Sex ratio and male sexual characters in a population of blue tits, Parus caeruleus

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Sex ratio and male sexual characters in a population of blue tits, Parus caeruleus"

Transcription

1 Behavioral Ecology doi: /beheco/ari089 Advance Access publication 12 October 2005 Sex ratio and male sexual characters in a population of blue tits, Parus caeruleus A. Dreiss, a M. Richard, a F. Moyen, b J. White, a A.P. Møller, c and E. Danchin d a Laboratoire Fonctionnement et Evolution des Systèmes Ecologiques, UMR 7625, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Bât. A, Case 237, 7ème étage, 7 quai Saint-Bernard, Paris Cedex 5, France, b Laboratoire d Ecologie Générale, Muséum d Histoire Naturelle, Brunoy, France, c Laboratoire de Parasitologie Evolutive, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Paris, France, and d Laboratoire Evolution et Diversité Biologique, Université Paul Sabatier, Toulouse, France Sex allocation theory proposes that parents should bias the sex ratio of their offspring if the reproductive value of one sex is greater than that of the other. In the monogamous blue tit (Parus caeruleus), males have a greater variance in reproductive success than females, and high-quality males have higher reproductive success than high-quality females due to extrapair paternity. Consequently, females mating with attractive males are expected to produce broods biased toward sons, as sons benefit more than daughters from inheriting their father s characteristics. Song and plumage color in birds are secondary sexual characters indicating male quality and involved in female choice. We used these male sexual traits in blue tits to investigate adaptive sex ratio manipulation by females. We did not find any relationship between male color ornamentation and brood sex ratio, contrary to previous studies. On the other hand, the length of the strophe bout (i.e., the mean number of strophes per strophe bout) of fathers was positively related with the proportion of sons in their broods. The length of the strophe bout is supposed to reflect male quality in terms of neuromuscular performance. We further showed that sons produced in experimentally enlarged broods had shorter strophe bouts than sons raised in reduced broods. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that females adjust the sex ratio of their broods in response to the phenotype of their mate. Key words: dawn chorus, male song, Parus caeruleus, plumage color, sex ratio. [Behav Ecol 17:13 19 (2006)] According to sex allocation theory, if parents could predict the relative reproductive value of their sons and daughters, individuals that are able to bias the sex ratio of their offspring toward the sex with the higher reproductive value would be favored by natural selection (Trivers and Willard, 1973). Trivers and Willard (1973) suggested that parental characteristics and condition, as factors determining offspring quality, are likely to be essential factors determining the fitness costs and benefits of overproducing sons or daughters. Parents that optimize the sex ratio of their brood accordingly should be selectively favored. Although the mechanism is unknown, certain parental characteristics, like male survival (Sheldon, 1999) and female body condition (Nager et al., 1999), have been found to be linked to an adaptive skew in brood sex ratio. Sexual attractiveness of a male may hypothetically affect the optimal sex ratio of his brood(s). If male attractiveness is transmitted to his sons, then sons of a highly attractive male may be of greater reproductive value than daughters. Conversely, daughters of unattractive males may be of relatively higher reproductive value than sons. If that were the case, it would be adaptive for both parents to bias the sex ratio in response to male secondary sexual characters. In birds, certain studies have shown evidence of adaptive bias (reviewed in Cockburn et al., 2002), although there is no evidence of adaptive bias across all studies conducted so far (Ewen et al., 2004). Within species and populations, great variability exists in sex ratios. For instance, in the collared flycatcher (Ficedula albicollis), brood sex ratio is Address correspondence to A. Dreiss. adreiss@snv.jussieu.fr. Received 27 October 2004; revised 25 July 2005; accepted 31 August Ó The Author Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. All rights reserved. Downloaded from For permissions, please journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org male biased when the male rearing the brood has a large forehead patch (Ellegren et al., 1996). This trait is a heritable secondary sexual character involved in female choice (Sheldon et al., 1997) and related to male condition (Gustafsson et al., 1995). However, other studies did not find such a trend. For instance, in the barn swallow (Hirundo rustica), females do not seem to adjust sex ratio in relation to their mate s tail length (Saino et al., 1999), a character that is involved in female choice (Møller, 1995) and which appears to signal viability (Møller, 1994). However, when the sex ratios produced by the same females mated in different years to males with different tail lengths were recorded, there was a strong positive relationship between difference in tail length of the two males and difference in brood sex ratio (Saino et al., 2002). This result emphasizes the importance of controlling for potentially confounding variables when attempting to analyze factors hypothesized to affect brood sex ratios. We studied brood sex ratios and their relationship with male sexual characters, that is to say male song and coloration, in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus), a socially monogamous passerine. Previous studies have shown that females mated to males with high survival prospects (Svensson and Nilsson, 1996) or with a plumage with high UV reflectance (Sheldon et al., 1999) bias hatching sex ratio of their broods in favor of sons. Another study on the great tit (Parus major) found a correlation between brood sex ratio and male body size (Kölliker et al., 1999). However, findings are inconsistent among blue tit populations because Leech et al. (2001) found no significant correlation between sex ratio and parental quality as reflected by biometrics or apparent parental survival in a study based on very large sample sizes. There are no studies of the relationship between song and offspring sex ratio in blue tits or any other species. Song in birds is known to be a secondary sexual character reliably reflecting male quality and involved in female choice (Andersson,

2 14 Behavioral Ecology 1994). Therefore, individual variation in song characteristics affects male reproductive success. Indeed, male blue tits that produce extrapair offspring have longer strophes (song units constituted of an uninterrupted succession of sounds and separated from other units by a pause) than males that lose paternity (Kempenaers et al., 1997). Furthermore, individual variation in song production depends on male quality. Tarsus length, an indicator of rearing conditions, is linked to song repertoire size in blue tits (Doutrelant et al., 2000). Strophe duration could also indicate male quality in blue tits because Bijnens (1988) found a correlation between strophe duration and apparent survival rate. Finally, song constitutes a costly signal and consequently is believed to represent an honest signal of quality (Gil and Gahr, 2002). In this study, we determined whether natural variation in brood sex ratio in a population of blue tits was related to song characteristics of the attending male during the dawn chorus. If song is a signal used by the female to assess mate quality, we predicted that females mated with males producing highquality songs should bias their broods toward sons. Furthermore, we aimed to investigate whether the natural crown color, a sexual signal showing sexual dimorphism (Andersson et al., 1998; Hunt et al., 1998), is related to sex ratio, as found experimentally by Griffith et al. (2003) and Sheldon et al. (1999). We also tested whether the sex ratio of extrapair chicks was male biased, as predicted by theory. Indeed, it is commonly assumed that extrapair males are of higher quality than social partners (Kempenaers et al., 1992), and thus extrapair young may have higher reproductive value than within-pair young. Nevertheless, recent studies of birds did not find evidence consistent with such a relationship (Leech et al., 2001; Saino et al., 1999; Sheldon and Ellegren, 1996; Westerdahl et al., 1997). Bird song is a complex trait that is affected not only by factors such as predation, aggressiveness, environmental quality, and ability to raise an immune response (Gil and Gahr, 2002) but also by developmental constraints (Buchanan et al., 2003; Spencer et al., 2004). In order to determine whether developmental conditions of chicks affect the subsequent development of their song, we manipulated brood size and recorded the songs of first-year reproducing males from experimentally enlarged, reduced, or control broods. Our aim was to test in this complementary experiment whether song characteristics are affected by developmental conditions in blue tits because this hypothesis remains untested so far and is crucial for arguments about adaptive sex ratio adjustment. METHODS The field work was carried out in 2002 and 2003 in the Parc Régional de la Forêt d Orient (Aube, France, 48 17# N, 4 17# E) as part of a long-term study on tit reproduction. Circa 1000 nest-boxes, set in 1998, are evenly spread over a total area of 500 ha. The forest consists mainly of sessile oak (Quercus petraea) and European hornbeam (Carpinus betulus). Nest-boxes were visited every 2 4 days to record the laying date of the first egg. The final clutch size was determined during the incubation period. After 12 days of incubation, the nests were visited every second day to determine hatching date and brood size. Parents were nest trapped 7 14 days after hatching, and the young were ringed 7 10 days after hatching. For all birds, we recorded body mass (to the nearest 0.25 g, using a Pesola balance) and tarsus length (to the nearest 0.1 mm). We collected three to four feathers in the middle of the crown of adult males for color measurements. From 23rd March to 24th April 2002 and 2003, 84 breeding males were observed during the dawn chorus. Observations and recordings began 1 h after astronomical twilight, before blue tit dawn chorus starts, and ended when the female exited the nest-box. All males were recorded during the presumed fertile period of the female, which probably occurs from a few days before the first egg until the day when the penultimate egg is laid (Mace, 1987). We only used male recordings done from 6 days before the first egg until the day of the sixth egg. Song recording and analysis Recordings were made using shotgun microphones Audio Technica AT835B connected to a Marantz PMD101 cassette recorder. Song was analyzed with the computer program Avisoft- SASlab Light ( Blue tit song at dawn is characterized by the repetition of the same strophe during a strophe bout, before the male changes strophe type (Poesel and Kempenaers, 2000; Figure 1). Different features of song output were estimated. Mean is given with SD, and repeatability is given for 10 males, which were recorded twice, using a one-way ANOVA (df ¼ 10,11): (1) mean strophe duration ( ; repeatability: R 2 ¼.85, p ¼.002); (2) singing rate (number of strophes per minute, in strophe bouts where pauses did not exceed 10 s) ( ; repeatability: R 2 ¼.84, p ¼.004); (3) strophe diversity estimated as the number of different strophes divided by the number of strophe bouts ( ; repeatability: R 2 ¼.88, p ¼.002); and (4) mean strophe bout length (total number of strophes per strophe bout) ( ; repeatability: R 2 ¼.89, p ¼.001). As the number of strophes sung during the entire dawn chorus was not significantly repeatable (N ¼ 13, R 2 ¼.66, p ¼.091), we only considered a dawn chorus if it contained more than 69 strophes, which is the upper limit of the 95% confidence interval of strophe bout length. We did not consider drift, which is a measure of performance change over time (Poesel and Kempenaers, 2000), because we did not find any Figure 1 (a) Sonograms of two types of strophes from a male blue tit. The segments represent strophe duration. (b) One example of a sequence of strophes sung at dawn, with the succession of three strophe bouts. Each segment represents a strophe.

3 Dreiss et al. Offspring sex ratio, male song, and male coloration 15 significant repeatability of this measure in our sample (N ¼ 10, R 2 ¼.54, p ¼.35). Color analysis We measured feather reflectance of the crown of 82 males observed at dawn with a portable spectroradiometer (Avantes USB-2000 calibrated from 200 to 850 nm) and a deuteriumhalogen light source (DH-2000 emitting from 215 to 1500 nm) connected with a 1.5-mm-diameter sensor inserted in a miniature black chamber (Théry et al., 2005). Reflectance spectra were taken at 90 relative to a 99% reflectance standard ( nm Spectralon) and to dark current (black velvet background). A reference and dark current calibration were taken before measuring feathers of each individual. For each individual bird, we computed a mean of five reflectance spectra, each measured with two feathers superimposed. Three objective parameters of color perception were calculated according to the method described by Endler (1990). Mean is given with SD and repeatability is given with a one-way ANOVA (df ¼ 82,362). (1) Brightness (spectral intensity) ( ; repeatability: R 2 ¼.89, p,.0001) was estimated by R , the sum of reflectance from 300 to 700 nm. (2) Hue (spectral location) ( ; repeatability: R 2 ¼.81, p,.0001) was defined as k max, the wavelength of maximum reflectance. (3) We used as measure of chroma (spectral purity), UV-chroma (R /R ) ( ; repeatability: R 2 ¼.81, p,.0001), which has been used in several studies of blue tit color (Andersson et al., 1998; Delhey et al., 2003; Griffith et al., 2003; Sheldon et al., 1999). Brood size manipulation In 2002, we performed a brood size manipulation experiment to test for effects of rearing condition on subsequent song performance by randomly transferring either one or two chicks from 39 broods and adding these to another 39 broods with the same hatching date. Chicks were transferred in heated boxes at the age of 1 3 days. Chicks from enlarged broods were all marked with the same color ring, while chicks from reduced broods were marked with another color. Molecular methods Blood was collected in capillary tubes (20 ll) and transferred into tubes containing 150 ll phosphate-buffered saline buffer with 2 mm ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid. Tissues were collected from dead embryos and stored in seven volumes of ethanol 70%. Samples were stored at 6 C, then digested all night at 55 C by adding 3 ll proteinase K 20 mg/ml and 3 ll Tris-NaCl (final concentrations [Tris] ¼ 10 mm; [NaCl] ¼ 25 mm). For DNA extraction, we used the QIAquick96 Kit (Qiagen, Courtaboeuf, France), according to the manufacturer s protocol. Sexing technique The sex of all nestlings from 84 broods was determined by polymerase chain reaction (PCR) amplification of the CHD1- W and CHD1-Z genes with the P2 and P8 primers (Griffiths et al., 1998). PCR products, separated by electrophoresis on 1% agarose gels containing ethidium bromide, were visualized under UV light. As a control, parents were also sexed and were all found to be of the expected sex. The eggs containing no visible embryos were discarded. However, because some of these eggs may still have been fertilized, our measure of the brood sex ratio may not correspond exactly to the primary sex ratio, sensu stricto. Paternity analysis Paternity was analyzed by PCR amplification of five microsatellite loci. The five pairs of primers used are from Dawson et al. (2000) and Tanner et al. (GenBank accession number AF041466). The primers were redesigned for multiplex purposes (detailed information can be obtained from the corresponding author on request). PCR products were separated and analyzed with an automatic monocapillary genetic analyzer Abi310. Due to high polymorphism, the probability of detection of extrapair young was.99. Statistical analyses Analyses were performed using SAS (1999). Sample values are given as mean 6 SE. Only strophe bout length differed significantly from normality in a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. In order to normalize this variable, we transformed it as ÿlog(1/x). Sex ratio, expressed as the proportion of males in a brood, was analyzed with generalized linear models with binomial error distribution, according to Wilson and Hardy (2002). We performed a simplification of the model by backward elimination of the nonsignificant interactions and variables. Second-degree interactions were included in the model, except interactions between correlated song variables. The significance of the final model was determined by a chi-square test between initial model deviance and final model deviance. Partial broods (never more than two chicks missing) were also included in the analyses as suggested by Fiala (1980). A few missing variables for some of the birds explain slightly varying sample size among tests. The analysis of the difference between extrapair and withinpair sex ratios was also performed with a generalized linear model with binomial error distribution. For each nest containing both extrapair and within-pair chicks, we determined the two measures of sex ratios (extrapair or within-pair). The sex ratio obtained was set as the dependent variable and origin (extrapair or within-pair) as the independent variable. Principal component analysis was used to summarize information of song features with a few independent variables (Table 1). The two first principal components Ch1 and Ch2, explaining 64% of the variance, were retained for further analysis. Only these two first principal components have an eigenvalue greater than 1. Ch1 is mainly explained by variables strophe bout length, strophe duration, and song rate and can be interpreted as song performance. Ch2 is mainly explained by strophe diversity and can be interpreted as song diversity. We analyzed the link between all male characteristics with Pearson correlations. The effect of brood manipulation on male song was analyzed with ANOVA procedures. We used the sequential Bonferroni correction to assess the table-wide type I error rate for multiple tests (Holm, 1979). Chick body mass after manipulation was analyzed with a MIXED procedure, which accounts for the nest as a random effect. Table 1 Loadings of the song variables on the four principal components extracted and cumulative variance associated with each Ch1 Ch2 Ch3 Ch4 Strophe bout length ÿ ÿ0.69 ÿ0.35 Song rate ÿ Strophe duration ÿ Diversity ÿ0.18 Cumulative variance 38% 64% 86% 100%

4 16 Behavioral Ecology RESULTS Brood size manipulation, offspring condition, and song Brood manipulation had an effect on chick condition as reflected by chick body mass (ÿ2, ÿ1, 0, 1, or 2 chicks added) (F 2,1061 ¼ 2.27, p,.023) after controlling for age at measurement (F 2,1061 ¼ 22.83, p,.0001). As expected, chicks from reduced broods were heavier than those from enlarged broods (mean 6 SE for broods reduced by two nestlings: , N ¼ 13; mean 6 SE for broods increased by two nestlings: , N ¼ 17). Five reproducing males were recorded in 2003 from more than 380 chicks from enlarged broods in 2002, while nine males were recorded from the 300 chicks from reduced broods. Song analyses of these males showed no significant differences between the two groups of males for strophe duration, song rate, and strophe diversity (strophe duration: R 2 ¼.04, F 1,11 ¼ 0.47, p ¼.51; song rate: R 2 ¼.09, F 1,11 ¼ 1.26, p ¼.28; strophe diversity: R 2 ¼.01, F 1,11 ¼ 0.06, p ¼.81). However, males that were reared in enlarged broods produced significantly shorter strophe bouts than males reared in reduced broods (Figure 2; R 2 ¼.41, F 1,11 ¼ 7.07, p ¼.024. This relation was no more significant after Bonferroni adjustment for four statistical tests: critical p-value:.05/4 ¼.0125). Male song and color Song rate was correlated with strophe duration and strophe bout length after Bonferroni correction (threshold value: a ¼.008 after correction; Table 2). No significant correlation was found between song features, male crown color characteristics, and male biometrics. Population sex ratio Mean brood sex ratio from the 84 broods was (mean 6 SE, minimum ¼ 0.1, maximum ¼ 1.0). Although sex ratio was strongly biased in some broods, its distribution did not differ significantly from a binomial distribution (v 2 ¼ 1.73, df ¼ 2, p ¼.41). Table 2 Pearson correlation coefficients among male dawn song traits Song rate Diversity Strophe bout length Strophe duration ÿ0.31** 0.14 Song rate * Diversity ÿ0.02 *p,.05; **p,.01 after Bonferroni adjustment for six statistical tests (critical p value:.05/6 ¼.0083;.01/6 ¼.0017). N ¼ 105. There was no significant correlation between laying date and sex ratio bias in the brood (difference from 0.5 brood sex ratio) (linear regression: R 2 ¼.0001, F 1,81 ¼ 0.01, p ¼.75). There was no significant relationship between brood sex ratio bias and clutch size (linear regression: R 2 ¼.0064, F 1,81 ¼ 0.52, p ¼.47). We did not find a significant relationship between sex ratio bias and the number of unhatched eggs (linear regression: R 2 ¼.0011, F 1,81 ¼ 0.08, p ¼.93). Song, color, and brood sex ratio In order to determine whether sex ratio varied with song, color, or morphology, we performed a generalized linear model, with sex ratio as the dependent variable and song and color variables, parental biometrics, laying date, and year as the independent variables. The final model explaining sex ratio in our population contains only the strophe bout length during the dawn chorus (Figure 3; v 2 ¼ 5.68, p ¼.017). Hence, we found no significant relationship between sex ratio and year, laying date, parental biometrics, crown color, or any of the first three song variables (strophe duration, song rate, strophe diversity) in our model. Within-pair sex ratio showed the same tendency, and the final model for within-pair sex ratio was even more significant (v 2 ¼ 6.76, p ¼.009). The final model (strophe bout length) and the initial model (all song variables, color variables, year, and the twoway interactions) did not differ significantly in change of variance (delta deviance ¼ 10.8, df ¼ 11, p ¼.46), showing that no other variable improved the final model. Figure 2 Strophe bout length at dawn of 1-year-old males from experimentally manipulated broods (normalized as ÿlog(1/x)), in relation to the manipulation undergone. The manipulation variable has three values: ÿ1 for decreased brood, 1 for increased brood, and 0 for nonexperimental brood with no manipulation. The males from nonmanipulated broods are a random selection of eight 1-year-old males in the population. Figure 3 Natural brood sex ratio in relation to the mean length of strophe bouts produced by the social male at dawn during the fertile period of the female (transformed as ÿlog(1/x)).

5 Dreiss et al. Offspring sex ratio, male song, and male coloration 17 The model performed with the principal components Ch1 and Ch2 gave a similar result. The final model contained the only variable Ch1 song performance (total sex ratio: v 2 ¼ 4.08, p ¼.043; within-pair sex ratio: v 2 ¼ 4.30, p ¼.038). Extrapair and within-pair sex ratio A total of 49% of the broods contained extrapair chicks. From a total of 850 chicks, 117 were extrapair young. There was no significant difference in sex ratio between within-pair and extrapair chicks (F ¼ 0.21, df ¼ 1, p ¼.65; sex ratio of withinpair offspring: ; sex ratio of extrapair offspring: ). DISCUSSION Strophe bout length (or another variable correlated with strophe bout length) was differentially affected by experimentally modifying rearing conditions in the nest, and observed strophe bout length of the attending male at a nest-box predicted offspring sex ratio. None of the other dawn song and crown color characters accounted for a significant amount of variance, raising the question why strophe bout length may be of more importance as a signal. The length of the strophe bout is supposed to reflect male quality as a short strophe bout is an indicator of neuromuscular tiredness (Carew, 2000). A male might be able to keep singing at a high rate by changing strophe types because each type requires a different use of the song apparatus (Lambrechts and Dhondt, 1988). Thus a male that produces long strophe bouts and that is capable of singing the same strophe a great number of times would reveal his endurance and his ability to perform without fatigue. Analyses of songs produced by the few males from sizemanipulated broods revealed that males from enlarged broods had shorter strophe bouts than males from reduced brood. Although the relation is not significant after Bonferroni correction, the variance explained is rather high (R 2. 40%). This experimental result suggests that nestling development may be linked to acquisition of song, especially strophe bout length. Brood size manipulation affected chick condition, which later may have consequences for access to resources. Consistent with this prediction, Nowicki et al. (2000) found a relationship between biometric measures of nestling great reed warblers (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) and their repertoire size during the first breeding season: well-fed young with a superior size displayed a more diversified song repertoire. Thus, male blue tits may signal their general condition and their ability to respond to developmental stress by producing long sequences of strophes during the dawn chorus. The observed link between brood sex ratio and male strophe bout length might suggest that females use this song characteristic as a cue reflecting the quality of their mate. A male-biased sex ratio in the brood was associated with longer strophe bouts during the dawn chorus. If sons inherit this trait, it may be advantageous for the female to adjust brood sex ratio toward males as sons would be of better quality or would enjoy increased mating success compared to sons produced by males with short strophe bouts (Ellegren et al., 1996; Sheldon et al., 1999). Although we have not tested whether females are able to detect differences in male strophe bout length, this trait has the largest variance among the song variables studied. Females thus have a large range of male phenotypes among which they could choose. Nevertheless, as this link is observational, other hypotheses could potentially explain the relation between brood sex ratio and male dawn chorus. Female blue tits may use other cues of male display than strophe bout length to assess mate quality, and such traits may be indirectly linked to this song variable. Environmental quality might also influence brood sex ratio. A territory with more food for offspring would improve their condition. Thus, parents would increase their fitness by selectively producing sons in a prime quality territory. For example, Appleby et al. (1997) found that the sex ratio of tawny owls (Strix aluco) was related to vole density in the territory. Territorial defense by male tits depends on song output (Krebs et al., 1978), and male singing performance may thus influence territory quality. Moreover, males producing long strophe bouts may be in better overall condition, allowing them to provide their mates with better or more food during the laying period and potentially also provide better or more food for their offspring. In this situation, females should bias their brood sex ratio toward sons because such chicks would be in better general condition. We did not find any influence of male UV plumage on sex ratio, contrary to studies by Griffith et al. (2003) and Sheldon et al. (1999). This may be due to the measurement methods as in these previous studies crown reflectance was determined on live birds while we measured reflectance on feathers. Moreover, Sheldon et al. (1999) measured the plumage during nest building when feathers may be less worn. Plumage may indeed deteriorate and fade during the reproductive period (Örnborg et al., 2002). In contradiction to this hypothetical explanation, Griffith et al. (2003) measured color during nest building and also during chick feeding and obtained similar results. Griffith et al. (2003) and Sheldon et al. (1999) focused on the same Swedish population of blue tits. Hence, the difference in the role of crown color may result from different selection pressures, for example, due to differences in environmental light conditions or due to cultural differences in use of traits among populations. Furthermore, we did not find any correlation between song and crown color features. Therefore, male song and color would represent two aspects of phenotypic quality and convey different types of information on male characteristics. Plumage color might be linked to male condition during molt that takes place several months before reproduction. We did not detect any significant difference in sex ratio between extrapair and within-pair young. This result is consistent with observations from other passerine studies (Saino et al., 1999; Sheldon and Ellegren, 1996; Westerdahl et al., 1997), although the power of these tests is low if the true effect size is small or intermediate. Sheldon and Ellegren (1996) suggested that females are unable to affect the sex ratio of specific eggs. However, if the physiological mechanism leading to sex ratio bias is linked to stimulation of the female by the song of her partner at dawn or to courtship feeding, any control by extrapair males must be limited. Because rearing conditions have a major influence on adult size and strophe bout length, male mates may be of much greater importance in influencing brood sex ratios than extrapair males. Although we found a relationship between strophe bout length and brood sex ratio, overall sex ratio in our population did not differ from binomial expectations. This apparent contradiction may suggest that females do not manipulate the sex ratio to a large extent. Although the mechanism remains unknown, the sex of chicks may be controlled by segregation of sex chromosomes (Ellegren et al., 1996), selective sorting of ova by the female (Emlen, 1997), or selective elimination of embryos of one sex. For the two last hypotheses, the costs in terms of invested resources, or delay in laying date, are obvious. However, we did not find any delay in laying date for broods with extreme sex ratios. If the female selectively eliminates embryos of one sex, there should be fewer eggs in more

6 18 Behavioral Ecology biased broods. However, that was not the case as we did not find a link between brood sex ratio bias and the number of unhatched eggs. This does not support the two latter mechanisms. Consequently, we concur with Charnov (1982) that as the sex ratio may depend on many complex factors, the imprecise estimation of different variables affecting fitness would favor small biases and few extreme brood sex ratios. Our data suggest a link between a component of male song and brood sex ratio in the blue tit. Previous studies of the same species have shown relationships between brood sex ratio and parental characteristics. Male survival may play a role in determining sex ratios (Griffith et al., 2003; Svensson and Nilsson, 1996; but see Leech et al., 2001), as may clutch size and breeding time (Griffith et al., 2003; but see Leech et al., 2001). These relationships are not always consistent in the same population in different years (Griffith et al., 2003), although consistency may occur within couples (Oddie and Reim, 2002). This suggests that it is important to determine the relative importance of these factors in determining brood sex ratio and the selection pressures that lead to such patterns. We thank the students for assistance with field work, especially Claire Loiseau, Lorien Pichegru, Nadia Silva, and Izabella Dworzynska. We also gratefully acknowledge Alain Jamet for his technical assistance. We would like to thank the French national forestry agency Office National des Forêts and the administration of the Parc Régional de la Forêt d Orient for their kind collaboration. The work was partly funded by the GDR2155 d Ecologie Comportementale. Ringing authorizations were given by the Centre de Recherches sur la Biologie des Populations d Oiseaux and manipulation authorization by the Direction Régionale de l Environnement. REFERENCES Andersson M, Sexual selection. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Andersson S, Örnborg J, Andersson M, Ultraviolet sexual dimorphism and assortative mating in blue tits. Proc R Soc Lond B 265: Appleby BM, Petty SJ, Blakey JK, Rainey P, MacDonald DW, Does variation of sex ratio enhance reproductive success of offspring in tawny owls (Strix aluco)? Proc R Soc Lond B 264: Bijnens L, Blue tit Parus caeruleus song in relation to survival, reproduction and biometry. Bird Study 35: Buchanan KL, Spencer KA, Goldsmith AR, Catchpole CK, Song as an honest signal of past developmental stress in the European Starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Proc R Soc Lond B 270: Carew TJ, Behavioral neurobiology. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates, Inc. Publishers. Charnov EL, The theory of sex allocation. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Cockburn A, Legge S, Double MC, Sex ratios in birds and mammals: can the hypotheses be disentangled? In: Sex ratios: concepts and research methods (Hardy ICW, ed). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; Dawson DA, Hanotte O, Greig C, Stewart IRK, Burke T, Polymorphic microsatellites in the blue tit Parus caeruleus and their cross-species utility in twenty songbird families. Mol Ecol 9: Delhey K, Johnsen A, Peters A, Andersson S, Kempenaers B, Paternity analysis reveals opposing selection pressures on crown coloration in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus). Proc R Soc Lond B 270: Doutrelant C, Blondel J, Perret P, Lambrechts MM, Blue tit song repertoire size, male quality and interspecific competition. J Avian Biol 31: Ellegren H, Gustafsson L, Sheldon BC, Sex ratio adjustment in relation to paternal attractiveness in a wild bird population. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93: Emlen ST, When mothers prefer daughters over sons. Trends Ecol Evol 12: Endler JA, On the measurement and classification of color in studies of animal color patterns. Biol J Linn Soc 41: Ewen JG, Cassey P, Møller AP, Facultative primary sex ratio variation: a lack of evidence in birds? Proc R Soc Lond B 271: Fiala KL, On estimating the primary sex ratio from incomplete data. Am Nat 115: Gil D, Gahr M, The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. Trends Ecol Evol 17: Griffith SC, Örnborg J, Russell AF, Andersson S, Sheldon BC, Correlations between ultraviolet coloration, overwinter survival and offspring sex-ratio in the blue tit. J Evol Biol 16: Griffiths R, Double MC, Orr K, Dawson RJG, A DNA test to sex most birds. Mol Ecol 7: Gustafsson L, Qvarnström A, Sheldon BC, Trade-offs between life-history traits and a secondary sexual character in male collared flycatchers. Nature 375: Holm S, A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat 6: Hunt S, Bennett ATD, Cuthill IC, Griffiths R, Blue tits are ultraviolet tits. Proc R Soc Lond B 265: Kempenaers B, Verheyen GR, Dhondt AA, Extrapair paternity in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus): female choice, male characteristics, and offspring quality. Behav Ecol 8: Kempenaers B, Verheyen GR, Vandenbroeck M, Burke T, Vanbroeckhoven C, Dhondt AA, Extra-pair paternity results from female preference for high quality males in the blue tit. Nature 357: Kölliker M, Heeb P, Werner I, Mateman AC, Lessells CM, Richner H, Offspring sex ratio is related to male body size in the great tit (Parus major). Behav Ecol 10: Krebs JR, Ashcroft R, Webber M, Song repertoires and territory defence in the Great tit. Nature 271: Lambrechts MM, Dhondt AA, The anti-exhaustion hypothesis: a new hypothesis to explain song performance and song switching in the great tit. Anim Behav 36: Leech DI, Hartley IR, Stewart IRK, Griffith SC, Burke T, No effect of parental quality or extrapair paternity on brood sex ratio in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus). Behav Ecol 12: Mace R, The dawn chorus in the great tit is directly related to female fertility. Nature 330: Møller AP, Male ornament size as a reliable cue to enhance offspring viability in the barn swallow. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 91: Møller AP, Sexual selection in the barn swallow (Hirundo rustica). V. Geographic variation in ornament size. J Evol Biol 8: Nager RG, Monaghan P, Griffiths R, Houston DC, Dawson R, Experimental demonstration that offspring sex ratio varies with maternal condition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96: Nowicki S, Hasselquist D, Bensch S, Peters S, Nestling growth and song repertoire size in great reed warblers: evidence for song learning as an indicator mechanism in mate choice. Proc R Soc Lond B 267: Oddie KR, Reim C, Egg sex ratio and paternal traits: using within-individual comparisons. Behav Ecol 13: Örnborg J, Andersson S, Griffith SC, Sheldon BC, Seasonal changes in a ultraviolet structural colour signal in blue tits, Parus caeruleus. Biol J Linn Soc 76: Poesel A, Kempenaers B, When a bird is tired from singing: a study of drift during the dawn chorus. Etologia 8:1 7. Saino N, Ambrosini R, Martinelli R, Pilastro A, Møller AP, Sexallocation in relation to parental age and paternal ornamentation of barn swallows. Mol Ecol 11: Saino N, Ellegren H, Møller AP, No evidence for adjustment of sex allocation in relation to paternal ornamentation and paternity in barn swallows. Mol Ecol 8: SAS, Version 8. Cary, North Carolina: SAS Institute Inc. Sheldon BC, Sex allocation: at the females whim. Curr Biol 9:R487 R489. Sheldon BC, Andersson S, Griffith SC, Örnborg J, Sendecka J, Ultraviolet colour variation influences blue tit sex ratios. Nature 402:

7 Dreiss et al. Offspring sex ratio, male song, and male coloration 19 Sheldon BC, Ellegren H, Offspring sex and paternity in the collared flycatcher. Proc R Soc Lond B 263: Sheldon BC, Merilä J, Qvarnström A, Gustafsson L, Ellegren H, Paternal genetic contribution to offspring condition predicted by size of male secondary sexual character. Proc R Soc Lond B 264: Spencer KA, Buchanan KL, Goldsmith AR, Catchpole CK, Developmental stress, social rank and song complexity in the European Starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Proc R Soc Lond B 271:S121 S123. Svensson E, Nilsson JA, Mate quality affects offspring sex ratio in blue tits. Proc R Soc Lond B 263: Théry M, Debut M, Gomez D, Casas J, Specific color sensitivities of prey and predator explain camouflage in different visual systems. Behav Ecol 16: Trivers RL, Willard DE, Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science 179: Westerdahl H, Bensch S, Hansson B, Hasselquist D, von Schantz T, Sex ratio variation among broods of great reed warblers Acrocephalus arundinaceus. Mol Ecol 6: Wilson K, Hardy ICW, Statistical analysis of sex ratios: an introduction. In: Sex ratios: concepts and research methods (Hardy ICW, ed). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press;

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen Primary sex ratio adjustment to experimentally reduced male UV attractiveness in blue tits Korsten, Peter; Lessells, CM; Mateman, AC; van der Velde, Marco; Komdeur, Jan Published

More information

Sex ratio adjustment in relation to paternal attractiveness in a

Sex ratio adjustment in relation to paternal attractiveness in a Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 11723-11728, October 1996 Evolution Sex ratio adjustment in relation to paternal attractiveness in a wild bird population (sexual selection/sex allocation/ficedula

More information

Offspring sexual dimorphism and sex-allocation in. relation to parental age and paternal ornamentation in the barn swallow

Offspring sexual dimorphism and sex-allocation in. relation to parental age and paternal ornamentation in the barn swallow Molecular Ecology (2002) 11, 15 1544 Offspring sexual dimorphism and sex-allocation in Blackwell Science, Ltd relation to parental age and paternal ornamentation in the barn swallow N. SAINO,* R. AMBROSINI,*

More information

Numerous studies of birds have shown that females copulate

Numerous studies of birds have shown that females copulate Experimental analysis of sperm competition mechanisms in a wild bird population Gábor Michl*, János Török*, Simon C. Griffith, and Ben C. Sheldon *Behavioural Ecology Group, Department of Systematic Zoology

More information

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences Males and females have the same genes. Why do the sexes often look and act so differently? Why is the male often insanely ornamented? (Or simply insane?)

More information

Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection

Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection Why study mating systems? What are the ecological conditions under which different mating systems arise? Implications for evolution mating is where genes are passed

More information

Bird song and parasites

Bird song and parasites Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2005) 59: 167 180 DOI 10.1007/s00265-005-0041-8 REVIEW László Zsolt Garamszegi Bird song and parasites Received: 29 June 2004 / Revised: 4 July 2005 / Accepted: 5 July 2005 / Published

More information

Fisher (1958) proposed that frequency-dependent selection

Fisher (1958) proposed that frequency-dependent selection Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 3: 294 298 Is variation in brood sex ratios adaptive in the great tit (Parus major)? A. N. Radford and J. K. Blakey Edward Grey Institute of Field Ornithology, Department

More information

Interval between clutches, fitness, and climate change

Interval between clutches, fitness, and climate change Interval between clutches, fitness, and climate change Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arl051 Advance Access publication 29 September 2006 Anders Pape Møller Laboratoire de Parasitologie Evolutive,

More information

Ultraviolet reflectance affects male-male interactions in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus ultramarinus)

Ultraviolet reflectance affects male-male interactions in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus ultramarinus) Ultraviolet reflectance affects male-male interactions in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus ultramarinus) Behavioral Ecology Vol. 15 No. 5: 805 809 doi:10.1093/beheco/arh083 Advance Access publication on June

More information

Sex ratio bias in the dung beetle Onthophagus taurus: adaptive allocation or sex-specific offspring mortality?

Sex ratio bias in the dung beetle Onthophagus taurus: adaptive allocation or sex-specific offspring mortality? Evol Ecol (2011) 25:363 372 DOI 10.1007/s10682-010-9423-0 RESEARCH ARTICLE Sex ratio bias in the dung beetle Onthophagus taurus: adaptive allocation or sex-specific offspring mortality? Clarissa M. House

More information

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen Mutual ornamentation, age, and reproductive performance in the European starling Komdeur, Jan; Oorebeek, M; van Overveld, T; Cuthill, IC; Cuthill, Innes C. Published in: Behavioral

More information

Testing Sensory Bias. Why are females choosy? Sensory Bias. Sensory bias. 1. Direct benefits. 2. Runaway sexual selection. 3.

Testing Sensory Bias. Why are females choosy? Sensory Bias. Sensory bias. 1. Direct benefits. 2. Runaway sexual selection. 3. Why are females choosy? 1. Direct benefits 2. Runaway sexual selection 3. Sensory bias 4. Good genes Sensory Bias 1. Runaway sexual selection predicts females can have preferences for male traits that

More information

April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice. Female choice

April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice. Female choice April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice Female choice In general, females are expected to be choosier than males because they have more to lose by making bad mating decisions Costs and Benefits: Possible

More information

Ethology. Hansjoerg P. Kunc*, Katharina Foerster, Etiënne L. M. Vermeirssen & Bart Kempenaers. Abstract

Ethology. Hansjoerg P. Kunc*, Katharina Foerster, Etiënne L. M. Vermeirssen & Bart Kempenaers. Abstract Ethology Experimentally Elevated Plasma Testosterone Levels Do Not Influence Singing Behaviour of Male Blue Tits (Parus caeruleus) During the Early Breeding Season Hansjoerg P. Kunc*, Katharina Foerster,

More information

Female choice in the sedge warbler, Acrocephalus schoenobaenus: multiple cues from song and territory quality

Female choice in the sedge warbler, Acrocephalus schoenobaenus: multiple cues from song and territory quality Female choice in the sedge warbler, Acrocephalus schoenobaenus: multiple cues from song and territory quality KATHERINE L. BUCHANAN AND CLIVE K. CATCHPOLE* School of Biological Sciences, Ro al Hollo a,

More information

Combined influence of maternal and paternal quality on sex allocation in red-capped robins

Combined influence of maternal and paternal quality on sex allocation in red-capped robins doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2005.01017.x Combined influence of maternal and paternal quality on sex allocation in red-capped robins D. K. DOWLING & R. A. MULDER Department of Zoology, University of Melbourne,

More information

In birds with biparental care, great variation exists in the

In birds with biparental care, great variation exists in the Behavioral Ecology Vol. 12 No. 4: 412 418 Do female pied flycatchers seek extrapair copulations with familiar males? A test of the incomplete knowledge hypothesis Tore Slagsvold, a Arild Johnsen, b Helene

More information

Adaptation and Optimality Theory

Adaptation and Optimality Theory Adaptation and Optimality Theory Prisoner s Dilemma game: An optimality model What is the optimal strategy when you are playing with different people of unknown reputation? Defect: We call this the evolutionarily

More information

Absolute and relative signals: a comparison between melanin- and carotenoid-based patches

Absolute and relative signals: a comparison between melanin- and carotenoid-based patches Absolute and relative signals: a comparison between melanin- and carotenoid-based patches J.C. Senar 1) & J. Quesada (Behavioural and Evolutionary Ecology Associate Research Unit (CSIC), Museu de Ciències

More information

Rafał Martyka 1*, Ewa B. Śliwińska 1, Mirosław Martyka 1, Mariusz Cichoń 2 and Piotr Tryjanowski 3

Rafał Martyka 1*, Ewa B. Śliwińska 1, Mirosław Martyka 1, Mariusz Cichoń 2 and Piotr Tryjanowski 3 Martyka et al. Frontiers in Zoology (2018) 15:25 https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-018-0272-y RESEARCH The effect of pre-laying maternal immunization on offspring growth and immunity differs across experimentally

More information

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise...

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise... 1. Female choice of males that have exaggerated characters has been explained by female choice of males that have better genes. Explain how female choice of higher quality males might lead to exaggerated

More information

Early birds are sexy: male age, dawn song and extrapair paternity in blue tits, Cyanistes (formerly Parus) caeruleus

Early birds are sexy: male age, dawn song and extrapair paternity in blue tits, Cyanistes (formerly Parus) caeruleus ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2006, 72, 531e538 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.022 Early birds are sexy: male age, dawn song and extrapair paternity in blue tits, Cyanistes (formerly Parus) caeruleus ANGELIKA POESEL*,

More information

The design of complex sexual traits in male barn swallows: associations between signal attributes

The design of complex sexual traits in male barn swallows: associations between signal attributes doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01135.x The design of complex sexual traits in male barn swallows: associations between signal attributes L. Z. GARAMSZEGI,* G. HEGYI, D. HEYLEN,* P. NINNI,à F. DE LOPE, M.

More information

Within-clutch variation in offspring sex determined by differences in sire body size: cryptic mate choice in the wild

Within-clutch variation in offspring sex determined by differences in sire body size: cryptic mate choice in the wild doi: 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00665.x SHORT COMMUNICATION Within-clutch variation in offspring sex determined by differences in sire body size: cryptic mate choice in the wild RYAN CALSBEEK* & BARRY SINERVO

More information

No evidence for differential maternal allocation to offspring in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus)

No evidence for differential maternal allocation to offspring in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus) Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 3: 340 346 No evidence for differential maternal allocation to offspring in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus) Jérémy Mazuc, a,b Olivier Chastel, b and Gabriele Sorci

More information

Seasonal variation in dawn song characteristics in the common nightingale

Seasonal variation in dawn song characteristics in the common nightingale ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2005, 70, 1265 1271 doi:.16/j.anbehav.2005.02.0 Seasonal variation in dawn song characteristics in the common nightingale HANSJOERG P. KUNC*, VALENTINAMRHEIN &MARCNAGUIB* *Department

More information

Song repertoire size predicts initial mating success in male song sparrows, Melospiza melodia

Song repertoire size predicts initial mating success in male song sparrows, Melospiza melodia ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 24, 68, 155 163 doi:1.116/j.anbehav.24.7.3 Song repertoire size predicts initial mating success in male song sparrows, Melospiza melodia JANE M. REID*, PETER ARCESE, ALICE L. E. V. CASSIDY,

More information

The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus

The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus Naturwissenschaften (2003) 90:577-582 DOI 10.1007/s00114-003-0479-3 SHORT COMMUNICATION Daniel J. Mennill Peter T. Boag Laurene M. Ratcliffe The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female black-capped

More information

Sex allocation and parental quality in tree swallows

Sex allocation and parental quality in tree swallows Sex allocation and parental quality in tree swallows Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arn081 Advance Access publication 4 August 2008 Kira E. Delmore, a Oddmund Kleven, b Terje Laskemoen, b Susan

More information

Experimental manipulation of tail length in female barn swallows (Hirundo rustica) affects their future reproductive success

Experimental manipulation of tail length in female barn swallows (Hirundo rustica) affects their future reproductive success Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 4: 451 456 Experimental manipulation of tail length in female barn swallows (Hirundo rustica) affects their future reproductive success José Javier Cuervo, a Anders Pape

More information

Against the odds? Nestling sex ratio variation in green-rumped parrotlets

Against the odds? Nestling sex ratio variation in green-rumped parrotlets Against the odds? Nestling sex ratio variation in green-rumped parrotlets Behavioral Ecology Vol. 15 No. 4: 607 613 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arh052 Amber E. Budden and Steven R. Beissinger Ecosystem Sciences

More information

Parental Feeding Rates in the House Sparrow, Passer domesticus: Are Larger-Badged Males Better Fathers?

Parental Feeding Rates in the House Sparrow, Passer domesticus: Are Larger-Badged Males Better Fathers? Ethology 108, 1011 1022 (2002) Ó 2002 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin ISSN 0179 1613 Parental Feeding Rates in the House Sparrow, Passer domesticus: Are Larger-Badged Males Better Fathers? Karen M. Voltura, P.

More information

Sexual selection. Intrasexual selection mating success determined by within-sex interactions e.g., male-male combat

Sexual selection. Intrasexual selection mating success determined by within-sex interactions e.g., male-male combat Sexual dimorphism Sexual selection Is sexual selection different from natural selection? Darwin saw them as distinct - only sexual selection could produce traits that compromise survival The basic principle

More information

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30)

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) FILM SESSIONS Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) 1. Talking to Strangers (60 min.) 1 This film surveys some of the general communication signals used by animals from diverse taxa,

More information

UNIT 9. PARENTAL CARE AND MATING SYSTEMS

UNIT 9. PARENTAL CARE AND MATING SYSTEMS UNIT 9. PARENTAL CARE AND MATING SYSTEMS SOURCES (for powerpoint format: http://wfsc.tamu.edu/jpackard/behavior/wfsc622/powerpoints.zip) required: Chapter 9 in Krebs & Davies (1993:208-243) remedial: "Rearing

More information

Female coloration indicates female reproductive capacity in blue tits

Female coloration indicates female reproductive capacity in blue tits doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01451.x Female coloration indicates female reproductive capacity in blue tits C. DOUTRELANT,*A.GRÉGOIRE,* N. GRNAC,* D. GOMEZ, M. M. LAMBRECHTS* & P. PERRET* *CEFE CNRS, 1919

More information

University of Groningen. The illusion of monogamy Bouwman, Karen Marian

University of Groningen. The illusion of monogamy Bouwman, Karen Marian University of Groningen The illusion of monogamy Bouwman, Karen Marian IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the

More information

Sexual selection and blue tit (Parus caeruleus) crown coloration

Sexual selection and blue tit (Parus caeruleus) crown coloration Sexual selection and blue tit (Parus caeruleus) crown coloration DISSERTATION der Fakultät für Biologie der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München vorgelegt von Kaspar Delhey durchgeführt am Max Planck

More information

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 1 Outline causes sexual selection vs. natural selection sexual selection during courtship male-male

More information

Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens. The following table shows the percentage of male fairy-wrens in various age and social status categories.

Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens. The following table shows the percentage of male fairy-wrens in various age and social status categories. Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens Superb fairy-wrens are small (10g) insectivorous birds found in woodlands and edge habitat throughout eastern Australia. They live in cooperative social groups composed

More information

The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction

The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction I. Introduction to Behavioral Ecology Behavioral ecologists test hypotheses about the adaptive function of a behavior Behavioral Ecology Behavioral ecologists

More information

Experimental and natural changes in the peacock's (Pavo cristatus} train can affect mating success

Experimental and natural changes in the peacock's (Pavo cristatus} train can affect mating success Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1994) 35:213-217 Springer-Verlag 1994 Marion Petrie - Tim Halliday Experimental and natural changes in the peacock's (Pavo cristatus} train can affect mating success Received: 18

More information

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen Is extrapair mating random? On the probability distribution of extrapair young in avian broods Brommer, Jon E.; Korsten, Peter; Bouwman, Karen A.; Berg, Mathew L.; Komdeur, Jan

More information

Animal Behavior. Thur March 24: Animal Behavior: outline. Questions: What, How, and Why. Behavior is shaped partly by inheritance

Animal Behavior. Thur March 24: Animal Behavior: outline. Questions: What, How, and Why. Behavior is shaped partly by inheritance Thur March 24: Animal Behavior Animal Behavior: outline Animal Behavior Questions Behavior is partly shaped by Inheritance Behavior is partly shaped by Learning Hormones and Behavior The Genetics of Behavior

More information

Reproduction. Chapter 7

Reproduction. Chapter 7 Reproduction Chapter 7 Sexual Selection Darwin's theory to explain traits that aren't obviously advantageous https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ll30qttsz9u Bower Birds Bower Birds She builds the nest, cares

More information

Sexual selection. 1) Sexual dimorphism. 2) Variation in mating success. 3) Sexual selection. 4) Female choice based on male ornaments

Sexual selection. 1) Sexual dimorphism. 2) Variation in mating success. 3) Sexual selection. 4) Female choice based on male ornaments Sexual selection 1) Sexual dimorphism 2) Variation in mating success 3) Sexual selection 4) Female choice based on male ornaments 5) The evolution of female preference 1) Sexual dimorphism http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/file:descent_of_man_-_figure_16.jpg

More information

Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Classification

Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Classification Introduction 1 Introduction 2 Individuals rarely mate at random for a number of reasons: Dispersal may be limited Individuals may or may not be able to self Individuals may reproduce asexually Individuals

More information

Beauty alone is insufficient: female mate choice in the barn swallow

Beauty alone is insufficient: female mate choice in the barn swallow Ecol Res (2018) 33: 3 16 DOI 10.1007/s11284-017-1527-3 SUZUKI AWARD Masaru Hasegawa Beauty alone is insufficient: female mate choice in the barn swallow Received: 16 September 2017 / Accepted: 27 October

More information

BROOD SEX RATIO VARIATION IN MOUNTAIN BLUEBIRDS (SIALIA CURRUCOIDES): EFFECTS OF ATTRACTIVENESS AND ENVIRONMENT

BROOD SEX RATIO VARIATION IN MOUNTAIN BLUEBIRDS (SIALIA CURRUCOIDES): EFFECTS OF ATTRACTIVENESS AND ENVIRONMENT Faculty of Science BROOD SEX RATIO VARIATION IN MOUNTAIN BLUEBIRDS (SIALIA CURRUCOIDES): EFFECTS OF ATTRACTIVENESS AND ENVIRONMENT 2015 ERICA S. BONDERUD B.Sc. Honours thesis BROOD SEX RATIO VARIATION

More information

Spatial patterns of extra-pair paternity: beyond paternity gains and losses

Spatial patterns of extra-pair paternity: beyond paternity gains and losses Journal of Animal Ecology 2015, 84, 518 531 doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12293 Spatial patterns of extra-pair paternity: beyond paternity gains and losses Lotte Schlicht*, Mihai Valcu and Bart Kempenaers Department

More information

November 4, 2009 Bioe 109 Fall 2009 Lecture 17 The evolution of mating systems. The evolution of sex ratio

November 4, 2009 Bioe 109 Fall 2009 Lecture 17 The evolution of mating systems. The evolution of sex ratio November 4, 2009 Bioe 109 Fall 2009 Lecture 17 The evolution of mating systems The evolution of sex ratio - let us define sex ratio as the proportion of males to females. - in discussing the evolution

More information

The effect of breeding density and male quality on paternity-assurance behaviours in the house sparrow, Passer domesticus

The effect of breeding density and male quality on paternity-assurance behaviours in the house sparrow, Passer domesticus DOI 10.1007/s10164-010-0217-1 ARTICLE The effect of breeding density and male quality on paternity-assurance behaviours in the house sparrow, Passer domesticus Herbert Hoi Hans Tost Matteo Griggio Received:

More information

Adaptive seasonal trend in brood sex ratio: test in two sister species with contrasting breeding systems

Adaptive seasonal trend in brood sex ratio: test in two sister species with contrasting breeding systems Adaptive seasonal trend in brood sex ratio: test in two sister species with contrasting breeding systems M. ANDERSSON,* J. WALLANDER,* L. ORING, E. AKST,à J. M. REED, &R.C.FLEISCHERà *Department of Zoology,

More information

Persistent sex-by-environment effects on offspring fitness and sex-ratio adjustment in a wild bird population

Persistent sex-by-environment effects on offspring fitness and sex-ratio adjustment in a wild bird population Journal of Animal Ecology 2015, 84, 473 486 doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12294 Persistent sex-by-environment effects on offspring fitness and sex-ratio adjustment in a wild bird population E. Keith Bowers*,

More information

Male reed buntings do not adjust parental effort in relation to extrapair paternity

Male reed buntings do not adjust parental effort in relation to extrapair paternity Behavioral Ecology Advance Access published February 16, 2005 Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/ari021 Male reed buntings do not adjust parental effort in relation to extrapair paternity Karen M. Bouwman,

More information

1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts)

1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts) Zoology 357 - Evolutionary Ecology - First Exam 1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts) b. Name two non-selectionist

More information

What is sex? What about asexual reproduction? What is sexual reproduction?

What is sex? What about asexual reproduction? What is sexual reproduction? BIOL2007 EVOLUTION OF SEX AND SEXUAL SELECTION Kanchon Dasmahapatra EVOLUTIONARY QUESTIONS ABOUT SEX Today: A) The evolution of sex 1) What is the advantage of sex? B) Ev. of sexual dimorphism - sexual

More information

Carotenoid availability in diet and phenotype of blue and great tit nestlings

Carotenoid availability in diet and phenotype of blue and great tit nestlings 14 The Journal of Experimental Biology 29, 14-115 Published by The Company of Biologists 26 doi:1.1242/jeb.289 Carotenoid availability in diet and phenotype of blue and great tit nestlings Clotilde Biard

More information

SYSTEM 1. Carmen Bessa-Gomes (a,*), Stéphane Legendre (b), Jean Clobert (a), Anders Pape Møller (c)

SYSTEM 1. Carmen Bessa-Gomes (a,*), Stéphane Legendre (b), Jean Clobert (a), Anders Pape Møller (c) MODELING MATING PATTERNS GIVEN MUTUAL MATE CHOICE: THE IMPORTANCE OF INDIVIDUAL MATING PREFERENCES AND MATING SYSTEM 1 Carmen Bessa-Gomes (a,*), Stéphane Legendre (b), Jean Clobert (a), Anders Pape Møller

More information

INBREEDING IN THE SEYCHELLES WARBLER: ENVIRONMENT-DEPENDENT MATERNAL EFFECTS

INBREEDING IN THE SEYCHELLES WARBLER: ENVIRONMENT-DEPENDENT MATERNAL EFFECTS Evolution, 58(9), 2004, pp. 2037 2048 INBREEDING IN THE SEYCHELLES WARBLER: ENVIRONMENT-DEPENDENT MATERNAL EFFECTS DAVID S. RICHARDSON, JAN KOMDEUR, 2 AND TERRY BURKE 3 Centre for Ecology, Evolution and

More information

Anti-predator behavior: Group defense - Many eyes - Selfish herd - Dilution effects - Tonic immobility. Dispersal:

Anti-predator behavior: Group defense - Many eyes - Selfish herd - Dilution effects - Tonic immobility. Dispersal: Note: These are just the topics that have been covered since Exam 2. The exams are cumulative, so all the material that we have covered in this course can be included in the Final. However, there will

More information

Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety

Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 277 283 Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety Mark W. Blows, 1 * David Berrigan 2,3 and George W. Gilchrist 3 1 Department of Zoology,

More information

Evolution of Mating Systems. Chapter 8

Evolution of Mating Systems. Chapter 8 Evolution of Mating Systems Chapter 8 Mating Systems-Chapter 8 1 Monogamy 2 Polyandry 3 Polygyny And the many combinations within! Why should a male be monogamous? 1 extension of guarding, little chance

More information

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection Intrasexual Natural Selection Sexual Selection Survival Mating Success Parental Care Intrasexual

More information

Cost/benefit approach

Cost/benefit approach Cost/benefit approach Care FEMALE Abandon MALE Care F: wp 2 WP 1 M: wp 2 WP 1 Abandon F: wp 1 WP 0 M: wp 1 (1+p M ) WP 0 (1+p M ) P 0,1,2 = probability eggs survive given 0, 1, or 2 parents W, w = eggs

More information

Mating systems and parental investment. Mating systems. Resource distribution. Polygyny. Pattern of matings in a population. Antithesis = promiscuity

Mating systems and parental investment. Mating systems. Resource distribution. Polygyny. Pattern of matings in a population. Antithesis = promiscuity 1 Mating systems and parental investment Mating systems Pattern of matings in a population green anole Antithesis = promiscuity Polygyny Scramble: no attempts to defend females, resources horseshoe crabs

More information

Sexual dimorphism in house sparrow eggs

Sexual dimorphism in house sparrow eggs Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2000) 48:353 357 Springer-Verlag 2000 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Pedro J. Cordero Simon C. Griffith José M. Aparicio David T. Parkin Sexual dimorphism in house sparrow eggs Received: 19 January

More information

Ch. 23 The Evolution of Populations

Ch. 23 The Evolution of Populations Ch. 23 The Evolution of Populations 1 Essential question: Do populations evolve? 2 Mutation and Sexual reproduction produce genetic variation that makes evolution possible What is the smallest unit of

More information

Female brood desertion increases with number of available mates in the Rock Sparrow

Female brood desertion increases with number of available mates in the Rock Sparrow JOURNAL OF AVIAN BIOLOGY 32: 68 72. Copenhagen 2001 Female brood desertion increases with number of available mates in the Rock Sparrow Andrea Pilastro, Luca Biddau, Guglielmo Marin and Toni Mingozzi Pilastro,

More information

Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs. Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs

Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs. Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs Female Choice in Gray Tree Frogs Research by Gerhardt et al. (1996). Males call to attract females. Males call to attract females Ribbet! Figure 11.27 Tiny and super cute!

More information

Multiple sexual ornaments in satin bowerbirds: ultraviolet plumage and bowers signal different aspects of male quality

Multiple sexual ornaments in satin bowerbirds: ultraviolet plumage and bowers signal different aspects of male quality Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 4: 503 509 Multiple sexual ornaments in satin bowerbirds: ultraviolet plumage and bowers signal different aspects of male quality Stéphanie M. Doucet and Robert Montgomerie

More information

THE EVOLUTION OF INDICATOR TRAITS FOR PARENTAL QUALITY: THE ROLE OF MATERNAL AND PATERNAL EFFECTS

THE EVOLUTION OF INDICATOR TRAITS FOR PARENTAL QUALITY: THE ROLE OF MATERNAL AND PATERNAL EFFECTS Vol. 150, No. 5 The American Naturalist November 1997 THE EVOLUTION OF INDICATOR TRAITS FOR PARENTAL QUALITY: THE ROLE OF MATERNAL AND PATERNAL EFFECTS Jason B. Wolf, 1, *, Allen J. Moore, 2 and Edmund

More information

Maternal correlates of brood sex ratio variation in the lekking lance-tailed manakin Chiroxiphia lanceolata

Maternal correlates of brood sex ratio variation in the lekking lance-tailed manakin Chiroxiphia lanceolata J. Avian Biol. 39: 198205, 2008 doi: 10.1111/j.2008.0908-8857.04165.x # 2008 The Authors. J. Compilation # 2008 J. Avian Biol. Received 15 January 2007, accepted 26 July 2007 Maternal correlates of brood

More information

REPORT Fisherian and ``good genes'' benefits of mate choice: how (not) to distinguish between them

REPORT Fisherian and ``good genes'' benefits of mate choice: how (not) to distinguish between them Ecology Letters, (2001) 4 : 322±326 REPORT Fisherian and ``good genes'' benefits of mate choice: how (not) to distinguish between them Hanna Kokko Division of Environmental and Evolutionary Biology, Institute

More information

Received: 16 July 2003 / Revised: 6 January 2004 / Accepted: 8 January 2004 / Published online: 27 February 2004 Springer-Verlag 2004

Received: 16 July 2003 / Revised: 6 January 2004 / Accepted: 8 January 2004 / Published online: 27 February 2004 Springer-Verlag 2004 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2004) 56:149 154 DOI 10.1007/s00265-004-0756-y ORIGINAL ARTICLE Claire Doutrelant Rita Covas Alain Caizergues MornØ du Plessis Unexpected sex ratio adjustment in a colonial cooperative

More information

Extra-pair paternity, offspring mortality and offspring sex ratio in the socially monogamous coal tit (Parus ater)

Extra-pair paternity, offspring mortality and offspring sex ratio in the socially monogamous coal tit (Parus ater) Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2006) 60: 563 571 DOI 10.1007/s00265-006-0201-5 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Verena Dietrich-Bischoff. Tim Schmoll. Wolfgang Winkel. Sven Krackow. Thomas Lubjuhn Extra-pair paternity, offspring

More information

Kin Selection and Family Conflicts

Kin Selection and Family Conflicts The Evolution of Sex and its Consequences HS 2011 Part II: Consequences Kin Selection and Family Conflicts Kin Selection: Altruism and Conflict Blue tit (Parus caeruleus) Burying beetle (Nicrophorus vespilloides)

More information

Tawny Owls Strix aluco with reliable food supply produce male-biased broods

Tawny Owls Strix aluco with reliable food supply produce male-biased broods Ibis (2007), 149, 98 105 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Tawny Owls Strix aluco with reliable food supply produce male-biased broods KASI B. DESFOR, JACOBUS J. BOOMSMA & PETER SUNDE* Department of Population

More information

Ibis (2002), 144, *Corresponding author. Estación Experimental de Zonas Aridas, CSIC, Almería, Spain.

Ibis (2002), 144, *Corresponding author. Estación Experimental de Zonas Aridas, CSIC, Almería, Spain. Ibis (2002), 144, 236 247 Blackwell Science Ltd Extrapair paternity in the Hoopoe Upupa epops: an exploration of the influence of interactions between breeding pairs, non-pair males and strophe length

More information

3/26/ Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy. 1. Fecundity

3/26/ Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy. 1. Fecundity Sexual Dimorphism 1. Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy A. sexual reproduction involving the fusion of two dissimilar gametes;individuals producing the

More information

Manipulating the perceived opportunity to cheat: an experimental test of the active roles of male and female zebra finches in mate guarding behavior

Manipulating the perceived opportunity to cheat: an experimental test of the active roles of male and female zebra finches in mate guarding behavior DOI 10.1007/s00265-013-1532-7 ORIGINAL PAPER Manipulating the perceived opportunity to cheat: an experimental test of the active roles of male and female zebra finches in mate guarding behavior Leah C.

More information

Reproductive investment and parasite susceptibility in the Great Tit

Reproductive investment and parasite susceptibility in the Great Tit Functional Ecology 1997 Reproductive investment and parasite susceptibility in the Great Tit K. ALLANDER* Department of Zoology, Villavägen 9, S-752 36 Uppsala, Sweden Summary 1. Reproduction and parasite

More information

AN EQUITABLE MARRIAGE: A FOCAL STUDY OF A BARN SWALLOW (HIRUNDO RUSTICA) NEST ON LUNDY

AN EQUITABLE MARRIAGE: A FOCAL STUDY OF A BARN SWALLOW (HIRUNDO RUSTICA) NEST ON LUNDY AN EQUITABLE MARRIAGE: A FOCAL STUDY OF A BARN SWALLOW (HIRUNDO RUSTICA) NEST ON LUNDY by THOMAS E. DICKINS Department of Psychology, School of Science and Technology, Middlesex University, London, NW4

More information

Genetic basis of inheritance and variation. Dr. Amjad Mahasneh. Jordan University of Science and Technology

Genetic basis of inheritance and variation. Dr. Amjad Mahasneh. Jordan University of Science and Technology Genetic basis of inheritance and variation Dr. Amjad Mahasneh Jordan University of Science and Technology Segment 1 Hello and welcome everyone. My name is Amjad Mahasneh. I teach molecular biology at Jordan

More information

Lecture K3- Behavioral Ecology Dr. Kopeny

Lecture K3- Behavioral Ecology Dr. Kopeny 4/17 Included on Test #4! Lecture K3- Behavioral Ecology Dr. Kopeny Mates, Families and Societies Male Prairie Chickens on Lek (Booming Ground) displaying male Prairie Chicken two male Prairie Chickens

More information

The natural plasma testosterone profile of male blue tits during the breeding season and its relation to song output

The natural plasma testosterone profile of male blue tits during the breeding season and its relation to song output JOURNAL OF AVIAN BIOLOGY 33: 269 275, 2002 The natural plasma testosterone profile of male blue tits during the breeding season and its relation to song output Katharina Foerster, Angelika Poesel, Hansjörg

More information

Sex-Biased Dispersal: A Result of a Sex Difference in Breeding Site Availability

Sex-Biased Dispersal: A Result of a Sex Difference in Breeding Site Availability vol. 171, no. 6 the american naturalist june 008 Sex-Biased Dispersal: A Result of a Sex Difference in Breeding Site Availability Debora Arlt * and Tomas Pärt Department of Ecology, Swedish University

More information

UV-COLOURATION: SEXUAL SELECTION AND

UV-COLOURATION: SEXUAL SELECTION AND Theses of the dissertation UV-COLOURATION: SEXUAL SELECTION AND MATERNAL INVESTMENT IN EUROPEAN GREEN LIZARD (LACERTA VIRIDIS) Katalin Bajer Department of Systematic Zoology and Ecology Eötvös Loránd University

More information

Polygyny in animals From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Polygyny in animals From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Page 1 of 6 Polygyny in animals From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Polygyny (/pəˈlɪdʒɪniː/; from Neo-Greek πολυγυνία from πολύ- poly- "many", and γυνή gyne "woman" or "wife") [1] is a mating system

More information

EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly.

EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly. Name: 1 NAME: EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly. 1) Describe the following concepts in a few sentences (2 points each)

More information

Lecture Outline. Darwin s Theory of Natural Selection. Modern Theory of Natural Selection. Changes in frequencies of alleles

Lecture Outline. Darwin s Theory of Natural Selection. Modern Theory of Natural Selection. Changes in frequencies of alleles 1. Basics of Natural Selection Lecture Outline 2. How to test for the key components of natural selection a. Variation b. Heritability c. Can the trait respond to selection? d. What are the selective forces?

More information

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli Lecture 19: Animal Behavior I. Background A. Animal behavior reflects and arises from biological properties 1. Exhibited behavior defends on the physiological systems and processes unique to a given organism

More information

Effect of act on fitness of recipient

Effect of act on fitness of recipient Kin selection (inclusive fitness) and social behavior These worker ants are sterile, and they differ greatly from their queens and males. Darwin was troubled by social-insect workers, because they have

More information

Commentary , USA 2 Department of Biology, Trinity University, San Antonio, TX 78212, USA

Commentary , USA 2 Department of Biology, Trinity University, San Antonio, TX 78212, USA Ibis (2012), 154, 439 443 Commentary It isn t always sexy when both are bright and shiny: considering alternatives to sexual selection in elaborate monomorphic species KEITH A. TARVIN 1 * & TROY G. MURPHY

More information

Inbreeding avoidance under different null models

Inbreeding avoidance under different null models Journal of Animal Ecology 2009, 78, 778 788 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01544.x Inbreeding avoidance under different null models Blackwell Publishing Ltd of random mating in the great tit Marta Szulkin

More information

13.17 Natural selection cannot fashion perfect organisms

13.17 Natural selection cannot fashion perfect organisms 13.17 Natural selection cannot fashion perfect organisms 1. Selection can only act on existing variation Natural selection cannot conjure up new beneficial alleles 2. Evolution is limited by historical

More information

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at Experimental and Natural Changes in the Peacock's (Pavo cristatus) Train Can Affect Mating Success Author(s): Marion Petrie and Tim Halliday Source: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, Vol. 35, No. 3

More information

Genetic similarity, extrapair paternity, and offspring quality in Savannah sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis)

Genetic similarity, extrapair paternity, and offspring quality in Savannah sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis) Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arl031 Advance Access publication 25 August 2006 Genetic similarity, extrapair paternity, and offspring quality in Savannah sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis) Corey

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.1185550/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Cryptic Sex-Ratio Bias Provides Indirect Genetic Benefits Despite Sexual Conflict Robert M. Cox* and Ryan Calsbeek

More information