PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE"

Transcription

1 This article was downloaded by: [Michigan State University] On: 16 September 2009 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number ] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: Registered office: Mortimer House, Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: Early detection and avoidance of threatening faces during passive viewing Mark W. Becker a ; Brian Detweiler-Bedell b a Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA b Lewis & Clark College, Portland, OR, USA First Published on: 09 March 2009 To cite this Article Becker, Mark W. and Detweiler-Bedell, Brian(2009)'Early detection and avoidance of threatening faces during passive viewing',the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,62:7, To link to this Article: DOI: / URL: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

2 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2009, 62 (7), Short article Early detection and avoidance of threatening faces during passive viewing Mark W. Becker Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA Brian Detweiler-Bedell Lewis & Clark College, Portland, OR, USA To evaluate whether there is an early attentional bias towards negative stimuli, we tracked participants eyes while they passively viewed displays composed of four Ekman faces. In Experiment 1 each display consisted of three neutral faces and one face depicting fear or happiness. In half of the trials, all faces were inverted. Although the passive viewing task should have been very sensitive to attentional biases, we found no evidence that overt attention was biased towards fearful faces. Instead, people tended to actively avoid looking at the fearful face. This avoidance was evident very early in scene viewing, suggesting that the threat associated with the faces was evaluated rapidly. Experiment 2 replicated this effect and extended it to angry faces. In sum, our data suggest that negative facial expressions are rapidly analysed and influence visual scanning, but, rather than attract attention, such faces are actively avoided. Keywords: Emotion; Eye tracking; Threat; Attention; Fear. Sparse attentional capacity severely limits the number of objects that can be apprehended at any instant (Becker & Pashler, 2005). Thus it would seem important for attention to be allocated to the most relevant aspects of a visual scene. This has led a number of researchers to suggest that attention is preferentially allocated to sources of potential threat within the environment (e.g., Lundqvist & Öhman, 2005). Indeed, it is been suggested that subcortical projections leading to the amygdala allow for the rapid evaluation of the threatening stimuli, and, based on that evaluation, attention may be allocated preferentially to such stimuli (LeDoux, 2000; however, see Cowey, 2004). Given the clear evolutionary advantage of such a system, it has been suggested that this mechanism is phylogenetically old and should be present in all people (Mogg & Bradley, 1998). Although this theory is intuitively appealing, a number of findings have begun to challenge this view. Recent neuroimaging (Bishop, Jenkins, & Correspondence should be addressed to Mark W. Becker, Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824, USA. becker54@msu.edu # 2009 The Experimental Psychology Society 1257 DOI: /

3 BECKER AND DETWEILER-BEDELL Lawrence, 2007; Pessoa, 2005) and electrophysiological (Holmes, Vuilleumier, & Eimer, 2003) studies suggest that the amygdala is activated by threat only in conditions of low load; when a primary task is made more demanding, the amygdala does not seem to react to an incidental threatening stimulus. This finding challenges the automatic nature of the amygdala s response, thereby limiting the extent to which the amygdala can act as a rapid, automatic alarm system. Although these findings limit the situations under which this system may function, they are still broadly consistent with the view that there is a bias to orient attention towards threatening stimuli, provided that adequate resources are available to support the evaluation of the threatening stimuli by the amygdala. Behavioural research, however, has begun to challenge the claim that there is a default bias to attend preferentially to threatening stimuli. Although there are a number of reports suggesting that threatening objects (Öhman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001) and faces (Lundqvist & Öhman, 2005) capture attention, other researchers have failed to replicate these results (Koster, Verschuere, Burssens, Custers, & Crombez, 2007). In addition, a number of reports comparing anxious or phobic participants to typical participants fail to demonstrate attentional capture by threatening objects (Caseras, Garner, Bradley, & Mogg, 2007) or faces (Fox, 2002) among typical participants. The failure to find evidence for attentional capture by threatening stimuli among nonanxious participants seems problematic for the theory that almost all people should have a bias to attend rapidly to such stimuli. There are, however, two ways that a proponent of the theory might discount these contrary results. First, one might argue that typical lab settings and stimuli are so innocuous that only participants who are especially sensitive to threat (e.g., phobic or anxious participants) will evaluate lab-based stimuli as threatening; nonanxious people will not perceive them as threatening. To evaluate this explanation, researchers have attempted to make the lab experience less innocuous by pairing subsets of their stimuli with loud noise bursts, thus conditioning a more powerful fear response among participants. Nevertheless, even under these conditions, results are mixed. Some researchers find that attention is captured by fear-conditioned stimuli (Koster, Crombez, Van Damme, Verschuere, & De Houwer, 2004), but others fail to observe such an effect (Stormark, Hugdahl, & Posner, 1999). Thus there is no clear evidence that the failure to find consistent effects is due to the stimuli not being threatening enough. Second, one might use the aforementioned neurophysiological findings to claim that disparate behavioural results arise because the cognitive load of the primary task varies across experiments. With low-load tasks, the warning system is activated, and one finds evidence for the rapid reorienting of attention towards threat. With high-load tasks, there are insufficient resources to activate the warning system, and one fails to find evidence for the rapid reorienting of attention towards threat. Consistent with this explanation, most behavioural experiments investigating this issue use tasks that require participants to make overt decisions and responses (e.g., visual search, Posner cueing, or dot probe tasks). These primary tasks may influence the likelihood of the threatening stimulus receiving adequate resources to activate the amygdala. In addition, the tasks impose attentional sets. These attentional sets may create their own biases to look toward or away from threatening objects. In sum, there may be a default attentional bias toward threat, but experimental tasks may interfere with the ability to find the effect either by imposing an attentional set that creates its own bias or by increasing the perceptual or attentional load to such an extent that the amygdala is not able to evaluate a stimulus as threatening. Alternatively, it is possible that the presumed default bias to attend to threatening stimuli may not exist within nonanxious people. In order to avoid potential task-related confounds and to evaluate whether there is a default bias to shift attention towards threatening stimuli, we investigated overt shifts of attention while participants performed a passive viewing 1258 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 62 (7)

4 AVOIDANCE OF THREAT task. The lack of a primary task is the boundary condition of a low-load primary task. That is, removing explicit task demands should allow any default attentional bias, however slight, to exert its influence. Of course, in the absence of an explicit search or decision task, such a bias would have to be observed through participants eye movements. Thus we tracked participant s eye movements while they passively viewed displays. For several reasons, we chose to use facial expressions of emotion as the stimuli for this passive viewing task. Basic facial expressions of emotion are thought to be relatively universal (Ekman, 1973) and are detected via the low spatial frequencies that are available in the periphery (Vuilleumier & Pourtois, 2007). Moreover, they lead to rapid amygdala activation (Palermo & Rhodes, 2007; Sergerie, Chochol, & Armony, 2008; however, see Cowey, 2004) and can be detected subcortically (de Gelder, Vroomen, Pourtois, & Weiskrantz, 1999). All of these reasons suggest that facial expressions of emotion should be particularly effective, naturalistic stimuli in an investigation of how and to what extent potential threats influence visual attention. EXPERIMENT 1 Method Participants A total of 25 college undergraduates with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated. Procedure Participants were told that we were interested in the eye movements that people make when looking at faces. They were asked to view a series of images depicting people s faces and to look at the images as they normally would. Thus the task was a passive viewing task with no explicit search instructions. A series of 64 images was shown to the participant. Each image was displayed for four seconds and was then replaced with a central fixation point. Participants had to maintain fixation on the central fixation point in order to begin the next trial. Each image consisted of four Ekman faces that appeared for four seconds at the corners of an imaginary square ( of visual angle from nose to nose) centred on fixation (Figure 1). The Ekman images were cropped into ovals (78 high 58 wide) depicting only the face. In each display, three of the faces were neutral, and the fourth depicted either happiness or fear. In half of the displays, all four images appeared in an upright orientation; in the remaining half, all four images appeared upside down in order to provide an experimental control with visual properties nearly equivalent to those of the corresponding experimental stimulus. The 64 images represented the complete counterbalancing of four Ekman models each displaying one of two emotions at each of four locations in each of two orientations. Eye-tracking procedures Participants were fitted with an EyeLinkII videobased eye tracker. The tracker sampled eye position at 500 Hz and recorded participants gaze along the x, y pixel coordinates of the screen. Prior to beginning the experimental trials, the eye tracker was individually calibrated by having the participant fixate a series of small bullseyes that appeared in nine positions distributed throughout the screen. The eye tracker was adjusted and recalibrated until the maximum tracking error across the nine positions was less than 0.4 degrees of visual angle. Between each trial participants had to fixate a central fixation point in order to verify that the calibration was still accurate and correct for any drift across trials. This ensured that the eye tracker s accuracy was maintained throughout the experiment. Results and discussion To evaluate whether overt attention was rapidly diverted to the emotional faces, we evaluated two dependent variables: the ordinal fixation number of the first fixation on the emotional face and the distance of the scan path made by the eye before it reached the emotional face. The ordinal fixation number of the first fixation was obtained by THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 62 (7) 1259

5 BECKER AND DETWEILER-BEDELL Figure 1. Example of the four types of displays used in Experiment 1. Left panels show a happy condition and its inverted control. Right panels show a fear condition and inverted control. counting the number of fixations made within a scene prior to first fixating on the emotional face. This indicates how many other locations received overt attentional deployments before the emotional face received attention. The scan path distance was derived by calculating the distance between each fixation and summing the distance travelled by the eye before its first fixation on the emotional face. This measured whether the eye traced a more or less direct path to the emotional face. Both are measures of the attentional priority given to the emotional face relative to the display s neutral faces. Two 2 2 repeated measure analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with two levels of facial emotion (fearful, happy) and two levels of display orientation (upright, inverted) were conducted, one with ordinal fixation number and one with scan path distance as the dependent variable. Rather than finding evidence for a rapid diversion of attention towards threat, both measures suggest that participants actively avoided an upright fearful face (Figure 2, top panels). The critical emotion by orientation interaction was significant for ordinal fixation number, F(1, 24) ¼ 5.82, p ¼.024, and for scan path distance, F(1, 24) ¼ 6.66, p ¼.016. These effects were driven by the fact that the participants avoided looking at the upright fearful face. A planned contrast comparing the upright fearful face to a combination of the three other conditions ordinal fixation number, F(1, 24) ¼ 11.36, p ¼.003; scan path distance, F(1, 24) 1260 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 62 (7)

6 AVOIDANCE OF THREAT Figure 2. Top panels plot the data from Experiment 1, and bottom panels plot the data from Experiment 2. The leftward panels (A1 and A2) plot the mean ordinal fixation number of the first fixation on the emotional faces as function of facial expression and orientation. The middle panels (B1 and B2) plot the total distance the eye scanned prior to first fixating on the emotion faces. Error bars for Panels A and B are withinsubject error bars (Loftus & Masson, 1994). The rightward panels plot the percentage of first saccades, post stimulus onset, that ended on the emotional face for each condition. The dashed line is the chance probability of looking at a given face first. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. ¼ 8.16, p ¼.009 or to only the upright happy face ordinal fixation number, F(1, 24) ¼ 10.91, p ¼.003; scan path distance F(1, 24) ¼ 7.18, p ¼.013 produced significant differences for both dependent measures. This pattern of results suggests that participants actively avoided the upright fearful face, leading to both later overt shifts of attention to the fearful face and longer scan paths to the face. Importantly, we found clear evidence that the negative faces were evaluated rapidly. The bias to avoid a fearful face occurred as early as the first eye movement post stimulus onset (Figure 2). Given that there were four faces in the display, the chance odds of making the saccade to a particular face were.25. Single sample t tests show that the observed probability of making the first saccade to an upright happy face, t(24) ¼ 0.89, p ¼.380, inverted happy face, t(24) ¼ 0.53, p ¼.603, and inverted fearful face, t(24) ¼ 21.92, p ¼.067, were within error of this chance value. However, the odds of making the first saccade to the upright fearful face was significantly below chance, t(24) ¼ 3.73, p ¼.001, suggesting that the bias to avoid the fearful face began even before the first eye movement was made away from the central fixation point (e.g., within the first 300 ms of the display). In summary, Experiment 1 suggests that threat is evaluated rapidly and results in active avoidance of, rather than overt attention towards, a fearful face. EXPERIMENT 2 Conceivably, proponents of the view that attention is allocated rapidly to potential threats might argue that a fearful face is not the source of threat per se, suggesting that in such a context the threat would be located elsewhere in the display. Under this explanation, the rapid detection of fear should THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 62 (7) 1261

7 BECKER AND DETWEILER-BEDELL result in a rapid shift of attention not toward the fearful face, but away from it toward the object eliciting the fear. To test this explanation in Experiment 2, we replicated the previous experiment while including an additional 32 trials in which a single angry face was paired with three neutral faces (the 32 additional displays completely mirrored the set of angry and happy images). Like fear, anger is associated with threat. Unlike fear, the angry person is the source of threat. Thus, under the alternative explanation of Experiment 1, we should find rapid shifts of attention towards the angry faces but away from the fearful ones. A total of 25 undergraduates, who did not participate in Experiment 1, participated in this second study. Results and discussion Two 3 2 ANOVAs were run, one for each dependent variable (Figure 2, bottom panels). There was a significant main effect of emotion ordinal fixation number, F(2, 48) ¼ 4.50, p ¼.016; scan path distance, F(2, 48) ¼ 5.27, p ¼.009 and a significant emotion by orientation interaction ordinal fixation number, F(2, 48) ¼ 3.58, p ¼.035; scan path distance, F(2, 48) ¼ 3.11, p ¼.054. The main effect of orientation was not significant ordinal fixation number, F(1, 24) ¼ 2.99, p ¼.096; scan path distance, F(1, 24), 1. To further investigate how these effects related to the different emotional conditions, we first eliminated the angry condition and replicated the analyses that were performed for Experiment 1. These analyses confirmed the finding that participants actively avoided the upright fearful face. For both the ordinal fixation number data and the scan path distance data, there was a significant emotion by orientation interaction ordinal, F(1, 24) ¼ 5.97, p ¼.022; scan path, F(1, 24) ¼ 5.92, p ¼.023. A planned contrast comparing the upright fearful face to the three other conditions was significant ordinal, F(1, 24) ¼ 8.88, p ¼.007; scan path, F(1, 24) ¼ 8.27, p ¼.008 and the upright fearful face was fixated less directly than the upright happy face ordinal, F(1, 24) ¼ 7.01, p ¼.014; scan path, F(1, 24) ¼ 8.17, p ¼.009. This pattern of results replicated those of Experiment 1, clearly demonstrating that people actively avoid looking at a fearful face. Next we examined whether attention would be allocated rapidly to an angry face a face that both indicates and is the source of potential threat. Instead of finding evidence that people rapidly shifted overt attention towards the angry face, we once again found that participants avoided the angry faces, regardless of whether they appeared upright or inverted. A 2 2 ANOVA comparing the happy and angry conditions found a main effect of emotion ordinal, F(1, 24) ¼ 9.91, p ¼.004; scan path, F(1, 24) ¼ 8.57, p ¼.007 but no main effect of orientation (both Fs, 1) nor an emotion by orientation interaction ordinal, F(1, 24) ¼ , p ¼.235; scan path, F(1, 24) ¼ 3.467, p ¼.075. Planned comparisons (two-tailed paired t tests) between the upright angry condition and the upright happy condition revealed that the angry face was fixated later than the happy face in terms of both ordinal fixation number, t(24) ¼ 3.179, p ¼.004, and scan path distance, t(24) ¼ 4.08, p,.001. The upright angry condition did not differ significantly from the upright fearful face on either measure (both p..27). These data suggest that participants treat an upright angry face very similarly to an upright fearful face. That is, they avoid making overt shifts of attention to it. Participants also seemed to avoid the inverted angry face. The inverted angry face was looked at later than the upright happy face, t(24) ¼ 2.52, p ¼.038, for ordinal fixation number; t(24) ¼ 2.25, p ¼.034, for scan path distance, but did not differ from the upright angry or upright fearful conditions for either dependent measure (all ps..13). The finding that people actively avoid both the upright and the inverted angry faces may suggest that these faces were avoided based on their lowlevel visual salience, rather than their emotional content. Although we cannot rule out this saliency-based explanation, we think it is unlikely. For this explanation to hold, one must assume that the angry face was less salient than the three neutral faces in the display. Given that saliency is determined by how disparate an object s visual 1262 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 62 (7)

8 AVOIDANCE OF THREAT features are relative to the other features in a display, it seems unlikely that the face expressing a unique emotion would be the least salient. Instead, we favour the interpretation that the angry face is avoided because it continues to be associated with threat, even when inverted. Regardless of how one interprets the inverted angry condition, our data are quite clear that an upright angry face does not attract early attention. Similar to Experiment 1, the active avoidance developed fairly rapidly. People made fewer initial eye movements towards the upright fearful, t(24) ¼ 2.70, p ¼.013, upright angry, t(24) ¼ 2.76, p ¼.011, and inverted angry faces, t(24) ¼ 2.02, p ¼.054, than one would have expected by chance (Figure 2). Taken together, the results from Experiments 1 and 2 suggest that potential threat (either fearful face or angry faces) is evaluated rapidly but that overt attention actively avoids the threatening stimuli rather than being drawn to them. SUMMARY In two experiments we tracked eye movements while participants passively viewed displays consisting of one emotional face among three neutral faces in order to test the theory that fearful and angry facial expressions are evaluated rapidly and should cause a rapid shift of attention toward such stimuli. Although the passive viewing task should have been very sensitive to default attentional biases, we found no evidence that overt attention was biased toward fearful or angry faces. Instead, people tended to actively avoid looking at these faces. This avoidance was evident very early in scene viewing (i.e., within 300 ms of display onset), suggesting that the threat associated with the faces was evaluated rapidly while the faces were still in the periphery. Thus, our main findings are that negative facial expressions are rapidly analysed, but rather than attract attention, they are actively avoided. Importantly, in our experiment, participants had no primary task, and thus our findings cannot be explained based on task-related attentional sets biasing attention away from threat or the primary task load impairing the ability of the amygdala to evaluate the threat. Instead, it appears that the threat was evaluated rapidly, but this evaluation led to avoidance rather than attraction. We believe this passive viewing task is a very sensitive test of the theory that there is a persistent, underlying bias to attend toward threatening stimuli. One could imagine that such a bias exists, but will not manifest itself under situations in which short-term goals supersede this underlying bias. However, in our passive viewing task, there were no short-term goals to compete with such bias, and it therefore should have manifested itself, if it existed. Our failure to find such a bias is strong evidence against the view that there is an underlying bias to orient towards threat in a rapid and reflexive manner. Instead, our results are consistent with a growing body of literature that fails to find a default tendency for attention to be preferential allocated towards threat in typical participants (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007), and it is consistent with an emerging view that the bias among typical participants may, in fact, be away from threatening stimuli towards positive stimuli (Frewen, Dozois, Joanisse, & Neufeld, 2008). REFERENCES Original manuscript received 6 November 2008 Accepted revision received 8 December 2008 First published online 9 March 2009 Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans- Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, Becker, M. W., & Pashler, H. (2005). Awareness of the continuously visible: Information acquisition during preview. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, Bishop, S. J., Jenkins, R., & Lawrence, A. D. (2007). Neural processing of fearful faces: Effects of anxiety THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 62 (7) 1263

9 BECKER AND DETWEILER-BEDELL are gated by perceptual capacity limitations. Cerebral Cortex, 17, Caseras, X., Garner, M., Bradley, B. P., & Mogg, K. (2007). Biases in visual orienting to negative and positive scenes in dysphoria: An eye movement study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, Cowey, A. (2004). The 30th Sir Frederick Bartlett lecture: Fact, artefact, and myth about blindsight. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., Pourtois, G., & Weiskrantz, L. (1999). Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. Neuroreport, 10, Ekman, P. (1973). Darwin and facial expression: A century of research in review. Oxford, UK: Academic Press. Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: The role of anxiety and awareness. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J. A., Joanisse, M. F., & Neufeld, R. W. J. (2008). Selective attention to threat versus reward: Meta-analysis and neuralnetwork modeling of the dot-probe task. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, Holmes, A., Vuilleumier, P., & Eimer, M. (2003). The processing of emotional facial expression is gated by spatial attention: Evidence from eventrelated brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion, 4, Koster, E. H. W., Verschuere, B., Burssens, B., Custers, R., & Crombez, G. (2007). Attention for emotional faces under restricted awareness revisited: Do emotional faces automatically attract attention? Emotion, 7, LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, Lundqvist, D., & Öhman, A. (2005). Emotion regulates attention: The relation between facial configurations, facial emotion, and visual attention. Visual Cognition, 12, Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitivemotivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, Palermo, R., & Rhodes, G. (2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia, 45, Pessoa, L. (2005). To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, Sergerie, K., Chochol, C., & Armony, J. L. (2008). The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, Stormark, K. M., Hugdahl, K., & Posner, M. I. (1999). Emotional modulation of attention orienting: A classical conditioning study. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, Vuilleumier, P., & Pourtois, G. (2007). Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: Evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia, 45, THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2009, 62 (7)

Does Imminent Threat Capture and Hold Attention?

Does Imminent Threat Capture and Hold Attention? Emotion Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 4, No. 3, 312 317 1528-3542/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.3.312 Does Imminent Threat Capture and Hold Attention? Ernst H.

More information

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut This article was downloaded by: [Boston College] On: 08 November 2012, At: 09:04 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (1), 159-165 Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions SOWON HAHN AND SCOTT D. GRONLUND University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma Using a visual search

More information

Lora-Jean Collett a & David Lester a a Department of Psychology, Wellesley College and

Lora-Jean Collett a & David Lester a a Department of Psychology, Wellesley College and This article was downloaded by: [122.34.214.87] On: 10 February 2013, At: 16:46 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA This article was downloaded by: [Hicks, Joshua A.][Texas A&M University] On: 11 August 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 915031380] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered

More information

Attention for Emotional Faces Under Restricted Awareness Revisited: Do Emotional Faces Automatically Attract Attention?

Attention for Emotional Faces Under Restricted Awareness Revisited: Do Emotional Faces Automatically Attract Attention? Emotion Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 7, No. 2, 285 295 1528-3542/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.285 Attention for Emotional Faces Under Restricted Awareness

More information

Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo

Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo This article was downloaded by: [Costanza Scaffidi Abbate] On: 29 July 2013, At: 06:31 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Online publication date: 08 June 2010

Online publication date: 08 June 2010 This article was downloaded by: [Vrije Universiteit, Library] On: 1 June 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 907218003] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Visual Awareness and the Detection of Fearful Faces

Visual Awareness and the Detection of Fearful Faces Emotion Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 5, No. 2, 243 247 1528-3542/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.243 Visual Awareness and the Detection of Fearful Faces Luiz

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[university of Virginia] On: 26 November 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 785020474] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods

Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods Animal Ecology Publications Animal Ecology 1996 Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods Clay L. Pierce National Biological Service, cpierce@iastate.edu

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Advanced Projects R&D, New Zealand b Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Online publication date: 30 March 2011

Advanced Projects R&D, New Zealand b Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Online publication date: 30 March 2011 This article was downloaded by: [University of Canterbury Library] On: 4 April 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 917001820] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in

More information

Emotion. Enhanced Attentional Capture in Trait Anxiety

Emotion. Enhanced Attentional Capture in Trait Anxiety Emotion Enhanced Attentional Capture in Trait Anxiety Jason S. Moser, Mark W. Becker, and Tim P. Moran Online First Publication, November 7, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0026156 CITATION Moser, J. S., Becker, M.

More information

To link to this article:

To link to this article: This article was downloaded by: [University of Notre Dame] On: 12 February 2015, At: 14:40 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

VISUAL ATTENTION MECHANISMS UNDERLYING THE EMOTIONAL DOT PROBE TASK. Melville M. Malone. Thesis. Submitted to the faculty of the

VISUAL ATTENTION MECHANISMS UNDERLYING THE EMOTIONAL DOT PROBE TASK. Melville M. Malone. Thesis. Submitted to the faculty of the VISUAL ATTENTION MECHANISMS UNDERLYING THE EMOTIONAL DOT PROBE TASK By Melville M. Malone Thesis Submitted to the faculty of the Graduate School of Vanderbilt University in partial fulfillment of requirements

More information

Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014.

Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014. This article was downloaded by: [Dimitrios Pnevmatikos] On: 14 November 2014, At: 22:15 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

To link to this article:

To link to this article: This article was downloaded by: [University of Kiel] On: 24 October 2014, At: 17:27 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 580 584 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027885 OBSERVATION Templates

More information

Fear Selectively Modulates Visual Mental Imagery and Visual Perception

Fear Selectively Modulates Visual Mental Imagery and Visual Perception Fear Selectively Modulates Visual Mental Imagery and Visual Perception The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation

More information

The Fearful-Face Advantage Is Modulated by Task Demands: Evidence From the Attentional Blink

The Fearful-Face Advantage Is Modulated by Task Demands: Evidence From the Attentional Blink Emotion 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 10, No. 1, 136 140 1528-3542/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0017814 BRIEF REPORTS The Fearful-Face Advantage Is Modulated by Task Demands: Evidence From

More information

Derakshan, N., & Koster, E.H.W. (2010). Processing efficiency in visual search in

Derakshan, N., & Koster, E.H.W. (2010). Processing efficiency in visual search in 1 Derakshan, N., & Koster, E.H.W. (2010). Processing efficiency in visual search in anxiety: An eye-movement registration study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 1180-1185. Processing efficiency in

More information

Cognitive Enhancement Using 19-Electrode Z-Score Neurofeedback

Cognitive Enhancement Using 19-Electrode Z-Score Neurofeedback This article was downloaded by: [Lucas Koberda] On: 22 August 2012, At: 09:31 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington This article was downloaded by: [ ] On: 30 June 2011, At: 09:44 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: This article was downloaded by: [Chiara, Andrea Di] On: 30 December 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 931692396] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [CDL Journals Account] On: 24 September 2008 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 786945862] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

Anne A. Lawrence M.D. PhD a a Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada Published online: 11 Jan 2010.

Anne A. Lawrence M.D. PhD a a Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada Published online: 11 Jan 2010. This article was downloaded by: [University of California, San Francisco] On: 05 May 2015, At: 22:37 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered

More information

The Primacy of Negative Interpretations When Resolving the Valence of Ambiguous Facial Expressions

The Primacy of Negative Interpretations When Resolving the Valence of Ambiguous Facial Expressions Research Report The Primacy of Negative Interpretations When Resolving the Valence of Ambiguous Facial Expressions Psychological Science 21(7) 901 907 The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav

More information

Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces

Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces Neuroscience Letters xxx (2005) xxx xxx Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces Amanda Holmes a,, Monika Kiss b, Martin Eimer b a School of Human and Life Sciences,

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

Electrocortical evidence for rapid allocation of attention to threat in the dot-probe task

Electrocortical evidence for rapid allocation of attention to threat in the dot-probe task doi:10.1093/scan/nsu098 SCAN (2015) 10, 577^583 Electrocortical evidence for rapid allocation of attention to threat in the dot-probe task Emily S. Kappenman, 1 Annmarie MacNamara, 2 and Greg Hajcak Proudfit

More information

Trait Anxiety and Perceptual Load as Determinants of Emotion Processing in a Fear Conditioning Paradigm

Trait Anxiety and Perceptual Load as Determinants of Emotion Processing in a Fear Conditioning Paradigm Emotion 2011 American Psychological Association 2012, Vol. 12, No. 2, 236 249 1528-3542/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0025321 Trait Anxiety and Perceptual Load as Determinants of Emotion Processing in a Fear

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Visual attention to emotion in depression: Facilitation and withdrawal processes

Visual attention to emotion in depression: Facilitation and withdrawal processes COGNITION AND EMOTION 2012, 26 (4), 602614 Visual attention to emotion in depression: Facilitation and withdrawal processes Blair E. Wisco 1, Teresa A. Treat 2, and Andrew Hollingworth 2 1 Department of

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction

More information

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and 77 5 Discussion The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and implicit learning systems. Chapter 2 described the effects of performing a working memory task

More information

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making Jan M. Wiener (jwiener@bournemouth.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University Poole, BH12 5BB, UK Olivier De

More information

Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures

Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2001, 54A (3), 665 681 Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures Jenny Yiend and Andrew Mathews MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK

More information

Show Me the Features: Regular Viewing Patterns. During Encoding and Recognition of Faces, Objects, and Places. Makiko Fujimoto

Show Me the Features: Regular Viewing Patterns. During Encoding and Recognition of Faces, Objects, and Places. Makiko Fujimoto Show Me the Features: Regular Viewing Patterns During Encoding and Recognition of Faces, Objects, and Places. Makiko Fujimoto Honors Thesis, Symbolic Systems 2014 I certify that this honors thesis is in

More information

Fear detection and visual awareness in perceiving bodily expressions Stienen, B.M.C.; de Gelder, Bea

Fear detection and visual awareness in perceiving bodily expressions Stienen, B.M.C.; de Gelder, Bea Tilburg University Fear detection and visual awareness in perceiving bodily expressions Stienen, B.M.C.; de Gelder, Bea Published in: Emotion Document version: Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of

More information

Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal

Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal BBS-D-15-00893_ Mather_ Montagrin & Sander Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal Alison Montagrin 1,2,3 * & David Sander 1,2 1 Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, 2 Department of Psychology

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

Task relevance of emotional information affects anxiety linked attention bias in visual search

Task relevance of emotional information affects anxiety linked attention bias in visual search Task relevance of emotional information affects anxiety linked attention bias in visual search Article Accepted Version Dodd, H. F., Vogt, J., Turkileri, N. and Notebaert, L. (2017) Task relevance of emotional

More information

TIME COURSE OF ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN ANXIETY: MEASURING EYE GAZE FOR ANGRY FACES IN WOMEN AND MEN. A Dissertation MILAGROS EVARDONE

TIME COURSE OF ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN ANXIETY: MEASURING EYE GAZE FOR ANGRY FACES IN WOMEN AND MEN. A Dissertation MILAGROS EVARDONE TIME COURSE OF ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN ANXIETY: MEASURING EYE GAZE FOR ANGRY FACES IN WOMEN AND MEN A Dissertation by MILAGROS EVARDONE Submitted to the Office of Graduate Studies of Texas A&M University in

More information

Taking control of reflexive social attention

Taking control of reflexive social attention Cognition 94 (2005) B55 B65 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Taking control of reflexive social attention Jelena Ristic*, Alan Kingstone Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia,

More information

Signals of threat do not capture, but prioritize attention: a classical conditioning. approach

Signals of threat do not capture, but prioritize attention: a classical conditioning. approach 1 Signals of threat do not capture, but prioritize attention: a classical conditioning approach Lies Notebaert, Geert Crombez, Stefaan Van Damme, and Jan De Houwer, Ghent University, Belgium Jan Theeuwes

More information

Wild Minds What Animals Really Think : A Museum Exhibit at the New York Hall of Science, December 2011

Wild Minds What Animals Really Think : A Museum Exhibit at the New York Hall of Science, December 2011 This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 10:40 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Focusing on fear: Attentional disengagement from emotional faces

Focusing on fear: Attentional disengagement from emotional faces Europe PMC Funders Group Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Vis cogn. 2005 ; 12(1): 145 158. doi:10.1080/13506280444000076. Focusing on fear: Attentional disengagement from emotional

More information

Fear of faces: a psychophysiological investigation of facial affect processing in social phobia

Fear of faces: a psychophysiological investigation of facial affect processing in social phobia University of Wollongong Research Online University of Wollongong Thesis Collection 1954-2016 University of Wollongong Thesis Collections 2004 Fear of faces: a psychophysiological investigation of facial

More information

Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies

Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies Supplemental Material for Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies Timo Stein, Kiley Seymour, Martin N. Hebart, and Philipp Sterzer Additional experimental details Participants Volunteers

More information

The Time Course of Attentional Disengagement from Angry Faces in. Social Anxiety

The Time Course of Attentional Disengagement from Angry Faces in. Social Anxiety Time Course of Attentional Disengagement 1 Running head: ATTENTIONAL DISENGAGEMENT IN SOCIAL ANXIETY The Time Course of Attentional Disengagement from Angry Faces in Social Anxiety Jun Moriya 1,2 and Yoshihiko

More information

A Tale of Two Tasks: Comparing Steady-state Visually Evoked Potentials with the Dot Probe as Measures of the Anxiety-Related Threat Bias

A Tale of Two Tasks: Comparing Steady-state Visually Evoked Potentials with the Dot Probe as Measures of the Anxiety-Related Threat Bias City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works School of Arts & Sciences Theses Hunter College Summer 8-10-2017 A Tale of Two Tasks: Comparing Steady-state Visually Evoked Potentials with the Dot

More information

Visual search for schematic emotional faces: angry faces are more than crosses. Daina S.E. Dickins & Ottmar V. Lipp

Visual search for schematic emotional faces: angry faces are more than crosses. Daina S.E. Dickins & Ottmar V. Lipp 1 : angry faces are more than crosses Daina S.E. Dickins & Ottmar V. Lipp School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, QLD, 4072, Australia Running head: Search for schematic emotional faces Address

More information

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The number line effect reflects top-down control Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),862-868???-??? The number line effect reflects top-down control JELENA RISTIC University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: This article was downloaded by: [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] On: 24 November 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 923160581] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in

More information

The Effects of Social Anxiety and State Anxiety on Visual Attention: Testing the Vigilance- Avoidance Hypothesis. J.

The Effects of Social Anxiety and State Anxiety on Visual Attention: Testing the Vigilance- Avoidance Hypothesis. J. Running Head: VISUAL ATTENTION AND SOCIAL ANXIETY The Effects of Social Anxiety and State Anxiety on Visual Attention: Testing the Vigilance- Avoidance Hypothesis J. Suzanne Singh 1 Michelle C. Capozzoli

More information

Motivation Versus Aversive Processing During Perception

Motivation Versus Aversive Processing During Perception Emotion Motivation Versus Aversive Processing During Perception Srikanth Padmala and Luiz Pessoa Online First Publication, April 7, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036112 CITATION Padmala, S., & Pessoa,

More information

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object Supplemental materials for: The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object David Whitney*, David A. Westwood, & Melvyn A. Goodale* *Group on Action and Perception, The

More information

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Infant and Child Development Inf. Child Dev. 10: 39 45 (2001) DOI: 10.1002/icd.245 Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Olivier Pascalis a, *, Elisabeth

More information

Richard Lakeman a a School of Health & Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Lismore, Australia. Published online: 02 Sep 2013.

Richard Lakeman a a School of Health & Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Lismore, Australia. Published online: 02 Sep 2013. This article was downloaded by: [UQ Library] On: 09 September 2013, At: 21:23 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

1 Visual search for emotional expressions: Effect of stimulus set on anger and happiness. superiority

1 Visual search for emotional expressions: Effect of stimulus set on anger and happiness. superiority 1 Visual search for emotional expressions: Effect of stimulus set on anger and happiness superiority Ruth A. Savage 1, Stefanie I. Becker 1 & Ottmar V. Lipp 2 1 School of Psychology, University of Queensland,

More information

Published online: 17 Feb 2011.

Published online: 17 Feb 2011. This article was downloaded by: [Iowa State University] On: 23 April 2015, At: 08:45 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[university of Virginia] On: 26 November 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 785020474] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Differences in holistic processing do not explain cultural differences in the recognition of facial expression

Differences in holistic processing do not explain cultural differences in the recognition of facial expression THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2017 VOL. 70, NO. 12, 2445 2459 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1240816 Differences in holistic processing do not explain cultural differences

More information

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a This article was downloaded by: [University of Minnesota] On: 16 August 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 917397643] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

NOT FOR DISTRIBUTION. Integrating Behavioral and Electrocortical Measures of Attentional Bias Toward Threat

NOT FOR DISTRIBUTION. Integrating Behavioral and Electrocortical Measures of Attentional Bias Toward Threat 11 INTRODUCTION Integrating Behavioral and Electrocortical Measures of Attentional Bias Toward Threat Annmarie MacNamara, Emily S. Kappenman, Sarah R. Black, Jennifer N. Bress, and Greg Hajcak The term

More information

Differential Viewing Strategies towards Attractive and Unattractive Human Faces

Differential Viewing Strategies towards Attractive and Unattractive Human Faces Differential Viewing Strategies towards Attractive and Unattractive Human Faces Ivan Getov igetov@clemson.edu Greg Gettings ggettin@clemson.edu A.J. Villanueva aaronjv@clemson.edu Chris Belcher cbelche@clemson.edu

More information

No prior entry for threat-related faces: Evidence from temporal order judgments

No prior entry for threat-related faces: Evidence from temporal order judgments No prior entry for threat-related faces 1 1 Running head: NO PRIOR ENTRY FOR THREAT-RELATED FACES 2 3 4 No prior entry for threat-related faces: Evidence from temporal order judgments 5 6 Antonio Schettino

More information

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related?

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related? Shackman, Afterword Q2 1 Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related? Alexander J. Shackman Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, and Maryland Neuroimaging

More information

Gaze Patterns When Looking at Emotional Pictures: Motivationally Biased Attention 1

Gaze Patterns When Looking at Emotional Pictures: Motivationally Biased Attention 1 Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 28, No. 3, September 2004 ( C 2004) Gaze Patterns When Looking at Emotional Pictures: Motivationally Biased Attention 1 Manuel G. Calvo 2,4 and Peter J. Lang 3 Pictures of

More information

The Color of Similarity

The Color of Similarity The Color of Similarity Brooke O. Breaux (bfo1493@louisiana.edu) Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, LA 70504 USA Michele I. Feist (feist@louisiana.edu) Institute

More information

Emotional Priming of Pop-Out in Visual Search

Emotional Priming of Pop-Out in Visual Search Emotion Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 2008, Vol. 8, No. 2, 151 161 1528-3542/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.151 Emotional Priming of Pop-Out in Visual Search Dominique

More information

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2009, 16 (1), 57-61 doi:10.3758/pbr.16.1.57 The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words DANIEL SMILEK, GRAYDEN J. F. SOLMAN, PETER MURAWSKI, AND

More information

Jennifer E. Corbett a, Nicole Wurnitsch b, Alex Schwartz c & David Whitney b a Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento,

Jennifer E. Corbett a, Nicole Wurnitsch b, Alex Schwartz c & David Whitney b a Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, This article was downloaded by: [University of California, Berkeley] On: 21 June 2012, At: 17:00 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered

More information

BRIEF REPORT. December 18, 2009

BRIEF REPORT. December 18, 2009 Emotion-induced AB and social anxiety 1 Running Head: Emotion-induced AB and social anxiety De Jong, P.J., Koster, E.H.W., van Wees, R., & Martens, A.M. (2010). Angry facial expressions hamper subsequent

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: This article was downloaded by: [University of Cardiff] On: 3 March 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 906511392] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report Research Report CHANGING FACES: A Detection Advantage in the Flicker Paradigm Tony Ro, 1 Charlotte Russell, 2 and Nilli Lavie 2 1 Rice University and 2 University College London, London, United Kingdom

More information

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search VISUAL COGNITION, 0000, 00 (0), 111 Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search Walter R. Boot Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee,

More information

Exploring the Function of Selective Attention and Hypervigilance for Threat in Anxiety

Exploring the Function of Selective Attention and Hypervigilance for Threat in Anxiety Accepted Manuscript Exploring the Function of Selective Attention and Hypervigilance for Threat in Anxiety Helen J. Richards, Valerie Benson, Nick Donnelly, Julie A. Hadwin PII: S0272-7358(13)00136-0 DOI:

More information

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining 8 the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining items was included. Participants were asked to decide whether a different item was included in the array,

More information

A negative compatibility effect in priming of emotional faces

A negative compatibility effect in priming of emotional faces Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 908-912 A negative compatibility effect in priming of emotional faces JENNIFER D. BENNETT University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

This is a repository copy of Differences in holistic processing do not explain cultural differences in the recognition of facial expression.

This is a repository copy of Differences in holistic processing do not explain cultural differences in the recognition of facial expression. This is a repository copy of Differences in holistic processing do not explain cultural differences in the recognition of facial expression. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/107341/

More information

Social Development Vol 00 No Month 2016 doi: /sode.12184

Social Development Vol 00 No Month 2016 doi: /sode.12184 Social Development Vol 00 No. 00 00 00 Month 2016 doi: 10.1111/sode.12184 Biases in Attention for Social Stimuli in Children are Associated with Patterns of Infant Attachment: A Brief Report Paul Meinz,

More information

First published on: 16 December 2009

First published on: 16 December 2009 This article was downloaded by: [Tel Aviv University] On: 19 February 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 908121066] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and

More information

Final Exam PS 217, Fall 2010

Final Exam PS 217, Fall 2010 Final Exam PS 27, Fall 200. Farzin, et al. (200) wrote an article that appeared in Psychological Science titled: Spatial resolution of conscious visual perception in infants. The abstract read: Humans

More information

The effect of cognitive load in emotional attention and trait anxiety: An eyemovement

The effect of cognitive load in emotional attention and trait anxiety: An eyemovement 1 The effect of cognitive load in emotional attention and trait anxiety: An eyemovement study. Nick Berggren 1, Ernst H.W. Koster 2 & Nazanin Derakshan 1 1 Department of Psychological Science, Birkbeck

More information

2/19/2011. Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois University February 18, 2011

2/19/2011. Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois University February 18, 2011 Lifetime prevalence estimates for PTSD are about 8%; however, the majority of Americans experience a traumatic event in their lifetimes (Kessler et al, 1995: NCS) Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois

More information

Why do we look at people's eyes?

Why do we look at people's eyes? Journal of Eye Movement Research 1(1):1, 1-6 Why do we look at people's eyes? Elina Birmingham University of British Columbia Walter Bischof University of Alberta Alan Kingstone University of British Columbia

More information

The synergy of top-down and bottom-up attention in complex task: going beyond saliency models.

The synergy of top-down and bottom-up attention in complex task: going beyond saliency models. The synergy of top-down and bottom-up attention in complex task: going beyond saliency models. Enkhbold Nyamsuren (e.nyamsuren@rug.nl) Niels A. Taatgen (n.a.taatgen@rug.nl) Department of Artificial Intelligence,

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: This article was downloaded by: [University of Regensburg] On: 20 April 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 917724617] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1. Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in

HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1. Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1 Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in Heterogeneous Backgrounds Belinda M. Craig, Stefanie I. Becker, &

More information

Emotion Improves and Impairs Early Vision Bruno R. Bocanegra and René Zeelenberg

Emotion Improves and Impairs Early Vision Bruno R. Bocanegra and René Zeelenberg PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Emotion Improves and Impairs Early Vision Bruno R. Bocanegra and René Zeelenberg Erasmus University Rotterdam ABSTRACT Recent studies indicate that emotion enhances

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Developmental Differences in Infants Attention to Social and Nonsocial Threats

Developmental Differences in Infants Attention to Social and Nonsocial Threats THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS OF INFANT STUDIES Infancy, 1 13, 2016 Copyright International Congress of Infant Studies (ICIS) ISSN: 1525-0008 print / 1532-7078 online DOI: 10.1111/infa.12167

More information

The roles of encoding, retrieval, and awareness. in change detection.

The roles of encoding, retrieval, and awareness. in change detection. Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (4), 610-620 The roles of encoding, retrieval, and awareness in change detection MELISSA R. BECK AND MATTHEW S. PETERSON George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia AND BONNIE

More information

Attentional Selection Is Biased Toward Mood-Congruent Stimuli

Attentional Selection Is Biased Toward Mood-Congruent Stimuli Emotion Attentional Selection Is Biased Toward Mood-Congruent Stimuli Mark W. Becker, and Mallorie Leinenger Online First Publication, May 23, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0023524 CITATION Becker, M. W., & Leinenger,

More information