CULTURE AND MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF POWER: IMPLICATIONS FOR CONSUMERS INFORMATION-PROCESSING STRATEGIES, JUDGMENTS, AND INFLUENCE ATTEMPTS

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "CULTURE AND MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF POWER: IMPLICATIONS FOR CONSUMERS INFORMATION-PROCESSING STRATEGIES, JUDGMENTS, AND INFLUENCE ATTEMPTS"

Transcription

1 CULTURE AND MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF POWER: IMPLICATIONS FOR CONSUMERS INFORMATION-PROCESSING STRATEGIES, JUDGMENTS, AND INFLUENCE ATTEMPTS BY CARLOS J. TORELLI THESIS PROPOSAL Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Business Administration in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2006 Urbana, Illinois

2 2 ABSTRACT This research examines the link between culture and consumer phenomena by focusing on the relationship between cultural orientation and mental representations of power. We distinguish among cultures that foster associations of power with statusenhancing concerns, those that encourage associations of power with concerns for the welfare of others, and those that do not emphasize the use of power as a theme for organizing social information. The research builds upon the relatively new distinction in the study of culture between horizontal (valuing equality) and vertical (emphasizing status and power) cultural orientations. We analyze implications of this distinction in view of research about the effects of power on goal activation and motivated impression formation. Integrating these approaches, we develop a framework for linking cultural orientation with distinct mental representations of power. This helps us to predict the power-related goals that individuals with different cultural orientations activate in a given context. In turn, these goals determine individuals information-processing strategies when evaluating product information, and their subsequent judgments and actions. We develop an empirical plan to collect data for the proposed framework and discuss the implications to the broader domains of consumer behavior.

3 3 TABLE OF CONTENTS 1. INTRODUCTION 6 2. THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK: 10 CULTURE AS AN ANTECEDENT OF PEOPLE S MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF POWER 2.1 What is Power? Two Types of Power Power and Culture Mental Representations of Power across Cultures Power and Individualism Power and Collectivism CONSEQUENCES OF ACTIVATING DISTINCT TYPES OF 31 MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF POWER FOR CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 3.1 Mechanisms for Priming Power and Goal Activation Consequences of the Activation of Power-Related Goals in 35 Consumer Settings Consequences for Consumers Information-Processing 37 Strategies Consequences for Consumers Behavioral Intentions 41 and Influence Attempts Consequences for Product Evaluations Summary of Theoretical Framework and Hypotheses METHODOLOGY Overview Antecedents of Power: Examining the Culture Power link Experiment 1a: Examining the Culture Power Link 53 Using Perceptions of Powerful Others Experiment 1b. Replication Using Membership in 58 Cultural Groups Experiment 1c: Examining the Culture Power Link 60 Measuring Own Power-Related Beliefs Experiment 1d: Examining the Culture Power Link 67 Using Multiple Measures 4.3 Priming Mechanisms and Activation of Power-Related 81 Cognitions and Goals Experiment 2a: Effect of a Power Prime on the Activation 82 of Self-Related Cognitions Experiment 2b: Effect of a Power Prime on the Activation 84 of Power-Related Goals Experiment 2c: Effect of Having Power over In-Groups 86 on the Activation of Power-Related Goals among VC Individuals Experiment 2d: Effect of Status- and Helping-Related Cues 87

4 4 on the Activation of Power-Related Goals 4.4 Consequences of the Activation of Power-Related Goals on 88 Consumer Behavior Experiment 3a: Effect of Status- and Helping-Related 90 Cues on Consumers Information Processing, Evaluations, and Behavioral Intentions Experiment 3b: Effect of Having Power on Consumers 92 Information Processing and Influence Attempts Experiment 3c: Increase in Strength of Behavioral 94 Tendencies Associated with Power-Related Goals 4.5 General Discussion 96 APPENDIX 1. Instrument Used in Experiments 1a and 1b 98 APPENDIX 2. Scales Used in Experiment 1c 102 APPENDIX 3. Instrument Used in Experiment 1d 105 APPENDIX 4. Word-fragment completion task used to prime power 108 APPENDIX Statements Task 109 APPENDIX 6. Alternative power prime situation used in Experiments 2b 110 APPENDIX 7. Product Message Used in Experiments 2b and 2d 111 APPENDIX 8. Power prime situation Involving In-groups used in 112 Experiment 2c APPENDIX 9. Product Message Used in Experiment 2d 113 APPENDIX 10. Product Message Used in Experiment 3a 114 REFERENCES 115

5 5 LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES Figure 3.1 Goal-Means Structures Characterizing Individuals with 32 different Cultural Orientations Figure 3.2 Theoretical Framework 47 Table 4.1 Correlations Experiment 1a 56 Table 4.2 Correlations Experiment 1c 65 Table 4.3 Stimuli and Dependent Variables used in Experiment 1d 71 Table 4.4 Correlations Experiment 1d 76 Figure 4.3 SEM used in Experiment 1d 77 Table 4.5 Correlations between IND COL and Personalized/ 80 Socialized Power

6 6 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Boscolo luxury hotel Exedra. Beauty treatments, sauna, steam baths, solarium and Jacuzzi tubs all know how to embrace, pamper and envelop guests in a breath of fresh energy. We are the Stanford Financial Group, a global network of affiliated companies that together form a powerful resource of financial services. American Family Insurance. For over 75 years, we've made it our business to take care of you. Check recent comparison quotes in Progressive.com and then decide if we are right for you. What do all these messages have in common? Which one is likely to be more persuasive in a given culture? What would it reveal about the link between culture and consumer psychology? We argue in this research for viewing these appeals as invoking motives related to power and for studying the link between power and cultural variables. Power is a fundamental aspect of everyday social life (Cartwright, 1959), a basic force in social relationships (Fiske, 1993), a key determinant of people s strivings (Winter, 1973), and a central element in shaping relationships among members of distribution channels (Gaski, 1984). As central as power is to theoretical inquiries in the social sciences, and especially in business-to-business relationships, it has received very little attention in consumer behavior (Aaker, 2006). In this research, we show that a focus on power, and more specifically on the interplay between power and cultural orientation, leads to novel predictions about consumer psychology phenomena.

7 7 Our understanding of the role of culture in consumer behavior has increased dramatically in the recent years. We know that some cultures are more likely than others to use certain types of ad appeals (Alden, Hoyer, & Lee, 1993; Choi & Miracle, 2004; Han & Shavitt, 1994). Consumers evaluate more favorably culturally matched ad appeals than mismatched appeals (Aaker & Maheswaran, 1997; Agrawal & Maheswaran, 2005; Han & Shavitt, 1994). Cultural orientation also impacts the nature of information processing (Aaker, 2000; Aaker & Maheswaran, 1997; Aaker & Sengupta, 2000) and the types of goals brought to mind (Aaker & Lee, 2001) when consumers evaluate product messages. However, in spite of these significant advances in knowledge, we still need further refinements that can afford a more nuanced understanding of the link between culture and consumer phenomena (Maheswaran & Shavitt, 2000). We propose that such refinements can be obtained by focusing on the relationship between cultural orientation and mental representations of power, and linking to a relatively new distinction in the study of culture between horizontal (valuing equality) and vertical (emphasizing status and power) cultural orientations. By further delineating the broader individualism and collectivism categories into their horizontal and vertical distinctions (Singelis, Triandis, Bhawuk, & Gelfand, 1995; Triandis, 1995; Triandis, Chen, & Chan, 1998; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998), we distinguish among cultures that foster associations of power with status-enhancing concerns, those that encourage associations of power with concerns for the welfare of others, and those that do not emphasize the use of power as a theme for organizing social interactions. We analyze implications from these distinctions in view of recent research about the effects of power on the activation of goals (see Bargh, Raymond, Pryor, &

8 8 Strack, 1995; Chen, Lee-Chai, & Bargh, 2001) and on motivated impression formation (Fiske, 1993; Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003). We integrate the results from this analysis and develop a framework for linking cultural orientation with distinct mental representations of power and for predicting the impact from the activation of these representations on consumers information-processing strategies, judgments, and influence attempts. Intentionally or not, marketers can communicate brand personalities and meanings that can provide power-related cues. For instance, the Boscolo luxury hotel Exedra s message above might communicate to consumers that they hold a higher level of power (relative to average consumers) in a relationship driven by a focus on serving and pampering. In contrast, the advertisement from Stanford Financial Group might endow consumers with a sense of status and superior expertise acknowledged by others. American Family Insurance might be trying to communicate a concern for the enhancement of consumers welfare, whereas Progressive might be conveying a leveled playing-field in order to start an egalitarian interaction with future customers. The framework developed in this research helps us to predict the power-related goals activated by consumers with different cultural orientations upon encountering these messages. This will determine the type of product information individuals attend to (or ignore) and the product judgments they make. In the pages that follow, we first focus on culture as an antecedent of power, and review relevant literature about power and culture to identify the link between mental representations of power and cultural orientation. Next, we focus on the consequences of activating distinct types of mental representations of power on impression formation and

9 9 interpersonal influence processes in the context of consumer-brand relationships. From this review, we develop a theoretical framework to describe the link between culture and mental representations of power. We predict the effects from these relationships on consumers information-processing strategies, judgments, and influence attempts. We develop an empirical plan to collect data to provide evidence for the relation between cultural orientation and distinct mental representations of power. We provide evidence for the effect of activating these representations on consumers attention, memory, product evaluations, and behavioral intentions. Finally, we discuss the implications from our findings to broader domains of consumer behavior and suggest an agenda for future research.

10 10 CHAPTER 2 THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK: CULTURE AS AN ANTECEDENT OF PEOPLE S MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF POWER 2.1 What is Power? As pervasive as power is in human affairs, it is as difficult to define (Lukes, 1986). Definitions of power vary according to the research question (e.g., Is power resisted? or Is it legitimized? ), the level of analysis (e.g., countries, groups, dyads, or individuals), and the variables of interest (e.g., power distribution or sources of power). Some definitions focus on the actor s intentions (e.g., power motive, Winter, 1973), on people s mental associations with power (e.g., goals and concepts associated with power, Bargh et al., 1995), on the asymmetrical control that one party has over another (e.g., disproportionate ability to control others, Fiske, 1993), on the capacity to withhold resources and administer punishments (e.g., Keltner et al., 2003), and on the target s potential resistance in response to the actor (e.g., get the target to do something he/she would not do otherwise, Gaski, 1984). To complicate things even further, power is also frequently used interchangeably with related concepts such as authority, dominance, force, influence, and control. In view of this state of affairs, it is not surprising that a search of articles with the word power in its title and published in the top leading journals in psychology, sociology, political science, and business-to-business marketing yields a total of 740 articles. A focused review of some of the alternative definitions of power indicated above suggests that there are two important elements when conceptualizing social power: capacity and intention. Capacity refers to resources that the individual can deliver (i.e.,

11 11 rewards and punishments) to the target of the power attempt (Keltner et al., 2003) or the ability to control others outcomes (Fiske, 1993). Intention refers to underlying motives, desires, and goals on the side of the actor to elicit a particular effect on the target (McClelland, 1987; Winter, 1973). Both elements are essential for defining power. However, we see these two elements as independent dimensions that may or may not coincide in a particular situation (see Winter, 1973, for a discussion). For instance, one can think about an actor that effectively has control over the outcomes of a relationship (e.g., a customer who can take his/her funds from a bank account with the click of a mouse-button), but no intention to have an effect on the other party. One can also conceive a desire of an actor to have an effect on a target (e.g., a dissatisfied consumer wanting to take a brand off the market) but lacking the ability to do so. We adopt in this research a broad definition of power as the capacity of an actor to produce intended effects on the behaviors and emotions of others (Winter, 1973). This conceptualization of power includes both the desire to reach a specific state (i.e., motive and goals) and the resources or actions that one would use to get to that state (i.e., means of attainment). We argue that power has an effect in bringing to mind goals-means knowledge structures that can be instrumental in the regulation of behavior (see Shah, Kruglanski, & Friedman, 2003). Thus, we study power s effects in terms of the motives (McClelland, 1987; Uleman, 1972; Veroff, 1957; Winter, 1973) and goal representations (Bargh et al., 1995; Chen et al., 2001) activated by power-related stimuli, and the actions individuals perform in order to affect others behaviors and emotions (Winter, 1973). In addition, we focus on culture as a critical antecedent of the associations people establish

12 12 with power (McClelland, 1973, 1987; McClelland, Davis, Wanner, & Kalin, 1972; Wanner, 1972; Winter, 1973, 1988). 2.2 Two Types of Power The above discussion suggests that, beyond individual differences in the drive to affect others, people can differ both in terms of self-defining goals associated with power (i.e., what we want to do with power) and in terms of the preferred contexts and means for exercising power (i.e., when and how one has effects on others, Bargh & Alvarez, 2001). If so, are there qualitatively different types of power? The answer to this question appears to be yes. We are all familiar with reports of the misuse of power for attaining selfish goals at the expense of others. People frequently associate powerful individuals with actions aimed at advancing one s personal goals, needs, and ideas (see Kipnis, 1976). Extreme examples of these powerholders are infamous dictators such as Adolf Hitler and Josef Stalin. However, it is not difficult to think about powerholders who exercise power in a socially responsible manner, attending and responding to the needs of others. Extreme examples of this second category of powerholders are famous figures like Mother Teresa and Ghandi. Thus, we can think about a first type of power associated with selfish goals attained at the expense of others and about a second type of power associated with prosocial goals of being responsible for others (Bargh & Alvarez, 2001). The distinction between the two types of power just described was originally captured by McClelland s distinction between personalized and socialized power motives (McClelland, 1973; McClelland, Wanner, & Vanneman, 1972; McClelland & Wilsnack, 1972). The personalized power motive is tuned toward seeking to win out over

13 13 active adversaries. This motive is aroused by putting a person in a personal dominance situation and is associated with heavy drinking, gambling, having more aggressive impulses, and collecting prestige supplies (e.g., luxury items or distinctive brands). Personalized power is associated with egoistic goals of influencing and being praised by others for one s benefit. These goals are attained by engaging in forceful actions, influence attempts, and/or behaviors aimed at impressing and signaling power and status to undifferentiated audiences (Bargh & Alvarez, 2001; McClelland, 1987; Winter, 1973, 1993a). In contrast, the socialized power motive expresses itself in thoughts of exercising power for the benefit of others and by feelings of ambivalence about holding power. This motive is associated with joining organizations where one can have an impact on people s lives. It relates to pursuing prosocial goals for the benefit of some other person or cause and to avoiding negative effects on others (Winter, 1973). These goals are attained by joining organizations where one can help others or by directly providing unsolicited help to others (Bargh & Alvarez, 2001; Frieze & Boneva, 2001; McClelland, 1973; Winter, 1973, 1993a). We have thus far argued that people who strive for power may associate these power desires with distinct patterns of goal-means structures. Cultural values and socialization experiences are a critical antecedent of the associations people establish with power (McClelland, 1973, 1987; McClelland, Davis et al., 1972; Wanner, 1972; Winter, 1973, 1988). Power expressions depend on culture (McClelland, Davis et al., 1972; Wanner, 1972; Winter, 1993a), social class (Winter, 1996), one s sense of social responsibility (McClelland, 1987), and gender role socialization (Stewart & Chester, 1982; Winter, 1988). Culture introduces variations in what is considered to be power and

14 14 the paths used to attain power (Winter, 1993a). In addition, culture impacts the development of the power motive into egoistic power associated with aggression and dominance (i.e., personalized or egoistic power) or into nurturant power associated with helping others (i.e., socialized or prosocial power, Whiting & Edwards, 1973; Whiting & Whiting, 1975). 2.3 Power and Culture Societies differ in terms of the inequalities in the distribution of power among their members. In certain societies some powerful members control the outcomes of less powerful others, whereas in other societies individuals are more or less similar in terms of their levels of power. Hofstede (1980; 2001) analyzed these differences in the distribution of power at the societal-level using the concept of power distance, or the degree of inequality between a less powerful individual and a more powerful other from the same social system. He classified countries along a continuum based on a power distance index (PDI). This index reflects the degree to which the less powerful members of organizations and institutions in a society perceive and accept inequalities in power. PDI is a characteristic of social systems, not of individuals. It indicates which societies are more hierarchical (high PDI) and which ones are more egalitarian (low PDI). However, it is not an approach to measuring the power concerns of a given individual. A related distinction in the study of culture between vertical (V) and horizontal (H) cultural orientations relates more directly to individual s beliefs and values associated with power (Singelis et al., 1995; Triandis, 1995; Triandis et al., 1998; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998). Individuals in vertical societies emphasize power and hierarchy, and see themselves as different from others in status. In contrast, in horizontal societies

15 15 individuals value equality and see themselves as equals to others in status. The vertical dimension resembles power distance at the individual level (Triandis, 1995). Thus, the H/V distinction offers a theoretical framework to study individuals mental associations with power as nurtured by different cultures. Triandis and colleagues suggested the H/V distinction to delineate the broadly used classification of individualism (IND) and collectivism (COL) (see Singelis et al., 1995; Triandis, 1995; Triandis et al., 1998; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998). These broad categories discriminate among individuals from different societies by focusing on the defining aspects of the self, the priority of personal goals, the types of relationships that people emphasize, and the importance of attitudes and norms as determinants of behaviors (Triandis, 1995; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998). In individualistic cultures, people tend to view the self as independent of others. They prefer independent relationships with others, subordinate the goals of their in-groups to their own personal goals, and emphasize exchange rather than communal relationships. In collectivistic cultures, in contrast, people tend to view the self in relation to others. They prefer interdependent relationships with others, subordinate their personal goals to those of their in-groups, and emphasize communal rather than exchange relationships (Hofstede, 1980; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1995). The H/V distinction comes from the observation that American individualism differs from Swedish individualism in much the same way that Japanese collectivism differs from the collectivism of the Israeli kibbutz. In horizontal, individualist cultural contexts (HI; e.g., Sweden, Denmark, Norway, Australia), people value uniqueness and are likely to say I want to do my own thing, but are not especially interested in

16 16 becoming distinguished and achieving high status. In vertical, individualist societies (VI; e.g., U.S., Great Britain, France), people strive for becoming distinguished and acquiring status via competition. These individuals are concerned about winning and are likely to be annoyed when others are distinguished. In horizontal, collectivist cultural contexts (HC; e.g., exemplified historically by the Israeli Kibbutz), people see themselves as similar to others, emphasize common goals with others, interdependence, and sociability, but they do not submit to authority. These individuals are likely to say I feel good when I cooperate with others. Finally, in vertical, collectivistic societies (VC; e.g., Korea, Japan, India), people emphasize the subordination of their goals to those of their ingroups, they submit to the will of authority, and support competitions of their in-groups with out-groups. These individuals are likely to say I respect the decisions made by my groups. Cross-cultural data concerning the development of the power motive (Whiting & Whiting, 1975) shed some light on the link between power and culture. Whiting and Whiting studied the social behavior of children from six different cultures (Japan, the Philippines, India, Kenya, Mexico, and the U.S.). The families studied were primarily subsistence farmers (except in the U.S.). They observed and recorded the social behavior of children from these families in natural settings (e.g., in their homes, playgrounds, etc.) and classified the observed behaviors into different categories (e.g., nurturance, succorance, sociability, dominance, aggression, reprimands, touches, and responsibility). Using multidimensional scaling techniques they grouped the different behaviors in two dimensions. The first dimension captured children s power development along a continuum from prosocial power (i.e., nurturant-responsibility behaviors) to egoistic

17 17 power (i.e., dominance-dependance behaviors). The second dimension depicted children s behaviors along a sociability-aggression continuum. Whiting and Whiting found that children from simple societies exhibited the highest scores in prosocial power, whereas those from complex societies exhibited the highest scores in egoistic power. Children from simple societies continuously performed chores aimed at improving the well-being of the family (e.g., caring for younger siblings or helping with procurement of food and supplies), whereas those from complex societies did not. Societies were classified in terms of complexity (complex vs. simple) based on the following characteristics: occupational specialization (highly specialized vs. no specialization), the use of cash (cash vs. non-cash economies), and the presence or not of a centralized political and legal system. Based on a review of ethnographies, Triandis (1995) suggests that simple and tight societies, like the ones studied by Whiting and Whiting, are associated with maximal collectivism. This is the horizontal collectivism exemplified by theocracies, monasteries, or the Israeli kibbutz. Triandis argues that in these low-stratified societies there are no hierarchies and individuals show high levels of interdependence. People share their daily duties in an egalitarian fashion and independent actions are looked upon disdainfully. In contrast, complex cultures, like those from large Western cities, are associated with maximal individualism. This is the vertical individualism that fosters independence and competition. Reinterpreting Whiting and Whiting s (1975) findings within the IND-COL framework suggests that people from horizontal collectivistic cultures, which encourage the development of a more interdependent and responsible sense of family relationships, would display greater nurturant or socialized power. In contrast, people from vertical

18 18 individualistic cultures, which emphasize individuality and achievement instead, would display greater egoistic or personalized power (see Gardner & Seeley, 2001, for a similar claim). It is precisely the concern with the well-being of others that has been proposed to underlie gender differences in relational interdependence (Cross & Madson, 1997; Gabriel & Gardner, 1999) and in the development of a socialized versus personalized power motive (Stewart & Chester, 1982; Winter, 1988). Additional cross-cultural data about the associations between power and heavy drinking across different cultures provide further support for these ideas. McClelland and colleagues (McClelland, Davis et al., 1972; Wanner, 1972) studied the correlation between folk-tale content and drinking habits of different cultures. They determined the frequency of occurrence of power-related terms (e.g., vigorous activity, war, and hunting) among the folk-tale stories and correlated it with alcohol consumption across different societies. They found that sober societies are better organized, hierarchical, and supportive. These are often agricultural communities that give wide and strong support to their members and that stress inhibition and respect. In contrast, societies that do not provide this type of support to their members, and that instead expect them to be assertive yet obedient, can produce internal conflicts that are solved by dreams of being powerful in a primitive, impulsive way induced by alcohol consumption. The heaviest drinking occurred in hunting societies that emphasized individual s strivings for personalized power in their folk-tales. They argued that in these societies men are required to be continually assertive and successful (e.g., bring meat from a kill) but are also prevented from gaining permanent prestige (e.g., potential loss of prestige from not bringing a kill). As a consequence, they tend to drink as an immediate means of gaining momentary

19 19 feelings of personalized power. In contrast, in sober societies, individuals are provided with cohesive, self-conscious in-groups to which each of its members are loyal. This in turn provides means of gaining lasting positions in the eyes of others. Such groups also require that the individual limit his own self-assertiveness in the interest of group cohesion. In these societies, individuals drink little and show little concern with personalized power. Hunting societies are typically assumed to be more individualistic (see Triandis, 1995). The heavy drinking societies described by McClelland and colleagues resemble those vertical individualistic societies that continuously stress a need for winning and performing, whereas the most sober societies resemble the more nurturing agricultural societies that tend to be collectivistic. Reinterpretation of McClelland and colleagues findings, along with those from Whiting and Whiting, suggest that there are differences between collectivists and individualists in their associations with power. Furthermore, these findings suggest that we can refine predictions about the link between culture and mental representations of power by focusing on the H-V distinction nested within the broader IND-COL categories. This distinction will specify which individuals are more likely to have well established mental representations of power. It will also anticipate the type of power-related goals (e.g., egoistic vs. prosocial goals) associated with power among individuals with different cultural orientations. This will help us to predict the conditions under which a given individual will activate a power-related goal as well as the nature of this goal. 2.4 Mental Representations of Power across Cultures

20 20 We focus in this research on studying power s effects in terms of the motives and goal representations activated by power-related stimuli. An underlying assumption for this approach is that there are power-goal representations in people s minds. Before embarking on a more detailed analysis of the nuanced relationships between culture and these mental representations, let us first discuss the empirical evidence for the existence and nature of mental representations of power. As with other goal structures, powerrelated goal systems are mental representations that include a general need being served by relatively few abstract goals, which in turn are served by a larger number of concrete activities, or means, and that can be learned, altered or activated (see Kruglanski, 1996; Shah, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2002; Shah & Kruglanski, 2000; Shah et al., 2003). Once activated by environmental stimuli, these goals can operate outside of awareness and guide cognitive and behavioral processes (see Bargh, 1990; Bargh & Barndollar, 1996; Bargh & Chartrand, 2000). Is there a cognitive representation of power linked to the power-related goals people pursue? The literature offers at least two different perspectives on this issue. One perspective is that of power as a concept or schema (i.e., similar to other concepts like honesty, sincerity, etc.) that can be activated by situational, power-related cues (see Bargh et al., 1995; Chen et al., 2001). Within this view, people would have mental representations of power linked simultaneously to environmental cues and to powerrelated goals. Encountering a power-related cue triggers the activation of the power concept which subsequently activates other concepts and goals associated with it. An alternative view of power is that of an implicit motive. McClelland (McClelland, 1987; McClelland, Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989) characterizes an

21 21 implicit motive as an unconscious and not readily expressed concern of an individual that energizes behavior. More specifically, Winter (1973) views the power motive as a disposition to strive for certain kinds of goals, or to be affected by certain kinds of incentives (p. 17). Although power as an implicit motive is assumed to operate unconsciously (McClelland et al., 1989), it affects the explicit goals pursued to express it (i.e., goals that are normative for exercising power, McClelland et al., 1989). It also determines the contexts individuals associate with power (Winter, 1973). Implicit motives impact the encoding and retrieval of experiences related to its expression (see Woike, 1995; Woike, Lavezzary, & Barsky, 2001; Woike, McLeod, & Goggin, 2003). An implicit power concern leads individuals to the ongoing use of power for categorizing human interaction (Winter, 1973). These two views of power converge in indicating that people have mental representations of goals and contexts associated with power. Although the conceptualization of power adopted here is congruent with both views, we will favor the view of power as an enduring motive that facilitates the organization of concepts, goals, and domains into power-related mental structures 1. Ongoing expression of this motive facilitates the learning of power strategies and of behaviors and contexts instrumental for exercising power, which helps to populate these power-related mental structures with information. Furthermore, ongoing exposure to situations where one has the capacity to control others (i.e., controlling resources that can impact others outcomes) facilitates the priming of power upon encountering such situations, which in turn activates the motivational system aimed at impacting others (Bargh & Alvarez, 2001; Keltner et al., 1 We frequently refer to these structures as power-related structures or structures associated with power in the sense that an enduring striving for power facilitated the cognitive association among the elements (goals, behaviors or means, contexts, and cues) within the structure.

22 ). Thus, priming of power brings to mind power-related goals that direct individual s actions toward goal achievement. There is empirical evidence for the activation of goal structures by a power prime. Bargh et al. (1995) analyzed the cognitive association between power and sex among individuals high and low in likelihood to sexually harass (LSH measures the likelihood to use leverage in a situation to gain sexual favors from a woman). To test the hypothesis that power and sex are associated in men high in LSH, they presented men high and low in LSH with power-related words (e.g., authority, executive, boss, influence, rich, and control). The findings showed that, among men high in LSH, power became salient and unconsciously activated sex-oriented goals. These sex-related goals were not activated among individuals low in LSH, which supports the notion that individuals selectively associate power with certain types of goals based on their experiences when exercising power. Chen et al. provided further support for power-goal cognitive representations in a study about the goals associated with power among individuals with different relationship orientations. They hypothesized that exchange-oriented individuals (i.e., who give a benefit to a partner with the expectation of receiving comparable benefits in return) would associate power with self-interest goals, whereas communal-oriented individuals (i.e., focused on responding to the needs and interests of others) would associate power with responsibility goals. They activated power among the two groups of individuals by either presenting them with power-related words or by having them sit in a professor s chair (which unobtrusively activated power in an undergraduate participant population). Upon being primed with power, individuals with an exchange orientation activated self-

23 23 centered goals and exhibited behaviors aimed at benefiting themselves over others. When given the option to divide experimental tasks between themselves and other participants, they took a smaller part of the experimental burden on themselves. In contrast, individuals with a communal orientation activated goals of responsibility toward others and, in the same scenario, took a greater part of the experimental burden on themselves. These findings link to our previous discussion about the impact of collectivism and individualism on the development of personalized vs. socialized power motives in children. In combination, they suggest that culturally nurtured views of the self impact the goals and behaviors that people cognitively associate with power (see also Gardner & Seeley, 2001). Individualists, similar to exchange-oriented individuals, would associate power with self-focused cognitions and goals (e.g., acquire status) and instances in which these goals can be achieved (e.g., when being admired by others for one s achievements or abilities). In contrast, collectivists, similar to communal-oriented individuals, would associate power with other-focused cognitions and goals (e.g., caring about others and making sacrifices for others) and instances in which these goals can be achieved (e.g., when helping others) Power and Individualism Thus far, we have argued that individualists associate power with egoistic goals. These associations would lead them to believe that powerful others pursue goals for their own benefit and that these powerful others take advantage of relative power to pursue their personal and selfish goals (see Bargh & Alvarez, 2001). When primed with power, individualists will activate self-focused, status-seeking goals and perform actions aimed at influencing others for achieving these goals. However, would all individualists have

24 24 these strong associations with power-related cues? We believe that the answer is no. Vertical individualists, who are particularly concerned about achieving status and personalized power (Singelis et al., 1995; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998), are especially likely to have strong links in memory between power and other constructs due to ongoing power concerns that lead to frequent activations of power. These individuals will use power as a theme for organizing their social interactions (see Winter, 1973), which facilitates the development of well established cognitive associations between power, egoistic goals, and means of attainment among varied contexts and situations. In addition to cross-cultural data about the development of egoistic power in children (Whiting & Whiting, 1975) and about the fantasies of individuals from societies that constantly encourage winning (McClelland, Davis et al., 1972; Wanner, 1972), cross-national and within-culture data using cultural orientation provide support for these ideas. A VI cultural orientation is positively related to achievement and power values, whereas an HI orientation is positively related to self-direction (Oishi, Schimmack, Diener, & Suh, 1998). Similarly, Soh and Leong (2002) reported that in both the U.S. and Singapore a VI orientation was best predicted by power values and HI by self-direction values. People with a VI orientation are particularly concerned about achieving status, whereas those with an HI orientation are not. Cross-national research in the U.S. (a VI society) and Denmark (an HI society) indicates that people with a VI orientation give more importance to displays of success and gaining of influence than people with an HI orientation (Nelson & Shavitt, 2002). Overall, individuals with a VI orientation, who use power and status for organizing social relations, will have abundant power-related experiences and well

25 25 established mental representations of personalized power. Power-related cues should easily bring to mind personal goals of influencing, impacting, and being praised by others. These individuals will likely engage in forceful actions, influence attempts, and/or behaviors aimed at impressing undifferentiated audiences as an expression of personalized power (see McClelland, 1987; Winter, 1973). Their power actions will likely use status and prestige as instrumental means (McClelland, 1973; Winter, 1972, 1973, 1993a) and will focus on approach-related behaviors where individuals act with little deliberation and inhibition (Galinsky, Gruenfeld, & Magee, 2003; Keltner et al., 2003; McClelland, Wanner et al., 1972). We do not anticipate similar effects among individuals with an HI orientation. The lack of power and status-related concerns among these individuals will lead to relatively weak associations between power and other constructs. Power-related stimuli that easily arouse a power motive among individuals with a VI orientation will not have the same impact among individuals with an HI orientation. Thus, encountering powerrelated primes should not lead to an increased accessibility of egoistic goals among these individuals Power and Collectivism We have stated that collectivists associate power with prosocial goals of helping others. These individuals are likely to believe that powerful others pursue goals for the benefit of others and would not take advantage of relative power to pursue personal and selfish goals. When primed with power, collectivists will activate prosocial goals aimed at helping others. Because these individuals are not particularly concerned about

26 26 signaling power and status to undifferentiated audiences, power-related cues are not likely to have the same effects they have among individualists. Helping behaviors driven by a socialized power motive have been related to socialization patterns that encourage interdependence (McClelland, 1975), to people s values of cooperating with others without evoking feelings of obedience (McClelland, 1973), and to concerns for helping both close others and others in general (Frieze & Boneva, 2001). These descriptions clearly fit the values and characteristics of individuals with HC orientations. These individuals emphasize common goals with others, interdependence, and cooperation, but they do not submit to authority (Singelis et al., 1995; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998). They see themselves as being similar to others and focus both on important in-groups (e.g., co-workers and relatives) and similar others in general (as captured by items such as "I feel good when I cooperate with others," Triandis & Gelfand, 1998) in their social relationships. They emphasize nurturance, altruism, and concern for others (Triandis, 1995). We anticipate that individuals with an HC orientation will use socialized power for organizing their social interactions. In turn, this will lead to well established cognitive associations between power, prosocial goals, and helping actions among varied contexts and situations. In addition to cross-cultural data about the development of prosocial power in children (Whiting & Whiting, 1975) and about the fantasies of individuals from societies that encourage loyalty and provide support to their members (McClelland, Davis et al., 1972; Wanner, 1972), cross-national and within-culture data using cultural orientation provide support for these ideas. Oishi et al. (1998) observed that HC orientation correlated positively with a focus on social relationships. Along similar lines, Triandis

27 27 and Gelfand (1998) reported that HC was predicted by sociability and interdependence. Further, Nelson and Shavitt (2002) found in a cross-national sample that HC correlates with sociable and benevolent values in both the United States and Denmark. Some indirect support is also provided by reported findings that women, who tend to be high in an HC orientation (Lalwani & Shavitt, 2006), display greater nurturant or socialized power for reasons similar to those of individuals from simple cultures (see Gardner & Seeley, 2001; Winter, 1988). Overall, individuals with HC orientations will have abundant experiences expressing socialized power and well established mental associations of power with prosocial goals. Cues associated with socialized power (e.g., a person in need or requests for advice) should easily bring to mind prosocial goals of helping others. These individuals will likely engage in unsolicited helping behaviors and actions aimed at doing good to others typically associated with individuals who strive for socialized power (Frieze & Boneva, 2001; McClelland, 1973; Winter, 1973). They will have weak associations between power and egoistic goals and negative attitudes toward dominance and social inequalities (see Strunk & Chang, 1999). In this regard, horizontality among HC individuals relates to lack of concerns with personalized power (i.e., status and hierarchies). However, their focus on interdependence and the well being of others leads them to strive for prosocial goals associated with socialized power. These are concerns that are not equally relevant among individuals with an HI orientation who see themselves as independent from others and focus on appearing as self-reliant. We anticipate that individuals with VC orientations will also have cognitive associations between power and prosocial goals. However, important qualifications need

28 28 to be made. In VC cultures, people can act in individualistic or collectivistic ways depending on their location in the social hierarchy (Triandis, 1995). One s position in the social hierarchy will affect mental associations with power for individuals with a VC orientation. Power will be less of a concern for low-status individuals who only need to defer to authority. Mental representations of power may not be very strong for these individuals. Power will be more of a concern for those individuals high in the social hierarchy that are responsible for in-groups of lower status and for also assuring that one s group vigorously competes with out-groups (and exploits them if necessary). This contrasts with the uninhibited exercise of power among individuals with a VI orientation. These individuals focus on self-gains and do not need to restrain their power attempts by considering the consequences from their actions on those of a lower status. In contrast, VC individuals are constrained in their power attempts by their responsibilities toward ingroup members who depend on them. Ongoing experiences expressing power for the benefit of in-groups will lead individuals with a VC orientation to associate power with prosocial goals of caring for the well-being of others. However, individuals with a VC orientation see in-groups as holding a higher status relative to out-groups and they have few skills in dealing with strangers (Triandis, 1995). Their concerns with sacrificing their personal goals for the sake of in-group goals (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998) would suggest that prosocial goals associated with power may be constrained to interactions with ingroups. High levels of prejudice and discrimination among vertical collectivists (Triandis, 1995) suggest that their mental representations of power associated with out-groups should not be linked to prosocial goals. Furthermore, hostile social behaviors and harsh treatments toward out-groups (Triandis, 1995) may lead to associations of power with

29 29 anti-social goals similar to those of individuals with a VI orientation (who also exhibit high levels of prejudice against others). Although VC individuals will have associations of power with prosocial goals of benefiting in-groups, they will share with VI individuals the concerns with personalized power and hierarchy that characterize vertical societies. However, VI individuals exercise power indiscriminately to achieve one s selfish goals, whereas VC individuals exercise power within the constraints of their responsibilities toward in-groups. Some empirical findings support these ideas. The tendency to favor products from one s own country over foreign products emerges strongly in Japan (a VC culture) driven by values that stress hierarchy and respect for the in-group. VC is positively correlated with a sense of obligation within a social hierarchy (Chan & Drasgow, 2001), with authoritarianism (Kemmelmeier et al., 2003), with the acceptance of ideas advocated by a high-status agent even if they challenge the perspective assumed by the majority (Ng & Van Dyne, 2001), and with taking on burdens that are greater than one s share when one is higher in the hierarchy (Wade-Benzoni et al., 2002). Overall, individuals with a VC orientation will associate power with pro-social goals related to in-groups and with egoistic goals of winning out over others when involving out-groups. In addition, these mental representations of power would be dependent on the individual s role in the social hierarchy. From the discussion in this chapter, the following hypotheses related to the link between culture and mental representations of power are derived: H1: Individualism will be associated with perceptions that powerful others pursue egoistic goals, whereas collectivism will be associated with perceptions that powerful others pursue prosocial goals of being responsible for others.

30 30 H2: A VI cultural orientation will be associated with well established cognitive representations of personalized power. Individuals with this orientation will be more likely than those with any other orientation to express power by engaging in forceful actions, signaling status, and impressing others. These behaviors will be manifest in their power-related fantasies and thinking about them will lead to experienced positive emotions. H2a: A VI cultural orientation will be positively related to dispositions to abuse power for one s own benefit and unrelated to dispositions to help others. H3: An HC cultural orientation will be associated with well established cognitive representations of socialized power. Individuals with this orientation will be more likely than those with any other orientation to express power by providing unsolicited help to others. Thinking about these behaviors will lead to experienced positive emotions. H3a: An HC cultural orientation will be negatively related to dispositions to abuse power for one s own benefit and positively related to dispositions to help others.

31 31 CHAPTER 3 CONSEQUENCES OF ACTIVATING DISTINCT TYPES OF MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF POWER FOR CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 3.1 Mechanisms for Priming Power and Goal Activation The hypothesized differences in mental representations of power among individuals with different cultural orientations in chapter 2 suggest consequences for the goal-means structures individuals will activate upon encountering power-related primes. These primes will bring to mind power-related structures that are well represented in memory. Figure 3.1 depicts the goal-means structures characterizing individuals with different cultural orientations. Individuals with a VI orientation possess strong mental representations of personalized power (and weak ones of socialized power), which include both egoistic goals of achieving status and influencing others for one s own benefit and varied instrumental actions/contexts to fulfill these goals (e.g., signaling prestige through possessions or vigorous actions). In the case of individuals with an HI orientation, although power can be associated with selfish goals, these associations should be weakly established in memory. Individuals with an HC orientation have strong mental representations of socialized power (and weak ones of personalized power), which include both prosocial goals of caring for the well-being of others and instrumental means /contexts to fulfill these goals (e.g., helping others in a task or giving advice to others). Finally, individuals with a VC orientation will have relatively equally strong associations of power with prosocial goals involving in-groups and with egoistic goals involving out-groups.

Lecture 9. Control and Personality. Professor Ian Robertson

Lecture 9. Control and Personality. Professor Ian Robertson Lecture 9 Control and Personality Professor Ian Robertson SEE KELTNER ET AL PSYCH REVIEW (FROM LECTURE 8) Why does Power increase behavioural activation? Power is correlated with increased resources. Powerful

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION 92 THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION 1. Motivation is a highly dynamic construct that is constantly changing in reaction to life experiences. 2. Needs and goals are constantly growing and changing. 3. As individuals

More information

THE EFFECTS OF CULTURAL ORIENTATION ON PERCEPTIONS OF POWER THREAT JIMMY SHIANG YANG WONG DISSERTATION

THE EFFECTS OF CULTURAL ORIENTATION ON PERCEPTIONS OF POWER THREAT JIMMY SHIANG YANG WONG DISSERTATION THE EFFECTS OF CULTURAL ORIENTATION ON PERCEPTIONS OF POWER THREAT BY JIMMY SHIANG YANG WONG DISSERTATION Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in

More information

reward based power have ability to give you what you want. coercive have power to punish

reward based power have ability to give you what you want. coercive have power to punish Chapter 7 Finding and Using Negotiation Power Why Power Important to Negotiators? Seeking power in negotiations from 1 of 2 perceptions: 1. Negotiator believes he has less power than other party 2. Negotiator

More information

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology:

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Defining Psychology Behaviorism: The scientific study of how rewards and punishment in the environment affect human and non-human behavior Empirical approach: vary contingencies of

More information

Culture and Social Behavior, Self and Identity. Chapter 13. Defining the Concept of Self CULTURE AND THE CONCEPT OF SELF

Culture and Social Behavior, Self and Identity. Chapter 13. Defining the Concept of Self CULTURE AND THE CONCEPT OF SELF Outline Culture and Social Behavior, Self and Identity Chapter 13 Culture and the Concept of Self-Concept Cultural Differences in Self-Concept Culture and Identity Self-Esteem and Self-Enhancement Culture

More information

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE CHAPTER FIFTEEN Motivation INTRODUCTION Many of us have unrealized abilities. Some of us could run marathons, others could write novels, and still others could get straight A s in management classes. But

More information

Materialism and Cultural Orientation: The Role of Vertical/Horizontal Indi vidualism within and across Cultures

Materialism and Cultural Orientation: The Role of Vertical/Horizontal Indi vidualism within and across Cultures Materialism and Cultural Orientation: The Role of Vertical/Horizontal Indi vidualism within and across Cultures Ellen Garbarino, University of Sydney, ellen.garbarino@sydney.edu.au Julie A. Lee, University

More information

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Copyright 2015 McGraw-Hill Education. All rights reserved. No reproduction or distribution without the prior written consent of McGraw-Hill Education. Learning

More information

Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality. Copyright Allyn & Bacon (2009)

Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality. Copyright Allyn & Bacon (2009) Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality Roots in Gestalt Psychology Human beings seek meaning in their environments We organize the sensations we receive into meaningful perceptions Complex stimuli

More information

Test Bank. Chapter 2: The Cultural Context. Multiple Choice

Test Bank. Chapter 2: The Cultural Context. Multiple Choice Test Bank Chapter 2: The Cultural Context Multiple Choice 1. Which of the following types of cultures emphasize individual goals over group goals? a. individualistic cultures b. large power distance cultures

More information

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d.

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d. Chapter 10: Cognitive Processing of Attitudes 1. McGuire s (1969) model of information processing can be best described as which of the following? a. Sequential b. Parallel c. Automatic 2. Which of the

More information

CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK

CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK 2.0.0 INTRODUCTION The details about introduction, rationale of the present study, statement of the problem objectives of the study, hypotheses of the study, delimitation

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological needs & implicit motives Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2016 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION In the format provided by the authors and unedited. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION VOLUME: 1 ARTICLE NUMBER: 0056 Online Supplement On the benefits of explaining herd immunity in vaccine advocacy Cornelia Betsch

More information

Chapter 13. Social Psychology

Chapter 13. Social Psychology Social Psychology Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? Social Psychology Interpreting the Behavior of Others Behaving in the Presence of Others Establishing Relations With Others Social

More information

Applied Social Psychology Msc.

Applied Social Psychology Msc. Applied Social Msc. Course Course names Course description codes MSPSY501* Applied Social This module will discuss, at advanced level: The cognitive system, conceptual systems, expectation, explanation

More information

MHR Chapter 5. Motivation: The forces within a person that affect his or her direction, intensity and persistence of voluntary behaviour

MHR Chapter 5. Motivation: The forces within a person that affect his or her direction, intensity and persistence of voluntary behaviour MHR Chapter 5 Motivation: The forces within a person that affect his or her direction, intensity and persistence of voluntary behaviour Employee Engagement: Individual s emotional and cognitive motivation,

More information

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz MULTIPLE CHOICE: 1. We usually adapt our behavior to the demands of the social situation, and in ambiguous situations: A) We take our cues from the behavior of others. B) We will do the same thing that

More information

PSYC 210 Social Psychology

PSYC 210 Social Psychology South Central College PSYC 210 Social Psychology Course Information Description Total Credits 4.00 Pre/Corequisites PSYC100 or consent of instructor. Course Competencies Social Psychology introduces the

More information

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology Preface p. xv Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology p. 3 Sociology, Psychology, or Both? p.

More information

PSYCHOLOGY. The Psychology Major. Preparation for the Psychology Major. The Social Science Teaching Credential

PSYCHOLOGY. The Psychology Major. Preparation for the Psychology Major. The Social Science Teaching Credential Psychology 1 PSYCHOLOGY The Psychology Major Psychology is the scientific study of human and animal behavior and the cognitive and biological processes that underlie it. The objective of USD s psychological

More information

Traits and Personality. Chapter 10

Traits and Personality. Chapter 10 Traits and Personality Chapter 10 Gordon Allport 1897-1967 1967 Grew up near Cleveland Spent much of his childhood alone College at Harvard A visit to Freud led him to find a new approach Young boy with

More information

Department of Psychological Sciences Learning Goals and Outcomes

Department of Psychological Sciences Learning Goals and Outcomes Department of Psychological Sciences Learning Goals and Outcomes Upon completion of a Bachelor s degree in Psychology, students will be prepared in content related to the eight learning goals described

More information

WHAT IS SELF? MODULE-IV OBJECTIVES 16.1 CONCEPT OF SELF. What is Self? Self and Personality. Notes

WHAT IS SELF? MODULE-IV OBJECTIVES 16.1 CONCEPT OF SELF. What is Self? Self and Personality. Notes What is Self? MODULE-IV 16 WHAT IS SELF? Self is focus of our everyday behaviour and all of us do have a set of perceptions and beliefs about ourselves. This kind of self concept plays important role in

More information

Chapter 14. Social Psychology. How Does the Social Situation Affect our Behavior? Social Psychology

Chapter 14. Social Psychology. How Does the Social Situation Affect our Behavior? Social Psychology Chapter 14 Social Psychology This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited by law: Any public performance or display, including transmission of

More information

Social Psychology. What We Will Cover in This Section. Roles. PDF Created with deskpdf PDF Writer - Trial ::

Social Psychology. What We Will Cover in This Section. Roles. PDF Created with deskpdf PDF Writer - Trial :: Social Psychology Dr. Dennis C. Sweeney What We Will Cover in This Section Conformity Attitudes Group Behavior Prejudice Roles Socially defined expectations that we expect individuals to fulfill in a given

More information

The four chapters in Part I set the stage. Chapter 1 moves from the implicit common sense theories of everyday life to explicit theories that are

The four chapters in Part I set the stage. Chapter 1 moves from the implicit common sense theories of everyday life to explicit theories that are Preface This volume is designed as a basic text for upper level and graduate courses in contemporary sociological theory. Most sociology programs require their majors to take at least one course in sociological

More information

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT Study Focus Empathy s impact on: O Human Interactions O Consequences of those interactions O Empathy Development Study Purpose to examine the empathetic

More information

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping Amanda Palmer, Ken Sinclair and Michael Bailey University of Sydney Paper prepared for presentation

More information

FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER

FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER 1 ST SEMESTER 2017 ASSIGNMENT 2 ORGANISATIONAL BEHAVIOUR OSB611S 1 Page1 OSB611S - FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER FOR ASSIGNMENT 2-2016 Dear student The purpose of this tutorial letter

More information

Cambridge Public Schools SEL Benchmarks K-12

Cambridge Public Schools SEL Benchmarks K-12 Cambridge Public Schools SEL Benchmarks K-12 OVERVIEW SEL Competencies Goal I: Develop selfawareness Goal II: Develop and Goal III: Develop social Goal IV: Demonstrate Goal V: Demonstrate skills to demonstrate

More information

THE CUSTOMER SERVICE ATTRIBUTE INDEX

THE CUSTOMER SERVICE ATTRIBUTE INDEX THE CUSTOMER SERVICE ATTRIBUTE INDEX Jane Doe Customer Service XYZ Corporation 7-22-2003 CRITICAL SUCCESS ATTRIBUTES ATTITUDE TOWARD OTHERS: To what extent does Jane tend to maintain a positive, open and

More information

Chapter 15 Learning Objectives with SubQuestions PERSON PERCEPTION: FORMING IMPRESSIONS OF OTHERS

Chapter 15 Learning Objectives with SubQuestions PERSON PERCEPTION: FORMING IMPRESSIONS OF OTHERS Chapter 15 Learning Objectives with SubQuestions PERSON PERCEPTION: FORMING IMPRESSIONS OF OTHERS #1) Explain how schemas, stereotypes, and other factors contribute to subjectivity in person perception

More information

CHAPTER NINE INTERPERSONAL DETERMINANTS OF CONSUMER BEHAVIOR

CHAPTER NINE INTERPERSONAL DETERMINANTS OF CONSUMER BEHAVIOR CHAPTER NINE INTERPERSONAL DETERMINANTS OF CONSUMER BEHAVIOR CHAPTER OBJECTIVES Differentiate between customer behavior and consumer behavior Explain how marketers classify behavioral influences on consumer

More information

Key Concepts of Feminist Therapy. Chapter 12. Four Approaches to Feminist Therapy. Four Approaches to Feminist Therapy

Key Concepts of Feminist Therapy. Chapter 12. Four Approaches to Feminist Therapy. Four Approaches to Feminist Therapy Feminist Therapy Chapter 12 Key Concepts of Feminist Therapy Problems are viewed in a sociopolitical and cultural context The client knows what is best for her life and is the expert on her own life Emphasis

More information

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE Personal Report JOHN SMITH 2017 MySkillsProfile. All rights reserved. Introduction The EIQ16 measures aspects of your emotional intelligence by asking you questions

More information

Scope insensitivity in helping decisions: Is it a matter of culture and values?

Scope insensitivity in helping decisions: Is it a matter of culture and values? Posted November 9, 2015 Scope insensitivity in helping decisions: Is it a matter of culture and values? Tehila Kogut 1, Paul Slovic 2, 3 & Daniel Västfjäll 2 ------------------- Manuscript January 2015

More information

Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams

Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams The current plan is that there are 5 exams with 50 multiple choice items that will cover two chapters. Each chapter is planned to have 25 multiple choice items.

More information

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals SOCIAL INFLUENCE 2011, 6 (1), 15 21 The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals Esther S. Jeong 1, Yue Shi 1, Anna Baazova 1, Christine

More information

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT Van Der Velde / Guide to Business Research Methods First Proof 6.11.2003 4:53pm page 1 Part I PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT Van Der Velde / Guide to Business Research Methods First Proof 6.11.2003 4:53pm

More information

THE EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ATTRIBUTE INDEX

THE EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ATTRIBUTE INDEX THE EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ATTRIBUTE INDEX "He who knows others is learned He who knows himself is wise" Lao Tse Jerry Doe Financial Analyst XYZ Corporation 7-11-2003 THE EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ATTRIBUTE

More information

CHAPTER OUTLINE CHAPTER SUMMARY

CHAPTER OUTLINE CHAPTER SUMMARY CHAPTER 14 PERSONALITY IN PERSPECTIVE: OVERLAP AND INTEGRATION CHAPTER OUTLINE Similarities Among Perspectives Psychoanalysis and Evolutionary Psychology: The Structural Model Psychoanalysis and Evolutionary

More information

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use ACM, forthcoming. This is the author s version of the work. It is posted here by permission of ACM for your personal use. Not for redistribution. The definitive version will be published soon. Citation:

More information

Motivation, Conflict, Emotion. Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry

Motivation, Conflict, Emotion. Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry Motivation, Conflict, Emotion Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry Motivation Motivation is the psychological feature that arouses an organism to action toward a desired goal and

More information

UNDERSTANDING YOUR REPORT

UNDERSTANDING YOUR REPORT UNDERSTANDING YOUR REPORT Knowledge of an individual's motivators help to tell us WHY they do things. A review of an individual's experiences, references, education and training help to tell us WHAT they

More information

24/10/13. Surprisingly little evidence that: sex offenders have enduring empathy deficits empathy interventions result in reduced reoffending.

24/10/13. Surprisingly little evidence that: sex offenders have enduring empathy deficits empathy interventions result in reduced reoffending. Professor Tony Ward Law, D. R. & Ward, T. (2011). Desistance from sexual offending: Alternatives to throwing away the keys. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Ward, T., & Durrant, R. (2011). Evolutionary behavioural

More information

The Effect of Regulatory Focus and Delay Type on Consumers Reactions to Delay

The Effect of Regulatory Focus and Delay Type on Consumers Reactions to Delay The Effect of Regulatory Focus and Delay Type on Consumers Reactions to Delay Wan Yang School of Hospitality Management The Penn State University Anna S. Mattila School of Hospitality Management The Penn

More information

Psychology and Social Behaviour

Psychology and Social Behaviour 18 Psychology and Social Behaviour Introduction In this chapter, social behaviour is explained. Four types of social behaviour namely, cooperation, competition, altruism and aggression are also identified.

More information

Chapter 2 INDIVIDUAL BEHAVIOR AND PROCESSES

Chapter 2 INDIVIDUAL BEHAVIOR AND PROCESSES Chapter 2 INDIVIDUAL BEHAVIOR AND PROCESSES Individual Behavior, Personality, and Values McGraw-Hill/Irwin McShane/Von Glinow OB Copyright 2010 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights Values, Personality,

More information

PYSC 224 Introduction to Experimental Psychology

PYSC 224 Introduction to Experimental Psychology PYSC 224 Introduction to Experimental Psychology Session 7 Personality and Culture Lecturer:, Dept. of Psychology Contact Information: mamankwah-poku@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing

More information

The Attribute Index - Leadership

The Attribute Index - Leadership 26-Jan-2007 0.88 / 0.74 The Attribute Index - Leadership Innermetrix, Inc. Innermetrix Talent Profile of Innermetrix, Inc. http://www.innermetrix.cc/ The Attribute Index - Leadership Patterns Patterns

More information

AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF LEADER-MEMBER EXCHANGE IN CHINA, AND THE ROLE OF GUANXI IN THE LMX PROCESS

AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF LEADER-MEMBER EXCHANGE IN CHINA, AND THE ROLE OF GUANXI IN THE LMX PROCESS UNIVERSITY OF SOUTHERN QUEENSLAND AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF LEADER-MEMBER EXCHANGE IN CHINA, AND THE ROLE OF GUANXI IN THE LMX PROCESS A Dissertation submitted by Gwenda Latham, MBA For the award of Doctor

More information

CHAPTER 15. Social Psychology. Lecture Overview. Introductory Definition PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY. Social Cognition.

CHAPTER 15. Social Psychology. Lecture Overview. Introductory Definition PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY. Social Cognition. Social Psychology CHAPTER 15 Social Cognition Lecture Overview Social Influence Social Relations Applying Social Psychology to Social Problems Introductory Definition Social Psychology: scientific study

More information

EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY. Pages Social Identity 4:22

EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY. Pages Social Identity 4:22 EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY Pages 106 108 Social Identity 4:22 HENRI TAJFEL S SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY Individuals strive to improve their self image by trying to enhance their selfesteem, based on:

More information

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site SEUNGHO CHO shcho72@gmail.com LAURA RICHARDSON WALTON lwalton@comm.msstate.edu Mississippi

More information

Co-Cultures co- cultural

Co-Cultures co- cultural Challenges of Diversity in Dealing with Emergency Response Robert C. Chandler, PhD Chair, Communication Division Center for Communication and Business Pepperdine University Robert C. Chandler, Ph.D., 2006

More information

The pursuit of wellbeing: The Culture Paradigm

The pursuit of wellbeing: The Culture Paradigm 2017 The pursuit of wellbeing: The Culture Paradigm SANDHYA RAJAGOPAL COACHINGWITHSANDHYA.COM 6/19/2017 Wellbeing is a multidimensional construct, ensuring a balance in life by considering all aspects

More information

CHANGING BEHAVIOUR SHOULD WE FOCUS ON THE INDIVIDUAL? THE EXAMPLE OF ENVIRONMENTALLY-RELEVANT BEHAVIOURS

CHANGING BEHAVIOUR SHOULD WE FOCUS ON THE INDIVIDUAL? THE EXAMPLE OF ENVIRONMENTALLY-RELEVANT BEHAVIOURS CHANGING BEHAVIOUR SHOULD WE FOCUS ON THE INDIVIDUAL? THE EXAMPLE OF ENVIRONMENTALLY-RELEVANT BEHAVIOURS DR. BENJAMIN GARDNER 11TH JUNE 2015 Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience Department

More information

Sample Report. Sample Report Report. Fa c i l i tat or s (05/13) 180

Sample Report. Sample Report Report. Fa c i l i tat or s (05/13) 180 Sample Report Report Sample Report Fa c i l i tat or s R E P O R T F A C I L I T A T O R S R E P O RT (05/13) 180 PREPARE/ENRICH Customized Version Facilitator: David H. Olson Date Completed: 09/26/11

More information

WORKING PAPER ON SEXUAL HEALTH AND SCIENCE

WORKING PAPER ON SEXUAL HEALTH AND SCIENCE WORKING PAPER ON SEXUAL HEALTH AND SCIENCE Coordinators: Laura Carpenter & Steven Epstein I. INTRODUCTION At our meetings during the Crossing Boundaries, Workshopping Sexualities conference sponsored by

More information

Professor Tony Ward. Empathy, altruism and the treatment of sex offenders.

Professor Tony Ward. Empathy, altruism and the treatment of sex offenders. Professor Tony Ward Empathy, altruism and the treatment of sex offenders. Key References Law, D. R. & Ward, T. (2011). Desistance from sexual offending: Alternatives to throwing away the keys. New York,

More information

Expressions of the Self in Individualistic vs. Collective Cultures: a cross-cultural-perspective teaching module

Expressions of the Self in Individualistic vs. Collective Cultures: a cross-cultural-perspective teaching module Psychology Learning and Teaching Volume 11 Number 3 2012 www.wwwords.co.uk/plat Expressions of the Self in Individualistic vs. Collective Cultures: a cross-cultural-perspective teaching module BERNARDO

More information

ABAP Core Competencies for Psychoanalysis

ABAP Core Competencies for Psychoanalysis ABAP 2017 ABAP Core Competencies for Psychoanalysis AMERICAN BOARD FOR ACCREDITATION IN PSYCHOANALYSIS CORE COMPETENCIES SUB-COMMITTEE, COMMITTEE ON ACCREDITATION ABAP 2017 Background for the Categories

More information

Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the nomological network of social axioms

Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the nomological network of social axioms Personality and Individual Differences 41 (2006) 201 211 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the nomological network of social axioms Sylvia Xiaohua

More information

Knowledge Building Part I Common Language LIVING GLOSSARY

Knowledge Building Part I Common Language LIVING GLOSSARY Knowledge Building Part I Common Language LIVING GLOSSARY Community: A group of people who share some or all of the following: socio-demographics, geographic boundaries, sense of membership, culture, language,

More information

Culture and Consumer Behavior

Culture and Consumer Behavior CHAPTER 25 Culture and Consumer Behavior Sharon Shavitt, Hyewon Cho, and Aaron J. Barnes Consumer behavior and marketing are global phenomena, and understanding how consumers around the world respond to

More information

Chapter Two: Cross-Cultural Research Methods

Chapter Two: Cross-Cultural Research Methods Chapter Two: Cross-Cultural Research Methods Types of Cross-Cultural Research: Method Validation Studies... *Validy refers to whether or not a scale, test or measure accurately measures what it is supposed

More information

Psy2005: Applied Research Methods & Ethics in Psychology. Week 14: An Introduction to Qualitative Research

Psy2005: Applied Research Methods & Ethics in Psychology. Week 14: An Introduction to Qualitative Research Psy2005: Applied Research Methods & Ethics in Psychology Week 14: An Introduction to Qualitative Research 1 Learning Outcomes Outline the General Principles of Qualitative Research Compare and contrast

More information

Maintaining and Improving Motivation. Presented by: Dr. Sal Massa

Maintaining and Improving Motivation. Presented by: Dr. Sal Massa Maintaining and Improving Motivation Presented by: Dr. Sal Massa Today s Objectives Talk about motivational issues. Discuss some theory. Field your questions. Develop some practical strategies. What is

More information

PERSON PERCEPTION AND INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION

PERSON PERCEPTION AND INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION Person Perception and Interpersonal Attraction MODULE-V 22 PERSON PERCEPTION AND INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION We have already noted, achieving a sense of self is an important achievement. A neonate may not

More information

WSC 2018 SCIENCE. Science of Memory

WSC 2018 SCIENCE. Science of Memory WSC 2018 SCIENCE Science of Memory Schema 101 A schema describes a pattern of thought or behavior that organizes categories of information and the relationships among them. It can also be described as

More information

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16)

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16) MSP Feedback Guide 2009 Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16) Feedback to Test Takers Introduction (page 2 of the report) The Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16) measures aspects of your

More information

Culture & Personality. Outline. How Do We Compare? Kimberley A. Clow. What is Culture Approaches to Culture. Summary. Comparative Approach

Culture & Personality. Outline. How Do We Compare? Kimberley A. Clow. What is Culture Approaches to Culture. Summary. Comparative Approach Culture & Personality Kimberley A. Clow kclow2@uwo.ca http://instruct.uwo.ca/psychology/257e-570 Office Hour: Thursdays 2-3pm Office: S302 Outline What is Culture Approaches to Culture Comparative Approach

More information

VOLUME B. Elements of Psychological Treatment

VOLUME B. Elements of Psychological Treatment VOLUME B Elements of Psychological Treatment Module 2 Motivating clients for treatment and addressing resistance Approaches to change Principles of Motivational Interviewing How to use motivational skills

More information

CHAPTER OUTLINE CHAPTER SUMMARY

CHAPTER OUTLINE CHAPTER SUMMARY CHAPTER 13 THE SELF-REGULATION PERSPECTIVE CHAPTER OUTLINE From Cognition to Behavior Intentions Goals Goal Setting Implementation Intentions and the Importance of Strategies Deliberative and Implemental

More information

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR)

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR) Discussion Questions The concept of IR INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR) 1. Define interpersonal relationship. 2. List types of interpersonal relationship. 3. What are the advantages and disadvantages of

More information

Philosophy of Physical Activity

Philosophy of Physical Activity chapter Chapter 5 Philosophy of Physical Activity 5 Philosophy of Physical Activity Scott Kretchmar and Cesar R. Torres Philosophical Thinking in Physical Activity Reflection is the key. Various types

More information

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH Labovitz School of Business & Economics, University of Minnesota Duluth, 11 E. Superior Street, Suite 210, Duluth, MN 55802 Motivational Influences in Consumer Behavior:

More information

SUMMARY chapter 1 chapter 2

SUMMARY chapter 1 chapter 2 SUMMARY In the introduction of this thesis (chapter 1) the various meanings contributed to the concept of 'dignity' within the field of health care are shortly described. A fundamental distinction can

More information

GE SLO: Ethnic-Multicultural Studies Results

GE SLO: Ethnic-Multicultural Studies Results GE SLO: Ethnic-Multicultural Studies Results Background: This study was designed to assess the following College s Ethnic/Multicultural Studies General Education SLO. Upon completion of this GE requirement

More information

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration Dr. Chan-Chien Chiu, Associate Professor of Taiwan Hospitality & Tourism College, Taiwan ABSTRACT This

More information

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) 1 VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) Thank you for providing us with the opportunity to revise our paper. We have revised the manuscript according to the editors and

More information

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question Trivia Question Where did win one for the gipper come from? Chapter 9 What are the 3 components of motivation? 3 major categories of motivation. Major theories of motivation. How the theories are applied

More information

Core Competencies for Peer Workers in Behavioral Health Services

Core Competencies for Peer Workers in Behavioral Health Services Core Competencies for Peer Workers in Behavioral Health Services Category I: Engages peers in collaborative and caring relationships This category of competencies emphasized peer workers' ability to initiate

More information

Personality means how a person affects others and how he understands and views himself as well as the pattern of inner and outer measurable traits,

Personality means how a person affects others and how he understands and views himself as well as the pattern of inner and outer measurable traits, Personality means how a person affects others and how he understands and views himself as well as the pattern of inner and outer measurable traits, and the person situation interaction. A person s general

More information

Personal Talent Skills Inventory

Personal Talent Skills Inventory Personal Talent Skills Inventory Sales Version Inside Sales Sample Co. 5-30-2013 Introduction Research suggests that the most effective people are those who understand themselves, both their strengths

More information

What is Social Psychology

What is Social Psychology What is Social Psychology Social psychology is a scientific concept that seeks to explain how the thoughts, feelings, and behaviors of individuals are influenced by the presence of others, or lack of others.

More information

Sociology Matters. Culture and Socialization. Culture and Socialization. What is Culture? What is Culture? 9/5/2012

Sociology Matters. Culture and Socialization. Culture and Socialization. What is Culture? What is Culture? 9/5/2012 2-2 Richard T. Schaefer Chapter Two: Culture and Socialization Sociology Matters Fifth Edition McGraw-Hill/Irwin Culture and Socialization 2-3 Culture and Socialization 2-4 What is Culture? Around the

More information

Chapter 01: The Study of the Person

Chapter 01: The Study of the Person Chapter 01: The Study of the Person MULTIPLE CHOICE 1. Which of the following is NOT part of the psychological triad? a. behavior c. psychological health b. thoughts d. feelings C DIF: Easy REF: The Study

More information

MIDTERM EXAMINATION Spring 2009 MGT502- Organizational Behaviour (Session - 2) Question No: 1 ( Marks: 1 ) - Please choose one Which one of the following is NOT a characteristic of group in organization?

More information

Breaking the Bias Habit. Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute

Breaking the Bias Habit. Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute Breaking the Bias Habit Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute P e r c e n t W o m e n Percent Women Bachelor's Degrees, Selected Fields

More information

Understanding Consumer Usage of Product Magnitudes Through Sorting Tasks

Understanding Consumer Usage of Product Magnitudes Through Sorting Tasks Cornell University School of Hotel Administration The Scholarly Commons Articles and Chapters School of Hotel Administration Collection 12-1999 Understanding Consumer Usage of Product Magnitudes Through

More information

CHARACTERISTICS OF THE GIFTED

CHARACTERISTICS OF THE GIFTED CHARACTERISTICS OF THE GIFTED Before you read the characteristics please keep in mind: This is only a partial list. Don t expect your child to display all or even most of these characteristics. There are

More information

TTI SUCCESS INSIGHTS Personal Interests, Attitudes and Values TM

TTI SUCCESS INSIGHTS Personal Interests, Attitudes and Values TM TTI SUCCESS INSIGHTS Personal Interests, Attitudes and Values TM "He who knows others is learned. He who knows himself is wise." Lao Tse CATHERINE 2-29-2008 Sculpt your Destiny 4545 Contour blvd. #B San

More information

Social Psychology 220

Social Psychology 220 Social Psychology 220 Social Psychology: #3 Winter 09-10 Bruce J. Diamond, ME.d, Ph.D. Office: Science Hall # 240 Office Cyberspace Phone: 973-720 x3400 Email: diamondb@wpunj.edu Room: Cyberspace Course

More information

Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and

Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and private study only. The thesis may not be reproduced elsewhere

More information

Psychological Approach to Comparative Education Aneela Farooq Afshan Nisar

Psychological Approach to Comparative Education Aneela Farooq Afshan Nisar Psychological Approach to Comparative Education Aneela Farooq Afshan Nisar Psychology Psychology is the scientific study of the mind and behaviour. Psychology is a multifaceted discipline and includes

More information

Core Competencies for Peer Workers in Behavioral Health Services

Core Competencies for Peer Workers in Behavioral Health Services BRINGING RECOVERY SUPPORTS TO SCALE Technical Assistance Center Strategy (BRSS TACS) Core Competencies for Peer Workers in Behavioral Health Services OVERVIEW In 2015, SAMHSA led an effort to identify

More information