MENTAL ROTATION IN A CALIFORNIA SEA LION (ZALOPHUS CALIFORNIANUS)

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "MENTAL ROTATION IN A CALIFORNIA SEA LION (ZALOPHUS CALIFORNIANUS)"

Transcription

1 The Journal of Experimental Biology, (1997) Printed in Great Britain The Company of Biologists Limited 1997 JEB MENTAL ROTATION IN A CALIFORNIA SEA LION (ZALOPHUS CALIFORNIANUS) B. MAUCK 1 AND G. DEHNHARDT 2, * 1 Universität Köln, Institut für Tierphysiologie, Weyertal 119, D-5931 Köln, Germany and 2 Universität Bonn, Zoologisches Institut, Poppelsdorfer Schloß, D Bonn, Germany Accepted 25 February 1997 Mental rotation is a widely accepted concept that suggests an analogue mode of visual informationprocessing in certain visuospatial tasks. Typically, these tasks demand the discrimination between the image and mirror-image of rotated figures, for which human subjects need an increasing reaction time depending on the angular disparity between the rotated figures. In pigeons, tests of this kind yielded a time-independent rotational invariance, suggested as being the result of a non-analogue information-processing that has evolved in response to the horizontal plane that birds perceive from above while flying. Given that marine mammals use the water surface as the horizontal plane for orientation while diving, the ability of a California sea lion to mentally rotate twodimensional shapes was tested. Using a successive twoalternative matching-to-sample procedure, the animal had to decide between the image and mirror-image of a Summary previously shown sample. Both stimuli were rotated by a multiple of 3 with respect to the sample. The animal s reaction time was measured by a computer-controlled touch-screen device, rewarding the animal for pressing its snout against the stimulus matching the sample. A linear regression analysis of the animal s mean reaction time against the angular rotation of the stimulus yielded a significant correlation coefficient. Thus, the present data can be explained by the mental rotation model, predicting an image-like representation of visual stimuli in this species. The present results therefore correspond well with those found for human subjects, but are inconsistent with the data reported for pigeons. Key words: mental rotation, mental representation, visuospatial information, mirror-image discrimination, California sea lion, Zalophus californianus. Introduction Mental representation is one of the central concepts of comparative cognition. Many researchers have tried to determine how objects in our environment are represented mentally and the nature and structure of these representations. The response behaviour of subjects in appropriate experiments could indicate the type and function of the representations used in different tasks. In a classical study, Shepard and Metzler (1971) found that the time it took human subjects to decide whether two figures were identical or mirror-images of each other was a linear function of the angular disparity between these figures. The interpretation of these results was that the subjects used an analogue transformation process which Shepard and Metzler (1971) called mental rotation, suggesting an image-like representation of visual information. Furthermore, it was argued that the process of imagining the rotation of an object should be equivalent to the process of perceiving the physical rotation of an existing object (Shepard and Cooper, 1982). It was also supposed that the same neural substrate that is involved in the perception of real motion also enables us to imagine the motion of objects retained in our memory (Jolicoeur and Cavanagh, 1992; Corballis and McLaren, 1982). Several studies have shown that factors such as the dimensionality of stimuli (Shepard and Metzler, 1988) or weightlessness (Matsakis et al. 1993) may affect the response characteristics of subjects, but the process of mental rotation has always been confirmed. Nevertheless, the analogue nature of the mental rotation process is still the subject of some debate (the so-called analogue-propositional debate; Pylyshyn, 1973, 1979a,b, 1981). In spite of these doubts, the idea of an imagelike representation has been suggested as the best explanation for such experimental results (Roitblat, 1987). Accepting the model of mental rotation for humans, the question remains whether the same mechanism underlies the visual information-processing of other organisms in such tasks. Hollard and Delius (1982) tested the ability of humans and pigeons to discriminate between the image and mirror-image of rotated two-dimensional stimuli in a matching-to-sample task. Their results indicated that humans used mental rotation, *Author for correspondence.

2 13 B. MAUCK AND G. DEHNHARDT whereas the reaction times of pigeons showed no linear increase with the angle of rotation. On the one hand, Hollard and Delius (1982) explained this rotational invariance by the pigeons ability to distinguish image and mirror-image as easily as arbitrarily different forms. While Lohmann et al. (1988) could not confirm this hypothesis, Delius and Hollard (1995) found evidence that pigeons discriminate mirror-images, relative to arbitrary shapes, more easily than do humans. On the other hand, Hollard and Delius (1982) as well as Delius and Hollard (1995) argued that the pigeons rotational invariance could have evolved in response to ecological demands. Birds use the ground as the horizontal reference plane while flying. This could have favoured the evolution of a special visual information-processing system that allows birds to recognise the landscape from the bird s-eye view in any orientation without any delay. Conversely, it is speculated that hominids may have secondarily lost this ability of efficient visual recognition regardless of relative orientations. Species living in an aquatic environment could use the water surface as a horizontal reference plane while diving. Pinnipeds, for example, can be observed making use of the dark silhouettes of prey contrasting with the bright water surface while hunting (Hobson, 1966). Furthermore, in visual discrimination experiments, seals and sea lions are often described as swimming upside down and on their side, which does not affect their choice accuracy. On the basis of such observations, Schusterman and Thomas (1966) hypothesized that visual perception in these marine mammals might be different from that of terrestrial mammals. We therefore wondered whether, during the course of evolution, ecological demands could have triggered an information-processing system similar to that suggested for pigeons, resulting in an equivalent rotational invariance in mental rotation tasks. Results that coincided with those found by Hollard and Delius (1982) would indicate a convergent evolution of informationprocessing systems. Materials and methods Subjects Owing to the scant availability of suitable test animals as well as the lengthy preparation period required for such animals, the present investigation is a case study. It was conducted at the Dolphinarium Münster, Germany, using a 6-year-old male California sea lion (Zalophus californianus Lesson). The animal was experimentally naive but, apart from the experiments, it was trained for the daily shows at the dolphinarium. Experiments were performed during the morning hours in two sessions, before and after the first show. Because the animal was fed for the last time each day in the late afternoon, there was a natural food deprivation of approximately 16 h before the first session started. Experiments were performed in a resting pool that allowed the separation of the subject from the other sea lions and dolphins kept at the dolphinarium. Stimuli and test apparatus Twelve two-dimensional stimuli similar to those of the study by Hollard and Delius (1982) were used for testing reaction times, and ten additional stimuli were used in the course of acquisition (Fig. 1). A test stimulus consisted of nine black squares (27 mm 27 mm) attached to each other at their sides and forming an asymmetrical shape. These shapes and their mirror-images were computer-designed, rotated by various multiples of 3, and printed in the centre of a white stimulus card (21 cm 25 cm). A diameter of 17 cm was not exceeded when shapes were rotated, and each shape occupied 12 % of the area of the stimulus card. The stimulus cards were shrinkwrapped in foil to protect them from water. In contrast to the study of Hollard and Delius (1982), stimuli were rotated both clockwise and counter-clockwise with respect to the previously shown upright sample shape; rotations of more than 18 are therefore regarded as smaller angles of the opposite direction. The computer-controlled experimental device was designed to present stimuli in a successive two-alternative matching-tosample procedure. The apparatus was positioned on a dry platform at the edge of the pool and could easily be reached by the animal (Fig. 2). At a distance of 1.5 m from the platform, a stationing hoop was installed immediately above the water surface. The test apparatus consisted of a black polyvinylchloride (PVC) board with three Perspex windows side by side in the lower part. The bottom edge of each window was approximately cm above the water surface. Behind each window was a halogen light (75 W) installed in a box constructed of opaque PVC. Stimulus cards could be mounted reversed in front of the windows. A stimulus was only visible when the respective stimulus card was illuminated from behind. With the light in the box turned off, the window appeared to be a white sheet. Above the right-hand and lefthand windows, light barriers were installed, with their infrared beams running vertically at a distance of approximately 1 cm in front of the stimulus cards. Breaking of the light barrier was detected by the computer and was used during a trial to switch off the stimuli and to record the animal s reaction time and choice, thereby functioning as a touch-screen device. The test apparatus was connected to a control unit sited in the vicinity of the experimenter. The control unit was used to start a trial and revealed information about the animal s response (correct choice or error). The control unit was connected to a portable PC set up in a room next to the experimental chamber. Customdesigned software was used to control the hardware and to record the animal s reaction time and response. Reaction time could be measured with an accuracy of.5 s. Experimental procedure At the beginning of a trial, the stimulus cards were mounted reversed in front of the three windows of the apparatus. Following a hand gesture, the animal positioned its head in the stationing hoop, facing the apparatus (Fig. 3A). In order to avoid giving the animal any cues, the experimenter hid behind the apparatus as soon as the animal reached its position in the

3 Mental rotation in a sea lion 1311 stationing hoop. A trial was started by the experimenter by pressing a button at the control unit, whereupon the computer switched on the light behind the sample in the middle of the apparatus. After the sample had been shown for 5 s, the computer switched it off and the two comparison stimuli were presented without delay. One comparison stimulus represented the image of the previously shown sample, while the other was its mirror-image. In mental rotation trials, both comparison stimuli were rotated in the same direction by a multiple of 3. The sea lion was rewarded for responding to the image of the sample. The appearance of the two comparison stimuli was the signal for the sea lion to choose. At the same time, the computer started to measure the reaction time. After leaving the hoop, the animal was asked to approach the apparatus and to press its snout against one stimulus card, thereby breaking the infrared light beam (Fig. 3B). The computer switched off all stimuli and recorded the animal s reaction time, while the control unit showed whether the response was correct. Correct choices were rewarded by a fish (Sprattus sprattus), but there was no punishment for incorrect choices. Sessions were composed according to pseudorandom schedules (Gellerman, 1933) concerning the presentation of comparison stimuli at both positions of the apparatus and the sequence of test stimuli. A session consisted of 24 trials. Learning criterion was defined as the animal s performance of at least 8 % correct choices (χ 2 -test, P<.1) in at least two successive sessions. Nevertheless, in order to establish the sea lion s performance during acquisition, more sessions were sometimes conducted after the animal had reached the criterion. Since the animal was experimentally naive, it had to become acquainted with the matching procedure as well as with image/mirror-image discriminations before tests with rotated shapes could be performed. For this reason, the study was subdivided into various phases of acquisition and testing. Results Acquisition of matching and image/mirror-image discriminations Training on the matching procedure was started by presenting the animal with two shapes (circle versus angular shape, nos 1 and 2 in Fig. 1) that were judged to be highly discriminable from each other. After a strong side bias during the first four sessions (resulting in 5 % correct choices, Fig. 4A), the sea lion showed a steady improvement in performance and exceeded 8 % correct choices in the tenth session. After the twelfth session, the animal s performance remained stable at greater than 85 % correct choices. The addition of a third stimulus (no. 3, triangle), which was paired against each of the initial stimuli, had only a minor effect on the animal s performance (Fig. 4B). The results of the next phase of acquisition, in which the animal was required for the first time to perform image/mirrorimage discriminations, are shown in Fig. 5. Two asymmetrical shapes (nos 4 and 5) and their mirror-images were used as stimuli in their normal upright orientation, both resembling those used later in tests with rotated shapes. With these new discrimination requirements, choice accuracy fell to chance level during the first two sessions, but then (with one exception) showed a steady increase until the sea lion reached criterion in sessions /11 and, after a small decline, in sessions 13/14 (Fig. 5A). We then replaced shapes 4 and 5 by eight novel ones (nos 6 13, Figs 1, 5B), all being equally represented during a session. As before, these shapes were paired against their mirror-images and were shown only in their normal upright orientation. This increase in complexity of the discrimination task again led to a decline in choice accuracy (Fig. 5B). Only after the nineteenth session did the sea lion s performance remain consistently above 8 % correct choices. Up to this stage of the experiment, the animal had seen all shapes and their mirror-images in their normal upright orientation. Before tests with rotated stimuli could be started, the sea lion had to become acquainted with the discrimination of such stimuli. For this purpose, shape no. 6 was presented not only in the orientation but also rotated by 3 and in both directions. The other seven stimuli (nos 7 13) continued to be shown exclusively in their normal upright orientation. While performance in non-rotation trials generally surpassed that of rotation trials during the first five sessions, this discrepancy in choice accuracy disappeared during the next five sessions (Fig. 6). Finally, the sea lion performed at greater than 87.5 % correct choices in both types of discrimination task. Subsequently, in addition to shape 6, Fig. 1. The stimuli used in the course of acquisition and during testing.

4 1312 B. MAUCK AND G. DEHNHARDT Control unit Mirror A Detector of infrared barrier Reflector of infra-red beam Presentation of the sample Stationing hoop Computer connection Box with halogen lamp Fig. 2. Schematic drawing of the experimental apparatus. B shapes 4, 5 and 14 were also rotated by 3,, 9 and 1. In the course of sessions, the animal was presented with tasks of increasing difficulty with regard to the angle of rotation (results not shown). The final performance of % correct choices in rotation as well as non-rotation trials was considered to be a good basis for the subsequent measurement of reaction times. Measurement of reaction times Testing was subdivided into six test series of 8 12 sessions each (Fig. 7). In each series, two stimuli were used which were unknown to the animal except in their normal upright orientation. The animal was familiarised in a few sessions before testing started with the normal upright orientation of novel stimuli, which had been presented neither at orientation nor rotated (nos 12, 13, 17 22). Sixty-one test sessions were performed, each consisting of trials. Rotation of stimuli in both possible directions (clockwise and counterclockwise) was well-balanced and the sequence of test stimulus presentation was determined according to pseudorandom schedules (Gellerman, 1933). In order to determine a mean reaction time as reference in every series, up to trials with stimuli in the normal orientation ( ) were interspersed in every session. The animal s performance was above chance (χ 2 -test, P<.5) in of 61 sessions; in 5 sessions, performance was 8 % or greater of correct choices (χ 2 -test, P<.1, Fig. 7). In all but one test series, performance was worst in the first session, but clearly improved in the second session. No significant preference for one direction of rotation could be detected from the number of correct choices (χ 2 -test, P<.5). Mean error rates and mean reaction times with standard deviations for correct responses were analysed for every test series and summarised in an overall evaluation as a function of the absolute angle of rotation (Fig. 8A,B). In spite of the increasing mean error rate from less than % at 3 to Fig. 3. The experimental procedure. During presentation of the sample, the animal was stationed in a hoop (A). After being presented with the comparison stimuli, the animal made its choice by pressing its snout against one of the shapes (B). Photograph taken by H. Müller-Elsner. approximately 3 % at 9, before it decreased again at higher angles of rotation (Fig. 8A), performance was always significantly above chance (χ 2 -test, P<.5). Mean reaction times were calculated from correct choices in 439 trials ( ), 126 trials (3 ), 125 trials ( ), 126 trials (9 ), 281 trials (1 ), 283 trials (15 ) and 88 trials (18 ). Although data points indicate a sigmoid curve with a slight decline in reaction time for rotations of 15 and 18, linear regression analysis yielded a significant correlation (r=.873, P<.5) between mean reaction time and absolute angle of rotation (Fig. 8B). This correlation was unaffected in separate analyses of reaction times for clockwise and counterclockwise rotations (clockwise, r=.76, P<.5; counterclockwise, r=.9, P<.1). Discussion The first basic requirement for testing the model of mental rotation was the animal s success in performing matching-tosample tasks. The sea lion mastered the initial training problems with little difficulty, and its performance remained

5 Mental rotation in a sea lion 1313 Correct choices (%) A Number of sessions Fig. 4. The animal s acquisition of the matching rule. (A) Performance during training with two stimuli. (B) Performance after adding a third stimulus. well above chance in sessions with novel stimulus configurations and comparable with that of the pigeons in the study by Hollard and Delius (1982). Unlike Hollard and Delius, we do not conclude from this performance that the sea lion applied a concept-like matching rule (same different or identity concept) for solving mental rotation problems. For the interpretation of such a generalised matching concept, a sufficiently large sample size of first-trial data is necessary (Thomas and Noble, 1988; Oden et al. 1988; Schusterman and Kastak, 1993), which most experiments on mental rotation do not provide. Although California sea lions have been shown to fulfil this first-trial criterion in concept formation experiments (Hille, 1988; Kastak and Schusterman, 1994), this ability is not essential for solving mental rotation problems. On the contrary, the constant use of the images of the test shapes as sample and S+, while their mirror-images were always designated as S, may have favoured the formation of stimulus-specific rules rather than a concept of same or different. Furthermore, the use Correct choices (%) A B Number of sessions Fig. 5. Acquisition of mirror-image discriminations. (A) Performance during training with two asymmetrical shapes. (B) Performance after replacing the two initial shapes with eight new ones. B Correct choices (%) Number of sessions Fig. 6. Performance after introducing a rotated stimulus., correct choices in all trials;, correct choices in rotation trials only;, correct choices in non-rotation trials only;, stimulus used in rotation trials. of a generalised matching concept and the use of stimulusspecific rules are not mutually exclusive (Roitblat and von Fersen, 1992), so that the sea lion could have used both strategies simultaneously. Whichever discrimination rule a subject applies to a mental rotation task, as long as it maintains a high level of performance, the nature of the rule should not affect reaction times. The sea lion also complied with the second basic requirement for this study in reliably performing image/mirrorimage discriminations, which cannot be taken for granted in animals. Pigeons have much more difficulty in discriminating mirror-image patterns than discriminating arbitrarily different pairs of patterns (Lohmann et al. 1988). They find mirrorimage patterns especially difficult to discriminate if the patterns are reflected along their vertical axis (Todrin and Blough, 1983). However, Delius and Hollard (1995) found evidence that pigeons at least in comparison with humans have less difficulty with mirror-images than with arbitrary shapes. Regarding primates, bushbabies (Galago senegalensis) have been shown to be greatly confused by mirror-image discriminations (Sanford and Ward, 1986), whereas baboons (Papio anubis) have little difficulty in solving such tasks (Hopkins et al. 1993). Our sea lion also had little difficulty in performing mirror-image discriminations. Nevertheless, during testing with rotated stimuli, which can be assessed as an additional source of difficulty, performance was worst in the first session in all but one series. According to D Amato et al. (1985), low performance levels in first sessions in matching-to-sample tasks can be explained by the so-called novelty effect, a disruptive, neophobic response of subjects. Transfer criterion is therefore often determined as in the

6 1314 B. MAUCK AND G. DEHNHARDT 9 Correct choices (%) 8 7 Fig. 7. Performance during the six test series. Rotated test shapes and angles of rotation are shown below the respective data curves. 5, 3, 3, 1, 1, 1, 1, 9, , 18 15, 18 3 Number of sessions 4 5 study by Hollard and Delius (1982) neglecting the first session after a change of stimuli. Although our sea lion s performance seems to be impaired by a novelty effect, choice accuracy differed significantly from chance level in all but one of the first sessions with novel stimulus configurations. While Schusterman and Thomas (1966) demonstrated, even before the phenomenon of mental rotation had been described, that a California sea lion readily recognizes shapes irrespective of their spatial orientation, the present results show that this ability remains unaltered when complex image/mirror-image discriminations are required. As predicted by the model of mental rotation, the animal s mean reaction times increased linearly with angular disparity. The overall reaction time is composed of both the time it takes the animal to approach the apparatus and the time used in decision-making. Since our sea lion had to cover a comparatively long distance from its stationing hoop to the test board, its overall reaction times compared with those of human subjects or pigeons (Hollard and Delius, 1982) were rather long. However, since the time it took the sea lion to approach the apparatus can be assumed to be relatively constant, the increase in overall reaction time may be ascribed to an increasing time required to make a decision. As in many other studies on mental rotation (Just and Carpenter, 1976; Shepard and Cooper, 1982; Folk and Duncan Luce, 1987; Kail, 1991; Corballis and Manalo, 1993; Corballis and Sidey, 1993), the sea lion s reaction times deviate from linearity at some angles of rotation. Although, at first glance, a sigmoid curve seems to provide a better fit to the data presented in Fig. 8B, a decrease in reaction time at angles of 15 and 18 can be explained by the model of mental rotation. Shepard and Cooper (1982) reported that, beyond 18, reaction times are bimodally distributed, with the upper mode corresponding to the linear extrapolation of the reaction-time function to angles larger than 18 and the lower mode corresponding to the linear extrapolation of that function backwards to a point at the same distance from 18. This suggests that reaction time is not necessarily determined by the angular disparity between two stimuli but rather by the particular direction the long or the short way that the subject takes to mentally rotate a stimulus. Furthermore, a subject might change its direction of mental rotation during a trial for two reasons: either when it recognises that the other way round would be shorter, or when it recognises that mental rotation of a particular asymmetrical stimulus would be easier in one of two possible directions. Taking this into consideration, reaction times that are shifted upwards are to be expected, especially for higher angles (below 18 ), while a switch in direction of mental rotation would seldom occur at 18, because at this angle it takes the same Errors (%) Time (s) A B Angle of rotation (degrees) Fig. 8. Overall evaluation of the animal s performance and reaction time. (A) Mean error rate as a function of the angle of rotation. (B) Mean reaction times (±S.D., N= ) as a function of the angle of rotation.

7 Mental rotation in a sea lion 1315 time to rotate the shape in either direction. Although these arguments could explain deviations from linearity for some angles, a case study such as this one cannot prove these considerations. The rotational invariance found in pigeons and the nonincreasing reaction time function of baboons when they see shapes in their left optical hemifield (Vauclairs et al. 1993) are assumed to be adaptations which have arisen phylogenetically because of the demand to operate visually on a horizontal reference plane (Hollard and Delius, 1982; Delius and Hollard, 1995). In spite of similar ecological demands on pinnipeds, which use the water surface as the horizontal reference plane, we could detect no rotational invariance in our sea lion. In fact, our data can be explained assuming a mental rotation of an image-like representation of visual stimuli, a result that is normally obtained in experiments with human subjects. An image-like representation of visual information in California sea lions has also been concluded from artificial language comprehension tests (Schusterman and Krieger, 1986), in which the animals transformed gestural signs referencing specific object properties into image-like representations of those objects. Obviously, ecological demands such as a horizontal reference plane are not necessarily correlated with the evolution of information-processing systems that facilitate a rotational invariance. However, given the variety of methods used in studies on mental rotation, it should be asked whether certain methodological details could trigger a rotational invariance. Besides differences in the intensity of training, our study differed from that of Hollard and Delius (1982) in the way in which the stimuli were rotated. We demanded from our animal mental rotation in both possible directions by presenting it with stimuli which were rotated clockwise as well as counterclockwise with respect to the previously shown sample shape. In contrast, the stimuli used in the study of Hollard and Delius (1982) were rotated only clockwise with respect to the sample. Assuming that the comparison stimuli are to be rotated back to the normal upright position of the sample, the pigeons would have had to perform a counterclockwise mental rotation. Delius and Hollard (1995) also varied the angle of the sample shapes, whereas the comparison shapes were presented in the normal upright orientation. At first glance, this suggests that subjects in this case would have had to perform a clockwise mental rotation, rotating the comparison stimuli back to the sample s orientation. Nevertheless, the use of a simultaneous testing procedure as well as the growing familiarity with stimuli in their normal upright orientation may have favoured the opportunity to mentally rotate the sample instead of the comparison stimuli, resulting in a counterclockwise rotation as well. With regard to these differences concerning the direction of mental rotation it is interesting to consider the extent to which both cerebral hemispheres are involved in visuospatial processes. While, in general, studies which tried to estimate the nature of human hemispheric specialisation for mental rotation have produced inconsistent results (e.g. Ornstein et al. 198; Deutsch et al. 1988; Fischer and Pellegrino, 1988; Dittuno and Mann, 199), the results from those that examined the involvement of both hemispheres in clockwise and counterclockwise mental rotation are more consistent. Burton et al. (1992) showed that counterclockwise rotation was processed more efficiently by the left hemisphere/right visual field, coinciding with the results of Corballis and Sergent (1989). Cook et al. (1994) found a more efficient hemispheric cooperation with the left hemisphere taking the part of active manipulation of a mental image, while the reference role is performed by the right hemisphere. During the past two decades, various studies have also shown a strong cerebral lateralization in the visual system of pigeons. In most cases, a dominance of the left hemisphere/right visual field was found for certain visual tasks (Güntürkün, 1985; Güntürkün and Kesch, 1987; von Fersen and Güntürkün, 199). However, until now, there has been no study that examined the influence of this visual lateralization in pigeons on the ability to perform mental rotation tasks. Certainly, it would be interesting to determine whether the pigeons dominance of the left hemisphere favours performance in tasks that demand counterclockwise rotation, as might be supposed bearing in mind the results of Burton et al. (1992). If this hypothesis were to be confirmed, it could explain the consistently fast reaction times found for pigeons. In the present study, we provide the first evidence for mental rotation in a non-primate species. The appearance of this mental rotation effect in spite of similar ecological demands for birds and pinnipeds could also be ascribed to the fact that the horizontal reference plane is not as important to pinnipeds as has been concluded from their aquatic life-style. However, if ecological demands are responsible for the phenomenon of rotational invariance in pigeons, it should also be detected in other avian species. Appropriate experiments are currently being carried out. We are grateful to Martin Dambach for his support throughout this study. We are especially indebted to Wolfgang Antpöhler for technical assistance. References BURTON, L. A., WAGNER, M., LIM, C. AND LEVY, J. (1992). Visual field differences for clockwise and counterclockwise mental rotation. Brain Cogn. 18, COOK, N., FRÜH, H., MEHR, A., REGARD, M. AND LANDIS, T. (1994). Hemispheric cooperation in visuospatial rotations: Evidence for a manipulation role for the left hemisphere and a reference role for the right hemisphere. Brain Cogn. 25, CORBALLIS, M. C. AND MANALO, R. (1993). Effect of spatial attention on mental rotation. Neuropsychologia 31, CORBALLIS, M. C. AND MCLAREN, R. (1982). Interaction between perceived and imagined rotation. J. exp. Psychol. Human Percept. Perform. 8, CORBALLIS, M. C. AND SERGENT, J. (1989). Hemispheric specialization for mental rotation. Cortex 25,

8 1316 B. MAUCK AND G. DEHNHARDT CORBALLIS, M. C. AND SIDEY, S. (1993). Effects of concurrent memory load on visual-field differences in mental rotation. Neuropsychologia 31, D AMATO, M. R., SALMON, D. P. AND COLOMBO, M. (1985). Extent and limits of the matching concept in monkeys (Cebus apella). J. exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Proc. 11, DELIUS, J. D. AND HOLLARD, V. D. (1995). Orientation invariant pattern recognition by pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens). J. comp. Psychol. 9, DEUTSCH, G., BOURBON, W. T., PAPANICOLAOU, A. C. AND EISENBERG, H. M. (1988). Visuospatial tasks compared via activation of regional cerebral blood flow. Neuropsychologia 26, DITTUNO, P. L. AND MANN, V. A. (199). Right hemisphere specialization for mental rotation in normals and brain damaged subjects. Cortex 26, FISCHER, S. C. AND PELLEGRINO, J. W. (1988). Mental rotation and orientation-invariant object recognition. Brain Cogn. 7, FOLK, M. D. AND DUNCAN LUCE, R. (1987). Effects of stimulus complexity on mental rotation rate of polygons. J. exp. Psychol. Human Percept. Perform. 13, GELLERMAN, L. W. (1933). Chance orders of alternating stimuli in visual discrimination experiments. J. genet. Psychol. 42, 6 8. GÜNTÜRKÜN, O. (1985). Lateralization of visually controlled behavior in pigeons. Physiol. Behav. 34, GÜNTÜRKÜN, O. AND KESCH, S. (1987). Visual lateralization during feeding in pigeons. Behav. Neurosci. 1, HILLE, P. (1988). Versuche zur Ungleicherkennnung beim Kalifornischen Seelöwen (Zalophus californianus) unter besonderer Berücksichtigung der Begriffsbildung. MSc thesis, University of Münster. HOBSON, E. S. (1966). Visual orientation and feeding in seals and sea lions. Nature 2, HOLLARD, V. D. AND DELIUS, J. D. (1982). Rotational invariance in visual pattern recognition by pigeons and humans. Science 218, HOPKINS, W. D., FAGOT, J. AND VAUCLAIR, J. (1993). Mirror-image matching and mental rotation problem solving by baboons (Papio papio): Unilateral input enhances performance. J. exp. Psychol. gen. 122, JOLICOEUR, P. AND CAVANAGH, P. (1992). Mental rotation, physical rotation and surface media. J. exp. Psychol. Human Percept. Perform. 18, JUST, M. A. AND CARPENTER, P. A. (1976). Eye fixations and cognitive processes. Cognit. Psychol. 8, KAIL, R. (1991). Controlled and automatic processing during mental rotation. J. exp. Child Psychol. 51, KASTAK, D. AND SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. (1994). Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). Anim. Learn. Behav. 22, LOHMANN, A., DELIUS, J. D., HOLLARD, V. D. AND FRIESEL, M. F. (1988). Discrimination of shape reflections and shape orientations by Columba livia. J. comp. Psychol. 2, MATSAKIS, Y., LIPSHITS, M., GURFINKEL, V. AND BERTHOZ, A. (1993). Effects of prolonged weightlessness on mental rotation of threedimensional objects. Exp. Brain Res. 94, ODEN, D. L., THOMPSON, R. K. R. AND PREMACK, D. (1988). Spontaneous transfer of matching in infant chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. J. exp. Psychol. Animal Behav. Proc. 14, ORNSTEIN, R., JOHNSTONE, J., HERON, J. AND SWENCIONIS, C. (198). Differential right hemisphere engagement in visuospatial tasks. Neuropsychologia 18, PYLYSHYN, Z. W. (1973). What the mind s eye tells the mind s brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychol. Bull. 8, PYLYSHYN, Z. W. (1979a). The rate of mental rotation of images: A test of a holistic analogue hypothesis. Mem. Cogn. 7, PYLYSHYN, Z. W. (1979b). Validating computational models: A critique of Anderson s indeterminacy of representation claim. Psychol. Rev. 86, PYLYSHYN, Z. W. (1981). The imagery debate: Analogue media versus tacit knowledge. Psychol. Rev. 88, ROITBLAT, H. L. (1987). Introduction to Comparative Cognition. New York: Freeman. ROITBLAT, H. L. AND VON FERSEN, L. (1992). Comparative cognition: Representations and processes in learning and memory. A. Rev. Psychol. 43, SANFORD, C. G. AND WARD, J. P. (1986). Mirror-image discrimination and hand preference in the bushbaby (Galago senegalensis). Psychol. Rec. 36, SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. AND KASTAK, D. (1993). A California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) is capable of forming equivalence relations. Psychol. Rec. 43, SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. AND KRIEGER, K. (1986). Artificial language comprehension and size transposition by a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). J. comp. Psychol., SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. AND THOMAS, T. (1966). Shape discrimination and transfer in the California sea lion. Psychon. Sci. 5, SHEPARD, R. N. AND COOPER, L. A. (1982). Mental Images and their Transformations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. SHEPARD, R. N. AND METZLER, J. (1971). Mental rotation of threedimensional objects. Science 171, SHEPARD, S. AND METZLER, D. (1988). Mental rotation: Effects of dimensionality of objects and type of task. J. exp. Psychol. Human Percept. Perform. 14, THOMAS, R. K. AND NOBLE, L. M. (1988). Visual and olfactory oddity learning in rats: What evidence is necessary to show conceptual behavior? Anim. Learn. Behav. 16, TODRIN, D. C. AND BLOUGH, D. S. (1983). The discrimination of mirror-image forms by pigeons. Percept. Psychophys. 34, VAUCLAIRS, J., FAGOT, J. AND HOPKINS, W. D. (1993). Rotation of mental images in baboons when the visual input is directed to the left cerebral hemisphere. Psychol. Sci. 4, VON FERSEN, L. AND GÜNTÜRKÜN, O. (199). Visual memory lateralization in pigeons. Neuropsychologia 28, 1 7.

Developmental Changes in the Interference of Motor Processes with Mental Rotation

Developmental Changes in the Interference of Motor Processes with Mental Rotation Developmental Changes in the Interference of Motor Processes with Mental Rotation Andrea Frick (a.frick@psychologie.unizh.ch) Cognitive and Developmental Psychology, Department of Psychology, University

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses? Mental Imagery What is Imagery? What is nature of the images? Exact copy of original images? Represented in terms of meaning? If so, then how does the subjective sensation of an image arise? What is the

More information

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 J'OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 446-452 (1963) Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 A visual configuration, or pattern, has qualities over and above those which can be specified

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus californianus)

Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (4), 427 435 Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) DAVID KASTAK University of California, Santa Cruz, California

More information

Same/different discrimination learning with trial-unique stimuli

Same/different discrimination learning with trial-unique stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (3), 644-650 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.3.644 Same/different discrimination learning with trial-unique stimuli Daniel I. Brooks and Edward A. Wasserman University of Iowa,

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection

Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection Vijay Laxmi, J. N. Carter and R. I. Damper Image, Speech and Intelligent Systems (ISIS) Research Group Department of Electronics and Computer Science University

More information

References. Carter, D.E., & Werner, T.J. (1978). Complex learning and information processing by

References. Carter, D.E., & Werner, T.J. (1978). Complex learning and information processing by References Carter, D.E., & Werner, T.J. (1978). Complex learning and information processing by pigeons: a critical analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 565-601. Cook, R.G., Katz,

More information

Visual Transformation of Size

Visual Transformation of Size Journal ol Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1975, Vol. 1, No. 3, 214-220 Visual Transformation of Size Glaus Bundesen and Axel Larsen Copenhagen University, Denmark To investigate

More information

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity J. A. Richardson and I. Davies Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford GU2 5XH, Surrey, United

More information

The Role of Mental Rotations in Primate-inspired Robot Navigation

The Role of Mental Rotations in Primate-inspired Robot Navigation The Role of Mental Rotations in Primate-inspired Robot Navigation Ronald C. Arkin School of Interactive Computing Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA U.S.A. 30332 Abstract The use of a primate's

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Using Visual Information for Spatial Advice-giving in a Primate-inspired Autonomous Robot

Using Visual Information for Spatial Advice-giving in a Primate-inspired Autonomous Robot Using Visual Information for Spatial Advice-giving in a Primate-inspired Autonomous Robot Ronald Arkin, Lakshmi Velayudhan, and Michael Pettinati School of Interactive Computing Georgia Tech, Atlanta,

More information

Pattern recognition in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): The role of the spatial organisation of stimulus parts

Pattern recognition in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): The role of the spatial organisation of stimulus parts Behavioural Brain Research 181 (2007) 96 109 Research report Pattern recognition in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): The role of the spatial organisation of stimulus parts Carlo De Lillo a,, Giovanna

More information

Left hemispheric advantage for numerical abilities in the bottlenose dolphin

Left hemispheric advantage for numerical abilities in the bottlenose dolphin Behavioural Processes 68 (2005) 179 184 Short report Left hemispheric advantage for numerical abilities in the bottlenose dolphin Annette Kilian a,b,, Lorenzo von Fersen b, Onur Güntürkün a a Department

More information

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons Entire Set of Printable Figures For Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons Cook In one experiment, Cerella (1980) trained pigeons to discriminate intact drawings of Charlie Brown from normal drawings

More information

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists Visual Imagery Study of Imagery Banned by behaviorists Possible subject of study in cognitive psychology Cognitive psychology is distinguished from the earlier behaviorism by its claim that there are internal

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment

More information

Dynamics of Color Category Formation and Boundaries

Dynamics of Color Category Formation and Boundaries Dynamics of Color Category Formation and Boundaries Stephanie Huette* Department of Psychology, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN Definition Dynamics of color boundaries is broadly the area that characterizes

More information

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition 1 Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition Intro Memory Empirical Studies Interf MR Scan LTM Codes DCT Imagery & Spatial Cognition Rel Org Principles ImplEnc SpatEq Neuro Imaging Critique StruEq Prop

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Infant and Child Development Inf. Child Dev. 10: 39 45 (2001) DOI: 10.1002/icd.245 Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Olivier Pascalis a, *, Elisabeth

More information

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object Supplemental materials for: The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object David Whitney*, David A. Westwood, & Melvyn A. Goodale* *Group on Action and Perception, The

More information

Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans 1

Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans 1 Japanese Psychological Research 1997, Volume 39, No. 3, 236 252 Special Issue: Cognition and behavior of chimpanzees Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans 1 NOBUAKI

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS BRUC W. HAMILL VISUAL PRCPTION OF STRUCTURD SYMBOLS A set of psychological experiments was conducted to explore the effects of stimulus structure on visual search processes. Results of the experiments,

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Reorientation by Slope Cues in Humans. Daniele Nardi, Amanda Y. Funk, Nora S. Newcombe & Thomas F. Shipley

Reorientation by Slope Cues in Humans. Daniele Nardi, Amanda Y. Funk, Nora S. Newcombe & Thomas F. Shipley Reorientation by Slope Cues in Humans Daniele Nardi, Amanda Y. Funk, Nora S. Newcombe & Thomas F. Shipley Department of Psychology, Temple University, Spatial Intelligence and Learning Center, Philadelphia,

More information

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze Psychology, 2014, 5, 451-460 Published Online April 2014 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.55055 Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception Competing Frameworks in Perception Lesson II: Perception module 08 Perception.08. 1 Views on perception Perception as a cascade of information processing stages From sensation to percept Template vs. feature

More information

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception Competing Frameworks in Perception Lesson II: Perception module 08 Perception.08. 1 Views on perception Perception as a cascade of information processing stages From sensation to percept Template vs. feature

More information

Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus. Rhiannon D. Thomas

Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus. Rhiannon D. Thomas Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus Rhiannon D. Thomas A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

More information

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Sarah J. Miles (smiles25@uwo.ca) and John Paul Minda (jpminda@uwo.ca) Department of Psychology The University of Western Ontario London, ON N6A 5C2 Abstract

More information

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3 Perception Chapter 8, Section 3 Principles of Perceptual Organization The perception process helps us to comprehend the confusion of the stimuli bombarding our senses Our brain takes the bits and pieces

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces N. F. Troje* and D. Kersten *Max-Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik, Spemannstr. 38, 72076 Tübingen, Germany Department of Psychology, University

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science

2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science 2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science Stanislas Dehaene Chair in Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture No. 4 Constraints combination and selection of a

More information

Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning

Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning Mary Hegarty Department of Psychology University of California, Santa Barbara Santa Barbara, CA 93106 hegarty@psych.ucsb.edu Abstract This paper examines capacity

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

The Regression-Discontinuity Design

The Regression-Discontinuity Design Page 1 of 10 Home» Design» Quasi-Experimental Design» The Regression-Discontinuity Design The regression-discontinuity design. What a terrible name! In everyday language both parts of the term have connotations

More information

Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of first- and second-order relations

Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of first- and second-order relations Anim Cogn (2003) 6 : 77 86 DOI 10.1007/s10071-003-0159-x ORIGINAL ARTICLE Jennifer Vonk Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of first- and second-order relations

More information

Human experiment - Training Procedure - Matching of stimuli between rats and humans - Data analysis

Human experiment - Training Procedure - Matching of stimuli between rats and humans - Data analysis 1 More complex brains are not always better: Rats outperform humans in implicit categorybased generalization by implementing a similarity-based strategy Ben Vermaercke 1*, Elsy Cop 1, Sam Willems 1, Rudi

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Underwater hearing in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): Expansion and interpretation of existing data

Underwater hearing in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): Expansion and interpretation of existing data MARINE MAMMAL SCIENCE, **(*): *** *** (*** 2011) C 2011 by the Society for Marine Mammalogy DOI: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2011.00473.x Underwater hearing in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): Expansion

More information

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 152 162 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000053 Memory Systems

More information

A Search for Derived Stimulus Relations in Rats

A Search for Derived Stimulus Relations in Rats A Search for Derived Stimulus Relations in Rats MARK GALIZIO UNC WILMINGTON TXABA HOUSTON TX MARCH, 2018 These projects were supported in part by NIH grant DA29252 WHERE DO NOVEL STIMULUS RELATIONS COME

More information

PERCEPTION AND ACTION

PERCEPTION AND ACTION PERCEPTION AND ACTION Visual Perception Ecological Approach to Perception J. J. Gibson in 1929 Traditional experiments too constrained Subjects cannot move their heads Study of snapshot vision Perception

More information

Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence. Edwin Blake

Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence. Edwin Blake Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence Edwin Blake edwin@cs.uct.ac.za This Talk 2 Perception Bottom-up Top-down Integration Presence Bottom-up Top-down BIPs Presence arises from

More information

Male and female guppies differ in speed but not in accuracy in visual. discrimination learning. Supplementary materials

Male and female guppies differ in speed but not in accuracy in visual. discrimination learning. Supplementary materials Male and female guppies differ in speed but not in accuracy in visual discrimination learning Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato and Angelo Bisazza Supplementary materials 1. Additional methods Ethical statement Experiments

More information

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice Multiple Choice 1. Which structure is not part of the visual pathway in the brain? a. occipital lobe b. optic chiasm c. lateral geniculate nucleus *d. frontal lobe Answer location: Visual Pathways 2. Which

More information

The Comparative Psychology of Avian Visual Cognition

The Comparative Psychology of Avian Visual Cognition 83 The Comparative Psychology of Avian Visual Cognition Robert G. Cook 1 Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts Abstract How do small-brained, highly mobile animals like birds

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action Categorization and Discrimination Animals respond to stimuli in ways that suggest they form categories. Pigeons

More information

3 MODELS AND COMPONENTS OF IMAGERY

3 MODELS AND COMPONENTS OF IMAGERY 3 MODELS AND COMPONENTS OF IMAGERY 3.1 Kosslyn s model Research concerning how cognitive processes operate in mental imagery generation is a task for the supporters of the analog model. These authors,

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Exp Brain Res (2002) 142:349 353 DOI 10.1007/s00221-001-0899-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Richard A. Abrams Mark B. Law Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Received: 10 May 2000 / Accepted: 4 September

More information

Discrimination of Structure: I. Implications for Connectionist Theories of Discrimination Learning

Discrimination of Structure: I. Implications for Connectionist Theories of Discrimination Learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2001, Vol. 27, No. 3, 206-218 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/01/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0097-7403.27.3.206

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information

Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification. and. Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect. Nila K Leigh

Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification. and. Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect. Nila K Leigh 1 Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification and Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect Nila K Leigh 131 Ave B (Apt. 1B) New York, NY 10009 Stuyvesant High School 345 Chambers

More information

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli Lecture 19: Animal Behavior I. Background A. Animal behavior reflects and arises from biological properties 1. Exhibited behavior defends on the physiological systems and processes unique to a given organism

More information

CONCEPTUAL BEHAVIOR IN RATS: CROSS MODALITY NON-MATCHING-TO- SAMPLE USING THREE DIMENSIONAL AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rachel A. Eure

CONCEPTUAL BEHAVIOR IN RATS: CROSS MODALITY NON-MATCHING-TO- SAMPLE USING THREE DIMENSIONAL AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rachel A. Eure CONCEPTUAL BEHAVIOR IN RATS: CROSS MODALITY NON-MATCHING-TO- SAMPLE USING THREE DIMENSIONAL AND OLFACTORY STIMULI Rachel A. Eure A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial

More information

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Laboratory Equipment & Test Procedures. Motor learning and control historically has built its discipline through laboratory research. This has led to the

More information

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

CANTAB Test descriptions by function CANTAB Test descriptions by function The 22 tests in the CANTAB battery may be divided into the following main types of task: screening tests visual memory tests executive function, working memory and

More information

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* Perception & Psychophysics 1973. Vol. 13. So. 2.356-360 Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* ANGEL VASSILEV+ Institu te of Physiology. Bulgarian Academy of Sciences.

More information

Mental Rotation is Not Easily Cognitively Penetrable

Mental Rotation is Not Easily Cognitively Penetrable Mental Rotation is Not Easily Cognitively Penetrable The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation Published Version

More information

Behavioral and Brain Asymmetries in Nonhuman Species

Behavioral and Brain Asymmetries in Nonhuman Species Behavioral and Brain Asymmetries in Nonhuman Species Analogous System Similar in function; But not in origin or structure Homologous System Share a common structure and common origins Right-hand bias for

More information

Change Blindness. Change blindness is the inability to recognize changes to objects in the environment

Change Blindness. Change blindness is the inability to recognize changes to objects in the environment Change Blindness Change blindness is the inability to recognize changes to objects in the environment after interruption, i.e. an object in the environment changes while an individual is looking away,

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Supporting Information Variances and biases of absolute distributions were larger in the 2-line

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

Discriminating the Relation Between Relations: The Role of Entropy in Abstract Conceptualization by Baboons (Papio papio} and Humans (Homo sapiens)

Discriminating the Relation Between Relations: The Role of Entropy in Abstract Conceptualization by Baboons (Papio papio} and Humans (Homo sapiens) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 200, Vol. 27, No. 4, 36-328 Copyright 200 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/0/55.00 DOI: 0.037//0097-7403.27.4.36 Discriminating

More information

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization Acoust. Sci. & Tech. 24, 2 (23) PAPER The role of low components in median plane localization Masayuki Morimoto 1;, Motoki Yairi 1, Kazuhiro Iida 2 and Motokuni Itoh 1 1 Environmental Acoustics Laboratory,

More information

A Direct Object of Perception

A Direct Object of Perception E-LOGOS Electronic Journal for Philosophy 2015, Vol. 22(1) 28 36 ISSN 1211-0442 (DOI 10.18267/j.e-logos.411),Peer-reviewed article Journal homepage: e-logos.vse.cz A Direct Object of Perception Mika Suojanen

More information

CHASING WITH A MODEL EYE

CHASING WITH A MODEL EYE J. exp. Biol. 137, 399-409 (1988) 399 Printed in Great Britain The Company of Biologists Limited 1988 CHASING WITH A MODEL EYE BY STEPHEN YOUNG Department of Pure and Applied Biology, Imperial College,

More information

Dynamic shifts of pigeon local/global attention

Dynamic shifts of pigeon local/global attention Anim Cogn (2002) 5 :233 243 DOI 10.1007/s10071-002-0152-9 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Thane Fremouw Walter T. Herbranson Charles P. Shimp Dynamic shifts of pigeon local/global attention Received: 28 January 2002

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

How Animals Classify Friends and Foes

How Animals Classify Friends and Foes CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE How Animals Classify Friends and Foes Ronald J. Schusterman, 1 Colleen J. Reichmuth, and David Kastak Long Marine Laboratory, Institute of Marine Sciences, University

More information

Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces

Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces Perception, 1999, volume 28, pages 483 ^ 487 DOI:10.1068/p2901 Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces Nikolaus F Trojeô Max-Planck Institut fïr biologische Kybernetik, Spemannstrasse 38, 72076

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Javier R. Movellan Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0515 movellan@inc.ucsd.edu Thomas

More information

innate mechanism of proportionality adaptation stage activation or recognition stage innate biological metrics acquired social metrics

innate mechanism of proportionality adaptation stage activation or recognition stage innate biological metrics acquired social metrics 1 PROCESSES OF THE CORRELATION OF SPACE (LENGTHS) AND TIME (DURATIONS) IN HUMAN PERCEPTION Lev I Soyfer To study the processes and mechanisms of the correlation between space and time, particularly between

More information

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons by Thomas A. Daniel A thesis proposal submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Auburn University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree

More information

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

SPATIAL UPDATING 1. Do Not Cross the Line: Heuristic Spatial Updating in Dynamic Scenes. Markus Huff. Department of Psychology, University of Tübingen

SPATIAL UPDATING 1. Do Not Cross the Line: Heuristic Spatial Updating in Dynamic Scenes. Markus Huff. Department of Psychology, University of Tübingen SPATIAL UPDATING 1 Huff, M., & Schwan, S. (in press). Do not cross the line: Heuristic spatial updating in dynamic scenes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0293-z The final publication

More information

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge Kimery R. Levering (kleveri1@binghamton.edu) Kenneth J. Kurtz (kkurtz@binghamton.edu) Department of Psychology, Binghamton

More information

Individual differences in the mixture ratio of rotation and nonrotation trials during rotated mirror/normal letter discriminations

Individual differences in the mixture ratio of rotation and nonrotation trials during rotated mirror/normal letter discriminations Mem Cogn (2012) 40:594 613 DOI 10.3758/s13421-011-0172-2 Individual differences in the mixture ratio of rotation and nonrotation trials during rotated mirror/normal letter discriminations Jordan A. Searle

More information

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l Journal oj Experimental Psychology 12, Vol. 63, No. 3, 289-293 REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l MAURICE HERSHENSON 2 Brooklyn College In measuring reaction time (RT) to simultaneously

More information

LEA Color Vision Testing

LEA Color Vision Testing To The Tester Quantitative measurement of color vision is an important diagnostic test used to define the degree of hereditary color vision defects found in screening with pseudoisochromatic tests and

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents

Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents Michail Maniadakis 1 and Marc Wittmann 2 and Panos Trahanias 1 1- Foundation for Research and Technology - Hellas, ICS, Greece 2- Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology

More information