Acquisition of bar-pressing under interval and ratio-schedules in the presence and absence of stimuli correlated with water delivery

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Acquisition of bar-pressing under interval and ratio-schedules in the presence and absence of stimuli correlated with water delivery"

Transcription

1 EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2009, 10, NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2009) 19 Acquisition of bar-pressing under interval and ratio-schedules in the presence and absence of stimuli correlated with water delivery Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Jazmín Carvajal, Ulises Valdez, Carlos Torres, and Alfredo Mayoral University of Guadalajara After one-session shaping, eight rats were exposed to interval or ratio schedules in the absence and attenuation of the usual dispenser-based stimuli correlated with the delivery of water, and eight rats to normal delivery water conditions. Half of the rats were exposed to a (fixed-interval) FI 1-min schedule; the other half was exposed to a (fixed-ratio) FR 10 schedule of water reinforcement. Experimental phases consisted of two exposures to an FI or FR schedule interpolated with one (continuous reinforcement) CRF schedule, and a final (fixed-time) FT period. Rats did not show typical scallop-like or break-andrun patterns of responding. Rats responded more in the absence of reinforcement delivery correlated stimuli during the CRF interpolated phase than rats in the presence of such stimuli. Responding and obtained reinforcers were higher during the CRF schedule than under the FI and FR schedules. Responding during the FT period was similar to that in the prior FI or FR phase. The results are discussed in terms of the central role of transitions from CRF reinforcement in order to achieve high rates and characteristic patterns of FI and FR schedules. Keywords: Interval schedules; Ratio schedules; Rats; Continuous reinforcement; Reinforcementdelivery-correlated-stimuli Simple schedules of intermittent reinforcement originated in the analysis of the extinction ratio in conditioning (Skinner, 1938). Fixedinterval (FI) and fixed ratio (FR) schedules consisted in the manipulation of the extinction ratio through periodic delivery of reinforcement for a given response and in the independent manipulation of the time between successive reinforcers, respectively. Later on (Ferster & Skinner, 1957), intermittent schedules were systematized in terms of variations from the continuous reinforcement procedure (CRF). Fixed- and variable-interval schedules consisted in prescribing a non-functional time for the reinforcement of a single response, so that they could be conceived as CRF schedules with an Address correspondence to the first author at Centro de Estudios e Investigaciones en Comportamiento, Apartado Postal 5-374, Zapopan, México. ribes@cencar.udg.mx 19 unsignalled time-out requirement from the previous reinforcement. Fixed- and variable-ratio schedules could be seen as a direct transformation of the one-response requirement of the CRF schedule (Fixed-ratio 1) into a requirement of more than one response per reinforcement. The study of reinforcement schedules developed as the systematic analysis and combination of response-reinforcer requirements based upon two prescription rules: the passage of time (interval schedules), or the counting of responses (ratio schedules) prior to the response to be reinforced (Schoenfeld & Cole, 1972). Ferster and Skinner (1957) showed that interval and ratio performances followed different modes of transition and patterning as documented through cumulative records. Although parametric restrictions applied, fixedinterval performance was usually characterized

2 20 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Jazmín Carvajal, Ulises Valdez, Carlos Torres, and Alfredo Mayoral as a positively accelerated pattern of responding between reinforcers (a scallop-like pattern, in the case of longer intervals or during initial training) or as a post-reinforcer zero-response state followed by a high-rate state (in the case of shorter intervals or after extended training), while fixed-ratio performance consisted of a constant, high-rate pattern of responding preceded by a post-reinforcement pause (steplike pattern). The consistency of these findings suggested two different processes regulating scheduled-controlled behavior: temporal discrimination and counting behavior. Although other factors participate in both types of schedule-controlled performance, it has been widely accepted that their determination heavily relies on the discrimination of the time elapsed since the preceding reinforcement and of the number of responses since reinforcement, respectively (Crossman, Heaps, Nunes, & Alferink, 1974; DeCasper & Zeiler, 1977; Harzem, Lowe, & Spencer, 1978; Priddle-Higson, Lowe, & Harzem, 1976). However, reinforcement involves a different contingency in the case of interval and ratio schedules. If contingency consists in how the distribution of responses determines the distribution of reinforcers (Schoenfeld, Cole, Lang, & Mankoff, 1973), in ratio schedules the frequency of reinforcers is directly dependent on the frequency of responses, whereas in interval schedules the temporal restriction imposed by the schedule breaks this dependence and reinforcement distribution tends to be regular or constant, irrespectively of variations in response frequency (Dews, 1970). Ferster and Skinner (1957) stressed the importance of gradual and consistent transitions from CRF schedules to interval and ratio reinforcement in order to obtain stable patterns of performance typical of both types of schedules. Although shaping is used as a default procedure to condition an operant response (as bar-pressing or key-pecking), given the topography of responses normally selected and the size of the experimental chamber, rats and pigeons, if allowed, show operant levels higher than zero prior to any shaping or reinforcement procedure (Skinner, 1938; Ribes & López- Valadéz, 1979). The effect of the shaping (or auto-shaping) procedure is to increase the likelihood of the contact between a given response and the presentation of a stimulus consequence. The limited experimental literature on CRF schedules shows that, compared to intermittent interval and ratio reinforcement, continuous reinforcement produces less variation in several properties or dimensions of responding: topography, effort, duration, and location (Eckerman & Lanson, 1969; Margulies, 1961; Notterman & Mintz, 1965; Stebbins & Lanson, 1962). If simple intermittent schedules are seen as cases of local extinction parameters, response patterning typical of interval and ratio schedules would result from the sequence of local extinction parameters manipulated after stable performance under continuous reinforcement. The immediate exposure to ratio and interval schedules after shaping, without the consistent presentation of continuous reinforcement, should prevent that behavior could make contact with programmed contingencies of reinforcement. Millenson and Hurwitz (1961) found that after training under a CRF, extinction increased the dispersion of two IRT classes with longer runs of only one of the IRT classes. Boren (1955) and Mechner (1958) suggested that these changes in extinction after continuous reinforcement may constitute the mechanism through which fixed ratio patterning is developed, since runs in extinction centered along the fixed-ratio requirement. A similar mechanism may take place during fixedinterval conditioning. Reinforcement delivery involves other stimulus changes in the experimental chamber, namely, the sound of the mechanism providing the reinforcer at the time that the general light of the chamber is switched off and the water or food dispenser s light is switched on. Skinner (1938) pointed out that conditioning the lever-pressing response in rats actually consisted of a chain of reflexes ending with seizing the food. The sound of the food magazine and the propioceptive stimulation, correlated with lever pressing at the moment of the reinforcer delivery, were thought to be important intermediate variables affecting the acquisition and rate of the conditioned response. Skinner showed that secondary conditioning in acquisition

3 Acquisition Under Interval and Ratio-Schedules 21 could be obtained by following lever pressing with the sound of the food magazine previously correlated with food delivery. He also showed that similar extinction effects could be observed by eliminating the sound of the magazine and maintaining food deliveries, or by leaving the magazine empty but maintaining the correlated sound. Skinner explained these effects in terms of the discriminative and secondary reinforcing properties of the various stimuli in the chain of reflexes involved in pressing the lever for food. Present accounts of interval and ratio schedule-controlled performance are based on discriminatory or inhibitory processes that depend on the occurrence of the reinforcing stimulus after shaping and exposure to continuous reinforcement, and gradual steps in the transition from continuous to intermittent reinforcement (Ferster & Skinner, 1957; Carr & Reynolds, 1974; Harzem, Lowe, & Priddle-Higson, 1978; Staddon, 1970, 1974; Zeiler & Solano, 1982). However, there is no research about two important procedural variables related to acquisition of schedule-controlled behavior: 1) the effects of the immediate exposure to intermittent reinforcement after shaping, and 2) the effects of the absence or presence of the reinforcement delivery correlated stimuli (RDCS), such as the sound and light of the dispenser or turning the house light off. If prior continuous reinforcement and the stimuli correlated with the delivery of reinforcers are important for the acquisition and maintenance of schedulecontrolled behavior, then the performance of interval and ratio schedules should change in their absence. Hypotheses regarding time and count discriminations should be redirected toward the discriminative properties of stimuli correlated with the presentation of the reinforcer, and the role of continuous reinforcement as a necessary antecedent of response patterning under intermittent schedules. Typical ratio schedule performance should be disrupted in a larger degree than interval schedule performance because of the dependence relation that the former schedule prescribes between response and reinforcement frequencies. An experiment was done on bar pressing in rats, to evaluate the effects of omitting sustained continuous reinforcement before exposing the rats to non-signaled fixed interval or ratio schedules. To evaluate the effects of the sound and light correlated with the delivery of water on the acquisition and patterning of performance under these schedules, half of the rats were not exposed to them. CRF and Fixed-time (FT) reinforcement probes were included in order to test the sensitivity of behavior to reinforcement frequency and contingency, after direct exposure to intermittent interval and ratio schedules. Method Subjects Four groups of four male Wistar old albino rats each, six-months old (Groups 1 and 2), and twelve-months old (Groups 3 and 4) at the start of the experiment, were used. All of the rats were experimentally naive and maintained under a daily schedule of 22½ hrs water deprivation. After each experimental session, the rats had free access to water for 30 min. They also had free access to solid food (Purina Chow) in their home cages; the rats weights ranged between 380 and 400 gr. The rats were run once a day in four separate experimental chambers from 9:00 to 10:00, from 10:00 to 11:00 AM, and from 5 to 7:30 PM, six days per week for 70 sessions. Apparatus Four standard operant conditioning chambers for rats, two Coulbourn Instruments (E10-10TC) and two custom-made (30 x 25.5 x 32 cm), were housed in sound-attenuating cubicles. Each chamber had a water dispenser (E14-05) at the bottom center of the front panel, as well as one lever on the right side of the water dispenser, 26 mm from the right wall, and 25 mm from the grid floor. The lever required 0.24 N to operate. One 28-V bulb provided general illumination and was located in the upper section of the front panel above the water dispenser; a second 28-V bulb was located in the water dispenser itself. For Groups 1 and 2, the sound produced by the water dispenser was attenuated by slowing down the delivery mechanism. The attenuation of the sound by the dispenser did not delay the delivery of water.

4 22 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Jazmín Carvajal, Ulises Valdez, Carlos Torres, and Alfredo Mayoral Table 1. Description of the experimental design. Phase 1 30 Sessions Phase 2 5 sessions Phase 3 30 sessions Phase 4 5 sessions Groupn 1 W/O CS FI 1 CRF FI 1 FT 1 Group 2 W CS FI 1 CRF FI 1 FT 1 Group 3 W/O CS FR 10 CRF FR 10 FT Yoked Group 4 W CS FR 10 CRF FR 10 FT Yoked W/O CS: Without correlated stimuli W CS: With correlated stimuli The house light was on all the time and the light of the water dispenser was never lit. For Groups 3 and 4 the water dispenser mechanism operated normally, and the house light was out and the light of the water dispenser on when water was delivered. A reinforcer consisted of a 0.01 cm 3 drop of water available for 3 s in a small cup. All responses were recorded with their time of occurrence. An AT386-TURBO PC computing system with a DIO Paraport interface scheduled events and recorded responses. The program was written in PASCAL 7.0. Procedure The rats were trained to press the lever through a hand-shaping procedure. Training ended when each rat obtained 100 reinforcers in a single session. Rats in Groups 1 and 2 were shaped under the attenuated reinforcement delivery procedure, whereas rats in Groups 3 and 4 were shaped under the normally operating dispenser mechanism. Table 1 describes the experimental design in this study. Half of the rats (R1 to R4, and R9 to R12) were exposed to two experimental phases with fixed-interval schedules and two probe periods. The other half of the rats (R5 to R8, and R13 to R16) were exposed to fixed-ratio schedules and two probe periods. Experimental phases and probe periods consisted of a fixed number of sessions, irrespectively of any performance criterion. Experimental phases consisted of 30 sessions in order to evaluate possible effects of an extended exposure to the initial reinforcement schedule. Probe periods consisted of 5 sessions. Each session lasted 60 min. Rats R1 to R4 and rats R9 to R12 were exposed to the following sequence: a) fixed-interval (FI) 1-min schedule, b) CRF probe, c) FI 1-min schedule, and d) fixed-time (FT) 1-min probe. Rats R5 to R8 and rats 13 to 16 were exposed to the following sequence: a) fixed-ratio (FR) 10 schedule, b) CRF probe, c) FR 10 schedule, and d) yoked FT probe. For rats R5 to R8 and rats R13 to 16, the value of the yoked FT schedule used in the probe period was calculated individually so as to match the mean interval between reinforcers in the previous FR experimental phase. Probes were designed to evaluate the effects of reinforcing every response or a constant frequency of reinforcement delivery after the exposure to interval or ratio schedules. Rats R1 to R8 were exposed to the absence or attenuation of stimuli correlated with the delivery of reinforcement (RDCS), whereas R9 to R16 were exposed to the normal procedure including these stimuli. Results Figure 1 shows the median number and standard deviation of responses and water deliveries per session for the four groups of rats. During the first phase, rats in the four groups responded under the direct exposure to FI or FR schedules of water. Responding was higher in Groups 1 and 3 without RDCS, although median number of bar-pressing responses never exceeded 30 responses per session. It was also observed that responding was higher in the FI groups than in the FR groups under equivalent conditions (presence or absence of RCS). Me-

5 Acquisition Under Interval and Ratio-Schedules 23 MEDIAN NUMBER OF RESPONSES AND WATER DELIVERIES PER SESSION GROUP 1 (W/O CORRELATED STIMULI) FI CRF FI FT GROUP 3 (W/O CORRELATED STIMULI) FR CRF FR FT FI CRF FI FT Figure 1. Shows the median number of responses and standard deviations for each group of rats during experimental and probe phases. dian number of water deliveries per session was about 20 for the FI groups, whereas it was lower than 10 in the FR groups. During the CRF probe, responding increased in the four groups of rats, with larger number of responses for rats in the FI group without RDCS. Median number of responses per session was slightly higher EXPERIMENTAL CONDITION 0 GROUP 2 (W CORRELATED STIMULI) GROUP 4 (W CORRELATED STIMULI) FR CRF FR FT Responses Water Deliveries than 300 in Group 1, and about 100 in Group 4 that showed the extreme values in this period. Median number of water deliveries was always lower than the median number of responses, since rats used to continue pressing the bar during the water delivery period. In the second experimental phase with repeated exposure to the FI and FR schedules, median number of responses and water deliveries were similar to those in the first phase, excepting Group 1 which showed more than 100 responses and around 40 water deliveries per session. During the last FT probe, median number of responses and water deliveries tended to be slightly lower than in the previous FI or FR period. Standard deviations show that variation in the number of responses was larger during CRF and FR than during FI sessions. Figure 2 shows the same data for each individual rat. Figure 3 shows the cumulative records for one rat of each group during the experimental and probe periods. Visual inspection of cumulative records show that response patterning under Figure 2. Shows the median number of responses and standard deviations for each rat during experimental and probe phases.

6 24 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Jazmín Carvajal, Ulises Valdez, Carlos Torres, and Alfredo Mayoral FR and FI schedules in this experiment do not correspond to performances usually obtained after a sequential training involving shaping, continuous reinforcement and gradual transitions. FI performance did not show positive acceleration between reinforcers or two-state, short-ratio-like patterns, and FR performance did not show break-and-run patterns or changes Figure 3. Shows cumulative recordings for one rat of each group in each of the experimental and probe phases.

7 Acquisition Under Interval and Ratio-Schedules 25 in the PRP size correlated with reinforcement frequency. Discussion The results of this study show that, after exposure to a single shaping session, rats acquired lever pressing under FI and FR schedules, both in the presence and in the absence and attenuation of the stimuli usually correlated with reinforcement delivery (RDCS). However, the performances developed under these conditions, as well as those under subsequent exposure to a CRF schedule probe, were not similar to those obtained under standard procedures. The latter procedures, not only involve the sound of the reinforcement delivery mechanism and the illumination of the dispenser at the time the house light is turned off, but also include gradual transitions in the intermittent delivery of reinforcers after several sessions of shaping and continuous reinforcement. As a general result, the performances observed in this study differed from the idealized scalloping and pause-and-run patterns described in textbooks (Reynolds, 1968), as well as from the two-state patterns observed after extended training (Cumming & Schoenfeld, 1958; Ferster & Skinner, 1957; Schneider, 1969; Shull & Brownstein, 1970; Branch & Gollub, 1974). All rats without RDCS (except R7 under a FR schedule) responded in the first session after shaping. However, in spite of some sessions without responses during the initial phase, responding was different under the FI and the FR schedules. Rats under the FI schedule performed with an overall range of responses in the first session, and then showed an increasing and relatively stable frequency of responses and obtained reinforcers during the remainder of the experimental phases. In contrast, rats under the FR schedule either showed a large number of responses in the first session (100 to 500) or emitted less than 10 responses. In the following experimental phases, responding under the ratio schedules was generally variable. Rats in the presence of RDCS showed lower frequencies of responding than rats in the absence of RDCS. Different performances were also observed under the FI and the FR schedule during the initial phase. Rats under the FI schedule showed a high number of responses in the first session (70 to 500), obtaining from 20 to 35 reinforcers. In the following experimental phase, responding never exceeded 200 responses per session and obtained reinforcers varied from 8 to 35. Rats under the FR schedule emitted from 55 to 200 responses in the first session, but showed less variability in the following sessions than the rats in the absence of RDCS, obtaining less than 10 reinforcers in most sessions. During probe sessions, CRF performance was similar to that previously reported by Hurwitz (1958). Responding tended to accelerate after the first reinforcements, with periods of no responding alternated with further high frequency of responses. In the presence of the CRF, rats showed a large number of responses (a small number of them non-reinforced because they occurred during reinforcer presentation). Responding under FT schedules was slightly reduced relative to the preceding phase in all rats (Lachter, 1971; Lattal & Maxey, 1971; Lattal, 1972). The responding during the CRF probe shows that low responding under the FI and FR schedules was due to the peculiar contingencies that arose as result of the initial contact with intermittent reinforcement. In fact, in spite of the large number of responses during the CRF probe, responding during the second exposure to the FI and FR schedules did not increase, suggesting the importance of transitions in the development of local contingencies in intermittent schedules. If intermittent reinforcement is seen as a local parameter of extinction, the responding observed during FI and FR schedules fit with Schoenfeld (1968) remarks, that in extinction is obtained what has been reinforced in conditioning. Local extinction and conditioning are confounded when acquisition is developed under intermittent reinforcement, without a previous stable period of continuous reinforcement. The large number of reinforcers obtained during CRF probe in the absence of RDCS suggests that the terminal within-session decrease in response frequency reported under interval schedules, cannot be attributed to a satiation

8 26 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Jazmín Carvajal, Ulises Valdez, Carlos Torres, and Alfredo Mayoral or habituation threshold, and it may be due to factors not yet identified (Carlton, 1961; Killeen, 1995; McSweeney & Roll, 1998; Reese & Hogenson, 1962). Most of the water available in each session was consumed. At the end of every session, water had to be refilled in each dispenser, showing that the rats consumed from 2 to 20 ml of water per session. The non-occurrence of the patterns usually associated with fixed-ratio and fixed-interval schedules may be an effect of the absence of exposure to continuous reinforcement and transitions prior to the exposure to interval or ratio schedules. Nevertheless, RDCS seemed to influence the total output of responses per session. Scallop-like, two-state, and break-and-run patterns have been considered as evidence of temporal discriminations and counting processes taking place in fixedinterval and fixed-ratio behavior, respectively. In the case of fixed-interval behavior, it has been assumed that the reinforcement occurrence plays the role of a discriminative or inhibitory stimulus regarding the next reinforcement occurrence. In the case of fixed-ratio behavior, the presentation of reinforcement produces a pause whose length is inversely related to the ratio requirement, and serves to discriminate the size of the required number of responses, like a response- counting marker. However, it seems that in our experiment, the presentation of the reinforcer with or without correlated stimuli was not sufficient to allow for the discrimination of the inter-reinforcement interval or of the number of responses required by the schedule. The importance of signaling the moment of reinforcement delivery has been shown by Reed (2003), who found that the presentation of a brief tone or a diffuse light correlated with the delivery of food reinforcement, enhanced the frequency of responding on FI schedules. However, the effect of signaling reinforcement delivery depended on the local rates correlated with the reinforcer presentation. Reed (1989) found that when three responses were required at the end of a variable interval, the signal increased response rate, whereas the same signal produced a decrease in rate when only one response was required at the end of the interval. These results suggest that reinforcement delivery correlated stimuli play different roles in FI and FR schedules. Since FI schedules impose restrictions to reinforcement and response distribution in time, reinforcement presentation may signal both eating behavior (reinforcement onset) and not responding (post-reinforcement pause) controlled by the absence of reinforcement in the immediate subsequent period (Schildkraut, 1982). On the contrary, CRF and FR schedules do not impose restrictions to reinforcement and response distribution. Thus, in CRF and FR schedules reinforcement presentation seems to interrupt ongoing operant behavior, at the time that induces additional responding after the consummatory response. This interpretation is partially supported by our finding that CRF reinforcement produced high response rates without reinforcement-correlated stimuli. Discriminating the time between reinforcers and the number of responses might be the outcome of a behaviorinterrupting effect of the stimuli accompanying reinforcement presentation (Guthrie, 1935, 1939; Mueller & Schoenfeld, 1954). Although rats in this experiment did not show the consistent or usual patterns found under fixed-interval and fixed-ratio schedules, responding was sensitive to the response-reinforcer contingency. Contrary to what could be expected from a single shaping session and the immediate exposition to the intermittent schedules, all rats except one (R7) responded on their first experimental session, and some of them with a large number of responses. Reinforcement presentation was effective without any stimulus correlated with its delivery. This finding is consistent with the effectiveness of the one-session shaping procedure and with the high rates developed under the CRF probes. The sensitivity to the reinforcement contingency is additionally confirmed by the fact that, in contrast to food reinforcement that consists in a pellet dropping down to a tray, water reinforcement is not an intruding event in the chamber, and the rat has to look for it inside the dispenser. The exposure to the CRF probe resulted in increases of response frequency, suggesting that responding was sensitive to the one-to-one dependency between responses and reinforcers and, therefore, to increments in the local density of reinforcers. On the other hand, the higher frequencies of responding obtained in the

9 Acquisition Under Interval and Ratio-Schedules 27 absence of RDCS suggest that stimuli correlated with reinforcement do not necessarily develop conditioned reinforcing functions (Wike, 1966; Hendry, 1969; Davison & Baum, 2006). The results of this study reminds of the need to conceive schedule performance as the outcome of complex sequences of transitions of response-reinforcer contingencies. The quantitative effects of reinforcers seem to be contrived by the differential patterning of behavior resulting of response-reinforcer episodes. Responses occur as part of molar segments of behavior including other non-measured responses (Schoenfeld & Farmer, 1970; Staddon & Simmelhag, 1971). Further parametric research is needed to understand the patterning and quantitative relations between identified operant responses and other responses as an outcome of transitions from continuous to intermittent reinforcement. References Boren, J.J. (1955). Response rate and resistance to extinction as functions of fixed ratio. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University. Branch, M.N., & Gollub, L.R. (1974). A detailed analysis of the effects of d-amphetamine on behavior under fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, Carlton, P.L. (1961). The interacting effects of deprivation and reinforcement schedule. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, Carr, E.G., & Reynolds, G.S. (1974) Temporal inhibition: Effects of changes of reinforcement and rate of responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, Crossman, E.K., Heaps, R.S., Nunes, D.L., & Alferink, L.A. (1974). The effects of number of responses on pause length with temporal variables controlled. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, Cumming, W.W., & Schoenfeld, W.N. (1958) Behavior under extended exposure to a highvalued fixed-interval schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, Davison, M., & Baum, W.M. (2006). Do conditional reinforcers count? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 86, DeCasper, A.J., & Zeiler, M.D. (1977). Time limits for completing fixed ratios. IV. Components of the ratio. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, Dews, P.B. (1970) The theory of fixed-interval responding. In W.N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules (pp ). New York: Appleton Century Crofts. Eckerman, D.A., & Larson, R.N. (1969). Variability of response location for pigeons responding under continuous reinforcement, intermittent reinforcement, and extinction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, Ferster, C.B., & Skinner, B.F. (1957) Schedules of Reinforcement. New York: Appleton Century Crofts. Guthrie, E.R. (1935). The psychology of learning. New York: Harper & Row. Guthrie, E.R. (1939). The effect of outcome on learning. Psychological Review, 46, Harzem, P., Lowe, C.F., & Priddle-Higson, P.J. (1978) Inhibiting function of reinforcement: Magnitude effects on variable-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 30, Harzem, P., Lowe, C.F., & Spencer, P.T. (1978) Temporal control of behavior: Schedule interactions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 30, Hendry, D.P.(1969). Conditioned reinforcement. Homewood, Ill: Dorsey Press. Hurwitz, H. M. B. (1958). A source of error in estimating the number of reinforcements in a lever pressing apparatus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, Killeen, P.R. (1995). Economics, ecologics, and mechanics: The dynamics of responding under conditions of varying motivation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, Lachter, G.D. (1971). Some temporal parameters of non-contingent reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 16, Lattal, K.A., & Maxey, G.C. (1971). Some effects of response independent reinforcers in

10 28 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Jazmín Carvajal, Ulises Valdez, Carlos Torres, and Alfredo Mayoral multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 16, Lattal, K.A.(1972). Response-reinforcer independence and conventional extinction after fixed-interval and variable-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, Margulies, S. (1961). Response duration in operant level, regular reinforcement, and extinction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 4, Mechner, F. (1958). Sequential dependencies of the lengths of consecutive response runs. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1, McSweeney, F. K., & Roll, J.M. (1998). Do animals satiate or habituate to repeatedly presented reinforcers?. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, Millenson, J. R., & Hurwitz, H. M. B. (1961). Some temporal and sequential properties of behavior during conditioning and extinction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 4, Mueller, C.G., & Schoenfeld, W.N. (1954). Edwin R. Guthrie. In W.K. Estes, S. Koch, K. MacCorquodale, P.E. Meehl, C.G. Mueller, W.N. Schoenfeld, and W.S. Verplanck (Eds.), Modern learning theory (pp ). New York: Appleton Century Crofts. Notterman, J.M., & Mintz, D.E. (1965). Dynamics of response. New York: J. Wiley. Priddle-Higson, P.J., Lowe, C.F., & Harzem, P. (1976). Aftereffects of reinforcement on variable-ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, Reed, P. (1989). Influence of interresponse time reinforcement on signalled-reward effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, Reed, P. (2003). The effect of signaled reinforcement on rats fixed-interval responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 79, Reese, T.W., & Hogenson, M.J. (1962). Food satiation in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, Reynolds, G.S. (1968). A primer of operant conditioning. Glenview, Ill.: Scott Foresman. Ribes, E. & López, F. (1979) La adquisición de operantes concurrentes bajo un programa señalado de reforzamiento definido temporalmente. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 5, Schildkraut, V. (1982). The effects of independently varying the temporal position of onset and offset of an intruded stimulus in a fixed-interval schedule. Doctoral dissertation, The City University of New York. Schneider, B.A. (1969). A two-state analysis of fixed-interval responding in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, Schoenfeld, W. N. (1968). On the difference in resistance to extinction following regular and periodic reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 11, Schoenfeld, W.N., & Farmer, J. (1970). Reinforcement schedules and the behavior stream. In Schoenfeld, W.N. (Ed.), The theory Of reinforcement schedules (pp ). New York: Appleton Century Crofts. Schoenfeld, W.N., & Cole, B.K. (1972). Stimulus schedules: The t-tau systems. New York: Harper & Row. Schoenfeld, W.N., Cole, B.K., Lang, J., & Mankoff, R. (1973). Contingency in behavior theory. In F.J. McGuigan, D.B. Lumsden (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to conditioning and learning (pp ). New York: Winston. Shull, R.L., & Brownstein, A.J. (1970). Interresponse time duration in fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement: Control by ordinal position and time since reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14, Skinner, B.F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton Century Crofts. Staddon, J.E.R. (1970) Effect of reinforcement duration on fixed-interval responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, Staddon, J.E.R. (1974). Temporal control, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 81, Staddon, J.E.R., & Simmelhag, V. (1971). The superstition experiment: A reexamination of

11 Acquisition Under Interval and Ratio-Schedules 29 its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review, 78, Stebbins, W. C., & Lanson, R. N. (1962). Response latency as a function of reinforcement schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 5, Wike, E.L. (1966). Secondary reinforcement: Selected experiments. New York: Harper & Row. Zeiler, M.D., & Solano, J.M. (1982) Responses and pauses: Discrimination and a choice catastrophe. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37,

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 79-91 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 79 Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats Rogelio Escobar National Autonomous University of Mexico

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 261-268 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) CONCURRENT SCHEULES OF PRIMARY AN CONITIONE REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' ONAL W. ZIMMERMAN CARLETON UNIVERSITY Rats responded

More information

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1998, 70, 165 183 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1 Japanese Psychological Research 1998, Volume 40, No. 2, 104 110 Short Report Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response

More information

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 5, NUMBF- 4 OCITOBER, 1 962 THE EFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES OF PRIMARY AND CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT JOAN G. STEVENSON1 AND T. W. REESE MOUNT HOLYOKE

More information

Human Schedule Performance with Hypothetical Monetary Reinforcement

Human Schedule Performance with Hypothetical Monetary Reinforcement EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2001, 2, 225-234 NUMBER 2 (WINTER 2001)225 Human Schedule Performance with Hypothetical Monetary Reinforcement University College London, UK Experiments examined the

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Phil Reed. Learn Behav (2011) 39:27 35 DOI /s Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

Phil Reed. Learn Behav (2011) 39:27 35 DOI /s Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010 Learn Behav (211) 39:27 35 DOI 1.17/s1342-1-3-5 Effects of response-independent stimuli on fixed-interval and fixed-ratio performance of rats: a model for stressful disruption of cyclical eating patterns

More information

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior.

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR TECHNIQUES FOR ESTABLISHING SCHEDULES WITH WHEEL RUNNING AS REINFORCEMENT IN RATS IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA 1993, 60, 219-238 NUMBER 1

More information

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction PUNISHMENT A ND RECO VER Y D URING FIXED-RA TIO PERFORMA NCE' NATHAN H. AZRIN2 ANNA STATE HOSPITAL When a reinforcement is delivered according to a fixed-ratio schedule, it has been found that responding

More information

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, 215 231 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 00, 79, 6 NUMBER (MARCH) RESPONDING FOR SUCROSE AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: EFFECTS OF SUCROSE CONCENTRATION AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCER DURATION

More information

Schedules of Reinforcement

Schedules of Reinforcement Schedules of Reinforcement MACE, PRATT, ZANGRILLO & STEEGE (2011) FISHER, PIAZZA & ROANE CH 4 Rules that describe how will be reinforced are 1. Every response gets SR+ ( ) vs where each response gets 0

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1974 Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer Richard H. Weiss Western

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/ Research Commons at the University of Waikato Copyright Statement: The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). The thesis

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER UNIVERSITY OF IOWA 1969, 12, 451-461 NUMBER

More information

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT'

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' D. ALAN STUBBS AND PHILIP J. SILVERMAN UNIVERSITY OF MAINE, ORONO AND WORCESTER

More information

Pausing time allocation and constraint on output

Pausing time allocation and constraint on output University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 1992 Pausing time allocation and constraint on output Nora N. Kametani

More information

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010 Notes Textbook used Week 1-3: Bouton, M.E. (2016). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. 2nd Ed. Sinauer Week 4-6: Rieger, E. (Ed.) (2014) Abnormal Psychology:

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Transfer of persistence across reinforced behaviors

Transfer of persistence across reinforced behaviors Animal Learning & Behavior 1979, 7 (4), 493-498 Transfer of persistence across reinforced behaviors ROBERT EISENBERGER University ofdelaware, Newark, Delaware 19711 ROBERT TERBORG Calvin College, Grand

More information

Learning new response sequences

Learning new response sequences Behavioural Processes, 32 (1994) 147-162 Q 1994 Elsevier Scienw B.V. All rights reserved 0376-6357/94/$07.00 BEPROC 00549 Learning new response sequences Alliston K. Reid Department of Psychology, Eastern

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

Operant Conditioning B.F. SKINNER

Operant Conditioning B.F. SKINNER Operant Conditioning B.F. SKINNER Reinforcement in Operant Conditioning Behavior Consequence Patronize Elmo s Diner It s all a matter of consequences. Rewarding Stimulus Presented Tendency to tell jokes

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information

PERIODIC RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTIGUITY: TEMPORAL CONTROL BUT NOT AS WE KNOW IT! MICHAEL KEENAN University of Ulster at Coleraine

PERIODIC RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTIGUITY: TEMPORAL CONTROL BUT NOT AS WE KNOW IT! MICHAEL KEENAN University of Ulster at Coleraine The Psychological Record, 1999, 49, 273-297 PERIODIC RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTIGUITY: TEMPORAL CONTROL BUT NOT AS WE KNOW IT! MICHAEL KEENAN University of Ulster at Coleraine Two experiments using rats

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY

CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY By ANDREW LAWRENCE SAMAHA A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A.

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December

More information

Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data

Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 2004, 36 (4), 661 669 Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data PAULO GUILHARDI and RUSSELL M.

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2005 April ; 31(2): 213 225. Timing in Choice Experiments Jeremie Jozefowiez and Daniel T. Cerutti

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Illinois Wesleyan University Digital Commons @ IWU Honors Projects Psychology 2000 Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Sara J. Estle '00 Illinois Wesleyan University Recommended Citation

More information

A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule*

A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2 (3),168-172. A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule* JOHN HARSH and PETRO BADA Bowling Green State University,

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

RESURGENCE OF INTEGRATED BEHAVIORAL UNITS

RESURGENCE OF INTEGRATED BEHAVIORAL UNITS JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2007, 87, 5 24 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) RESURGENCE OF INTEGRATED BEHAVIORAL UNITS GUSTAVO BACHÁ-MÉNDEZ 1,ALLISTON K. REID 2, AND ADELA MENDOZA-SOYLOVNA 1 1 FACULTAD

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

Babel Revisited Kennon A. Lattal

Babel Revisited Kennon A. Lattal The Behavior Analyst 1981, 4, 143-152 No. 2 (Fall) Describing Response-Event Relations: Babel Revisited Kennon A. Lattal West Virginia University and Alan D. Poling Western Michigan University The terms

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez The Psychological Record, 28, 58, 57 528 Some Effects of Magnitude of Reinforcement on Persistence of Responding Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman The University of Minnesota Angel Jimenez The University

More information

Reinforced variation and selection

Reinforced variation and selection Animal Learning & Behavior 1993, 21 (2), 83-91 Reinforced variation and selection ALLEN NEURINGER Reed College, Portland, Oregon Long-Evans rats were reinforced for generating variable sequences of four

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 84 (2010) 511 515 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Changes in stimulus control during guided skill

More information

Variability as an Operant?

Variability as an Operant? The Behavior Analyst 2012, 35, 243 248 No. 2 (Fall) Variability as an Operant? Per Holth Oslo and Akershus University College Correspondence concerning this commentary should be addressed to Per Holth,

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title Evocation of Behavioral Change by the Reinforcer is the Critical Event in both the Classical and Operant Procedures Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/15x1q931

More information

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form Supervisor: Date first assessed: Individual being Supervised: Certification being sought: Instructions: Please mark each item with either a 0,1,2 or 3 based on rating scale Rating Scale: 0 - cannot identify

More information

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE By ANTHONY L. DEFULIO A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE

More information

TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND MARC N.

TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND MARC N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2008, 90, 207 218 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules

Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules Animal Learning & Behavior 1979, 7 (2), 203-210 Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules NANCY K. INNIS University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research TWO-KEY CONCURRENT RESPONDING: CHOICE AND DELAYS OF REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS Presented to The Faculty of the Division of Graduate Studies and Research By George

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/ Research Commons at the University of Waikato Copyright Statement: The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 994 (New Zealand). The thesis

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 6 Appetitive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 6 Appetitive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 6 Appetitive Conditioning Midterm Results Animals Search and rescue dogs -- http://www.sardogsus.org/ Dog Tricks Appetitive Conditioning Appetitive something desirable

More information

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 009, 9, 367 377 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) WITHIN-SUBJECT REVERSIBILITY OF DISCRIMINATIVE FUNCTION IN THE COMPOSITE-STIMULUS CONTROL OF BEHAVIOR STANLEY J. WEISS,

More information

INDUCED POLYDIPSIA1 JOHN L. FALK UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN. a large number of these small meals. Under. CRF, inter-reinforcement time is short and

INDUCED POLYDIPSIA1 JOHN L. FALK UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN. a large number of these small meals. Under. CRF, inter-reinforcement time is short and JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 9, NUMBER I JANUARY, 1966 THE MOTIVATIONAL PROPERTIES OF SCHEDULE- INDUCED POLYDIPSIA1 JOHN L. FALK UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN Schedule-induced polydipsia

More information

Omission training compared with yoked controls and extinction in multiple-schedule discrimination learning*

Omission training compared with yoked controls and extinction in multiple-schedule discrimination learning* AnimalLearning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2 (4), 317-324 Omission training compared with yoked controls and extinction in multiple-schedule discrimination learning* CHARLES N. UHL and ANDREW L. HOMER University

More information

Schedules of Reinforcement 11/11/11

Schedules of Reinforcement 11/11/11 Schedules of Reinforcement 11/11/11 Reinforcement Schedules Intermittent Reinforcement: A type of reinforcement schedule by which some, but not all, correct responses are reinforced. Intermittent reinforcement

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

The Post-Reinforcement Pause and Terminal Rate In Fixed-Interval Schedules

The Post-Reinforcement Pause and Terminal Rate In Fixed-Interval Schedules Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-1971 The Post-Reinforcement Pause and Terminal Rate n Fixed-nterval Schedules Charles A. Lund Utah State

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 42, No. 4, 401 414 2329-8456/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000112 Contextual

More information

CORRELATED DELAY OF REINFORCEMENT 1

CORRELATED DELAY OF REINFORCEMENT 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1961, Vol. 54, No. 2, 196-203 CRRELATED DELAY F REINFRCEMENT 1 It is well established that variations in an operant response can be differentiated by

More information

The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance

The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1972 The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance Deborah Ann Cory Western

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Volume 6, Number 2 Submitted: September 8, 2000 Resubmitted: January 22, 2001 Accepted: January 23, 2001 Publication date: January 26, 2001 THE EFFICACY OF REINFORCEMENT

More information

edited by Derek P. Hendry'

edited by Derek P. Hendry' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR INFORMATION ON CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT A review of Conditioned Reinforcement, edited by Derek P. Hendry' LEWIS R. GOLLUB2 UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND 1970,

More information

TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: A REVIEW AND ANALYSIS TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG

TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: A REVIEW AND ANALYSIS TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 91, 257 286 NUMBER 2(MARCH) TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: A REVIEW AND ANALYSIS TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA Token reinforcement procedures

More information

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee The Psychological Record, 1997,47,145-166 THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served

More information

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-009 The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Wesley Paul Thomas Utah

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change*

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2 (4), 262-266 Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* ALAN RUSSELLt and PETER H. GLOW University ofadelaide,

More information

Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness

Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2013 Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness Kerin A. Weingarten University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Follow this

More information

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/85/$00.75 1985, VOl. 11, No. 4, 548-564 Predictive Accuracy and the

More information

ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS. MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison

ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS. MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 603-620 ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison SHELLY TERRY-CAIN

More information

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF OTHER BEHAVIOR AND RESPONSE SUPPRESSION:

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF OTHER BEHAVIOR AND RESPONSE SUPPRESSION: psychological Record, 1993, 43, 271-288 DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF OTHER BEHAVIOR AND RESPONSE SUPPRESSION: THE EFFECTS OF THE RESPONSE-REINFORCEMENT INTERVAL THOMAS S. RIEG, NELSON F. SMITH, and STUART

More information

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 348 370 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Chris Hope and Angelo Santi Wilfrid

More information

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965, Vol. 59, No. 1, 13-17 AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 GORDON BOWER, RONALD STARR, AND LEAH LAZAROVITZ Stanford

More information

What is Learned? Lecture 9

What is Learned? Lecture 9 What is Learned? Lecture 9 1 Classical and Instrumental Conditioning Compared Classical Reinforcement Not Contingent on Behavior Behavior Elicited by US Involuntary Response (Reflex) Few Conditionable

More information

Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery

Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 491 499 Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery GINESA LÓPEZ-CRESPO and MIGUEL RODRÍGUEZ Universidad de Almería, Almería,

More information

Teaching Classical and Operant Conditioning in a Laboratory-Based Course: Eight Effective Experiments

Teaching Classical and Operant Conditioning in a Laboratory-Based Course: Eight Effective Experiments Teaching Classical and Operant Conditioning in a Laboratory-Based Course: Eight Effective Experiments Eric S. Murphy & Robert J. Madigan University of Alaska Anchorage We teach a junior-level experimental

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

RESURGENCE OF LEVER PRESSING DURING A DRO CHALLENGE. Jennifer L. Hudnall, B.A.

RESURGENCE OF LEVER PRESSING DURING A DRO CHALLENGE. Jennifer L. Hudnall, B.A. RESURGENCE OF LEVER PRESSING DURING A DRO CHALLENGE By Jennifer L. Hudnall, B.A. Submitted to the graduate degree program Applied Behavioral Science and the Graduate Faculty of the University of Kansas

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

ing the fixed-interval schedule-were observed during the interval of delay. Similarly, Ferster

ing the fixed-interval schedule-were observed during the interval of delay. Similarly, Ferster JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAIOR 1969, 12, 375-383 NUMBER 3 (MAY) DELA YED REINFORCEMENT ERSUS REINFORCEMENT AFTER A FIXED INTERAL' ALLEN J. NEURINGER FOUNDATION FOR RESEARCH ON THE NEROUS

More information

Classical Conditioning. Learning. Classical conditioning terms. Classical Conditioning Procedure. Procedure, cont. Important concepts

Classical Conditioning. Learning. Classical conditioning terms. Classical Conditioning Procedure. Procedure, cont. Important concepts Learning Classical Conditioning Pavlov study of digestion dogs salivate before getting food learning as signal detection: emphasis on what happens before a given behavior Classical conditioning terms Stimulus:

More information