An evaluation of early and late stage attentional processing of positive and negative information in dysphoria

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "An evaluation of early and late stage attentional processing of positive and negative information in dysphoria"

Transcription

1 COGNITION AND EMOTION 2007, 21 (4), An evaluation of early and late stage attentional processing of positive and negative information in dysphoria Matthew S. Shane and Jordan B. Peterson University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Depressive disorders may be characterised by hyperattention toward negative information, hypoattention toward positive information, or a combination of both processing biases. In two studies, a dot-probe task was utilised to better ascertain the specific direction and time-course of these biases. In both studies, the dysphoric group showed significantly less attentional allocation toward positive stimuli than the non-dysphoric group. In study two, the dysphoric group also showed greater attentional allocation toward depression-specific stimuli. Importantly, the bias toward depression-specific stimuli, and the bias away from positive stimuli, were uncorrelated with each other. It may be that both biases can act as sufficient, but not necessary, characteristics of dysphoric processing. An additional possibility is that the relative level of each bias type may best characterise dysphoric processing. Each of these possibilities is discussed in turn. A current controversy in research on information processing in depression concerns the existence and nature of attentional biases in depressed individuals. Cognitive models based on schemas (Beck, 1976) or associative networks (Bower, 1981) predict that individuals with depressive disorders should show mood-congruent processing biases at all stages of attentional processing (Ingram, Miranda, & Segal, 1998). Thus, according to these models, depressed individuals should manifest hypervigilant orienting towards negatively valenced information in their current environment (early stage attentional processing) as well as increased sustained attention and Correspondence should be addressed to: Matthew S. Shane, Clinical Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, The MIND Institute, 1201 Yale Blvd. NE, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA. mshane@themindinstitute.org. This research was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. We gratefully acknowledge Chad Ebesutani and Laura Elinson for their help with data collection, and a number of anonymous reviewers for their excellent feedback. # 2007 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business DOI: /

2 790 SHANE AND PETERSON rumination (later stage attentional processing) on such negative information (see Posner & Peterson, 1990, or LaBerge, 1995, for a discussion of early and late stage attentional processing). Empirical support for these hypotheses has been mixed, however. While a number of studies have identified such mood-congruent processing biases (Bradley, Mogg, & Lee, 1997; Gotlib & Cane, 1987; Ingram & Ritter, 2000; Mogg, Bradley, & Williams, 1995), others have failed to identify such biases (Hill & Dutton, 1989; MacLeod, Mathews, & Tata, 1986). Conversely, still other studies have reported that depression may be characterised by a lack of attentional bias towards positive information, rather than an active bias towards negative information (Gotlib, McLachlan, & Katz, 1988; McCabe & Gotlib, 1995; McCabe, Gotlib, & Martin, 2000). This inconsistency has made it difficult for researchers to form definitive conclusions regarding the nature of attentional processing in depressive disorders. A variety of methodologies have been adapted from cognitive psychology to investigate selective attention in depressive and anxious disorders, including the emotional Stroop task (Mathews & MacLeod, 1986), the deployment-of-attention task (DOAT; Gotlib et al., 1988), and the dotprobe task (MacLeod et al., 1986). The latter two have proven particularly useful because of their ability to distinguish between early and late stage attentional processing. In the dot-probe task, which has been utilised in the present study, pairs of words or pictures (e.g., one negatively valenced word and one neutrally valenced word) are presented on screen (e.g., for 500 ms or 1000 ms) followed by a dot probe in the location of one of the two preceding stimuli. Participants are required to press one of two buttons to indicate which side of the screen the probe appeared on. Prior research has determined that participants reaction times are faster when the probe appears in the position of the attended-to stimulus. Thus, attentional bias scores are calculated by subtracting probe detection speeds when the probe appears in the place of a negatively valenced stimulus from probe detection speeds when the probe appears in the place of a neutral stimulus. Short stimulus durations (e.g., up to 500 ms) have been used to measure early stage orienting processes, whereas longer stimulus durations (e.g., 1000/ ms) have been employed when later stage sustained attentional processing is being evaluated. The DOAT works in a similar fashion, but uses a forced-choice format rather than relying on reaction times, asking participants to decide which of two coloured bars appeared first (in reality they both appear simultaneously, but it has been found that the attended-to bar seems to appear first). Researchers utilising relatively short stimulus durations (up to 500 ms post-stimulus onset) have generally failed to find consistent biases in depressed individuals attentional processing. Mogg et al. (1995) found

3 ATTENTIONAL PROCESSING IN DYSPHORIA 791 no evidence of attentional biases in depressed individuals when the probe appeared 500 ms post-stimulus. Bradley et al. (1997) reported a similar lack of attentional bias, both at 14 ms and 500 ms post-stimulus, in naturally dysphoric individuals, and Mathews, Ridgeway, and Williamson (1996) presented data suggestive of biases only after 1000 ms post-stimulus. The consistency of these null findings suggest that depressive disorders, unlike anxiety disorders (Broadbent & Broadbent, 1988; Mogg & Bradley, 1998), may not be characterised by hypervigilant orienting or pre-attention towards negative environmental stimuli (Bradley et al., 1997; Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Mathews, 1997; see, however, Bradley, Mogg, & Williams, 1994, and Luecken, Tartaro, & Appelhans, 2004, for potentially contrary evidence demonstrating biases during subliminal presentations). In contrast, when longer stimulus durations have been utilised (e.g., 1000/ ms), some evidence for depression-related attentional biases has been found. Mogg et al. (1995) found that clinical depression predicted negative attentional biases at 1500 ms post-stimulus, and Bradley et al. (1997) and Mathews et al. (1996) both found similar biases at 1000 ms post-stimulus. In each of these studies, depressed individuals were found to manifest increased sustained attention towards the negative stimuli at these longer stimulus durations, despite not showing orienting mechanisms more characteristic of anxiety disorders. This pattern of attentional deployment has led Williams et al. (1997) to hypothesise that the locus of depressionrelated biases may rest in later stage, elaborative processes. This interpretation remains consistent with a rich literature base demonstrating a high degree of ruminative behaviour in depressed (Beck, 1976; Harrington & Blankenship, 2002; Watkins & Brown, 2002) and dysphoric (Roberts, Gilboa, & Gotlib, 1998) individuals. A number of additional studies have failed to demonstrate even later stage attentional biases in depression, however (e.g., Hill & Dutton, 1989; MacLeod & Chong, 1998; Neshat- Doost, Moradi, Taghavi, Yule, & Dalgleish, 2000) and alternative hypotheses have been raised. Murphy et al. (1999) have, for instance, suggested that depressed individuals may have a more difficult time shifting attention away from negative stimuli once attention has been allocated, thus causing them to remain focused on the negative information after initial orientation in that direction (see also Ellenbogen, Schwartzman, Stewart, & Walker, 2002; Rinck & Becker, 2004). It should be noted that these two hypotheses are not necessarily mutually exclusive. Difficulty disattending to negative stimuli could, for instance, play a role in the depressed individual s increased ruminative behaviour. To further cloud the issue, Gotlib et al. (1988) have conversely suggested that it may be normal, rather than depressed, individuals who show biased

4 792 SHANE AND PETERSON attentional processing. Utilising the DOAT, Gotlib et al. (1988) found that whereas dysphoric individuals attended equally to negatively, positively and neutrally valenced words, non-dysphoric individuals attended more often to positive than to negative or neutral words. Thus, whereas the nondysphoric participants were characterised by biased attention toward positive stimuli, the dysphoric participants showed a lack of such bias in their attentional processing. This suggests that attentional processing biased toward positively valenced information may act as a protective factor against the onset of depression and dysphoria (see Taylor & Brown, 1988, who have explicitly suggested that the use of positive illusions, which are cognitive filters that preferentially screen out negative information, is commonplace, is a sign of mental health, and is negatively correlated with the onset of depressive symptomatology). Gotlib and colleagues have replicated their findings a number of times, in both clinically depressed patients (McCabe & Gotlib, 1995) and previously depressed patients (McCabe et al., 2000; see, however, Gotlib, Krasnoperova, Yue, & Joormann, 2004, for a recent non-replication using the dotprobe task). Only one study (McCabe & Toman, 2000) has, to date, varied the stimulus durations in the DOAT, however (750 ms has been standard), making determination of early and late stages of attentional processing within this task difficult. Thus, the present set of studies was designed to further investigate the extent to which dysphoria is characterised by hypervigilance to negative information, hypovigilance to positive information, or a combination of both processing biases. To this end, dysphoric and non-dysphoric individuals (as determined by Beck Depression Inventory scores [BDI]; Beck & Steer, 1993), were asked to perform a modified version of the dot-probe task, during which three different stimulus pairings were presented: positive neutral, negativeneutral, and neutral neutral (filler). Reaction times on positive neutral trials were used to index biases toward positive stimuli while reaction times on negative neutral trials were used to index biases toward negative stimuli. Separation of positive and negative stimulus presentations was implemented quite purposely. Although evaluating the nature of dysphorics attentional biases within situations characterised by the simultaneous presentation of both positive and negative stimuli may hold certain advantages (e.g., parsimony), such simultaneous presentation makes it impossible to determine the individual influence of each stimulus type. In such mixed-trials, for instance, a bias away from the negative stimulus, or a bias toward the positive stimulus, would be difficult to differentiate through reaction-time measures. A major purpose of the present study was to investigate the specific nature of positive and negative

5 ATTENTIONAL PROCESSING IN DYSPHORIA 793 biases in dysphoria; thus, use of mixed positive negative trials would not have served our purposes well. 1 The present set of studies was also intended to investigate a number of specific issues regarding the nature of positive and negative biases. First, we sought to determine whether these biases constitute global biases in attentional processing, as predicted by cognitive models based on schemas (Beck, 1976) or associative networks (Bower, 1981), or conversely are specific to either early or late stages of processing. The time of onset of probes was thus varied across trials, and appeared either after a short duration (500 ms in study one, and 200 ms in study two) or a long duration (1500 ms in both studies). Evidence of attentional biases on the short-duration trials is often interpreted as indicative of biases in early stage attentional processing, whereas evidence of attentional biases on the long-duration trials is often interpreted as indicative of later stage attentional biases. Second, we wanted to evaluate the possibility that the positive and negative biases may exist as independent markers of dysphoria. Davidson, Pizzagalli, Nitschke, & Putnam (2002) have suggested that symptoms of depression related to positive and negative emotions may manifest independently, and may be underlain by distinct proximal causes. By using separate trials to calculate positive and negative biases, it allows for the independence of these biases to be evaluated in several ways. First, and most directly, the correlation between the positive and negative biases could be calculated. A low correlation could suggest independence between attention allocated toward positive stimuli and attention allocated toward negative stimuli. In addition, we were particularly interested in the possibility that dysphoric individuals may manifest both positive and negative biases, but that each bias may show a distinct temporal pattern. A further possibility is that dysphoria may be characterised by a relative bias toward negative information (in relation to positive information), rather than an absolute bias toward either bias type (see Siegle, Ingram, & Matt, 2002, for the suggestion that dysphorics negative biases may come at the expense of attention allocated to more positive information). Thus, a final aim of the present research was to evaluate the predictive value of bias 1 It is important to recognise the benefits and drawbacks of our approach of using positive/ neutral and negativeneutral trials, rather than integrated positive negative trials. As was pointed out by an anonymous reviewer, our use of separate positiveneutral and negative neutral trials could serve to minimise a true correlation between positive and negative attentional biases. While acknowledging this, given that not all situations need be characterised by the presence of both positive and negative stimuli, and because almost no situation (and perhaps no situation) could be characterised by the presence of only positive and negative stimuli, use of mixed positive-negative trials in the absence of any non-valent stimuli, could cause an equal enhancement of the true correlation. Future research would benefit from a careful analysis of the benefits and drawbacks of each approach.

6 794 SHANE AND PETERSON differentials (BD), which were calculated by subtracting participants biases to negative information from their biases to positive information. By simultaneously taking into account the magnitude of both bias types, BDs may prove capable of identifying smaller effect sizes than either absolute bias. Theoretically, if BDs show superior prediction than positive and negative biases it would imply that dysphoric individuals need not consistently show strong attentional bias away from negative stimuli, nor strong bias toward positive stimuli. Rather, any combination of attentional processing resulting in a relative preference for negative stimuli may serve as the crucial characteristic underlying dysphoric processing. Method STUDY ONE Participants. Ninety-seven undergraduate psychology students at the University of Toronto volunteered for the study, in partial fulfilment of course credit. Of these participants, 66 were women and 29 were men. Their ages ranged from 1831 (M /20.43, SD /2.12). Assessment of dysphoria. Participants completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck & Steer, 1993), which has been shown in numerous studies to be a reliable and valid measure of severity of depressive/dysphoric symptomatology in college (Oliver & Burkham, 1979), community, and patient samples (for a review see Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). Those scoring 6 and below on the BDI were classified as non-dysphoric, whereas those scoring 10 and above were classified as dysphoric. By removing those scoring between 7 and 9, potential difficulty regarding categorising slightly dysphoric individuals was avoided. These cut-off scores are consistent with previous research utilising the BDI to establish research groups (e.g., Tennen, Hall, & Affleck, 1995), and is in line with Kendall, Hollon, Beck, Hammen, and Ingram s (1987) recommendations regarding use of the BDI in non-clinical samples. Only those participants who met these cutoffs (N /73) were included in the final analyses. In addition to the BDI, participants also completed the short-form of the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale (TMAS; Bendig, 1956), because prior research has demonstrated depressive and anxious disorders to be highly comorbid. The TMAS is a 20-item, true/false questionnaire that has been shown to correlate highly with other measures of trait anxiety and negative affectivity (Watson & Clark, 1984). Description of affective stimuli. Stimuli were taken from the International Affective Picture System (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1995), which

7 ATTENTIONAL PROCESSING IN DYSPHORIA 795 contains normative ratings of affective valence for each picture on a 9-point scale from unpleasant (1) to pleasant (9). Forty positive, 40 negative and 120 neutral pictures were chosen, based largely on these valence ratings. Negative pictures were selected in accordance with their relevance to sadness and threat, and included depictions of illness, disaster, drug injections and homelessness. Positive pictures included puppies, sunsets, sweet desserts and flowers. Neutral pictures were largely depictions of household items, such as lamps or chairs. Each emotional picture (positive or negative) was paired with a neutral picture, matching as closely as possible within each pair for colour and shape of the focal object. In total, there were 40 positive neutral, 40 negativeneutral, and 20 neutral neutral trials. The latter trial type acted as filler, and was prepared so that emotionally valent stimuli were not displayed on each trial, and to decrease the opportunity for participants to determine the nature of the task. Sexually explicit scenes from the IAPS were not included in the stimulus sets, as their influence on general arousal processes remains somewhat unclear. Procedure. After obtaining informed consent, participants completed the BDI and the short-form of the TMAS. Participants next completed the dot-probe task (designed using E-Prime software; PSInet Corporation) on a Pentium II computer and a 15-inch monitor, in a quiet room in the laboratory. The dot-probe task consisted of 10 practice, and 100 experimental trials (comprising the 40 positive neutral, 40 negativeneutral and 20 neutral neutral picture pairs). The practice trials were formed by using neutral neutral picture pairs not included in the experimental trials. Each trial started with a central fixation cross for 500 ms, followed by a picture pair, with half the pairs being displayed for 500 ms and half for 1500 ms. The positive or negative pictures appeared on the right or left side of the screen with equal frequency, as did the probe. In addition, the probe appeared in the place of the emotionally valent pictures on half of the trials, and in the place of the neutral picture on half the trials. For the neutral neutral trials, the probe appeared on the right or left an equal number of times. The trials were presented in a different random order for each participant. The dot probe was displayed immediately following the presentation of the picture pair, and participants were required to press one of two keys to indicate which side of the screen the probe appeared on. The left index finger was used to hit the Z key for probes located on the left side, and the right index finger was used to press the / (slash) key for probes located on the right side. Participants were informed to respond as quickly as they could, while remaining accurate. The probe remained displayed until the participant responded, or for a maximum of 10 seconds. The intertrial interval was 1000 ms. Upon completion of the dot-probe task, participants were awarded course credit, debriefed, and allowed to leave.

8 796 SHANE AND PETERSON Results Group characteristics. Women (M /8.30, SD/5.72) showed slightly higher BDI scores than men (M /7.92, SD /5.95), however, this difference did not prove significant, t(71) /0.07, p /.79. Subsequent data was thus collapsed across gender. As expected, the dysphoric group (M/14.10, SD/3.84) was characterised by significantly higher levels of dysphoria than the nondysphoric group (M/4.05, SD /2.01; t(71) / /14.55, d /3.47, p B/.001). In addition, there was a trend toward the dysphoric group manifesting higher anxiety than the non-dysphoric group (dysphoric group: M/29.73, SD/4.18; non-dysphoric group: M /27.98, SD/3.90; t(71) /1.84, d /0.43, p/.07). Attentional biases. Data from two participants (one dysphoric, one nondysphoric) were not included in the data set due to complications during the administration of the dot-probe task (the computer crashed in the middle of the task). Latency data from trials with errors (1.25% of all trials) were excluded from the analyses, as were latencies below 200 ms (2 trials) and greater than 700 ms (2.21% of all trials) after inspection of box and whisker plots. These cutoff points, as well as the percentage of trials excluded based on those cut-offs, are comparable with that of previous research utilising the dot-probe paradigm (Bradley, Mogg, Falla, & Hamilton, 1998; Mogg, Millar, & Bradley, 2000). Mean RTs were collected for each group (see Table 1) and attentional bias scores were calculated for each trial type by subtracting the mean RTs when probes were in the same location as the emotional pictures from the mean RTs when they were in the opposite location (Bradley et al., 1998; Mogg et al., 2000). Thus, bias to negative stimuli was calculated from participant s RTs on negative neutral trials, and bias to positive stimuli was calculated from participant s RTs on positiveneutral trials. Positive values of each bias score reflect attentional vigilance toward the valenced picture, whereas negative values reflect attentional avoidance of the valenced picture. These two bias scores were found to be uncorrelated with one another, r/.07, ns. These attentional bias scores were entered into a 2 /2/2 mixed-design ANOVA, with valence and duration as within-subject variables and dysphoria as a between-subject variable. 2 A significant main effect of 2 Before calculating attentional bias scores, we entered the raw RT data into a 2/2/2/2/ 2 ANOVA with valence (positive, negative), duration (500 ms, 1500 ms), picture location (left, right), and probe location (left, right) as within-subject variables, and dysphoria (high, low) as a between-subject variable. Significant main effects of picture location, F(1, 70)/7.97, p B/.01, duration, F(1, 70)/178.25, p B/.001 and valence, F (1, 70)/88.30, p B/.001 were identified. Of particular note, the predicted four-way interaction of dysphoria / valence / picture location / probe location reached significance, F(1, 70)/4.83, p/.03, which provided the basis for the calculation of the attentional bias scores used in the remainder of the analyses. The five-way interaction involving duration did not reach significance, F(1, 70)/1.46, p /.2.

9 ATTENTIONAL PROCESSING IN DYSPHORIA 797 TABLE 1 Mean response latencies to probes (in ms): Study one Picture valence Level of Exposure Picture Probe dysphoria duration location location Positive Negative Low 500 ms Left Left (56.34) (62.14) Left Right (61.07) (59.62) Right Left (66.66) (63.58) Right Right (59.14) (58.18) 1500 ms Left Left (49.77) (56.67) Left Right (46.17) (62.17) Right Left (54.31) (61.41) Right Right (50.15) (50.10) High 500 ms Left Left (53.05) (67.51) Left Right (67.89) (65.70) Right Left (58.59) (69.51) Right Right (63.27) (60.31) 1500 ms Left Left (61.32) (67.68) Left Right (62.74) (73.28) Right Left (63.49) (60.96) Right Right (57.22) (50.05) duration was revealed, F(1, 70)/8.59, h 2 /.11, pb/.01, as was a significant interaction between dysphoria and valence, F(1, 70)/4.11, h 2 /.06, pb/.05. The three-way dysphoria /valence/duration interaction did not reach significance, F(1, 70) /1.28, p /.20. We also performed a similar ANOVA including participants TMAS scores as a covariate to consider the possibility that the observed effects may be due to higher levels of anxiety in the dysphoric group. In this analysis, the main effect of duration disappeared (p /.50), however the predicted dysphoria/valence interaction remained significant, F(1, 69)/3.65, h 2 /.05, p/.05. Furthermore, the anxiety/valence interaction was nonsignificant, F(1, 69)/.19, p/ Dissection of the significant dysphoria /valence interaction allowed for an initial investigation into the influence of positivity and negativity biases in dysphoria. Figure 1a depicts this interaction in graphical form, with early and late biases collapsed. As can be seen, non-dysphoric individuals showed a slight vigilance toward positive stimuli, whereas dysphoric individuals showed a more pronounced avoidance of positive stimuli. Planned comparisons confirmed that these attentional biases differed significantly from 3 Interpretation of these covariate analyses need be made with caution. Miller and Chapman (2001) have pointed out that using a covariate that is highly correlated with the measure of interest may regress out substantial variance, and may leave a variable with little in common with the original construct. With appropriate caution, we believe these covariate analyses remain useful, however, we also believe it important to note these issues of interpretation.

10 SHANE AND PETERSON (a) 10 Dysphoric Nondysphoric PB NB (b) PB E NB E (c) PB L NB L Figure 1. Level of bias toward/away from positive and negative stimuli on (a) all trials (b) shortduration trials, and (c) long-duration trials. Note : PB/bias to positive stimuli; NB/bias to negative stimuli; PB E /bias to positive stimuli on early trials; NB E /bias to negative stimuli on early trials PB L /bias to positive stimuli on late trials; NB L /bias to negative stimuli on late trials.

11 ATTENTIONAL PROCESSING IN DYSPHORIA 799 each other, t(70) /2.13, d /.50, p /.04. However, neither bias differed from zero [dysphoric: t(29) / /1.55, d /.20, p /.13; non-dysphoric: t(41) /1.33, d /.28, p /.19]. In contrast, both dysphoric and non-dysphoric individuals showed tendencies to avoid the negative stimuli, and this avoidance reached significance for the non-dysphoric participants, t(41) / /2.20, d /.33, p/.03. Temporal characteristics. Because this study represented one of the first investigations of temporal differences between biases toward positive and negative stimuli, we examined the pattern of biases on short- and longduration trials separately, despite the nonsignificant three-way interaction involving duration (see Figures 1b and 1c). On short-duration trials, the dysphoric group showed pronounced avoidance of the positive pictures that differed from zero, t(29) / /2.35, d /.42, p /.03, and from the nondysphoric group, t(70) /1.76, d /.41, p/.08, and unbiased processing of the negative pictures. The non-dysphoric group, in contrast, showed the opposite pattern of processing: pronounced avoidance of the negative pictures that differed from zero, t(41) / /3.59, d /.54, p B/.001, but not from the dysphoric group, t(70) / /1.63, d /.39, p /.11, and equal processing of the positive pictures. On long-duration trials, dysphoric individuals showed no evidence of biases, while the non-dysphoric individuals showed pronounced bias toward positive stimuli, t(41) /2.52, d/.39, p/.02. Bias differentials. Finally, we wanted to investigate the possibility that the relative magnitude of individuals biases toward positive and negative stimuli may provide a more robust indicator of dysphoric processing than either absolute bias. To this end, we calculated bias differential (BD) scores for each participant by subtracting their bias toward negative stimuli from their bias toward positive stimuli. Positive BDs thus indicate an overall bias toward positive stimuli, whereas negative BDs indicate an overall bias toward negative stimuli. BDs were found to correlate significantly with biases toward both positive and negative stimuli, and also correlated significantly with BDI scores, r//.23, p/02. Furthermore, the nondysphoric group was characterised by highly positive BDs (M/13.13, SD/ 31.27) that differed significantly from baseline, t(41) /2.72, d/.42, p/.01, and from the dysphoric group, t(70) /1.93, d /.46, p/.05. In contrast, the dysphoric group manifested slightly negative BDs (M//3.34, SD/41.04), which did not differ from zero, t(29) /0.45, p B/.60. Thus, non-dysphoric individuals showed overall attentional patterns skewed toward positive stimuli, while the dysphoric individuals showed a mild, but nonsignificant, skewing toward negative stimuli. To investigate the possibility that BDs added to the prediction of dysphoria over and above either absolute bias

12 800 SHANE AND PETERSON TABLE 2 Regression models using absolute biases and bias differentials to predict BDI: Study one Predictor Standardised coefficient (b) p-value Model p-value Model 1 Bias to positive stimuli / Model 2 Bias to positive stimuli / Bias differential / Model 1 Bias to negative stimuli Model 2 Bias to negative stimuli / Bias differential / score, a series of regression analyses were conducted in which the absolute biases toward positive and negative stimuli were entered in block one, and BD was entered in block two. 4 Results demonstrate that BD successfully entered into both regression models in block two, while knocking the absolute bias scores out of the model (see Table 2). Finally, early and late BDs were considered separately. Non-dysphoric individuals showed positive BDs at both early and late stages of attentional processing (early: M /3.92, SD /47.09; late: M/12.87, SD/53.68), while dysphoric individuals showed negative BDs at both stages of processing (early: M / /24.31, SD /72.13; late: M/ /14.78, SD/90.82). A repeated measures ANOVA with early and late BDs as within-subjects variables failed to reach significance (p /.95). Discussion In the present study, only non-dysphoric individuals showed significant avoidance of negative stimuli, while dysphoric individuals showed significantly less attention to positive stimuli than non-dysphoric individuals. On the whole, these findings are consistent with a myriad of previous research identifying one or the other of these biases in dysphoric and depressed populations (Bradley et al., 1997; Gotlib et al., 1988; Ingram & Ritter, 2000; Mogg et al., 1995). Our results do differ from previous findings in a number of ways, however. First, most previous work has found that individuals in depressive states show pronounced vigilance toward negative stimuli. In contrast, we report unbiased processing in dysphoric individuals, and pronounced avoidance 4 For these continuous analyses, all participants including those with BDI scores between 6 and 10 were included in the analyses. Results remained similar when excluding this group, however, the nature of continuous analyses required inclusion of the full sample of participants.

13 ATTENTIONAL PROCESSING IN DYSPHORIA 801 of negative information in non-dysphoric individuals. One reason for these divergent results may pertain to the use of IAPS slides in the present study. Recent work suggests that the use of depression-specific stimuli may be preferable to the use of broadly negatively-valenced stimuli, which can include a combination of threat-related, depression-related, and disgustrelated stimuli, not all of which may be expected to elicit depressionrelated biases (Gotlib et al., 2004; Westra & Kuiper, 1997). The IAPS slides used in the present study may then have constituted too heterogeneous a group of negative pictures to appropriately evaluate the processing biases present in dysphoria. This could have had two effects. First, the extent to which the IAPS slides elicited biases within the dysphoric individuals may have been somewhat reduced. Second, the inclusion of anxiety- and disgust-related stimuli may have unintentionally tapped alternate processing biases that could have been present within the non-dysphoric population. In study two, then, we ran a similar version of the dot-probe task utilised in study one, but presented depression-specific words, instead of negatively valenced IAPS pictures. Thus, three word-pair types were utilised in study two: positiveneutral, depression neutral, and neutral neutral. Positive neutral trials were used to index participant s bias toward positive stimuli, and depressionneutral trials were used to index participant s bias toward depression-specific stimuli. Our predictions were similar to study one, in that we predicted the dysphoric group to manifest increased processing of the depression-specific words. In addition, based on the results of study one, we anticipated that the dysphoric group may also show increased avoidance of positive stimuli. A second difference between the results of the present study and those of previous research pertains to the temporal characteristics of the attentional biases. Previous research has fairly consistently, although not unanimously, reported a late-stage bias toward negative stimuli in depressed or dysphoric individuals, and, conversely, has failed to show the corresponding early stage bias (e.g., Suslow, Junghanns, & Arolt, 2004). The present study, in contrast, found no significant differences between early and late stage processing when evaluating the relevant repeated-measures ANOVA, and subsequent exploratory analyses, if anything, revealed stronger biases in the dysphoric group during early stage processing. To this we have two comments. First, the present findings are not without precedent, as a number of recent studies have reported increases in early stage*and possibility even preconscious*susceptibility to stimuli in depression. Lundh, Wikstrom, Westerlund, & Ost (1999), for instance, found that subliminal presentations of panic-related words correlated significantly with level of trait depression. Luecken et al. (2004) have shown similar effects of subliminal stimuli in a dot-probe task not unlike the

14 802 SHANE AND PETERSON presently devised task. These studies evaluated attentional patterns earlier, even, than the 500 ms timeframe utilised in the present study and together with the present findings suggest the possibility of early stage biases in dysphoric processing. Second, it should be noted that the dysphoric group showed early stage processing biases not on negative neutral trials, but on positive neutral trials. Previous work demonstrating late stage biases in depression and dysphoria have reported such biases with regard to negative information. We are, however, unaware of any previous work evaluating the temporal patterning of biases to positive stimuli in depression or dysphoria. It may be that biases to positive information have a different temporal pattern than biases to negative information. In this regard, it is interesting to note that the dysphoric group only showed a bias toward positive information on shortduration trials. Another possibility, however, is that the early stage duration utilised in the present study (500 ms) may not have sufficiently tapped early stage attentional processing. Recent evidence has suggested that individuals are capable of shifting their attention within ms of stimulus onset (Chun & Wolfe, 2001; Duncan, Ward, & Shapiro, 1995), and thus by 500 ms, we may have already been evaluating later stages of attentional processing. This could explain why we did not obtain a significant interaction with duration in study one, and may further explain why we found such pronounced biases during early stage processing. To this end, study two reduced the probe onset time in the early stage trials to 200 ms, in order to ensure that we were accurately indexing the initial orienting tendency of all participants. Method STUDY TWO Participants. Eighty-one undergraduate psychology students at the University of Toronto volunteered for the study, in partial fulfilment of course credit. Of these participants, 51 were women and 30 were men. Their ages ranged from 1829 (M /20.20, SD /1.74). Assessment of dysphoria. As in study one, participants completed the BDI, as well as the TMAS. Again, as in study one, those scoring 6 and below on the BDI were classified as non-dysphoric, whereas those scoring 10 and above were classified as dysphoric (consistent with Tennen et al., 1995, and in line with Kendall et al. s, 1987, recommendations). Sixty-six participants (39 non-dysphoric, 27 dysphoric) met this final criterion and were included in the subsequent analyses.

15 ATTENTIONAL PROCESSING IN DYSPHORIA 803 Description of affective stimuli. Words were utilised in study two rather than pictures because previous studies have utilised depression-specific words with considerable success (Bradley et al., 1997; Yovel & Mineka, 2005), and because creating a collection of depression-specific pictures seemed a somewhat daunting task. Three word lists were thus developed: a positive word list (40 words, e.g., success, party, joy), a depression-related word list (40 words, e.g., gloomy, bleak, failure), and a neutral word list (100 words, e.g., range, vary, directly). Each word list was matched for both word length and word frequency (Kuchera & Francis, 1967), and three independent judges rated each word on 7-point Likert scales for its level of positivity, anxiety-relatedness, depression-relatedness, and arousability. To this end, each depression-related word was rated as more related to depression than to anxiety. In addition, the positivity and arousal ratings of the positive words were matched to the depression-relatedness and arousal ratings of the depression-related words. Thus, differences in the degree of positivity/ depression-relatedness or the degree of arousal could not be used to provide alternative explanations of the findings. Procedure. After obtaining informed consent, participants were seated in front of a computer with a 15 inch monitor, and completed the dot-probe task. The task was identical to that described in study one, with the following four exceptions. First, words were used as stimuli, rather than pictures. Second, each word pair was presented twice, once for each duration-type. Of these, half of the word-pairs appeared for the first time on the short-duration trials, while the other half appeared for the first time on the long-duration trials. Third, since each word pair was presented twice, the number of trials was doubled from that of study one. Thus, 10 practice trials (utilising 20 unique neutralneutral word pairs) were followed by 200 experimental trials (comprising 80 positiveneutral, 80 negativeneutral and 40 neutralneutral word pairs). This was done in order to increase the number of observations used to calculate each bias score, and thus to increase the reliability of the task. Fourth, the short-duration trials had a probe onset of 200 ms, rather than 500 ms as was utilised in study one. The probe onset for long-duration trials remained at 1500 ms. The final difference between the procedure in study two was that participants completed the BDI and TMAS after completing the dot-probe task, rather than before. Mild concern was raised regarding the possibility that completing these scales before performing the dot-probe task may have led to subtle priming of attention toward the negative stimuli. In order to ensure this did not influence the data for study two, we reversed the order of administration in the second study.

16 804 SHANE AND PETERSON Results Group characteristics. Once again, women (M/9.82, SD/9.21) showed slightly higher BDI scores than men (M /8.83, SD /8.88), however this difference was not significant, t(64) /.47, p/.64. Thus, as in study one, subsequent analyses were conducted with data collapsed across gender. As expected, the dysphoric group (M /19.15, SD /9.65) was characterised by significantly higher levels of dysphoria than the non-dysphoric group [M / 3.38, SD /1.63], t(64) / /8.40, d /2.83, p B/.001, and also showed significantly higher levels of comorbid anxiety [dysphoric: M /31.37, SD / 4.07; non-dysphoric: M /26.94, SD /3.77], t(64) / /4.47, d /.17, p B/.001. Attentional biases. As in study one, latency data from trials with errors (B/1% of all trials) were excluded. Similarly, after inspection via box and whisker plots, latencies below 200 ms or greater than 700 ms (1.73% of all trials) were also excluded from analyses. These exclusion criteria remain consistent with those utilised in study one, as well as with the extant dotprobe literature (Bradley et al., 1998; Mogg et al., 2000). Mean RTs were collected for each group, and are displayed in Table 3. As in study one, attentional bias scores were calculated for each trial type by subtracting the mean RTs when probes were in the same location as the emotional pictures from the mean RTs when they were in the opposite location (Bradley et al., 1998; Mogg et al., 2000). Thus, positive neutral trials were used to calculate biases toward/away from positive stimuli, and depressionneutral trials were used to calculate biases toward/away from depression-specific stimuli. As in study one, these two biases were found to be largely uncorrelated, r/.09, ns. These attentional bias scores were entered into a 2 /2/2 mixed-design ANOVA, with valence and duration as within-subject variables, and dysphoria as a between-subject variable. 5 Neither the main effect of duration, F(1, 63) /0.12, p /.73, nor valence, F(1, 63) /1.83, p /.18, reached significance. The predicted dysphoria / valence interaction was highly significant, F(1, 63)/7.49, h 2 /.11, pb/.008. As in study one, the three-way interaction involving duration did not reach significance, F(1, 63) /0.003, p/.96. A second similar ANOVA was 5 Once again, before calculating attentional bias scores, we entered the raw RT data into a 2/2/2/2/2 ANOVA with valence (positive, negative), duration (200 ms, 1500 ms), picture location (left, right), and probe location (left, right) as within-subject variables, and dysphoria (high, low) as a between-subject variable. Significant main effects of duration, F(1, 63)/479.80, p B/.001, and valence, F(1, 63)/15.01, p B/.001 were identified. The predicted four-way interaction of dysphoria/valence/picture location / probe location reached significance, F (1, 63)/9.93, p/.002, which provided the basis for the calculation of the attentional bias scores used in the remainder of the analyses. The five-way interaction involving duration did not reach significance, F(1, 63)/1.66, p/.20.

17 ATTENTIONAL PROCESSING IN DYSPHORIA 805 TABLE 3 Mean response latencies to probes (ms): Study two Picture valence Level of Exposure Picture Probe dysphoria duration location location Positive Negative Low 200 ms Left Left (22.12) (23.92) Left Right (37.79) (37.71) Right Left (25.23) (18.08) Right Right (31.26) (32.04) 1500 ms Left Left (18.72) (40.30) Left Right (28.09) (39.28) Right Left (22.73) (38.28) Right Right (36.70) (47.46) High 200 ms Left Left (20.57) (20.59) Left Right (34.17) (32.55) Right Left (19.95) (22.26) Right Right (40.00) (36.69) 1500 ms Left Left (29.57) (27.89) Left Right (32.01) (28.04) Right Left (25.51) (33.26) Right Right (26.88) (46.87) conducted with participants TMAS scores included as a covariate, and this analysis yielded a similar valence/dysphoria interaction, F(1, 62) /8.96, h 2 /.13, p/.004. In addition, the valence/anxiety interaction was nonsignificant, F(1, 62) /1.59, p/.21. Thus, as in study one, the results appear to have been carried predominantly by level of dysphoria, rather than comorbid anxiety. Figure 2a depicts the dysphoria /valence interaction. Non-dysphoric individuals showed a slight, although nonsignificant, vigilance toward the positive stimuli, t(38) /1.22, p/.23, while dysphoric individuals showed a more pronounced avoidance of the positive stimuli, t(25) / /2.51, d/.55, p /.02. Planned comparisons confirmed these attentional patterns to differ significantly from one another, t(63) /3.03, d /.77, p /.004. In addition, dysphoric participants manifested a bias toward the depression-specific stimuli, t(25) /2.17, d /.39, p /.04. Planned comparisons confirmed that the two groups dysphoric-specific bias scores differed significantly from each other, t(63) / /1.97, d /.51, pb/.05. Temporal differences. Despite the nonsignificant three-way interaction involving duration, we once again examined the data for early and late duration trials separately (Figures 2b and 2c). These analyses revealed two separate effects. First, the dysphoric group showed significant avoidance of the positive stimuli on short-duration trials, t(25) / /2.23, d/.45, p/.03,

18 SHANE AND PETERSON (a) 20 Dysphoric Nondysphoric (b) 20 PB DSB PB E DSB E (c) PB L DSB L Figure 2. Level of bias toward/away from positive and depression-specific stimuli on (a) all trials (b) short-duration trials, and (c) long-duration trials. Note : PB/Bias to positive stimuli; DSB/bias to depression-specific stimuli; PB E /bias to positive stimuli on early trials; DSB E /bias to depressionspecific stimuli on early trials; PB L /bias to positive stimuli on late trials; DSB L /bias to depressionspecific stimuli on late trials.

19 ATTENTIONAL PROCESSING IN DYSPHORIA 807 which differed from the non-dysphoric group, t(63) /3.15, d/.76, p/.002. Second, the dysphoric group showed significant vigilance toward dysphoricspecific stimuli on long-duration trials, t(25) /2.37, d/.47, p/.03, which differed from the non-dysphoric group, t(63) / /2.36, d/.61, p/.02. The non-dysphoric group showed no evidence of either bias at either stimulus duration. Bias differentials. Finally, bias difference (BD) scores were once again calculated for each participant by subtracting their bias toward positive stimuli from their bias toward depression-specific stimuli. Analyses indicated that the non-dysphoric group was characterised by somewhat positive BDs (M /6.74, SD/32.68), while the dysphoric group manifested highly negative BDs (M / /22.62, SD/36.47). These BDs differed significantly from each other, t(63) /3.39, d/.85, p B/.001, but only the dysphoric group s negative BD differed significantly from zero [dysphoric: t(25) /3.16, d /.62, p /.004; non-dysphoric: t(38) / /1.29, p/.20]. Thus, similar to study one, dysphoric individuals manifested considerably more negative BDs than non-dysphoric individuals, a result that may have been further enhanced by the use of depression-specific content. Regression analyses entering the absolute bias scores in block one, followed by BD in block two, indicated that BD significantly entered into the model in which depressionspecific bias was entered in block one (specific bias: b / /.206, p/.26; BD: b/.355, p/.05), but failed to enter the model when positivity bias was entered in block one (see Table 4). It should be noted, however, that BD did knock the bias to positive stimuli out of the regression model when entered in block two. However, neither predictor remained significant in this model. Finally, early and late BDs were considered separately. Non-dysphoric individuals showed neutral or positive BDs at both early and late stages of attentional processing (early: M / /.87, SD /49.26; late: M /2.63, SD / 46.78), while dysphoric individuals showed negative BDs at both stages of processing (early: M/ /20.27, SD /46.89; late: M / /15.79, SD/49.36). A repeated measures ANOVA with early and late BDs as within-subjects variables failed to reach significance, p /.95. Discussion The results of study two extend the findings from study one in a number of important ways. First, whereas study one demonstrated that dysphoric individuals manifested a bias away from positive stimuli at 500 ms poststimulus onset, the results of the second study indicate that a similar bias away from positive stimuli can be identified as early as 200 ms post-stimulus onset. Two-hundred milliseconds was chosen as the stimulus duration for the

20 808 SHANE AND PETERSON TABLE 4 Regression models using absolute biases and bias differentials to predict BDI: Study two Predictor Standardised coefficient (b) p-value Model p-value Model 1 Bias to positive stimuli / Model 2 Bias to positive stimuli / Bias differential / Model 1 Bias to depression-specific stimuli Model 2 Bias to depression-specific stimuli / Bias differential / second study because research demonstrates that ms is generally required in order to perform a voluntary disengagement and re-engagement of attentional gaze (Chun & Wolfe, 2001; Duncan et al., 1995). Thus, at 200 ms we can be relatively assured that the direction of the participants initial orienting in response to the dual-word display was being evaluated, and further that dysphoric individuals show initial orienting responses away from positive stimuli. Research on orienting responses often claim that such responses occur automatically. Although we are hesitant to characterise this dysphoric bias as automatic, the results of the present studies do suggest that they are likely the result of particularly early stage processes. This notion is consistent with a handful of recent studies which have also revealed evidence of early stage abnormalities in depressives attention to negative stimuli (Luecken et al., 2004; Lundh et al., 1999). Second, by using depression-specific stimuli in study two, in contrast to the more general category of negatively valent stimuli utilised in study one, a strong bias toward the depression-related stimuli was revealed within the dysphoric group. A number of recent investigations have suggested that depressive and dysphoric individuals manifest a specific bias toward depression-specific information, which may not generalise to other negatively valent stimuli (Gotlib et al., 2004; Westra & Kuiper, 1997). The results of study two are consistent with these observations, and suggest that the inability to demonstrate a significant bias toward the negative stimuli in study one may have been due to the more heterogeneous collection of IAPS slides used. The present study thus confirms the importance of utilising depression-specific stimuli when investigating processing biases in dysphoria, and provides further evidence to indicate that dysphoria is characterised by biased attention toward such depression-related stimuli. The depression-specific nature of this bias is intriguing. A number of cognitive and neural models have been proposed to explain how early, and perhaps pre-attentive, systems may preferentially process threat-related stimuli. We are unaware of any similarly conceptualised system for the early

Selective attention and threat: Quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task

Selective attention and threat: Quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task Behaviour Research and Therapy 45 (2007) 607 615 Shorter communication Selective attention and threat: Quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task Elske Salemink a,,

More information

Attention Allocation and Incidental Recognition of Emotional Information in Dysphoria

Attention Allocation and Incidental Recognition of Emotional Information in Dysphoria DOI 10.1007/s10608-010-9305-3 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Attention Allocation and Incidental Recognition of Emotional Information in Dysphoria Alissa J. Ellis Christopher G. Beevers Tony T. Wells Ó Springer Science+Business

More information

Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures

Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2001, 54A (3), 665 681 Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures Jenny Yiend and Andrew Mathews MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK

More information

Memory for emotional faces in naturally occurring dysphoria and

Memory for emotional faces in naturally occurring dysphoria and Running Head: Memory for emotional faces Memory for emotional faces in naturally occurring dysphoria and induced negative mood Nathan Ridout*, Aliya Noreen & Jaskaran Johal Clinical & Cognitive Neurosciences,

More information

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining 8 the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining items was included. Participants were asked to decide whether a different item was included in the array,

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for affectively valenced and neutral stimuli in depression: Evidence from a novel matching task

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for affectively valenced and neutral stimuli in depression: Evidence from a novel matching task COGNITION AND EMOTION 2011, 25 (7), 12461254 BRIEF REPORT Memory for affectively valenced and neutral stimuli in depression: Evidence from a novel matching task Ian H. Gotlib 1, John Jonides 2, Martin

More information

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials for Wisneski and Skitka Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction Table of Contents 2 Pilot Studies 2 High Awareness

More information

2/19/2011. Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois University February 18, 2011

2/19/2011. Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois University February 18, 2011 Lifetime prevalence estimates for PTSD are about 8%; however, the majority of Americans experience a traumatic event in their lifetimes (Kessler et al, 1995: NCS) Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA COGNITION AND EMOTION 2007, 21 (3), 681688 BRIEF REPORT Vulnerability to depressive symptoms: Clarifying the role of excessive reassurance seeking and perceived social support in an interpersonal model

More information

Visual attention to emotion in depression: Facilitation and withdrawal processes

Visual attention to emotion in depression: Facilitation and withdrawal processes COGNITION AND EMOTION 2012, 26 (4), 602614 Visual attention to emotion in depression: Facilitation and withdrawal processes Blair E. Wisco 1, Teresa A. Treat 2, and Andrew Hollingworth 2 1 Department of

More information

Irish Journal of Psychology, 20 (1):

Irish Journal of Psychology, 20 (1): Provided by the author(s) and University College Dublin Library in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite the published version when available. Title Mood congruent memory bias of individuals

More information

Selective attention for masked and unmasked threatening words in anxiety: Effects of trait anxiety, state anxiety and awareness

Selective attention for masked and unmasked threatening words in anxiety: Effects of trait anxiety, state anxiety and awareness Bond University epublications@bond Humanities & Social Sciences papers Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences 3-1-2010 Selective attention for masked and unmasked threatening words in anxiety: Effects

More information

Internet-delivered assessment and manipulation of anxiety-linked attentional bias: Validation of a free-access attentional probe software package

Internet-delivered assessment and manipulation of anxiety-linked attentional bias: Validation of a free-access attentional probe software package Behavior Research Methods 2007, 39 (3), 533-538 Internet-delivered assessment and manipulation of anxiety-linked attentional bias: Validation of a free-access attentional probe software package COLIN MACLEOD,

More information

Coherence and Specificity of Information-Processing Biases in Depression and Social Phobia

Coherence and Specificity of Information-Processing Biases in Depression and Social Phobia Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 113, No. 3, 386 398 0021-843X/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.386 Coherence and Specificity of

More information

Attentional biases in processing emotional facial expressions: Effects of state anxiety, trait anxiety and awareness

Attentional biases in processing emotional facial expressions: Effects of state anxiety, trait anxiety and awareness Bond University epublications@bond Faculty of Society and Design Publications Faculty of Society and Design 11-4-2014 Attentional biases in processing emotional facial expressions: Effects of state anxiety,

More information

Attentional Disengagement and Stress Recovery 0. Attentional Disengagement Predicts Stress Recovery in Depression: An Eye-tracking. Study.

Attentional Disengagement and Stress Recovery 0. Attentional Disengagement Predicts Stress Recovery in Depression: An Eye-tracking. Study. Attentional Disengagement and Stress Recovery 0 Attentional Disengagement Predicts Stress Recovery in Depression: An Eye-tracking Study Alvaro Sanchez 1, Carmelo Vazquez 1, Craig Marker 2, Joelle LeMoult

More information

the Chinese University of Hong Kong, China Correspondence should be addressed to: Samuel M.Y. Ho;

the Chinese University of Hong Kong, China Correspondence should be addressed to: Samuel M.Y. Ho; IBIMA Publishing Advances in Cancer: Research & Treatment http://www.ibimapublishing.com/journals/acrt/acrt.html Vol. 2013 (2013), Article ID 813339, 7 pages DOI: 10.5171/2013.813339 Research Article A

More information

Affective Interference: An Explanation for Negative Attention Biases in Dysphoria?

Affective Interference: An Explanation for Negative Attention Biases in Dysphoria? Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 26, No. 1, February 2002 ( C 2002), pp. 73 87 Affective Interference: An Explanation for Negative Attention Biases in Dysphoria? Greg J. Siegle, 1,4 Rick E. Ingram,

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

Differential Reactivity of Attention Biases in Patients with Social Anxiety Disorder

Differential Reactivity of Attention Biases in Patients with Social Anxiety Disorder International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 2015, 15, 3, 425-431 Printed in Spain. All rights reserved. Copyright 2015 AAC Differential Reactivity of Attention Biases in Patients with

More information

Fear of faces: a psychophysiological investigation of facial affect processing in social phobia

Fear of faces: a psychophysiological investigation of facial affect processing in social phobia University of Wollongong Research Online University of Wollongong Thesis Collection 1954-2016 University of Wollongong Thesis Collections 2004 Fear of faces: a psychophysiological investigation of facial

More information

BRIEF REPORT. The effects of optimism and pessimism on updating emotional information in working memory. Sara M. Levens 1 and Ian H.

BRIEF REPORT. The effects of optimism and pessimism on updating emotional information in working memory. Sara M. Levens 1 and Ian H. COGNITION AND EMOTION 2012, 26 (2), 341350 BRIEF REPORT The effects of optimism and pessimism on updating emotional information in working memory Sara M. Levens 1 and Ian H. Gotlib 2 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

Focusing on fear: Attentional disengagement from emotional faces

Focusing on fear: Attentional disengagement from emotional faces Europe PMC Funders Group Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Vis cogn. 2005 ; 12(1): 145 158. doi:10.1080/13506280444000076. Focusing on fear: Attentional disengagement from emotional

More information

Journal of Anxiety Disorders

Journal of Anxiety Disorders Journal of Anxiety Disorders 24 (2010) 657 662 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Anxiety Disorders Attention training for reducing spider fear in spider-fearful individuals Hannah E.

More information

Threat-Related Attentional Bias in Anxious and Nonanxious Individuals: A Meta-Analytic Study

Threat-Related Attentional Bias in Anxious and Nonanxious Individuals: A Meta-Analytic Study Psychological Bulletin Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 133, No. 1, 1 24 0033-2909/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1 Threat-Related Attentional Bias in Anxious

More information

Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence

Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence Blackwell Science, LtdOxford, UKADDAddiction1360-0443 2003 Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs98Original ArticleBiases in eye movements in smokerskarin Mogg et al. RESEARCH REPORT

More information

Fear of evaluation in social anxiety: Mediation of attentional bias to human faces

Fear of evaluation in social anxiety: Mediation of attentional bias to human faces Fear of evaluation in social anxiety: Mediation of attentional bias to human faces Author Sluis, Rachel, Boschen, Mark Published 2014 Journal Title Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

More information

INTERPRETIVE BIAS IN ANXIETY: THE SEARCH FOR AN ELUSIVE COGNITIVE EFFECT. Lucia Fung. BSocSc (Hons) of The University of Western Australia

INTERPRETIVE BIAS IN ANXIETY: THE SEARCH FOR AN ELUSIVE COGNITIVE EFFECT. Lucia Fung. BSocSc (Hons) of The University of Western Australia INTERPRETIVE BIAS IN ANXIETY: THE SEARCH FOR AN ELUSIVE COGNITIVE EFFECT Lucia Fung BSocSc (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of The University of Western Australia

More information

Attentional Bias and Mood Persistence as Prospective Predictors of Dysphoria

Attentional Bias and Mood Persistence as Prospective Predictors of Dysphoria Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 27, No. 6, December 2003 ( C 2003), pp. 619 637 Attentional Bias and Mood Persistence as Prospective Predictors of Dysphoria Christopher G. Beevers 1,2,3 and Charles

More information

The effect of depressed mood on the interpretation of ambiguity, with and without negative mood induction

The effect of depressed mood on the interpretation of ambiguity, with and without negative mood induction COGNITION AND EMOTION 2007, 21 (3), 614645 The effect of depressed mood on the interpretation of ambiguity, with and without negative mood induction M. A. Suzie Bisson and Christopher R. Sears University

More information

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1983, Vol. 51, No. 5, 721-725 Copyright 1983 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation

More information

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times CHAPTER 4 Lars Kuchinke, Melissa L.-H. Võ, Lars Michael, and Arthur M. Jacobs 5 Abstract In a lexical decision experiment emotional valence and emotional

More information

Supplementary Material. Participants completed the 90-item Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ;

Supplementary Material. Participants completed the 90-item Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ; Prefrontal mediation 1 Supplementary Material 1. Questionnaires Participants completed the 90-item Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ; Watson and Clark, 1991, Watson, Clark, Weber, Smith Assenheimer,

More information

Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not...

Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not... Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not... Taylor, AJ, Bendall, RCA and Thompson, C Title Authors Type URL Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not... Taylor, AJ, Bendall,

More information

Repressive Coping and Pain: Shifts in Attention from Distress toward Physical Pain May Partly Explain Conversion

Repressive Coping and Pain: Shifts in Attention from Distress toward Physical Pain May Partly Explain Conversion Repressive Coping and Pain: Shifts in Attention from Distress toward Physical Pain May Partly Explain Conversion John W. Burns, Ph.D., Phillip J. Quartana, Ph.D. Erin Elfant, Ph.D. Department of Psychology

More information

Blinded by Emotion: Target Misses Follow Attention Capture by Arousing Distractors in RSVP

Blinded by Emotion: Target Misses Follow Attention Capture by Arousing Distractors in RSVP 1 Blinded by Emotion: Target Misses Follow Attention Capture by Arousing Distractors in RSVP Karen M. Arnell Kassandra V. Killman & David Fijavz Brock University Participants are usually able to search

More information

Cognition and Depression: Current Status and Future Directions

Cognition and Depression: Current Status and Future Directions I ANRV407-CP06-11 ARI 18 December 2009 17:42 R E V I E W S E C N A D V A N Cognition and Depression: Current Status and Future Directions Ian H. Gotlib 1 and Jutta Joormann 2 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy Volume 17, Number 2, Summer 1999 CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

More information

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers.

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. 1 This work is a result of the combined efforts of myself and my research advisor, Dr.

More information

Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat?

Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat? Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 1325 1335 www.elsevier.com/locate/brat Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat?

More information

UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY. The Effects of a Sad Mood Induction on Attention Disengagement from Emotional Images in. Remitted and Never Depressed Women

UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY. The Effects of a Sad Mood Induction on Attention Disengagement from Emotional Images in. Remitted and Never Depressed Women UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY The Effects of a Sad Mood Induction on Attention Disengagement from Emotional Images in Remitted and Never Depressed Women by Stephanie Laurie Marie Korol A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE

More information

Negative affect varying in motivational intensity influences scope of memory

Negative affect varying in motivational intensity influences scope of memory Cognition and Emotion ISSN: 0269-9931 (Print) 1464-0600 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcem20 Negative affect varying in motivational intensity influences scope of memory A.

More information

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 580 584 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027885 OBSERVATION Templates

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

Disengagement of attention from facial emotion in unipolar depression

Disengagement of attention from facial emotion in unipolar depression Blackwell Science, LtdOxford, UKPCNPsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences1323-13162005 Blackwell Science Pty LtdDecember 2005596723729Original ArticleDisengagement of attention in depressions. P. Karparova

More information

Paper II. Wang, C. E., Brennen, T., & Holte, A. (2005). Decreased approach motivation in depression. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, in press.

Paper II. Wang, C. E., Brennen, T., & Holte, A. (2005). Decreased approach motivation in depression. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, in press. Paper II Wang, C. E., Brennen, T., & Holte, A. (2005). Decreased approach motivation in depression. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, in press. Decreased approach motivation in depression 1 RUNNING

More information

The Interaction of Mood and Rumination in Depression: Effects on Mood Maintenance and Mood-Congruent Autobiographical Memory

The Interaction of Mood and Rumination in Depression: Effects on Mood Maintenance and Mood-Congruent Autobiographical Memory J Rat-Emo Cognitive-Behav Ther (2009) 27:144 159 DOI 10.1007/s10942-009-0096-y ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Interaction of Mood and Rumination in Depression: Effects on Mood Maintenance and Mood-Congruent Autobiographical

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Depressive implicit associations and adults reports of childhood abuse

BRIEF REPORT. Depressive implicit associations and adults reports of childhood abuse COGNITION AND EMOTION 2011, 25 (2), 328333 BRIEF REPORT Depressive implicit associations and adults reports of childhood abuse Ashley L. Johnson, Jessica S. Benas, and Brandon E. Gibb Binghamton University

More information

Automatic and Effortful Processing of Self-Statements in Depression

Automatic and Effortful Processing of Self-Statements in Depression Paper III Wang, C. E., Brennen, T., & Holte, A. (2006). Automatic and effortful processing of self-statements in depression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 35, 117-124. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy Vol 35,

More information

Original Papers. Memory bias training by means of the emotional short-term memory task

Original Papers. Memory bias training by means of the emotional short-term memory task Original Papers Polish Psychological Bulletin 2015, vol 46(1), 122-126 DOI - 10.1515/ppb-2015-0016 Borysław Paulewicz * Agata Blaut ** Aleksandra Gronostaj *** Memory bias training by means of the emotional

More information

Attention for Emotional Faces Under Restricted Awareness Revisited: Do Emotional Faces Automatically Attract Attention?

Attention for Emotional Faces Under Restricted Awareness Revisited: Do Emotional Faces Automatically Attract Attention? Emotion Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 7, No. 2, 285 295 1528-3542/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.285 Attention for Emotional Faces Under Restricted Awareness

More information

A Behavioral Attention Task for Investigating Rumination in Borderline Personality Disorder: Final Report

A Behavioral Attention Task for Investigating Rumination in Borderline Personality Disorder: Final Report Kaleidoscope Volume 11 Article 68 July 2014 A Behavioral Attention Task for Investigating Rumination in Borderline Personality Disorder: Final Report Jacob Folsom Follow this and additional works at: https://uknowledge.uky.edu/kaleidoscope

More information

Running Head: INFORMATION PROCESSING BIASES IN GAD. Information processing biases in generalised anxiety disorder. Sarra Hayes and Colette R.

Running Head: INFORMATION PROCESSING BIASES IN GAD. Information processing biases in generalised anxiety disorder. Sarra Hayes and Colette R. Running Head: INFORMATION PROCESSING BIASES IN GAD Information processing biases in generalised anxiety disorder Sarra Hayes and Colette R. Hirsch Institute of Psychiatry, King s College London. For citation:

More information

Heuristics and criterion setting during selective encoding in visual decision making: Evidence from eye movements

Heuristics and criterion setting during selective encoding in visual decision making: Evidence from eye movements VISUAL COGNITION, 2012, 20 (9), 11101129 Heuristics and criterion setting during selective encoding in visual decision making: Evidence from eye movements Elizabeth R. Schotter, Cainen Gerety, and Keith

More information

Biased Processing of Emotional Information in Girls at Risk for Depression

Biased Processing of Emotional Information in Girls at Risk for Depression Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 116, No. 1, 135 143 0021-843X/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.135 Biased Processing of Emotional

More information

Mood-Induced Shifts in Attentional Bias to Emotional Information Predict Ill- and Well-Being

Mood-Induced Shifts in Attentional Bias to Emotional Information Predict Ill- and Well-Being Emotion 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 11, No. 2, 241 248 1528-3542/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0022572 Mood-Induced Shifts in Attentional Bias to Emotional Information Predict Ill- and

More information

Mood-congruent free recall bias in anxious individuals is not a consequence of response bias

Mood-congruent free recall bias in anxious individuals is not a consequence of response bias Europe PMC Funders Group Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Memory. 2006 May ; 14(4): 393 399. doi:10.1080/09658210500343166. Mood-congruent free recall bias in anxious individuals is

More information

Gaze Patterns When Looking at Emotional Pictures: Motivationally Biased Attention 1

Gaze Patterns When Looking at Emotional Pictures: Motivationally Biased Attention 1 Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 28, No. 3, September 2004 ( C 2004) Gaze Patterns When Looking at Emotional Pictures: Motivationally Biased Attention 1 Manuel G. Calvo 2,4 and Peter J. Lang 3 Pictures of

More information

Measures. What is child physical abuse? Theories of CPA. Social Cognition and CPA risk. Social Information Processing Model (Milner, 1994)

Measures. What is child physical abuse? Theories of CPA. Social Cognition and CPA risk. Social Information Processing Model (Milner, 1994) What is child physical abuse? Social Cognition and Child Physical Abuse Risk: Research Updates and Future Directions Julie L. Crouch, Ph.D., Director Center for the Study of Family Violence and Sexual

More information

HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1. Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in

HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1. Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1 Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in Heterogeneous Backgrounds Belinda M. Craig, Stefanie I. Becker, &

More information

Investigating the Role of Intolerance of Uncertainty in GAD Using a Probe Discrimination. Task

Investigating the Role of Intolerance of Uncertainty in GAD Using a Probe Discrimination. Task Investigating the Role of Intolerance of Uncertainty in GAD Using a Probe Discrimination Task BY ALEXANDER A. JENDRUSINA B.S., Michigan State University, 2010 THESIS Submitted as partial fulfillment of

More information

Effects of Trait Anxiety and Depression on Working Memory Updating

Effects of Trait Anxiety and Depression on Working Memory Updating University of Colorado, Boulder CU Scholar Undergraduate Honors Theses Honors Program Spring 016 Effects of Trait Anxiety and Depression on Working Memory Updating Joy Walters joy.walters@colorado.edu

More information

Selective interpretation in anxiety: Uncertainty for threatening events

Selective interpretation in anxiety: Uncertainty for threatening events COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2001, 15 (3), 299 320 Selective interpretation in anxiety: Uncertainty for threatening events Manuel G. Calvo and M. Dolores Castillo University of La Laguna, Spain The role of event

More information

Moderators of the Relationship between. Cognitive Bias and Depressive Symptoms. A Senior Honors Thesis

Moderators of the Relationship between. Cognitive Bias and Depressive Symptoms. A Senior Honors Thesis Moderators of the Relationship between Cognitive Bias and Depressive Symptoms A Senior Honors Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for Graduation with Distinction in Psychology in

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Cognitive Bias Modification: Induced Interpretive Biases Affect Memory

Cognitive Bias Modification: Induced Interpretive Biases Affect Memory Trinity University Digital Commons @ Trinity Psychology Faculty Research Psychology Department 2-2011 Cognitive Bias Modification: Induced Interpretive Biases Affect Memory Tanya B. Tran University of

More information

Johanna Boettcher 1,2 *, Linda Leek 3, Lisa Matson 3, Emily A. Holmes 4, Michael Browning 5, Colin MacLeod 6, Gerhard Andersson 7,8, Per Carlbring 1

Johanna Boettcher 1,2 *, Linda Leek 3, Lisa Matson 3, Emily A. Holmes 4, Michael Browning 5, Colin MacLeod 6, Gerhard Andersson 7,8, Per Carlbring 1 Internet-Based Attention Bias Modification for Social Anxiety: A Randomised Controlled Comparison of Training towards Negative and Training Towards Positive Cues Johanna Boettcher 1,2 *, Linda Leek 3,

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

Michael Armey David M. Fresco. Jon Rottenberg. James J. Gross Ian H. Gotlib. Kent State University. Stanford University. University of South Florida

Michael Armey David M. Fresco. Jon Rottenberg. James J. Gross Ian H. Gotlib. Kent State University. Stanford University. University of South Florida Further psychometric refinement of depressive rumination: Support for the Brooding and Pondering factor solution in a diverse community sample with clinician-assessed psychopathology Michael Armey David

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Personality and Individual Differences 53 (1) 13 17 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Attentional

More information

NEGATIVITY BIAS AS A PREDICTOR OF EMOTIONAL REACTIVITY TO A STRESSFUL EVENT THESIS. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for

NEGATIVITY BIAS AS A PREDICTOR OF EMOTIONAL REACTIVITY TO A STRESSFUL EVENT THESIS. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for NEGATIVITY BIAS AS A PREDICTOR OF EMOTIONAL REACTIVITY TO A STRESSFUL EVENT THESIS Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in the Graduate School of The Ohio

More information

Spatiotemporal brain dynamics underlying Attentional Bias Modifications. Etienne Sallard, Léa Hartmann, Lucas Spierer

Spatiotemporal brain dynamics underlying Attentional Bias Modifications. Etienne Sallard, Léa Hartmann, Lucas Spierer Spatiotemporal brain dynamics underlying Attentional Bias Modifications Etienne Sallard, Léa Hartmann, Lucas Spierer Day of cognition meeting 7th October 2015 Attentional bias Tendency to orient attention

More information

Running head: COMBINED COGNITIVE BIAS HYPOTHESIS IN DEPRESSION 1. The Combined Cognitive Bias Hypothesis in Depression: A State-of-the-art

Running head: COMBINED COGNITIVE BIAS HYPOTHESIS IN DEPRESSION 1. The Combined Cognitive Bias Hypothesis in Depression: A State-of-the-art Running head: COMBINED COGNITIVE BIAS HYPOTHESIS IN DEPRESSION 1 The Combined Cognitive Bias Hypothesis in Depression: A State-of-the-art Jonas Everaert a, Ernst H.W. Koster a and Nazanin Derakshan b a

More information

Selective Attention to Affective Stimuli and Clinical Depression Among Youths: Role of Anxiety and Specificity of Emotion

Selective Attention to Affective Stimuli and Clinical Depression Among Youths: Role of Anxiety and Specificity of Emotion Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 119, No. 3, 491 501 0021-843X/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019609 Selective Attention to Affective Stimuli and Clinical Depression

More information

Investigating the Efficacy of Attention Bias Modification in Reducing High Spider Fear: The Role of Individual Differences in Initial Bias

Investigating the Efficacy of Attention Bias Modification in Reducing High Spider Fear: The Role of Individual Differences in Initial Bias Accepted Manuscript Investigating the Efficacy of Attention Bias Modification in Reducing High Spider Fear: The Role of Individual Differences in Initial Bias Elaine Fox, Konstantina Zougkou, Chris Ashwin,

More information

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The number line effect reflects top-down control Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),862-868???-??? The number line effect reflects top-down control JELENA RISTIC University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger Emotion 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 11, No. 1, 47 51 1528-3542/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021238 Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship

More information

THE INFLUENCE OF NEGATIVE AFFECT ON SELF-REFERENT SENTENCE PROCESSING AND MEMORY PERFORMANCE. Austin Greg Fitts

THE INFLUENCE OF NEGATIVE AFFECT ON SELF-REFERENT SENTENCE PROCESSING AND MEMORY PERFORMANCE. Austin Greg Fitts THE INFLUENCE OF NEGATIVE AFFECT ON SELF-REFERENT SENTENCE PROCESSING AND MEMORY PERFORMANCE BY Austin Greg Fitts Submitted to the graduate degree program in Cognitive Psychology and the Faculty of the

More information

Subjective Responses to Emotional Stimuli During Labeling, Reappraisal, and Distraction

Subjective Responses to Emotional Stimuli During Labeling, Reappraisal, and Distraction Emotion 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 11, No. 3, 468 480 1528-3542/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0023503 Subjective Responses to Emotional Stimuli During Labeling, Reappraisal, and Distraction

More information

Running head: SHYNESS MINDSET AND ATTENTIONAL BIAS 1. Shyness Mindset Beliefs and Attentional Bias: An Experimental Study. Amber M.

Running head: SHYNESS MINDSET AND ATTENTIONAL BIAS 1. Shyness Mindset Beliefs and Attentional Bias: An Experimental Study. Amber M. Running head: SHYNESS MINDSET AND ATTENTIONAL BIAS 1 Shyness Mindset Beliefs and Attentional Bias: An Experimental Study Amber M. Chamberlain Northern Illinois University Authors Note This paper describes

More information

Published online: 05 Jun 2009.

Published online: 05 Jun 2009. This article was downloaded by: [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] On: 16 December 2013, At: 13:13 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954

More information

David M. Fresco, Ph.D.

David M. Fresco, Ph.D. THE ASSOCIATION OF RUMINATION TO GENERALIZED ANXIETY DISORDER AND TO DEFICITS IN EMOTION REGULATION David M. Fresco, Ph.D. Kent State University Kent, OH. 1 Speak to me as to thy thinkings, As thou dost

More information

Psychometric properties of startle and corrugator response in NPU, Affective Picture Viewing, and. Resting State tasks

Psychometric properties of startle and corrugator response in NPU, Affective Picture Viewing, and. Resting State tasks Psychometric properties of startle and corrugator response in NPU, Affective Picture Viewing, and Resting State tasks Jesse T. Kaye, Daniel E. Bradford, & John J. Curtin Supplemental Materials Method Self-report

More information

Behaviour Research and Therapy

Behaviour Research and Therapy Behaviour Research and Therapy 49 (2011) 406e412 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behaviour Research and Therapy journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat Effect of visual perspective on

More information

Cognitive Biases in Depression and Eating Disorders

Cognitive Biases in Depression and Eating Disorders Cogn Ther Res (2011) 35:68 78 DOI 10.1007/s10608-009-9279-1 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Cognitive Biases in Depression and Eating Disorders Jessica S. Benas Brandon E. Gibb Published online: 14 November 2009 Ó Springer

More information

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search VISUAL COGNITION, 0000, 00 (0), 111 Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search Walter R. Boot Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee,

More information

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1995 The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the

More information

Neuroticism, preattentive and attentional biases towards threat, and anxiety before and after a severe stressor: a prospective study

Neuroticism, preattentive and attentional biases towards threat, and anxiety before and after a severe stressor: a prospective study Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004) 767 778 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Neuroticism, preattentive and attentional biases towards threat, and anxiety before and after a severe stressor: a

More information

The Stroop Effect The Effect of Interfering Colour Stimuli Upon Reading Names of Colours Serially ABSTRACT

The Stroop Effect The Effect of Interfering Colour Stimuli Upon Reading Names of Colours Serially ABSTRACT The Stroop Effect The Effect of Interfering Colour Stimuli Upon Reading Names of Colours Serially ABSTRACT This experiment, a partial duplication of the work of Stroop (l935) l, aimed to demonstrate the

More information

Supplementary Materials Are taboo words simply more arousing?

Supplementary Materials Are taboo words simply more arousing? Supplementary Materials Are taboo words simply more arousing? Our study was not designed to compare potential differences on memory other than arousal between negative and taboo words. Thus, our data cannot

More information

Brooding rumination and attentional biases in currently non-depressed individuals: an eye-tracking study

Brooding rumination and attentional biases in currently non-depressed individuals: an eye-tracking study COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2017 VOL. 31, NO. 5, 1062 1069 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1187116 BRIEF ARTICLE Brooding rumination and attentional biases in currently non-depressed individuals: an

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

Brooding and Pondering: Isolating the Active Ingredients of Depressive Rumination with Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Brooding and Pondering: Isolating the Active Ingredients of Depressive Rumination with Confirmatory Factor Analysis Michael Armey David M. Fresco Kent State University Brooding and Pondering: Isolating the Active Ingredients of Depressive Rumination with Confirmatory Factor Analysis Douglas S. Mennin Yale University

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2014 Vol. 28, No. 6, 1090 1099, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.866936 BRIEF REPORT Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder James E. Sorenson,

More information

Cognitive Bias Modification: Retrieval Practice to Simulate and Oppose Ruminative Memory Biases

Cognitive Bias Modification: Retrieval Practice to Simulate and Oppose Ruminative Memory Biases Trinity University Digital Commons @ Trinity Psychology Faculty Research Psychology Department 1-2017 Cognitive Bias Modification: Retrieval Practice to Simulate and Oppose Ruminative Memory Biases Paula

More information

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger Trait emotion and startle eyeblink 1 Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger David M. Amodio New York University Eddie Harmon-Jones Texas

More information

Speeded Detection and Increased Distraction in Fear of Spiders: Evidence From Eye Movements

Speeded Detection and Increased Distraction in Fear of Spiders: Evidence From Eye Movements Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 114, No. 2, 235 248 0021-843X/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.235 Speeded Detection and Increased

More information

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Manard et al. BMC Neuroscience 2014, 15:7 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Marine Manard 1,2, Delphine Carabin

More information

Illusory Correlation and Group Impression Formation in Young and Older Adults

Illusory Correlation and Group Impression Formation in Young and Older Adults Journal of Gerontology: PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES 2000, Vol. 55B, No. 4, P224 P237 Copyright 2000 by The Gerontological Society of America Illusory Correlation and Group Impression Formation in Young and

More information