Amphetamine-Induced Conditioned Activity Is Insensitive to Perturbations Known to Affect Pavlovian Conditioned Responses in Rats

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Amphetamine-Induced Conditioned Activity Is Insensitive to Perturbations Known to Affect Pavlovian Conditioned Responses in Rats"

Transcription

1 Behavioral Neuroscience Vol. 112, No, 5, Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc /98/$3.00 Amphetamine-Induced Conditioned Activity Is Insensitive to Perturbations Known to Affect Pavlovian Conditioned Responses in Rats Serge H. Ahmed, Luis Stinus, and Martine Cador University of Victor Segalen Bordeaux II Psychostimulant-induced conditioned activity is characterized by the presence of a hyperactivity in drug-free rats exposed to an environment previously paired with the effects of a psychostimulant. This phenomenon is thought to result from a Pavlovian conditioning process. This hypothesis predicts that conditioned activity will he sensitive to perturbations known to affect classical conditioned responses. In direct contrast with this prediction, the authors report here that conditioned activity is insensitive to (a) the temporal order between the stimulant injection and the exposure to the environment, (b) unsignaled stimulant injections between drug-environment pairings, and (c) drug preexposures before the start of drug-environment pairings. It is concluded that the stimulant effects responsible for the establishment of conditioned activity may not be amenable to a Pavlovian associative process. Drug-free rats exposed to an environment previously paired wilh a psychostimulant are hyperactive as compared with rats for which this environment has been previously paired with the vehicle (Barr et al., 1983; Beninger & Harm, 1983; Gold, Swerdlow, & Koob, 1988; Pickens & Crowder, 1967; Stewart & Vezina, 1991). It is thought that this phenomenon, called conditioned activity, depends on a Pavlovian conditioning process whereby some of the drug effects act as an unconditioned stimulus (US) and the environment acts as a conditioned stimulus (CS). During repeated environment-stimulant pairings, an association forms between the relevant drug US and the environmental CS. This associative account of conditioned activity has provided an explanatory basis for the behavioral sensitization observed after repeated stimulant and opiate administration (Ahmed, Stinus, Le Moal, & Cador, 1993; Hinson & Poulos, 1981; Post, Lockfeld, Squillace, & Contel, 1981; Stewart & Vezina, 1988; Tilson & Rech, 1973) and plays a significant role in some current theories of drug addiction (Robinson & Berridge, 1993; Stewart, de Wit, & Eikelboom, 1984; Wise & Bozarth, 1987). The Pavlovian account mainly relics on the fact that conditioned activity has features similar to a classical conditioned response (CR; i.e., stimulus specificity and extinction-like phenomena; for a critical review, see Ahmed, Serge H. Ahmed, Luis Stinus, and Marline Cador, University of Victor Segalen Bordeaux II, Bordeaux, France. This research was supported by grants from the Institut National de la Sante et de la Recherche Medicale, Paris, France and Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Paris, France and by the Conseil Regional d'aquitaine, Bordeaux, France. We would like to thank Charles Heyser, John Walker, and Pilar Flores for their comments on a draft of the article, Cecile Spielevoy for her help in conducting some of the experiments reported, and Megan Beschen for proofreading the manuscript. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Serge H. Ahmed, who is now at the Department of Neuropharmacology, Scripps Research Institute, North Torrey Pines Road, La Jolla, California Electronic mail may be sent to aserge@sage.scripps.edu. 1995). Note, however, that these similarities are not sufficient to establish the associative status of conditioned activity. First, they are also compatible with nonassociative mechanisms, involving, for example, a deficit of habituation to the would-be paired environment (e.g., Ahmed, Stinus, Le Moal, & Cador, 1995). Second, data exist that do not support the view of conditioned activity as a Pavlovian CR. There is no evidence that conditioned activity grows with the number of pairings (Ahmed, Stinus, et al., 1995; Hoffman & Wise, 1992; Mazurski & Beninger, 1991; Pickens & Crowder, 1967), which is surprising for a putative Pavlovian learning process. Indeed, even the magnitude of conditioned taste aversion, which is well-known to develop after a single conditioning trial, continues to increase with repeated trials (e.g., Figure 3 in Grigson. 1997). Moreover, conditioned activity does not vary with the dose of the psychostimulant, although the unconditioned drug effect does (Herz & Beninger, 1987; Martin-Iverson & McManus, 1990; Schiff, 1982; but see Tilson & Rech, 1973). Finally, it has been shown recently that the form of conditioned activity differs substantially from the unconditioned behaviors induced by stimulant drugs (Martin-Iverson & Fawcett, 1996). In view of the equivocal status of conditioned activity, the present series of experiments is intended to probe directly its putative associative basis. For this, it is necessary to verify whether perturbations known to interfere with the formation and expression of a CS-US association affect conditioned activity. We test whether conditioned activity is sensitive to the CS-US temporal order (Experiment 1), to unsignaled occurrences of the drug US between CS-US trials (Experiment 2), and to the US preexposure effect (Experiment 3). All of these perturbations are known to affect Pavlovian CRs by deteriorating the predictive relationship between the CS and the US (e.g., Durlach, 1989; Mackintosh, 1983; Rescorla, 1988). Subjects General Method Two-month-old male Wistar rats (Tffa Credo, Lyon, France), weighing g on arrival, were used in all experiments. The 1167

2 1168 AHMED, ST1NUS, AND CADOR rats were housed in groups of 5 and maintained in a 12-hr light-dark cycle, beginning at 6 a.m., and temperature-controlled environment. Food and water were freely available. Before the start of the experiment, each rat was handled every other day for 30 s (spread over 8 days after their arrival in the laboratory). From the beginning of the experiment, they were weighed in the afternoon of the days before the amphetamine-injection days. Apparatus The experiment involved two distinct environments: A and B. They were located in the same sound- and light-attenuated experimental room, which was distinct from the colony room. Environment A was an activity cage. Each cage was built of metal wire mesh and measured 20 X 25 X 36 cm. Two photocell beams were located across the long axis 2 cm above the floor, to enable measurement of general activity. The activity cages were linked to a computer, which recorded each photocell beam break. For Environment B, Plexiglas boxes with transparent walls were used. These boxes, slightly smaller than the activity cages, were inserted into the activity cages to allow the measurement of activity. This was done so that the photocell beams were located 2 cm above the floor of the box equidistant from the middle of the box, as in the activity cages. These two environments featured olfactory, tactile, and visual cues that were different from one another and from those of the home cage. In A, olfactory cues were provided by a diluted solution of Aniseed (pure anise extract; Schilling, Hunt Valley, MD); in B, there were no explicit odor cues. Different textures of the floor represented the tactile cues: In A, the floor was rough; in B, it was wire mesh. The visual cues were represented by different patterns on the walls: In A, there was no explicit pattern; in B, there were horizontal black and white adhesive strips. Drugs d-amphetamine sulfate was dissolved in an isotonic 0.9% (wt/wt) NaCl solution. The dose used was 1.25 mg/ml/kg, expressed as the weight of the salt (see Ahmed et al., 1993, for the choice of the dose). All injections were given subcutaneously. General Experimental Design All experiments involved at least two phases: a drugenvironment pairing phase and a test phase. The pairing phase consisted of successive 2-day sessions. On the 1st day of each session, all rats received an amphetamine injection, but half of them were placed in A and the other half in B. On the 2nd day of each session, rats received a vehicle injection and were placed in the environment that had not been paired with the amphetamine on the 1st day. Thus, for each rat there was an amphetamine (CS + ) and a vehicle (CS )-paired environment. The test phase followed the last session of the drug-environment pairing phase and lasted 2 days. During the 1st test day, rats received a vehicle injection and were placed in the CS +. During the 2nd test day, the procedure was identical to the 1 st test day, except that rats were exposed to the CS. We have previously shown that conditioned activity is not affected by the order of testing (see Experiment 1 in Ahmed, Stinus, etal., 1995). Experiment 1: Effect of the Drug US-CS+ Delay on Conditioned Activity In a typical Pavlovian conditioning trial, the US follows in time the onset of the CS. When the CS-US temporal relationship is arranged differently, with the US presented simultaneously or before the CS, the acquisition of the CR is either abolished or, in a few cases, strongly attenuated (Heth, 1976; Heth & Rescorla, 1973; Mahoney & Ayres, 1976). Therefore, if conditioned activity develops because the environment predicts the drug US, then its acquisition should be affected by presenting the drug US before exposing rats to the would-be CS+. Experiment 1 was intended to test this prediction by using amphetamine as the drug US and different drug US-CS+ delays. Method The experiment involved three groups of rats (n 10-11). During the 1st day of each drug-pairing session, all groups were transported to the experimental room and then received amphetamine. Depending on the group, they were placed in the CS + at different times after the drug injection: 0 min (corresponding to a time preceding the onset of the drug effect), 25 min (corresponding to a time close to the peak of the drug effect), or 50 min (corresponding to the start of the decline of the drug effect). These delays were based on a pilot study of the time course of amphetamine-induced hyperactivity. Thus, the stimulant effect occurred after and at different times before exposure to the CS+ in the 0-min, 25-min, and 50-min groups, respectively. During the delay period, rats remained in their home cage. The duration of exposure to CS+ was 120, 95, and 70 min for the 0-min, 25-min, and 50-min groups, respectively. The duration of exposure was reduced in the 25-min and 50-min groups (a) to ensure that all three groups were removed from the CS+ exactly 120 min after the amphetamine injection and (b) to avoid keeping rats of the 25-min and 50-min groups in the presence of the CS+ while no longer under the drug effects. During the 2nd day of each session, the procedure was identical except that rats received a vehicle injection. After six successive pairing sessions, the rats were tested as described in the General Method section. Results and Discussion Responses to amphetamine and vehicle during the drugpairing phase. The upper part of Figure 1 shows the mean total activity recorded during the first 60 min after each amphetamine injection in the CS +. A two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a slight but significant increase of amphetamine-induced hyperactivity across injections, F(5, 135) = 9.82, p <.0001, but the magnitude of this sensitization was not influenced by the drug US-CS + delay, Group X Injections interaction, F(10, 135) = Although there was a trend that amphetamine-induced hyperactivity was lower in the 25-min and 50-min groups than in the 0-min group, it did not reach statistical significance, group, F(2, 27) = The lower part of Figure 1 represents the level of activity recorded after each vehicle injection in the CS -. A two-way ANOVA revealed a significant decrease of activity across injections, F(5, 135) = 51.37, p <.0001, but the magnitude of this habituation did not differ between groups: group, F(2. 27) = 1.97; Group X Injections interaction, F(10, 135) = Activity in the CS+ and the CS during the test phase. Figure 2 shows the mean total activity during the first 60 min after exposure to the CS + and CS- during the test phase for

3 AMPHETAMINE-INDUCED CONDITIONED ACTIVITY t; 2000 amph ACQUISITION SESSIONS Figure I. Mean (±SEM) total activity counts recorded during the 1st hr after each amphetamine (amph)- and each vehicle-injection session. Depending on the experimental group (n = per group), rats were exposed to the conditioned stimulus (CS+) 0, 25, or 50 min after each amphetamine injection (1.25 mg/kg, sc). The same procedure was used for vehicle injections given in the CS. each experimental group. A two-way ANOVA revealed that the level of activity in the CS + was greater than in the CS -, F(l, 27) = 29.33, p < However, the magnitude of this difference was similar in the three experimental groups group, F(2, 27) = 1.66; Group X Environment interaction, F(1, 27) = 0.52 suggesting that the development of conditioned activity was not influenced by the drug US- CS + delay. To assess possible differences in the time course of conditioned activity between the three experimental groups, we performed a three-way ANOVA with one between-subjects factor (experimental groups) and two withinsubject factors (time: six time bins of 10 min each; test: CS + vs. CS-). Figure 3 contains the results. This analysis revealed a significant effect of environment, F(l, 27) = 29.33, p <.0001, and a significant Environment X Time interaction, F(5, 135) = 7.79, p <.0001, suggesting that conditioned activity in the CS+ decreased with time. However, this time course was not significantly influenced by the drug US-CS+ delay, Group X Environment X Time interaction, F(10, 135) = Several issues need to be emphasized. First, the habituation pattern observed after repeated exposures to the CS was not influenced by the duration of exposure. For example, the 50-min group, which was allowed to explore the CS- for a period of 70 min, showed virtually the same rate of habituation across sessions compared with the 0-min group, which was allowed a 120-min period of exploration. This result was not surprising, however, because in grouphoused rats, most of the exploratory activity in a novel environment occurs within thelsthourof exposure (Ahmed, Cador, Le Moal, & Stinus, 1995). Second, as expected, amphetamine-induced hyperactivity in the CS+ tended to be more pronounced in the 0-min group, for which no delay was interposed between the drug injection and the exposure to the CS +. Although not significant, this difference was more pronounced between the 0-min and 50-min groups. With repeated injections, a slight sensitization to this effect of amphetamine was observed, but its magnitude was not influenced by the drug US-CS+ delay. Finally and most important, the drug US-CS+ delay had no significant impact on the acquisition or expression of conditioned activity. Rats exposed to the CS+ 25 or 50 min after the amphetamine injections exhibited a level of conditioned activity comparable to the control group. Moreover, the time course of conditioned activity was unchanged by the drug US-CS+ delay. 600 «500 z O 400 o min 25 min 50 min < min 25 min 50 min DEIAY US-CS+ Figure 2. Effect of the drug unconditioned stimulus-conditioned stimulus (US-CS+) delay on amphetamine-induced conditioned activity. Each drug US-CS + delay (0, 25, or 50 min) represents a different group of rats (n = per group). Each group was exposed after a vehicle injection to the CS + on Day 1 and to the CS - on Day 2 of the test phase. Bars represent mean (± SEM) total activity counts registered during the 1st hr after a vehicle injection TIME (10-min bins) Figure 3. Effect of the drug unconditioned stimulus-conditioned stimulus (US-CS+) delay on the time course of conditioned activity. Each drug US-CS + delay (0, 25, or 50 min) represents a different group of rats (n = per group). Each group was exposed after a vehicle injection to the CS + on Day 1 and to the CS on Day 2 of the test phase. Points represent mean (± SEM) activity counts measured every 10 min during the 1st hour of exposure to both the CS+ and the CS-.

4 1170 AHMED, STINUS, AND CADOR Experiment 2: Effect of Uasignaled Drug US on Conditioned Activity It has been established that to be a reliable predictor, the CS should signal to the subject a period in which the likelihood of US occurrences is increased compared with a condition in which the CS is absent (Dickinson, 1980; Durlach, 1989; Rescorla, 1988). For example, if the experimenter makes the probability of a shock US in the absence of the CS (i.e., p[us/no CS]) equal to that in the presence of the CS (i.e., pfus/csl), the development of a fear CR to the CS is abolished (Rescorla, 1968). The manipulation of p[us/no CS] is achieved by presenting the US alone (i.e., unsignaled presentation) between CS-US pairings. If conditioned activity depends on the predictive value of the CS+, then it would be affected by giving unsignaled amphetamine injections between CS+-drug US pairings. Experiment 2 was intended to test this prediction. Method This experiment involved three groups of rats (n = 10). Each group received six successive drug-pairing sessions (S1 to S6), but the CS+ was paired with amphetamine only during SI, S3, S4. and S6; during the remaining sessions (S2 and S5), the CS+ was paired with a vehicle injection (see Table 1). By reducing Ihe likelihood of amphetamine effects in the presence of the CS+, this partial conditioning procedure was intended to facilitate the detrimental effect amphetamine injections given in the absence of the CS + have on conditioned activity (for an explanation on the use of this procedure, see Durlach, 1983; Rescorla, 1968). The groups only differed from each other by the number of amphetamine injections received in the absence of the CS+ (unsignaled injections). The procedure used to administer unsignaled injections was as follows. Every day, 3 to 6 hr after having been removed from the CS + or the CS and returned to the colony room, rats were brought back to the experimental room, where they received a second injection of either vehicle or amphetamine. After the injection, rats remained in their home cages for 2 hr before being returned to the colony room. The 4-0 group received only vehicle injections (the first number stands for amphetamine injections given in the CS +, and the second number stands for unsignaled amphetamine injections). In contrast, Ihe 4-4 and 4-8 groups received four and eight unsignaled amphetamine injections, respectively, semirandomly interspersed with vehicle injections. Thus, in the 4-0 group, receiving the drug was entirely contingent on the presence of the CS+. In contrast, in the 4-4 and 4-8 groups, receiving the drug in the absence of the CS+ was equally or more likely than in its presence, respectively. Results and Discussion Responses to amphetamine and vehicle during the drugpairing phase. The upper part of Figure 4 shows the mean total activity recorded during the first 60 min after the four amphetamine injections given in the CS +. A two-way ANOVA revealed a slight but significant increase of amphetamine-induced hypcractivity across injections, F(3, 81) = Table 1 Regimen of Unsignaled Amphetamine Injections Used in Experiment 2 Amphetamine injections Unsignaled (3-6 hr later) Paired Trials Environment withcs+ group group group Session 1 Day 1 CS Day 2 cs- CS Session 2 Day 3 CS + Day 4 CS- cs Session 3 DayS CS Day 6 cs- Session 4 Day 7 CSt + DayS cs- Session 5 Day 9 CS+ Day 10 cs- CS Session 6 Day 11 CS Day 12 cs- Note. Each drug-environment pairing session lasted 2 days, for a total of six sessions. On even days, rats were exposed to the amphetamine-paired (CS + ) environment but received amphetamine (indicated by +) only during Days 1, 5, 7, and 11; the remaining days, they received vehicle (indicated by ). On odd days, rats were exposed to the vehicle-paired environment (CS ) after having been given vehicle. In addition to amphetamine injections paired with the CS +, rats received 0 (4-0 group). 4 (4-^ group), or 8 (4-8 group) unsignaled amphetamine injections, interspersed semirandomly with vehicle injections. These injections were given in the experimental context 3 to 6 hr after the exposure to the CS+ or the CS-. The number of unsignaled amphetamine injections given the days after exposure to the CS + was equal to that following exposure to the CS.

5 AMPHETAMINE-INDUCED CONDITIONED ACTIVITY , p <.005, and the magnitude of this sensitization was influenced by the number of amphetamine injections received in the absence of the CS+, Group X Injections interaction, F(6, 81) = 2.74, p <.02. A one-way ANOVA performed for each amphetamine injection revealed only a marginally significant difference between groups at the fourth amphetamine injection, F(2, 27) = 3.22, p =.056. During this injection, the activity of the 4-4 group was greater than that of the 4 0 group (p <.05; Newman-Keuls test), which was similar to that of the 4-8 group. The lower part of Figure 4 represents the level of activity recorded after each vehicle injection in the CS-. A two-way ANOVA revealed a significant decrease of activity across injections, F(5, 135) = 55.44, p <.0001, but the magnitude of this habituation was not influenced by the number of unsignaled amphetamine injections: group, F(2, 27) = 0.21; Group X Injections interaction, F(10, 135) = Activity in the CS+ and the CS during the test phase. Figure 5 shows the mean total activity during the first 60 min after exposure to the CS+andCS during the test phase for each experimental group. A two-way ANOVA revealed that the level of activity in the CS+ was greater than in the CS, F(\, 27) = 14.70, p <.001. However, the magnitude of this difference was similar in the three experimental groups, group F(2, 27) = 1.66, Group X Environment interaction, F(2, 27) = 0.52, suggesting that the development of conditioned activity was not influenced by the number of unsignaled amphetamine injections. Three important findings emerged from Experiment 2. First, unsignaled amphetamine injections did not affect the level of activity in the CS, suggesting that the relatively heavy drug treatment used in Experiment 2 did not induce amph ACQUISITION SESSIONS Figure 4. Mean (±SEM) total activity counts recorded during the 1st hr after each amphetamine injection paired with the amphetamine (amph) environment conditioned stimulus (CS+; i.e., during Sessions 1, 3,4, and 6 of the drug-environment pairing phase) and each vehicle injection paired with the vehicle environment CS (from Session 1 to Session 6). Depending on the experimental group (n = 10 per group), and in addition to the four amphetamine injections paired with the CS +, rats received zero (4-0 group), four (4-4 group), or eight (4-8 group) unsignaled amphetamine injections distributed pseudorandomly between the drug-environment pairing sessions o UNSIGNALED AMPHETAMINE Figure 5. Effect of unsignaled amphetamine injections on amphetamine-induced conditioned activity. Unsignaled amphetamine injections were pseudorandomly distributed between the drugenvironment pairing sessions. Different numbers of unsignaled amphetamine injections (0, 4, and 8) were administered to different groups of rats («= 10 per group). During the test phase, each group was exposed after a vehicle injection to the amphetaminepaired environment (CS+) on Day 1 and to the vehicle-paired environment (CS ) on Day 2. Bars represent mean (±SEM) total activity counts registered during the 1 st hr after a vehicle injection. any general behavioral hypo- or hyperactivity. Second, unsignaled amphetamine injections slightly sensitized the rats to amphetamine when injected in the CS+. However, the relationship between the number of unsignaled injections and the magnitude of this sensitization was not linear. Indeed, although the 4-4 group received fewer unsignaled amphetamine injections than the 4-8 group, it appeared more sensitized than the latter group. This interpretation should be tempered, however, because the relative decrease in locomotor activity observed in the 4-8 group may also reflect a sensitization in some competing behavioral stereotypies (Segal & Mandell, 1974). Finally, the data obtained during the test phase revealed that the partial conditioning procedure used in Experiment 2 had a detrimental effect on conditioned activity (compare the difference in activity in the CS+ and the CS- observed in the 4-0 group with that observed in Experiments 1 and 3; see also Ahmed, Stinus, et al., 1995). As already noted, partial conditioning was used to increase the effect of unsignaled USs (Durlach, 1983; Rescorla, 1968). However, despite the use of this procedure, unsignaled amphetamine injections did not prevent the development of conditioned activity. Rather, those injections seemed to prevent the detrimental effect of the partial conditioning procedure. Experiment 3: Effect of Drug US Preexposures on the Development of Conditioned Activity Another way to reduce the predictive value of the CS consists of preexposing the subject to the US alone before the start of the conditioning. US preexposures usually have a detrimental effect on the acquisition of a CR (for a review, see Randich & LoLordo, 1979). This detrimental effect is

6 1172 AHMED, STINUS, AND CADOR amph ACQUISITION SESSIONS Figure 6. Mean (±5 M) total activity counts recorded during the 1st hr after each amphetamine (amph)-injection session and each vehicle-injection session. Depending of the experimental group (n = 10 per group), rats were preexposed 0 (O^PREE group), 5 (5-PREE group) or 10 (10-PREE group) times to amphetamine (1.25 mg/kg, sc) before the start of the drug-environment pairing phase. thought to result from a blocking mechanism (see Kamin, 1969), wherehy the experimental context of the US preexposures plays the role of the blocking stimulus (Durlach, 1989; Randich & Ross, 1985; Tomie, 1976). Experiment 3 was intended to assess whether repeated preexposures to amphetamine before conditioning block the establishment of conditioned activity. Method The experiment involved three groups of rats (n 10). Before the start of the drug-pairing phase, all rats were transported each day for 10 consecutive days to the experimental room. From Day 1 to Day 10, the 0-PREE group received one daily vehicle injection and then were left in their home cage for 2 hr. The two other groups were preexposed to the training dose of amphetamine: The 5-PREE group received 1 daily vehicle injection on the first 5 days and 1 daily amphetamine injection on the last 5 days; the 10-PREE group received 10 daily amphetamine injections. Note that during the preexposure phase, all the details of the injection procedure (i.e., transport and handling) were similar to those used during the drug-pairing phase. After a 24-hr rest period after the last injection, all rats received three successive drug-pairing sessions, after which they were tested, as described in the General Method section. The relatively small number of pairings used in this experiment was intended to increase the sensitivity of our procedure to the detrimental effect of US preexposures. Results and Discussion Responses to amphetamine and vehicle during the drugpairing phase. The upper part of Figure 6 shows the mean total activity recorded during the first 60 min after each amphetamine injection in the CS +. A two-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference between groups, F(2, 27) = 3.97, p <.04. This difference was due to an increased response to amphetamine in the amphetamine-preexposed groups compared with the vehicle-preexposed group (p <.03; Newman-Keuls test). Moreover, there was a slight but significant increase of amphetamine-induced hyperactivity across injections, F(2, 54) = 6.37, p <.005, but the magnitude of this sensitization was not influenced by the number of amphetamine preexposures, Group X Injections interaction, F(4, 54) = The lower part of Figure 6 represents the level of activity recorded after each vehicle injection in the CS-. A two-way ANOVA revealed a significant decrease of activity across injections, F(2, 54) = 74.47, p <.0001, but the magnitude of this habituation was not influenced by the number of amphetamine preexposures, group, F(2, 27) = 0.61; Group X Injections interaction, f (4, 54) = Activity in the CS+ and the CS during the test phase. Figure 7 shows the mean total activity during the first 60 min after exposure to the CS + andcs during the test phase for each experimental group. A two-way ANOVA revealed that the level of activity in the CS + was greater than in the CS -, F(l, 27) = 30.76, p < However, the magnitude of this difference was similar in the three experimental groups, group, F(2, 27) = 2.46, Group X Environment interaction, F(2, 27) = 0.28, suggesting that the development of conditioned activity was not influenced by preexposure to amphetamine. In Experiment 3, several points deserve attention. First, repeated preexposure to amphetamine did not affect the activity recorded in the CS- during the drug-environment pairing phase, suggesting that amphetamine pretreated rats respond to environmental novelty and habituate to it in a manner similar to drug-naive rats. Second, amphetamine preexposure sensitized rats to subsequent amphetamine injections in the CS +. In addition to showing that our preexposure regimen was effective, this result also suggests AMPHETAMINE PREEXPOSURES Figure 7. Effect of amphetamine preexposures on amphetamineinduced conditioned activity. Amphetamine preexposures were given before the start of the drug-environment pairing phase. Different numbers of amphetamine preexposures (0, 5, and 10) were administered to different groups of rats (n = 10 per group). During the test phase, each group was exposed, after a vehicle injection, to the amphetamine-paired environment (CS+) on Day 1 and to the vehicle-paired environment (CS ) on Day 2. Bars represent mean (± SEM) total activity counts registered during the 1st hr after a vehicle injection.

7 AMPHETAMINE-INDUCED CONDITIONED ACTIVITY 1173 that sensitization can be expressed in an environment that has never been directly paired with the drug (see also Ahmed et a!., 1993; Anagnostaras & Robinson, 1996). Finally, and most important, in contrary to the known deleterious effect of US preexposure on the acquisition of a classical CR, repeated preexposure to amphetamine had no detrimental effect on conditioned activity. This negative outcome is also surprising for another reason. Although amphetaminepreexposed rats exhibited a higher UR to amphetamine, this was not paralleled by a higher level of conditioned activity. This result is congruent with data showing that in sharp contrast to the UR to the drug, conditioned activity does not increase with the training dose (see introduction). As we have suggested (Ahmed et al., 1993), this latter finding could indicate that once a critical level of stimulation is reached, conditioned activity fully develops in an all-or-nothing manner. General Discussion The present study was designed to verify whether stimulant-induced conditioned activity results from an association between the drug-paired environment and some drug US. It was hypothesized that this association developed through a Pavlovian conditioning process, by which a previously neutral environment became a reliable predictor of the drug US. The present data, however, do not support this account. First, amphetamine-induced conditioned activity can manifest in an environment that follows in time the drug US (Experiment 1). Second, conditioned activity appears in an environment that is not a reliable predictor of the drug US (Experiments 2 and 3). The following sections arc intended to discuss possible limitations of the present data and to draw some implications for future studies. Is Conditioned Activity Anticipatory in Nature? In Experiment 1, conditioned activity appeared almost completely insensitive to the drug US-CS+ delay. This finding can be interpreted in several ways. First, it may incidentally be considered as one of the seldom instances of a successful backward conditioning (see Mackintosh, 1983). This is unlikely, however, because CRs obtained in backward conditioning are usually lower in magnitude than CRs obtained in traditional forward conditioning (e.g., Heth, 1976; Heth & Rescorla, 1973). Second, that conditioned activity manifested despite a 50-min drug US-CS+ delay could simply indicate that the relevant drug US begins at 50 min after the drug injection. Although plausible, this is not supported by the time course of conditioned activity observed in the 0-min group. Indeed, if the drug US was really delayed in time, conditioned activity would have been more apparent at a time point close to the putative onset of the US, a phenomenon called inhibition of delay (Pavlov, 1927). Third, Experiment 1 shows also that pairing either the ascending or the descending component of the stimulant effect with the CS+ makes no detectable difference. Because the descending part may partly reflect the intervention of opponent processes (Solomon, 1977; Solomon & Corbit, 1974), these data could suggest that these processes play no significant role in conditioned activity. In conclusion, the negative outcome of Experiment 1 confirms a previous study (Pickens & Crowder, 1967) and indicates that in contrast to a Pavlovian CR (Hollis, 1982; Siegel, 1985), stimulantinduced conditioned activity is not likely an anticipatory response to a forthcoming drug US. Does Conditioned Activity Depend on a Predictive CS-US Relationship? Both Experiments 2 and 3 suggest that conditioned activity is insensitive to the predictive value of the CS +. In Experiment 2, increasing the likelihood to receive the drug US in the absence of the CS+ did not have any detrimental influence on conditioned activity. In Experiment 3, preexposing rats to the drug US alone before pairing it with the CS + had no effect on the acquisition or expression of conditioned activity. This latter finding confirms an earlier study that also failed to document an effect of amphetamine preexposures (5 in total) on conditioned rotations (Drew & Click. 1988). To interpret these data, one should be reminded that the detrimental effect of both unsignaled USs and US preexposures has been shown to depend on an association between the US and the training context (i.e., the background external stimuli present during CS-US pairings). Such a context-us association is thought either to block the acquisition of the CS-US association (Durlach, 1989; Kamin, 1969; Rescorla & Wagner, 1972) or to interfere with its behavioral expression (e.g., Miller & Matzel, 1988). In this light, the negative outcome of Experiments 2 and 3 could be attributed to a lack of contextual conditioning. This is unlikely because caution was taken to administer the unsignaled amphetamine injections or to preexpose rats to amphetamine in the presence of the experimental context in which the CS+ was embedded. This context represents a rich set of stimuli originating from (a) the transport of the rats from the colony room to the experimental room by using a metal cart; (b) the distinctive features of the experimental room (e.g., its continuous white noise, its illumination, and its particular smells); (c) the distinctive stimuli of the injection ritual (handling and subcutaneous injections). This whole experimental context was maintained throughout the experiment to facilitate, if any, its association with the drug US. Moreover, similar contextual cues have been shown to control the expression of opiate-induced tolerance phenomena (for a review, see Siegel, 1989). Although contextual conditioning was likely to occur in our experimental conditions, it still remains possible that the relative strength of the context-us association was too low to interfere with the formation or expression of the CS+/ drug US association. However, there are reasons to believe that this was not the case. First, in Experiment 2, in the 4-8 group, which received the highest number of unsignaled injections, amphetamine was paired 66.6% of the time with the context alone (8 out of 12 occasions). This probability is equal to the probability of receiving amphetamine in the presence of the CS+ (4 out of 6 occasions). This equiprobability is known to completely prevent the acquisition or

8 1174 AHMED, STINUS, AND CADOR expression of a Pavlovian CR (Dickinson, 1980; Durlach, 1989; Rescorla, 1968). Second, in Experiment 3, it would be difficult to argue that amphetamine preexposures were insufficient because in the 10-PREE group, for example, amphetamine was paired to the experimental context alone three times more than to the CS +. In summary, the apparent insensitivity of conditioned activity to unsignaled USs or US preexposures observed in Experiments 2 and 3 is not likely to be the result of a lack of contextual conditioning and may suggest that conditioned activity does not depend on a predictive relationship between the drug US and the environment. Is the Relevant Drug US Malleable? There is another possible limitation of the present study. In the three experiments performed, the environment where rats experienced amphetamine effects when not in the CS + was always the home cage. There, rats had the opportunity to interact with their habitual cage mates. There is evidence that some of the behavioral effects of stimulant drugs can be strongly influenced by the environmental setting (e.g., Beck, Chow, & Cooper, 1986; Ellinwood & Kilbey, 1975). For example, it has been recently shown that the stimulant effect of amphetamine is much higher in a novel environment than in a familiar environment (Badiani, Anagnostaras, & Robinson, 1995). Although the mechanism of this effect is still unknown, it is possible that the US generated by amphetamine in one environmental setting is quantitatively or qualitatively different from the one generated in another setting. Therefore, our failure to affect conditioned activity could depend on the fact that the amphetamine US perceived in the CS+ was different from the amphetamine US experienced in the home cage. First, in Experiment 1, the drug US-CS+ delay could have been ineffective simply because by definition the US related to the CS+ could not occur before exposure to this environment. Second, US preexposures or unsignaled USs were ineffective because what was paired with the experimental context during preexposure or between CS+/US pairings was a drug US different from the drug US experienced in the CS +. This explanation surely warrants further studies. What Is the Nature of Conditioned Activity? To date, two major hypotheses of conditioned activity have been proposed. According to the first, conditioned activity develops because stimulants induce a deficit in habituation to the novelty of the would-be paired environment (Ahmed, Stinus, et al., 1995; Damianopoulos & Carey, 1992; Gold et al., 1988). This hypothesis seems to be supported by two lines of evidence: (a) The magnitude and time course of conditioned activity are similar to those of novelty-induced activity (Ahmed, Stinus, et al., 1995), and (b) the extinction of conditioned activity nicely parallels the behavioral habituation observed in a novel environment (Ahmed, Stinus, et al., 1995; see also Damianopoulos & Carey, 1992). However, the fact that conditioned activity could fully develop in a previously well familiarized environment contradicts this hypothesis (Ahmed, Oberling, Di Scala, & Sandner, 1996). According to the second hypothesis, the Pavlovian hypothesis tested here, conditioned activity develops because of a conditioned association between the drug-paired environment and some drug US. However, as indicated by the present findings, this associative account may not be valid. This conclusion is consistent with the insensitivity of conditioned activity to increases in drug doses and drug-pairing sessions (see introduction). Moreover, it could help researchers understand why conditioned activity is unaffected by lesioning the basolateral amygdala (Ahmed, Stinus, et al., 1995; Brown & Fibiger, 1993), a brain region known to mediate stimulus reward association in rodents (Cador, Robbins, & Everitt, 1989; Everitt, Cador, & Robbins, 1989). What then is the nature of conditioned activity? At present, this question is difficult to answer because the nature of the so-called stimulant US remains unknown. Indeed, although it is widely held that it is interoceptive, one does not know which sensory modalities it affects, what its exact temporal characteristics are, and, if any, what its spatial features are. Consequently, in the case of so-called stimulant conditioning, it is not possible to specify either the content of what is supposedly learned or the form of the particular drug US representation. This situation contrasts with conditioning involving prototypical Pavlovian USs, which are usually well-identified phasic and localized changes in the environment (e.g., Dickinson, 1980; Holland, 1990; Mackintosh, 1983; Miller & Barnet, 1993). One common way to solve this indeterminacy has been to rely on the observed effects of a drug to guess some of the properties of drug USs. Unfortunately, this strategy is limited by the fact that observed drug effects can reflect primary or secondary physiological processes that do not necessarily parallel the drug US of interest (for a related discussion, see Eikelboom & Stewart, 1982). In this general context, the present failure to observe any significant effect of perturbations known to affect classical CRs on conditioned activity could indicate that due to some special features of stimulant USs, they may not be amenable to a Pavlovian associative process. This conclusion could have important implications for the understanding of related phenomena, such as behavioral sensitization and drug addiction. Clearly, more studies are needed to resolve this important issue. In conclusion, the present study shows that conditioned activity is insensitive to experimental perturbations known to affect the acquisition or expression of a Pavlovian CR. Although these data suggest that conditioned activity is not likely to be mediated by a Pavlovian associative process, such a conclusion will not be firmly established until more is known about the nature of so-called stimulant USs. This will require an equal focus on similarities and differences between stimulant USs and more traditional exteroceptive USs. References Ahmed, S. H. (1995). Bases associative^ et non associative.-* de la sensibilisation aux drogues stimulantes. [Associative and nonas-

9 AMPHETAMINE-INDUCED CONDITIONED ACTIVITY 1175 sociative basis of stimulants-induced behavioral sensitization]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Victor Segalen Bordeaux II, Bordeaux, France. Ahmed, S. H., Cador, M., Le Moal, M., & Stinus, L. (1995). Social deprivation enhances the vulnerability of male Wistar rats to stressor- and amphetamine-induced behavioral sensitization. Psychopharmacology, 117, Ahmed, S. H., Oberling, P., Di Scala, G., & Sandner, G. (1996). Amphetamine-induced conditioned activity does not result from a failure of rats to habituate to novelty. Psychopharmacology, 123, Ahmed, S. H., Stinus, L., Le Moal, M., & Cador, M. (1993). Controlling interindividual differences in the unconditioned response to amphetamine in the study of environment-dependent sensitization. Behavioural Pharmacology, 4, Ahmed, S. H., Stinus, L., Le Moal, M., & Cador, M. (1995). Amphetamine-induced conditioned activity: Comparison with novelty-induced activity and role of the basolateral amygdala. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, Anagnostaras, S. G., & Robinson, T. E. (1996). Sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of amphetamine: Modulation by associative learning. Behavioral Neumsdence, 110, Badiani, A., Anagnostaras, S. G., & Robinson, T. E. (1995). The development of sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of amphetamine is enhanced in a novel environment. Psychcpharmacology, 117, Barr, G. A., Sharpless, N. S., Cooper, S., Shiff, S., Paredes, W., & Bridget. W. H. (1983). Classical conditioning, decay and extinction of cocaine-induced hyperactivily and stereotypy. Life Sciences, 33, Beck, C. H. M., Chow, H. L., & Cooper, S. J. (1986). Initial environment influences amphetamine-induced stereotypy: Subsequently environment changes have little effect. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 46, Beninger, R. J., & Hahn, B. L. (1983). Pimozide blocks establishment but not expression ot amphetamine-produced environmentspecific conditioning. Science, 220, Brown, E. E., & Fibiger, H. C. (1993). Differential effects of excitoloxic lesions of the amygdala on cocaine-induced conditioned locomotion and conditioned place preference. Psychopharmacology, 113, Cador, M., Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (1989). Involvement of the amygdala in stimulus-reward associations: Interaction with the ventral striatum. Neuroscience, 30, Damianopoulos, E. N., & Carey, R. J. (1992). Pavlovian conditioning of CNS drug effects: A critical review and new experimental design. Review in the Neurosciences, 3, Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Drew, K. L., & Glick, S. D. (1988). Characterization of the associative nature of sensitization to amphetamine-induced circling behavior and of environment dependent placebo-like response. Psychopharmacology, 95, Durlach, P. J. (1983). Effect of signaling intertrial unconditioned stimuli in autoshaping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, Durlach, P. J. (1989). Learning and performance in Pavlovian conditioning: Are failures of contiguity failures of learning or performance? In S. B. Klein & R. R. Mowrer (Eds.), Contemporary learning theories: Pavlovian conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Eikelboom, R., & Stewart, J. (1982). Conditioning of drug-induced physiological responses. Psychological Review, 89, Ellinwood, E. H., & Kilbey, M. M. (1975). Amphetamine stereotypy: The influence of environmental factors and prepotent behavioral patterns on its topography and development. Biological Psychiatry, 10, Everitt, B. J., Cador. M., & Robbins, T. W. (1989). Interactions between the amygdala and the ventral striatum in stimulusreward associations: Studies using a second-order schedule of sexual reinforcement. Neuroscience, 30, Gold, L. H., Swerdlow, N. R., & Koob, G. F. (1988). The role of the mesolimbic dopamine in conditioned locomotion produced by amphetamine. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, Grigson, P. S. (1997). Conditioned taste aversions and drugs of abuse: A reinterpretation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, Herz, R. S., & Beninger, R. J. (1987). Comparison of the ability of (+) amphetamine and caffeine to produce environmentspecific conditioning. Psychopharmacology, 92, Heth, C. D. (1976). Simultaneous and backward fear conditioning as a function of number of CS-UCS pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, Heth, C. D., & Rescorla, R. A. (1973). Simultaneous and backward fear conditioning in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 434^143. Hinson, R. E., & Poulos, C. X. (1981). Sensitization to the behavioral effects of cocaine: Modification by Pavlovian conditioning. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 15, Hoffman, D. C., & Wise, R. A. (1992). Locomotor-activating effects of the D 2 agonist bromocriptine show environmentspecific sensitization following repeated injections. Psychopharmacology, 107, Holland, P. C. (1990). Event representation in Pavlovian conditioning: Image and action. Cognition, 37, Hollis, K. L. (1982). Pavlovian conditioning of signal-centered action patterns and autonomic behavior: A biological analysis of [unction. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 12, Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In B. A. Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishment and aversive behavior (pp ). New York: Appleton- Century-Crofts. Mackintosh, N. J. (1983). Conditioning and associative learning. New York: Oxford University Press. Mahoney, W. ]., & Ayres, J. J. B. (1976). One-trial simultaneous and backward tear conditioning as reflected in conditioned suppression of licking in rats. Animal Learning and Behavior, 4, Martin-Iverson, M. T., & Fawcett, S. L. (1996) Pavlovian conditioning of psychomotor stimulant-induced behaviours: Has convenience led us astray? Behavioural Pharmacology, 7, 24^11. Martin-Iverson, M. T., & McManus, D. J. (1990). Stimulantconditioned locomotion is nol affected by blockade of D! and/or D 2 dopamine receptors during conditioning. Brain Research, 521, Mazurski, E. J., & Beninger, R. J. (1991). Effects of selective drugs for dopaminergic D] and D 2 receptors on conditioned locomotion in rats. Psychopharmacology, 105, Miller, R. R., & Barnct, R. C. (1993). The role of time in elementary association. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, Miller, R. R., & Matzcl, L. D. (1988). The comparator hypothesis: A response rule for the expression of associations. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp ). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London: Oxford University Press. Pickens, R. W., & Crowder, W. F. (1967). Effects of CS-US interval

10 1176 AHMED, STINUS, AND CADOR on conditioning of drug response, with assessment of speed of conditioning. Psychopharmacologia (Berlin). //, Post, R. M., Lockfeld, A., Squillace, K., & Contel, N. R. (1981). Drug-environment interaction: Context dependency of cocaineinduced behavioral sensitization. Life Sciences, 28, Randich, A., & LoLordo, V. M. (1979). Associative and nonassociative theories of the UCS preexposure phenomenon: Implications for Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 86, Randich, A., & Ross, R. T. (1985). Contextual stimuli mediate the effects of pre- and postexposure to the unconditioned stimulus in conditioned suppression. In P. D. Balsam & A. Tomie (Eds.), Context and learning, (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rescorla, R. A. (1968). Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 1-5. Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It's not what you think it is. American Psychologist, 43, Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations of the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforceinent. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning.- H. Current theory and research (pp ). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews, 18, Schiff, S. R. (1982). Conditioned dopaminergic activity. Biological Psychiatry, 17, Segal, D. S., & Mandell, A. J. (1974). Long-term administration of amphetamine: Progressive augmentation of motor activity and stereotypy. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 2, Siegel, S. (1985). Drug-anticipatory responses in animals. In L. White, B. Tursky, & G. Schwartz (Eds.), Placebo: Theory, research and mechanisms (pp ). New York: Guilford Press. Siegel, S. (1989). Pharmacological conditioning and drug effects. In A. J. Goudie & M. W. Emmett-Oglesby (Eds.), Psychoactive drugs: Tolerance and sensitization (pp ). Clifton, NJ: Humana Press. Solomon, R. L. (1977). An opponent process theory of acquired motivation: The affective dynamics of addiction. In J. D. Maser & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Psychopathology: Animal models (pp ). San Francisco: Freeman. Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, Stewart, J., de Wit, H., & Eikelboom, R. (1984). Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychological Review, 91, Stewart, J., & Vezina, P. (1988). Conditioning and behavioral sensitization. In P. W. Kalivas & C. D. Barnes (Eds.), Sensitization In the nervous system, (pp ). Caldwell, NJ: Telford Press. Stewart, J., & Vezina, P. (1991). Extinction procedures abolish conditioned stimulus control but spare sensitized responding to amphetamine. Behavioural Pharmacology, 2, Tilson, H. A., & Rech, R. H. (1973). Conditioned drug effects and absence of tolerance to d-amphetamine induced motor activity. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, I, Tomie, A. (1976). Interference with autoshaping by prior context conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, Wise, R. A., & Bozarth, M. A. (1987). A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. Psychological Review, 94, Received November 20, 1997 Revision received April 3, 1998 Accepted April 29, 1998

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

Learning and Motivation

Learning and Motivation Learning and Motivation 4 (29) 178 18 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Motivation journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Context blocking in rat autoshaping: Sign-tracking

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (2), 213-219 Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning THOMAS HIGGINS and ROBERT A. RESCORLA University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Principal Features. "First, the term learning does not apply to (168)

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Principal Features. First, the term learning does not apply to (168) Dikran J. Martin Psychology 110 Name: Date: Lecture Series: Chapter 5 Learning: How We're Changed Pages: 26 by Experience TEXT: Baron, Robert A. (2001). Psychology (Fifth Edition). Boston, MA: Allyn and

More information

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(2),199-203 Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning PETER C. HOLLAND and DESWELL T. FORBES University ofpittsburgh, Pittsburgh,

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

What is Learned? Lecture 9

What is Learned? Lecture 9 What is Learned? Lecture 9 1 Classical and Instrumental Conditioning Compared Classical Reinforcement Not Contingent on Behavior Behavior Elicited by US Involuntary Response (Reflex) Few Conditionable

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Learn Behav (2011) 39:79 86 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0022-x Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Gabriel Rodríguez & Gumersinda Alonso Published

More information

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms Q0667 QJEP(B) si-b03/read as keyed THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 56B (1), 43 55 Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative

More information

NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 2002 VOL. 27, NO American College of Neuropsychopharmacology

NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 2002 VOL. 27, NO American College of Neuropsychopharmacology Conditioned Locomotion Is Not Correlated with Behavioral Sensitization to Cocaine: An Intra-Laboratory Multi-Sample Analysis Gregory Hotsenpiller, Ph.D., and Marina E. Wolf, Ph.D. Pre-clinical and clinical

More information

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 28, 200 215 (1997) ARTICLE NO. LM960958 Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure MICHELLE SYMONDS AND GEOFFREY HALL University

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ISSN: 1747-0218 (Print) 1747-0226 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20 Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive

More information

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1989, Vol. 15, No. 3, 232-241 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-740389$00.75 Contextual Effects in Conditioning,

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of September 1999 Taste quality and extinction of

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition

Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 13, 391-416 (1982) Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition A. G. BAKER AND PIERRE MERCIER McGill University The hypothesis that latent inhibition could be reduced by extinguishing

More information

Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning

Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (2), 225-232 Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning JASPER WARD-ROBINSON, MICHELLE SYMONDS, and GEOFFREY

More information

ISIS NeuroSTIC. Un modèle computationnel de l amygdale pour l apprentissage pavlovien.

ISIS NeuroSTIC. Un modèle computationnel de l amygdale pour l apprentissage pavlovien. ISIS NeuroSTIC Un modèle computationnel de l amygdale pour l apprentissage pavlovien Frederic.Alexandre@inria.fr An important (but rarely addressed) question: How can animals and humans adapt (survive)

More information

Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online

Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online The use of an unpleasant sound as the unconditional stimulus in aversive Pavlovian conditioning experiments that involve children and adolescent participants Author Neumann, David, Waters, Allison, Westbury

More information

Unit 6 Learning.

Unit 6 Learning. Unit 6 Learning https://www.apstudynotes.org/psychology/outlines/chapter-6-learning/ 1. Overview 1. Learning 1. A long lasting change in behavior resulting from experience 2. Classical Conditioning 1.

More information

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience Dopamine and Learning Michael Dorris Associate Professor of Physiology & Neuroscience Studies dorrism@biomed.queensu.ca http://brain.phgy.queensu.ca/dorrislab/ NSCI 324 Systems

More information

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Psicológica (2004), 25, 45-65. Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Matías López*, Raúl Cantora*, and Luis Aguado** 1 * Universidad de

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Classical Conditioning Learning through Association Ivan Pavlov discovered the form of learning called Classical

More information

The US-Preexposure Effect in Lithium-Induced Flavor-Aversion Conditioning Is a Consequence of Blocking by Injection Cues

The US-Preexposure Effect in Lithium-Induced Flavor-Aversion Conditioning Is a Consequence of Blocking by Injection Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2004, Vol. 30, No. 1, 58 66 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.1.58

More information

Associative learning

Associative learning Introduction to Learning Associative learning Event-event learning (Pavlovian/classical conditioning) Behavior-event learning (instrumental/ operant conditioning) Both are well-developed experimentally

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning Operant Conditioning The nature of reinforcement: Premack s probability differential theory Response deprivation

More information

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION Tnesis for the Degree of M. A. MiCHiGAN STATE UNWERSITY RODNEY CHARLES HOWARD 1977 f L13 1-;- R Y Michiga: State gr? Umw 1y ABSTRACT TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES

More information

Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats

Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1980, 8 (1), 67-74 Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats ELIZABETH S. WITCHER and JOHN J. B. AYRES University ofmassachusetts,

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0039-1 Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Cody W. Polack & Mario A. Laborda & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 July 2011 # Psychonomic

More information

Context and Pavlovian conditioning

Context and Pavlovian conditioning Context Brazilian conditioning Journal of Medical and Biological Research (1996) 29: 149-173 ISSN 0100-879X 149 Context and Pavlovian conditioning Departamento de Psicologia, Pontifícia Universidade Católica

More information

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery L132 NT MJA/cla Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 60-73 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.60 Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery Gonzalo P. Urcelay University of Cambridge, Cambridge,

More information

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Chapter Title Attention and Pavlovian Conditioning Copyright Year 2011 Copyright Holder Springer Science+Business Media, LLC Corresponding Author

More information

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2004, Vol. 30, No. 2, 104 117 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.2.104

More information

Partial reinforcement effects on learning and extinction of place preferences in the water maze

Partial reinforcement effects on learning and extinction of place preferences in the water maze Learning & Behavior 2008, 36 (4), 311-318 doi: 10.3758/LB.36.4.311 Partial reinforcement effects on learning and extinction of place preferences in the water maze José Prados University of Leicester, Leicester,

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival Threats are much better reinforcers Fear is a prime

More information

Conditioning of fear and conditioning of safety in rats

Conditioning of fear and conditioning of safety in rats Conditioning of fear and conditioning of safety in rats Grazyna Walasek, Malgorzata Wqsierska and Kazimierz Zielinski Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, 3 Pasteur

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

by popular demand: Addiction II

by popular demand: Addiction II by popular demand: Addiction II PSY/NEU338: Animal learning and decision making: Psychological, computational and neural perspectives drug addiction huge and diverse field of research (many different drugs)

More information

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn?

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Defining Learning A relatively permanent change in behavior or the potential for behavior that results from experience Results from many life experiences, not just structured

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION Except where reference is made to the works of others, the work described in this dissertation is my own or was done in collaboration

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Chapter 5 Study Guide Chapter 5 Study Guide Practice Exam Questions: Which of the following is not included in the definition of learning? It is demonstrated immediately Assuming you have eaten sour pickles before, imagine

More information

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit Denniston, J. C., Savastano, H. I., Blaisdell, A. P., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Cue competition as a retrieval deficit. Learning and Motivation, 34(1): 1-31. (Feb 2003) Published by Elsevier (ISSN: 1095-9122).

More information

Perceptual learning transfer in an appetitive Pavlovian task

Perceptual learning transfer in an appetitive Pavlovian task Learn Behav (2017) 45:115 123 DOI 10.3758/s13420-016-0245-y Perceptual learning transfer in an appetitive Pavlovian task Antonio A. Artigas 1 & Jose Prados 2 Published online: 5 August 2016 # Psychonomic

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. What is Learning? Learning is the process that allows us to adapt (be flexible) to the changing conditions

More information

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1 The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Lecture 5.1 David S. Touretzky Based on notes by Lisa M. Saksida November, 2015 Outline Classical and instrumental conditioning The Rescorla

More information

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (4), 379-390 Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste MICHELLE SYMONDS and GEOFFREY HALL University of York, York, England

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 121, No. 5, 1043 1052 0735-7044/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.5.1043 Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors

More information

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2003, Vol. 29, No. 1, 39 48 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.29.1.39

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) Classical vs. Instrumental The modern view is that these two types of learning involve similar

More information

Interval duration effects on blocking in appetitive conditioning

Interval duration effects on blocking in appetitive conditioning Behavioural Processes 71 (2006) 318 329 Interval duration effects on blocking in appetitive conditioning Dómhnall Jennings 1, Kimberly Kirkpatrick Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington,

More information

Bronze statue of Pavlov and one of his dogs located on the grounds of his laboratory at Koltushi Photo taken by Jackie D. Wood, June 2004.

Bronze statue of Pavlov and one of his dogs located on the grounds of his laboratory at Koltushi Photo taken by Jackie D. Wood, June 2004. Ivan Pavlov http://physiologyonline.physiology.org/ cgi/content/full/19/6/326 Bronze statue of Pavlov and one of his dogs located on the grounds of his laboratory at Koltushi Photo taken by Jackie D. Wood,

More information

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts By Erin Marie Fleming

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 4 (Cont.) Indirect Conditioning Applications of Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 4 (Cont.) Indirect Conditioning Applications of Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 (Cont.) Indirect Conditioning Applications of Conditioning Extinction Extinction a method for eliminating a conditioned response. Extinction paradigm: Present the

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

CATS IN SHORTS. Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions. Holger Ursin March Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen.

CATS IN SHORTS. Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions. Holger Ursin March Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen. CATS IN SHORTS Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions Holger Ursin March 2009 Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen Alarm Activation Load Stress Response Stress Stressor Stimuli Stimulus expectancy

More information

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Learning and Motivation 32, 349 366 (2001) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1089, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Charlotte Bonardi

More information

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience.

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience. Chapter 6: Learning Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience. Conditioning = a process in which environmental stimuli and behavioral processes become connected Two types of

More information

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Behavioural Brain Research 110 (2000) 97 108 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Stephen Maren *, William Holt Department of Psychology

More information

Rescorla-Wagner (1972) Theory of Classical Conditioning

Rescorla-Wagner (1972) Theory of Classical Conditioning Rescorla-Wagner (1972) Theory of Classical Conditioning HISTORY Ever since Pavlov, it was assumed that any CS followed contiguously by any US would result in conditioning. Not true: Contingency Not true:

More information

DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D.

DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D. DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D. Psychology Department American University Washington, DC 20016 Phone: 202-885-1725 Email: kearns@american.edu EDUCATION 2005 American University Washington, DC Ph.D., Psychology

More information

Stimulus competition in the absence of compound conditioning

Stimulus competition in the absence of compound conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (1), 3-14 Stimulus competition in the absence of compound conditioning HELENA MATUTE and OSKAR PINEÑO Universidad de Deusto, Bilbao, Spain Most associative theories

More information

Study Plan: Session 1

Study Plan: Session 1 Study Plan: Session 1 6. Practice learning the vocabulary. Use the electronic flashcards from the Classical The Development of Classical : The Basic Principles of Classical Conditioned Emotional Reponses:

More information

Decremental Carryover Effects of Sucrose Ingestion in the Negative Anticipatory Contrast Procedure in Rats

Decremental Carryover Effects of Sucrose Ingestion in the Negative Anticipatory Contrast Procedure in Rats Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1995, Vol. 21, No. 4, 304-317 Copyright 3995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/95/S3.00 Decremental Carryover Effects

More information

Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning

Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning PART II NEUROSCIENCE 1996, UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning Rudolf Cardinal St John s College, Cambridge. Supervised by Dr C.H. Bennett and Professor N.J.

More information

A history of morphine-induced taste aversion learning fails to affect morphine-induced place preference conditioning in rats

A history of morphine-induced taste aversion learning fails to affect morphine-induced place preference conditioning in rats Learn Behav (2013) 41:433 442 DOI 10.3758/s13420-013-0118-6 A history of morphine-induced taste aversion learning fails to affect morphine-induced place preference conditioning in rats Heather E. King

More information

Reference place conditioning procedure with cocaine: Increased sensitivity for measuring associatively motivated choice behavior in rats

Reference place conditioning procedure with cocaine: Increased sensitivity for measuring associatively motivated choice behavior in rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2010 Reference place conditioning procedure with

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition Denniston, James C., Robert P. Cole, and Ralph R. Miller. (1998). "The role of temporal relationships in the transfer of conditioned inhibition." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

More information

Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment

Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment Learning and Motivation 38 (2007) 172 192 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment Oskar Pineño a, *, Jessica M. Zilski a, Todd R. Schachtman b a Dpto.

More information

The attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus that signals an intravenous injection of cocaine

The attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus that signals an intravenous injection of cocaine Behavioural Brain Research 169 (2006) 320 324 Research report The attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus that signals an intravenous injection of cocaine Jason M. Uslaner, Martin J. Acerbo, Samantha

More information

Learning. Learning. Stimulus Learning. Modification of behavior or understanding Is it nature or nurture?

Learning. Learning. Stimulus Learning. Modification of behavior or understanding Is it nature or nurture? Learning Chapter 6 Learning Modification of behavior or understanding Is it nature or nurture? Stimulus Learning Habituation: when you pay less attention to something over time response starts out strong

More information

Abundant research has been devoted to understanding anxiety

Abundant research has been devoted to understanding anxiety Fear Conditioning in Virtual Reality Contexts: A New Tool for the Study of Anxiety Johanna M. Baas, Monique Nugent, Shmuel Lissek, Daniel S. Pine, and Christian Grillon Background: Context conditioning

More information

The development of sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of amphetamine is enhanced in a novel environment

The development of sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of amphetamine is enhanced in a novel environment Psychopharmacology (1995) 117:443-452 Springer-Verlag 1995 Aldo Badiani Stephan G. Anagnostaras Terry E. Robinson The development of sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of amphetamine is

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Using Taste Aversion as a Tool to Explore Context Conditioning and Perceptual Learning. Geoffrey Hall Department of Psychology University of York

Using Taste Aversion as a Tool to Explore Context Conditioning and Perceptual Learning. Geoffrey Hall Department of Psychology University of York Hall 1 Using Taste Aversion as a Tool to Explore Context Conditioning and Perceptual Learning Geoffrey Hall Department of Psychology University of York I want to write about two things here, and neither,

More information

The contribution of latent inhibition to reduced generalization after pre-exposure to the test stimulus

The contribution of latent inhibition to reduced generalization after pre-exposure to the test stimulus Behavioural Processes 71 (2006) 21 28 The contribution of latent inhibition to reduced generalization after pre-exposure to the test stimulus Maria del Carmen Sanjuan a,, Gumersinda Alonso b, James Byron

More information

Conditioned and Unconditioned Behavioral-Cognitive Effects of a Dopamine Antagonist in Rats

Conditioned and Unconditioned Behavioral-Cognitive Effects of a Dopamine Antagonist in Rats Behavioral Neuroscience 2000, Vol. 114. No. 6. 1251-1255 Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0735-70«/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.6.1251 Conditioned and Unconditioned

More information

Sensitization of Salt Appetite Is Associated With Increased Wanting but Not Liking of a Salt Reward in the Sodium-Deplete Rat

Sensitization of Salt Appetite Is Associated With Increased Wanting but Not Liking of a Salt Reward in the Sodium-Deplete Rat Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 120, No. 1, 206 210 0735-7044/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.1.206 Sensitization of Salt Appetite Is Associated

More information

Contextual and behavioral control of antipsychotic sensitization induced by haloperidol and olanzapine

Contextual and behavioral control of antipsychotic sensitization induced by haloperidol and olanzapine University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2012 Contextual and behavioral control of antipsychotic

More information

A response rule for positive and negative stimulus interaction in associative learning and performance

A response rule for positive and negative stimulus interaction in associative learning and performance Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (6, 1115-1124 A response rule for positive and negative stimulus interaction in associative learning and performance Oskar Pineño Hofstra University, Hempstead, New

More information

PSYC 337 LEARNING. Session 4 Classical Conditioning II. Lecturer: Dr. Inusah Abdul-Nasiru Contact Information:

PSYC 337 LEARNING. Session 4 Classical Conditioning II. Lecturer: Dr. Inusah Abdul-Nasiru Contact Information: PSYC 337 LEARNING Session 4 Classical Conditioning II Lecturer: Dr. Inusah Abdul-Nasiru Contact Information: iabdul-nasiru@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education 2014/2015

More information

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Undergraduate Honors Theses 5-2014 Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? Tarra L. Baker Follow

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Chapter 6. Learning: The Behavioral Perspective

Chapter 6. Learning: The Behavioral Perspective Chapter 6 Learning: The Behavioral Perspective 1 Can someone have an asthma attack without any particles in the air to trigger it? Can an addict die of a heroin overdose even if they ve taken the same

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli Charlotte Bonardi 1, Esther Mondragón 2, Ben Brilot 3 & Dómhnall J. Jennings 3 1 School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Centre for Computational

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information