Evidence for a Magnitude Effect in Temporal Discounting With Pigeons

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Evidence for a Magnitude Effect in Temporal Discounting With Pigeons"

Transcription

1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2012, Vol. 38, No. 1, American Psychological Association /12/$12.00 DOI: /a BRIEF REPORT Evidence for a Magnitude Effect in Temporal Discounting With Pigeons Randolph C. Grace University of Canterbury Rebecca J. Sargisson University of Waikato K. Geoffrey White University of Otago A magnitude effect in human intertemporal choice is well established larger rewards or outcomes are discounted over time at a lower rate than are smaller rewards. However, many recent studies have failed to find a corresponding effect in nonhuman animals. Here we report a magnitude effect in temporal discounting for pigeons choices involving a tradeoff between reward delay and amount. Pigeons chose between a small reward (1-s access to food) after a 2-s delay, and a large reward (4.5-s access to food) after a 28-s delay. Across conditions, the delays to the small and large rewards were increased or decreased, respectively. Temporal discounting functions obtained through a value-estimation procedure showed clear evidence of a magnitude effect: The value of the large reward decreased more slowly with increasing delay than the value of the small reward. We linked this result to a nonlinear relationship between choice and the delays associated with the small and large rewards. The nonlinearity was contrary to the generalized matching law but was predicted by the contextual choice model. Our results confirm the existence of a magnitude effect in nonhuman temporal discounting, showing that this adaptation is not unique to humans. Keywords: temporal discounting, magnitude effect, self-control, choice, pigeons Research on intertemporal choice investigates the effect of time in decision making. Many choices involve tradeoffs between the amount of a reward and its delay from the time of choice. It is now well established that the value of a reward is discounted according to a hyperbolic function of delay for both humans and nonhumans (Ainslie, 1974; Green, Fisher, Perlow, & Sherman, 1981; Kirby, 1997: Mazur, 1984; Myerson & Green, 1995; Rodriquez & Logue, 1988; see Green & Myerson, 2004, for review). One of the most pervasive phenomena in intertemporal choice is the so-called magnitude effect: Larger rewards are discounted over time at a lower rate than are smaller rewards. The magnitude effect has been obtained with humans choosing between different kinds of outcomes, including money (Benzion, Rapoport, & Yagil, 1988; Grace & McLean, 2005; Green, Myerson, & McFadden, 1997; Myerson & Green, 1995; Thaler, 1981), health (Chapman, 1996), tipping scenarios (Chapman & Winquist, 1998), employment (Schoenfelder & Hantula, 2003), and personal relationships (Tayler, Arantes, & Grace, 2009). Randolph C. Grace, Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand; Rebecca J. Sargisson, Department of Psychology, University of Waikato, Hamilton, New Zealand; and K. Geoffrey White, Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Randolph C. Grace, Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand, or K. Geoffrey White, Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. randolph.grace@canterbury.ac.nz or geoff.white@otago.ac.nz Although the magnitude effect is a robust finding for humans, research with nonhumans has failed to find similar results. Richards, Mitchell, de Wit, and Seiden (1997, Experiment 3) used a titration procedure to determine rats indifference points for a standard, delayed amount of water that is, the amount of water available immediately that was equally preferred to the standard, delayed, amount. Across conditions, the standard amounts (100 l, 150 l, and 200 l) and delays (0 s, 2 s, 4 s, 8 s, and 16 s) were varied factorially. Richards et al. found that the present value of the standard reward (defined as the point of indifference between immediately available water and the delayed standard amount, divided by the standard amount) decreased with delay and did not differ significantly depending on the standard amount. Green, Myerson, Holt, Slevin, and Estle (2004) used a similar adjustingamount procedure to determine indifference points for rats and pigeons for various amount delay combinations. Green et al. also found that the rate of discounting was not affected by reward amount. Similarly, Freeman, Green, Myerson, and Woolverton (2009) did not find an effect of reward amount on temporal discounting in rhesus monkeys. Additionally, the magnitude effect in rats does not occur for rewards differing in quality as well as amount (Calvert, Green, & Myerson, 2010), in contrast with human studies where there are clear magnitude effects for outcomes differing in quality (Estle, Green, Myerson, & Holt, 2007), and amount (Green & Myerson, 2004). The above procedures involved a series of discrete trial choices from which indifference points are inferred. Grace (1999) used an alternative method to test for a magnitude effect in nonhuman temporal discounting. He used a concurrent chains procedure in 102

2 A MAGNITUDE EFFECT IN NONHUMAN ANIMALS 103 which pigeons responded to two concurrently available keys in a choice phase to produce access to one of two schedules of food reinforcement in the outcome phase. An advantage of the procedure is that preference is sensitive to differences in the outcome schedules. When the keys in the choice phase were red, the amount of food reinforcement was small for both alternatives, whereas when the keys were green the amount of reinforcement was large. The average delay to reinforcement associated with both outcomes was changed across conditions. The pigeons preferred the outcome with the shorter delay to a similar extent regardless of whether the reinforcement amounts were large or small, as measured by the relative response rate to the concurrently available keys. That is, there was no evidence for a magnitude effect. Taken together, the above studies appear to provide strong evidence against the existence of a magnitude effect in nonhumans, because results were similar for different species (rats, pigeons, and monkeys), types of reward (liquid and food), and choice procedures (adjusting delay, adjusting amount, and concurrent chains). However, we were skeptical about this conclusion for two reasons. First, there is considerable evidence of cross-species generality in research on choice: Not only do variables such as delay, amount, rate, and probability of reward have qualitatively similar effects across species, but quantitatively as well. For example, a hyperbolic function provides an excellent description of the relationship between reward value and delay for every species that has been studied including pigeons, rats, monkeys, and humans (see Green & Myerson, 2004, for review). Second, the magnitude effect is adaptive because it increases the likelihood of maximizing the long-term benefit to the organism when faced with a tradeoff between amount and delay of reward. These situations are often described as involving self-control (Rachlin, 2000) because the individual can either choose impulsively by selecting the smaller, more immediate reward, or show self-control by choosing the larger, more delayed reward. It seemed unlikely to us that such a fitness-enhancing adaptation would not have evolved in nonhumans, particularly as no theoretical explanation for the magnitude effect in humans has emerged that would attribute it to an ability that might not be shared by nonhumans (e.g., language). Here we asked whether pigeons would show a magnitude effect in a self-control situation in which they faced a tradeoff between delay and amount of reward. We used a concurrent-chains procedure so that pigeons would repeatedly experience the same delay amount combinations, as compared with titration or adjusting procedures where delay amount combinations change within sessions. The baseline condition was a choice between a smaller, sooner reward (SS; 1-s access to grain delayed by 2 s) and a larger, later reward (LL; 4.5-s access to grain delayed by 28 s). Across conditions, when the choice keys were red the SS delay was constant while the LL delay was increased. When the choice keys were green, the LL delay was constant while the SS delay was increased. By holding one delay constant and varying the other it was possible to trace out temporal discounting functions. Using a simple method for preference scaling described below, we obtained discounting functions for both the small and large amount outcomes over the range of delays from 2 s to 28 s.the critical question was whether the value of the small outcome would decrease more rapidly as a function of increasing delay, indicative of a magnitude effect. Subjects Method Five pigeons (Columba livia), between 8 and 13 years of age were individually housed and had free access to water and grit. The pigeons were weighed daily and fed a mixture of wheat, corn, peas, and pellets if their weight fell below 85% of their free-feeding weights. All pigeons had previous experience with delayed matching-to-sample procedures (but not concurrent chains) and required no training. Apparatus Five Med Associates Inc. chambers, each measuring 295 mm high 295 mm wide 245 mm deep, were used. Three translucent plastic response keys, 21 mm in diameter, were recessed 10 mm into the front panel of each chamber, 210 mm from the grid floor, and 60 mm apart. Only left and right keys were used and could be illuminated red, green, or white. The keys required a force of at least 0.15 N to be operated. A Gerbrands hopper situated behind an aperture 125 mm below the center key provided access to wheat when raised. An infrared beam inside the hopper aperture was broken when the pigeon inserted its head, allowing precise timing of food delivery from the time of head entry. The hopper was illuminated with a 1-W white bulb when raised. A computer linked to the chambers via a Med Associates Inc. interface presented stimuli and recorded the pigeons responses. Procedure Each session lasted for 60 trials or 90 minutes, whichever came first. Sessions were conducted 7 days a week. Each trial began with both side keys lit either red or green, signaling the beginning of the choice phase. Trials with red and green in the choice phase directly alternated throughout the session. Responses to left and right keys during the choice phase were recorded and a single response on one of the keys allowed access to an outcome phase at intervals averaging 30 s according to a variable interval (VI) schedule. Variable interval schedules were arranged concurrently on left and right keys, separately on red and green trials. The variable interval for each key on each trial (red or green) was chosen randomly from one of four lists of 12 intervals constructed from Fleshler and Hoffman s (1962) progression. The schedules on left and right keys were nonindependent, in that when the interval programmed for the schedule on one key timed out and stopped the timer for that key, thus setting up an opportunity for access to the outcome phase, the timer for the schedule on the other key also stopped. Access to the outcome phase on each trial, therefore, was only possible via a response on the key associated with the timed-out interval. As a result, the different events in the outcome phases were experienced equally often in each session. This procedure was first suggested by Stubbs and Pliskoff (1969) and allows preference in the choice phase to be influenced by amount and delay of reward in the outcome phase but not by frequency of reward which is kept equal by the procedure. With VI 30-s schedules, the average time spent in the choice phase on each trial was 15 s.

3 104 GRACE, SARGISSON, AND WHITE If the outcome phase was entered via a left key peck, the left key was then lit white and the right key was dark. The right key was lit white (and the left key dark) if entry was via the right key. In the outcome phase, the first response after a fixed interval (FI) of time had elapsed was followed by food delivery. In five separate conditions, different FI schedules operated on the left and right keys in the outcome phase following red and green choice phases. The FI schedules in the different conditions are listed in Table 1. The five pigeons completed the five conditions in different orders according to a Latin square design in which each condition occupied each of five places in the order across the five pigeons. Each condition was conducted for a minimum of 20 sessions. After completing all five conditions, each pigeon repeated the five conditions in the same order, thus completing at least 200 sessions. When the fixed interval in the outcome phase timed out, different durations of access to wheat followed left and right responses. In both red and green components, the first response on the left key after the FI had elapsed was rewarded with 1-s access to wheat and the first response on the right key with 4.5-s access to wheat. An intertrial interval (ITI) followed food delivery on each trial. The ITI duration was chosen so that the interval from the beginning of the outcome phase and the beginning of the next choice phase was 60 s. Results Table 1 Outcome Phase Schedules and Reinforcer Durations in Red and Green Components Condition Red Green Left Right Left Right 1 FI2s FI2s FI2s FI28s 2 FI2s FI6s FI6s FI28s 3 FI 2 s FI 15 s FI 15 s FI 28 s 4 FI 2 s FI 24 s FI 24 s FI 28 s 5 FI 2 s FI 28 s FI 28 s FI 28 s Reinforcer duration 1 s 4.5 s 1 s 4.5 s Figure 1. The upper panel shows response allocation, measured as the logarithm of the ratio of responses made to the left and right alternatives, as a function of the logarithm of the FI schedule value associated with the 4.5-s outcome (red component; filled squares) or 1-s outcome (green component; unfilled squares). Data are averaged across pigeons, and bars indicate one standard error. The center and lower panels show, respectively, predictions of the generalized matching law (GML; Equation 1) and contextual choice model (CCM; Equation 2) for the average data. Model predictions are shown by dashed lines. See text for more explanation. Responses to the left and right keys were summed over the last five sessions in each condition. Because an analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed that there were no significant effects or interactions related to replication, data were aggregated across replications. The top panel of Figure 1 shows preference for the SS outcome relative to the delayed outcome, measured as the logarithm of the ratio of responses to the left and right keys. The preference measure in Figure 1 was averaged across pigeons separately for responding in the red and green components, and was plotted as a function of the logarithm of the FI schedule value for the changing alternative (log delay). An ANOVA with delay and component as factors found a significant effect of component, F(1, 4) 9.38, p.05, and a significant Component Delay interaction, F(4, 16) 30.36, p.001. The main effect of component indicated that preference for the left alternative was overall greater when the delay associated with the SS outcome was 2 s, and the interaction confirmed that as expected, preference for the SS outcome in the red component increased as the delay for the LL outcome increased, but decreased in the green component as the delay for the SS outcome increased. Our primary goal was to determine if the rates of temporal discounting differed for the large and small rewards. To do this we used a procedure to obtain estimates of relative value for each of the amount delay combinations. We assumed that choice in the initial links was proportional to the relative value of the outcome schedules (the validity of this assumption is examined in the Discussion). The value of the SS outcome with 2-s delay was

4 A MAGNITUDE EFFECT IN NONHUMAN ANIMALS 105 arbitrarily assigned a modulus of 1, and the values of the nine other amount-delay combinations (1 s amount, 6-, 15-, 24-, and 28-s delays; and 4.5-s amount and 2-, 6-, 15-, 24-, and 28-s delays) were estimated as free parameters using nonlinear optimization, such that the variance accounted for in data for individual pigeons was maximized. The estimated values provided a good description of results, accounting for an average of 95% of the variance in data for individual pigeons. The left panel of Figure 2 shows the average values for the large (4.5 s; red component) and small (1 s; green component) rewards, as a function of delay (FI schedule). The right panel shows normalized data, obtained by dividing the value for delays greater than 2 s by the value for the reward with 2-s delay. For all delays, the relative value of the large reward was greater than the corresponding relative value of the small reward. An ANOVA with delay and amount as factors confirmed a significant Amount Delay interaction, F(3, 12) 4.25, p.05, and a main effect of amount, F(1, 4) 7.25, p.05. Post hoc tests (Fisher least significant difference) showed that the relative value of the large reward was significantly greater (p.05) than the small reward for delays of 6, 15, and 24 s, whereas the difference for 28 s was not significant (p.11). This confirms that a magnitude effect was obtained in the present data, as the value of the large reward decreased more slowly with delay than the value of the small reward. Finally we asked whether the data were compatible with models for behavioral choice (see Grace & Hucks, in press, for review). As applied to the current procedure, the generalized matching law (GML) predicts that response allocation, measured as the logarithm of the response ratio, is determined according to the following equation (Baum, 1974; Baum & Rachlin, 1969; Logue, Rodriguez, Pena Correal & Mauro, 1984): log B L a B D log 1/D R a R 1/D M log A L log b, (1) L A R where B indicates responses, and D and A indicate delay and amount of reward, subscripted for the left and right alternatives. There are three parameters: Sensitivity to delay (a D ) and amount of reward (a M ), and response bias (log b). Thus according to the GML, response allocation is determined additively by the log delay and amount ratios, each weighted by a sensitivity parameter, and response bias. Grace (1994) proposed an extension of the generalized matching law, called the contextual choice model (CCM) because it assumed that delay and amount sensitivities were affected by the temporal context of choice the average times spent in the choice and outcome phases of the concurrent chains procedure. The corresponding equation for CCM is log B L T o B R T D log 1/D R a c a 1/D M log A L L A R logb, (2) where T o and T c are the average times spent, per trial, in the outcome and choice phases of the procedure. According to CCM, sensitivity to delay and amount depend on the relative time spent in the outcome and choice phases, with sensitivities increasing as the duration of the outcome phases increases relative to the duration of the choice phase. Figure 1 shows fits of the GML (center panel) and CCM (bottom panel) to the average data. Overall, CCM provided a better fit, accounting for 91% of the variance (root mean square error [RMSE] 0.15), as compared with the GML which accounted for 78% (RMSE 0.24). CCM predicts a nonlinear relationship such that choice becomes more extreme at longer overall durations, which is the source of the magnitude effect in the present data. Consistent with CCM, choice for the larger reward was greater when the delays were 28 s 28 s than when they were 2 s 2 s. Indeed, it has been shown earlier that sensitivity to amount increases with increasing delay in the outcome phase (White & Pipe, 1987). Thus, CCM is able to predict the magnitude effect for pigeons choice in concurrent chains as a consequence of an interaction between delay and amount, such that sensitivity depends on the overall delay duration. Discussion Contrary to previous studies, we found clear evidence of a magnitude effect with pigeons: The relative value of a large reward Figure 2. The left panel shows values estimated for the 4.5-s outcome (red component) and 1-s outcome (green component). The value of the 1-s outcome, 2-s delay was set to a modulus of 1. The right panel shows normalized values obtained by dividing the value for delays 2 s by the value of the outcome at 2 s ( present value ). Data are averaged across pigeons, and bars indicate one standard error.

5 106 GRACE, SARGISSON, AND WHITE (4.5-s access to food) decreased more slowly as its delay increased from 2 s to 28 s than the corresponding value of a small reward (1-s access to food). Values were estimated from response ratios when pigeons pecked two concurrently available keys to produce the large and small rewards. The magnitude effect occurred because preference (measured as the log response ratio) varied with delay and amount ratios, but the relationship was nonlinear: Sensitivity to relative delay and amount increased as overall delay increased. This nonlinearity was contrary to the generalized matching law (Baum, 1974) but was predicted by the contextual choice model (Grace, 1994). In demonstrating the magnitude effect in pigeons, we are reassured that, as for many other behavioral phenomena, nonhuman animals exhibit an adaptive response which can maximize long-term benefit to the organism in a choice situation involving a trade-off between amount and delay of reward. The assumption that initial-link responding in concurrent chains can be used to measure the value of the terminal-link schedules has been controversial (for review see Grace & Savastano, 2000). The major problem was Fantino s (1969) result that initial-link duration affected preference independently of the terminal-link schedules. These difficulties prompted Mazur (1984) to introduce the adjusting-delay procedure, in which preference is registered with a single response. Mazur s work was highly influential, as similar discrete-trial procedures that yield indifference points and the hyperbolic-decay model that he proposed have been widely used in subsequent research on discounting with humans (Green & Myerson, 2004). The effect of initial-link duration means that choice in concurrent chains cannot provide a measure of absolute terminal-link value. However, the magnitude effect is a change in relative value specifically, that the value of a large reward decreases more slowly relative to its immediate value than does the relative value of a small reward. There is no reason to expect that our results would have been fundamentally different if, for example, a longer initial-link schedule had been used. Simulations using fits of CCM to the present data show that a similar magnitude effect would have been obtained using a wide range of initial-link durations. Because preference for the larger over smaller reward was more extreme when delays to both were 28 s than 2 s, if response allocation is interpreted as measuring relative value, then the value of the smaller reward must have decreased more as delay increased from2sto28s.butnote that if value is defined in terms of CCM, that is, reward amounts and delays and the associated sensitivity parameters, there would be no magnitude effect because CCM predicts the nonlinearity in Figure 1 with the same sensitivity parameters applying to both rewards. Thus, our conclusion depends on defining value directly in terms of response allocation. Although research on temporal discounting with titration procedures and on choice with free-operant procedures like concurrent chains has evolved somewhat independently, the value estimation method used here offers a way to integrate research in these domains. It yields estimates of relative value from responding in concurrent chains that could be compared, in principle, with results from titration procedures. Although concurrent chains has not featured prominently in discounting research, it provides an alternative method for assessing the relationship between delay and value that is arguably no less valid than the adjusting-delay procedure. Grace (1996) showed that pairs of fixed and variable-delay indifference points obtained for individual pigeons from the adjusting-delay procedure were equally preferred as terminal links in concurrent chains, suggesting that the determiners of schedule value were the same in the two procedures. Their similarity is further underscored by the fact that models which assume a common relationship between delay and value are able to describe results from both procedures (Grace, 1996; Mazur, 2001). Finally, it is worth noting that indifference points in the adjusting-delay procedure are also affected by factors other than reward value, including trial order (Robles, Vargas, & Bejarano, 2009) and contrast effects (Dai, Grace, & Kemp, 2009). Thus, both procedures should be viewed as providing measures of relative, not absolute value. Why were our results different from previous research with nonhumans? Richards et al. (1997); Green et al. (2004); Calvert et al. (2010) and others used an adjusting-amount or titration procedure to obtain indifference points for delayed rewards in rats and pigeons. On each trial the animals make a single choice between an outcome with a fixed amount and delay to reward ( standard ), and an outcome with an immediate reward but for which the amount is gradually adjusted over trials. If animals choose the adjusting alternative, then its amount is decreased, whereas if they choose the standard alternative, the adjusting amount is increased. In these studies, discounting functions obtained after substantial training with a standard amount-delay combination did not change with the size of the standard amount. For example, if rats indifference point for a reward of 5 pellets with an 8-s delay was 2.5 pellets, then the indifference point for a 20-pellet reward with an 8-s delay would be 10 pellets. By contrast, if a magnitude effect had been present then the indifference point for the 20-pellet reward would be larger, say 15 pellets. Thus a magnitude effect would be adaptive because it would increase the overall amount of food the animal earned during the session. The absence of a magnitude effect in previous studies might reflect the relative insensitivity of choice to the size of the standard delayed amount which varies across conditions, although it is possible that the animals discriminate the contingency between their choice and changes in the adjusting reward. In the present experiment, the standard amounts were varied within sessions (across red and green choice trials). Accordingly, the contrast between within-session self control choices (SS verses LL) was emphasized. An additional factor that might contribute to the absence of a magnitude effect in titration or adjusting procedures is related to the time between trials. Unlike for humans, who when given appropriate instructions can specify an indifference point directly (e.g., Chapman, 1996), indifference points for animals must be inferred from a series of choices. Typically, these choices oscillate between runs for the standard alternative and runs for the adjusting alternative, with the indifference point estimated as the average (Mazur, 1987). Previous studies have shown that rats and pigeons are more sensitive to reward amounts in discrete-trial choice procedures when the response opportunities are more closely spaced in time (e.g., Mazur, 2010; Williams, 1992). Recently, Mazur and Biondi (2011) showed that the indifference point obtained from an adjusting-delay procedure increased with increasing time between trials. It is possible that evidence for a magnitude

6 A MAGNITUDE EFFECT IN NONHUMAN ANIMALS 107 effect would be obtained if a short ITI was used in the adjustingamount procedure. Our results show that the magnitude effect is not an adaptation unique to humans. Although several explanations for the magnitude effect in humans have been proposed, none has been generally accepted. These include separate mental accounts for large and small rewards (Thaler, 1981), specific characteristics of the utility function for money ( increasing proportional sensitivity, IPS; Loewenstein & Prelec, 1992), and the reward differential hypothesis (i.e., that choices are more likely to be made on the basis of reward differences rather than ratios when the magnitudes are large; Grace & McLean, 2005). However, these accounts have not withstood empirical scrutiny (see Grace & McLean, 2005; Green et al., 1997). Recently, Killeen (2009) proposed a general model for discounting which assumes that the effects of utility and time are subtractive. Killeen s theory is able to predict the magnitude effect even though the scaling parameter for time is independent of reward amount. It is interesting to note that in this respect, Killeen s account is similar to CCM, which assumes that delay and amount sensitivities are constant but their effects are modulated by overall duration. Developing an integrated account of the magnitude effect with potential generality for both humans and nonhumans is an important goal for future research. References Ainslie, G. W. (1974). Impulse control in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, doi: /jeab Baum, W. M., & Rachlin, H. C. (1969). Choice as time allocation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, doi: / jeab Baum, W. M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, doi: /jeab Benzion, U., Rapoport, A., & Yagil, J. (1988). Discount rates inferred from decisions: An experimental study. Management Science, 35, doi: /mnsc Calvert, A. L., Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2010). Delay discounting of qualitatively different reinforcers in rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, doi: /jeab Chapman, G. B., & Winquist, J. R. (1998). The magnitude effect: Temporal discount rates and restaurant tips. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, doi: /bf Chapman, G. B. (1996). Temporal discounting and utility for health and money. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, doi: / Dai, Z., Grace, R. C., & Kemp, S. (2009). Reward contrast in delay and probability discounting. Learning & Behavior, 37, doi: /LB Estle, S. J., Green, L., Myerson, J., & Holt, D. D. (2007). Discounting of monetary and directly consumable rewards. Psychological Science, 18, doi: /j x Fantino, E. (1969). Choice and rate of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, doi: / jeab Fleshler, M., & Hoffman, H. S. (1962). A progression for generating variable-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, doi: /jeab Freeman, K., Green, L., Myerson, J., & Woolverton, W. (2009). Delay discounting of saccharin in rhesus monkeys. Behavioural Processes, 82, doi: /j.beproc Grace, R. C., & Hucks, A. D. (in press). The allocation of operant behavior. In K. A. Lattal, T. D. Hackenberg, & G. J. Madden (Eds.), Handbook of behavior analysis. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Grace, R. C., & McLean, A. P. (2005). Integrated versus segregated accounting and the magnitude effect in temporal discounting. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, doi: /bf Grace, R. C., & Savastano, H. I. (2000). Temporal context and conditioned reinforcement value. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, doi: / Grace, R. C. (1994). A contextual model of concurrent-chains choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, doi: /jeab Grace, R. C. (1996). Choice between fixed and variable delays to reinforcement in the adjusting-delay procedure and concurrent chains. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, doi: / Grace, R. C. (1999). The matching law and amount-dependent exponential discounting as accounts of self-control choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, doi: /jeab Green, L., Fisher, E. B. Jr., Perlow, S., & Sherman, L. (1981). Preference reversal and self-control: Choice as a function of reward amount and delay. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 1, Green, L., Myerson, J., & McFadden, E. (1997). Rate of temporal discounting decreases with amount of reward. Memory & Cognition, 25, doi: /bf Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, doi: / Green, L., Myerson, L., Holt, D. D., Slevin, J. R., & Estle, S. J. (2004). Discounting of delayed food rewards in pigeons and rats: Is there a magnitude effect? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, doi: /jeab Killeen, P. R. (2009). An additive-utility model of delay discounting. Psychological Review, 116, doi: /a Kirby, K. N. (1997). Bidding on the future: Evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, doi: / Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (1992). Anomalies in intertemporal choice: Evidence and an interpretation. In G. Loewenstein & J. Elster (Eds.), Choice over time (pp ). New York, NY: Sage. Logue, A. W., Rodriguez, M. L., Pena Correal, T. E., & Mauro, B. (1984). Choice in a self-control paradigm: Quantification of experienced-based differences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, doi: /jeab Mazur, J. E., & Biondi, D. R. (2011). Effects of time between trials on rats and pigeons choices with probabilistic delayed reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, doi: / jeab Mazur, J. E. (1984). Tests for an equivalence rule for fixed and variable reinforcer delays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, doi: / Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Vol. 5. The effects of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mazur, J. E. (2001). Hyperbolic value addition and general models of animal choice. Psychological Review, 108, doi: / x Mazur, J. E. (2010). Distributed versus exclusive preference in discretetrial choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 36, doi: /a Myerson, J., & Green, L. (1995). Discounting of delayed rewards: Models

7 108 GRACE, SARGISSON, AND WHITE of individual choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, doi: /jeab Rachlin, H. C. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Richards, J. B., Mitchell, S. H., de Wit, H., & Seiden, L. S. (1997). Determination of discount functions in rats with an adjusting-amount procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, doi: /jeab Robles, E., Vargas, P. A., & Bejarano, R. (2009). Within-subject differences in degree of delay discounting as a function of order of presentation of hypothetical cash rewards. Behavioural Processes, 81, doi: /j.beproc Rodriquez, M. L., & Logue, A. W. (1988). Adjusting delay to reinforcement: Comparing choice in pigeons and humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, doi: / Schoenfelder, T., & Hantula, D. A. (2003). A job with a future? Delay discounting, magnitude effects and domain independence of utility for career decisions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62, doi: /S (02) Online First Publication Stubbs, D. A., & Pliskoff, S. S. (1969). Concurrent responding with fixed relative rates of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, doi: /jeab Tayler, S., Arantes, J., & Grace, R. C. (2009). Temporal discounting for monetary and close relationship outcomes. Personal Relationships, 16, doi: /j x Thaler, R. (1981). Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Economics Letters, 8, doi: / (81) White, K. G., & Pipe, M. E. (1987). Sensitivity to reinforcer duration in a self-control procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, doi: /jeab Williams, B. A. (1992). Dissociation of theories of choice by temporal spacing of choice opportunities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 18, doi: / APA-published journal articles are now available Online First in the PsycARTICLES database. Electronic versions of journal articles will be accessible prior to the print publication, expediting access to the latest peer-reviewed research. All PsycARTICLES institutional customers, individual APA PsycNET database package subscribers, and individual journal subscribers may now search these records as an added benefit. Online First Publication (OFP) records can be released within as little as 30 days of acceptance and transfer into production, and are marked to indicate the posting status, allowing researchers to quickly and easily discover the latest literature. OFP articles will be the version of record; the articles have gone through the full production cycle except for assignment to an issue and pagination. After a journal issue s print publication, OFP records will be replaced with the final published article to reflect the final status and bibliographic information. Received March 28, 2011 Revision received July 11, 2011 Accepted August 4, 2011

Integrated versus segregated accounting and the magnitude effect in temporal discounting

Integrated versus segregated accounting and the magnitude effect in temporal discounting Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (4), 732-739 Integrated versus segregated accounting and the magnitude effect in temporal discounting RANDOLPH C. GRACE and ANTHONY P. MCLEAN University of Canterbury,

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

HHS Public Access Author manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 April 01.

HHS Public Access Author manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 April 01. Delay Discounting: Pigeon, Rat, Human Does it Matter? Ariana Vanderveldt, Luís Oliveira, and Leonard Green Washington University in St. Louis Abstract Delay discounting refers to the decrease in subjective

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

WITHIN-SUBJECT COMPARISON OF REAL AND HYPOTHETICAL MONEY REWARDS IN DELAY DISCOUNTING MATTHEW W. JOHNSON AND WARREN K. BICKEL

WITHIN-SUBJECT COMPARISON OF REAL AND HYPOTHETICAL MONEY REWARDS IN DELAY DISCOUNTING MATTHEW W. JOHNSON AND WARREN K. BICKEL JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2002, 77, 129 146 NUMBER 2(MARCH) WITHIN-SUBJECT COMPARISON OF REAL AND HYPOTHETICAL MONEY REWARDS IN DELAY DISCOUNTING MATTHEW W. JOHNSON AND WARREN K.

More information

Discounting in pigeons when the choice is between two delayed rewards: implications for species comparisons

Discounting in pigeons when the choice is between two delayed rewards: implications for species comparisons Original Research Article published: 17 August 2011 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00096 Discounting in pigeons when the choice is between two delayed rewards: implications for species comparisons Amanda L. Calvert,

More information

Discounting of Monetary and Directly Consumable Rewards Sara J. Estle, Leonard Green, Joel Myerson, and Daniel D. Holt

Discounting of Monetary and Directly Consumable Rewards Sara J. Estle, Leonard Green, Joel Myerson, and Daniel D. Holt PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Discounting of Monetary and Directly Consumable Rewards Sara J. Estle, Leonard Green, Joel Myerson, and Daniel D. Holt Washington University ABSTRACT We compared

More information

Amount of Reward Has Opposite Effects on the Discounting of Delayed and Probabilistic Outcomes

Amount of Reward Has Opposite Effects on the Discounting of Delayed and Probabilistic Outcomes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1999, Vol. 25, No. 2,418-427 Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/99/S3.0O Amount of Reward Has Opposite

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 January 01.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 January 01. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2015 January ; 41(1): 148 162. doi:10.1037/xlm0000029. Discounting of Monetary Rewards that are Both

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

A COMPARISON OF DISCOUNTING PARAMETERS OBTAINED THROUGH TWO DIFFERENT ADJUSTING PROCEDURES: BISECTION AND UP-DOWN. Thomas Anatol da Rocha Woelz

A COMPARISON OF DISCOUNTING PARAMETERS OBTAINED THROUGH TWO DIFFERENT ADJUSTING PROCEDURES: BISECTION AND UP-DOWN. Thomas Anatol da Rocha Woelz A COMPARISON OF DISCOUNTING PARAMETERS OBTAINED THROUGH TWO DIFFERENT ADJUSTING PROCEDURES: BISECTION AND UP-DOWN Thomas Anatol da Rocha Woelz Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 89 (2012) 212 218 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes j o ur nal homep age : www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Providing a reinforcement history

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Behavioural Processes 66 (2004) 213 233 Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Raymond C. Pitts, Stacy

More information

Decision biases in intertemporal choice and choice under uncertainty: Testing a common account

Decision biases in intertemporal choice and choice under uncertainty: Testing a common account Memory & Cognition 2006, 34 (3), 589-602 Decision biases in intertemporal choice and choice under uncertainty: Testing a common account GRETCHEN B. CHAPMAN and BETHANY J. WEBER Rutgers University, Piscataway,

More information

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement Learning & Behavior 2005, 33 (3), 309-323 Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement GLENN S. BROWN and K. GEOFFREY WHITE University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand In two experiments, pigeons responding

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND

ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2008, 89, 157 167 NUMBER 2(MARCH) ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND IDAHO STATE UNIVERSITY

More information

DELAYED REWARD AND COST DISCOUNTING. JAMES G. MURPHY Auburn University. RUDY E. VUCHINICH and CATHY A. SIMPSON University of Alabama, Birmingham

DELAYED REWARD AND COST DISCOUNTING. JAMES G. MURPHY Auburn University. RUDY E. VUCHINICH and CATHY A. SIMPSON University of Alabama, Birmingham The Psychological Record, 2001, 51, 571-588 DELAYED REWARD AND COST DISCOUNTING JAMES G. MURPHY Auburn University RUDY E. VUCHINICH and CATHY A. SIMPSON University of Alabama, Birmingham Participants (N

More information

EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT. Lee Davis Thomas

EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT. Lee Davis Thomas EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT Lee Davis Thomas A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington for Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 12-1991 Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio

More information

Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers

Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 1-2012 Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers Corina

More information

TIME DISCOUNTING FOR PRIMARY AND MONETARY REWARDS *

TIME DISCOUNTING FOR PRIMARY AND MONETARY REWARDS * TIME DISCOUNTING FOR PRIMARY AND MONETARY REWARDS * ERNESTO REUBEN Columbia University and IZA PAOLA SAPIENZA Northwestern University, NBER, and CEPR LUIGI ZINGALES University of Chicago, NBER, and CEPR

More information

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 79-91 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 79 Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats Rogelio Escobar National Autonomous University of Mexico

More information

USING A SELF-CONTROL TRAINING PROCEDURE TO INCREASE APPROPRIATE BEHAVIOR MARK R. DIXON AND LINDA J. HAYES

USING A SELF-CONTROL TRAINING PROCEDURE TO INCREASE APPROPRIATE BEHAVIOR MARK R. DIXON AND LINDA J. HAYES JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1998, 31, 203 210 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 1998) USING A SELF-CONTROL TRAINING PROCEDURE TO INCREASE APPROPRIATE BEHAVIOR MARK R. DIXON AND LINDA J. HAYES UNIVERSITY OF NEVADA

More information

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee The Psychological Record, 1997,47,145-166 THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task

Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task Analysis of Gambling Behavior Volume 11 Article 2 2017 Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task Nathaniel C. Rice 1. United States Army Medical Research Institute of Chemical Defense,

More information

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Learning and Motivation 39 (2008) 71 94 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Luís Oliveira,

More information

Subjective Hazard Rates Rationalize Irrational Temporal Preferences

Subjective Hazard Rates Rationalize Irrational Temporal Preferences Subjective Hazard Rates Rationalize Irrational Temporal Preferences Christian C. Luhmann (christian.luhmann@stonybrook.edu) Department of Psychology Stony Brook, NY 11794 USA Michael T. Bixter (michael.bixter@stonybrook.edu)

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2008 Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional

More information

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 217, 17, 123 135 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 1

More information

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 264-270 Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99163 Five pigeons

More information

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL'

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1973, 2, 393-43 NUMBER 3 (NOVEMBER) PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' M. C. DAVISON AND W. TEMPLE UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND AND

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens. Alan Poling 2

Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens. Alan Poling 2 Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens Amber A. Grant 1, T. Mary Foster 1, William Temple 1, Surrey Jackson, 1 Jennifer Kinloch 1 and Alan Poling 2 Affiliation of

More information

TIMING, REWARD PROCESSING AND CHOICE BEHAVIOR IN FOUR STRAINS OF RATS WITH DIFFERENT LEVELS OF IMPULSIVITY ANA I. GARCÍA AGUIRRE

TIMING, REWARD PROCESSING AND CHOICE BEHAVIOR IN FOUR STRAINS OF RATS WITH DIFFERENT LEVELS OF IMPULSIVITY ANA I. GARCÍA AGUIRRE TIMING, REWARD PROCESSING AND CHOICE BEHAVIOR IN FOUR STRAINS OF RATS WITH DIFFERENT LEVELS OF IMPULSIVITY by ANA I. GARCÍA AGUIRRE B.A., Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, 27 A THESIS submitted

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1987, 47, 57-72 CHOICE BETWEEN RELIABLE AND UNRELIABLE OUTCOMES: MIXED PERCENTAGE-REINFORCEMENT IN CONCURRENT CHAINS MARCIA L. SPETCH AND ROGER DUNN DALHOUSIE

More information

Operant matching. Sebastian Seung 9.29 Lecture 6: February 24, 2004

Operant matching. Sebastian Seung 9.29 Lecture 6: February 24, 2004 MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.29J, Spring 2004 - Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Instructor: Professor Sebastian Seung Operant matching Sebastian Seung 9.29 Lecture 6: February

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013)

On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013) On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013) Pier-Olivier Caron To cite this version: Pier-Olivier Caron. On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013):

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

Suppression of cocaine self-administration in monkeys: effects of delayed punishment

Suppression of cocaine self-administration in monkeys: effects of delayed punishment DOI 10.1007/s00213-011-2501-3 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Suppression of cocaine self-administration in monkeys: effects of delayed punishment William L. Woolverton & Kevin B. Freeman & Joel Myerson & Leonard

More information

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 152 162 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000053 Memory Systems

More information

CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS. DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton

CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS. DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton The Psychological Record, 2001, 51, 549-560 CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton College students repeatedly chose between

More information

On applying the matching law to between-subject data

On applying the matching law to between-subject data On applying the matching law to between-subject data Pier-Olivier Caron To cite this version: Pier-Olivier Caron. On applying the matching law to between-subject data. Animal Behaviour, Elsevier Masson,

More information

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2013, 100, NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER)

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2013, 100, NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 213, 1, 165 173 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) VARYING THE COSTS OF SUNK COSTS: OPTIMAL AND NON-OPTIMAL CHOICES IN A SUNK-COST TASK WITH HUMANS RAUL AVILA 1,RACHELLE

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Learn Behav. 2009 November ; 37(4): 357 364. doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.357. Behavioral Momentum and Relapse of Extinguished Operant Responding

More information

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 3-2016 Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Andrew R. Craig Utah State University

More information

THE EFFECT OF CHANGES IN AVAILABILITY ON FOOD SELECTION AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF DEPRiVATION. TERRY W. BELKE Mount Allison University

THE EFFECT OF CHANGES IN AVAILABILITY ON FOOD SELECTION AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF DEPRiVATION. TERRY W. BELKE Mount Allison University The Psychological Record, 2000, 50, 529-541 THE EFFECT OF CHANGES IN AVAILABILITY ON FOOD SELECTION AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF DEPRiVATION TERRY W. BELKE Mount Allison University TAMMI Y. KWAN Dalhousie University

More information

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez The Psychological Record, 28, 58, 57 528 Some Effects of Magnitude of Reinforcement on Persistence of Responding Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman The University of Minnesota Angel Jimenez The University

More information

Law of effect models and choice between many alternatives

Law of effect models and choice between many alternatives University of Wollongong Research Online Australian Health Services Research Institute Faculty of Business 2013 Law of effect models and choice between many alternatives Michael Alexander Navakatikyan

More information

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 348 370 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Chris Hope and Angelo Santi Wilfrid

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2005 April ; 31(2): 213 225. Timing in Choice Experiments Jeremie Jozefowiez and Daniel T. Cerutti

More information

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2012, 97, 85 100 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

More information

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/ Research Commons at the University of Waikato Copyright Statement: The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). The thesis

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

Exploring the Reliability and Validity of the Experiential Discounting Task

Exploring the Reliability and Validity of the Experiential Discounting Task Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 2012 Exploring the Reliability and Validity of the Experiential Discounting Task Rochelle R. Smits Utah State

More information

Overview. Simple Schedules of Reinforcement. Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement. Combined Schedules of Reinforcement BEHP 1016

Overview. Simple Schedules of Reinforcement. Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement. Combined Schedules of Reinforcement BEHP 1016 BEHP 1016 Why People Often Make Bad Choices and What to Do About It: Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement F. Charles Mace, Ph.D., BCBA-D University of Southern Maine with Jose Martinez-Diaz,

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December 2008 APPROVED:

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 74-84 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.74 Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure PATRICK VAN ROOYEN AND ANGELO

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

k/2. Thus, Re is measured in the same units

k/2. Thus, Re is measured in the same units JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR INCREASING AND SIGNALING BACKGROUND REINFORCEMENT: EFFECT ON THE FOREGROUND RESPONSE-REINFORCER RELATION TERRY W. BELKE AND GENE M. HEYMAN HARVARD UNIVERSITY

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

Does Working Memory Load Lead to Greater Impulsivity? Commentary on Hinson, Jameson, and Whitney (2003)

Does Working Memory Load Lead to Greater Impulsivity? Commentary on Hinson, Jameson, and Whitney (2003) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 2, 443 447 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.443

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A.

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December

More information

Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers

Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2008 Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers Christopher Aaron Podlesnik

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

Individual Differences in Delay Discounting. Tiffany Galtress & Kimberly Kirkpatrick Kansas State University

Individual Differences in Delay Discounting. Tiffany Galtress & Kimberly Kirkpatrick Kansas State University Individual Differences in Delay Discounting Tiffany Galtress & Kimberly Kirkpatrick Kansas State University Background Delay discounting refers to the decrease in value of a reward the longer it takes

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

Effects of response effort on self-control in rats

Effects of response effort on self-control in rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1998,26 (4),408-415 Effects of response effort on self-control in rats JOHN J. CHELONIS, A. W. LOGUE, REBECCA SHEEHY, and JIANBIN MAO State University ofnew York, Stony Brook,

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 261-268 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) CONCURRENT SCHEULES OF PRIMARY AN CONITIONE REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' ONAL W. ZIMMERMAN CARLETON UNIVERSITY Rats responded

More information

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2009, Vol. 35, No. 3, 293 311 2009 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013926 Resistance to Change Within

More information

Applicability to foraging simulation of a reinforcement schedule controlling the response energy of pigeons

Applicability to foraging simulation of a reinforcement schedule controlling the response energy of pigeons Learn Behav (2013) 41:425 432 DOI 10.3758/s13420-013-0117-7 Applicability to foraging simulation of a reinforcement schedule controlling the response energy of pigeons Masanori Kono Published online: 15

More information

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2015, 103, 269 287 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG

More information

Packet theory of conditioning and timing

Packet theory of conditioning and timing Behavioural Processes 57 (2002) 89 106 www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Packet theory of conditioning and timing Kimberly Kirkpatrick Department of Psychology, Uni ersity of York, York YO10 5DD, UK Accepted

More information

QUANTIFICATION OF DRUG CHOICE WITH THE GENERALIZED MATCHING LAW IN RHESUS MONKEYS MIKHAIL N. KOFFARNUS AND JAMES H. WOODS

QUANTIFICATION OF DRUG CHOICE WITH THE GENERALIZED MATCHING LAW IN RHESUS MONKEYS MIKHAIL N. KOFFARNUS AND JAMES H. WOODS JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2008, 89, 209 224 NUMBER 2(MARCH) QUANTIFICATION OF DRUG CHOICE WITH THE GENERALIZED MATCHING LAW IN RHESUS MONKEYS MIKHAIL N. KOFFARNUS AND JAMES H. WOODS

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 8-2012 Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Meredith

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information

Adaptive intertemporal preferences in foraging-style environments

Adaptive intertemporal preferences in foraging-style environments ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 17 June 2013 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00093 Adaptive intertemporal preferences in foraging-style environments Michael T. Bixter* and Christian C. Luhmann Department

More information

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/ Research Commons at the University of Waikato Copyright Statement: The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). The thesis

More information

Impulsive choice behavior in four strains of rats: evaluation of possible models of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Impulsive choice behavior in four strains of rats: evaluation of possible models of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder This is the author s final, peer-reviewed manuscript as accepted for publication. The publisher-formatted version may be available through the publisher s web site or your institution s library. Impulsive

More information

Perception of Anticipatory Time in Temporal Discounting

Perception of Anticipatory Time in Temporal Discounting Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 2, No. 2, 91 101 1937-321X/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0017686 Perception of Anticipatory Time in Temporal

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information