Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm"

Transcription

1 Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm Akira Miyake a, *, Michael J. Emerson a, Francisca Padilla b, Jeung-chan Ahn a a Department of Psychology, University of Colorado at Boulder, 345 UCB, Boulder, CO , USA b Department of Psychology, University of Granada, Campus de Cartuja s/n 18071, Spain Abstract Articulatory suppression has been shown to increase switch costs in the list paradigm (e.g., [J. Exp. Psychol.: General 130 (2001) 641, J Memory Language 48 (2003) 148]). The present dual-task study examined whether this effect generalizes to the random task cuing paradigm. Participants performed color or shape judgments according to explicit word cues (COLOR or SHAPE) or less transparent letter cues (C for the color task and S for the shape task). In the word cue condition, the switch cost was equivalent for the articulatory suppression and the control (no dual-task) conditions, but, in the letter cue condition, the switch cost was significantly greater for the articulatory suppression condition than for the control condition. These results suggest that inner speech may be recruited as a tool for retrieving and activating the relevant task goal when the task cue is not transparent and hence imposes nonnegligible retrieval demand. Ó 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. PsycINFO classification: 2340; 2346; 2720 Keywords: Inner speech; Articulatory suppression; Task switching; Goal retrieval; Verbal control * Corresponding author. Tel.: address: miyake@psych.colorado.edu (A. Miyake) /$ - see front matter Ó 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi: /j.actpsy

2 124 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) Introduction Language plays an important role in everyday living. Although not the only medium, the central role of language in human communication is incontrovertible. The utility of language is not restricted to interpersonal communication, however. It can also shape and support intrapersonal thinking processes in various ways (Carruthers, 2002; Gentner & Goldin-Meadow, 2003; Sokolov, 1972; Vygotsky, 1934/1986). One such intrapersonal cognitive function attributed to language is the regulation of thought and action (Barkley, 1997; Luria, 1961). In particular, at least among adults, inner (or subvocal) speech is considered to play some role in regulating one s behavior (Carlson, 1997; Sokolov, 1972; see also Gruber & Goschke, 2004). In this sense, inner speech (and, more generally, language) can be viewed as a contributing factor to efficient executive control. The current study examined the role of inner speech in one aspect of executive control processes, mental set shifting, and tested the hypothesis that inner speech supports task switching performance by serving as a goal retrieval aid. Although the regulatory role of inner speech in executive control has not been studied extensively from modern cognitive psychological perspectives, it seems clear that, at least under some circumstances, adults spontaneously recruit inner speech to facilitate their set shifting performance. For example, Dunbar and Sussman (1995) and, more recently, Cinan and Tan or (2002) showed that concurrent articulation of simple irrelevant words or phrases (i.e., articulatory suppression) significantly impaired healthy adults performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, a wellknown neuropsychological test of executive functioning related to set shifting ability (Miyake et al., 2000). In particular, the type of errors that increased under concurrent articulatory suppression was perseveration errors (i.e., failure to shift to a new task-relevant category), which patients with frontal lobe damage are known to predominantly make on this test. The negative impact of articulatory suppression on set shifting has also been demonstrated in a few recent studies that required participants to switch back and forth between two mental operations (Baddeley, Chincotta, & Adlam, 2001; Emerson & Miyake, 2003; Goschke, 2000; Saeki & Saito, in press; see also Kray, Eber, & Lindenberger, 2004). These studies used either the list paradigm, originally developed by Jersild (1927), or its variant, the precued task sequence paradigm, to examine the role of inner speech in task switching performance. In these paradigms, tasks are performed either in pure-task blocks (i.e., participants perform only one task) or in alternating-task blocks (i.e., participants alternate between two different tasks). In Baddeley et al. (2001) study, for example, participants performed various secondary tasks as they completed addition and subtraction problems in pure-task lists (i.e., addition only or subtraction only) or alternating lists (i.e., alternated between addition and subtraction). The key finding of the study was that the switch cost, defined as the difference between the completion time for the alternatingtask list and the average completion time for the pure-task lists, increased considerably under articulatory suppression (e.g., reciting the days of the week or saying the repeatedly). In contrast, other secondary tasks without a clear verbal compo-

3 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) nent, such as spatial tapping (tapping four pegs of a square in clockwise direction) and a marble transfer task (moving marbles one at a time from one container to another), had a much smaller or negligible effect on switch costs. Similar dissociations have been reported in other studies using the list paradigm, regardless of whether the tasks to be switched between involved mental arithmetic or judgments related to numbers and letters (Emerson & Miyake, 2003; Saeki & Saito, in press). The negative effect of articulatory suppression on switch costs has also been shown with the precued task sequence paradigm (Goschke, 2000, Experiment 2). In this study, participants were presented with a colored letter (e.g., a green B) on each trial and judged either the color (red or green) or the identity (A or B) of the stimulus letter. Each experimental block consisted of trial pairs and, at the beginning of each block, participants were instructed to perform the same task on both stimuli of each trial pair (i.e., color only or letter only) or to switch from one task to the other within the pair (i.e., perform the letter task on the first item and the color task on the second item or vice versa). During the 1500 ms interval provided between the manual response to the first stimulus and the presentation of the second stimulus, half of the participants were instructed to say the task name ( color or letter ) aloud once, whereas the other half were instructed to say an irrelevant word ( Monday or Tuesday ) once instead. The results indicated that saying an irrelevant word caused a significant increase in switch cost relative to saying the task name. Moreover, the switch cost for the irrelevant word condition was comparable to the cost observed in a different experiment (Goschke, 2000, Experiment 1) when participants had only a short (14 ms) preparatory interval, whereas the switch cost for the task name condition was virtually identical to the cost observed when participants had a long (1500 ms) preparatory interval without any verbalization requirement. These results suggest that irrelevant articulation is particularly harmful if it is required precisely when the task goal information for the forthcoming trial must be retrieved (or reactivated). The evidence reviewed above suggests that inner speech may be involved in the retrieval or reactivation of the relevant task goal from long-term memory and may serve as an efficient internal self-cuing device that signals which task to perform next (Baddeley et al., 2001; Emerson & Miyake, 2003). Consistent with this interpretation, the presence of explicit external cues, such as the + and ) signs for addition and subtraction operations, considerably reduced the magnitude of the negative impact of articulatory suppression on switch costs (Baddeley et al., 2001; Emerson & Miyake, 2003; Saeki & Saito, in press). Similarly, underlining either the letter or the number in each letter number pair (e.g., B6) and thereby making obvious which stimulus to process also greatly reduced the magnitude of the articulatory suppression effect on switch costs (Saeki & Saito, in press). These manipulations are likely to have substantially reduced or even eliminated the need for internally retrieving the relevant task goal on each trial, hence minimizing the negative effect of articulatory suppression on switch costs. Further pointing to the role of inner speech in goal retrieval, manipulations that do not reduce the need

4 126 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) for goal retrieval or internal self-cuing (e.g., adding or subtracting 1 vs. 3 vs. 6) did not have any influence on the magnitude of the articulatory suppression effect on switch costs, even though they strongly influenced the overall list completion times (Emerson & Miyake, 2003). Thus, a fair amount of existing evidence indicates that, as far as the list and precued task sequence paradigms are concerned, inner speech plays an important supporting role in task switching performance. A question remains, however, as to whether the results based on these paradigms will be obtained in other prevalently used paradigms in the task switching literature, such as the random task cuing paradigm (e.g., H ubner, Futterer, & Steinhauser, 2001; Mayr, 2001; Mayr & Keele, 2000; Meiran, 1996; Meiran, Chorev, & Sapir, 2000). In the random task cuing paradigm, a task cue indicating which task to perform is presented on each trial, typically before the target stimulus is displayed on the computer screen. As the name indicates, a task cue is chosen randomly for each trial and, thus, whether the forthcoming trial requires a task switch or not is unpredictable. Note that, because a task cue is presented on each trial, the random task cuing paradigm does not require participants to keep track of the task sequence to perform the correct task on a given trial, unlike the list and precued task sequence paradigms. Given these unique characteristics of the random task cuing paradigm, recruiting inner speech mechanisms for self-cuing purposes may no longer be needed and, if so, the effect of articulatory suppression on switch costs might be greatly reduced or even completely absent in the random task cuing paradigm. This would be particularly true if the role of inner speech in task switching performance in the list and precued task sequence paradigms is restricted to dealing with the sequencing requirement and has nothing to do with the process of goal retrieval and activation. The main purpose of the present study was to determine whether and to what extent inner speech serves as a goal retrieval aid by focusing on the random task cuing paradigm and minimizing the need for keeping track of the task sequence. The experiment reported below used two component tasks that were nonverbal in nature: making a color judgment (red or green) or a shape judgment (circle or triangle). Participants completed blocks of trials in which these tasks were presented in a random sequence, and a cue specifying which task to be performed was presented prior to the presentation of a bivalent stimulus (e.g., a triangle superimposed on top of a green color patch) on each trial. Two types of cues were used in the experiment to evaluate the involvement of inner speech in the process of goal retrieval and activation. One type of cue was a word that directly indicated the task name (i.e., COLOR or SHAPE), and the other type of cue was a letter (i.e., C for the color classification task, S for the shape classification task). The word cue type was chosen because word cues indicating the task name have been used in several previous studies of task switching (e.g., Mayr & Keele, 2000; Mayr & Liebscher, 2001) and because explicit, familiar word cues such as COLOR and SHAPE were considered likely to quickly activate a representation of the appropriate task goal. Moreover, according to Gathercole and Baddeley (1993), the role of inner speech (or, more specifically, the phonolog-

5 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) ical loop subsystem of working memory) in the reading of highly familiar words is essentially none, except when complex judgments about phonological structure [is] required (p. 232). Thus, the activation of appropriate task representations was expected to take place automatically even under concurrent articulatory suppression. The letter cue type was chosen because it had some degree of association with the task name (i.e., the first letter of the task name) and hence was relatively easy to memorize, but was uncommon enough that it was unlikely to activate the task representations as efficiently as word cues would. Although this cue type manipulation may seem subtle, we chose letter cues because we wanted to avoid using intentionally misleading cues (e.g., the word COLOR for the shape task and the word SHAPE for the color task) or completely arbitrary cues that might be hard to remember (e.g., X for the color task, Y for the shape task). For the main part of the experiment, participants performed the color and shape tasks with both cue types in five secondary task conditions: concurrent foot tapping, concurrent articulatory suppression, cue-triggered verbalization of the task name (i.e., the say task name condition used by Goschke, 2000), and two conditions without any secondary task requirement (control-long and control-short). In four of the five conditions (foot tapping, articulatory suppression, say task name, and control-long), the preparation interval between the cue and the stimulus was 1200 ms. It has been well established that long preparation intervals allow participants to mentally prepare for the forthcoming task, which typically results in reduction of the switch cost (e.g., Meiran, 1996, Meiran et al., 2000; Rogers & Monsell, 1995). In the fifth condition (control-short), the preparation interval was only 200 ms. This condition was included for comparison to the control-long condition to ensure that participants did indeed try to mentally prepare in advance of the stimulus with a long preparation time. For each cue type, we compared the magnitude of the switch costs in the five secondary task conditions (control-short, control-long, say task name, foot tapping, and articulatory suppression). For the word cues, the switch cost in the control-short condition should be significantly larger than the costs in the four conditions with a long preparation time. However, because word cues provide the task name that participants would otherwise need to retrieve on their own, the switch costs in the control-long, say task name, foot tapping, and articulatory suppression conditions should not differ significantly from each other. For the letter cues, the control-short condition should again have a larger cost than most of the other four secondary task conditions. In contrast to the word cues, however, the letter cues should still impose some retrieval demand because of the relatively weak association between the letters (C or S) and the task name. For this reason, the switch costs under concurrent articulatory suppression should be significantly larger than the costs in the control-long, say task name, and foot tapping conditions. In fact, if articulatory suppression hinders or considerably slows down the retrieval of the task goal when letter cues are presented, the switch cost in this condition may be as large as that in the controlshort condition.

6 128 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) Method 2.1. Participants The participants were 48 undergraduate students from the University of Colorado at Boulder who received monetary compensation or partial course credit for taking part in the experiment Design, materials, and procedure The experiment consisted of two parts administered in a single session that lasted approximately 90 min. In the first part, participants performed several blocks of pure-task trials in which only one task (either color or shape judgment) was performed in a given block of trials. In the second part, participants performed several blocks of mixed-task trials in which each trial was preceded by a cue indicating which task to perform. For both parts, participants first went through training or practice trials to familiarize themselves with the task requirements. They then performed target trials, with or without secondary tasks. The tasks for both parts were programmed with the PsyScope software (Cohen, MacWhinney, Flatt, & Provost, 1993) and were run on Power Macintosh 7200 computers. Button-press responses were recorded from stimulus onset to the nearest millisecond using a button box with an internal timer Pure-task trials Before starting the target trials, participants completed a total of 150 training trials, administered in five blocks of 30 trials each, to master the response-mapping rules. The responses for both color and shape tasks were mapped onto two buttons in a bivalent manner (e.g., the left button for red and circle and the right button for green and triangle ). Throughout this experiment (including both pure-task and mixed-task trials), the response-mapping rules were kept constant for each participant, although we counterbalanced the response mappings across participants by randomly assigning 12 participants to each of the four counterbalancing conditions. In each training trial, a stimulus was presented without any cue in the middle of the computer screen until a response was made. If the response was correct, the next trial commenced after a 600 ms intertrial interval (ITI). If it was incorrect, the computer beeped and an error message (i.e., incorrect ) was displayed for 1000 ms followed by an ITI of 600 ms. In the first three blocks of the training trials, the stimulus was univalent and was applicable to only one task. In the first block, a black line-drawing of either a circle or square shape was presented on each trial, whereas in the second block, a squareshaped color patch (red or green) was presented. In the third block, either a shape or a color patch was presented in a random sequence (e.g., a black line drawing of a square on one trial, followed by a red color patch on the next). The circle was 27 mm in diameter, the triangle was 25 mm wide and 28 mm tall, and the square-shaped color patch was 36 mm on each side for both colors (the viewing distance was

7 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) approximately 60 cm). In the last two blocks of the training session, the stimulus was bivalent and consisted of a shape (either circle or triangle) superimposed on a square color patch (either red or green). Participants performed the shape task throughout the fourth block and the color task for the last, fifth block. The order of administering the five training blocks was fixed for all participants. After completing these response-mapping training trials, participants performed six blocks of target pure-task trials with 65 trials in each block. As in the last two blocks of training trials, the stimulus for each target trial was bivalent. The trial setup was identical to the training trials (including the absence of any task cue) except that participants no longer received an error message for incorrect responses but were informed of their accuracy (percentage correct) at the end of each block. The six blocks for the target pure-task trials were created by crossing the task type (color or shape) with the secondary task requirement (control, foot tapping, or articulatory suppression). The six blocks were organized in two sets of three so that participants performed one task in a fixed order of all three secondary task conditions and then repeated the procedure for the other task. For example, a participant might perform the color task with concurrent articulatory suppression in the first block, without any secondary task in the second block, and with concurrent foot tapping in the third block, and then repeat this secondary task order for the shape task in the remaining three blocks. The order of the three secondary task conditions and the order of the two task types were counterbalanced across participants. During pilot testing, it became clear that, despite explicit instructions, some participants failed to switch tasks between the third and fourth blocks and thus showed almost chance-level performance in the fourth block. To minimize the occurrence of this perseveration problem, we presented a visual reminder for 3 s at the beginning of each block that clearly indicated which task to perform for that block (e.g., Respond to Shape ). The secondary task procedure was similar to that used by Emerson and Miyake (2003). When the participants first encountered the articulatory suppression and foot tapping conditions, the experimenter described the relevant secondary task and then demonstrated how to perform it. A metronome was set to beat at the rate of 750 ms per beat for both secondary tasks. Participants were instructed to tap a foot once per beat in the foot tapping condition. They were allowed to choose whichever foot they preferred to use for tapping, but were asked not to switch in the middle of a block. In the articulatory suppression condition, participants were required to say either Tuesday or Thursday once per beat. Half of the participants were randomly assigned to say Tuesday for the color-task-only block and Thursday for the shape-task-only block, and the other half did the opposite. Participants first practiced the secondary task for 30 s to make sure that they could perform the task correctly. After this brief practice, the participant performed the appropriate secondary task concurrently with the color or shape task. The experimenter started the trial block after participants began the relevant secondary task. Performance on the foot tapping and articulatory suppression tasks was closely monitored by the experimenter. There was no secondary task requirement in the control condition, but the metronome was turned on to equate the level of external noise across conditions.

8 130 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) Mixed-task trials After completing the pure-task trials and taking a short break, participants performed blocks of mixed-task trials. The sequence of events that took place during the mixed-task trials is schematically illustrated in Fig. 1. To familiarize themselves with the random task cuing procedure, participants first practiced mixed-task trials without any secondary task requirements for two blocks of 50 trials each. Each practice trial began with the onset of a cue, presented 9 mm above the stimulus and approximately 7 mm in height. The cue appeared 1200 ms before the stimulus and indicated which task was to be performed. The cue was a word printed in capital letters (COLOR or SHAPE) in one block of practice trials and was a capital letter (C for the color task and S for the shape task) in the other block of practice trials, with the order of these two blocks randomized for each participant. The stimulus was the same as in the target pure-task trials and consisted of a shape (circle or triangle) superimposed on a square-shaped color patch (red or green) presented in the middle of the screen. The cue and the stimulus then remained onscreen until a response was made. During this practice phase, immediate feedback was given for incorrect responses using the same procedure as in the pure-task trials. Word Cue Letter Cue SHAPE Cue: 200 ms or 1200 ms C SHAPE Stimulus: Until response C Time Intertrial Interval: 600 ms COLOR Cue: 200 ms or 1200 ms S Fig. 1. A schematic illustration of the sequence of events that happened in the word cue and letter cue conditions for the mixed-trial blocks. A cue was presented for either 200 ms (for the control-short condition) or1200 ms (for the control-long, say task name, foot tapping, and articulatory suppression conditions), indicating which of the two tasks should be performed (COLOR or C for color judgment and SHAPE or S for shape judgment). A bivalent stimulus (a color patch superimposed on top of a shape) appeared and remained on the display until a manual response was made. The intertrial interval was 600 ms.

9 After practicing the mixed-task trials with both cue types, participants performed 10 blocks of 110 target mixed-task trials each. Because performing concurrent articulatory suppression or foot tapping for an extended period of time may be tiring, each block was split into two runs of 55 trials apiece, with a brief break provided between runs. The trial setup for the target mixed-task trials was identical to the practice trials (see Fig. 1) except that feedback was no longer provided for incorrect responses. The 10 target blocks were created by crossing the two cue types (word cue or letter cue) with five secondary task conditions (control-short, control-long, articulatory suppression, foot tapping, and say task name). The order of the 10 target blocks was randomized for each participant, with the constraint that the same cue type not be repeated more than two blocks in a row. The control-short and control-long conditions differed only in terms of cue-to-stimulus interval (CSI). The CSI was 200 ms for the control-short condition and 1200 ms for the control-long condition. The CSI in the articulatory suppression, foot tapping, and say task name conditions was always 1200 ms. The secondary task requirements for the foot tapping and articulatory suppression conditions were the same as those in the pure-task trials. In the foot tapping condition, participants tapped a foot once per metronome beat (i.e., every 750 ms). In the articulatory suppression condition, participants said Tuesday throughout one block (e.g., the letter-cue block) and Thursday throughout the other block (e.g., the word-cue block), also at the same metronome pace. The days assigned to the word and letter cue blocks were counterbalanced across participants. As in the pure-task trials, participants were instructed to begin the relevant secondary task before the experimenter started the block, and the experimenter closely monitored performance on the secondary tasks. Having already practiced these secondary tasks, however, participants were not asked to practice the secondary tasks individually again. A new secondary task condition added for the mixed-task trials was the say task name condition, modeled after the Goschke (2000) study (Experiment 2). In this condition, participants were instructed to say the task name aloud ( color for the cues C or COLOR and shape for the cues S or SHAPE) immediately after the presentation of the task cue on each trial (i.e., during the CSI). As was the case with the first part of the experiment with pure-task trials, the metronome was set at the rate of 750 ms per beat for all target blocks, including the control and say task name conditions, to equate the noise level Preliminary data analysis A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) The first three trials in each run of the target pure-task trials and the target mixedtask trials were considered as warm-up trials and were excluded from analysis, leaving 62 maximum analyzable pure-task target trials per block and 104 maximum analyzable mixed-task target trials per block. The analyses reported below for the pure-task trials are based on reaction times (RTs) for correct trials, and the analyses for the mixed-task trials are based on RTs for correct trials in which the response to the previous trial was also correct. To prevent a small number of deviant data points

10 132 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) from exerting undue influence on the mean RTs, we first excluded extreme RTs (RT < 100 ms or RT > 5000 ms) from analysis for both the pure-task trials and the mixed-task trials (0.15% and 0.03% of the total observed excluded, respectively) and then applied a logarithmic transformation to the remaining raw RTs. For ease of interpretation, the numbers mentioned throughout Section 3 and presented in the figures are backtransformed. For the RT data, we also conducted statistical analyses with trimmed RTs (specifically, any RTs that were longer than 3 standard deviations above a participant s mean RT in that condition were replaced with the value of the mean plus 3 standard deviations). The results with this trimmed RT method (without any transformation) were qualitatively the same as those based on log RTs. Thus, we only present the results from the log-transformed data. Regarding the error data, the raw proportions of errors tended to show a positive skew indicative of a floor effect. Thus, we subjected the error data to an arcsine transformation to achieve a satisfactory level of normality before performing ANOVAs. For ease of interpretation, the descriptive statistics presented below are untransformed. For all of the statistical analyses presented in this article, we used an alpha level of The effect size measure reported for the ANOVA results is partial eta squared ðg 2 Þ. 3. Results 3.1. Pure-task trials RT data The pure-task trial RTs were analyzed in a 2 3 repeated-measures ANOVA, with factors of primary task (color or shape judgment) and secondary task (control, articulatory suppression, or foot tapping). The RTs in the color (M ¼ 391 ms) and shape (M ¼ 406 ms) tasks were similar, but the 15-ms difference between these two primary tasks was reliable F ð1; 47Þ ¼7:14, MSE ¼ 0:003, p ¼ 0:010, g 2 ¼ 0:132. There was also a marginally significant interaction between the primary task and the secondary task, F ð2; 94Þ ¼2:84, MSE ¼ 0:002, p ¼ 0:064, g 2 ¼ 0:057, such that a small RT difference between the color and shape tasks was slightly magnified by the dual-task demands. The main focus for the pure-task trials, however, was on the effect of the secondary task requirement. This main effect, illustrated in Fig. 2, was statistically significant (M ¼ 354, 405, and 440 ms for the control, articulatory suppression, and foot tapping conditions, respectively), F ð2; 94Þ ¼ 48:58, MSE ¼ 0:005, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:508. A planned comparison of the control condition to the average of the foot tapping and articulatory suppression conditions indicated that there was a significant overall dual-task effect, F ð1; 47Þ ¼93:83, MSE ¼ 0:002, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:666, with longer RTs in the dual-task conditions (M ¼ 422 ms) than in the control condition (M ¼ 354 ms).

11 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) Reaction Time (ms) Control Foot Tapping Articulatory Suppression Fig. 2. The mean reaction times (back-transformed from log-based means) for the color and shape puretask trials. Error bars indicate standard errors (also back-transformed). The reaction times in the two dual-task conditions were longer than those in the control condition, and concurrent foot tapping led to longer reaction times than concurrent articulatory suppression. Interesting to note, another planned comparison showed that the RTs were significantly longer with concurrent foot tapping (M ¼ 440 ms) than with concurrent articulatory suppression (M ¼ 405 ms), F ð1; 47Þ ¼12:19, MSE ¼ 0:003, p ¼ 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:206. This result suggests that, although both secondary tasks significantly lengthened pure-task trial RTs, it was more difficult to combine the color and shape judgment tasks with foot tapping (a 24% increase in RT) than with articulatory suppression (a 14% increase). Thus, any negative effect of articulatory suppression on switch costs observed for the mixed-task trials reported below cannot be interpreted in terms of a greater attentional demand or difficulty associated with articulatory suppression than with foot tapping Error data Overall, the error rates for the pure-task trials were low, and there were no statistically significant effects. The main effect of primary task, F ð1; 47Þ ¼0:07, MSE ¼ 0:007, p ¼ 0:786, g 2 ¼ 0:002, was absent, with essentially equivalent proportions of errors for the color (M ¼ 0:048, SE ¼ 0:003) and shape (M ¼ 0:049, 1 Given that the primary task and the secondary task both required lateralized motor responses in the foot tapping condition, some sort of motor-level interference may have contributed to the greater dualtask effect found for the foot tapping condition. For example, pressing a left button with the left index finger might be somewhat difficult if the tapping is done with the left foot and the timing of the manual response does not synchronize with that of the tapping response. In this regard, it is possible that the nature of dual-task demands is somewhat different for concurrent foot tapping and concurrent articulatory suppression. However, we should note that both dual-task conditions required participants to keep up with the pace of metronome beats (i.e., tapping a foot once or saying an irrelevant word like Tuesday once every 750 ms) for the pure-task trials as well as for the mixed-task trials. Thus, any RT differences between the two conditions found in this study cannot be explained purely in terms of differences in the temporal characteristics of the two secondary task requirements.

12 134 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) SE ¼ 0:004) tasks. There were also no significant differences among the control (M ¼ 0:044, SE ¼ 0:004), foot tapping (M ¼ 0:052, SE ¼ 0:004), and articulatory suppression (M ¼ 0:050, SE ¼ 0:004) conditions, F ð2; 94Þ ¼ 1:79, MSE ¼ 0:008, p ¼ 0:172, g 2 ¼ 0:037. The interaction between the two factors was also not significant, F ð2; 94Þ ¼1:47, MSE ¼ 0:005, p ¼ 0:234, g 2 ¼ 0: Mixed-task trials RT data According to the predictions outlined earlier, the RT difference between the repeat and switch trial types (i.e., the switch cost) in the five secondary task conditions should depend on the cue type. When word cues were available, the explicit nature of the cues should minimize retrieval demands and hence should yield small switch costs even under concurrent articulatory suppression. In contrast, when letter cues were presented, the mapping between the cue and the task is less transparent and may still impose some degree of retrieval demand. If so, articulatory suppression should have a greater negative impact on switch costs compared to the control condition. To test these predictions, we first conducted separate analyses for the word cue and the letter cue conditions. The RT data are graphically presented in Fig. 3A (for the word cue condition) and Fig. 3B (for the letter cue condition). For the word cue condition, an analysis of the interaction between secondary task and trial type (i.e., repeat vs. switch) indicated that there were significant differences in switch costs among the five secondary task conditions, F ð4; 188Þ ¼12:16, MSE ¼ 0:001, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:206. As illustrated in Fig. 3A, however, switch costs were uniformly low except in the control-short condition. A planned comparison of the switch costs in the control-short (M ¼ 110 ms) and control-long (M ¼ 42 ms) conditions indicated that the costs were significantly reduced with a long preparation time, F ð1; 47Þ ¼31:25, MSE ¼ 0:001, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:399, replicating the wellknown preparation effect (e.g., Meiran, 1996; Rogers & Monsell, 1995). When the control-short condition was excluded and the analysis focused on only the four secondary task conditions in which the CSI was 1200 ms (control-long, say task name, foot tapping, and articulatory suppression), the secondary task by trial type interaction was no longer significant, F ð3; 141Þ ¼0:86, MSE ¼ 0:001, p ¼ 0:462, g 2 ¼ 0:018, suggesting that the switch costs across these four conditions were essentially equivalent. Thus, as predicted, the word cues appear to have automatically activated a representation of the task goal, thereby eliminating any negative effect of articulatory suppression on switch costs. The RT data for the letter cue condition are displayed in Fig. 3B. As with the word cues, the RT differences between the task-repeat and task-switch trials (i.e., switch costs) significantly varied across the five secondary task conditions, F ð4; 188Þ ¼18:07, MSE ¼ 0:001, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:278. Again, long preparation times led to a significant reduction in switch cost (M ¼ 137 ms vs. 43 ms for the control-short and control-long conditions, respectively), F ð1; 47Þ ¼ 31:97, MSE ¼ 0:001, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:405. In contrast to the results of the word cue condition, however, when the control-short condition was excluded from analysis, a significant secondary

13 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) (A) Reaction Time (ms) Word Cues Repeat Switch Control- Short Control- Long Say Task Name Foot Tapping Articulatory Suppression (B) 800 Reaction Time (ms) Letter Cues Control- Short Control- Long Say Task Name Foot Tapping Secondary Task Condition Articulatory Suppression Fig. 3. The mean reaction times (back-transformed from log-based means) for the word cues (A) and letter cues (B) for each secondary task condition in the mixed-task trials. Error bars indicate standard errors (also back-transformed) and switch cost values are printed on top of the bars for each secondary task condition. With word cues, the switch cost was larger in the control-short condition than in the articulatory suppression condition. With letter cues, the switch cost was larger in the control-short condition and in the articulatory suppression condition, and these two costs were not different from each other. task by trial type interaction was present, F ð3; 141Þ ¼ 10:03, MSE ¼ 0:001, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:176, suggesting there were still some differences in the switch cost magnitudes among the four conditions with a long preparation time. As Fig. 3B indicates, the only condition that showed a markedly different magnitude of switch cost was the articulatory suppression condition. In fact, the switch cost in the articulatory suppression condition (M ¼ 116 ms) was not significantly different from the cost in the control-short condition (M ¼ 137 ms), F ð1; 47Þ ¼2:17, MSE ¼ 0:001, p ¼ 0:148, g 2 ¼ 0:036, suggesting that, when letter cues were presented, articulatory suppression interfered with task name retrieval and thereby made advance preparation for the upcoming task as difficult as when participants had only a short time to prepare. As this comparison between the word cue and letter cue conditions indicates, the effects of articulatory suppression were dependent on the cue type, as predicted. A significant three-way interaction between trial type (repeat vs. switch), secondary

14 136 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) task, and cue type (letter vs. word) further confirms that the data patterns for the word cue condition and for the letter cue condition were different, F ð4; 188Þ ¼4:52, MSE ¼ 0:001, p ¼ 0:002, g 2 ¼ 0:088. Moreover, when the articulatory suppression condition was excluded from analysis, the same three-way interaction was no longer significant, F ð3; 141Þ ¼1:90, MSE ¼ 0:001, p ¼ 0:132, g 2 ¼ 0:039. These results strongly suggest that the significant three-way interaction critically depended on the data from the articulatory suppression condition and that the effect of cue type was much stronger for the switch costs in the articulatory suppression condition than in the other four conditions Error data Table 1 presents a summary of error data for the mixed-task trials. In the word cue condition, more errors were made on task switch trials than on task repeat trials, F ð1; 47Þ ¼180:79, MSE ¼ 0:007, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:794. Error rates also differed among the five secondary task conditions, F ð4; 188Þ ¼ 6:78, MSE ¼ 0:008, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:126, primarily because of the relatively low error rate in the control-long condition. The interaction between trial type and secondary task condition was not significant, F ð4; 188Þ ¼1:37, MSE ¼ 0:007, p ¼ 0:247, g 2 ¼ 0:028, indicating that the error switch costs were equivalent among the five secondary task conditions. The results for the letter cues were identical. There was a significant difference between the repeat and switch trial types, F ð1; 47Þ ¼114:30, MSE ¼ 0:008, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:709. Error rates were also reliably different among the secondary task conditions, F ð4; 188Þ ¼5:59, MSE ¼ 0:007, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:106. There were no significant differences in error switch costs across the five secondary task conditions, F ð4; 188Þ ¼0:34, MSE ¼ 0:006, p ¼ 0:849, g 2 ¼ 0:008. Given this similarity of the error data patterns for the two cue types, it is not surprising that, when the data for the two conditions were combined and the cue type Table 1 Summary of descriptive statistics (proportion of errors) for the mixed-task errors and error switch costs Condition Repeat Switch Switch cost M SE M SE M Word cues Control-short Control-long Say task name Foot tapping Articulatory suppression Letter cues Control-short Control-long Say task name Foot tapping Articulatory suppression

15 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) was treated as an additional factor, the three-way interaction was not significant, F ð4; 188Þ ¼0:46, MSE ¼ 0:006, p ¼ 0:767, g 2 ¼ 0:010. In this respect, the error data differ from the RT data, but the finding that the error switch costs were uniform across different secondary task conditions for both cue types rules out the possibility that the critical three-way interaction observed for the RT data was an artifact of a speed accuracy tradeoff. 4. Discussion The current study examined the role of inner speech in task switching performance using the random task cuing paradigm. The most important finding was the different patterns of results obtained for the word cue and letter cue conditions. Specifically, when participants were presented with letter cues (C or S), articulatory suppression caused an increase in switch cost compared to the other secondary task conditions with a long preparation time (CSI ¼ 1200 ms). In contrast, when the cues explicitly indicated the task names (COLOR or SHAPE) that participants would otherwise have had to generate or retrieve by themselves, concurrent articulatory suppression did not have any negative effect on switch costs. Given that the two cue types differed in the extent to which they automatically activate the task goal information, these results support the hypothesis that inner speech serves as a convenient and reliable medium for retrieving and activating the task goal for the forthcoming operation (Emerson & Miyake, 2003), at least among young adults (Kray et al., 2004). An additional finding from the control-short condition seems consistent with this interpretation. As Fig. 3 indicates, the switch cost in the control-short condition was 110 ms in the word cue condition and 137 ms in the letter cue condition. This 27 ms difference, tested in terms of the cue type by trial type interaction, was marginally significant, F ð1; 47Þ ¼2:89, MSE ¼ 0:001, p ¼ 0:096, g 2 ¼ 0:056. This somewhat smaller switch cost found for the word cue condition when the preparation time was short (CSI ¼ 200 ms) makes sense if, as we assumed, the explicit word cues activate the task goal information more rapidly or more directly (i.e., without going through the inner speech based retrieval process) than do the less transparent letter cues. Just as well-learned directional words (e.g., left ) seem capable of directing one s attention automatically toward those directions even when they are not predictive of the locations of subsequently presented targets (Hommel, Pratt, Colzato, & Godijn, 2001), it might be the case that such well-learned words as color or shape can automatically direct one s attention toward the designated stimulus dimension and thereby reduce the effect of perceptual interference from the other, task-irrelevant dimension. When the cues are less explicit or less well-learned, however, the process of translating those cues into more direct task instructions is needed at least on some proportion of trials, and this translation process seems to implicate the retrieval of task names via inner speech. As these considerations suggest, the main findings and conclusions from the current study are consistent with those from previous dual-task studies using the list and

16 138 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) precued task sequence paradigms (Baddeley et al., 2001; Emerson & Miyake, 2003; Goschke, 2000; Saeki & Saito, in press): Although participants often recruit inner speech to support their task switching performance, they do so only when there are no strong or well-learned external cues available that automatically activate the appropriate task goal information or even automatically direct their attention toward the appropriate stimulus dimension. In this sense, inner speech may not be a necessary component of task switching performance, but it serves as an efficient and reliable tool or supporting mechanism that participants count on when the need for exerting endogenous control is strong (Emerson & Miyake, 2003). The current study goes beyond the previous studies by focusing on a paradigm that eliminates the need for actively keeping track of the task sequence (i.e., the random task cuing paradigm) and showing more clearly that one function of inner speech is to serve as a retrieval aid for task goals. Despite these overall commonalities between the current study and the previous studies, there are also some differences between the random task cuing paradigm and the list or precued task sequence paradigm concerning the effect of articulatory suppression. Perhaps the most obvious difference is that the absolute magnitude of the increase in switch cost caused by concurrent articulatory suppression (compared to the control condition) is much larger in the list paradigm than in the random task cuing paradigm. For example, in the Emerson and Miyake (2003) experiments (Experiments 1 4), the switch cost increases due to articulatory suppression were in the ms range, but, in the current study, the increase was only 21 ms in the word cue condition (42 ms cost in the control-long condition vs. 63 ms cost in the articulatory suppression condition) and 73 ms in the letter cue condition (43 ms cost vs. 116 ms cost, respectively). One possible reason for this marked difference in the absolute magnitude of the articulatory suppression effect between the two paradigms may be that the role of inner speech in the list paradigm goes significantly beyond the verbally mediated retrieval of task goals. As we emphasized earlier, one key characteristic of the list paradigm absent in the random task cuing paradigm is that participants are required to keep track of the task sequence for the alternating-task lists (e.g., alternating between addition and subtraction). Given that language is inherently linear and may be suitable for sequencing information (Carlson, 1997; Emerson & Miyake, 2003), it is likely that, in the list paradigm, inner speech was responsible not just for retrieving and activating task goals but also for actively keeping track of the task sequence for the alternating lists, although neither Baddeley et al. (2001) nor Emerson and Miyake (2003) clearly differentiated these potentially separable functions of inner speech in their discussion. We should note, however, that these increases in switch costs represent raw numbers and do not take into account the magnitudes of the switch costs in the control conditions, which were quite different across the two paradigms. In fact, when the negative effect of articulatory suppression on switch costs is expressed in terms of percentage increase using the switch cost in the control condition as the baseline, the results from the two paradigms are much more similar, with percentage increases of % in the Emerson and Miyake (2003) experiments and percentage increases

17 A. Miyake et al. / Acta Psychologica 115 (2004) of 50% and 170% in the current study for the word and letter cue conditions, respectively. Thus, although participants in the current study were not required to keep track of the task sequence, the percentage increase in cost due to articulatory suppression was substantial, particularly in the letter cues condition (170%). These observations suggest that, even though the absolute magnitude of the articulatory suppression effect may be different for the two paradigms, the contribution of inner speech in the random task cuing paradigm may be as substantial as that in the list paradigm, at least when the cues still impose some degree of retrieval demand. One interesting question raised by the results of the current study concerns which type of switch cost is affected by articulatory suppression. It is possible to calculate at least two types of switch cost, namely, (a) the cost associated with switching from one task to another (e.g., switch RT minus repeat RT for the mixed task trials), called local or specific switch costs, and (b) the cost of mixing two tasks in a trial sequence compared to always performing one task (e.g., repeat RT from the mixedtask trials minus RT for the pure-task trials), called global or mixing costs (Kray & Lindenberger, 2000; Mayr, 2001). These two types of switch costs are potentially dissociable, as indicated by the findings that reliable age-related changes have been observed for the latter type of switch cost (Kray & Lindenberger, 2000; Mayr, 2001; Meiran & Gotler, 2001), but not for the former type (Kray & Lindenberger, 2000; Mayr & Liebscher, 2001; Salthouse, Fristoe, McGuthry, & Hambrick, 1998; see Kray, Li, & Lindenberger, 2002, for an exception). The results of the current study have shown that, unlike aging, articulatory suppression significantly affects local switch costs (i.e., repeat vs. switch RTs in mixedtask trials), at least when the letter cues are presented (Fig. 3B). We interpreted this effect specific to the letter cue condition in terms of the involvement of inner speech in the retrieval of task goals when the cues are not direct or explicit enough. In addition, a comparison of the pure-task RTs (Fig. 2) with the repeat trial RTs from the mixed-task blocks (Fig. 3) suggests that global costs also significantly increase with concurrent articulatory suppression. In fact, it is interesting that, although the RTs were significantly longer in the foot tapping condition (M ¼ 440 ms) than in the articulatory suppression condition (M ¼ 405 ms) for the pure-task trials (as reported earlier), the RTs for the repeat trials of the mixed task blocks show the opposite pattern: The RTs were significantly longer under articulatory suppression than under foot tapping for both the word cue condition (M ¼ 560 ms vs. 509 ms, respectively), F ð1; 47Þ ¼10:59, MSE ¼ 0:004, p ¼ 0:002, g 2 ¼ 0:183 and the letter cue condition (M ¼ 582 ms vs. 521 ms), F ð1; 47Þ ¼14:16, MSE ¼ 0:004, p < 0:001, g 2 ¼ 0:231. One interpretation of this intriguing reversal of the dual-task effect is that, for the mixed-task trials, participants use inner speech to retrieve the task goal information even for task-repeat trials, perhaps because the task order is unpredictable in the random task cuing paradigm. Although this account seems plausible and deserves further investigation, one finding that seems somewhat inconsistent with this proposal is that the RT difference between the articulatory suppression and foot tapping conditions for the repeat trials of the mixed-task blocks was equivalent for the word cues and for the letter cues (51 ms vs. 61 ms, respectively), F ð1; 47Þ ¼0:18, MSE ¼ 0:003, p ¼ 0:674, g 2 ¼ 0:004.

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Voluntary Task Switching: Chasing the Elusive Homunculus

Voluntary Task Switching: Chasing the Elusive Homunculus Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2005, Vol. 31, No. 4, 683 702 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.4.683

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Chunking away task-switch costs: a test of the chunk-point hypothesis

Chunking away task-switch costs: a test of the chunk-point hypothesis Psychon Bull Rev (2015) 22:884 889 DOI 10.3758/s13423-014-0721-3 BRIEF REPORT Chunking away task-switch costs: a test of the chunk-point hypothesis Darryl W. Schneider & Gordon D. Logan Published online:

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Sticky Rules: Integration Between Abstract Rules and Specific Actions

Sticky Rules: Integration Between Abstract Rules and Specific Actions Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2005, Vol. 31, No. 2, 337 350 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.2.337

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Task switching: The effect of task recency with dual- and single-affordance stimuli

Task switching: The effect of task recency with dual- and single-affordance stimuli THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (7), 1255 1276 Task switching: The effect of task recency with dual- and single-affordance stimuli Petroc Sumner and Lubna Ahmed Imperial College

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases switch costs in the explicit task-cuing procedure

Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases switch costs in the explicit task-cuing procedure Journal Memory & Cognition 2006,?? 34 (?), (6),???-??? 1250-1259 Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases switch costs in the explicit task-cuing procedure GORDON D.

More information

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Laboratory Equipment & Test Procedures. Motor learning and control historically has built its discipline through laboratory research. This has led to the

More information

Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage:

Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage: Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 70 78 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy Task switching in video game players: Benefits of selective

More information

The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: Patterns of selective interference in subitizing and counting

The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: Patterns of selective interference in subitizing and counting Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (4), 675-681 The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: Patterns of selective interference in subitizing and counting LANA M. TRICK University of Guelph,

More information

The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch Catherine M. Arrington and Gordon D. Logan

The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch Catherine M. Arrington and Gordon D. Logan PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch Catherine M. Arrington and Gordon D. Logan Vanderbilt University ABSTRACT Task-switching paradigms are widely used to study executive

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

The Task Switching Paradigm. PDF created with pdffactory trial version

The Task Switching Paradigm. PDF created with pdffactory trial version The Task Switching Paradigm Overview Executive functions Task switching paradigms Simple phenomena Complex phenomena Executive Control Executive processes deal with the control of action, thought, and

More information

Current Task Activation Predicts General Effects of Advance Preparation in Task Switching

Current Task Activation Predicts General Effects of Advance Preparation in Task Switching Current Task Activation Predicts General Effects of Advance Preparation in Task Switching Edita Poljac, Ab de Haan, and Gerard P. van Galen Nijmegen Institute for Cognition and Information Radboud University

More information

TESTING THE INTERACTION OF STIMULUS REPETITION WITH SWITCH COSTS ACROSS AGE GROUPS

TESTING THE INTERACTION OF STIMULUS REPETITION WITH SWITCH COSTS ACROSS AGE GROUPS TESTING THE INTERACTION OF STIMULUS REPETITION WITH SWITCH COSTS ACROSS AGE GROUPS A Thesis Presented to The Academic Faculty by Susan LaGrone In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

More information

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B87 Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized Wim Gevers*,

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences

Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 970-976 Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences Darryl W. Schneider Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee Exploring the hierarchical control relationship

More information

Differential Effects of Cue Changes and Task Changes on Task-Set Selection Costs

Differential Effects of Cue Changes and Task Changes on Task-Set Selection Costs Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2003, Vol. 29, No. 3, 362 372 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.362

More information

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2002, Vol. 28, No. 3, 445 457 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.445

More information

RUNNING HEAD: Control mechanisms in task switching. The Role of Working Memory Capacity and Interference Resolution Mechanisms in Task.

RUNNING HEAD: Control mechanisms in task switching. The Role of Working Memory Capacity and Interference Resolution Mechanisms in Task. 1 RUNNING HEAD: Control mechanisms in task switching The Role of Working Memory Capacity and Interference Resolution Mechanisms in Task Switching Corinne Pettigrew 1 and Randi C. Martin 2 Affiliations:

More information

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES by John W. Brelsford, Jr. and Richard C. Atkinson TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 July 21, 1967 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES!, Reproduction

More information

Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010.

Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010. Replacing the frontal lobes? 1 Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010. Ben R. Newell 1 Christopher

More information

The Effects of Recent Practice on Task Switching

The Effects of Recent Practice on Task Switching Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 5, 919 936 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.919

More information

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (7), 1236-1247 doi: 10.3758/MC/36.7.1236 An action sequence held in memory can interfere with response selection of a target stimulus, but does not interfere with response activation

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist M------- P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION Name: Date of Birth: Date of Evaluation: 05-28-2015 Tests Administered: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition

The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (4), 603-609 The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition MIRIAM GADE Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany AND IRING

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction

More information

Control processes in voluntary and explicitly cued task switching

Control processes in voluntary and explicitly cued task switching THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Vol. 67, No. 10, 1944 1958, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.879390 Control processes in voluntary and explicitly cued task switching Michael

More information

Task-Switching Performance With 1:1 and 2:1 Cue Task Mappings: Not So Different After All

Task-Switching Performance With 1:1 and 2:1 Cue Task Mappings: Not So Different After All Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2011, Vol. 37, No. 2, 405 415 2011 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/11/$10 DOI: 10.1037/a0021967 Task-Switching Performance

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

Contextual control over task-set retrieval

Contextual control over task-set retrieval Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (8), 2047-2053 doi:10.3758/app.72.8.2047 BRIEF REPORTS Contextual control over task-set retrieval MATTHEW J. C. CRUMP AND GORDON D. LOGAN Vanderbilt University,

More information

Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1. Feature Integration and Task Switching:

Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1. Feature Integration and Task Switching: Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1 Feature Integration and Task Switching: Diminished Switch Costs After Controlling for Stimulus, Response, and Cue Repetitions James R. Schmidt and

More information

innate mechanism of proportionality adaptation stage activation or recognition stage innate biological metrics acquired social metrics

innate mechanism of proportionality adaptation stage activation or recognition stage innate biological metrics acquired social metrics 1 PROCESSES OF THE CORRELATION OF SPACE (LENGTHS) AND TIME (DURATIONS) IN HUMAN PERCEPTION Lev I Soyfer To study the processes and mechanisms of the correlation between space and time, particularly between

More information

Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs

Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 15 (0), 000±000 Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs Joachim Hoffmann, Andrea Kiesel, and Albrecht Sebald University

More information

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K.

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K. Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration John Wearden Keele University U.K. In this talk I want to illustrate the use of the techniques of production, reproduction, and verbal estimation

More information

Post-response stimulation and the Simon effect: Further evidence of action effect integration

Post-response stimulation and the Simon effect: Further evidence of action effect integration VISUAL COGNITION, 2002, 9 (4/5), 528 539 Post-response stimulation and the Simon effect: Further evidence of action effect integration Marc Grosjean and J. Toby Mordkoff Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania

More information

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES Journal of Experimental Vol. 51, No. 4, 1956 Psychology CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES KENNETH H. KURTZ AND CARL I. HOVLAND Under conditions where several concepts are learned concurrently

More information

Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration

Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:1694 1709 DOI 10.3758/s13414-011-0135-5 Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration Tomonari Shimomura & Takatsune Kumada Published

More information

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan University of Alberta The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm by Shawn Tan A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial

More information

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008 Task Switching Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008 Monsell, 2003 Task Switching Basic phenomena Reaction time (RT) cost for switching tasks Higher error rate Diminished if prepared for switch Slower RT

More information

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation The description and presentation of results involves a number of topics. These include scales of measurement, descriptive statistics used to summarize

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction Marc Pomplun and Sindhura Sunkara Department of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts at Boston 100 Morrissey

More information

Motor Programs Lab. 1. Record your reaction and movement time in ms for each trial on the individual data Table 1 below. Table I: Individual Data RT

Motor Programs Lab. 1. Record your reaction and movement time in ms for each trial on the individual data Table 1 below. Table I: Individual Data RT Motor Programs Lab Introduction. This lab will simulate an important experiment performed by Henry and Rogers (1960). The task involved the subject responding to an external signal then executing a simple,

More information

Switching Between Tasks of Unequal Familiarity: The Role of Stimulus-Attribute and Response-Set Selection

Switching Between Tasks of Unequal Familiarity: The Role of Stimulus-Attribute and Response-Set Selection Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 2, 455 469 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.455

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article VISUAL SEARCH REMAINS EFFICIENT WHEN VISUAL WORKING MEMORY IS FULL Geoffrey F. Woodman, Edward K. Vogel, and Steven J. Luck University of Iowa Abstract Many theories of attention have

More information

Task-Set Switching and Long-Term Memory Retrieval

Task-Set Switching and Long-Term Memory Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2000, Vol. 26, No. 5, 1124-1140 0278-7393tI~0/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.5.1124

More information

Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-Signal Paradigm

Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-Signal Paradigm Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2011, Vol. 37, No. 2, 392 404 2010 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021800 Balancing Cognitive Demands:

More information

Bivalency is Costly: Bivalent Stimuli Elicit Cautious Responding

Bivalency is Costly: Bivalent Stimuli Elicit Cautious Responding Bivalency is Costly: Bivalent Stimuli Elicit Cautious Responding Todd S. Woodward, 1,2 Beat Meier, 1 Christine Tipper, 1 and Peter Graf 1 1 Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver,

More information

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 396 409 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.396

More information

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI?

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI? Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI? Franklin P. Tamborello, II Phillip H. Chung Michael D. Byrne Rice University Department of Psychology 61 S. Main,

More information

Selecting a Response in Task Switching: Testing a Model of Compound Cue Retrieval

Selecting a Response in Task Switching: Testing a Model of Compound Cue Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 1, 122 136 2009 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013744 Selecting a Response

More information

On the Difficulty of Task Switching: Assessing the Role of Task-Set Inhibition

On the Difficulty of Task Switching: Assessing the Role of Task-Set Inhibition On the Difficulty of Task Switching: Assessing the Role of Task-Set Inhibition Mei-Ching Lien Oregon State University Eric Ruthruff University of New Mexico David Kuhns Oregon State University This study

More information

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making Matthew A J Apps 1,2, Laura Grima 2, Sanjay Manohar 2, Masud Husain 1,2 1 Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience,

More information

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences 2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences Stanislas Dehaene Chair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture n 5 Bayesian Decision-Making Lecture material translated

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

Color naming and color matching: A reply to Kuehni and Hardin

Color naming and color matching: A reply to Kuehni and Hardin 1 Color naming and color matching: A reply to Kuehni and Hardin Pendaran Roberts & Kelly Schmidtke Forthcoming in Review of Philosophy and Psychology. The final publication is available at Springer via

More information

Separating Habit and Recollection: Memory Slips, Process Dissociations, and Probability Matching

Separating Habit and Recollection: Memory Slips, Process Dissociations, and Probability Matching Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1996, Vol. 22, No. 6,1323-1335 Copyright 19% by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/96/$3.00 Separating Habit and

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

RUNNING HEAD: EXPLORING TASK-SET RECONFIGURATION. Exploring task-set reconfiguration with random task sequences

RUNNING HEAD: EXPLORING TASK-SET RECONFIGURATION. Exploring task-set reconfiguration with random task sequences RUNNING HEAD: EXPLORING TASK-SET RECONFIGURATION Exploring task-set reconfiguration with random task sequences Emilio G. Milán, Daniel Sanabria, Francisco Tornay, & Antonio González Departamento de Psicología

More information

Acta Psychologica 141 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica

Acta Psychologica 141 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica Acta Psychologica 141 (2012) 270 275 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/ locate/actpsy Gaze cues influence memory but not for long Michael

More information

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 703 708 Test review Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., 2002 1. Test description The Trail Making Test

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (6), 1111-1116 doi:10.3758/pbr.15.6.1111 Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Mei-Ching Lien Oregon

More information

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SESORY COSTRAIS? TOWARD A UIFIED THEORY OF GEERAL TASK DIFFICULTY Hanna Benoni and Yehoshua Tsal Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University hannaben@post.tau.ac.il

More information

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/24/2018: Lecture 05-2 Note: This Powerpoint presentation

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms

Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms Daryl Fougnie and René Marois Vanderbilt

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 2, 434 445 2009 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014529 Influence of Visual Stimulus

More information

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

CANTAB Test descriptions by function CANTAB Test descriptions by function The 22 tests in the CANTAB battery may be divided into the following main types of task: screening tests visual memory tests executive function, working memory and

More information

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1 Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen gert.haubensak@t-online.de Abstract Two experiments are reported in which the participants judged

More information

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning GSTF Journal of Psychology (JPsych) Vol. No., August 5 Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning Hironori Oto 79 Received 6 Jul 5 Accepted 9 Aug 5 Abstract This paper

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

On How to be Unpredictable: Evidence from the Voluntary Task-Switching Paradigm. Ulrich Mayr. University Bonn, Germany.

On How to be Unpredictable: Evidence from the Voluntary Task-Switching Paradigm. Ulrich Mayr. University Bonn, Germany. In press: Psychological Science On How to be Unpredictable: Evidence from the Voluntary Task-Switching Paradigm Ulrich Mayr University Bonn, Germany Theodor Bell University of Oregon Running head: Voluntary

More information

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching Ayla Barutchu 1 *, Stefanie I. Becker 2, Olivia Carter 3, Robert Hester 3, Neil L. Levy 1,4 1 Florey Institute

More information

Modeling Control Strategies in the N-Back Task

Modeling Control Strategies in the N-Back Task Modeling Control Strategies in the N-Back Task Ion Juvina (ijuvina@cmu.edu) & Niels A. Taatgen (taatgen@cmu.edu) Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, 5000 Forbes Avenue Pittsburgh, PA

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the U.S. Office of Naval Research 1 Introduction During

More information

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Mem Cogn (2011) 39:1222 1231 DOI 10.3758/s13421-011-0105-0 Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Dawn M. McBride & Jaclyn K. Beckner & Drew

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Psychon Bull Rev (2017) 24:1651 1657 DOI 10.3758/s13423-017-1226-7 BRIEF REPORT Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Sébastien De Schrijver 1 & Pierre Barrouillet 1 Published

More information

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 J'OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 446-452 (1963) Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 A visual configuration, or pattern, has qualities over and above those which can be specified

More information