TWO TYPES OF CONDITIONED REFLEX AND A PSEUDO TYPE[*] From the Biological Laboratories of Harvard University

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "TWO TYPES OF CONDITIONED REFLEX AND A PSEUDO TYPE[*] From the Biological Laboratories of Harvard University"

Transcription

1 TWO TYPES OF CONDITIONED REFLEX AND A PSEUDO TYPE[*] From the Biological Laboratories of Harvard University B.F. Skinner (1935)[1] Classics in the History of Psychology An internet resource developed by Christopher D. Green York University, Toronto, Ontario (Return to Classics index) TWO TYPES OF CONDITIONED REFLEX AND A PSEUDO TYPE[*] From the Biological Laboratories of Harvard University B.F. Skinner (1935)[1] First published in Journal of General Psychology, 12, A conditioned reflex is said to be conditioned in the sense of being dependent for its existence or state upon the occurrence of a certain kind of event, having to do with the presentation of a reinforcing stimulus. A definition that includes much more than this simple notion will probably not be applicable to all cases. At almost any significant level of analysis a distinction must be made between at least two major types of conditioned reflex. These may be represented, with examples, in the following way (where S = stimulus, R = response, (S - R) = reflex, -> = "is followed by," and [ ] = "the strength of" the inclosed reflex): Given such a sequence, where [S 1 -R 1 ] ~ = 0,[2] conditioning occurs as a change in [S o -R o ] - an increase in strength (positive conditioning) in (a) and a decrease (negative conditioning) in (b).[p.67] Given the simultaneous or successive presentation of S' o and S' 1, where [S' 1 ] ~ =0,[2] conditioning occurs as an increase in [S' o ]. Differences between the types are as follows: 1. In Type I, S o ->R o -> S 1, where R o necessarily intervenes between the stimuli; in Type II, S' o ->S' 1, where R' o is ignored.[3] In I, R o is important; it becomes the conditioned response. In II, R' o is irrelevant (except when it is relevant in another sense by conflicting with R' 1 ) and may actually disappear.

2 Since conditioning of the second type may take place even when S' 1 occurs after R' o, Paradigm II, Example (c), may be written for this case as follows: light r > food salivation, when it is identical with I. But the result is not to reduce the two types to a single form. Both kinds of conditioning proceed simultaneously but separately. If r is "turning toward the light," for example, and if the food is withheld until turning takes place, [light - turning] will increase according to I while [light-salivation] will increase according to II. The same result is obtained with negative conditioning. Example (d) gives, upon delaying S' 1, light r > shock flexion, etc., where [light--r] will decrease according to I, while [light-flexion] increases according to II. In the special case in which R o and R' 1 are of the same form, the two kinds can apparently not be separated. If, for example, some unconditioned salivation is supposed to be elicitable by a light,[4] we may substitute "salivation" for r, to obtain light - salivation I > food - salivation II. Both [light - salivation I] and [light - salivation II] will increase, with apparently no possible distinction. This is a very special case and is also in no sense a reduction to a single type. 2. In I, (S o ) occurs normally in the absence of (S 1 - R 1 ), [p.68] and its strength may be measured without interfering with the reinforcing action of S 1. In II S' 1 must be withheld whenever a measurement of [S' o ], the conditioned reflex, is taken, because S' 1 also evokes R' 1. Some amount of extinction necessarily ensues in the second case. 3. Since S' 1 must be withheld in measuring [S' o ], R' 1 must be independent of any property of S' 1 not possessed by S' o. In Example (c) salivation may become attached to the light as a conditioned response of Type II; but seizing, chewing, and swallowing, which are also responses to S' 1, must not be included in the paradigm since they require the presence of parts of S' 1 which cannot be supplied by S' o. A special restriction on Paradigm II is therefore necessary. Where S' o is of a very simple sort (a tone, for example), the properties possessed in addition to S' o by S' 1 are practically equal to S' 1, and we may express the restriction in terms of a general distinction between two kinds of response. The first kind require no external point of reference in their elicitation or description. Typical examples are: glandular activities (salivation), local muscular responses (flexion, wink, breathing movements, production of sounds), and facilitation and inhibition.[5] The second kind require points of reference for their elicitation or description which are not supplied by the organism itself, but by the stimulus. Examples are: orientation toward the source of a sound, approaching a light, and touching, seizing and manipulating objects (such as a lever or food). Our present rule is that responses of the second kind cannot be substituted for R' 1 in Paradigm II, unless S' also supplies the required points of reference. 4. In Type I, [S o ] ~ = 0 before conditioning takes place. The reflex-to-be-conditioned must be elicited at least once as an unconditioned "investigatory" reflex. In Type II, [S' o ] may begin at zero and usually does. In Type I the state of the reflex is "conditioned" by the occurrence of the reinforcing sequence, but its existence is not. A distinction between a conditioned and an unconditioned relfex is here less significant, because all examples of the [p.69] former have necessarily been examples of the latter. There are no exclusively

3 conditioned reflexes in this type. Since [S' o ] may begin at zero, a new reflex may be created in conditioning of the second type. And since practically any stimulus may be attached to R' 1 in Paradigm II, a very large number of new reflexes can thus be derived. Conditioning of Type I, on the other hand, is not a device for increasing the repertory of reflexes; R o continues to be elicited by the one stimulus with which it began. There are three reflexes in Paradigm II, but only two in I. 5. The significant change in Type I may be either an increase or a decrease in strength; in Type II it is an increase only, even when [S' o ] does not begin at zero. In Type I stimuli may be divided into two classes, positively and negatively conditioning, according to whether they produce an increase or decrease when used as reinforcement. The distinction cannot be made in Type II, where a reflex may be negative in another sense (a reflex of "avoidance," for example), but where its strength only increases during conditioning. 6. In Type I the conditioned reflex (S o ) may be associated with any drive; in Type II the reflex (S' o ) is necessarily attached to the drive specified by R' 1. This point may require some comment. In the present use of the term a drive is an inferred variable of which the strength of a group of reflexes is a function (2). Hunger, for example, is a variable (H) a change in which is responsible for concurrent changes in the strength (a) of all unconditioned reflexes concerned with the ingestion of food, (b) of all conditioned reflexes (of either type) in which the reinforcing stimulus is concerned with the ingestion of food, and (c) to a much lesser extent of all "investigatory" reflexes. In Paradigm I, Example a (lever - pressing) is originally a function of H to some slight extent under (c) above. After conditioning it varies with H according to (b), over a wide range probably equal to that of any unconditioned reflex under (a). Conditioning of Type I is really the becoming attached to a group of reflexes varying as a function of some drive. This is a much more comprehensive description of the process than to define it as an increase in strength, where the drive is assumed to remain constant at a significant value. But the identity of H in the present case is [p.70] determined only by our choice of a reinforcing reflex. Given (S 1 - R 1 ) of another drive, say thirst, then (S o ) will become conditioned by attaching itself to the group varying with thirst, and will not vary with H except to some slight extent under (c). This is a characteristic wholly lacking in Type II. Here R' is originally part of the unconditioned reflex and the drive to which it belongs is definitely fixed. 7. A minor difference is in the way in which the stimulus-to-be-conditioned usually acts. In Type I, S o is usually part of a larger field, and R o occurs as the result of the eventual prepotency of S o over other stimuli. In Type II, S' o is usually suddenly presented to the organism. The significance of this difference, which is not absolute, will appear later. We shall now consider a third type of relation which involves a discrimination. It may be based upon a conditioned reflex of either type, but we shall begin with I. To establish a discrimination subdivide S o into two classes on the basis of a selected property or component member (3, 4). For example, let the lever stimulate either in the presence of a light (L), when the stimulus may be written as S A B..L.. (subcripts indicate properties or components), or in the dark, when the stimulus is S A.B.... Continue to reinforce the re-response to one of them, say S A.B..L.., and extinguish or negatively condition the response to the other by breaking the sequence at S 1 or by introducing as S 1 of the negatively conditioning kind (Difference 5). When this has been

4 done, [S A.B..L. ---R o ] > [S A.B.. ---R o ]. And at any value of the underlying drive such that (S A.B..L --- R o ) is usually elicited but (S AB ---R o ) is not, there exists the following condition. Given an organism in the presence of S AB.. ordinarily unresponsive, the presentation of L will be followed by a response. For the sake of comparison we may set up a paradigm in imitation of II as follows: The relation between the light and the response to the lever might be called pseudo-conditioned reflex. It has some of the characteristics of Type II: the original response to the light is irrelevant [p.71] (Difference 1); the relation may be wholly absent prior to the "conditioning" (Difference 4); it changes in a positive direction only (Difference 5); and the "stimulus" is usually of the presented kind (Difference 7). In all these respects it differs from Type I, although the example is based upon a reflex of that type. I many other respects it differs from both types. A reinforcing reflex is not included in the paradigm, but must be added as a third or fourth reflex. The response is not principally to the light, but to the lever; the light is only a component member of the whole stimulus, and "light-pressing" is not legitimately the expression of a reflex. The lever cannot be removed to show the conditioned effectivelness of the light as in Type II; instead, the response to the lever alone must be extinguished - a characteristic that we have not met before. In spite of these differences it is often said (in similar cases) that the light becomes the "conditioned stimulus for the response to the lever" just as it becomes the stimulus for salivation. This is a confusion with Type II which obviously arises from a neglect of the extinguished reflex. The relation of pressing the lever to the lever itself is ignored and only the relation to the light taken into account. The lever comes to be treated, not as a source of stimulation, but as part of the apparatus, relevant to the response only for mechanical reasons. When the discrimination is based upon a response not requiring an external point of reference (Difference 3), the chance of this neglect increases enormously. If we substitute "flexion of a leg" for "pressing a lever" (and continue for the moment with Type I), S o in Paradigm I is not directly observable; we simply wait until a flexion appears, then reinforce. Having established (S o ) as a conditioned reflex of some strength, we subdivide our inferred S o as before, extinguish (S AB.. ), and reinforce (S AB..L.. ). When the discrimination has been set up, we have a condition in which the organism is ordinarily unresponsive but immediately responds with flexion upon presentation of the light. Our inability to demonstrate S o makes it difficult to show the discriminative nature of this relation; but it is by no means impossible to find other grounds, as we may see by comparing it with a true reflex of Type II. Let the presentation of the light be followed by a shock to the foot until the light alone elicits flexion. The resulting reflex is superficially similar to the relation of light and [p.72] flexion that we have just examined, but fundamentally the two cases are unlike. Assuming that no immediate difference can in fact be detected,[6] we may still show differences by referring forward or backward to the history of the organism. The two relations have been established in different ways and their continued existence depends upon reinforcement from different stimuli. The discriminative drive relation also varies with an arbitrarily chosen drive, while the conditioned reflex is necessarily attached to the drive to which shock-flexion belongs. These differences are chiefly due, however, to the use of a conditioned reflex of Type I in setting up the discrimination. In a pseudo-conditioned reflex based upon Type II the distinction is much less sure. Here we are invariably able to neglect the extinguished member because R' 1 is of the kind not requiring an external point of reference (Difference 3), and we can minimize its importance in other ways. Given a conditioned reflex of this kind: if we establish a discrimination between the tone and the tone-plus-a light (reinforcing the resonse to the

5 latter), we obtain the following condition: an organism in the presence of the tone, ordinarily unresponsive, will respond upon presentation of the light. The only difference between this relation and a true reflex of Type II is the extinction of the response to the tone, which is evidence that a discrimination has taken place. The reinforcement of tone and light should condition responses to both of these stimuli; but we observe that the organism is unresponsive in the presence of the tone alone. Now, this surviving difference may be reduced at will by reducing the significance of S' o in the basic reflex of the pseudo-type. If we lower the intensity of the tone or choose another stimulus of a less important kind, we may approach as closely as we please to a conditioned reflex of Type II. We cannot actually reach Type II in [p.73] this way, but we can easily reach a point at which our pseudo-reflex is identical with any actual experimental example of that type. This is true because some amount of discrimination is practically always involved in cases of Type II. When we put a dog into a stand, present a light and then food, the food reinforces not only the light but the stimulation from the stand. Merely putting the dog into the stand again should elicit salivation according to Paradigm II. In practice this is a disturbing effect, which must be eliminated through extinction. So long as it occurs, any actual case of Type II must be formulated as a pseudo-conditioned reflex. If S G is the stimulation affecting the organism in addition to S o, then S o Paradigm II should read S G + S o. The effect upon S G is extinguished through lack of reinforcement in the absence of S o, and the result is a discrimination: an organism is the presence of S G, ordinarily unresponsive, responds when S o is added. The importance of this criticism will depend upon the relative magnitudes of S G and S o. In the optimal experiment S G may be reduced to a value that is insignificant in comparison with ordinary values of S o. The partially discriminative nature of Type II is invevitable. It is not important in Type I because of Difference 1. Paradigm I contains an implicit specification that S o is active or has just acted at the moment of reinforcement, since it specifies that S 1 is to be withheld until R o has occurred. The reinforced stimulus is really S o and not S G + S o (it is the lever, in our example, not the whole stimulating field presented by the apparatus). Paradigm II contains no specification of the activity of S o ; and the reinforcing action of S' 1 must be supposed to extend to S G as well as to S o. In practice an active state at the moment of reinforcement is usually insured by presenting S o suddenly.[7] This might be included as an additional provision in Paradigm II, but the provision really required is that S o, and no part of S G, be active at the moment of reinforcement. This is not easily arranged. We cannot wholly avoid the generalized action of the reinforcement in Type II because of the lack of dependence of S' 1 upon R' o. One characteristic of the pseudo-conditioned reflex is the variety [p.74] of the forms of its "stimulus". We have assumed that in our two fundamental paradigms any stimulus had ultimately the dimensions of energy (although we have often used the shorthand device of speaking of the source of the energy - as, for example, "lever"). In the pseudo type, however, the "stimulus" can be a single property. It can be the intensity of the stimulus, or some such qualitative aspect as pitch or hue. It can be a change from one value of a property to another, or the absence of a property, or a duration. The reason why this is possible is that the other properties of the stimulus can be relegated to S G for extinction. If the pitch of a tone is to be a conditioned "stimulus," the tone itself must first become one also, and the response to its other properties must be extinguished by extinguishing the responses to tones of other pitches. In a true conditioned reflex this cannot be done. Although it is common to speak of properties as stimuli (1), the presence of a property in the position of a stimulus is a certain indication that a pseudo-conditioned reflex is really in quesiton. A property alone cannot be used in either true type because it implies extinction; most of the real stimulus must be relegated to S G, and the requirement that the value of S G be negligible cannot therefore be satisfied.

6 The position of a pseudo-conditioned reflex may be summarized as follows. When the pseudoreflex is based upon a reflex of Type I and when R o requires external points of reference, there are important practical and theoretical reasons why a separate formulation is demanded. When R o does not require external points of reference, there are fewer differences, but a separate formulation is still necessary. When the pseudo type is based upon a reflex of Type II, the distinction is weakened but should still be made, except when S G can be reduced to a very low value relative to S' o. In the last case a practical distinction is impossible, not because of an identity of types, but because of the failure of Type II to appear experimentally in a pure form. It is a tempting hypothesis that II is not an authentic type but may be reduced to a discrimination based on Type I. But this has not been shown; we have not reduced the pseudo type to Type II or vice versa. Nor have we come very near it. The present pseudo-reflex which resembles II most closely requires of that type for its establishment. It is probably more than a coincidence that [p.75] a discrimination based upon Type I has so many of the properties of II, but the reduction to a single type appears from our present evidence to be highly improbable, desirable though it would be as an immense simplification. The differences that we have noted are not easily disposed of. Still more improbable is a reduction of I to II, since the first step supplied by the pseudo type is then lacking. To the differences we have listed might be added differences in the parts played by the two types in the economy of the organism. The essence of Type II is the substitution of one stimulus for another, or, as Pavlov has put it, signalization (1). It prepares the organism by obtaining the elicitation of a response before the original stimulus has begun to act, and it does this by letting any stimulus that has incidently accompanied or anticipated the original stimulus act in its stead. In Type I there is no substitution of stimuli and consequently no signalization. Type I acts in another way: the organism selects from a large repertory of unconditioned reflexes those of which the repetition is important with respect to certain elementary functions and discards those of which it is unimportant. The conditioned response of Type I does not prepare for the reinforcing stimulus, it produces it. The stimulus-to-be-conditioned is never in any sense incidental. Type I plays the more important rôle. When an organism comes accidentally[8] upon a new kind of food, which it seizes and eats, both kinds of conditioning presumably occur. When the visible radiation from the food next stimulates the organism, salivation is evoked according to Paradigm II. This secretion remains useless until the food is actually seized and eaten. But seizing and eating will depend upon the same accidental factors as before unless conditioning of Type I has also occurred - that is, unless the strength of the reflex (food-seizing) has increased. Thus while a reflex of Type II prepares the organism, a reflex of Type I obtains the food for which the preparation is made. And this is in general a fair characterization of the relative importance of the two types. As Pavlov has said, conditioned stimuli are important in providing saliva before food is received, but "even greater is their importance when they evoke the motor component of the complex reflex of nutrition, i.e., [p.7 6] when they act as stimuli to the reflex of seeking food." (1, p.13).[9] Although "the reflex of seeking food" is an unfortunate expression, it refers clearly enough to behavior characteristic of Type I. Footnotes [*] Accepted for publication by Carl Murchison of the Editorial Board and received in the Editorial Office, June 4, [1] Society of Fellows, Harvard University. [2] This expression specifies the presence of some amount of drive (2).

7 [3] For convenience we shall omit the case of simultaneous stimuli in Type II. [4] See Difference 4 below for this general requirement in Type I. [5] Where conditioned facilitation and inhibition are defined by substituting for R' 1 in Paradigm II the expressions "Incr. [S" - R"]" and "Decr. [S" - R"]" respectively. (Incr. = "increase in"; Decr. = "decrease in"). [6] This is a generous assumption since some evidence for the presence of S o can usually be found. A difference in the character of the response might also be shown (in the case of the true reflex it may be accompanied by changes in breathing rate, for example, which would be lacking in the pseudo reflex). [7] This is our explanation of Difference 7. Another explanation might be added. If S' o is active for any length of time prior to S 1 it will have an extinguishing effect. This cannot be said of Type I. [8] That is to say, as the result of weak investigatory reflexes. [9] This is a doubly interesting statement because Pavlov has confined his own investigations practically exclusively to conditioned reflexes of the second type. It ought to be said that he usually regards this type as adequate for the whole field. Thus he says that the "function of the hemispheres" is signalization (1, p.17), although signalization is, as we have seen, a characteristic of Type II only. References 1. Pavlov, I.P. Conditioned reflexes (Trans. & ed. by G.V. Anrep) London: Oxford Univ. Press, Pp xvi Skinner, B.F. Drive and reflex strength: I. J. Gen. Psychol., 1932, 6, The rate of establishment of a discrimination. J. Gen. Psychol., 1933, 9, The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. J. Gen. Psychol., 1935, 12, Harvard University Cambridge, Massachusetts DEUX TYPES DU RÉFLEXE CONDITIONEL ET UN PSEUDO-TYPE (Résumé) On définit deux types principaux du réflexe conditionnel et donne une liste des différences entre eux. Un pseudo-réflexe conditionel, où il s'agit d'une discrimination, peut être basé sur le Type I pour donner quelques-unes des propriétés du Type II. Quand it est basé sur le Type II il se peut dans un cas spécial, qu'on ne puisse le distinguer de ce type, mais cela est dû au manque du Type II de se montrer expérimentalement dans une forme pure et n'indique pas une réduction à un seul type. Dans une pseudo-réflexe le "stimulus" peut avoir la forme d'une seule propriété, mais cela n'est possible dans l'un ni l'autre vrai type. On note la place de

8 chaque type dans l'économie de l'organisme. Skinner ZWEI TYPEN DES BEDINGTEN REFLEXES UND EIN PSEUDOTYPUS (Referat) Zwei Haupttypen des bedingten Reflexes werden bestimmt und Unterschiede zwischen ihnen angegeben. Ein pseudobedingter Reflex, der eine Unterscheidung voraussetzt kann auf Typus I beruhen, so dass er einige der Eigenschaften von Typus II aufweist. Wenn er auf Typus II beruht, kann er in einem besonderem Fall nicht unterscheidbar von jenem Typus sein, aber dies ist die Folge der Nichterscheinung des Typus II experimentell in einer reinen Form und beweist eine Zurückführung auf einen einzelnen Typus nicht. Bei einem Pseudoreflex kann der "Reiz" die Form einer einzelnen Eigenschaft besitzen, aber dies ist nicht möglich in den beiden wahren Typen. Die Stellung jedes Typus in der Ökonomie des Organismus wird bemerkt. Skinner

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events.

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. behaviorism - the view that psychology (1) should be an objective science

More information

DEFINITION. Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience

DEFINITION. Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience LEARNING DEFINITION Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience WHAT DO WE LEARN? 1. Object :we learn objects with

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events acquisition associative learning In classical conditioning, the initial stage, when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

Classical & Operant Conditioning. Learning: Principles and Applications

Classical & Operant Conditioning. Learning: Principles and Applications Classical & Operant Conditioning Learning: Principles and Applications Which Pen Would You Choose? The researchers placed the participants in the room. In this room the participants first viewed purple

More information

Classical Conditioning II. Generalization and Discrimination. Generalization Example. Generalization/Discrimination; Time and the Order of Events

Classical Conditioning II. Generalization and Discrimination. Generalization Example. Generalization/Discrimination; Time and the Order of Events Classical Conditioning II Generalization/Discrimination; Time and the Order of Events Generalization and Discrimination Generalization responding to a stimulus because of its similarity to the CS. Discrimination

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback acquisition associative learning in classical conditioning the initial stage when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

Audio: In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2

Audio: In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2 Psychology 312: Lecture 2 Psychology as a Science Slide #1 Psychology As A Science In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2 Outline Psychology is an empirical science.

More information

What is Learned? Lecture 9

What is Learned? Lecture 9 What is Learned? Lecture 9 1 Classical and Instrumental Conditioning Compared Classical Reinforcement Not Contingent on Behavior Behavior Elicited by US Involuntary Response (Reflex) Few Conditionable

More information

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher.

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher. 1. A stimulus change that increases the future frequency of behavior that immediately precedes it. 2. In operant conditioning, a reinforcement schedule that reinforces a response only after a specified

More information

I. Classical Conditioning

I. Classical Conditioning Learning Chapter 8 Learning A relatively permanent change in an organism that occur because of prior experience Psychologists must study overt behavior or physical changes to study learning Learning I.

More information

Association. Operant Conditioning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Learning to associate two events. We learn to. associate two stimuli

Association. Operant Conditioning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Learning to associate two events. We learn to. associate two stimuli Myers PSYCHOLOGY (7th Ed) Chapter 8 Learning James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University Worth Publishers Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience Association

More information

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov ( )

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov ( ) Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (1849-1936) Chapter 7 1 Ivan Petrovich Pavlov 1. Born in Ryazan, Russia on Sep 14, 1849. 2. Studied the digestive system and won the Nobel Prize in 1904 for physiology and medicine.

More information

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation.

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation. Unit 06 - Overview How We Learn and Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Operant Conditioning s Applications, and Comparison to Classical Conditioning Biology, Cognition, and Learning Learning By

More information

ELEMENTS OF PSYCHOPHYSICS Sections VII and XVI. Gustav Theodor Fechner (1860/1912)

ELEMENTS OF PSYCHOPHYSICS Sections VII and XVI. Gustav Theodor Fechner (1860/1912) ELEMENTS OF PSYCHOPHYSICS Sections VII and XVI Gustav Theodor Fechner (1860/1912) Translated by Herbert Sidney Langfeld (1912) [Classics Editor's note: This translation of these passages from Fechner's

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

CHAPTER 7 LEARNING. Jake Miller, Ocean Lakes High School

CHAPTER 7 LEARNING. Jake Miller, Ocean Lakes High School CHAPTER 7 LEARNING Jake Miller, Ocean Lakes High School Learning: Defined Learning: Relatively permanent change in [observable] behavior due to experience NOT temporary changes due to disease, injury,

More information

Unit 6 Learning.

Unit 6 Learning. Unit 6 Learning https://www.apstudynotes.org/psychology/outlines/chapter-6-learning/ 1. Overview 1. Learning 1. A long lasting change in behavior resulting from experience 2. Classical Conditioning 1.

More information

Learning. Learning. Learning

Learning. Learning. Learning Learning PSY 1000: Introduction to Psychology Learning Monkeys beginning to wash their food before they eat it The rituals that athletes perform before/during contests Birds learning to flutter their wings

More information

Classical Conditioning. Learning. Classical conditioning terms. Classical Conditioning Procedure. Procedure, cont. Important concepts

Classical Conditioning. Learning. Classical conditioning terms. Classical Conditioning Procedure. Procedure, cont. Important concepts Learning Classical Conditioning Pavlov study of digestion dogs salivate before getting food learning as signal detection: emphasis on what happens before a given behavior Classical conditioning terms Stimulus:

More information

Learning theory provides the basis for behavioral interventions. The USMLE behavioral science section always contains questions relating to learning

Learning theory provides the basis for behavioral interventions. The USMLE behavioral science section always contains questions relating to learning Learning theory provides the basis for behavioral interventions. The USMLE behavioral science section always contains questions relating to learning theory. Learning theory does not apply to behaviors

More information

Chapter 7 - Learning

Chapter 7 - Learning Chapter 7 - Learning How Do We Learn Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Defining Learning Learning a relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience.

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning Operant Conditioning The nature of reinforcement: Premack s probability differential theory Response deprivation

More information

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5 Learning AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5 Learning Learning is a lasting change in behavior or mental process as the result of an experience. There are two important parts: a lasting change a simple reflexive reaction

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

an ability that has been acquired by training (process) acquisition aversive conditioning behavior modification biological preparedness

an ability that has been acquired by training (process) acquisition aversive conditioning behavior modification biological preparedness acquisition an ability that has been acquired by training (process) aversive conditioning A type of counterconditioning that associates an unpleasant state (such as nausea) with an unwanted behavior (such

More information

For her own breakfast she ll project a scheme Nor take her tea without a stratagem. --- Edward Young ( ) Mathematics of a Lady Tasting Tea

For her own breakfast she ll project a scheme Nor take her tea without a stratagem. --- Edward Young ( ) Mathematics of a Lady Tasting Tea For her own breakfast she ll project a scheme Nor take her tea without a stratagem. --- Edward Young (1683-1765) "Come 1ittle girl, you seem To want my cup of tea And will you take a little cream? Now

More information

Myers PSYCHOLOGY. (7th Ed) Chapter 8. Learning. James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University. Worth Publishers

Myers PSYCHOLOGY. (7th Ed) Chapter 8. Learning. James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University. Worth Publishers Myers PSYCHOLOGY (7th Ed) Chapter 8 Learning James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University Worth Publishers Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience Association

More information

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives Theoretical Perspectives REBT (1955) Albert Ellis Action & Result Oriented Teaches how to identify self-defeating thoughts Replaces thoughts w/ life enhancing ones 1 A B C s of personality formation: A

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. What is Learning? Learning is the process that allows us to adapt (be flexible) to the changing conditions

More information

PSYC 221 Introduction to General Psychology

PSYC 221 Introduction to General Psychology PSYC 221 Introduction to General Psychology Session 5 Learning Lecturer: Dr. Joana Salifu Yendork, Psychology Department Contact Information: jyendork@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing

More information

Organizational Behaviour

Organizational Behaviour Bachelor of Commerce Programme Organizational Behaviour Individual Behaviour Motivational concepts and application The Da Vinci Institute for Technology Management (Pty) Ltd Registered with the Department

More information

Strengthening Operant Behavior: Schedules of Reinforcement. reinforcement occurs after every desired behavior is exhibited

Strengthening Operant Behavior: Schedules of Reinforcement. reinforcement occurs after every desired behavior is exhibited OPERANT CONDITIONING Strengthening Operant Behavior: Schedules of Reinforcement CONTINUOUS REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE reinforcement occurs after every desired behavior is exhibited pro: necessary for initial

More information

Learning. Association. Association. Unit 6: Learning. Learning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Different Types of Learning

Learning. Association. Association. Unit 6: Learning. Learning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Different Types of Learning Unit 6: Learning Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience experience (nurture) is the key to learning Different Types of Learning Classical -learn by association

More information

Classical and Instrumental Conditioning. Lecture 8

Classical and Instrumental Conditioning. Lecture 8 Classical and Instrumental Conditioning Lecture 8 1 Basic Procedure for Classical Conditioning CS (Bell) US (Meat Powder) CR (Salivation) UR (Salivation) 2 Acquisition Major Phenomena of Classical Conditioning

More information

What is Learning? Learning: any relatively permanent change in behavior brought about by experience or practice

What is Learning? Learning: any relatively permanent change in behavior brought about by experience or practice CHAPTER 5 learning What is Learning? Learning: any relatively permanent change in behavior brought about by experience or practice When people learn anything, some part of their brain is physically changed

More information

Seminar: Skinner s Analysis of Verbal Behavior PRIVATE EVENTS

Seminar: Skinner s Analysis of Verbal Behavior PRIVATE EVENTS Seminar: Skinner s Analysis of Verbal Behavior PRIVATE EVENTS Dr Emily Kerwin Regis College Private Events in a Natural Science Behavior analysis seeks to explain behavior by identifying environmental

More information

Chapter 6. Learning: The Behavioral Perspective

Chapter 6. Learning: The Behavioral Perspective Chapter 6 Learning: The Behavioral Perspective 1 Can someone have an asthma attack without any particles in the air to trigger it? Can an addict die of a heroin overdose even if they ve taken the same

More information

Unit 6 REVIEW Page 1. Name: Date:

Unit 6 REVIEW Page 1. Name: Date: Unit 6 REVIEW Page 1 Name: Date: 1. Little Albert was conditioned by John B. Watson to fear furry white rats. After conditioning, Albert also showed fear to rabbits, dogs, and fur coats. This best illustrates

More information

John Broadus Watson, 1930

John Broadus Watson, 1930 John Broadus Watson, 1930 J. B. Watson believed in nurture and accepted the proclamation of John Locke which presented the mind as a blank slate upon which experience writes its message In what many have

More information

Chapter 3. Outline for this lecture

Chapter 3. Outline for this lecture Chapter 3 Elicited Behaviors and Classical Conditioning Outline for this lecture Elicited Behaviors Reflexes Fixed Action Patterns Opponent Process Theory of Emotion Classical Conditioning Characteristics

More information

Lecture 5: Learning II. Major Phenomenon of Classical Conditioning. Contents

Lecture 5: Learning II. Major Phenomenon of Classical Conditioning. Contents Lecture 5: Learning II Contents Major Phenomenon of Classical Conditioning Applied Examples of Classical Conditioning Other Types of Learning Thorndike and the Law of Effect Skinner and Operant Learning

More information

Exploring the Possibility of the Unconscious Imitation of Others Desires 1

Exploring the Possibility of the Unconscious Imitation of Others Desires 1 Philosophia OSAKA No.6, 2011 63 Yukio IRIE (Osaka University) Exploring the Possibility of the Unconscious Imitation of Others Desires 1 Desire and imitation are popular subjects of research in the field

More information

Objectives. 1. Operationally define terms relevant to theories of learning. 2. Examine learning theories that are currently important.

Objectives. 1. Operationally define terms relevant to theories of learning. 2. Examine learning theories that are currently important. Objectives 1. Operationally define terms relevant to theories of learning. 2. Examine learning theories that are currently important. Learning Theories Behaviorism Cognitivism Social Constructivism Behaviorism

More information

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES Journal of Experimental Vol. 51, No. 4, 1956 Psychology CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES KENNETH H. KURTZ AND CARL I. HOVLAND Under conditions where several concepts are learned concurrently

More information

Classical Conditioning. AKA: Pavlovian conditioning

Classical Conditioning. AKA: Pavlovian conditioning Classical Conditioning AKA: Pavlovian conditioning What is it? A type of learning where a stimulus gains the power to cause a response because it predicts another stimulus that already produces that response.

More information

Classical Conditioning & Operant Conditioning

Classical Conditioning & Operant Conditioning Classical Conditioning & Operant Conditioning What is Classical Conditioning? Learning Objective: Students will be able to describe the difference between Classical and Operant Conditioning. How Do We

More information

New ways of analysing sequences of actions (with an application to the predatory behaviour of Eurobellia Moesta)

New ways of analysing sequences of actions (with an application to the predatory behaviour of Eurobellia Moesta) March 31 1984 Outline of a seminar given in Cambridge 1 by H. Rouanet and J.-M. Bernard 2 New ways of analysing sequences of actions (with an application to the predatory behaviour of Eurobellia Moesta)

More information

Chapter Six. Learning. Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning

Chapter Six. Learning. Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Chapter Six Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Part One: Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning is conditioning by association, such as a pet getting excited

More information

Multivariable Systems. Lawrence Hubert. July 31, 2011

Multivariable Systems. Lawrence Hubert. July 31, 2011 Multivariable July 31, 2011 Whenever results are presented within a multivariate context, it is important to remember that there is a system present among the variables, and this has a number of implications

More information

Co-localized sensations resulting from simultaneous cold and warm stimulation. By: Torsten Thunberg

Co-localized sensations resulting from simultaneous cold and warm stimulation. By: Torsten Thunberg Co-localized sensations resulting from simultaneous cold and warm stimulation By: Torsten Thunberg If one dips one hand into cold

More information

Learning. Learning. relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience

Learning. Learning. relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience Cat Video Cat Roundup Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience Association We learn by association Our minds naturally connect events that occur in sequence

More information

Module One: Booklet #7

Module One: Booklet #7 Module One: Booklet #7 Is a relatively permanent change in behaviour that results from experience There are various ways is which people learn (we will look at some learning styles in Module 2) We will

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) Classical vs. Instrumental The modern view is that these two types of learning involve similar

More information

AND ITS VARIOUS DEVICES. Attitude is such an abstract, complex mental set. up that its measurement has remained controversial.

AND ITS VARIOUS DEVICES. Attitude is such an abstract, complex mental set. up that its measurement has remained controversial. CHAPTER III attitude measurement AND ITS VARIOUS DEVICES Attitude is such an abstract, complex mental set up that its measurement has remained controversial. Psychologists studied attitudes of individuals

More information

THE USE OF MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS IN DEVELOPMENT THEORY: A CRITIQUE OF THE APPROACH ADOPTED BY ADELMAN AND MORRIS A. C. RAYNER

THE USE OF MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS IN DEVELOPMENT THEORY: A CRITIQUE OF THE APPROACH ADOPTED BY ADELMAN AND MORRIS A. C. RAYNER THE USE OF MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS IN DEVELOPMENT THEORY: A CRITIQUE OF THE APPROACH ADOPTED BY ADELMAN AND MORRIS A. C. RAYNER Introduction, 639. Factor analysis, 639. Discriminant analysis, 644. INTRODUCTION

More information

A Difference that Makes a Difference: Welfare and the Equality of Consideration

A Difference that Makes a Difference: Welfare and the Equality of Consideration 84 A Difference that Makes a Difference: Welfare and the Equality of Consideration Abstract Elijah Weber Philosophy Department Bowling Green State University eliweber1980@gmail.com In Welfare, Happiness,

More information

Behavioural Approach. in Psychology

Behavioural Approach. in Psychology Behavioural Approach in Psychology Behaviorism (aka Learning Theory) The behaviorist movement began in America in 1913 with John Watson Watson felt that psychological research had to be scientific 3 Main

More information

Modules. PART I Module 26: How We Learn and Classical Conditioning

Modules. PART I Module 26: How We Learn and Classical Conditioning UNIT VI Learning 1 Modules PART I Module 26: How We Learn and Classical Conditioning Part II Module 27: Operant Conditioning Part III Module 28: Operant Conditioning s Applications, and Comparison to Classical

More information

METHODS* following surgical resection of one occipital lobe indicated absence. fields. Results with the two methods are presented.

METHODS* following surgical resection of one occipital lobe indicated absence. fields. Results with the two methods are presented. SENSITIVITY TO LIGHT IN A CASE OF HYSTERICAL BLINDNESS STUDIED BY REINFORCEMENT- INHIBITION AND CONDITIONING METHODS* L. H. COHEN, E. R. HILGARD, AND G. R. WENDT A previous study of sensitivity to light

More information

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce Learning Chapter 7 Design a Brain The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce What characteristics are needed to behave adaptively and reproduce successfully? Consider: nearly

More information

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction 1 2 3 4 5 Myers Psychology for AP* Unit 6: Learning Unit Overview How Do We Learn? Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Learning by Observation How Do We Learn? Introduction Learning Habituation

More information

Learning and conditioning

Learning and conditioning AP Psych Review Assignment Spring 2009 Chapter and Topic of this Review Guide: Learning and conditioning Vocab Term Definition of Term Example Learning Any relatively permanent change in behavior that

More information

FUNCTIONAL ASSESSMENT: HYPOTHESIZING PREDICTORS AND PURPOSES OF PROBLEM BEHAVIOR TO IMPROVE BEHAVIOR-CHANGE PLANS

FUNCTIONAL ASSESSMENT: HYPOTHESIZING PREDICTORS AND PURPOSES OF PROBLEM BEHAVIOR TO IMPROVE BEHAVIOR-CHANGE PLANS APDT Journal Sept 2009 Susan G. Friedman, Ph.D. Word Count: 2,427 Biographical Sketch: Susan G. Friedman, Ph.D., is a psychology professor at Utah State University, specializing in applied behavior analysis

More information

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn?

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Defining Learning A relatively permanent change in behavior or the potential for behavior that results from experience Results from many life experiences, not just structured

More information

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning Chapter 6: Learning 1 Learning 1. In perception we studied that we are responsive to stimuli in the external world. Although some of these stimulus-response associations are innate many are learnt. 2.

More information

Associative Learning

Associative Learning Learning Learning Targets 1. Distinguish general differences between principles of classical conditioning, operant conditioning, and observational learning (e.g., contingencies). 2. Describe basic classical

More information

Learning Chapter 6 1

Learning Chapter 6 1 Learning Chapter 6 1 Learning is a relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience. 2 Stimulus- Stimulus Learning Learning to associate one stimulus with another. 3 Response- Consequence

More information

An Alternative Explanation for Premack & Premack

An Alternative Explanation for Premack & Premack Dana Spiegel danas@mit.edu 3/9/98 9.85 An Alternative Explanation for Premack & Premack When interpreting the results of experiments that test the cognitive abilities of infants, we must be careful not

More information

Indian Institute of Technology Kanpur National Programme on Technology Enhanced Learning (NPTEL) Course Title A Brief Introduction to Psychology

Indian Institute of Technology Kanpur National Programme on Technology Enhanced Learning (NPTEL) Course Title A Brief Introduction to Psychology Indian Institute of Technology Kanpur National Programme on Technology Enhanced Learning (NPTEL) Course Title A Brief Introduction to Psychology Lecture 9 Learning by Prof. Braj Bhushan Humanities & Social

More information

Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior

Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior Multiple Choice Questions 1. A theory is a(n): A. plausible or scientifically acceptable, well-substantiated explanation of some aspect of the

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

Psychology in Your Life

Psychology in Your Life Sarah Grison Todd Heatherton Michael Gazzaniga Psychology in Your Life FIRST EDITION Chapter 6 Learning 2014 W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. Section 6.1 How Do the Parts of Our Brains Function? 6.1 What Are

More information

PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 6 LEARNING PowerPoint Image Slideshow

PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 6 LEARNING PowerPoint Image Slideshow PSYCHOLOGY Chapter 6 LEARNING PowerPoint Image Slideshow Learning? What s that? A relatively permanent change in behavior brought about by experience or practice. Note that learning is NOT the same as

More information

The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016

The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016 The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016 Jose Martinez-Diaz, Ph.D., BCBA-D FIT School of Behavior Analysis and ABA Technologies, Inc.

More information

Learning: Some Key Terms

Learning: Some Key Terms Learning: Some Key Terms Learning: Relatively permanent change in behavior due to experience Reinforcement: Any event that increases the probability that a response will recur Focus on what can be seen

More information

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Chapter 6/9: Learning Chapter 6/9: Learning Learning A relatively durable change in behavior or knowledge that is due to experience. The acquisition of knowledge, skills, and behavior through reinforcement, modeling and natural

More information

The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the Rescorla-Wagner Predictions: Relative Validity

The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the Rescorla-Wagner Predictions: Relative Validity Methods of Psychological Research Online 4, Vol. 9, No. Internet: http://www.mpr-online.de Fachbereich Psychologie 4 Universität Koblenz-Landau The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the

More information

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms Q0667 QJEP(B) si-b03/read as keyed THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 56B (1), 43 55 Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative

More information

Outline. History of Learning Theory. Pavlov s Experiment: Step 1. Associative learning 9/26/2012. Nature or Nurture

Outline. History of Learning Theory. Pavlov s Experiment: Step 1. Associative learning 9/26/2012. Nature or Nurture Outline What is learning? Associative Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning History of Learning Theory Nature or Nurture BEHAVIORISM Tabula Rasa Learning: Systematic,

More information

Associative Learning

Associative Learning Learning Learning Associative Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Biological Components of Learning Cognitive Components of Learning Behavioral Therapies Associative

More information

Establishing Operations and the Mand

Establishing Operations and the Mand The Analysis of Verbal Behavior 1988, 6, 3-9 Establishing Operations and the Mand Jack Michael Western Michigan University In Verbal Behavior Skinner identifies a small number of elementary verbal relations,

More information

Chapter 7. Learning From Experience

Chapter 7. Learning From Experience Learning From Experience Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? Learning From Experience Noticing and Ignoring Learning What Events Signal Learning About the Consequences of Our Behavior

More information

GCSE PSYCHOLOGY UNIT 2 LEARNING REVISION

GCSE PSYCHOLOGY UNIT 2 LEARNING REVISION GCSE PSYCHOLOGY UNIT 2 LEARNING REVISION GCSE PSYCHOLOGY UNIT 2 CLASSICAL CONDITIONING LEARNING LEARNING= Is a relatively permanent change in behaviour due to experience Some behaviours are learnt, but

More information

Psychology, Ch. 6. Learning Part 1

Psychology, Ch. 6. Learning Part 1 Psychology, Ch. 6 Learning Part 1 Two Main Types of Learning Associative learning- learning that certain events occur together Cognitive learning- acquisition of mental information, by observing or listening

More information

Experimental Psychology PSY 433. Chapter 9 Conditioning and Learning

Experimental Psychology PSY 433. Chapter 9 Conditioning and Learning Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 9 Conditioning and Learning Midterm Results Score Grade N 29-34 A 9 26-28 B 4 23-25 C 5 20-22 D 2 0-19 F 4 Top score = 34/34 Top score for curve = 33 What is Plagiarism?

More information

Learning. Learning. Habituation. Sensitization. Habituation and Sensitization

Learning. Learning. Habituation. Sensitization. Habituation and Sensitization Learning Learning Adaptive change in behavior that results from past experiences Nonassociative Learning Habituation Sensitization Classical Conditioning Instrumental Learning/Operant Conditioning Ability

More information

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Copyright 2015 McGraw-Hill Education. All rights reserved. No reproduction or distribution without the prior written consent of McGraw-Hill Education. Learning

More information

PSYC 337 LEARNING. Session 3 Classical Conditioning. Lecturer: Dr. Inusah Abdul-Nasiru Contact Information:

PSYC 337 LEARNING. Session 3 Classical Conditioning. Lecturer: Dr. Inusah Abdul-Nasiru Contact Information: PSYC 337 LEARNING Session 3 Classical Conditioning Lecturer: Dr. Inusah Abdul-Nasiru Contact Information: iabdul-nasiru@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education 2014/2015

More information

Dikran J. Martin Introduction to Psychology

Dikran J. Martin Introduction to Psychology Dikran J. Martin Introduction to Psychology Name: Date: Lecture Series: Chapter 7 Learning Pages: 32 TEXT: Lefton, Lester A. and Brannon, Linda (2003). PSYCHOLOGY. (Eighth Edition.) Needham Heights, MA:

More information

Conditioning The Behavior of the Listener. Conditioning the Behavior of the Listener

Conditioning The Behavior of the Listener. Conditioning the Behavior of the Listener Conditioning The Behavior of the Listener Henry D. Schlinger, Jr. California State University, Los Angeles Norwegian Association for Behavior Analysis April 2013 Conditioning the Behavior of the Listener

More information

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 4

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 4 Learning AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 4 Learning Learning is a lasting change in behavior or mental process as the result of an experience. There are two important parts: a lasting change a simple reflexive reaction

More information

Developmental Psychology and Learning SOCIAL EDUCATION COURSE Academic year 2014/2015

Developmental Psychology and Learning SOCIAL EDUCATION COURSE Academic year 2014/2015 Developmental Psychology and Learning SOCIAL EDUCATION COURSE Academic year 2014/2015 EXTENDED SUMMARY Lesson #6 Monday, Oct. 27 th 2014: 19:00/21:00 Friday, Oct. 31 th 2014, 09:00/11:00 LESSON PLAN: HUMAN

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Classical Conditioning Learning through Association Ivan Pavlov discovered the form of learning called Classical

More information

Learning. Learning is the relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience.

Learning. Learning is the relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience. Chapter 7 Learning Learning Learning is the relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience. Learning The Mexican Gray wolves had problems after being released back into the wild,

More information

27- Classical Conditioning 1 of 5

27- Classical Conditioning 1 of 5 27- Classical Conditioning 1 of 5 Which Pen Would You Choose? The researchers placed the participants in a room. In this room the participants first viewed purple pens. As the participants sat staring

More information

Theories of Learning

Theories of Learning Theories of Learning Learning Classical conditioning Classical conditioning in real life Operant conditioning Operant conditioning in real life Learning and the mind Watson s Extreme Environmentalism Give

More information

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments Consciousness and Cognition 9, 215 219 (2000) doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0438, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

More information

Basic characteristics

Basic characteristics Learning Basic characteristics The belief that the universe is lawful and orderly The occurrence of phenomena as a function of the operation of specific variables Objective observation Controlled experiments

More information

6 Knowing and Understanding the World

6 Knowing and Understanding the World 6 Knowing and Understanding the World 6.1 Introduction You must have observed a human child. If you wave your hands in front of the eyes of a new born you will see that the child automatically closes her

More information