Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published August 21, 2006

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published August 21, 2006"

Transcription

1 Advance Access published August 21, 2006 doi: /cercor/bhl060 Human Ventral Parietal Cortex Plays a Functional Role on Visuospatial Attention and Primary Consciousness. A Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study In this paper, we used repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rtms) in 18 normal subjects to investigate whether the ventral posterior parietal cortex (PPC) plays a causal role on visuospatial attention and primary consciousness and whether these 2 functions are linearly correlated with each other. Two distinct experimental conditions involved a similar visual stimuli recognition paradigm. In Consciousness experiment, number of consciously perceived visual stimuli was lower by about 10% after rtms (300 ms, 20 Hz, motor threshold intensity) on left or right PPC than after sham (pseudo) rtms. In Attentional Posner s experiment, these stimuli were always consciously perceived. Compared with sham condition, parietal rtms slowed of about 25 ms reaction time to go stimuli, thus disclosing effects on endogenous covert spatial attention. No linear correlation was observed between the rtmsinduced impairment on attention and conscious perception. Results suggest that PPC plays a slight but significant causal role in both visuospatial attention and primary consciousness. Furthermore, these high-level cognitive functions, as modulated by parietal rtms, do not seem to share either linear or simple relationships. Keywords: primary consciousness, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rtms), ventral posterior parietal cortex, visuospatial attention Introduction Primary consciousness is an early process by which a conscious perceptive image of a sensory stimulus emerges by cortical activity, as opposed to parallel not conscious, automatic processing of the same stimulus. As a simple operational definition, here visual primary consciousness was considered as the process underlying immediate subject s self-report about an adequate visual stimulation given to himself or herself. In line with Pinker s (1997) theoretical view, that report implies subjective phenomenal experience ( sentience ) and the ability to report on the content of short-term phenomenal/mental experience ( access to information ). Primary consciousness is strictly connected to selective attention. High-focused visual attention toward an external object is related to a vivid and prolonged conscious perceptive image of the object, whereas a low attentive level is related to a dummy perceptive image. Ventral posterior parietal cortex (PPC) seems to be deeply involved in both visuospatial selective attention and primary consciousness. Patients with lesions including ventral PPC (especially of the right hemisphere) are not able to consciously perceive unilateral stimuli at visual hemifield or hemibody contralateral to the lesion (i.e., neglect, Vallar and others 1988; Driver and Vuilleumier 2001; Corbetta and others 2005). In Claudio Babiloni 1--3, Fabrizio Vecchio 1,3, Simone Rossi 4, Alberto De Capua 5, Sabina Bartalini 4, Monica Ulivelli 4 and Paolo Maria Rossini 2,3,6 1 Dipartimento Fisiologia Umana e Farmacologia, Universita` degli Studi di Roma La Sapienza, Rome, Italy, 2 IRCCS Centro S. Giovanni di Dio-F.B.F., Brescia, Italy, 3 A.Fa.R. Dipartimento Neuroscienze, Osp. FBF, Isola Tiberina, Rome, Italy, 4 Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Sezione Neurologia, Universita` Siena, Italy, 5 Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Sezione Psichiatria, Universita` Siena, Italy and 6 Clinica Neurologica, Campus Biomedico, Universita` di Roma, Italy some cases, they just miss contralesional stimuli at left visual hemifield or hemibody during bilateral stimulations (i.e., hemiinattention or extinction). This lack of primary consciousness is supposed to rely on a pathological bias in spatial attention toward the hemifield or hemibody contralateral to the intact left hemisphere (Posner and others 1984; Vallar and others 1988; Desimone and Duncan 1995; Corbetta and others 2005). The causal role of PPC in visuospatial selective attention and primary consciousness has been also proven by single-pulse or repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rtms), which can transiently interfere with the functions of the stimulated cortical area (Oliveri and others 1999, 2000; Walsh and Cowey 2000; Sack and Linden 2003; Rossi and Rossini 2004; Hung and others 2005; Campana and others 2006; Fuggetta and others 2006). Converging evidences indicate that TMS on the PPC modulated conscious perceptive deficits of patients suffering from neglect of visual or somatic stimuli at contralesional side, and that these deficits can even be transiently induced in normal subjects (Pascsual-Leone and others 1994; Oliveri and others 1999, 2000, 2002; Walsh and Rushworth 1999; Fierro and others 2000, 2001; Hilgetag and others 2001; Bjoertomt and others 2002; Brighina and others 2002; Muri and others 2002). The mentioned data suggest a simple linear relationship between attention and primary consciousness and a causal role for both functions of PPC. However, such a simple relationship is challenged by several lines of evidence. Residual selective attention can be observed in particular epileptic patients that, during complex partial seizures, can still perform aimed actions (e.g., bringing and drinking a glass of water; getting out of the room) without any apparent perceptive conscious experience; the same is true in sleepwalkers and in subjects under hypnotic commands (Damasio 1998). In these cases, sentience and/or access to information of the visual consciousness seems to be affected in some way. The relationship among selective attention, primary consciousness, and PPC might be much less simple than those emerging from field literature. In the present study, we wondered whether human ventral PPC plays a causal role on visuospatial attention and primary consciousness and whether these 2 functions are correlated with each other. For this purpose, rtms was applied over right or left ventral PPC of normal subjects during 2 experiments. The first experiment tested the functional relationship between PPC activity and primary visual consciousness (Babiloni and others 2005, 2006), whereas the second experiment those between PPC and visuospatial attention (Posner and others 1984; Thuth and others 2005). In particular, 3 working hypotheses were tested. The first hypothesis was that rtms of ventral PPC could reduce the number of consciously seen cue stimuli. The Ó The Author Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org

2 Babiloni second hypothesis was that rtms of ventral PPC could impair visuospatial attention and could slow the reaction time to predicted go stimuli (valid trials = spatial congruence between cue and go stimuli). The third hypothesis was that the predicted effects of rtms on visuospatial primary consciousness and attention were linearly correlated each other. Materials and Methods Subjects Experiments were performed on 18 healthy adult volunteers (with ages spanning from 20 to 40 years). Edinburgh Inventory for right handedness was 77.5% (±9 standard error [SE]). They had no previous psychiatric or neurological history. Their sight was normal or corrected-tonormal. All experiments were undertaken with the understanding and written consent of each participant according to Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association and the standards established by the Author s Institutional Review Board. The present study was approved by local ethical committee. Experimental Tasks The subjects were seated in a comfortable reclining armchair. They kept their forearms resting on the armchairs, with the right index finger resting between 2 buttons of a mouse connected to a computer. The computer monitor was placed in front of them at a distance of about 100 cm. As aforementioned, 2 distinct experiments were performed (Fig. 1). The first experimental paradigm served to emphasize the strict functional relationships between ventral PPC and primary visual consciousness. In the Consciousness experiment, a preliminary procedure ascertained the individual perceptive threshold time (ms) of the cue stimulus. The cue stimulus was a white circle (diameter of about 0.5 of visual angle) appearing at 6 right or left side of the background central white cross (diameter of 0.5 ). The cue stimulus was preceded and followed by a masking visual stimulus formed by 2 Xs (large about 0.8 ), located at 6 right and left sides of the central white cross. The subjects had to say seen any time they perceived the position of the cue stimulus. In detail, the procedure was as follows. The duration of the cue stimulus varied randomly trial-by-trial within the following values: 20, 40, 60, 80, 100, 120, 140, 160 ms for 80 trials (10 for each duration value). The procedure was repeated 7 times. The threshold time was defined as the duration of the cue stimulus determining about 50% of correct stimulus detections within a series of 10 trials, in the majority of the 7 repetitions of the procedure. Just before starting with the experimental session, this threshold time was systematically varied up and down (10 ms) for some preliminary trials, to verify the stability of that time based on subject s self-report. There was always a confirmation of the threshold time of cue presentation as indicated by the above preliminary procedure. Subject-by-subject, the mean threshold time of the cue stimulus was 72 ms (±7.9 SE). Of note, a previous study allowed to ascertain that individual values of perceptive threshold remained substantially stable for the entire recording session (Babiloni and others 2006). The sequence of the visual stimuli was as follows (Fig. 1): 1) the masking stimulus Xs lasting 5.5 s; 2) the cue stimulus small circle appearing on the right or left (50%) monitor side for the threshold time; 3) the masking stimulus Xs lasting about 2 s; 4) a go (target) stimulus lasting about 0.5 s. The go stimulus was a small green circle with a diameter of about 0.5, which appeared on 6 right or left (50% of probability) side of the central white cross. The subjects had to press the left mouse button if the go stimulus appeared on the left monitor side, whereas they had to press the right mouse button if the go stimulus appeared on the right monitor side. The computer receiving the mouse inputs registered the corresponding reaction time and the side of the button pressed. Immediately after the hand motor response, the subjects had to say seen if they had detected the cue stimulus ( seen trial ) or not seen if they had missed the cue stimulus ( not seen trial ). Noteworthy, subjects denied the use of mental verbal codes for the cue or go stimuli after the experiment. Verbal self-report was registered by the manual note of an experimenter. The experimenter Figure 1. Left column: Sequence of events during a trial of the Consciousness experiment: 1) masking stimulus lasting 5.5 s; 2) cue stimulus appearing on the right or left monitor side for the threshold time giving about 50% of stimulus recognition; 3) masking stimulus lasting about 2 s (i.e., 2 s minus the threshold time); 4) go stimulus lasting about 0.5 s. The go stimulus was a green circle with a diameter of about 0.5, which appeared on the right or the left monitor side. Subjects had to click the left mouse button if the go stimulus appeared on the left monitor side, whereas they had to click the right mouse button if the go stimulus appeared on the right monitor side (selfreport). The computer receiving mouse inputs registered the corresponding reaction time and the side of the mouse button clicked. After hand motor response, subjects had to say seen if they had detected the cue stimulus ( seen trial) or not seen if they had missed the cue stimulus ( not seen trial). When required by the experimental demands, an rtms train (300 ms, 20 Hz) was delivered simultaneously to cue presentation. Right column: Sequence of events during a trial of the Attentional experiment. They are as the Consciousness experiment except that the cue stimulus lasted 500 ms, and no self-report was required. even controlled what the subjects watched on the computer monitor during the task. Of note, the same presentation time and physical features (shape, position, luminance etc.) characterized the cue stimuli consciously perceived ( seen trials ) and those not consciously perceived ( not seen trials ). Therefore, these features cannot explain the reason why some cue stimuli were seen and others were consciously not seen. The Attentional experiment tested whether the applied rtms train impaired specific processes of visuospatial attention. The Attentional experiment differed from the Consciousness experiment for the duration of the cue stimulus and the spatial relationships between cue and go stimuli. In the Attentional experiment, the cue stimulus lasted 500 ms for all subjects to produce 100% of conscious detection. Furthermore, the go stimulus was given to the same side of the cue stimulus in the 80% of the cases (valid trials; Posner and others 1980), to induce a covert spatial attention toward the portion of the monitor in which cue stimulus appeared. Finally, no self-report was required in the Attentional experiment. Procedures of rtms and Identification of Target Scalp Regions The rtms was delivered through a focal, figure-of-eight coil (outer diameter of each wing 7 cm), connected with a standard Mag-Stim Super Rapid stimulator (maximum output 2.2 Tesla). Individual resting excitability thresholds for left and right motor cortices stimulation were first determined by using the same coil and stimulator and following standardized procedures (Rossini and others 1994; Rossi and others 2001). Left and or right ventral PPC (epicenter in Broadman area [BA] 39) was stimulated by placing the anterior end of the junction of the 2 coil wings at the predetermined scalp site, respectively. A mechanical arm maintained the handle of the coil angled backward of about 45 away from the midline. This position was marked on a transparent bathing cap Page 2 of 7 Parietal Cortex, Attention, and Consciousness d and others

3 firmly adherent to the scalp. The procedure allowed the correct repositioning of the coil prior to the appearance of each warning signal. The left and right PPC locations were automatically identified on the subject s scalp using SofTaxic Navigator system, on the basis of digitized skull landmarks (nasion, inion, and 2 preauricular points) and about 40 scalp points (Fastrak Polhemus digitizer, Polhemus, Colchester, VT). Talairach coordinates of the putative cortical sites were automatically estimated by the SofTaxic Navigator Stereotaxic Navigator System (E.M.S. Italy, on the basis of an MRI-constructed stereotaxic template (estimated accuracy of ±0.5 cm, Talairach space; Fig. 2). Of note, error of TMS coil positioning (SofTax system) may be less than 1 cm across subjects, a negligible displacement with respect to the large cortical area of interest, namely, ventral PPC. Furthermore, the intensity of stimulation (100% of individual motor threshold) reasonably minimized the possibility of an insufficient interferential effect due to a mismatch between the scalp site and the underlying anatomy. Very similar scalp position and stimulation intensity have been previously used in TMS studies to interfere with cognitive functions of PPC such as sensory perception, mental rotation, grasping or visuospatial and verbal working memory, and episodic memory (Mottaghy and others 1999; Oliveri and others 1999, 2000; Rossi and others 2006). Trains of rtms (100% of individual motor threshold; length of each rtms train: 300 ms; rtms frequency: 20 Hz) were delivered from the onset of the cue stimulus presentation in both Consciousness and Attentional experiments. The length of the rtms train corresponded to the activation timing of PPC previously found in an electroencephalography (EEG) study carried out with the same paradigm (Babiloni and others 2006). In both Consciousness and Attentional experiments, each subjects performed 4 different times the same experimental task. In a block, the subjects were not stimulated by the rtms (baseline condition). In other 2 blocks, the sites of stimulation were either at left or at right ventral PPC. Finally, there was a block in which the rtms coil was centered at scalp vertex, but it was held perpendicularly to the scalp surface (sham condition). In the sham condition, scalp contact and discharging noise were practically equal to the active stimulation, but the induced magnetic field did not activate cortical neurons. Each of the 4 blocks (No stimulation, active rtms overlying the left or right ventral PPC, and sham rtms) included about 40 trials. To avoid a possible effect Figure 2. MRI-constructed stereotaxic template showing the coronal (a), sagittal (b), and axial (c) projections on the cortical surfaces of stimulation sites in the left hemisphere, corresponding to the intraparietal suclus (Talaraich coordinates of left ventral PPC: 43, 68, 39). Talaraich coordinates of the homologous right stimulation site (not shown) were 40, 66, 40. Such positions corresponded to an epicenter at BA 39. of adaptation to the coil scalp contact, the sequence of these 4 blocks was randomly delivered at group level. Statistical Analysis To test the causal role of ventral PPC on primary visual consciousness (first working hypothesis; see Introduction), an analysis of variance (ANOVA) test included factor Site of Stimulation (real left PPC, real right PPC, sham) and number of seen trials as a dependent variable. Duncan test was used for post hoc comparisons between sham andreal rtms conditions (P < 0.05). To test the causal role of ventral PPC on visual attention (second working hypothesis), an ANOVA test included factor Site of Stimulation (real left PPC, real right PPC, sham) and reaction time in milliseconds as a dependent variable. Duncan test was used for post hoc comparisons between sham and real rtms conditions (P < 0.05). To test the hypothesis of linear relationships among ventral PPC, attention and primary consciousness, we evaluated the linear correlation of the number of seen trials in the Consciousness experiment with the reaction time in the Attentional experiment (Pearson test; Bonferroni corrected, P < 0.05). Furthermore, we also evaluated the linear correlation of the variation of the number of seen trials in the Consciousness experiment with the variation of reaction time in the Attentional experiment with respect to the No Stimulation block (Pearson test; Bonferroni corrected, P < 0.05). Results Consciousness Experiment In all conditions of the Consciousness experiment, the percentage of correct responses (right mouse button press for right go stimulus; left mouse button press for left go stimulus) was higher than 95%. Trials associated with the conscious perception of cue stimuli were defined as seen trials. Across subjects mean percentage of the seen trials was 52.9% (±0.8 SE) in the baseline condition (no rtms), 52.2% (±0.6 SE) in the sham condition (pseudo rtms), 44.4% (±1.9 SE) after rtms at left PPC, and 45.9% (±1.7 SE) after rtms at right PPC. The hypothesis that rtms of PPC could reduce the number of consciously seen cue stimuli was evaluated by ANOVA including factor Site of rtms (real right PPC, real left PPC, sham). There was a statistically significant main effect (F 2,34 = 3.77; P < 0.03; left side of Fig. 3). Compared with the sham condition, the number of seen trials was significantly reduced after rtms at both left (P < 0.02) and right PPC (P < 0.04), reflecting the reduction of primary visual consciousness. Attentional Experiment In the Attentional experiment, the cue stimuli persisted enough to be always consciously perceived, and valid trials were those with spatial congruence between cue and go stimuli. In all conditions, the percentage of correct responses (right mouse button press for right go stimulus; left mouse button press for left go stimulus) for the valid trials was higher than 95%. Mean reaction time of the valid trials across subjects was 456 ms (±12 SE) in the baseline condition (no rtms). The presence of rtms, either real or sham, generally shortened reaction times: 418 ms (±10 SE) in the sham condition (pseudo rtms), 446 ms (±12 SE) after rtms at left PPC, and 438 ms (±11 SE) after rtms at right PPC. The hypothesis that rtms of PPC could slow the reaction time (vs. sham) to valid trials was evaluated by ANOVA including factor Site of rtms stimulation (real left PPC, real right PPC, sham). There was a statistically significant main effect (F 2,34 = 5.25; P < 0.01; right side of Fig. 3). Compared with the sham condition, the reaction time of valid trials increased after rtms Page3of7

4 Babiloni Figure 3. On the left: Results of the Consciousness paradigm showed that rtms at either left or right ventral PPC reduced the percentage of seen stimuli. On the right: Results of the Attentional Posner s paradigm showed that the same stimulations slowed reaction time to stimuli affected by cover spatial attention. at both left (P < 0.02) and right (P < 0.03) PPC, reflecting the reduction of visuospatial attention. Correlation between Results of the Consciousness and Attentional Experiments Pearson s test (P > 0.05) evaluated the hypothesis that the predicted effects of rtms on visuospatial attention and primary consciousness were correlated. The correlation was calculated between the number of seen trials after rtms at left or right PPC in the Consciousness experiment and the reaction time to valid trials after the same rtms in the Attentional experiment. First row of Figure 4 shows no statistically significant correlation (rtms at left PPC: r = 0.36, P = 0.13; rtms at right PPC: r = 0.18, P = 0.46). As a control, the correlation was also calculated between the difference (sham-rtms at left or right PPC) of seen trials in the Consciousness experiment and the difference (sham-rtms at left or right PPC) of reaction times to valid trials in the Attentional experiment. Second row of Figure 4 shows no statistically significant correlation (sham-rtms at left PPC: r = 0.31; P < 0.21; sham-rtms at right PPC: r = 0.14; P < 0.59). Control Analyses Trials of the Consciousness experiment were classified as follows: 1) congruent trials, in which the go stimulus appeared on the cued location; 2) incongruent trials, in which the go stimulus appeared on the uncued location. The congruent and the incongruent trials were equally numbered (50%). For the Attentional experiment, we considered only the valid trials. The reaction time for each trial class was defined as the period between the onset of the go stimulus and the mouse button click. The correctness of subjective reports about recognition performance of the Consciousness experiment was evaluated by an ANOVA analysis of reaction time to go stimuli in the baseline (no rtms) and sham conditions (pseudo rtms), which included factors Condition ( seen trials, not seen trials), Side of the cue stimulus (right, left), and Congruence (spatial congruence between cue stimulus and subsequent go stimulus; spatial incongruence between cue stimulus and subsequent go stimulus). The results showed statistically significant main factors Condition (F 1,18 = 5.84; P < 0.025) and Congruence (F 1,18 = 5.16; P < 0.035). According to the control hypothesis, mean reaction time was shorter in the seen trials than in the not seen ones regardless the spatial congruency between cue and go stimuli, thus demonstrating the global positive effect of conscious visual perception. Furthermore, the mean reaction time was shorter in the spatially congruent than incongruent trials regardless the consciousness of the stimuli, indicating that the requirement of a self-report induced a covert attention on cued location even when the cue stimuli were not consciously perceived ( not seen trials). The latter result was possibly due to covert attention on cued location regardless the conscious perception, which may be induced by the requirement of self-report (Ivanoff and Klein 2003; Babiloni and others 2006). The covert attention probably suppressed inhibition of return mechanism by which reaction time to go stimuli is typically lengthened by noninformative cue stimuli at the same location or hemifield (Posner and Cohen 1984). Well conduction of the Attentional experiment was evaluated by an ANOVA analysis of reaction time to go stimuli in the baseline (no rtms) and sham conditions, which included factor Condition (valid trials, invalid trials) and Side of the cue stimulus (right, left). The results showed a statistically significant main factor Condition (F 1,18 = 17.08; P < ). According to the control hypothesis, mean reaction time was shorter in the valid than in the invalid trials regardless of the spatial congruency between cue and go stimuli, thus demonstrating the global positive effect of covert attention on cued location. Discussion The rtms overlying left or right ventral PPC reduced the consciously perceived (cue) stimuli, thus confirming a certain causal role of PPC for primary visual consciousness. The small but significant magnitude of rtms effects on ventral PPC (about 10%) is likely due to the use of focal coil and near-threshold intensity of rtms. These 2 factors limited the induced neural interference in relation to the distributed cerebral networkspanning occipital, parietal, and temporal cortical areas of both hemispheres subserving primary visuospatial consciousness (Crick and Koch 1995; Ress and Heeger 2003; de Lafuente and Romo 2005; Fang and He 2005; Sergent and others 2005). Page 4 of 7 Parietal Cortex, Attention, and Consciousness d and others

5 Figure 4. On the top: Scatterplots of the linear correlation of the number of seen trials in the Consciousness experiment with the reaction time in the Attentional experiment (Pearson test; Bonferroni corrected, P < 0.05). On the bottom: Scatterplots of the linear correlation of the variation of the number of seen trials in the Consciousness experiment with the variation of reaction time in the Attentional experiment, after subtraction of the reaction time in No Stimulation block. No statistical significance emerged. Higher stimulation might have probably been more effective behaviorally, but carrying the risk of strong arousal (i.e., higher discharging noise and trigeminal afferents activation) disturbing the demanding cognitive performance, as already described for episodic memory (Rossi and others 2006). The involvement of bilateral ventral PPCs in primary visual consciousness probably occurs with peculiar aspects. Previous studies have emphasized the role of left hemisphere for consciousness, whereas others have favored right hemisphere (Shevrin 1992; Gazzaniga 1993; Henke and others 1993). Left hemisphere may subserve sequential organization of percept, linguistic elaboration of behavioral control, and theorization on experience flux (Gazzaniga 2000). Whereas, right hemisphere may subserve global visuospatial search and somatic perceptive processes, as revealed by neurological deficits such as neglect, visual extinction, prosopagnosia following righthemisphere damage (Berti and Rizzolatti 1992; Wallace 1994; Baudena and others 1995; Farah and Feinberg 1997; De Renzi 2000). In the Attentional experiment, cue stimuli were always consciously perceived in all conditions, thus indicating a full primary consciousness. Instead, attentional processes were modulated, as reflected by reaction time to go stimuli under covert visuospatial attention. Compared with the baseline condition (no rtms), the reaction time was faster after both sham and real rtms. These results are in line with previous findings showing that TMS coil sound per se shortens reaction time possibly due to enhanced arousal, intersensory, and/or motor output facilitation (Nickerson 1973; Rossi and others 2001, 2006). With respect to the sham, the real rtms of right or left ventral PPC slowed reaction time to go stimuli. It is reasonable that the slowing of the reaction time is mainly due to visuospatial sensory processing, although a slight influence on motor output cannot be excluded. These results corroborate the well-known notion that ventral PPC is a putative substrate of neglect and other visuospatial deficits (e.g., Vallar 2001; Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Karnath and others 2003; Corbetta and others 2005). In parallel, dorsal PPC regions along the intraparietal sulcus are supposed to contribute to spatial attention (Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Corbetta and others 2005; Thuth and others 2005), as shown by previous functional neuroimaging studies in healthy subjects (Nobre and others 1997; Kastner and others 1999; Corbetta and others 2000; Hopfinger and others 2000). Dorsal PPC would be deeply implied with motor component of the attentional processes. According to Posner s theoretical framework, visuospatial attention depends on 3 elementary mental operations that can be performed covertly: 1) disengagement from a current focus of attention (spatial disengagement); 2) directing attention to a new spatial location (spatial orienting); and 3) engagement of target. These attentional operations can be performed under endogenous control (e.g., voluntary orienting) or under the Page5of7

6 Babiloni influence of peripheral cues (e.g., reflexive orienting). It is likely that the present rtms of ventral PPC mainly effected spatial disengagement, in agreement with previous clinical studies showing that ventral PPC lesions are related to prominent spatial disengagement deficits (Posner and others 1984; Posner 1987; Friedrich and others 1998; Losier and Klein 2001). In addition, functional imaging studies have revealed an activation of ventral PPC accompanying disengagement of spatial attention in healthy subjects (Corbetta and others 2000). Conversely, patients with dorsal PPC damage would use endogenous information to facilitate and direct visuospatial attention (Posner and others 1984; Friedrich and others 1998; Smania and others 1998; Bartolomeo and Chokron 2002). In the current study, no linear or simple correlation emerged between the effects of rtms in the Consciousness and Attentional experiments; this is the first direct correlative evidence of parietal neural correlates of visual attention and consciousness, and requires to be carefully discussed in the following. As a premise, it should be remarked that the effect of rtms does not remain limited to the targeted brain region but spread transynaptically. Therefore, it can be speculated that ventral PPC is implied in some way with the cerebral networks subserving visuospatial attention and primary consciousness. Interaction of these networks might be nonlinear and too complex to cause simple relationships between visuospatial attention and primary consciousness, at least due to the focal and week TMS-induced perturbation of ventral PPC. In this sense, the present findings challenge the idea that visuospatial attention and primary consciousness are subserved by strictly interdependent networks. That questionable idea stems from a fatal association of several lines of evidence: 1) In neglect patients with (especially right) PPC lesion, both primary consciousness and selective attention are impaired and can be modulated by parietal rtms (Pascsual-Leone and others 1994; Walsh and Rushworth 1999; Fierro and others 2000, 2001; Oliveri and others 2000, 2002; Hilgetag and others 2001; Bjoertomt and others 2002; Brighina and others 2002; Muri and others 2002); 2) in healthy subjects, rtms of PPC transiently impairs both primary consciousness and selective attention to stimuli projected to the contralateral visual hemifield (Walsh and Rushworth 1999; Hilgetag and others 2001; Muri and others 2002); and 3) in healthy subjects, rtms of PPC paradoxically enhances both primary consciousness and selective attention to stimuli projected to the ipsilateral visual hemifield (Walsh and Rushworth 1999; Hilgetag and others 2001). The present results would enlighten previous EEG evidence in which functions of primary consciousness and attention were probed using the so-called oddball paradigm. In the oddball paradigm, subjects had to respond (i.e., moving or counting) after rare but not after frequent stimuli of a sequence. Therefore, both selective attention and conscious perception were involved. It has been shown that, compared with the unconscious ( subliminal ) perception of the rare stimuli, the conscious one evoked parietal P300 with higher amplitude (Shefrin and others 1988; Brazdil and others 1998, 2001, 2002). Keeping in mind the mentioned EEG evidence, the results of the current study suggest future oddball investigations performing separate modulations of attentional processes (i.e., varying the level of stimulus novelty) and primary consciousness (i.e., varying the rate of conscious stimulus perception), to disentangle the effects of selective attention and primary consciousness on cortical generators of P300. Conclusions To our knowledge, this is the first direct experimental evidence of functional effects of bilateral ventral PPC on both visuospatial attention and primary consciousness. At group level, the rtms overlying left or right ventral PPC reduced the number conscious perceptions and modified reaction time to stimuli affected by spatial cover attention. However, at individual level, there was no linear correlation between the above effects. The present results suggest that ventral PPC plays a slight but significant causal role on both visuospatial attention and primary consciousness, in line with the mentioned literature on spatial neglect. As a novelty, these cognitive functions, as modulated by parietal rtms, do not seem to share either linear or simple relationships. The present results also represent a neurophysiologic explanation for the well-known anecdotes reporting dissociations between selective attention and primary consciousness in epileptic subjects during seizures, in sleepwalkers, and in subjects under hypnotic commands (Damasio 1998). Notes The research was granted by Association Fatebenefratelli for Research (AFaR). We thank Prof. Fabrizio Eusebi for his continuous support. Conflict of Interest: None declared. Address correspondence to Dr Claudio Babiloni, Dipartimento di Fisiologia Umana e Farmacologia, Universita` degli Studi di Roma La Sapienza, P.le Aldo Moro 5, Rome, Italy. References Babiloni C, Vecchio F, Bultrini A, Romani GL, Rossini PM. Dec 28, Pre- and poststimulus alpha rhythms are related to conscious visual perception: a high-resolution EEG study. Cereb Cortex. doi: / cercor/bhj104 Babiloni C, Vecchio F, Miriello M, Romani GL, Rossini PM Visuospatial consciousness and parieto-occipital areas: a high-resolution EEG study. Cereb Cortex 16(1): Bartolomeo P, Chokron S Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26: Baudena P, Halgren E, Heit G, Clarke JM Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. III. Frontal cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 94: Berti A, Rizzolatti G Visual processing without awareness: evidence from unilateral neglect. J Cogn Neurosci 4: Bjoertomt O, Cowey A, Walsh V Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain 125: Brazdil M, Kuba R, Daniel P, Sochurkova D, Dobsik M, Rektor I Covert vision sign. Eur J Neurol 9(3): Brazdil M, Rektor I, Daniel P, Dufek M, Jurak P Intracerebral eventrelated potentials to subthreshold target stimuli. Clin Neurophysiol 112(4): Brazdil M, Rektor I, Dufek M, Jurak P, Daniel P Effect of subthreshold target stimuli on event-related potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 107: Brighina F, Bisiach E, Piazza A, Oliveri M, La Bua V, Daniele O, Fierro B Perceptual and response bias in visuospatial neglect due to frontal and parietal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in normal subjects. Neuroreport 13: Campana G, Cowey A, Walsh V Visual area V5/MT remembers what but not where. Cereb Cortex [Epub ahead of print]. Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Ollinger JM, McAvoy MP, Shulman GL Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 3(3): Corbetta M, Shulman GL Control of goal/directed and stimulus driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: Corbetta M, Tansy AP, Stanley CM, Astafiev SV, Snyder AZ, Shulman GL A functional MRI study of preparatory signals for spatial location and objects. Neuropsychologia 43(14): Page 6 of 7 Parietal Cortex, Attention, and Consciousness d and others

7 Crick F, Koch C Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? Nature 375(6527): Damasio AR Investigating the biology of consciousness. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 353(1377): de Lafuente V, Romo R Neuronal correlates of subjective sensory experience. Nat Neurosci 8(12): De Renzi E Disorders of visual recognition. Semin Neurol 20(4): [review]. Desimone R, Duncan J Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 18: [review]. Driver J, Vuilleumier P Perceptual awareness and its loss in unilateral neglect and extinction. Cognition 79(1--2): [review]. Fang F, He S Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways. Nat Neurosci 8(10): Farah MJ, Feinberg TE Consciousness of perception after brain damage. Semin Neurol 17(2): Fierro B, Brighina F, Oliveri M, Piazza A, La Bua V, Buffa D, Bisiach E Controlateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rtms in healthy subjects. Neuroreport 11: Fierro B, Brighina F, Piazza A, Oliveri M, Bisiach E Timing of right parietal and frontal cortex activity in visuo-spatial perception: a TMS study in normal individuals. Neuroreport 12: Friedrich FJ, Egly R, Rafal RD, Beck D Spatial attention deficits in humans: a comparison of superior parietal and temporal/parietal junction lesion. Neuropsychology 12: Fuggetta G, Pavone EF, Walsh V, Kiss M, Eimer M Cortico-cortical interactions in spatial attention: a combined ERP/TMS study. J Neurophysiol [Epub ahead of print]. Gazzaniga MS Brain mechanisms and conscious experience. Experimental and theoretical studies of consciousness (CIBA Foundation Symposium 174). Chichester, UK: Wiley. p Gazzaniga MS Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain 2000 Jul;123: Henke K, Landis T, Markowitsch HJ Subliminal perception of pictures in the right hemisphere. Conscious Cogn 2: Hilgetag C, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rtms-induced virtual lesion of human parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 4: Hopfinger JB, Buchel C, Holmes AP, Friston KJ A study of analysis parameters that influence the sensitivity of event-related fmri analyses. Neuroimage 11(4): Hung J, Driver J, Walsh V Visual selection and posterior parietal cortex: effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on partial report analyzed by Bundesen s theory of visual attention. J Neurosci 25(42): Ivanoff J, Klein RM Orienting of attention without awareness is affected by measurement-induced attentional control settings. J Vis 3(1): Karnath HO, Himmelbach M, Perenin MT Neglect-like behaviour in healthy subjects: dissociation of space exploration and goal directed pointing after vestibular stimulation. Exp Brain Res 153(2): Kastner S, Pinsk MA, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungereider LG Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron 22: Losier BJ, Klein RM A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25(1): Mottaghy FM, Hungs M, Brugmann M, Sparing R, Boroojerdy B, Foltys H, Huber W, Topper R Facilitation of picture naming after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 56: Muri RM, Buhler R, Heinemann D, Mosimann UP, Felbinger J, Shalaepfer TE, Hess CW Hemispheric asymmetry in visuospatial attention assessed with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Exp Brain Res 143: Nickerson RS Intersensory facilitation of reaction time: energy summation or preparation enhancement? Psychol Rev 80: Nobre AC, Sebestyen GN, Gitelman DR, Mesulam MM, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain 120: Oliveri M, Rossini PM, Cicinelli P, Traversa R, Pasqualetti P, Filippi NM, Caltagirone C Neurophysiological evaluation of tactile space perception deficits through transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Res Protoc 5(1): Oliveri M, Rossini PM, Filippi MM, Traversa R, Cicinelli P, Caltagirone C Specific forms of neural activity associated with tactile space awareness. Neuroreport 13(8): Oliveri M, Rossini PM, Traversa R, Cicinelli P, Filippi MM, Pasqualetti P, Tomaiuolo F, Caltagirone C Left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces contralesional extinction in patients with unilateral right brain damage. Brain 122(9): Pascsual-Leone A, Grafman J, Hallett M Explicit and implicit learning and maps of cortical motor output. Science 265: Pinker S How the mind works. London: Penguin Press Science S. Posner MI Cognitive neuropsychology and the problem of selective attention. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl 39: Posner MI, Cohen Y Components of visual orienting. In: Bumpy H, Bouwhuis TU, editors. Attention and performance, Volume X. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. p Posner MI, Snyder CR, Davidson BJ Attention and the detection of signals. J Exp Psychol 109(2): Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ, Rafal RD Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci 4(7): Ress D, Heeger DJ Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 6(4): Rossi S, Cappa SF, Babilioni C, Pasqualetti P, Miniussi C, Carducci F, Babiloni F, Rossini PM Prefrontal cortex in long term memory: an interference approach using magnetic stimulation. Nat Neurosci 4(9): Rossi S, Pasqualetti P, Zito G, Vecchio F, Cappa SF, Miniussi C, Babiloni C, Rossini PM Prefrontal and parietal cortex in episodic memory. An interference study by rtms. Eur J Neurosci 23(3): Rossi S, Rossini PM TMS in cognitive plasticity and the potential for rehabilitation. Trends Cogn Sci 8(6): Rossini PM, Barker AT, Berardelli A, Caramia MD, Caruso G, Gracco RQ, Dimitrijevic MR, Hallett M, Katayama Y, Lucking CH, and others Non invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 91: Sack AT, Linden DE Combining transcranial magnetic stimulation and functional imaging in cognitive brain research: possibilities and limitation. Brain Res Rev 43(1): Sergent C, Baillet S, Dehaene S Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink. Nat Neurosci 8(10): Shefrin SL, Goodin DS, Aminoff MJ Visual evoked response in the investigation of blindsight. Neurology 38: Shevrin H Subliminal perception, memory, and consciousness: cognitive and dynamic perspectives. In: Bornstein RF, Pittman TS, editors. Perception without awareness: cognitive, clinical, and social perspectives. New York: Guilford. p Smania N, Martini MC, Gambina G, Tomelleri G, Palamara A, Natale E, Marzi CA The spatial distribution of visual attention in hemineglect and extinction patients. Brain 121: Thuth G, Ives JR, Kampmann F, Pastor MA, Pascual-Leone A A new device and protocol for combining TMS and online recordings of EEG and evoked potentials. J Neurosci Methods 141(2): Vallar G Extrapersonal visual unilateral spatial neglect and its neuroanatomy. Neuroimage 14: Vallar G, Bisiach E, Cerizza M, Rusconi ML The role of the left hemisphere in decision-making. Cortex 24(4):605. Wallace MA Unconscious perception in neglect and extinction. In: Farah MJ, Ratcliff G, editors. The neuropsychology of high-level vision: collected tutorial essays. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. p Walsh V, Cowey A Transcranial magnetic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 1: Walsh V, Rushworth M A primer of magnetic stimulation as a tool for neuropsychology. Neuropsychologia 37: Page7of7

Facilitating visuospatial attention for the contralateral hemifield by repetitive TMS on the posterior parietal cortex

Facilitating visuospatial attention for the contralateral hemifield by repetitive TMS on the posterior parietal cortex Neuroscience Letters 382 (2005) 280 285 Facilitating visuospatial attention for the contralateral hemifield by repetitive TMS on the posterior parietal cortex Yun-Hee Kim a,b,, Soo-Jung Min b,1, Myoung-Hwan

More information

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso Control of visuo-spatial attention Emiliano Macaluso CB demo Attention Limited processing resources Overwhelming sensory input cannot be fully processed => SELECTIVE PROCESSING Selection via spatial orienting

More information

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Session 4 Virtual Lesion Approach I Alexandra Reichenbach MPI for Biological Cybernetics Tübingen, Germany Today s Schedule Virtual Lesion Approach : Study Design Rationale

More information

Effect of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation(TMS) on Visual Search Task

Effect of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation(TMS) on Visual Search Task INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF APPLIED BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING VOL.2, NO.2 2009 1 Effect of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation(TMS) on Visual Search Task K. Iramina, Guest member ABSTRACT Transcranial magnetic

More information

HEMIEXTINCTION INDUCED BY TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION OVER THE RIGHT TEMPORO-PARIETAL JUNCTION

HEMIEXTINCTION INDUCED BY TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION OVER THE RIGHT TEMPORO-PARIETAL JUNCTION Neuroscience 142 (2006) 119 123 HEMIEXTINCTION INDUCED BY TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION OVER THE RIGHT TEMPORO-PARIETAL JUNCTION I. G. MEISTER,* M. WIENEMANN, D. BUELTE, C. GRÜNEWALD, R. SPARING, N.

More information

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Hillyard(1973) Dichotic Listening Task N1 component enhanced for attended stimuli Supports early selection Effects of Voluntary

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

The Contribution of TMS-EEG Coregistration in the Exploration of the Human Connectome

The Contribution of TMS-EEG Coregistration in the Exploration of the Human Connectome IRCCS San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli The Contribution of TMS-EEG Coregistration in the Exploration of the Human Connectome Marta Bortoletto Carlo Miniussi Cognitive Neuroscience Section, The Saint

More information

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach Attentional Control 1 Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach Barry Giesbrecht & George R. Mangun Center for Mind & Brain University of California, Davis

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

Myers Psychology for AP*

Myers Psychology for AP* Myers Psychology for AP* David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2010 *AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board, which

More information

Frontoparietal Cortex Controls Spatial Attention through Modulation of Anticipatory Alpha Rhythms

Frontoparietal Cortex Controls Spatial Attention through Modulation of Anticipatory Alpha Rhythms The Journal of Neuroscience, May 6, 2009 29(18):5863 5872 5863 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Frontoparietal Cortex Controls Spatial Attention through Modulation of Anticipatory Alpha Rhythms Paolo Capotosto,

More information

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Neuroscience of Consciousness II 1 C83MAB: Mind and Brain Neuroscience of Consciousness II Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Consciousness State of consciousness - Being awake/alert/attentive/responsive Contents

More information

AUXILIARIES AND NEUROPLASTICITY

AUXILIARIES AND NEUROPLASTICITY AUXILIARIES AND NEUROPLASTICITY Claudio Babiloni, Ph.D. Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of Foggia (UNIFG), Italy UNIFG structured personnel involved Prof. Claudio Babiloni (Coordinator),

More information

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Evoked Potential Correlates of Conscious and Unconscious Vision in Parietal Extinction Patients

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Evoked Potential Correlates of Conscious and Unconscious Vision in Parietal Extinction Patients NeuroImage 14, S68 S75 (2001) doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0842, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Evoked Potential Correlates of Conscious and Unconscious

More information

Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits following TMS

Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits following TMS 501 Journal of Neuropsychology (2008), 2, 501 508 q 2008 The British Psychological Society The British Psychological Society www.bpsjournals.co.uk Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits

More information

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab This report details my summer research project for the REU Theoretical and Computational Neuroscience program as

More information

Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Lisa Koski, PhD, Clin Psy TMS Neurorehabilitation Lab Royal Victoria Hospital 2009-12-14 BIC Seminar, MNI Overview History, basic principles, instrumentation

More information

NeuroImage 44 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 44 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 44 (2009) 563 568 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg The role of the angular gyrus in the modulation of visuospatial attention

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

fmri (functional MRI)

fmri (functional MRI) Lesion fmri (functional MRI) Electroencephalogram (EEG) Brainstem CT (computed tomography) Scan Medulla PET (positron emission tomography) Scan Reticular Formation MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) Thalamus

More information

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Sensory to Motor Transformation Sensory information (visual,

More information

Event-related TMS over the right posterior parietal cortex induces ipsilateral visuospatial

Event-related TMS over the right posterior parietal cortex induces ipsilateral visuospatial COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE NEUROREPORT Event-related TMS over the right posterior parietal cortex induces ipsilateral visuospatial interference Gilles Pourtois, 1,2,CA Yves Vandermeeren, 2 Etienne Olivier

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention Cognitive Neuroscience Attention There are many aspects to attention. It can be controlled. It can be focused on a particular sensory modality or item. It can be divided. It can set a perceptual system.

More information

Left hand movements and right hemisphere activation in unilateral spatial neglect: a test of the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis

Left hand movements and right hemisphere activation in unilateral spatial neglect: a test of the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1350 1355 Left hand movements and right hemisphere activation in unilateral spatial neglect: a test of the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis Guido Gainotti a,, Roberta Perri

More information

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight? How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? What happens to visual-devoted brain structure in individuals who are born deprived of sight?

More information

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention University of New Mexico UNM Digital Repository Psychology ETDs Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2-9-2010 Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention David B.

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL CORRELATES OF VISUAL CONSCIOUSNESS: A REVIEW OF THEORIES AND EMPIRICAL STUDIES

EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL CORRELATES OF VISUAL CONSCIOUSNESS: A REVIEW OF THEORIES AND EMPIRICAL STUDIES EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL CORRELATES OF VISUAL CONSCIOUSNESS: A REVIEW OF THEORIES AND EMPIRICAL STUDIES Bachelor Degree Project in Cognitive Neuroscience 15 ECTS Spring term 2012 Granit Kastrati Supervisors:

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE BRAIN The central nervous system (CNS), consisting of the brain and spinal cord, receives input from sensory neurons and directs

More information

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Eiichi Hoshino 1,2, Ken Mogi 2, 1 Tokyo Institute of Technology, Department of Computational Intelligence and Systems Science. 4259

More information

Seeing through the tongue: cross-modal plasticity in the congenitally blind

Seeing through the tongue: cross-modal plasticity in the congenitally blind International Congress Series 1270 (2004) 79 84 Seeing through the tongue: cross-modal plasticity in the congenitally blind Ron Kupers a, *, Maurice Ptito b www.ics-elsevier.com a Center for Functionally

More information

1 Decoding attentional orientation from EEG spectra. Ramesh Srinivasan, Samuel Thorpe, Siyi Deng, Tom Lappas & Michael D'Zmura

1 Decoding attentional orientation from EEG spectra. Ramesh Srinivasan, Samuel Thorpe, Siyi Deng, Tom Lappas & Michael D'Zmura 1 Ramesh Srinivasan, Samuel Thorpe, Siyi Deng, Tom Lappas & Michael D'Zmura Department of Cognitive Sciences, UC Irvine, SSPA 3151, Irvine, CA 92697 5100 {r.srinivasan,sthorpe,sdeng,tlappas,mdzmura}@uci.edu

More information

An abundance of evidence suggests that the human primary

An abundance of evidence suggests that the human primary Unconscious processing of orientation and color without primary visual cortex Jennifer L. Boyer, Stephenie Harrison, and Tony Ro* Department of Psychology, Rice University, Houston, TX 77005-1892 Edited

More information

Giacomo Rizzolatti - selected references

Giacomo Rizzolatti - selected references Giacomo Rizzolatti - selected references 1 Rizzolatti, G., Semi, A. A., & Fabbri-Destro, M. (2014). Linking psychoanalysis with neuroscience: the concept of ego. Neuropsychologia, 55, 143-148. Notes: Through

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neural Netw. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 November 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neural Netw. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 November 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Neural Netw. 2006 November ; 19(9): 1447 1449. Models of human visual attention should consider trial-by-trial variability in preparatory

More information

Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients

Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients NeuroImage 14, S98 S102 (2001) doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0815, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients Anna Berti,*,1 Nicola Smania, and Alan Allport

More information

Neuroscience Tutorial

Neuroscience Tutorial Neuroscience Tutorial Brain Organization : cortex, basal ganglia, limbic lobe : thalamus, hypothal., pituitary gland : medulla oblongata, midbrain, pons, cerebellum Cortical Organization Cortical Organization

More information

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Selective Attention Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form,

More information

Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown

Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown Neurol Clin N Am 21 (2003) 575 607 Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown Shaun P. Vecera, PhD a, *, Matthew Rizzo, MD b a Department of Psychology, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242,

More information

Hemispheric Asymmetries in Fronto-Parietal Networks. Underlying Attentional Control. Felix Dücker

Hemispheric Asymmetries in Fronto-Parietal Networks. Underlying Attentional Control. Felix Dücker Hemispheric Asymmetries in Fronto-Parietal Networks Underlying Attentional Control Felix Dücker Felix Dücker, Maastricht 2013. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored

More information

Cortex and Mind Chapter 6

Cortex and Mind Chapter 6 Cortex and Mind Chapter 6 There are many aspects to attention. It can be controlled. It can be focused on a particular sensory modality or item. It can be divided. It can set a perceptual system. It has

More information

L'application de la TMS dans l'étude des fonctions cognitives

L'application de la TMS dans l'étude des fonctions cognitives L'application de la TMS dans l'étude des fonctions cognitives Chotiga Pattamadilok Laboratoire Parole et Langage Aix-en-Provence RMN- Oct. 2016 Magnetic stimulation Silvanus P. Thompson: on inserting the

More information

Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact left-hemisphere of patients with neglect

Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact left-hemisphere of patients with neglect doi:10.1093/brain/awn273 Brain (2008), 131,3147^3155 Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact left-hemisphere of patients with neglect Giacomo Koch, 1,2 Massimiliano Oliveri,

More information

Space amputation in neglect?

Space amputation in neglect? Space amputation in neglect? Bisiach & Luzzatti, Cortex 14:129-33, 1978 Space amputation in neglect? Revisited!? 71% 29% Visual only Imaginal + visual Bartolomeo, D Erme & Gainotti, Neurology 44:1710-4,

More information

HAL author manuscript

HAL author manuscript HAL author manuscript Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior 2008;44(2):10 The neural correlates of visual mental imagery: an ongoing debate Paolo Bartolomeo Inserm Unit

More information

Pain is more than an unpleasant feeling associated with a somatosensory sensation.

Pain is more than an unpleasant feeling associated with a somatosensory sensation. Where is my pain? Pain is more than an unpleasant feeling associated with a somatosensory sensation. It is an important signal that prompts identification, localization, and reaction to a potential physical

More information

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch!

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! How do we perceive objects held in the hand?! Touch receptors deconstruct objects to detect local features! Information is transmitted in parallel

More information

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest Experimental Design Kate Watkins Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford With thanks to: Heidi Johansen-Berg Joe Devlin Outline Choices for experimental paradigm Subtraction / hierarchical

More information

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 Psycholinguistics / Neurolinguistics Happy Hour!!! Tuesdays 7/11, 7/18, 7/25 5:30-6:30 PM @ the Boone Center

More information

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience Levels of organization Central Nervous System 1m 10 11 neurons Neural systems and neuroanatomy Systems 10cm Networks 1mm Neurons 100μm 10 8 neurons Professor Daniel Leeds

More information

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D.

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. The origins of localization The concept that different parts of the brain did different

More information

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention Chapter 6 Attention Attention William James, in 1890, wrote Everyone knows what attention is. Attention is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously

More information

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Overview Explain the significance of the turn to the brain in cognitive science Explain Mishkin and Ungerleider s hypothesis that there are two distinct visual systems Outline

More information

Higher Cortical Function

Higher Cortical Function Emilie O Neill, class of 2016 Higher Cortical Function Objectives Describe the association cortical areas processing sensory, motor, executive, language, and emotion/memory information (know general location

More information

Making the blindsighted see

Making the blindsighted see Neuropsychologia 45 (2007) 3346 3350 Note Making the blindsighted see Juha Silvanto a,, Alan Cowey b, Nilli Lavie a, Vincent Walsh a a Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychology,

More information

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg The concept that different parts of the brain did different things started with Spurzheim and Gall, whose phrenology became quite

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

The origins of localization

The origins of localization Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. The origins of localization The concept that different parts of the brain did different

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

The Central Nervous System

The Central Nervous System The Central Nervous System Cellular Basis. Neural Communication. Major Structures. Principles & Methods. Principles of Neural Organization Big Question #1: Representation. How is the external world coded

More information

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice Multiple Choice 1. Which structure is not part of the visual pathway in the brain? a. occipital lobe b. optic chiasm c. lateral geniculate nucleus *d. frontal lobe Answer location: Visual Pathways 2. Which

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

Attentional Networks and the Semantics of Consciousness 1

Attentional Networks and the Semantics of Consciousness 1 Attentional Networks and the Semantics of Consciousness 1 Michael I. Posner University of Oregon ABSTRACT The term consciousness is an important one in the vernacular of the western literature in many

More information

Uni- and cross-modal temporal modulation of tactile extinction in right brain damaged patients

Uni- and cross-modal temporal modulation of tactile extinction in right brain damaged patients Neuropsychologia 42 (2004) 1689 1696 Uni- and cross-modal temporal modulation of tactile extinction in right brain damaged patients Domenica Bueti a, Marcello Costantini b, Bettina Forster c, Salvatore

More information

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013 Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013 Summary of EEG findings The intent of the EEG monitoring was to see which

More information

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications An Overview of BMIs Luca Rossini Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications European Space Research and Technology Centre, European Space Agency Noordvijk, 30 th November 2009 Definition

More information

A Return to the Gorilla What Effects What

A Return to the Gorilla What Effects What A Return to the Gorilla What Effects What We Attend to and What We Don t Simons and Chabis found that although only 43% of people noticed the gorilla, there was no way to predict who would and who would

More information

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): 359-364 ISSN 0970-938X http://www.biomedres.info Investigating relative strengths and positions of electrical activity in the left and right hemispheres of the human brain

More information

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical Short Communication TheScientificWorldJOURNAL (2006) 6, 1805 1809 ISSN 1537-744X; DOI 10.1100/tsw.2006.291 Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical V. Frak 1,2, *, D. Bourbonnais 2, I. Croteau

More information

Contents. Boxes xii Preface xiii Acknowledgments. Background and Methods

Contents. Boxes xii Preface xiii Acknowledgments. Background and Methods Contents Boxes xii Preface xiii Acknowledgments xv PARTI Background and Methods 1 A Brief History of Cognitive Neuroscience 2 A Historical Perspective 4 The Brain Story 5 The Psychological Story 10 The

More information

An exploration of the role of the superior temporal gyrus in visual search and spatial perception using TMS

An exploration of the role of the superior temporal gyrus in visual search and spatial perception using TMS DOI: 10.1093/brain/awh244 Brain (2004), 127, 2307 2315 An exploration of the role of the superior temporal gyrus in visual search and spatial perception using TMS Amanda Ellison, Igor Schindler, Lara L.

More information

Learning Objectives.

Learning Objectives. Emilie O Neill, class of 2016 Learning Objectives 1. Describe the types of deficits that occur with lesions in association areas including: prosopagnosia, neglect, aphasias, agnosia, apraxia 2. Discuss

More information

Unilateral Spatial Neglect USN

Unilateral Spatial Neglect USN 86 214 28 2 Behavioural Inattention Test Catherine Bergego Scale 28 2 214 223 2008 Key Words unilateral spatial neglect language disturbance mechanism priming rehabilitation Unilateral Spatial Neglect

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

Water immersion modulates sensory and motor cortical excitability

Water immersion modulates sensory and motor cortical excitability Water immersion modulates sensory and motor cortical excitability Daisuke Sato, PhD Department of Health and Sports Niigata University of Health and Welfare Topics Neurophysiological changes during water

More information

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Theories of Sensory, short-term & long-term memories Memory & brain Cellular bases

More information

Jennifer T. Coull 1 and Anna C. Nobre 1,2

Jennifer T. Coull 1 and Anna C. Nobre 1,2 The Journal of Neuroscience, September 15, 1998, 18(18):7426 7435 Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

Acetylcholine (ACh) Action potential. Agonists. Drugs that enhance the actions of neurotransmitters.

Acetylcholine (ACh) Action potential. Agonists. Drugs that enhance the actions of neurotransmitters. Acetylcholine (ACh) The neurotransmitter responsible for motor control at the junction between nerves and muscles; also involved in mental processes such as learning, memory, sleeping, and dreaming. (See

More information

Top-down and bottom-up attentional guidance: investigating the role of the dorsal and ventral parietal cortices

Top-down and bottom-up attentional guidance: investigating the role of the dorsal and ventral parietal cortices Exp Brain Res (2010) 206:197 208 DOI 10.1007/s00221-010-2326-z RESEARCH ARTICLE Top-down and bottom-up attentional guidance: investigating the role of the dorsal and ventral parietal cortices Sarah Shomstein

More information

Attention and Scene Perception

Attention and Scene Perception Theories of attention Techniques for studying scene perception Physiological basis of attention Attention and single cells Disorders of attention Scene recognition attention any of a large set of selection

More information

Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain

Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain Motivating Questions Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D. Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory COGS1 class How do our brains perceive the mental states of others

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

9.00 BLINDSIGHT & NEGLECT. Professor John Gabrieli

9.00 BLINDSIGHT & NEGLECT. Professor John Gabrieli 9.00 BLINDSIGHT & NEGLECT Professor John Gabrieli Objectives Blindsight what it is brain basis unconscious perception Neglect what it is brain basis construction of visual attention Left optic tract Left

More information

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage INO056 10/18/04 6:34 PM Page 339 CHAPTER 56 ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage Leon Y. Deouell and Robert T. Knight ABSTRACT Maintaining a goal-directed behavior requires

More information

Sensorimotor Functioning. Sensory and Motor Systems. Functional Anatomy of Brain- Behavioral Relationships

Sensorimotor Functioning. Sensory and Motor Systems. Functional Anatomy of Brain- Behavioral Relationships Sensorimotor Functioning Sensory and Motor Systems Understanding brain-behavior relationships requires knowledge of sensory and motor systems. Sensory System = Input Neural Processing Motor System = Output

More information

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention Goldsmiths Research Online. The Authors. Originally published: Science vol.291 2 March 2001 1803-1806. http://www.sciencemag.org. 11 October 2000; accepted 17 January 2001 The Role of Working Memory in

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

Functional Neuroanatomy. IBRO ISN African Neuroscience School 4-13 th Dec 2014 Nairobi, Kenya

Functional Neuroanatomy. IBRO ISN African Neuroscience School 4-13 th Dec 2014 Nairobi, Kenya Functional Neuroanatomy IBRO ISN African Neuroscience School 4-13 th Dec 2014 Nairobi, Kenya What is/are the function(s) of the nervous system? Sensation Perception Visceral activities (Homeostasis) Behavior

More information

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp 1288-1290 Improved executive functioning following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation [Brief Communications] Moser,

More information

CONTENTS. Foreword George H. Kraft. Henry L. Lew

CONTENTS. Foreword George H. Kraft. Henry L. Lew EVOKED POTENTIALS Foreword George H. Kraft xi Preface Henry L. Lew xiii Overview of Artifact Reduction and Removal in Evoked Potential and Event-Related Potential Recordings 1 Martin R. Ford, Stephen Sands,

More information

Spatial Attention: Unilateral Neglect

Spatial Attention: Unilateral Neglect Spatial Attention: Unilateral Neglect attended location (70-90 ms after target onset). Self portrait, copying, line bisection tasks: In all cases, patients with parietal/temporal lesions seem to forget

More information

Cédric Lenoir, Maxime Algoet, Camille Vanderclausen, André Peeters, Susana Ferrao Santos, André Mouraux

Cédric Lenoir, Maxime Algoet, Camille Vanderclausen, André Peeters, Susana Ferrao Santos, André Mouraux Accepted Manuscript Report of one confirmed generalized seizure and one suspected partial seizure induced by deep continuous theta burst stimulation of the right operculo-insular cortex Cédric Lenoir,

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Organization of the nervous system. The withdrawal reflex. The central nervous system. Structure of a neuron. Overview

Organization of the nervous system. The withdrawal reflex. The central nervous system. Structure of a neuron. Overview Overview The nervous system- central and peripheral The brain: The source of mind and self Neurons Neuron Communication Chemical messengers Inside the brain Parts of the brain Split Brain Patients Organization

More information

Durham Research Online

Durham Research Online Durham Research Online Deposited in DRO: 08 February 2010 Version of attached file: Accepted Version Peer-review status of attached file: Peer-reviewed Citation for published item: Ellison, A. and Schindler,

More information