Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque,

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque,"

Transcription

1 Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Differential contributions of hippocampus, amygdala and perirhinal cortex to recognition of novel objects, contextual stimuli and stimulus relationships Sandra N. Moses a,b,, Carrie Cole b, Ira Driscoll c, Jennifer D. Ryan a a Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, 3560 Bathurst St., Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA c Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alta., Canada Received 24 May 2005; received in revised form 24 May 2005; accepted 25 May 2005 Available online 11 July 2005 Abstract This study examined contributions of the hippocampus, amygdala and perirhinal cortex to memory. Rats performed a cover task, and changes to stimulus identity or relationships were used to test incidental memory. Rats with hippocampal damage showed deficient responses to relationship changes, but demonstrated knowledge of the position and identity of the target object. They over-focused on the most predictive stimuli, and failed to acquire associations including surrounding cues. Rats with amygdala damage responded to changes involving distal stimuli, and showed deficient responses to novel objects and object relationships. These rats may be highly reliant on relational representations, resulting in a reduced salience for individual novel stimuli. Rats with perirhinal damaged responded to novel stimulus relationships and distal cues, but showed deficient responses to novel objects, suggesting that changes in identity had reduced salience. Implications for declarative and conjunctive hippocampal theories are discussed Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Learning; Memory; Novelty; Medial temporal; Incidental; Relational 1. Introduction Different neural structures may be involved in learning about different types of stimuli. The current study examines the effects of hippocampal, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions on recognition of changes in proximal objects, distal cues and the relationships among them. Evidence suggests that these structures are differentially involved in learning about stimulus relationships, distal stimulus identity and proximal object identity. Corresponding author. Tel.: x3362; fax: address: smoses@rotman-baycrest.on.ca (S.N. Moses) Hippocampus Theories regarding the role of hippocampal function have implicated this structure in configural, conjunctive and declarative memory. While these theories have subtle differences regarding the nature of the representations that are maintained by the hippocampus, all theories have a common thread in postulating that the hippocampus is critical for learning about relationships among stimuli [24]. An essential feature of this type of learning is that associations among multiple elements are combined to form a unique long-term memory representation [9,11,37 39,44]. Memory for spatial locations often requires relational memory, specifically when the subject uses cues to navigate to a goal from several different start locations [22,30,42] /$ see front matter 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi: /j.brainresbull

2 S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) However, some types of navigational tasks can be nonrelational in nature, and remain intact after hippocampal lesions. For example, rats with fornix lesions show normal performance on the Morris water task if they are released from the same start location and required to swim to the same goal location each time. They are subsequently impaired relative to controls when starting from a novel location. Compared to controls, their behavior is abnormally dependent on the particular cues directly in front of them [12]. This likely occurs because, without a relational representation, animals can only blend the distal cues, as viewed from one vantage point, into a unified representation. A relational representation is often composed of more than just spatial cues. For example, the simultaneous representation of discrete predictive cues and surrounding non-essential contextual information requires an intact hippocampus. Rats with hippocampal damage can learn a simple association between contextual cues and an aversive event, such as a shock. However, if a discrete stimulus is added that immediately precedes the aversive event, such as a tone, rats with hippocampal damage show fear responses to the tone, and not to the contextual cues [32,33]. Thus, rats with hippocampal damage over-focus on the stimulus most directly predictive of the event, and fail to acquire a relational representation including associations involving the surrounding cues. A similar phenomenon occurs with latent inhibition. Latent inhibition occurs when previous exposure to a stimulus results in slower acquisition of conditioned responses to that stimulus when it is paired with a positive or negative outcome. Hippocampal lesion rats show normal latent inhibition for discrete stimuli paired with reward. For example, pre-exposure to stimulus X leads to slower response acquisition rates for stimulus X compared to stimulus Y. However, hippocampal lesions lead to impaired latent inhibition for the stimuli surrounding the rewarded object. For example, following pre-exposure to stimulus X in context A rats with hippocampal lesions do not show normal reductions in response acquisition rates when trained in context A compared to context B [15]. They also fail to show a normal disruption in responding to a rewarded stimulus when transferred to a different context [15]. Again, these results suggest that the rats with hippocampal damage focus only on the cues that are most predictive of outcome, and fail to form a representation including the less-predictive surrounding cues. Taken together, the previous studies support the idea that rats with hippocampal damage lack a relational representation of the environment. Rather, they form a rigid association comprised of the stimulus/stimuli most immediately predictive of outcome. This representation likely excludes the relationships among the predictive stimulus/stimuli and the surrounding cues, the nature/identity of the surrounding cues and the relationship of the surrounding cues to each other. Thus, we anticipated that in a distraction paradigm rats with hippocampal damage would show impaired incidental learning about all cues surrounding those that immediately control their behavior. They were expected to be disrupted following changes to those essential cues, and not following changes in surrounding cues Amygdala The amygdala is important for associating objects or spatial relationships with positive or aversive outcomes in a passive classical conditioning setting [17,20,21,33,43].However, it is unnecessary for remembering information about objects or spatial relationships [1,16,20,28,35,47]. There is evidence that amygdala lesions may lead to subtle deficits in spatial navigation due to an impaired ability to form a meaningful association between a specific location and its predicted valence [23]. The amygdala also appears to mediate fear responses to novel stimuli or neophobia. Rats with amygdala lesions show attenuated neophobia for novel foods [5,10,36,43].In a familiar open-field setting, amygdala damage leads to abnormal responding to a novel proximal object, but exploration of novel distal cues remains unaffected [25]. This pattern of results may occur because the hippocampus can mediate responses to novel contextual information in the absence of an intact amygdala. In fact, the hippocampus and amygdala have competitive interactions for control of behavior. McDonald and White [21] demonstrated that additional relational knowledge of the cues in the test room, acquired in a pre-exposure session, led to slower acquisition of a cue preference task in normal rats. Disruption of the hippocampal system eliminated competition between amygdalaand hippocampal-mediated strategies, and hence rate of task acquisition was enhanced in fornix lesion rats. Conversely, it is possible that animals with amygdala damage may focus more on context and stimulus relationships, rather than on individual proximal items. We expected that rats with amygdala damage would show normal incidental learning about relationships among cues, but may show impaired responding to individual novel proximal objects. We expected them to show normal disruption by the presence of novel stimulus relationships and novel contextual stimuli, as these behaviors may be mediated by the hippocampus. However, abnormal responses were anticipated in response to individual novel objects Perirhinal cortex The perirhinal cortex is important for perception and memory of object features and identity but not spatial arrangements [3 5,7,8,13,18,19,23,27,29,34,46]. However, perirhinal cortex lesions can lead to subtle deficits in memory for spatial locations. This may occur because perirhinal lesions impair processing of cue identity information, which normally makes a non-essential contribution to solving a task that requires the formation of relationships among these cues [6,18,23,27]. It appears that when object identity becomes more important for solving a spatial task the perirhinal cortex becomes more essential to performance (for review,

3 64 S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) see [2]). The perirhinal cortex does not appear to be essential for relational memory [4], unless the only solution to a relational task requires simultaneous discrimination between several objects [23]. We expected that rats with perirhinal cortex lesions would show normal incidental learning about relationships among cues. On the other hand, we expected them to show impaired disruption following changes to identity or form of stimuli Behavioral predictions In the present study, rats were trained on a simple cover task. They were required to displace one object among five to obtain food reward. This is a goal-correlated task, as opposed to incidental, due to the explicit pairing of a specific behavioral response with a desired outcome. However, incidental learning about the non-rewarded cues can be measured using this paradigm. Once the rats acquired this task, different distal and proximal cues and relationships were manipulated as follows. The testing apparatus was rotated 90 relative to the distal cues in the room, and it was placed into a novel room with novel distal cues. These manipulations led to a change in the relationships between the proximal objects and the distal stimuli in the room. A familiar non-rewarded proximal object was replaced with a novel object, leading to a change in proximal stimulus relationships and non-essential object identity information. All of the objects were rearranged, including the target, which led to changes in relationships between the objects and with other proximal and distal stimuli. Object identity remained constant. All of the familiar objects, including the target, were replaced with novel ones, which led to a change in relationships between the objects and other proximal and distal stimuli, as well as changes in object identity information. The degree of disruption following each manipulation was recorded in terms of latency to displace the object, number of contacts with proximal objects prior to displacing the object, number of rears and number of errors. Disruption of task performance was taken as evidence that the animals detected novel stimuli or relationships. Increases in latency to displace the object were interpreted as the animals becoming distracted from the target object by salient changes in stimulus relationships or identity. Contacts with specific objects were interpreted as exploration of the proximal stimuli and rearing as exploration of distal stimuli, as in ref. [25]. Increases in errors were interpreted as evidence that behavior was controlled by several sets of stimuli, such as the stimuli comprising the target object and stimuli surrounding the target object (contextual cues). This could only become apparent when the manipulations led to a novel conflict between previously compatible stimulus arrangements, such as rotation of the apparatus relative to the distal stimuli. Our hypotheses were as follows: (1) animals with hippocampal damage would show abnormal disruption following changes to all contextual stimuli surrounding those cues immediately controlling their behavior, and would be only disrupted following changes to essential stimuli. This means that they would show no change in task performance in response to the rotation, novel room or non-rewarded novel object. They would only be disrupted when all the objects, including the target, are rearranged or replaced. (2) Animals with amygdala damage would show normal disruptions in the presence of novel distal stimuli and distal relationships, but would show abnormal disruption in the presence of novel proximal objects. This means that they would show normal disruption in response to the rotation and novel room, and would show impaired disruption for a novel non-rewarded object, rearrangement of all objects and all novel objects. (3) Animals with perirhinal cortex damage would show normal disruption in the presence of novel distal stimuli and distal relationships, but would show abnormal disruption in the presence of novel proximal objects. This means that they would show normal disruptions following the rotation, and the novel room, and would show impaired disruption for a novel non-rewarded object, rearrangement of all objects and all novel objects. 2. Methods 2.1. Subjects Thirty-two male Long Evans hooded rats were housed individually on a 12-h light:12-h dark cycle. Eight rats were given NMDA neurotoxic lesion of the amygdala, eight were given NMDA neurotoxic lesions of the hippocampus, eight were given NMDA neurotoxic lesions of the perirhinal cortex and eight were given sham surgery. Throughout the duration of testing, rats were maintained at 85% of their original bodyweight Surgery Rats were induced into anesthesia with 4% halothane, 2% oxygen and maintained at 2 2.5% halothane, 2% oxygen. They were placed into a stereotaxic apparatus. The scalp was opened using a scalpel and holes were drilled into the skull. Cannuli were then placed into the holes and lowered into the brain. A 10 mg/ml NMDA in buffered saline solution was injected through the cannuli using a micro infusion pump. For hippocampal lesions, the injection sites in relation to bregma were as follows: (1) anterior 3.1, lateral ±1.5, ventral 3.6; (2) anterior 4.1, lateral ±3.0, ventral 3.0; (3) anterior 5.0, lateral ±3.0, ventral 4.0; (4) anterior 5.3, lateral ±5.2, ventral 7.3; (5) anterior 6.0, lateral ±5.0, ventral 7.3. NMDA was injected at a rate of 0.15 l/min for 4 min. For amygdala lesions, the injection sites relative to bregma were as follows: anterior 2.5, lateral ±4.7, ventral 9.4. NMDA was injected at a rate of l/min for 4 min. For perirhinal cortex lesions, the injection sites relative to bregma were as follows: (1) anterior 5.0, lateral ±5.4, ventral 8.2; (2) anterior 6.8, lateral ±5.4, ventral 7.5.

4 S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) NMDA was injected at a rate of 0.1 l/min for 4 min into site 1 and 3 min into site 2. Cannuli were left in place for 3 4 min following each injection to allow the NMDA to diffuse into the tissue. The cannuli were then removed. The incision was closed using wound clips. Rats were given 0.2 cm 3 of benzodiazepine upon waking and then 0.1 cm 3 as needed to control seizures. 0.1 cm 3 morphine was given every 4 h to provide analgesia Histology All the rats were anesthetized with halothane, and were perfused intracardially with saline followed by a 10% formalin solution. The brains were removed and stored in a 10% formalin solution for 1 week. They were then placed in a formalin, 20% sucrose solution for 48 h. Using a cryostat they were sectioned coronally at 40 m and stained with cresyl violet. A microscopic examination was performed in order to assess the location and size of the lesions, in a similar manner to previous work [45] Behavioral testing Apparatus Rats were placed on a plastic cart (71.5 cm 64 cm 80 cm) which had outer edges extending 7 cm high. The cart was placed in a room in which all the distal cues and the relationships among them were fixed. The cart remained in a fixed position relative to the cues in the room throughout training. Five different plastic objects (about cm across and cm high) were placed along one of the long edges of the cart. Objects were placed about 8 cm apart. All objects contained a hole at the bottom, but were solid on top and one all sides (see Fig. 1). Duct tape was placed on one side of each Fig. 1. Schematic of room in which incidental learning experiment was conducted. Black shapes on the cart represent the objects used. object, on the outside facing away from the rat, connecting it to the cart. This was done in order to secure one side of the objects onto the cart, so that the objects could be tipped over but not moved. During the first week of training, an additional piece of duct tape was placed on the inside of all but one of the objects. This piece was placed across from the first piece of tape and secured to the cart, so that the objects could not be knocked over or moved, and the tape could not be seen. A small piece of Froot Loops cereal was attached to the top of the inside of each object where the rats could not reach it. This ensured that all of the objects smelled like Froot Loops. A radio was played in the room throughout the experiment to provide background noise Procedure For each rat, one of the five objects always had one accessible piece of Froot Loops cereal underneath it, and the other four never did (although all objects contained an inaccessible piece of cereal as described earlier). Rats were randomly assigned one of two objects as their correct object. For half of the rats, food was always under the second object from the left (object 2) and for the other half, food was always under the second objects from the right (object 4). Objects remained in a constant position throughout training. Each rat was placed on the cart at the center of the long side of the cart, opposite to the row of objects. They were placed on the cart with their back to the objects, and given 120 s to knock over the correct object in order to obtain food. Latency to knock over the object was recorded, as well as which objects the animals contacted, number of contacts and number of rears. Contacts with the object included touching of the objects with the forepaws or snout. Rearing consisted of the standing on the hind legs with forepaws in the air, and head tilted upwards. If the rat failed to knock over the correct object in 120 s, the object was knocked over by the experimenter. The rat was then given 60 s to eat the food, and then returned to their cage. Each rat was given 10 such trials per day. In between each trial for each rat, the objects and the cart were cleaned with a 0.1% alcohol solution. During the first week of training, only the correct object could be knocked over. Following this period, all objects could be knocked over, but only one contained accessible food. If the rat knocked over an incorrect object, it was immediately removed from the cart and placed in its cage. Number of correct responses for each rat was recorded. Rats were run in squads of eight animals. Each squad contained two rats with amygdala lesions, two with hippocampal lesions, two with perirhinal cortex lesions and two with sham lesions. Once each squad obtained an average correct responses/day of over 99%, and an average latency to knock over the object of below 10 s, different cue manipulations were carried out. (1) The cart was rotated 90 relative to the distal cues in the room, but the relationships among the proximal objects relative to each other and the cart remained constant. (2) The cart was placed in a new room that contained

5 66 S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) entirely new distal cues. The relationships among the objects relative to each other and the cart remained constant. (3) The center object (object 3), which was a red triangle, was replaced with a novel object, a slightly larger green cylinder. (4) The positions of the objects were rearranged pseudorandomly on each trial. (5) All of the objects were replaced with novel objects whose positions were pseudo-randomly varied on every trial Rotation, novel room and novel object For the first three manipulations, each change was maintained for all 10 trials on that day, and then on the following day all of the cues were returned to their original state, and each rat was given only 5 trials. If the average latency per squad was above 10 s on this day, an additional day of five trials with the cues in their original state was added. parietal cortex (Fig. 2). One rat suffered an infarct, which caused right visual cortex, right subicular cortex and right retrosplenial damage, as well as thinning of overlying cortex on the right. This animal, however, had 100% bilateral anterior, 100% left posterior and 50% right posterior hippocampal damage, and was not removed from the data analysis Amygdala The average lesion involved 65% of the anterior portion, 62% of the posterior portion and 64% of the entire amygdala. Some lesions included one of the following additional sights of damage: slight ventral caudate damage, slight disruption All objects rearranged For this manipulation the object that had been correct throughout training (either two or four, depending on assignment) remained the correct choice regardless of its location. Thus, reward was predicted by object identity and not position. For the first trial on this day, rats were able to knock down all of the objects. However, for the remaining 9 trials, and for all 10 trials on the next day, only the correct object could be knocked over. On the following day, all objects could be knocked over, and correct choice was recorded. On the next day all objects were returned to their original position and remained there for all five trials All new objects In this case, the position where the correct object had been throughout training was the correct choice. Thus, object position predicted reward, regardless of object form. For the first trial on this day, rats were able to knock down all of the objects. However, for the remaining 9 trials, and for all 10 trials on the next 3 days, only the correct object could be knocked over. The rats were then given 5 days of 10 trials on which all objects could be knocked over. 3. Results 3.1. Histology Hippocampus One of the eight animals that received hippocampal lesions was removed from the data analysis. This animal had an asymmetrical lesion encompassing lateral temporal and perirhinal cortices, right lateral occipital cortex and 20% of CA1 cells on the right side. The majority of the hippocampus was spared bilaterally. For the seven remaining animals, the average lesion involved 90% of the anterior portion, 78% of the posterior portion and 84% of the entire hippocampus. The largest lesions included damage to the entorhinal cortex, ventral subicular damage and thinning of the posterior Fig. 2. Examples of: (a) hippocampus, (b) amygdala and (c) perirhinal cortex lesions.

6 of left stria terminalis, right pyriform damage and very slight thinning of the ventral bank of the perirhinal cortex. Some lesions included sparing of the lateral nucleus, left endopyriform nucleus and right medial nucleus (Fig. 2). S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Perirhinal cortex The average lesion included 61% of the anterior portion, 68% of the posterior portion and 61% of the entire perirhinal cortex. Larger lesions included slight CA1 damage, damage to the corner of left posterior caudate, pyriform damage, TE3 and partial TE1 damage, ventral subiculum damage, entorhinal damage, ventral hippocampal damage and overlying cortical damage (Fig. 2) Behavioral results Interpretation of the different measures Increases in latency to displace the object were interpreted to suggest that the rats were distracted from the task by a particular manipulation. Contacts with the objects were interpreted as exploration directed at specific objects. Rearing was interpreted as exploration directed at distal cues. Decreases in the number of correct responses on the task following a manipulation were interpreted as a conflict between different sets of stimuli controlling behavior. For example, if behavior was controlled by an object and a set of distal cues, and when those distal cues changed position relative to the object the rats failed to correctly select the rewarded object, this suggests that the rats continued to respond to the reward contingency of the distal cues rather than to the object. The rats showed very little rearing in the current experiment and few manipulations elicited rearing. Therefore, rearing results are presented merely for descriptive purposes, and are not statistically quantified. Similarly, rats usually showed performance accuracies of 100% correct responses, and few manipulations lead to errors. Thus, error results are also presented for descriptive purposes only and not statistically quantified. The measures of latency and contacts with the objects were sensitive to all five manipulations. This leaves two variables and five different manipulations, which yields 10 overall ANOVAs. The conservative Bonferroni corrected p value thus becomes Most of the comparisons yielded Fig. 3. Correct responses on the first trial of the incidental learning task. All groups acquired this task with nearly 100% accuracy. Following the rotation of the cart relative to the distal cues the rats with sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions showed a decrease in number of correct responses. The rats with hippocampal lesions did not make any errors. Following the rearrangement of all the familiar objects, including the goal object, all groups showed a decrease in number of correct responses. Following the replacement of all the familiar objects with novel ones, including the goal object, all groups showed a decrease in the number of correct responses. p values of below However, since this is a rather stringent test for significance, we present all p values below 0.05 as well, but point out that these comparisons exceeded the allowable value according to the Bonferonni correction Baseline All groups acquired the task similarly, and performed with nearly 100% accuracy (see Fig. 3). Decreases in latency to displace the object and in the number of contacts with the objects occurred from the first 5 days to the last 5 days, and from the first trial to the last trial on each day (see Tables 1 and 2 for overall ANOVA). None of the groups showed any rearing on the last 5 days before the rotation, suggesting that they were not exploring the distal cues Rotation The rotation of the apparatus relative to the distal cues entailed a change in the relationships among the proximal objects on the apparatus and the distal cues in the room. This change led to a conflict of distal and proximal information for solving the task. That is, the location of the rewarded object Table 1 Overall ANOVA for contacts with the objects following each manipulation compared to the previous day over the first five trials Latency Habituation Rotation Novel room Novel object Rearrangement All new objects Group d.f. = 3,27 N.S ** ** N.S. N.S. N.S. Day d.f. = 1, ** ** ** N.S ** ** Trial d.f. = 4, ** ** ** N.S ** ** Group day d.f. = 3,27 N.S ** ** N.S. N.S. N.S. Group trial d.f. = 12,108 N.S ** ** N.S. N.S. N.S. Day trial d.f. = 4, ** ** ** N.S ** ** Group trial day d.f. = 4,108 N.S ** ** N.S. N.S. N.S. ** Denotes significance at p < in accordance with Bonferroni correction.

7 68 S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Table 2 Overall ANOVA for latency to displace the object following each manipulation compared to the previous day over the first five trials Contacts Habituation Rotation Novel room Novel object Rearrangement All new objects Group d.f. = 3,27 N.S ** N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. Day d.f. = 1, ** N.S ** ** ** ** Trial d.f. = 4, ** ** ** * ** ** Group day d.f. = 3,27 N.S. N.S. N.S N.S. N.S. N.S. Group day d.f. = 12,108 N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. Day trial d.f. = 4, ** ** N.S. N.S ** ** Group trial day d.f. = 4,108 N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. * Denotes significance at p < ** Denotes significance at p < in accordance with Bonferroni correction. was different relative to the distal cues, but not relative to the proximal cues. This manipulation permitted an investigation of the extent to which the animal s behavior was controlled by the distal proximal cue relationships, the distal cues alone or the proximal cues. Following the rotation of the cart relative to the cues in the room, performance of the sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesion groups was disrupted, suggested that these rats detected the change in stimulus relationships. These groups showed increases in latency to displace the object, contacts with the objects (Fig. 4) and incorrect responses (Fig. 3). In contrast, the hippocampal damaged group showed very little or no disruption on these measures (Table 3; see Tables 1 and 2 for overall ANOVA). Fig. 4. Behavioral changes in response to the rotation of the apparatus relative to the distal cue. Differences between rotation and baseline 1 are represented in (a and b). Rats with sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions showed significant increases in (a) latency to displace the object and (b) contacts with the objects on the first trial, but rats with hippocampal lesions did not show the same increase as the other groups. Follow-up ANOVA comparing performance on only the first trial following the manipulations revealed increased contacts with the objects (F(1,27) = , p = ) and increased latencies to knock over the target object (F(1,27) = , p = ). However, all groups were not disrupted to the same extent, as illustrated by significant group by time interactions (contacts: F(3,27) = 4.731, p = 0.009; latency: F(3,27) = 8.015, p = 0.001). Rats with sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions were disrupted to a greater extent than the rats with hippocampal lesions on the first trial following the rotation compared to the first trial on the previous day. Contrasts revealed that rats with hippocampal lesions showed greatly attenuated changes in contacts, latency and correct responses, suggesting a failure to react to the novel stimulus relationships. All groups, except for the hippocampal group, showed a significant increase in contacts on the first trial following the rotation (sham: t(1,7) = 3.366, p = 0.012; amygdala: t(1,7) = 5.426, p = 0.001; perirhinal cortex: t(1,7) = 5.346; p = 0.001; hippocampus: t(1,6) = 1.648, p = 0.150). All groups had significantly higher latencies following rotation (all t s < 2.0). However, contrasts comparing latency of the rats with hippocampal damage to that of the shams revealed a significantly lower latency in the hippocampal group following (t(1,27) = 3.750, p = 0.005), but not prior to (t(1,27) = 1.155, p = 0.275) the rotation. Only sham and amygdala lesion rats reared following the rotation. Following the rotation the sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesion groups showed a reduction in correct responses during the first trial following the rotation. In contrast, no rat with hippocampal damage made a single incorrect response (Fig. 3). Thus, the performance of rats with hippocampal lesions was significantly less disrupted by the rotation than that of the other groups. In summary, the rats with amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions showed increases in latency and contacts on measures of memory for the distal cues and distal cue relationships. These rats were affected by the conflicting information provided by the distal and proximal cues, demonstrated by increased number of errors. In contrast, the rats with hippocampal damage showed impairments on all measures of memory for the distal cues and distal proximal cue relationships, and were not affected by conflicting

8 S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Table 3 Summary of behavior of each lesion group compared to sham group following each manipulation Group Measure Rotation Novel room Novel object Rearrange all objects All novel objects Hippocampus Latency Faster Faster Normal trial 1; Normal faster trial 1 5 Contacts Fewer Fewer Fewer Normal Normal trial 1; fewer all 10 Errors Fewer (0) Normal Normal Amygdala Latency Normal Normal Normal Faster Contacts Normal Normal Fewer trial 1; Fewer More more trial 1 5 Errors Normal Normal Normal Perirhinal Latency Normal Normal Normal Faster Contacts Normal Normal Normal trial 1; Normal Normal more trial 1 5 Errors Normal Normal Normal Rats with hippocampal damage over-focused on the target object, and showed normal disruptions only following manipulations that involved that specific object. Rats with amygdala damage may have been biased to use a more relational strategy than sham rats, as they were disrupted following manipulations involving the distal cues and distal relationships, but not following changes to proximal objects. Rat with perirhinal damage showed mild abnormalities for object memory, as they were disrupted following all distal and proximal relational manipulations, but showed abnormalities in the presence of one novel object or all novel objects. Normal = same as shams; Faster = shorter latency than shams; Fewer = fewer contacts/errors than shams; More = more contacts than shams. information provided by proximal objects and the distal stimuli. Interestingly, these rats did show a small, extremely attenuated, yet significant increase in latency to displace the object. This finding suggests that they did detect the rotation, although they did not respond to it in a manner that resembled the response of the other groups. Thus, these rats were focusing on the objects rather than on the surrounding contextual stimuli and their relationships to the objects Novel room The placement of the apparatus in a novel room with novel distal cues entailed a change in the relationships among the proximal objects on the apparatus and the distal stimuli in the room, but did not lead to conflicting information between distal and proximal stimuli. This manipulation permitted an investigation of the extent to which the animal noticed changes in distal proximal relationships and changes to the distal stimulus identity. Performance of the sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesion rats was disrupted following the placement of the cart in the novel room, suggesting that these rats detected the change in distal stimulus identity and distal proximal stimulus relationships. These groups showed increased latency to displace the object and an increase in number of contacts with the objects (Fig. 5). In contrast, the hippocampal lesion group showed an attenuated disruption on all measures (Table 3; see Tables 1 and 2 for overall ANOVAs). Additionally, the amygdala damaged rats showed some rearing. Rats with sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions were disrupted to a greater extent than the rats with hippocampal lesions on the first trial in the novel room compared to the first trial on the previous day, suggesting that the novel stimuli and relationships had reduced salience for the rats with hippocampal damage. ANOVA revealed that all groups showed increased contacts with the objects (F(1,27) = , p = ), and increased latencies to displace the target object F(1,27) = , p = ). Fig. 5. Behavioral changes in response to the placement of the cart into a novel room. Differences between novel room and baseline 2 are represented in (a and b). Rats with sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions showed a significant increase in latencies to complete the task on the first trial, but rats with hippocampal lesions did not show the same increase as the other groups. Rats with sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions showed increases in contacts with the objects on the first trial and over the first five trials, but rats with hippocampal lesions did not.

9 70 S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) However, the different groups were not affected in the same way, illustrated by significant latency by group (F(3,27) = 6.522, p = 0.002). Contrasts revealed that all groups, except for the hippocampal group, had significantly higher latencies in the novel room (sham: t(1,7) = 8.725, p = ; amygdala: t(1,7) = 4.752, p = 0.002; perirhinal cortex: t(1,7) = 4.537, p = 0.003; hippocampus: t(1,6) = 1.784, p = 0.125). The hippocampal group also did not rear at all, in contrast to the other groups showing some rearing. Correct responses were unaffected by the novel room (Fig. 3). Thus, the performance of the hippocampal lesion rats was less disrupted in the novel room compared to the other groups. In summary, the rats with sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions showed increases in latency and number of contacts with the objects on measures of memory for the distal stimulus identity and distal proximal relationships. In contrast, the rats with hippocampal damage showed impairments on all measures of memory for the distal stimulus identity and distal proximal relationships Novel object The replacement of a familiar (non-essential) object with a novel one caused a subtle change in the distal proximal cue relationships, a change in the proximal proximal cue relationships and a change in object identity information. This manipulation may have been less salient than the others, since it did not elicit increases in latency to displace the object. Rather, the sham lesion rats showed an increased number of contacts with all of the objects, and specifically with the novel object. Rats with amygdala and perirhinal cortex damage showed abnormal contacts, and rats with hippocampal damage showed the most severely impaired contacts (Fig. 6; Table 3). Latency to displace the object, rearing and correct responses were unaffected (see Tables 1 and 2 for overall ANOVAs). Overall, the groups showed increased contacts with the novel object during only the first trial, compared to the previous day, illustrated by a significant main effect of the manipulation on contacts (F(1,27) = , p = 0.003). However, rats with hippocampal and amygdala lesions showed a different pattern of contacts than the sham rats. Planned comparisons revealed that rats with hippocampal damage contacted all the objects fewer times than all the other groups combined or than the shams alone (t s>2, p s < 0.05), but this was not the case on the previous day (t s > 1). In addition, unlike the rats with sham and perirhinal cortex lesions ( t s < 2), rats with amygdala and hippocampus lesions did not show increased contacts with the novel object compared to familiar object on the day before (t s < 1). We also investigated the behavior of the rats during the first five trials with the novel object compared to the previous day. All lesion groups showed different changes in behavior compared to the sham group. Sham (F(1,7) = 8.758, p = 0.021) and amygdala (F(1,7) = , p = 0.009) lesion rats showed Fig. 6. Behavioral changes in response to the replacement of a familiar, nonessential object with a novel object. Differences between novel object and baseline 3 are represented in (a c). No groups showed increases in latency to displace the object (a). Contacts with all the objects are represented in (b; note smaller scale). Rats with sham and amygdala lesions showed significant increases in contact with all the objects during the first trial and over all five trials. Rats with perirhinal cortex lesions did not show increased contacts over the first five trials. Rat with hippocampal lesions did not show increased contacts during the first trial or over the first five trials. Contacts with only the novel objects are represented in (c). Rats with sham and perirhinal cortex lesions showed significant increases in contacts with the novel object on the first trial. Rats with amygdala and hippocampal damage did not. Rats with sham and hippocampal lesions did not show signifcant increases in contact with the novel object over the first five trials. Rats with amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions showed increases in contacts with the novel object over the first five trials. increased contacts with all the objects, but perirhinal cortex and hippocampal lesion rats did not (note that these p values are above the Bonferroni correction). In addition, contacts with only the novel object were compared to contacts with only the familiar object in the same position on the previous day. The rats with amygdala (F(1,7) = , p = 0.008) and perirhinal cortex (F(1,7) = , p = 0.002) lesions showed increased contacts with the novel object over the first five trials, but the sham and hippocampal groups did not (note that the p value for the amygdala group is above the Bonferonni correction).

10 S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) In summary, the rats with amygdala damage did not show increased contacts with the novel object comparable to the shams during the first trial, but showed more contacts than the shams during the first five trials. The perirhinal cortex lesion group showed increased contacts with the novel object over the first five trials, and did not show an increase in contacts with all the objects, in contrast to the sham group. The rats with hippocampal damage failed to show increased contacts with both the novel object and with all the objects during the first trial or the first five trials. Thus, rat with amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions show abnormal exploration following the novel object, and the rats with hippocampal damage show the most severely impaired exploration. The performance of the rats with hippocampal damage appears to be abnormally reliant on the specific rewarded object, as the novel object did not lead to any disruption. Rats with amygdala damage may be less sensitive compared to the shams to the presence of a novel objects, as they respond to it later than the sham rats. Rats with perirhinal cortex initially respond to the novel object normally, however likely due to deficits in object perception and memory, they continue to explore it for longer than the shams Rearrangement of objects The rearrangement of all the familiar objects caused changes in the proximal distal and proximal proximal cue relationships. It also entailed a change in the relationship of the rewarded target object to all the other objects and to the distal cues. Identity of the object remained constant. Following the rearrangement of all familiar objects all groups showed increased latency to displace the object and contacts with the objects (Fig. 7; see Tables 1 and 2 for overall ANOVAs), suggesting that they were distracted by the novel relationships and re-explored the objects in their novel positions. However, rats with amygdala damage showed an abnormal change in contacts with the objects, suggesting abnormal exploration, and rats with hippocampal damage did not show a comparable change in latency to the other groups, suggesting they were less distracted by the novel relationships (Table 3). All groups showed comparable increases in number of incorrect responses (F(1,27) = , p < ) (Fig. 3). Rearing was unaffected. On the first trial with the object rearranged compared to the previous day, the sham, perirhinal cortex and hippocampal groups showed an increased number of contacts with the objects (all t s < 2.5, p s = ; note these p values are above the Bonferroni correction of 0.005). In contrast, the amygdala group damage failed to show an increased number of contacts (t(1,7) = 1.758, p = 0.122). We also examined behavior during the first five trials with the objects rearranged compared to the previous day. The hippocampal group did not show a normal increase in latency over the first five trials. This was illustrated by repeated measures ANOVAs conducted separately for each group that found significant time by trial interactions for Fig. 7. Behavioral changes in response to rearrangement of all the familiar objects, including the goal object. Differences between rearrangement of objects and baseline 4 are represented in (a and b). (a) All groups showed significant increases in latency to displace the object on the first trial and on the first five trials. (b) Rats with sham, perirhinal cortex and hippocampal lesions showed significant increases in contacts with the objects on the first trial and on all five trials. Rats with amygdala lesions did not show increased contacts on the first trial, but did show significantly increased contacts on the first five trials. the sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex group (all F s > 3, p s = ; note that these values are above the Bonferroni correction of 0.005), but not the hippocampal group. Rats with hippocampal lesions may not have been as distracted as the other groups, since they performed the task faster. All groups acquired this new version of the task, with object identity regardless of position predicting reward, and performed it with the same degree of accuracy. On the third day of training with object form predicting reward and object location varied pseudo-randomly from trial to trial, one-way ANOVAs comparing mean performance among all the groups revealed that the groups did not differ on number of contacts, latency, rearing or correct responses (all F s < 1). In summary, all the groups were disrupted, suggesting they had some knowledge of the position of the rewarded object relative to the proximal and/or distal cues in the room. The group with hippocampal damage was less distracted by the novel proximal relationships than the sham group. The group with amygdala lesions showed less exploration of the novel proximal relationships than the sham group, suggesting that the salience of the proximal objects may be reduced. The group with perirhinal cortex lesions did not differ from the sham group. All groups demonstrated knowledge of the identity of the rewarded target object, by learning to solve the task correctly.

11 72 S.N. Moses et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) All new objects The replacement of all the familiar objects with novel ones entailed a change in the proximal distal and proximal proximal cue relationships, as well as a change in the identity of the objects. Position at which reward was located relative to the apparatus and the room remained constant. On the first trial, following the replacement of all the familiar objects with novel ones compared to the previous day, all groups showed increased latencies to complete the task, and number of contacts with the objects (Fig. 8; see Tables 1 and 2 for overall ANOVAs), suggesting that they were distracted by, and explored, the novel objects. All groups showed increases in number of errors (Fig. 3). However, the groups with amygdala, perirhinal cortex and hippocampal lesions differed from the sham rats on some of these measures (Table 3). The amygdala group showed more exploration of the novel items, demonstrated by a greater number of contacts with the novel objects averaged over all 10 trials than all other groups combined (F(1,27) = 3.228, p = 0.003); although this effect was not significant when compared to the sham group alone. This effect is also illustrated by a repeated measures ANOVA comparing the groups mean number of contacts over all 10 trials with the novel objects and with the familiar objects on the previous day. This analysis yielded a significant Fig. 8. Behavioral changes in response to the replacement of all the familiar objects, including the goal object, with novel objects. Differences between replacement of objects and baseline 5 are represented in (a and b). (a) Rats with sham, amygdala and perirhinal cortex lesions showed significant increases in latency to displace the object on the first trial and on all five trials. Rats with perirhinal cortex lesions did not show increased latency on the first trial, but did show significantly increased latency on the first five trials. (b) All groups showed significant increases in contacts with the objects on all trials. Rats with hippocampal lesions showed significantly fewer mean contacts than the sham operated rats and rats with amygdala lesions showed a significantly greater number of mean contacts than the sham operated rats. interaction of group by day (F(3,27) = 6.306, p = 0.002). In addition, the rats with amygdala lesions were less distracted by the novel stimuli, as they did not show a normal increase in latency during the first trial. This effect is illustrated by repeated measures ANOVAs, conducted separately for each group, comparing the latency on first five trials with the new objects to the previous day. These analyses yielded a significant main effect of trial (F(4,28) = 3.559, p = 0.018), and a significant time by trial interaction (F(4,28) = 3.962, p = 0.011), for the amygdala lesion group (note that these p values are above the Bonferonni correction of 0.005). These effects were absent in all of the other groups. Rats with perirhinal cortex lesions failed to show an increased latency during the first trial (t(1,7) = 1.648, p = 0.121), suggesting that they may have been less distracted by the presence of the novel objects. All the other groups showed this increase in latency (sham: t(7) = 0.005; amygdala: t(7) = 0.026; hippocampus t(6) = 0.022; note that p values for the amygdala and hippocampus groups are above the Bonferonni correction of 0.005). Rats with hippocampal lesions showed less exploration of the novel objects, demonstrated by fewer mean contacts with the objects over all 10 trials than the sham group (t(1,27) = 2.957, p = 0.018) and than all other groups combined (t(1,27) = 3.240, p = 0.016; note these p values are above the Bonferroni correction of 0.005). This effect is also illustrated by the repeated measures ANOVA comparing the groups mean number of contacts over all 10 trials with the novel objects and with the familiar objects on the previous day. As described above, this analysis yielded a significant interaction of group by day (F(3,27) = 6.306, p = 0.002). Sham and perirhinal cortex lesion rats did not rear, but amygdala and hippocampal lesion rats showed some rearing, suggesting that these groups were exploring the distal contextual stimuli. Thus, all groups were disrupted by this manipulation. However, rats with hippocampal damage explored the objects less, as they showed fewer than normal mean contacts over all 10 trials, but did show some rearing. Rats with amygdala damage showed a greater than normal number of mean contacts over all 10 trials, and rearing, suggesting that they were exploring the relationships between the novel objects and the distal contextual cues more than the other groups. Rats with perirhinal cortex damage were less distracted by the novel objects, as they did not show an increased latency during the first trial following the replacement of all the objects. Thus, all of the lesion groups showed abnormalities in responding to the replacement of all the familiar objects with novel ones. All the groups were able to acquire and to perform the new version of the task, with position regardless of object identity predicting reward, with the same degree of accuracy. On the final day of training with object location predicting reward regardless of form, one-way ANOVAs comparing mean performance among all the groups revealed that the groups did not differ on number of contacts, latency, rearing or correct responses (all F s < 1).

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/330/6009/1408/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Paradoxical False Memory for Objects After Brain Damage Stephanie M. McTighe, Rosemary A. Cowell, Boyer D. Winters,

More information

Supplementary Methods

Supplementary Methods 1 Supplementary Methods Social Preference Test Subjects Seventy-four Long-Evans, male rats served as subjects (S-rats) in the foodpreference test, with 40 assigned to the CXT-Same (CXT-S) Condition and

More information

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT ACTA NEUROBIOL: EXP. 1988, 48: 117-121 Short communication LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT W. Jeffrey WILSON and Jennifer C. HALL Department of Psychological

More information

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats Learning and Memory PREVIOUS work has provided evidence of a role for the hippocampal formation in contextual as opposed to cue conditioning. Similar deficits have been observed after transection of the

More information

Limbic system outline

Limbic system outline Limbic system outline 1 Introduction 4 The amygdala and emotion -history - theories of emotion - definition - fear and fear conditioning 2 Review of anatomy 5 The hippocampus - amygdaloid complex - septal

More information

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

CANTAB Test descriptions by function CANTAB Test descriptions by function The 22 tests in the CANTAB battery may be divided into the following main types of task: screening tests visual memory tests executive function, working memory and

More information

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Hippocampal Pyramidal Neuron of Mice and Rats Figure 1 Open Field Apparatus Open Field Behavior

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Types and Structure of Memory Types of Memory Type of Memory Time Course Capacity Conscious Awareness Mechanism of Loss Sensory Short-Term and Working Long-Term Nondeclarative

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html Memory Working Memory Long-term Memory Declarative Memory Procedural Memory

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

Anterograde and retrograde memory for object discriminations and places in rats with perirhinal cortex lesions

Anterograde and retrograde memory for object discriminations and places in rats with perirhinal cortex lesions Behavioural Brain Research 114 (2000) 119 134 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Anterograde and retrograde memory for object discriminations and places in rats with perirhinal cortex lesions

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and 77 5 Discussion The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and implicit learning systems. Chapter 2 described the effects of performing a working memory task

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

Integrated Memory for Object, Place, and Context in Rats: A Possible Model of Episodic-Like Memory?

Integrated Memory for Object, Place, and Context in Rats: A Possible Model of Episodic-Like Memory? 1948 The Journal of Neuroscience, February 25, 2004 24(8):1948 1953 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Integrated Memory for Object, Place, and Context in Rats: A Possible Model of Episodic-Like Memory? Madeline

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal A. Project Description Objective of research: The objective of this study is to determine if hippocampal dopamine D1 receptors facilitate novel object

More information

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and Gallo Taste Learning and Memory in Aging Milagros Gallo, PhD Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and in humans involves conscious recall. 1 Visual recognition memory is

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

A systems neuroscience approach to memory

A systems neuroscience approach to memory A systems neuroscience approach to memory Critical brain structures for declarative memory Relational memory vs. item memory Recollection vs. familiarity Recall vs. recognition What about PDs? R-K paradigm

More information

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Behavioural Brain Research 110 (2000) 97 108 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Stephen Maren *, William Holt Department of Psychology

More information

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature,

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature, Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature, Vol. 435, pp. 1102-7. Sander Vaus 22.04.2015 The study

More information

/typeset2:/sco4/jobs2/elsevier/bbr/week.34/pbbr Tue Sep 11 07:37: Page Tue ARTICLE IN PRESS

/typeset2:/sco4/jobs2/elsevier/bbr/week.34/pbbr Tue Sep 11 07:37: Page Tue ARTICLE IN PRESS Behavioural Brain Research 000 (2001) 000 000 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Perspectives on object-recognition memory following hippocampal damage: lessons from studies in rats Dave G. Mumby

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Henry Molaison From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Henry Gustav Molaison (February 26, 1926 December 2, 2008), known widely as H.M., was an American memory disorder patient who had a bilateral medial

More information

Intact negative patterning in rats with fornix or combined perirhinal and postrhinal cortex lesions

Intact negative patterning in rats with fornix or combined perirhinal and postrhinal cortex lesions Exp Brain Res (2000) 134:506 519 DOI 10.1007/s002210000481 RESEARCH ARTICLE Timothy J. Bussey Rebecca Dias Edward S. Redhead John M. Pearce Janice L. Muir John P. Aggleton Intact negative patterning in

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Dissociable Effects of Lesions to the Perirhinal Cortex and the Postrhinal Cortex on Memory for Context and Objects in Rats

Dissociable Effects of Lesions to the Perirhinal Cortex and the Postrhinal Cortex on Memory for Context and Objects in Rats Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 119, No. 2, 557 566 0735-7044/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.2.557 Dissociable Effects of Lesions to the

More information

Complexity Made Simple

Complexity Made Simple Complexity Made Simple Bussey-Saksida Touch Screen Chambers for Rats and Mice Tasks translate directly to well established monkey and human touch models e.g. CANTAB High throughput screening of multiple

More information

How do Fornix-Fimbria Lesions Affect One-Way Active Avoidance Behavior?

How do Fornix-Fimbria Lesions Affect One-Way Active Avoidance Behavior? How do Fornix-Fimbria Lesions Affect One-Way Active Avoidance Behavior? Cem Kaner, Bob Osborne, Harvey Anchel, Mark Hammer & Abraham H. Black McMaster University 86 th Annual Convention of the American

More information

Pattern Memory Involves Both Elemental and Configural Processes: Evidence From the Effects of Hippocampal Lesions

Pattern Memory Involves Both Elemental and Configural Processes: Evidence From the Effects of Hippocampal Lesions Behavioral Neuroscience 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 125, No. 4, 567 577 0735-7044/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0023762 Pattern Memory Involves Both Elemental and Configural Processes:

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival Threats are much better reinforcers Fear is a prime

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu www.cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html n Learning and Memory n Human Communication n Emotion 1 What is memory? n Working Memory:

More information

Introduction to Systems Neuroscience. Nov. 28, The limbic system. Daniel C. Kiper

Introduction to Systems Neuroscience. Nov. 28, The limbic system. Daniel C. Kiper Introduction to Systems Neuroscience Nov. 28, 2017 The limbic system Daniel C. Kiper kiper@ini.phys.ethz.ch http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html LIMBIC SYSTEM The term limbic system mean

More information

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning Chapter 6: Learning 1 Learning 1. In perception we studied that we are responsive to stimuli in the external world. Although some of these stimulus-response associations are innate many are learnt. 2.

More information

Visual Memory Any neural or behavioural phenomenon implying storage of a past visual experience. E n c o d i n g. Individual exemplars:

Visual Memory Any neural or behavioural phenomenon implying storage of a past visual experience. E n c o d i n g. Individual exemplars: Long-term Memory Short-term Memory Unconscious / Procedural Conscious / Declarative Working Memory Iconic Memory Visual Memory Any neural or behavioural phenomenon implying storage of a past visual experience.

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A Hippocampus Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D. Common (Distributed) Model of Memory Processes Time Course of Memory Processes Long Term Memory DECLARATIVE NON-DECLARATIVE Semantic Episodic Skills

More information

Deficits in Attentional Orienting Following Damage to the Perirhinal or Postrhinal Cortices

Deficits in Attentional Orienting Following Damage to the Perirhinal or Postrhinal Cortices Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 118, No. 5, 1117 1122 0735-7044/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.1117 Deficits in Attentional Orienting

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni VISUAL AGNOSIA -damage to the extrastriate visual regions (occipital, parietal and temporal lobes) disrupts recognition of complex visual stimuli

More information

Interaction between the Postsubiculum and Anterior Thalamus in the Generation of Head Direction Cell Activity

Interaction between the Postsubiculum and Anterior Thalamus in the Generation of Head Direction Cell Activity The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1, 1997, 17(23):9315 9330 Interaction between the Postsubiculum and Anterior Thalamus in the Generation of Head Direction Cell Activity Jeremy P. Goodridge and Jeffrey

More information

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

A model of the interaction between mood and memory INSTITUTE OF PHYSICS PUBLISHING NETWORK: COMPUTATION IN NEURAL SYSTEMS Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 12 (2001) 89 109 www.iop.org/journals/ne PII: S0954-898X(01)22487-7 A model of the interaction between

More information

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions Behavioral Neuroscience 1996, Vol. 110, No. 5, 933-945 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/96/S3.00 Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal

More information

ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING. Felipe Schiffino

ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING. Felipe Schiffino ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING by Felipe Schiffino A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in partial

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

The Role of CA1 in the Acquisition of an Object Trace Odor Paired Associate Task

The Role of CA1 in the Acquisition of an Object Trace Odor Paired Associate Task Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 119, No. 3, 781 786 0735-7044/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.781 The Role of CA1 in the Acquisition of

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 215 (2010) 197 208 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Review The role of the human hippocampus

More information

POSTERIOR 1. situated behind: situated at or toward the hind part of the body :

POSTERIOR 1. situated behind: situated at or toward the hind part of the body : ANATOMICAL LOCATION Anatomy is a difficult subject with a large component of memorization. There is just no way around that, but we have made every effort to make this course diverse and fun. The first

More information

Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation

Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation So far we have talked about the hippocampus and: -coding of spatial locations in rats -declarative (explicit) memory -experimental evidence

More information

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight? How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? What happens to visual-devoted brain structure in individuals who are born deprived of sight?

More information

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience Dopamine and Learning Michael Dorris Associate Professor of Physiology & Neuroscience Studies dorrism@biomed.queensu.ca http://brain.phgy.queensu.ca/dorrislab/ NSCI 324 Systems

More information

Contingent Versus Incidental Context Processing During Conditioning: Dissociation After Excitotoxic Hippocampal Plus Dentate Gyrus Lesions

Contingent Versus Incidental Context Processing During Conditioning: Dissociation After Excitotoxic Hippocampal Plus Dentate Gyrus Lesions Contingent Versus Incidental Context Processing During Conditioning: Dissociation After Excitotoxic Hippocampal Plus Dentate Gyrus Lesions M. Good,* L. de Hoz, and R.G.M. Morris Centre for Neuroscience,

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention Cognitive Neuroscience Attention There are many aspects to attention. It can be controlled. It can be focused on a particular sensory modality or item. It can be divided. It can set a perceptual system.

More information

Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals

Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals Nissl stained coronal or horizontal sections were made from kainate injected (right) and saline injected (left) animals at different

More information

Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale

Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Marseille NeuroSTIC Grenoble, June 23 24, 2016 Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale Why is it important

More information

Two cortical systems for memoryguided

Two cortical systems for memoryguided Two cortical systems for memoryguided behaviour Charan Ranganath 1,2 and Maureen Ritchey 2 Abstract Although the perirhinal cortex (PRC), parahippocampal cortex (PHC) and retrosplenial cortex (RSC) have

More information

Suppl. Information Supplementary Figure 1. Strategy/latency analysis of individual mice during maze learning. a,

Suppl. Information Supplementary Figure 1. Strategy/latency analysis of individual mice during maze learning. a, Goal-oriented searching mediated by ventral hippocampus early in trial-and-error learning Ruediger, S, Spirig, D., Donato, F., Caroni, P. Suppl. Information Supplementary Figure 1. Strategy/latency analysis

More information

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1 Vision and Action Our ability to move thru our environment is closely tied to visual perception. Simple examples include standing one one foot. It is easier to maintain balance with the eyes open than

More information

Effects of baseline anxiety on response to kindling of the right medial amygdala

Effects of baseline anxiety on response to kindling of the right medial amygdala Physiology & Behavior 70 (2000) 67 80 Effects of baseline anxiety on response to kindling of the right medial amygdala Robert Adamec, Tanya Shallow Dept. of Psychology, Memorial University, St. John s,

More information

What do you notice?

What do you notice? What do you notice? https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=5v5ebf2kwf8&t=143s Models for communication between neuron groups Communication through neuronal coherence Neuron Groups 1 and 3 send projections to

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

Cognition in Parkinson's Disease and the Effect of Dopaminergic Therapy

Cognition in Parkinson's Disease and the Effect of Dopaminergic Therapy Cognition in Parkinson's Disease and the Effect of Dopaminergic Therapy Penny A. MacDonald, MD, PhD, FRCP(C) Canada Research Chair Tier 2 in Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging Assistant Professor

More information

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts By Erin Marie Fleming

More information

Target contact and exploration strategies in haptic search

Target contact and exploration strategies in haptic search Target contact and exploration strategies in haptic search Vonne van Polanen* Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest Astrid M.L. Kappers MOVE Research Institute, Faculty of Human Movement Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam,

More information

Perirhinal Cortex Lesions in Rats: Novelty Detection and Sensitivity to Interference

Perirhinal Cortex Lesions in Rats: Novelty Detection and Sensitivity to Interference Behavioral Neuroscience 2015 The Author(s) 2015, Vol. 129, No. 3, 227 243 0735-7044/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bne0000049 Perirhinal Cortex Lesions in Rats: Novelty Detection and Sensitivity to

More information

Retrograde amnesia in rats with lesions to the hippocampus on a test of spatial memory

Retrograde amnesia in rats with lesions to the hippocampus on a test of spatial memory Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 1580 1590 Retrograde amnesia in rats with lesions to the hippocampus on a test of spatial memory Gordon Winocur a, b, c, d,, Morris Moscovitch b, c, Douglas A. Caruana e, Malcolm

More information

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Outline Neuroanatomy of emotion Critical conceptual, experimental design, and interpretation issues in neuroimaging research Fear and anxiety Neuroimaging research

More information

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories? CASE 49 A 43-year-old woman is brought to her primary care physician by her family because of concerns about her forgetfulness. The patient has a history of Down syndrome but no other medical problems.

More information

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Neuroscience of Consciousness II 1 C83MAB: Mind and Brain Neuroscience of Consciousness II Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Consciousness State of consciousness - Being awake/alert/attentive/responsive Contents

More information

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice Multiple Choice 1. Which structure is not part of the visual pathway in the brain? a. occipital lobe b. optic chiasm c. lateral geniculate nucleus *d. frontal lobe Answer location: Visual Pathways 2. Which

More information

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1428 1442 Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Alessandra Schiavetto a, Stefan Köhler a, Cheryl L. Grady a, Gordon Winocur a,c,

More information

The Effects of Cytotoxic Perirhinal Cortex Lesions on Spatial Learning by Rats: A Comparison of the Dark Agouti and Sprague Dawley Strains

The Effects of Cytotoxic Perirhinal Cortex Lesions on Spatial Learning by Rats: A Comparison of the Dark Agouti and Sprague Dawley Strains Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 120, No. 1, 150 161 0735-7044/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.1.150 The Effects of Cytotoxic Perirhinal

More information

Insular Cortex Lesions and Morphine-Induced Suppression of Conditioned Stimulus Intake in the Rat

Insular Cortex Lesions and Morphine-Induced Suppression of Conditioned Stimulus Intake in the Rat Butler University Digital Commons @ Butler University Scholarship and Professional Work COPHS College of Pharmacy & Health Sciences 2009 Insular Cortex Lesions and Morphine-Induced Suppression of Conditioned

More information

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 152 162 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000053 Memory Systems

More information

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman BIOMED 509 Executive Control Cambridge University Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan UNM SOM JL Brigman 4-7-17 Symptoms and Assays of Cognitive Disorders Symptoms of Cognitive Disorders Learning & Memory

More information

The Rostral Thalamic Reticular Nucleus: How Lesions to the Rostral TRN effect performance in rats on a Visual Discrimination Task

The Rostral Thalamic Reticular Nucleus: How Lesions to the Rostral TRN effect performance in rats on a Visual Discrimination Task The Rostral Thalamic Reticular Nucleus: How Lesions to the Rostral TRN effect performance in rats on a Visual Discrimination Task Stewart J. Neifert and G. Daniel Weese Department of Psychology, Hampden-Sydney

More information

Contextual Fear Discrimination Is Impaired by Damage to the Postrhinal or Perirhinal Cortex

Contextual Fear Discrimination Is Impaired by Damage to the Postrhinal or Perirhinal Cortex Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 116, No. 3, 479 488 0735-7044/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.3.479 Contextual Fear Discrimination

More information

Episodic-Like Memory for What-Where-Which Occasion is Selectively Impaired in the 3xTgAD Mouse Model of Alzheimer s Disease

Episodic-Like Memory for What-Where-Which Occasion is Selectively Impaired in the 3xTgAD Mouse Model of Alzheimer s Disease Journal of Alzheimer s Disease 33 (2013) 681 698 DOI 10.3233/JAD-2012-121543 IOS Press 681 Episodic-Like Memory for What-Where-Which Occasion is Selectively Impaired in the 3xTgAD Mouse Model of Alzheimer

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Perirhinal Cortex Ablation Impairs Visual Object Identification

Perirhinal Cortex Ablation Impairs Visual Object Identification The Journal of Neuroscience, March 15, 1998, 18(6):2268 2275 Perirhinal Cortex Ablation Impairs Visual Object Identification Mark J. Buckley and David Gaffan Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford

More information

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats Physiology & Behavior 76 (2002) 117 129 The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats Peter C. Holland a, *, Gorica D. Petrovich b, Michela Gallagher b a Department

More information

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (2), 327-331 The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR THOMAS M. SPALEK AND VINCENT DI LOLLO Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia,

More information

I will explain the most important concepts of functional treatment while treating our 4 patients: Tom Clint Alice Dick

I will explain the most important concepts of functional treatment while treating our 4 patients: Tom Clint Alice Dick Every step we ve covered so far, from Evaluation to Clinical Reasoning to Establishing Goals and Preparation for Function has laid the groundwork for the final step: Treatment Using Functional Activities.

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5983/1288/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Induction of Fear Extinction with Hippocampal-Infralimbic BDNF Jamie Peters, Laura M. Dieppa-Perea, Loyda M. Melendez,

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Intelligent Adaption Across Space

Intelligent Adaption Across Space Intelligent Adaption Across Space Honors Thesis Paper by Garlen Yu Systems Neuroscience Department of Cognitive Science University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92092 Advisor: Douglas Nitz Abstract

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits following TMS

Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits following TMS 501 Journal of Neuropsychology (2008), 2, 501 508 q 2008 The British Psychological Society The British Psychological Society www.bpsjournals.co.uk Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits

More information

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Name: Student #: BEFORE YOU BEGIN!!! 1) Count the number of pages in your exam. The exam is 8 pages long; if you do not

More information

Contributions of Postrhinal and Perirhinal Cortex to Contextual Information Processing

Contributions of Postrhinal and Perirhinal Cortex to Contextual Information Processing Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2000, Vol, 114, No. 5, 882-894 0735-7044/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.5.882 Contributions of Postrhinal and

More information

the study of the body s physical structures

the study of the body s physical structures Chapter 1: Page 10 anatomy anterior (ventral) coronal (frontal) plane distal the study of the body s physical structures directional term meaning "toward the front" plane of reference which divides the

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949

Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949 Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras Lecture 5: Memory Consolidation Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949 McGaugh, 2 Squire: retention of TV shows after ECS At least 2 kinds

More information

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory Alejandro Lleras, 1 Ronald A. Rensink, 2 and James T. Enns

More information

Spatial memory deficits in patients with lesions to the right hippocampus and to the right parahippocampal cortex.

Spatial memory deficits in patients with lesions to the right hippocampus and to the right parahippocampal cortex. IN PRESS: NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA Volume 36, no 6, September 1998. Spatial memory deficits in patients with lesions to the right hippocampus and to the right parahippocampal cortex. Veronique D. Bohbot1* 1, Miroslav

More information