gradient echo, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FA = 90 o, FOV = 220 mm, 64 x 64, slice thickness 3.0 mm). A high-resolution T1 scan was also obtained (three-dimensional spoiled-gradient echo [3D-SPGR] with inversion recovery prep, time of inversion = 400 ms, TR = 9.0 ms, TE = 1.8 ms, FA = 15 o, FOV = 260 mm, 128 slices, 256 x 256, 1.2 mm slice). fmri Analyses First, data were sinc-interpolated in time, slice-by-slice. A six-parameter, least-squares minimization, motion correction algorithm, using the MCFLIRT program (FSL Analysis Group, FMRIB, Oxford, UK) was applied to realign all functional data. Subsequent processing was done using SPM2 (Wellcome Institute of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK). The T1-overlay was co-registered with a functional scan. Then the 3D-SPGR, high-resolution image was coregistered to the T1-overlay. The high-resolution image was anatomically normalized to the

5 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 5 MNI152 template. The resulting transformation parameters were then applied to the coregistered functional volumes. A 5-mm smoothing was applied to the functional volumes. The fmri time-series were analyzed using a modified general linear model. Messages Task included blocks of Tailored, Untailored, and Neutral messages. The regressors for the Self- Appraisal Task were self-referential judgment and valence judgment blocks. Regressors were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function and a time derivative was calculated. For all of the models, movement parameters were included as covariates. Parameter estimates for the different trials were obtained at each voxel within the brain. Contrast images were smoothed an additional 5 mm before entering into one-sample t-tests during the second-level random effects analysis. The main contrast results for Self-Appraisal Task (Self > Valence) and Messages Task (Tailored > Neutral, Untailored > Neutral) were thresholded at whole-brain family-wise correction of.05 with voxel extent of 50, and the specific contrast results for Tailored > Untailored were thresholded at whole-brain correction using false discovery rate of.05 with voxel extent of 50. Supplemental Results Behavioral differences between Quitters and Non-Quitters The results included the data from 87 participants who were available during the followup smoking-cessation outcome assessment. We could not reach 4 of the 91 subjects during their follow-up interview period. Out of 87 participants available during follow-up assessment, two reported their quit status, but declined to complete the entire follow-up interview. Quitters and Non-Quitters groups were comparable in age, education, years of smoking, number of cigarettes smoked per day, motivation to quit or confidence in quitting, and reasons for quitting. There was no difference in the reported nicotine patch use between Quitters and Non-Quitters. A multivariate analysis of variance was undertaken to determine differences in reported behaviors between Quitters and Non-Quitters four months after the intervention. Pillai s Trace was used as a global test for any group differences on all variables. There were a significant difference between Quitters and Non-Quitters (Pillai s trace =.368, p <.001) on the reported behaviors. We examined in depth which variables were significantly different between Quitters and Non- Quitters. Quitters were more likely to change their stress response since the program, rather than turn to smoking, Ms.80 vs..58, F(1,83) = 5.24, p =.03; Quitters were more likely to avoid situations that trigger smoking since the program,, Ms.69 vs..43,f(1,83) = 6.30, p =.01; Quitters felt a lot better about their health since the program, Ms 4.24 vs. 3.48,F(1,83) = 19.17, p <.001; Quitters have made fewer attempts to quit during the past 4-month period, Ms 2.11 vs. 5.40, F(1,83) = 11.28, p =.001; Quitters were overall more satisfied with the study program, Ms 8.89 vs. 8.23, F(1,83) = 4.32, p <.05; Quitters were overall more motivated to continue to stay smoke-free, Ms 9.42 vs. 8.43, F(1,83) = 8.88, p <.005, and were overall more confident that they would not start smoking again, Ms 8.73 vs. 7.78, F(1,83) = 4.10, p <.05; Quitters reported experiencing less severe withdrawal symptoms during the first 3 days of quitting, Ms 5.38 vs. 6.88, F(1,83) = 5.57, p =.02. Self Appraisal Task Behavioral Results. The Self-Appraisal Task consisted of the self-referential condition and the word valence condition. Using repeated measures ANOVA, we examined task

6 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 6 performance in both conditions with quitting status as a between-subjects variable. Overall, participants rated that 57.8% of the adjectives described them and 51.7% of the adjectives were positive in valence. This difference in judging frequency was significant, F(1,83) = 32.66, p <.001. (Out of 87 participants, there were 85 useful and complete behavioral data used in this analysis. The behavioral data set was missing for one participant, and another participant repeatedly gave the same response.) Non-Quitters were slightly more likely than Quitters to respond yes that an adjective described them or was of positive valence, Ms 56.3% vs. 53.3%, respectively, F(1, 83) = 4.02, p =.05. There was no interaction effect of quitting status and performance in self-referential and word valence judgments in the Self-Appraisal Task. fmri Results. Consistent with previous findings 4,5, self-referential evaluation, in contrast to valence judgment, of adjectives activated the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and precuneus/posterior cingulate regions across the 87 subjects. See Supplementary Figure 1 and Supplementary Table 2. Activations in these regions were not correlated with quitting. The results were presented at a statistical threshold of p <.05 with whole brain correction using family-wise error and extent threshold of 50 voxels. These results were saved as a mask to identify brain regions associated with self-related processing. x = 3 Supplementary Figure 1. Activations in the MPFC and precuneus/posterior cingulate during self-referential judgment, relative to valence judgment, in the Self-Appraisal Task, across 87 subjects. The coordinates are in MNI space. Supplementary Table 2. Peak activations of self-referential evaluation, relative to valance judgments, in the Self- Appraisal Task. Region Gyri / Subregions Precuneus / Posterior Cingulate Parietal lobe Middle cingulate gyrus Cuneus MPFC Medial frontal gyrus Superior frontal gyrus Anterior cingulate gyrus Medial orbital gyrus Inferior frontal gyrus L, insula Caudate Putamen BA x, y, z a T Z b k d 31, 7, 24, 23, 30, 5, 29, 19, 6 10, 9, 32, 11, 8, 25, 47, 24 3, 51, c , 60, c 2159

7 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 7 L Inferior Parietal Lobule 39, 19, 40 45, 72, L Angular Gyrus R Inferior Parietal Lobule 39, 40 57, 63, R Angular Gyrus L, left; R, right. a Stereotactic coordinates of the peak voxel from the Montreal Neurological Institute atlas, left/right (x), anterior/posterior (y), and superior/inferior (z), respectively. b SPM-generated Z score, significant at FWE p <.05. c Estimated Z score, significant at FWE p <.05. d Spatial extent of the cluster in voxels (5 voxel minimum). Messages Task Tailored messages and quitting. There was no difference in the type and length of Tailored statements received by Quitters and Non-Quitters. This rules out the possibility that any differences found between Quitters and Non-Quitters was due to incomparable stimuli received by the participants. Behavioral Results. Overall participants seemed to have been paying attention in the scanner during the Messages Task, as supported by results in the surprise memory task outside the scanner. The average proportion of correctly identifying an old statement was 0.73, which was greater than chance level of.50, t(87) = 13.32, p < Also, interestingly, participants seemed to accurately remember the Tailored statements more than the Untailored statements, contrast F(1,85) = 43.81, p <.001, and the Neutral statements, contrast F(1,85) = 44.27, p <.001. Similarly, the Untailored statements were remembered better than the Neutral statements, contrast F(1,85) = 7.01, p <. 01. The mean recognition accuracies for the Tailored, Untailored, and Neutral statements were 0.84, 0.72, and 0.63, respectively. This indicates preliminary evidence for levels of processing effect 8 with Tailored messages. While Tailored messages were remembered better than Untailored and Neutral messages, there was no relationship between better memory performance for the Tailored messages and smoking cessation (r = 0.05, ns). There was also no correlation between memory performance and dmpfc activation during Tailored messages (r =.09, ns). These results suggest that neither attention to messages nor memory performance predicts quitting, although self-related response in dmpfc during tailored message processing does. In the surprise recognition memory task, we presented only 10 old statements from the 50 statements for each message type presented in the scanner, and showed an equal number of new statements; the researchers have no information if memory performance for the statements would differ if participants were tested with all old statements with an equal number of lures. The researchers also have no information if memory performance between the two groups would have changed as a function of time. fmri Results: Tailored or Untailored messages relative to Neutral messages. We identified the neural activation patterns associated with the Tailored or Untailored smokingcessation messages relative to the Neutral messages in the whole sample. As expected, the Tailored and Untailored smoking-cessation messages produced a robust pattern of activation in the MPFC and precuneus/posterior cingulate regions (Supplementary Figures 2a & 2b).

8 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 8 Supplementary Figures 2c & 2d show that activations in the MPFC and precuneus/posterior cingulate regions overlapped with regions associated with self-appraisal processes, using the mask generated from the Self-Appraisal Task. These results extend previous work by Chua et al. 4, who showed differential activation patterns between High- and Low-Tailored messages in contrast to Untailored smoking-cessation messages, but did not contrast both Tailored and Untailored messages with Neutral messages, and the overlap between these activations with independently identified self-related processing regions. Supplementary Tables 3 and 4 list the brain regions activated more by the Tailored or Untailored messages, relative to the Neutral messages, and that survived the family-wise error correction of <.05 and cluster extent equal or greater than 50 voxels. A. x = 6 B. x = 6 C. x = 6 D. x = 6 Supplementary Figure 2. Activations in the MPFC and precuneus/posterior cingulate regions during processing of (A) Tailored messages and (B) Untailored messages, relative to Neutral messages, across 87 subjects. Common areas of activation in the MPFC and precuneus between (C) Tailored messages and self-related processing (Self > Valence), and between (D) Untailored messages and self-related processing. The coordinates are in MNI space. Supplementary Table 3. Peak activations to Tailored messages, relative to Neutral messages, in the Messages Task. Region Gyri / Subregions MPFC Superior frontal gyrus Medial frontal gyrus Anterior cingulate gyrus Middle frontal gyrus Supplementary motor area Medial orbital gyrus L Inferior Frontal Gyrus L inferior orbital gyrus L middle temporal gyrus L temporal pole L inferior temporal gyrus L superior temporal gyrus L insula BA x, y, z a T Z b k d 10, 9, 32, 11, 6, 8, 24 0, 54, c , 21, 38, 20, 45 51, 0, c 599

9 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 9 L fusiform gyrus R Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47, 38 54, 24, R inferior orbital gyrus R insula R temporal pole R superior temporal gyrus Precuneus / Posterior Cingulate 7, 31, 23, 24, 30 6, 60, c 1820 Middle cingulate gyrus Cuneus L Inferior Parietal Lobule 40, 39, 7 51, 63, c 668 L angular gyrus L superior temporal gyrus L middle temporal gyrus L Middle Frontal Gyrus 8, 6, 9 39, 15, L Middle Temporal Gyrus 21, 22 54, 30, R Inferior Parietal Lobule 40, 39 57, 51, R angular gyrus R supramarginal gyrus R superior temporal gyrus R Middle Temporal Gyrus 21, 38 51, 9, R inferior temporal gyrus R Cerebellum 27, 81, R Middle Frontal Gyrus 9, 6, 8 42, 18, R Middle Temporal Gyrus 21, 22 57, 30, L Cerebellum 24, 81, L, left; R, right. a Stereotactic coordinates of the peak voxel from the Montreal Neurological Institute atlas, left/right (x), anterior/posterior (y), and superior/inferior (z), respectively. b SPM-generated Z score, significant at FWE p <.05. c Estimated Z score, significant at FWE p <.05. d Spatial extent of the cluster in voxels (50 voxel minimum). Supplementary Table 4. Peak activations to Untailored messages, relative to Neutral messages, in the Messages Task. Region Gyri / Subregions MPFC Medial frontal gyrus Superior frontal gyrus Middle frontal gyrus Anterior cingulate gyrus Medial orbital gyrus Superior orbital gyrus Precuneus / Posterior Cingulate Middle cingulate gyrus Cuneus L Inferior Parietal Lobule L angular gyrus L supramarginal gyrus L superior temporal gyrus R Superior Frontal Gyrus R frontal medial gyrus R supplementary motor area R Inferior Parietal Lobule R angular gyrus BA x, y, z a T Z b k d 10, 9, 32, 8, 11, 24 3, 63, , 31, 23, 24, 6 0, 21, c , 39 48, 63, c 423 8, 10, 9, 6 27, 54, , 39 57, 54,

10 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 10 R supramarginal gyrus R superior temporal gyrus L Inferior Temporal Gyrus 20, 21 54, 6, L middle temporal gyrus L Middle Temporal Gyrus 21 66, 27, L inferior temporal gyrus L, left; R, right. a Stereotactic coordinates of the peak voxel from the Montreal Neurological Institute atlas, left/right (x), anterior/posterior (y), and superior/inferior (z), respectively. b SPM-generated Z score, significant at FWE p <.05. c Estimated Z score, significant at FWE p <.05. d Spatial extent of the cluster in voxels (50 voxel minimum). fmri Results: Tailored messages > Untailored messages. Supplementary Figure 3 and Supplementary Table 5 list the brain regions preferentially engaged by Tailored messages. These regions showed greater activation to Tailored messages relative to Untailored messages. A. x = 6 B. z = 3 Supplementary Figure 3. Activations in (A) the dmpfc, pre-supplementary motor area, precuneus and (B) the inferior frontal gyri during processing of Tailored messages relative to Untailored messages, across 87 subjects. The coordinates are in MNI space. Supplementary Table 5. Peak activations to Tailored messages, relative to Untailored messages, in the Messages Task. Region BA x, y, z a Z b k c Gyri / Subregions dmpfc 9, 8, 10, 6 3, 48, Medial frontal gyrus Superior Frontal Gyrus R Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47, 45, 46 51, 24, R inferior orbital gyrus R middle frontal gyrus R insula L Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47, 45, 44 48, 27, L inferior orbital gyrus L middle frontal gyrus L temporal lobe L insula L Precuneus 31, 7 9, 51, L middle cingulate gyrus L cuneus Pre-Supplementary Motor Area 9, 8, 10, 6 3, 24, Superior Frontal Gyrus Supplementary Motor Area L Superior Temporal Gyrus L middle temporal gyrus L angular gyrus 39, 22 54, 54,

11 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 11 L parietal lobule L supramarginal gyrus R Superior Temporal Gyrus 22, 21 57, 30, R middle temporal gyrus L Cerebellum 15, 78, L Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex 9, 8, 6 45, 12, L Middle Frontal Gyrus L precentral gyrus L medial frontal gyrus L inferior frontal gyrus R Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex 9, 8 42, 12, R middle frontal gyrus R medial frontal gyrus R inferior frontal operculum L Middle Temporal Gyrus L superior temporal gyrus 21, 22 69, 42, L, left; R, right; dmpfc, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex a Stereotactic coordinates of the peak voxel from the Montreal Neurological Institute atlas, left/right (x), anterior/posterior (y), and superior/inferior (z), respectively. b Z score, significant at FDR p <.05. c Spatial extent of the cluster in voxels (50 voxel minimum). Conjunctive Regions of Interest (ROI) mask. The overlap of self-related processing [Self > Valence, FWE.05, k = 50] and tailored message processing [Tailored > Untailored, FDR.05, k =50] yielded 3 common regions: dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (128 voxels), precuneus (60 voxels), and angular gyrus (30 voxels). These regions were both preferentially engaged by self-related processing and by Tailored messages. These regions were saved as a mask and treated as regions-of-interest in examining whether neural response in these regions during Tailored messages (relative to Neutral messages) predicted quitting. Results are presented in the main manuscript. x = -6 z = 33 Supplementary Figure 4. Conjunctive ROI mask from self-related processing and tailored message processing.

12 Neural response to tailored messages and quitting Page 12 Although it is true that the Tailored messages used second person pronouns ( you ) more than the Untailored messages did, the conjunctive mask required that the activation be present for both the Tailored > Untailored and the Self > Valence contrasts. Exactly the same words were used for the self-reference and valence judgment conditions (and no pronouns at all). Thus, the association between neural response to tailored messages and quitting cannot be due to a change in pronouns in the Tailored messages. Other outcome-predictive regions. We also examined if there are activations in regions outside the conjunctive mask that correlated with quitting. Using the regions preferentially engaged by Tailored messages over linguistically comparable Untailored messages (Supplement Table 5; FDR.05, k = 50) as regions-of-interest, and that did not overlap with self-related processing regions as defined by the Self-Appraisal task results, we found that greater activation in bilateral inferior frontal gyri during Tailored messages positively correlated with quitting (left BA 47; peak xyz coordinates: 51, 24, 3; ß =.54, S.E. =.19, Wald χ 2 = 8.29, p =.004; O.R. = 1.72; right BA 47; peak xyz coordinates: 48, 27, 9; ß =.69, S.E. =.23, Wald χ 2 = 8.9, p =.003; O.R. = 1.98). Of course, the role of inferior frontal gyrus in predicting quitting suggests that there could be other mechanisms involved, e.g., inhibitory control processing 9 or autobiographical memory 10 and related cognitive control processes 11 engaged by the tailored messages. Activity in other parts of the self-related processing network (ventral and rostral mpfc areas) also predicted quitting, but these areas are not included because they were not significantly activated by the Tailored > Untailored contrast. fmri Results and Gender: We did not find any gender differences in any of the fmri results in the Messages Task or the Self-Appraisal Task. References: 1. Strecher, V.J., et al. Am J Prev Med 34, (2008). 2. Chua, H.F., Chen, W. & Park, D.C. Gerontology 52, (2006). 3. Chua, H.F., Liberzon, I., Welsh, R.C. & Strecher, V.J. Biol Psychiatry 65, (2009). 4. Schmitz, T.W. & Johnson, S.C. Neuroimage 30, (2006). 5. Schmitz, T.W., Rowley, H.A., Kawahara, T.N. & Johnson, S.C. Neuropsychologia 44, (2006). 6. Velicer, W.F. & Prochaska, J.O. Addict Behav 29, (2004). 7. Noll, D.C., Genovese, C.R., Vazquez, A.L., O'Brien, J.L. & Eddy, W.F. Magn Reson Med 40, (1998). 8. Craik, F. L. Memory 10, (2002). 9. Swick, D., Ashley, V., & Turken, A.U. BMC Neurosci 2008, 9:12 (2008). 10. Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M.C. & Levine, B. Neuropsychologia 44, (2006). 11. Badre, D. & Wagner, A.D. Neuropscyhologia 45, (2007).

Supplementary Materials for

Supplementary Materials for Supplementary Materials for Folk Explanations of Behavior: A Specialized Use of a Domain-General Mechanism Robert P. Spunt & Ralph Adolphs California Institute of Technology Correspondence may be addressed

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/324/5927/646/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmpfc Valuation System Todd A. Hare,* Colin F. Camerer, Antonio

More information

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Neuron, Volume 70 Supplemental Information Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Luke J. Chang, Alec Smith, Martin Dufwenberg, and Alan G. Sanfey Supplemental Information

More information

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain 1 Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain Emily Falk, Elliot Berkman, Traci Mann, Brittany Harrison, Matthew Lieberman This document contains: Example

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of Placebo effects in fmri Supporting online material 1 Supporting online material Materials and Methods Study 1 Procedure and behavioral data We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was

More information

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Overview of the SIIPS1 development. The development of the SIIPS1 consisted of individual- and group-level analysis steps. 1) Individual-person

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Newman et al. 10.1073/pnas.1510527112 SI Results Behavioral Performance. Behavioral data and analyses are reported in the main article. Plots of the accuracy and reaction time data

More information

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Table S1: Brain regions involved in the adapted classification learning task Brain Regions x y z Z Anterior Cingulate

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Supplemental Information Table of Contents 2 Behavioral Data 2 Table S1. Participant demographics

More information

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Figure I. Changes in connectivity with the right anterior insular cortex. (A) wake > mild sedation, showing a reduction in connectivity between the anterior

More information

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion Kevin N. Ochsner, a, * Rebecca D. Ray, b Jeffrey C. Cooper, b Elaine R. Robertson, b Sita Chopra,

More information

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults Supplementary Material Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance across cognitive domains in older adults Emily E. Shaw 1,2, Aaron P. Schultz 1,2,3, Reisa A.

More information

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group 1 Supplemental Data Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group Additional inclusion criteria for the major depressive disorder group were: age of onset of

More information

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Supplemental Information Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Roselinde H. Kaiser, Ph.D., Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli, Ph.D., Daniel G. Dillon, Ph.D., Franziska Goer, B.S., Miranda

More information

Supplementary Digital Content

Supplementary Digital Content Supplementary Digital Content Contextual modulation of pain in masochists: involvement of the parietal operculum and insula Sandra Kamping a, Jamila Andoh a, Isabelle C. Bomba a, Martin Diers a,b, Eugen

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Green SA, Hernandez L, Tottenham N, Krasileva K, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. The neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Published

More information

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Online supplementary information for: Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Joel L. Voss, Brian D. Gonsalves, Kara D. Federmeier,

More information

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 SPECIAL ISSUE WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TE US ABOUT AND DIS? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 By: Angelika Dimoka Fox School of Business Temple University 1801 Liacouras Walk Philadelphia, PA

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Available online at www.sciencedirect.com R NeuroImage 18 (2003) 806 810 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Technical Note Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Andrea Mechelli,

More information

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Supplementary Information Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex David V. Smith 1-3, Benjamin Y. Hayden 1,4, Trong-Kha Truong 2,5, Allen W. Song 2,5, Michael L.

More information

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Supplementary Information Supplementary Figure 1. The mean parameter

More information

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Author's response to reviews Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Authors: Julia Miro (juliamirollado@gmail.com) Pablo Ripollès (pablo.ripolles.vidal@gmail.com)

More information

Neural correlates of recognition memory of social information in people with schizophrenia

Neural correlates of recognition memory of social information in people with schizophrenia Research Paper Neural correlates of recognition memory of social information in people with schizophrenia Philippe-Olivier Harvey, PhD; Martin Lepage, PhD Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montréal,

More information

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Neuroethics Publications Center for Neuroscience & Society 1-1-2009 The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy Andrea L. Glenn University

More information

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory

More information

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 341 356 Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Joseph B. Sala a,, Pia Rämä a,c,d, Susan M.

More information

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia 1 Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia Christine I. Hooker, Taylor L. Benson, Anett Gyurak, Hong Yin,

More information

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Brain electrical activity methods description and explanation of method state effects trait effects Positron emission tomography

More information

Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception

Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception Oliver Baumann & Jason Mattingley Queensland Brain Institute The University of Queensland The Queensland

More information

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Supplemental informations Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Stéphanie Lefebvre, PhD^1,2, Guillaume Baille, MD^4, Renaud Jardri MD, PhD 1,2 Lucie Plomhause, PhD

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 women and 16 men. One female had to be excluded from brain data analyses because of strong movement

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Redlich R, Opel N, Grotegerd D, et al. Prediction of individual response to electroconvulsive therapy via machine learning on structural magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA

More information

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer Science & Technology (IJIRCST) ISSN: 2347-5552, Volume-2, Issue-6, November-2014 Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using

More information

AN fmri EXAMINATION OF VISUAL INTEGRATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

AN fmri EXAMINATION OF VISUAL INTEGRATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, Vol. 8, No. 2 (2009) 175 202 c Imperial College Press Research Report AN fmri EXAMINATION OF VISUAL INTEGRATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA STEVEN M. SILVERSTEIN,,, SARAH BERTEN,,

More information

Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf

Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf Supplementary Material Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf Ella Striem-Amit 1*, Jorge Almeida 2,3, Mario Belledonne 1, Quanjing Chen 4, Yuxing Fang

More information

How to report my result using REST slice viewer?

How to report my result using REST slice viewer? How to report my result using REST slice viewer? Han Zhang Center for Cognition and Brain Disorders, Hangzhou Normal University napoleon1982@gmail.com 2013/12/30 Commonly, you got an activation for functional

More information

Supplementary materials. Appendix A;

Supplementary materials. Appendix A; Supplementary materials Appendix A; To determine ADHD diagnoses, a combination of Conners' ADHD questionnaires and a semi-structured diagnostic interview was used(1-4). Each participant was assessed with

More information

The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition

The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition Rachel J. Garoff-Eaton, Elizabeth A. Kensinger and Daniel L. Schacter Learn. Mem. 2007 14: 684-692 Access the most recent version at

More information

The Impact of Anxiety-Inducing Distraction on Cognitive Performance: A Combined Brain Imaging and Personality Investigation

The Impact of Anxiety-Inducing Distraction on Cognitive Performance: A Combined Brain Imaging and Personality Investigation The Impact of Anxiety-Inducing Distraction on Cognitive Performance: A Combined Brain Imaging and Personality Investigation Ekaterina Denkova 1, Gloria Wong 2, Sanda Dolcos 3, Keen Sung 1, Lihong Wang

More information

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes Brain (1998), 121, 1985 2002 Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes Anthony D. Wagner, 1 John E. Desmond, 1,2 Gary H. Glover 2 and John

More information

Title: Resting hyperperfusion of the hippocampus, midbrain and striatum

Title: Resting hyperperfusion of the hippocampus, midbrain and striatum Data supplement for Allen et al., Resting Hyperperfusion of the Hippocampus, Midbrain, and Basal Ganglia in People at High Risk for Psychosis. Am J Psychiatry (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15040485) Supplementary

More information

Supplemental Digital Content 4: Effect sizes

Supplemental Digital Content 4: Effect sizes Supplemental Digital Content 4: Effect sizes Effect size within each of the pre-defined regions of interest for each studied network across experimental conditions and for the correlation analysis. Legend

More information

Supplemental information online for

Supplemental information online for Supplemental information online for Sleep contributes to the strengthening of some memories over others, depending on hippocampal activity at learning. Géraldine Rauchs (1,2), Dorothée Feyers (1), Brigitte

More information

BRAIN RESEARCH 1429 (2012) Available online at

BRAIN RESEARCH 1429 (2012) Available online at Available online at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Incidental encoding of goal irrelevant information is associated with insufficient engagement of the dorsal frontal

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations What is Fear Conditioning? Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning Information and prediction: Animals use environmental signals to predict the occurrence of biologically significant events. Similar

More information

Reasoning and working memory: common and distinct neuronal processes

Reasoning and working memory: common and distinct neuronal processes Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 1241 1253 Reasoning and working memory: common and distinct neuronal processes Christian C. Ruff a,b,, Markus Knauff a,c, Thomas Fangmeier a, Joachim Spreer d a Centre for Cognitive

More information

The Hippocampus Remains Activated over the Long Term for the Retrieval of Truly Episodic Memories

The Hippocampus Remains Activated over the Long Term for the Retrieval of Truly Episodic Memories The Hippocampus Remains Activated over the Long Term for the Retrieval of Truly Episodic Memories Caroline Harand 1,2,3,4, Françoise Bertran 1,2,3,5, Renaud La Joie 1,2,3,4, Brigitte Landeau 1,2,3,4, Florence

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS: Appetitive and aversive goal values are encoded in the medial orbitofrontal cortex at the time of decision-making

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS: Appetitive and aversive goal values are encoded in the medial orbitofrontal cortex at the time of decision-making SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS: Appetitive and aversive goal values are encoded in the medial orbitofrontal cortex at the time of decision-making Hilke Plassmann 1,2, John P. O'Doherty 3,4, Antonio Rangel 3,5*

More information

doi: /brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease

doi: /brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease doi:10.1093/brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; 1165 1179 1165 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease Marianne J. U. Novak, 1 Jason D.

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Devenney E, Bartley L, Hoon C, et al. Progression in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a longitudinal study. JAMA Neurol. Published online October 26, 2015. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.2061.

More information

Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 2012, 2:11

Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 2012, 2:11 Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders This Provisional PDF corresponds to the article as it appeared upon acceptance. Fully formatted PDF and full text (HTML) versions will be made available soon. Neural

More information

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri Magnetic Resonance Imaging 24 (2006) 181 185 Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri Birgit Abler a, 4, Alard Roebroeck b, Rainer Goebel b, Anett

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/317/5835/215/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Prefrontal Regions Orchestrate Suppression of Emotional Memories via a Two- Phase Process Brendan E. Depue,* Tim Curran,

More information

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy Methods Participants We examined a group of twelve male pathological gamblers (ten strictly right handed, all smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy males,

More information

Supplementary Results: Age Differences in Participants Matched on Performance

Supplementary Results: Age Differences in Participants Matched on Performance Supplementary Results: Age Differences in Participants Matched on Performance 1 We selected 14 participants for each age group which exhibited comparable behavioral performance (ps >.41; Hit rates (M ±

More information

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way Mortality and morbidity attributable to use of addictive substances in the United States. The Association of American Physicians from 1999 60 million tobacco smokers in the U.S. 14 million dependent on

More information

doi: /brain/awq299 Brain 2011: 134; Neural representation of self-concept in sighted and congenitally blind adults

doi: /brain/awq299 Brain 2011: 134; Neural representation of self-concept in sighted and congenitally blind adults doi:10.1093/brain/awq299 Brain 2011: 134; 235 246 235 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Neural representation of self-concept in sighted and congenitally blind adults Yina Ma and Shihui Han Department of Psychology,

More information

2017, Joule Inc. or its licensors Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors.

2017, Joule Inc. or its licensors Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors. Results Validation: Reproducibility Figure S1. Reproducibility of the results of small-world parameters. Differences in topological properties of functional brain networks between bulimia nervosa (BN)

More information

The Cognitive Control of Memory: Age Differences in the Neural Correlates of Successful Remembering and Intentional Forgetting

The Cognitive Control of Memory: Age Differences in the Neural Correlates of Successful Remembering and Intentional Forgetting The Cognitive Control of Memory: Age Differences in the Neural Correlates of Successful Remembering and Intentional Forgetting Avery A. Rizio, Nancy A. Dennis* The Pennsylvania State University, Department

More information

Fundamentals of Brief Cessation Counseling Approaches

Fundamentals of Brief Cessation Counseling Approaches Fundamentals of Brief Cessation Counseling Approaches Jamie S. Ostroff Ph.D. Director, Smoking Cessation Program Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center Co-Project Leader Queens Quits! Cessation Center

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

Neural correlates of recognition memory for emotional faces and scenes

Neural correlates of recognition memory for emotional faces and scenes doi:10.1093/scan/nsq003 Neural correlates of recognition memory for emotional faces and scenes SCAN (2011) 6, 24 ^37 Michelle L. Keightley, 1,2,3,4,5 Kimberly S. Chiew, 6 John A. E. Anderson, 5,6 and Cheryl

More information

NeuroImage 59 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 59 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 59 (2012) 2908 2922 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Routes to the past: Neural substrates of direct and generative

More information

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018)

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) 1 / 22 Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) Jérôme Dockès, ussel Poldrack, Demian Wassermann, Fabian Suchanek, Bertrand

More information

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview Investigations in Resting State Connectivity Scott FMRI Laboratory Overview Introduction Functional connectivity explorations Dynamic change (motor fatigue) Neurological change (Asperger s Disorder, depression)

More information

Supplementary Information Appendix: Default Mode Contributions to Automated Information Processing

Supplementary Information Appendix: Default Mode Contributions to Automated Information Processing Supplementary Information Appendix: Default Mode Contributions to Automated Information Processing Authors: Deniz Vatansever a,b,c,1, David K. Menon a,d, Emmanuel A. Stamatakis a,b,d Affiliations: a Division

More information

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety brain research 1481 (2012) 13 36 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship

More information

GENDER-SPECIFIC SENSITVITY TO TIME-DISCREPANT TASK CONDITIONS OF REASONING DURING fmri

GENDER-SPECIFIC SENSITVITY TO TIME-DISCREPANT TASK CONDITIONS OF REASONING DURING fmri GENDER-SPECIFIC SENSITVITY TO TIME-DISCREPANT TASK CONDITIONS OF REASONING DURING fmri by Joshua M. Roberts A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of George Mason University in Partial Fulfillment

More information

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention Goldsmiths Research Online. The Authors. Originally published: Science vol.291 2 March 2001 1803-1806. http://www.sciencemag.org. 11 October 2000; accepted 17 January 2001 The Role of Working Memory in

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load

The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load Vogan et al. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 2014, 6:19 RESEARCH Open Access The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive

More information

Title of file for HTML: Peer Review File Description:

Title of file for HTML: Peer Review File Description: Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figure, Supplementary Tables, Supplementary Methods and Supplementary References Title of file for HTML: Peer Review File Description:

More information

Final Report 2017 Authors: Affiliations: Title of Project: Background:

Final Report 2017 Authors: Affiliations: Title of Project: Background: Final Report 2017 Authors: Dr Gershon Spitz, Ms Abbie Taing, Professor Jennie Ponsford, Dr Matthew Mundy, Affiliations: Epworth Research Foundation and Monash University Title of Project: The return of

More information

NeuroImage 45 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 45 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 45 (2009) 614 626 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fmri

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Cover Page. The handle  holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/32078 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Author: Pannekoek, Nienke Title: Using novel imaging approaches in affective disorders

More information

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Parkland College A with Honors Projects Honors Program 2015 Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Hannah Li Parkland College Recommended Citation Li, Hannah, "Social and Pragmatic Language

More information

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease Andrey V. Sokolov 1,3, Sergey V. Vorobyev 2, Aleksandr Yu. Efimtcev 1,3, Viacheslav S. Dekan 1,3, Gennadiy E. Trufanov

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Blame judgment task.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Blame judgment task. Supplementary Figure Blame judgment task. Participants viewed others decisions and were asked to rate them on a scale from blameworthy to praiseworthy. Across trials we independently manipulated the amounts

More information

Changes in Default Mode Network as Automaticity Develops in a Categorization Task

Changes in Default Mode Network as Automaticity Develops in a Categorization Task Purdue University Purdue e-pubs Open Access Theses Theses and Dissertations January 2015 Changes in Default Mode Network as Automaticity Develops in a Categorization Task Farzin Shamloo Purdue University

More information

Brain gray matter volume changes associated with motor symptoms in patients with Parkinson s disease

Brain gray matter volume changes associated with motor symptoms in patients with Parkinson s disease Kang et al. Chinese Neurosurgical Journal (2015) 1:9 DOI 10.1186/s41016-015-0003-6 RESEARCH Open Access Brain gray matter volume changes associated with motor symptoms in patients with Parkinson s disease

More information

Neural correlates of two imagined egocentric transformations

Neural correlates of two imagined egocentric transformations www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 35 (2007) 916 927 Neural correlates of two imagined egocentric transformations Sarah H. Creem-Regehr, Jayson A. Neil, and Hsiang J. Yeh Department of Psychology,

More information

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex in dual-task performance Fabienne Collette a,b, Laurence 01ivier b,c, Martial Van der Linden a,d, Steven Laureys b, Guy Delfiore b, André Luxen

More information

Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Appendix 1 to Sehmbi M, Rowley CD, Minuzzi L, et al. Age-related deficits in intracortical myelination in young adults with bipolar SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Supplementary Methods Intracortical Myelin (ICM)

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Histograms of original and phase-randomised data

Supplementary Figure 1. Histograms of original and phase-randomised data Log Supplementary Figure 1. Histograms of original and phase-randomised data BOLD signals histogram Denoised signals histogram Activity-inducing signals histogram Innovation signals histogram 20 15 10

More information

Dissociation between Dorsal and Ventral Posterior Parietal Cortical Responses to Incidental Changes in Natural Scenes

Dissociation between Dorsal and Ventral Posterior Parietal Cortical Responses to Incidental Changes in Natural Scenes Dissociation between Dorsal and Ventral Posterior Parietal Cortical Responses to Incidental Changes in Natural Scenes Lorelei R. Howard 1, Dharshan Kumaran 2, H. Freyja Ólafsdóttir 1, Hugo J. Spiers 1

More information

doi: /brain/awq006 Brain 2010: 133; Imaging memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: predicting the effects of temporal lobe resection

doi: /brain/awq006 Brain 2010: 133; Imaging memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: predicting the effects of temporal lobe resection doi:10.1093/brain/awq006 Brain 2010: 133; 1186 1199 1186 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Imaging memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: predicting the effects of temporal lobe resection Silvia B. Bonelli, 1,2

More information

Distinct neural bases of disruptive behavior and autism symptom severity in boys with autism spectrum disorder.

Distinct neural bases of disruptive behavior and autism symptom severity in boys with autism spectrum disorder. Himmelfarb Health Sciences Library, The George Washington University Health Sciences Research Commons Pediatrics Faculty Publications Pediatrics 1-1-2017 Distinct neural bases of disruptive behavior and

More information

The hippocampus operates in a threshold manner during spatial source memory Scott D. Slotnick a and Preston P. Thakral b

The hippocampus operates in a threshold manner during spatial source memory Scott D. Slotnick a and Preston P. Thakral b Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology 265 The hippocampus operates in a threshold manner during spatial source memory Scott D. Slotnick a and Preston P. Thakral b Long-term memory can be based on

More information

Behavioral Task Performance

Behavioral Task Performance Zacks 1 Supplementary content for: Functional Reorganization of Spatial Transformations After a Parietal Lesion Jeffrey M. Zacks, PhD *, Pascale Michelon, PhD *, Jean M. Vettel, BA *, and Jeffrey G. Ojemann,

More information

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest Experimental Design Kate Watkins Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford With thanks to: Heidi Johansen-Berg Joe Devlin Outline Choices for experimental paradigm Subtraction / hierarchical

More information

Session 1: Days 1-3. Session 4: Days Session 2: Days 4-7. Session 5: Days Session 3: Days Day 21: Quit Day!

Session 1: Days 1-3. Session 4: Days Session 2: Days 4-7. Session 5: Days Session 3: Days Day 21: Quit Day! Tobacco cessation overview calendar 21-Day Countdown to Quitting Session 1: Days 1-3 List health benefits of quitting. List expectations of overcoming your habits and addictions. List your top three Schedule

More information

Neural evidence for cultural differences in the valuation of positive facial expressions

Neural evidence for cultural differences in the valuation of positive facial expressions Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2016, 243 252 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv113 Advance Access Publication Date: 4 September 2015 Original Article Neural evidence for cultural differences in the valuation

More information

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1428 1442 Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Alessandra Schiavetto a, Stefan Köhler a, Cheryl L. Grady a, Gordon Winocur a,c,

More information