Visual-geometric illusions: unisex phenomena

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Visual-geometric illusions: unisex phenomena"

Transcription

1 Perception, 1979, volume 8, pages Visual-geometric illusions: unisex phenomena Clare Porac Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, V8W 2Y2 Canada Stanley Coren Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1W5, Canada Joan S Girgus Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08540, USA Mickey Verde Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia V8W 2Y2, Canada Received 4 September 1978, in revised form 19 January 1979 Abstract. The possibility of sex differences in responses to visual-geometric illusions was investigated with the use of forty-five illusion variants and a sample of 221 observers. No difference in illusion magnitude as a function of sex was found. A second experiment measured illusion decrement and transfer of decrement to other illusion configurations. Again there were no significant differences between male and female observers. 1 Introduction Over the past decade it has become clear that a single process cannot account for visual-geometric illusions. Rather, they seem to be the result of several mechanisms associated with the structure of the visual system as well as with higher-level cognitive strategies. For instance, the optical system of the eye seems to produce predictable distortions in figures with converging line elements (Chiang 1968; Coren 1969) while lateral inhibitory effects on the retina (von Bekesy 1967; Ganz 1966; Coren 1970) or in the visual cortex (Burns and Pritchard 1971) seem to account for some other distortions. Numerous cognitive-judgmental mechanisms have also been implicated in illusion formation, including habits associated with picture perception (Coren and Girgus 1975; Day 1972; Gillam 1973; Gregory 1963, 1968), comparative judgment of elements in an array (Restle 1971; Restle and Merryman 1968; Massaro and Anderson 1971; Girgus et al 1972), local or global averaging of extents (Pressey 1967, 1971), or confusion of test and accessory lines (Erlebacher and Sekuler 1969; Coren and Girgus 1973). Although one mechanism may predominate in any one configuration (Coren et al 1976), several different mechanisms may be active in a single distortion (Coren and Girgus 1974; Girgus et al 1975). Recent evidence has suggested that there may be sex-related differences in some of the factors which affect illusion formation, for instance, at the structural level, McGuinness (1976a, 1976b) reports visual acuity differences for males and for females. Optical factors, such as image blur, have been shown to affect illusion magnitude in some configurations (Coren 1969; Coren et al 1978; Ward and Coren 1976); therefore, one might expect differences in illusion strength between males and females in those figures, such as the Poggendorff and Muller-Lyer, where image blur may play a role in illusion formation. In a similar vein, Girgus et al (1977) have shown that the amount of pigmentation in the crystalline lens is greater for males. Pollack (1969) and Sjostrom and Pollack (1971) have provided evidence which suggests that lens pigmentation may affect illusion response. Again such a structural difference might lead one to expect sex-related differences in illusion response.

2 402 C Porac, S Coren, J S Girgus, M Verde At the level of cognitive-judgmental strategies there are also sex-related differences which could affect illusion magnitude. Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) have summarized evidence which suggests that males and females are very similar in their use of visual information when the tasks involve simple form matching or reproduction. However, males seem to perform with greater speed and accuracy on tasks which require more complex visual analyses. For example, adult males appear to be more efficient on the embedded-figures test where observers are asked to detect a simple figure which is hidden within a complex background (Bieri et al 1958; Goldstein and Chance 1965; Schwartz and Carp 1967). Also, performance on the rod-and-frame test, where observers are required to set a line to the bodily vertical when it is surrounded by a tilted frame, indicates that the settings made by females are more influenced by the direction of the tilted frame than those made by males (Bogo et al 1970; Gross 1959; Kato 1965; Morf et al 1971; Silverman et al 1973; Vaught 1965). Performance on both of these tests suggests that females have more difficulty with separating a judged element from its surrounding context than do males. Presently it is a contentious issue as to whether these differences in disembedding and other spatial abilities (such as the ability mentally to rotate forms into various orientations) are based upon differences in learned perceptual strategies or are rooted in biochemical variations between the sexes (Garron 1970; Maccoby and Jacklin 1974; Sherman 1967; Waber 1976, 1977); however, regardless of the point of view which is taken, these sex-related performance asymmetries may have important consequences for the perception of visual illusions. One of the oldest theories of illusions, dating back to Miiller-Lyer (1889), suggests that some illusory distortions may be caused by a confusion between test and inducing elements. It is argued that such confusion leads the observer to ascribe some of the properties of the accessory elements in the configuration to the test element which is being judged. Erlebacher and Sekuler (1969) have shown how such confusion tendencies may contribute to the Muller-Lyer illusion and Pressey (1967, 1971) has incorporated a variant of this same notion into his assimilation theory of illusions. Coren and Girgus (1973) provided direct evidence for the contribution of the confusion of test and accessory lines to illusion strength by demonstrating that as the test element becomes more discriminable from the inducing elements the magnitude of the obtained illusory distortion is reduced. Thus, when it is easier to separate the test element from the context, illusion magnitude is reduced. To the extent that confusion or assimilation of test and context elements is one factor in the formation of illusions, individual differences in viewing strategies or abilities to isolate the test element could alter the magnitude of the illusions. If males and females employ different viewing strategies or display different ability levels when attempting to isolate figural components from a context, then one might expect sex differences in illusion magnitude. Although sex differences have been found in the perception of illusory arrays which require three-dimensional reorganizations, such as Necker cube reversals (Immergluck and Mearini 1969), the studies which have concentrated on twodimensional static geometric illusions have tended to report a lack of sex differences (Fraisse and Vautrey 1956; Hartmann et al 1972; Pressey and Wilson 1978; Spitz et al 1970). However, these studies have utilized rather small samples and only a narrow range of illusion configurations. This could be a factor in their negative findings. In order to detect small but systematic differences a large sample should be employed. In addition, the investigation of a wide variety of illusion configurations would allow one to isolate the class of distortions associated with sex differences if any response asymmetries between males and females were uncovered. Such a comprehensive investigation could provide important insights both for the study of visual-geometric illusions and for the study of mechanisms which maintain sex-related

3 Visual-geometric illusions: unisex phenomena 403 differences and similarities in complex, visual-cognitive tasks. For these reasons, the experiments reported below were conducted. 2 Experiment 1 The first experiment attempted to assess whether there were sex differences in initial illusion magnitude using a full spectrum of forty-five geometric illusion variants. 2.1 Method Subjects. The sample was composed of 107 females and 114 males. All were undergraduate volunteers Stimuli. The stimuli were twelve of the most common illusion configurations. These are shown in figure 1: Poggendorff (figure la), Wundt (figure lb), Zollner (figure lc), Delboeuf (figure Id), Ebbinghaus (figure le), Ponzo (figure If), Jastrow (figure lg), Baldwin (figure lh), horizontal-vertical (figure li), Oppel-Kundt (figure lj), divided-line (figure Ik), and Sander parallelogram (figure 11). In addition, eleven variants of the Miiller-Lyer were included (figure 2). All of the illusions, except the Poggendorff, contain an overestimation and an underestimation distortion, and these were measured separately. Each illusion stimulus was drawn on a separate page (21 cm x 27-5 cm). In the lower left-hand quadrant of the page, there was a horizontal line on which the observer was asked to mark the apparent linear extent of the test dimension of the figure. The test extents which were measured in each figure were between 2 and 8 cm in length while the measurement line was always 15-5 cm. Thus, there was enough latitude to assess both overestimation and underestimation illusions. Coren and Girgus (1972a) have shown that this technique provides results that are comparable to those obtained by the method of adjustment, and Coren et al (1976) have demonstrated that this technique can be used to measure illusions of shape as well as extent. A small block of instructions, and an illustrative diagram in the upper right-hand corner of each page indicated which portion of each figure was to be judged Procedure. The illusion stimuli were assembled into booklets. Subjects received two such booklets, each of which contained the forty-five illusion configurations in different mixed orders. Each subject made two independent judgments on each illusion. These two judgments were averaged for the purpose of statistical analysis. Observers were tested in groups and response rate was self-paced, although subjects were urged to indicate their first impressions and not to linger over any of the arrays. 2.2 Results and discussion The mean illusion magnitude for each configuration was separately computed for males and for females. The absolute size of the illusory effect as a function of sex is shown for the twelve classical illusion variants in table 1. Table 2 shows the absolute illusion magnitude, as a function of sex, for the eleven variants of the Miiller-Lyer figure. For convenience, those segments which are usually overestimated in the various illusion configurations are indicated by a +, and those which were usually underestimated were indicated by a - placed next to the illusion name. A series of t tests was conducted to see if the differences between males and females was significant for any single configuration. This analysis revealed only three significant differences out of the forty-five illusion variants tested. These were for the Poggendorff, for the overestimated portion of the Baldwin illusion, and for the underestimated portion of the circle variation of the Muller-Lyer illusion; however, on the basis of chance alone, one would expect between two and three comparisons

4 404 C Porac, S Coren, J S Girgus, M Verde out of the forty-five to reach significance. Although females showed a greater illusion magnitude on 60% of the illusion configurations, once again this proportion was not a significant deviation from chance (z = 1 34). (a) (b) 7, If // / (c) K (d) (e) (0 D U (g) (h) (i) I - (j) (k) Figure 1. Variants of the classical illusions used in the stimulus set: (a) Poggendorff; (b)wundt; (c) Zollner; (d) Delboeuf; (e) Ebbinghaus; (f) Ponzo; (g) Jastrow; (h) Baldwin; (i) horizontalvertical; (j) Oppel-Kundt; (k) divided-line; (1) Sander parallelogram. (1)

5 Visual-geometric illusions: unisex phenomena 405 The only illusion configuration which showed a highly significant sex effect was the Poggendorff configuration. Here the illusion magnitude was 3 0 mm greater for males than for females. This difference is difficult to interpret, although it is consistent with a previous investigation of this pattern (Pressey and Sweeney 1970). A possible factor is based upon a consideration of the mechanism of the illusion. Gillam (1971) has suggested that this distortion arises because of pictorial depth cues implicitly present in the array. Females have been shown to differ from males in tasks involving three-dimensional responses to two-dimensional configurations, such as Necker cube reversals (Immergluck and Mearini 1969), and mental rotations (Tapley and Bryden 1977; Wilson et al 1975). Males seem to respond more readily to three-dimensional cues in such arrays. This might make them more responsive to implicit depth cues in the Poggendorff, resulting in greater illusion magnitude. Unfortunately, similar implied-depth arguments have been offered for the Miiller- Lyer illusion (Gregory 1968) and the horizontal-vertical illusion (Girgus and Coren 1975) and yet no sex differences emerge for these configurations. Thus, although a > < < - > (a) S \ (b) > < < > - = - (c) (d) ) C e^ 3 CEr-^ (e) (0 o oe-e iz (g) 00 (0 0) oo.... Figure 2. Variants of the Muller-Lyer illusion used in the stimulus set: (a) standard; (b) exploded; (c) no-shaft; (d) Piaget; (e) curve; (f) box; (g) circle; (h) lozenge; (i) Sanford; (j) minimal; (k) Coren dot.

6 406 C Porac, S Coren, J S Girgus, M Verde Table 1. Mean illusion magnitude for males and females on twelve classical illusion figures. Illusion Mean illusion magnitude (mm) Poggendorff Wundt+ a Wundt - b Zollner + Zollner - Delboeuf 4- Delboeuf- Ebbinghaus + Ebbinghaus - Ponzo + Ponzo - Jastrow + Jastrow - Baldwin + Baldwin - Horizontal-vertical + Horizontal-vertical - Oppel-Kundt + Oppel-Kundt- Divided line + Divided line - Sander parallelogram + Sander parallelogram - females males difference (females-males) (TV = 107) (7V=114) * * o overestimated segment; b - underestimated segment; * p < 005; ** p < POOL -o Table 2. Variant Mean illusion magnitude for males and females on eleven variants of the Muller-Lyer figure. Mean illusion magnitude (mm) Standard + a Standard - b Exploded + Exploded No shaft + No shaft - Piaget + Piaget Curved + Curved - Box + Box- Circle + Circle - Lozenge + Lozenge - Sanford + Sanford - Minimal + Minimal - Coren + Coren females (N = 107) males (TV = 114) difference (females--males) * overestimated segment; b - underestimated segment; *p<005.

7 Visual-geometric illusions: unisex phenomena 407 strong sex-related effect emerges for one illusion response, the source of the difference remains unclear. When we consider the overall pattern of results, given the sample size and the number of illusion configurations tested, the data suggest that sex-related asymmetries probably do not exist for responses to two-dimensional visual-geometric illusion arrays. 3 Experiment 2 As early as 1902, Judd observed that the magnitude of the perceptual distortion found in the Muller-Lyer illusion diminished with free inspection. This illusion decrement has been repeatedly confirmed for the Muller-Lyer (Coren and Girgus 1972b, 1974; Day 1962; Dewar 1968; Festinger et al 1968; Girgus et al 1973) and has also been shown to occur in other illusory configurations including the Poggendorff, the Zollner, the Wundt-Hering, and the Oppel-Kundt (Coren and Girgus 1972b; Coren and Hoenig 1972). A great deal of evidence seems to suggest that this decrement in illusion strength involves some form of perceptual learning. For instance, Burnham (1968), Coren and Hoenig (1972), Day (1962), and Festinger et al (1968) have shown that if the eyes are free to explore the configurations with saccadic movements, this results in a larger reduction in illusion magnitude. These investigators argue that erroneously long or erroneously short saccades allow observers to learn the direction and extent of the illusion, and are the information feedback upon which the recalibration of the illusory percept is based. There is other evidence which suggests that decrement of illusion magnitude involves a learning process. To begin with, the rate of decrement responds to traditional learning variables such as the spacing of trials (Dewar 1968; Mountjoy 1958). Furthermore, the total amount of illusion reduction is cumulative over periods of days or weeks (Girgus et al 1975; Judd 1902). Finally, the illusion decrement transfers from one configuration to another, and the magnitude of this transfer depends upon the perceived, rather than the structural, similarity between configurations (Coren and Girgus 1974). All of these lines of evidence seem to indicate that illusion decrement involves some form of perceptual learning. The data of experiment 1 gave no evidence for differences in illusion susceptibility as a function of sex. However, such differences could appear in the perceptual learning phenomena associated with visual-geometric illusions. This expectation is based upon reports which indicate that, while males are somewhat more proficient at spatial tasks than are females, females manifest superior performance in some perceptual learning or visual discrimination tasks (Laughlin and McGlynn 1967; Pishkin et al 1967; Stevenson et al 1968). If males and females differ with respect to the type of visual-perceptual learning involved in illusion decrement, one might expect significant variations between the sexes in the rate and/or the amount of illusion decrease over time and in the magnitude of the transfer of the decrement training. In order to test this hypothesis, a second experiment was conducted. 3.1 Method Subjects. Twenty male and twenty female volunteers participated in this study Stimuli. Three variants of the Muller-Lyer illusion were selected from the Coren and Girgus (1974) transfer experiment. The stimulus used for decrement training was the standard Muller-Lyer configuration which is shown as figure 2a. Two variants were used to test transfer of decrement. They were the exploded form (figure 2b), which has been scaled as highly similar to the standard form by Coren and Girgus (1974), and the Piaget form (figure 2d), which has been scaled as very dissimilar to the standard form.

8 408 C Porac, S Coren, J S Girgus, M Verde All figures were comprised of 1 mm lines. In the standard and exploded forms the shafts were 8 cm, the wings were 2 cm, and the angle between the wings and the horizontal was 45. In the exploded form, the gap between the wings and the shaft was 2 mm. For the Piaget form, the center shafts were 8 cm. For the apparently shorter segments the outside bars were 5 cm, while those of the apparently longer were 11 cm. The apparently longer segment of each illusion figure was made adjustable via a tongue-and-groove apparatus. The configuration was viewed at a distance of 40 cm from the observer. The black lines had a reflectance of 4 6% and the background had a reflectance of 74-7% Procedure. Each sex group was divided in half, with ten observers tested for transfer on the similar (exploded) form and ten tested for transfer on the dissimilar (Piaget) form of the illusion. Each observer made two initial settings of apparent equality on the transfer stimulus to which he or she was assigned. The settings were balanced as to starting position, with one half of them starting with the adjustable shaft clearly longer than the comparison length while the other half started with it clearly shorter. Following this, all observers inspected the standard form of the Muller-Lyer figure (figure 2a) for 5 min, moving their eyes sequentially from a%gle vertex to angle vertex. Settings to apparent equality were taken at 1 min intervals. Other investigators have shown that this procedure leads to sizeable reductions in illusion magnitude (Coren and Girgus 1972b; Girgus et al 1973). Following the inspection period, all observers made two additional settings to apparent equality on the initially tested illusion variant (either the Piaget or the exploded form). The rate of illusion decrement is given by the change in illusion magnitude during inspection. The amount of transfer is given by the difference between the initial (preinspection) and final (post inspection) measures on the nonpracticed figure. 3.2 Results and discussion The time course of illusion decrement is shown in figure 3. Since all of the observers inspected the standard form of the Muller-Lyer for 5 min, one may assess differences between the sexes in the rate of decrement by collapsing the practice data for males and for females across the specific transfer conditions. An analysis of variance indicated that significant reduction in illusion magnitude occurred over the inspection period (Fs >190 = 5-86; p < 0 001). There was no difference in the rate of decrement as a function of sex CF 138 = 1 13), nor was there a significant interaction between sex and time CF Sjl90 = 0-88). However, figure 3 gives some suggestion that illusion magnitude in the male group is decreasing at a slower rate than in the female group. Since the analysis of variance measure of interaction is not particularly females Illusion (mm) O en X^^ No o o 5 ' Time (min) Figure 3. The magnitude of the Muller-Lyer illusion as a function of inspection time both for males and for females.

9 Visual-geometric illusions: unisex phenomena 409 sensitive to small slope differences, a more powerful test for possible rate disparities was used. This was done by fitting a least-squares regression line through each individual observer's data. When this is done, the slope of each regression line is based upon six scores and provides a stable estimate of each observer's rate of decrement. For females the mean slope was -1-05, while for males it was 0-53 (indicating a rate of illusion decrease in mm min" 1 ). However, this difference is not statistically significant (7 38 = 1-39). Thus, there is no evidence for sex-related differences in illusion decrement. Coren and Girgus (1974) demonstrated that decrement of the Muller-Lyer transfers to other variants of this illusion as a function of the judged similarity between inspection and test configurations. Thus, one would expect that inspection of the standard form of the Muller-Lyer would bring about large amounts of transfer of the reduction in illusion magnitude to the similar (exploded, figure 2b) variant. On the other hand, very little transfer to the dissimilar (Piaget, figure 2d) variant would be expected. Table 3 presents the results from the present experiment which used this paradigm. The third column under each illusion variant heading represents the mean difference in illusion magnitude between the pretest and the posttest. As can be seen, there is very little pretest-posttest reduction in the Piaget form of the illusion both for males and for females. In fact, females tend to show a negative transfer in that they have a higher mean illusion magnitude after 5 min of inspection of the standard form than they had before inspection. Such results might be expected if individuals are fluctuating around a mean transfer value of zero with some observers showing a positive effect of the decrement training and others showing a negative effect. On the other hand, both males and females show positive transfer to the similar or exploded form. This reduction is on the order of 17% of the original illusion magnitude for the male observers and 25% for the female observers. When an analysis of variance was performed upon these pretest-posttest difference scores, it was found that the transfer effect as a function of the similarity of the variant to the standard figure was highly significant (F i>36 = 7-71; p < 0-01). These data, then, replicate the findings of Coren and Girgus (1974). However, when one looks at the effect of sex, there was no difference between males and females CFi ;36 = 1-36); also, there was no significant interaction between sex and form CF 1;36 = 2-53). Thus, there was no evidence for sex-related differences in either the rate or the magnitude of illusion decrement or the pattern of decrement transfer to other configurations. Table 3. Mean illusion magnitude (mm) on variants of the Muller-Lyer illusion used in the transfer task (total number of observers = 40; number of observers in each cell = 10). Similar (exploded Muller-Lyer) Dissimilar (Piaget Muller-Lyer) pretest posttest difference pretest posttest difference Males mean S.D Females mean a S.D a This rather large negative transfer effect was influenced by the atypical scores of one observer. When her responses were excluded the mean became and the standard deviation was Neither this new value nor the value reported in the table was significantly different from 0 when tested.

10 410 C Porac, S Coren, J S Girgus, M Verde 4 General discussion This pair of experiments attempted to investigate the possibility that sex differences exist in the perception of visual-geometric illusions. The first experiment tested for differences in static illusion magnitude across forty-five illusion variants. Despite the fact that a large sample of illusions was used and a large sample of observers, only one illusion configuration (the Poggendorff) produced a difference between male and female observers. The second experiment looked for differences in the dynamic properties of responses to visual-geometric illusions as manifested in illusion decrement and transfer of decrement between configuration variants. Once again, no reliable sex differences were found in either class of perceptual responses to the illusory arrays. Thus the results of the two experiments are quite consistent. In some respects, these results are surprising given the evidence which indicates that males and females may differ in certain structural characteristics of the eye. It is more surprising given that males and females respond differentially on spatial disembedding, visual discrimination, and visual tasks which require three-dimensional reorganizations. To the extent that such tasks indicate a particular cognitive or perceptual information processing bias, one might have expected sex differences to emerge in the magnitude of visual-geometric illusions and illusion-related phenomena. A recent study by Tapley and Bryden (1977) may provide an explanation for this inconsistency. In a systematic exploration of sex differences in the ability to perform mental rotations of objects (thought to be a type of visual-spatial ability), these investigators found that other tests of spatial ability may not be correlated with an observer's performance in the mental rotation task. Thus, it seems that nonverbal cognitive and visual-analytic skills may manifest themselves in specific situations with little or no transfer from one type of task to another. Thus, the sex-related response patterns which are seen in embedded-figure, rod-and-frame, visual-discrimination, and mental-rotation tasks may represent situation-specific behaviors which do not generalize to the type of visual analysis needed to produce greater or lesser degrees of illusion susceptibility. Acknowledgements. This research was supported in part by grants A0293 and A9783 from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and from the National Science Foundation ( ). Experiment 2 was submitted in partial fulfillment of the Bachelor of Arts Degree (Honours) at the University of Victoria by M Verde. References Bekesey G von, 1967 Sensory Inhibition (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press) Bieri J, Bradburn W, Galinsky N, 1958 "Sex differences in perceptual behavior" Journal of Personality Bogo N, Winget C, Gleser G C, 1970 "Ego defensive and perceptual style" Perceptual and Motor Skills Burnham C A, 1968 "Decrement in the Muller-Lyer illusion with saccadic and tracking eye movements" Perception and Psychophysics Burns B D, Pritchard R, 1971 "Geometrical illusions and the response of neurons in the cat's visual cortex to angle patterns" Journal of Physiology (London) Chiang C, 1968 "A new theory to explain geometrical illusions produced by crossing lines" Perception and Psychophysics Coren S, 1969 "The influence of optical aberrations on the magnitude of the Poggendorff illusion" Perception and Psychophysics Coren S, 1970 "Lateral inhibition and geometric illusions" Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Coren S, Girgus J S, 1972a "A comparison of five methods of illusion measurement" Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation Coren S, Girgus J S, 1972b "Illusion decrement in intersecting line figures" Psychonomic Science

11 Visual-geometric illusions: unisex phenomena 411 Coren S, Girgus J S, 1973 "Differentiation and decrement in the Muller-Lyer illusion" Perception and Psychophysics Coren S, Girgus J S, 1974 "Transfer of illusion decrement as a function of perceived similarity" Journal of Experimental Psychology Coren S, Girgus J S, 1975 "A size illusion based upon a minimal interposition cue" Perception Coren S, Girgus J S, Ehrlichman H, Hakstian A R, 1976 "An empirical taxonomy of visual illusions" Perception and Psychophysics Coren S, Hoehig P, 1972 "Eye movements and decrement in the Oppel-Kundt illusion" Perception and Psychophysics Coren S, Ward L M, Porac C, Fraser R, 1978 "The effect of optical blur on visual-geometric illusions" Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Day R H, 1962 "The effects of repeated trials and prolonged fixation on error in the Muller-Lyer figure" Psychological Monographs Day R H, 1972 "Visual spatial illusions: a general explanation" Science Dewar R E, 1968 "Distribution of practice and the Muller-Lyer illusion" Perception and Psychophysics Erlebacher A, Sekuler R, 1969 "Exploration of the Muller-Lyer illusion: confusion theory reexamined" Journal of Experimental Psychology Festinger L, White C W, Allyn M R, 1968 "Eye movements and decrement in the Muller-Lyer illusion" Perception and Psychophysics Fraisse P, Vautrey P, 1956 "The influence of age, sex and specialized training on the verticalhorizontal illusion" Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Ganz L, 1966 "Is the figural after-effect an after-effect?" Psychological Bulletin Garron D C, 1970 "Sex-linked recessive inheritance of spatial and numerical abilities, and Turner's syndrome" Psychological Review Gillam B, 1971 "A depth processing theory of the Poggendorff illusion" Perception and Psychophysics Gillam B, 1973 "The nature of size scaling in the Ponzo and related illusions" Perception and Psychophysics Girgus J S, Coren S, 1975 "Depth cues and constancy scaling in the horizontal-vertical illusion" Canadian Journal of Psychology Girgus J S, Coren S, Agdern M, 1972 "The interrelationship between the Ebbinghaus and Delboeuf illusions" Journal of Experimental Psychology Girgus J S, Coren S, Durant M, Porac C, 1975 "The assessment of components involved in illusion formation using a long term decrement procedure" Perception and Psychophysics Girgus J S, Coren S, Horowitz L, 1973 "Peripheral and central components in variants of the Muller-Lyer illusion" Perception and Psychophysics Girgus J S, Coren S, Porac C, 1977 "Independence of in vivo human lens pigmentation from U.V. light exposure" Vision Research Goldstein A G, Chance J E, 1965 "Effects of practice on sex-related differences in performance on embedded figures" Psychonomic Science Gregory R L, 1963 "Distortion of visual space as inappropriate constancy scaling" Nature (London) Gregory R L, 1968 '"Visual illusions" Scientific American 219(5) Gross F, 1959 "The role of set in perception of the upright" Journal of Personality Hartmann D P, Gelfand D M, Courtney R J Jr, Malouf R E, 1972 "Successive presentation of elements of the Muller-Lyer figure and CA, MA and IQ: an age extension and unsuccessful replication" Child Development Immergluck L, Mearini M C, 1969 "Age and sex difference in response to embedded figures and reversible figures" Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Judd C H, 1902 "Practice and its effects on the geometrical illusions" Psychological Review Kato N, 1965 "A fundamental study of rod-frame test" Japanese Psychological Research Laughlin P R, McGlynn R P, 1967 "Cooperative versus competitive concept attainment as a function of sex and stimulus display" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Maccoby E E, Jacklin C N, 1974 The Psychology of Sex Differences (Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press) Massaro D W, Anderson N H, 1971 "Judgmental model of the Ebbinghaus illusion" Journal of Experimental Psychology

12 412 C Porac, S Coren, J S Girgus, M Verde McGuinness D, 1976a "Away from a unisex psychology: individual differences in visual sensory and perceptual processes" Perception McGuinness D, 1976b "Sex differences in the organization of perception and cognition" in Exploring Sex Differences Eds B Lloyd, J Archer (New York: Academic Press) Morf N E, Kavanaugh R D, McConville M, 1971 "Intratest and sex differences on a portable rod and frame test" Perceptual and Motor Skills Mountjoy P T, 1958 "Effects of exposure time and inter-trial intervals upon decrement of the Muller-Lyer illusion" Journal of Experimental Psychology Muller-Lyer F C, 1889 "Optische Verteilungstauschungen" Dubois-Reymond Archiv fiir Anatomie und Psychologie supplementary volume, Pishkin V, Wolfgang A, Rasmussen E, 1967 "Age, sex, amount and type of memory information in concept learning" Journal of Experimental Psychology Pollack R H, 1969 "Some implications of genetic changes in perception" in Studies in Cognitive Development: Essays in Honour of Jean Piaget Eds D Elkind, J Flavell (New York: Oxford University Press) Pressey A W, 1967 "A theory of the Muller-Lyer illusion" Perceptual and Motor Skills Pressey AW, 1971 "An extension of assimilation theory to illusions of size, area, and direction" Perception and Psychophysics Pressey A W, Sweeney 0, 1970 "Age changes in the Poggendorff illusion as measured by a method of production" Psychonomic Science Pressey A W, Wilson A E, 1978 "Another look at age changes in geometric illusions" Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Restle F, 1971 "Visual illusions" in Adaptation-Level Theory Ed. M H Appley (New York: Academic Press) pp Restle F, Merryman C T, 1968 "An adaptation-level theory account of a relative-size illusion" Psychonomic Science Schwartz D W, Carp S A, 1967 "Field dependence in a geriatric population" Perceptual and Motor Skills Sherman J A, 1967 "Problem of sex differences in space perception and aspects of intellectual functioning" Psychological Review Silverman J, Buchshaum M, Stierlin H, 1973 "Sex difference in perceptual differentiation and stimulus intensity control" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Sjostrom K P, Pollack R H, 1971 "The effect of simulated receptor aging on two types of visual illusions" Psychonomic Science Spitz H H, Goettle D R, Diveley S L, 1970 "A comparison of retardates and normals on the Poggendorff and Oppel-Kundt illusions" Developmental Psychology Stevenson H W, Hale G A, Klein R E, Miller L K, 1968 "Interrelations and correlates in children's learning and problem solving" Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 33 Tapley S M, Bryden M P, 1977 "An investigation of sex differences in spatial ability: mental rotation of three-dimensional objects" Canadian Journal of Psychology Vaught G M, 1965 "The relationship of role identification and ego strength to sex differences in the rod-and-frame test" Journal of Personality Waber D P, 1976 "Sex differences in cognition: a function of maturation rate?" Science Waber D P, 1977 "Sex differences in mental abilities, hemispheric lateralization and rate of physical growth at adolescence" Developmental Psychology Ward L M, Coren S, 1976 "The effect of optically-induced blur on the magnitude of the MullerLyer illusion" Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Wilson J R, DeFries J C, McClearn G E, Vandenburg S G, Johnson R C, Rashad M N, 1975 "Cognitive abilities: use of family data as a control to assess sex and age differences in two ethnic groups" International Journal of Aging and Human Development P 1979 a Pion publication printed in Great Britain

Heritability in visual-geometric illusions: a family study

Heritability in visual-geometric illusions: a family study Perception, 1979, volume 8, pages 303-309 Heritability in visual-geometric illusions: a family study Stanley Coren Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia,

More information

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 916-920 Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion JOHN PREDEBON University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia Two experiments

More information

Spatial distortions within the Poggendorff figure and its variants: A parametric analysis

Spatial distortions within the Poggendorff figure and its variants: A parametric analysis Perception & Psychophysics 1977, Vol. 21 (2), 118-124 Spatial distortions within the Poggendorff figure and its variants: A parametric analysis KATHRYN QUINA-HOLLAND University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee,

More information

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON Journal of Experimental Psychology 1971, Vol. 89, No. 1, 147-151 JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION DOMINIC W. MASSARO» University of Wisconsin AND NORMAN H. ANDERSON University of California,

More information

Age changes in the Ponzo and filled-space illusions*

Age changes in the Ponzo and filled-space illusions* Perception & Psychophysics 1974, Vol. 15, No.2, 315-319 Age changes in the Ponzo and filled-space illusions* ALEXANDER W. PRESSEY University ofmanitoba, Winnipeg, R3T 2N2, Manitoba, Canada A generalization

More information

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 BLL R. BROWN AND THOMAS J. REBBN2 UNlVERSTY OF LOUSVLLE This experiment required Ss to make Markov-generated histoforms that were

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article GRASPING VISUAL ILLUSIONS: No Evidence for a Dissociation Between Perception and Action V.H. Franz, 1 K.R. Gegenfurtner, 1 H.H. Bülthoff, 1 and M. Fahle 2 1 Max-Planck-Institut für Biologische

More information

Crossing the line: Estimations of line length in the Oppel- Kundt illusion

Crossing the line: Estimations of line length in the Oppel- Kundt illusion Journal of Vision (2014) 14(8):20, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/14/8/20 1 Crossing the line: Estimations of line length in the Oppel- Kundt illusion Kyriaki Mikellidou Peter Thompson Department

More information

Connectedness underlies the underestimation of the horizontal

Connectedness underlies the underestimation of the horizontal Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79:1217 1226 DOI 10.3758/s13414-017-1309-6 Connectedness underlies the underestimation of the horizontal vertical illusion in L-shaped configurations Yongchun Cai 1 & Ci

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Large-Sample, Single Experiment Estimates of the Size of Gender. Differences on Visual Illusions, Maze Learning, and Mirror Drawing. Kenneth O.

Large-Sample, Single Experiment Estimates of the Size of Gender. Differences on Visual Illusions, Maze Learning, and Mirror Drawing. Kenneth O. McGraw 1 Running head: GENDER DIFFERENCES Large-Sample, Single Experiment Estimates of the Size of Gender Differences on Visual Illusions, Maze Learning, and Mirror Drawing Kenneth O. McGraw University

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (2), 327-331 The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR THOMAS M. SPALEK AND VINCENT DI LOLLO Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia,

More information

CHANGES IN VISUAL SPATIAL ORGANIZATION: RESPONSE FREQUENCY EQUALIZATION VERSUS ADAPTATION LEVEL

CHANGES IN VISUAL SPATIAL ORGANIZATION: RESPONSE FREQUENCY EQUALIZATION VERSUS ADAPTATION LEVEL Journal of Experimental Psychology 1973, Vol. 98, No. 2, 246-251 CHANGES IN VISUAL SPATIAL ORGANIZATION: RESPONSE FREQUENCY EQUALIZATION VERSUS ADAPTATION LEVEL WILLIAM STEINBERG AND ROBERT SEKULER 2 Northwestern

More information

Cognitive issues in visual perception

Cognitive issues in visual perception Cognitive issues in visual perception 1 Our perception of a visual stimulus depends Not only on what we see But also on how we interpret it 2 3 From seeing to perceiving Perception, interpretation, & comprehension

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Vision Seeing is in the mind

Vision Seeing is in the mind 1 Vision Seeing is in the mind Stimulus: Light 2 Light Characteristics 1. Wavelength (hue) 2. Intensity (brightness) 3. Saturation (purity) 3 4 Hue (color): dimension of color determined by wavelength

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces

Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces Vision Research 43 (2003) 1501 1505 Rapid communication Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces Adrian Schwaninger a,b, *, Stefan Ryf b, Franziska Hofer b

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* Perception & Psychophysics 1973. Vol. 13. So. 2.356-360 Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* ANGEL VASSILEV+ Institu te of Physiology. Bulgarian Academy of Sciences.

More information

The Horizontal Vertical Illusion: An Investigation in Context and Environment

The Horizontal Vertical Illusion: An Investigation in Context and Environment The Horizontal Vertical Illusion: An Investigation in Context and Environment, Theresa C. Cook, Lawrence Rosenblum Department of Psychology University of California, Riverside A B S T R A C T The Horizontal

More information

Psychology Unit 3 Test

Psychology Unit 3 Test 1 Choose the best answer. (2 pts each). Psychology Unit 3 Test 1. In bright light, the iris and the pupil to control the amount of light entering the eye. a. expands; constricts b. constricts; expands

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect. Read each question carefully and answer it completely. Multiple-choice questions are worth one point each, other questions

More information

THE EFFECTS OF STRIPED CLOTHING ON PERCEPTIONS OF BODY SIZE

THE EFFECTS OF STRIPED CLOTHING ON PERCEPTIONS OF BODY SIZE SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2012, 40(8), 1239-1244 Society for Personality Research http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2012.40.8.1239 THE EFFECTS OF STRIPED CLOTHING ON PERCEPTIONS OF BODY SIZE VIREN SWAMI

More information

The Impact of Schemas on the Placement of Eyes While Drawing.

The Impact of Schemas on the Placement of Eyes While Drawing. The Red River Psychology Journal PUBLISHED BY THE MSUM PSYCHOLOGY DEPARTMENT The Impact of Schemas on the Placement of Eyes While Drawing. Eloise M. Warren. Minnesota State University Moorhead Abstract.

More information

More Accurate Size Contrast Judgments in the Ebbinghaus Illusion by a Remote Culture

More Accurate Size Contrast Judgments in the Ebbinghaus Illusion by a Remote Culture Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 738 742 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.3.738

More information

Monocular asymmetries in recognition after an eye movement: Sighting dominance and dextrality

Monocular asymmetries in recognition after an eye movement: Sighting dominance and dextrality Perception & Psychophysics 1979, Vol. 25 (1),55-59 Monocular asymmetries in recognition after an eye movement: Sighting dominance and dextrality CLARE PORAC University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

The relevance of symmetry in line length perception

The relevance of symmetry in line length perception Perception, 2009, volume 38, pages 1428 ^ 1438 doi:10.1068/p6287 The relevance of symmetry in line length perception Pom Charras, Juan Lupia n ez Departamento de Psicolog a Experimental y de Fisiolog a

More information

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1996, 3 (2), 222 226 Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana and THOMAS J. PALMERI Vanderbilt University,

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect, which you ll acknowledge when you turn in your exam. Each multiple-choice question is worth 1 point and the value of the other

More information

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception Competing Frameworks in Perception Lesson II: Perception module 08 Perception.08. 1 Views on perception Perception as a cascade of information processing stages From sensation to percept Template vs. feature

More information

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception Competing Frameworks in Perception Lesson II: Perception module 08 Perception.08. 1 Views on perception Perception as a cascade of information processing stages From sensation to percept Template vs. feature

More information

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles?

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles? Carnegie Mellon University Research Showcase @ CMU Dietrich College Honors Theses Dietrich College of Humanities and Social Sciences 2006 Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize

More information

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity Perception & Psychophysics 1980,28 (3), 249-253 Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity STEPHEN HANDEL and JAMES W. RHODES University oftennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37916 Subjects

More information

1.1 FEATURES OF THOUGHT

1.1 FEATURES OF THOUGHT SEC 1 Page 1 of 7 1.1 FEATURES OF THOUGHT Thought can refer to the ideas or arrangements of ideas that result from thinking, the act of producing thoughts, or the process of producing thoughts. Despite

More information

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS BRUC W. HAMILL VISUAL PRCPTION OF STRUCTURD SYMBOLS A set of psychological experiments was conducted to explore the effects of stimulus structure on visual search processes. Results of the experiments,

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Supporting Information Variances and biases of absolute distributions were larger in the 2-line

More information

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features Dikran J. Martin Psychology 110 Name: Date: Lecture Series: Chapter 3 Sensation and Perception: Pages: 31 Making Contact with the World around Us TEXT: Baron, Robert A. (2001). Psychology (Fifth Edition).

More information

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 J'OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 446-452 (1963) Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 A visual configuration, or pattern, has qualities over and above those which can be specified

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Current Biology, in press Supplemental Information: Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Grigorios Sotiropoulos, Aaron R. Seitz, and Peggy Seriès Supplemental Data Detailed

More information

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay*

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* JACOB BECK and BRUCE AMBLER University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 other extreme, when no masking

More information

Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation

Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2008, 25 (7 8), 920 950 Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation Volker H. Franz and Karl R. Gegenfurtner University of Giessen, Giessen, Germany The finding

More information

Grouping does not require attention

Grouping does not require attention Perception & Psychophysics 2006, 68 (1), 17-31 Grouping does not require attention DOMINIQUE LAMY, HANNAH SEGAL, and LITAL RUDERMAN Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel Many theories of visual perception

More information

..." , \ I \ ... / ~\ \ : \\ ~ ... I CD ~ \ CD> 00\ CD) CD 0. Relative frequencies of numerical responses in ratio estimation1

... , \ I \ ... / ~\ \ : \\ ~ ... I CD ~ \ CD> 00\ CD) CD 0. Relative frequencies of numerical responses in ratio estimation1 Relative frequencies of numerical responses in ratio estimation1 JOHN C. BAIRD.2 CHARLES LEWIS, AND DANIEL ROMER3 DARTMOUTH COLLEGE This study investigated the frequency of different numerical responses

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE Isabelle Guay and Simon Grondin Université Laval, Québec, Canada Email: simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca ABSTRACT

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Javier R. Movellan Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0515 movellan@inc.ucsd.edu Thomas

More information

TEMPORAL CHANGE IN RESPONSE BIAS OBSERVED IN EXPERT ANTICIPATION OF VOLLEYBALL SPIKES

TEMPORAL CHANGE IN RESPONSE BIAS OBSERVED IN EXPERT ANTICIPATION OF VOLLEYBALL SPIKES TEMPORAL CHANGE IN RESPONSE BIAS OBSERVED IN ANTICIPATION OF VOLLEYBALL SPIKES Tomoko Takeyama, Nobuyuki Hirose 2, and Shuji Mori 2 Department of Informatics, Graduate School of Information Science and

More information

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity J. A. Richardson and I. Davies Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford GU2 5XH, Surrey, United

More information

Computational Architectures in Biological Vision, USC, Spring 2001

Computational Architectures in Biological Vision, USC, Spring 2001 Computational Architectures in Biological Vision, USC, Spring 2001 Lecture 11: Visual Illusions. Reading Assignments: None 1 What Can Illusions Teach Us? They exacerbate the failure modes of our visual

More information

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Limits to Iconic Memory 0 Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Ronald A. Rensink Departments of Psychology and Computer Science University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4 Canada Running Head: Limits

More information

How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations

How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations Motoyuki Saito (m-saito@kwansei.ac.jp) Department of Psychological Science, Kwansei Gakuin University Hyogo 662-8501, JAPAN Abstract

More information

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks*

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* ARNOLD D. WELLt University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 Multidimensional stimuli, which could vary on one, two, or all three

More information

Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot

Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot Rajani Raman* and Sandip Sarkar Saha Institute of Nuclear Physics, HBNI, 1/AF, Bidhannagar,

More information

Hemispheric performance in object-based attention

Hemispheric performance in object-based attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (1), 84-91 Hemispheric performance in object-based attention MONICA A. VALSANGKAR-SMYTH University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada CHRISTA-LYNN DONOVAN University

More information

Alternation blindness in the perception of binary sequences

Alternation blindness in the perception of binary sequences Alternation blindness in the perception of binary sequences Ru Qi Yu (ruqiyu@psych.ubc.ca) Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia Daniel Osherson (osherson@princeton.edu) Department of

More information

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3 Perception Chapter 8, Section 3 Principles of Perceptual Organization The perception process helps us to comprehend the confusion of the stimuli bombarding our senses Our brain takes the bits and pieces

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

Bar Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel. Ph.D. in Cognitive Psychology 1999.

Bar Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel. Ph.D. in Cognitive Psychology 1999. Yoav Arieh, Ph.D. Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology Montclair State University Montclair, NJ 07043 (973) 655-7639 ariehy@mail.montclair.edu EDUCATION Bar Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel.

More information

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli James S. Adelman (J.S.Adelman@warwick.ac.uk) Neil Stewart (Neil.Stewart@warwick.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, University

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect, so keep your eyes foveated on your own exam. I tend to think of a point as a minute, so be sure to spend the appropriate amount

More information

Controlling Stable and Unstable Dynamic Decision Making Environments

Controlling Stable and Unstable Dynamic Decision Making Environments Controlling Stable and Unstable Dynamic Decision Making Environments Magda Osman (m.osman@qmul.ac.uk) Centre for Experimental and Biological Psychology, Queen Mary University of London, London, E1 4NS

More information

Feature encoding and pattern classifications with sequentially presented Markov stimuli*

Feature encoding and pattern classifications with sequentially presented Markov stimuli* Feature encoding and pattern classifications with sequentially presented Markov stimuli* BLL R. BROWN and CHARLES E. AYLWORTH University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky 008 The major objective of this

More information

THE SYMBOLIC DISTANCE EFFECT FOR ALPHABETIC ORDER JUDGEMENTS : A SUBJECTIVE REPORT AND REACTION TIME ANALYSIS

THE SYMBOLIC DISTANCE EFFECT FOR ALPHABETIC ORDER JUDGEMENTS : A SUBJECTIVE REPORT AND REACTION TIME ANALYSIS Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (1978) 30, 33-43. THE SYMBOLIC DISTANCE EFFECT FOR ALPHABETIC ORDER JUDGEMENTS : A SUBJECTIVE REPORT AND REACTION TIME ANALYSIS J. M. E. HAMILTON AND A. J.

More information

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects Journal of Eye Movement Research 2(5):3, 1-6 Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects Candido V. B. B. Pessôa Edson M. Huziwara Peter

More information

Principals of Object Perception

Principals of Object Perception Principals of Object Perception Elizabeth S. Spelke COGNITIVE SCIENCE 14, 29-56 (1990) Cornell University Summary Infants perceive object by analyzing tree-dimensional surface arrangements and motions.

More information

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists Visual Imagery Study of Imagery Banned by behaviorists Possible subject of study in cognitive psychology Cognitive psychology is distinguished from the earlier behaviorism by its claim that there are internal

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Jennifer A. Kaminski (kaminski.16@osu.edu) Center for Cognitive Science, Ohio State University 10A

More information

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS?

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS? DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS? Emi Hasuo, Yoshitaka Nakajima, and Kazuo Ueda Graduate School of Design, Kyushu University -9- Shiobaru, Minami-ku, Fukuoka, - Japan

More information

Human experiment - Training Procedure - Matching of stimuli between rats and humans - Data analysis

Human experiment - Training Procedure - Matching of stimuli between rats and humans - Data analysis 1 More complex brains are not always better: Rats outperform humans in implicit categorybased generalization by implementing a similarity-based strategy Ben Vermaercke 1*, Elsy Cop 1, Sam Willems 1, Rudi

More information

Conformity. Jennifer L. Flint. The University of Southern Mississippi

Conformity. Jennifer L. Flint. The University of Southern Mississippi The modified Asch task: The Relationship between Actual Conformity and Self-Reported Conformity Jennifer L. Flint The University of Southern Mississippi 2 The modified Asch task: The Relationship between

More information

l3;~~?~~~,'0~'~~t~t:~:~~~~~~~~~~!,1

l3;~~?~~~,'0~'~~t~t:~:~~~~~~~~~~!,1 112 Sensation and Perception Line A should look longer, even though both lines are actually the same length. People who come from noncarpentered cultures that do not use right angles and corners often

More information

Exploring a brightness-drag illusion. Author. Published. Journal Title DOI. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Griffith Research Online

Exploring a brightness-drag illusion. Author. Published. Journal Title DOI. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Griffith Research Online Exploring a brightness-drag illusion Author Habota, Tina, Chappell, Mark Published 2011 Journal Title Perception DOI https://doi.org/10.1068/p6767 Copyright Statement 2011 Pion Ltd., London. The attached

More information

Key words: Perceptual development, depth from motion, texture perception, visual development, motor development

Key words: Perceptual development, depth from motion, texture perception, visual development, motor development Swiss Journal of Psychology 59 (2), 2000, 102 107 Visuo-motor development which causes detection of visual depth from motion and density cues 1 Keiichiro Tsuji 1, Keikichi Hayashibe 2 Masatoshi Hara 3

More information

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis Journal of Sound and Vibration Manuscript Draft Manuscript Number: JSV-D-10-00826 Title: Does filled duration illusion occur for very short time intervals? Article Type: Rapid Communication Keywords: time

More information

A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND. CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica

A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND. CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND CONTINGENT AFTER-EFFECT CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica A model is advanced to explain the McCollough effect and the contingent motion after-effect.

More information

Examining the Role of Object Size in Judgments of Lateral Separation

Examining the Role of Object Size in Judgments of Lateral Separation Examining the Role of Object Size in Judgments of Lateral Separation Abstract Research on depth judgments has found a small but significant effect of object size on perceived depth (Gogel & Da Silva, 1987).

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth Journal of Vision (2010) 10(4):1, 1 27 http://journalofvision.org/10/4/1/ 1 Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth Anna Seydell David C. Knill Julia Trommershäuser Department

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled?

How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 21, 72 (7), 1839-1853 doi:1758/app.72.7.1839 How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled? MIDORI TOKITA

More information

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH Laurence D. Smith University of Maine ldsmith@maine.maine.edu Ronald M. Pickett Marjan Trutschl Institute for Visualization and Perception

More information

Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements

Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements Vision Research 46 (2006) 2328 2335 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements Hyung-Chul O. Li * Department of Industrial Psychology, Kwangwoon

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Modeling Human Perception

Modeling Human Perception Modeling Human Perception Could Stevens Power Law be an Emergent Feature? Matthew L. Bolton Systems and Information Engineering University of Virginia Charlottesville, United States of America Mlb4b@Virginia.edu

More information

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 458 468 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.458

More information

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1 Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen gert.haubensak@t-online.de Abstract Two experiments are reported in which the participants judged

More information

Max Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik. Spemannstraße Tübingen Germany

Max Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik. Spemannstraße Tübingen Germany MP Max Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik Spemannstraße 38 72076 Tübingen Germany Technical Report No. 067 visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Volker

More information

Laboratory for Shape and Depth/Distance Perception

Laboratory for Shape and Depth/Distance Perception Name Laboratory for Shape and Depth/Distance Perception 1. Pictorial Depth Cues [Monocular Cues] a. Depth Cue Where it appears in the painting What time of day might be depicted in the painting and what

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions. Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions. The box interrupts the apparent motion. The box interrupts the apparent motion.

More information

PRESENTATION TITLE/S LEARNING OBJECTIVES START TIME FINISH TIME

PRESENTATION TITLE/S LEARNING OBJECTIVES START TIME FINISH TIME OEP VT for Art and Science DAY 1 What is Vision? The Anti-Gravity Circle Convergence & Divergence The Identification Circle The Speech & Language Circle Emergent Vision Processes The Space Representation

More information