Lesions of the Perirhinal Cortex Interfere With Conditioned Excitation but Not With Conditioned Inhibition of Fear

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Lesions of the Perirhinal Cortex Interfere With Conditioned Excitation but Not With Conditioned Inhibition of Fear"

Transcription

1 Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1997, Vol. 111, No. 3, /97/$3.00 Lesions of the Perirhinal Cortex Interfere With Conditioned Excitation but Not With Conditioned Inhibition of Fear William A. Falls, Karl T. Bakken, and Scott A. Heldt Northern Illinois University Posttraining lesions of the perirhinal cortex (Prh) have been shown to interfere with the expression of fear. This study assessed whether Prh lesions would also disrupt the inhibition of fear as measured with conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle. Following light + shock, noise ~ light-no shock conditioned-inhibition training, rats were given Prh lesions. The lesions interfered with the expression of fear-potentiated startle to the light. To assess whether conditioned inhibition was affected, the rats were given light + retraining without additional noise --* light - training. The noise-conditioned inhibitor retained its ability to inhibit fear-potentiated startle to the retrained light. These results suggest that the areas of the Prh that are essential for the initial expression of conditioned fear are not important for the expression of conditioned inhibition of fear. Considerable progress has been made in identifying the neural systems that are involved in conditioned fear (Davis, Campeau, Kim, & Falls, 1995; LeDoux, 1987; Mishkin & Appenzeller, 1987). With very few exceptions, these studies have focused on investigating the neural systems involved in the acquisition and expression of conditioned fear. However, fear is not only acquired and expressed, but under some circumstances fear can be reduced or inhibited. Very little is known about the neural systems that are involved in the inhibition of fear. Clearly, to more fully understand the neural systems involved in conditioned fear, it is necessary to also evaluate the neural systems that are involved in the inhibition of fear. Falls and Davis (1997) have detailed a procedure that may be particularly well suited for evaluating the neural systems that are involved in the inhibition of fear: conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle. In the basic fearpotentiated startle paradigm, conditioned fear is operationally defined as elevated startle amplitude in the presence of a light that has been previously paired with shock (Davis & Astrachan, 1978). Conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle was produced by giving rats training in which a light was paired with shock (i.e., light+), and a serial noise and light compound was presented without shock (i.e., noise---* light-). Following this training the rats showed fear-potentiated startle to the light, and significantly less fear-potentiated startle to the light when it was preceded by the noise-conditioned inhibitor, suggesting that the noise had acquired the ability to inhibit fear. Control experiments revealed that neither a novel noise nor a noise nonreinforced William A. Falls, Karl T. Bakken, and Scott A. Heldt, Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University. We are grateful to Gary Coover and James Corwin for their comments on a draft of this article. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to William A. Falls, Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University, De Kalb, Illinois Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to niu.edu. separately from the light (i.e., following light+, noise-, light- training) inhibited fear-potentiated startle to the light. These results suggest that the inhibitory effect of the noise was acquired through nonreinforcement of the serial noise --* light compound and cannot be explained solely in terms of an unconditioned effect of the noise on fearpotentiated startle to the light (cf. Papini & Bitterman, 1993; Swartzentruber, 1995). Additional experiments revealed that the noise-conditioned inhibitor passed both the transfer and retardation tests for conditioned inhibition (Rescorla, 1969). Specifically, following light+, noise ---* light- training, fear-potentiated startle to a separately trained conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., a tactile CS) was also inhibited by the noise (transfer) and subsequent acquisition of conditioned fear to the noise was retarded (retardation). Similar results were obtained when the stimulus modalities were reversed. Together these results suggest that the inhibitory effect of the noise cannot be understood solely in terms of an increase in attention to the noise, generalization decrement, external inhibition, or a modality-specific effect of a noise stimulus on fear produced by a light (cf. Papini & Bitterman, 1993). Rather, these results strongly suggest that the noise acquired the ability to inhibit fear produced by the light (Falls, 1993; Falls & Davis, 1997). Hence, this procedure may be well suited for evaluating the neural systems that are involved in the inhibition of fear. Much is known about the neural systems involved in the expression of fear-potentiated startle (Davis et al., 1995). Although the details have not been worked out, it is believed that the conditioned stimulus is processed through various subcortical sensory structures (Campeau & Davis, 1995a) to either the perirhinal cortex or primary sensory cortex and on to the lateral and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala (Campeau & Davis, 1995a; Rosen et al., 1991; Sananes & Davis, 1992). These amygdala nuclei project to the central nucleus of the amygdala, which in turn projects to the sensorimotor interface of the startle reflex pathway (Hitchcock & Davis, 1991; Koch & Ebert, 1993; Yeomans & Polard, 1993). Falls and Davis (1995b) suggest that one way to approach 476

2 PERIRHINAL CORTEX LESIONS 477 identifying the neural systems involved in conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle is to first outline the neural circuitry involved in the expression of fear and to then evaluate where along this circuitry a conditioned inhibitor affects fear-potentiated startle. Once this site is identified, it should be possible to determine those areas activated by a conditioned inhibitor by identifying the brain areas that project to this site in the fear-potentiated startle circuit. Following this logic, Fails and Davis (1995b) examined whether lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala would affect the performance of conditioned inhibition. If a conditioned inhibitor affects fear-potentiated startle at the central nucleus of the amygdala, lesioning it should produce a deficit in the expression of previously acquired conditioned inhibition. However, an obstacle in this study was that lesions of the central nucleus would produce a deficit in the expression of fear-potentiated startle (Campeau & Davis, 1995b; Hitchcock & Davis, 1986; Kim & Davis, 1993). This deficit would preclude the assessment of conditioned inhibition because fear would have to be present in order to assess the inhibition of fear (Falls & Davis, 1995b; LoLordo & Fairless, 1985). To overcome this problem, Fails and Davis (1995b) capitalized on a previous observation showing that fear-potentiated startle could be reacquired following lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala (Kim & Davis, 1993). Therefore, to evaluate the effect of central nucleus lesions on the expression of conditioned inhibition, rats were given conditioned inhibition training followed by lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala. The rats were then given light + shock retraining with no further conditioned inhibition training (i.e., no further noise ~ light- training). Falls and Davis found that conditioned inhibition was retained following lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala. This suggested that areas of the central nucleus important for the expression of fear-potentiated startle were not critical for the expression of conditioned inhibition. Furthermore, these results suggested that the conditioned inhibitor affected fear-potentiated startle elsewhere in the circuit, either afferent or efferent to the central nucleus of the amygdala. The perirhinal cortex (Prh) is also an important component of the neural systems involved in fear-potentiated startle. Lesions of the Prh produce deficits in the expression of fear-potentiated startle to both visual (Campeau & Davis, 1995a; Rosen et al., 1991) and auditory (Campeau & Davis, 1995a) conditioned stimuli. However, the role of the Prh in the inhibition of fear-potentiated startle is not known. It is possible that the conditioned inhibitor reduces fear by some action in the Prh, perhaps by altering the processing of fear-related (e.g., CS) information. Alternatively, the Prh could be a structure through which the conditioned inhibitor is processed en route to its insertion at lower levels of the fear-potentiated startle circuit. The purpose of the present study was to evaluate whether the Prh plays a role in the expression of conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle. As with the central nucleus of the amygdala, evaluating the involvement of the Prh in the expression of conditioned inhibition is complicated by the fact that lesions of the Prh interfere with the expression of fear-potentiated startle. However, we have found that rats sustaining posttraining lesions of the Prh can be made to show fear-potentiated startle if they are given light+ retraining after the lesion. This allows us to evaluate whether lesions of the Prh interfere with expression of conditioned inhibition acquired before the lesion. Experiment 1 Experiment 1 evaluated whether the Prh is important for the expression of previously acquired conditioned inhibition. Two groups of rats were given light+, noise ---, lightconditioned inhibition training (Fails & Davis, 1997), Next, one group was given lesions of the Prh, and the other was given sham lesions. Both groups were then given light+ retraining with no further conditioned inhibition training. If the Prh is important for the expression of conditioned inhibition, the noise should not retain its ability to inhibit fear-potentiated startle to the retrained light. If, on the other hand, the Prh is not important for the expression of conditioned inhibition, the noise should retain its ability to inhibit fear-potentiated startle to the retrained light. Subjects Me~od A total of 20 experimentally naive, male, albino Sprague- Dawley rats were obtained from the Northern Ilfinois University Psychology Department rat colony. The rats weighed between 350 and 400 g at the start of the experiment. All rats were housed singly in hanging wire mesh cages (18 25 x 20 cm) and maintained on a 12-hr light-dark cycle (lights on at 7 a.m.) with food and water continuously available. Apparatus Conditioning and fear-potentiated startle testing were conducted in four identical stabilimeter devices. Each stabilimeter consisted of an 8 X cm Plexiglas and wire mesh cage suspended between compression springs within a steel frame. The floor of each stabilimeter consists of four 4-mm diameter stainless steel bars spaced 20 mm apart, through which shock could be administered. Cage movement results in displacement of an accelerometer with the resulting voltage being proportional to the velocity of displacement. The analog output of the accelerometer was amplified (Fintronics Accelerometer Amplifier, Model FA-560, Orange, CT) and digitized on a scale of 0 to 4,096 units by a MacADIOS II board (GW Instruments, Somerville, MA) interfaced to a Macintosh Power PC 7100/66. Startle amplitude was defined as the peak to peak accelerometer voltage that occurred during the first 200 ms after the onset of the startle stimulus. Each stabilimeter was located within a separate ventilated, sound attenuating chamber (58 x 41 x 36 cm; Industrial Acoustics, Bronx, NY). Ventilation fans produced background noise of 55 db sound pressure level (SPL). The startle stimulus was a 50-ms burst of white noise having a rise-decay time of 2.5 ms. The startle stimulus was provided through a high-frequency speaker (Radio Shack Super Tweeter, Tandy, Fort Worth, TX) located 2 cm from the rear of each stabilimeter. Three startle stimulus intensities were used, 95, 105, and 115 db SPL. The light CS was produced by an

3 478 FALLS, BAKKEN, AND HELDT 8-W fluorescent bulb located 2 cm from the rear of each stabilimeter. The light was controlled by a specially designed light control unit (Fintronics, Orange, CT) that allowed a near instantaneous rise time (100 ~ts) and control of light intensity. The light luminance was 630 footlamberts (6,781 ix). The noise CS was a 70-dB SPL white noise that was band-pass filtered with a Krohn-Hite bandpass filter (Model 3100A, Avon, MA) with high and low passes both set at 4 khz. The noise CS was delivered through an 8-in. full-range speaker (Radio Shack, Model C) located 15 cm to the left of each stabilimeter. The unconditioned stimulus was scrambled shock generated by two constant-current shock generators (Lafayette, Model 82404/5-SS, Lafayette, IN) located outside of the isolation chambers. Shock intensity was 0.6 ma. The timing and presentation of stimuli were controlled by SuperScope II data acquisition and control software (GW Instruments, Somerville, MA) running on a Macintosh Power PC 7100/66. Procedure Initial startle test. Before conditioned inhibition training, a brief test session was given to familiarize the rats to handling, the apparatus, and the startle stimulus. Rats were placed in the stabilimeter and 5 min later presented with 10 startle stimuli at each of three intensities, 95, 105, and 115 db, for a total of 30 startle stimuli. The three noise-burst intensities were presented in balanced irregular order with a 30-s interval between successive stimuli. These pretest data were not used. Training. The basic experimental procedure is outlined in Table 1. Training was conducted in two phases. In Phase 1 the rats were given pairings of a light and shock. This training was meant to establish the light as a conditioned excitor of fear. This was followed by Phase 2 training that included nonreinforcement of a serial noise --~ light compound meant to establish the noise as a conditioned inhibitor of fear. Phase 1 began 1 day after the pretest. On two consecutive days, the rats were placed in the stabilimeters and 5 min later given 15 light+ trials consisting of a 4-s light coterminating with a 500-ms, 0.6-mA shock. The mean intertrial interval (ITI; defined for training as the interval between successive lights) was 2 min (range min). Phase 2 training began 1 day after the completion of Phase 1. The rats were placed in the stabilimeters and 5 min later given the first training trial. Training consisted of 5 light+ trials identical to those given in Phase l, intermixed with 15 nonreinforced, serial noise --~ light compound trials. The noise stimulus was also 4 s long. The stimuli were presented serially such that the offset of the noise was coincident with the onset of the light. The two trial types were presented in a pseudorandom sequence with a mean ITI of 2 min (range 1,5-2.5 min). Presurgery testing. Conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle was assessed 1 day after the last training day. The rats were placed in the stabilimeter and, after a 5-min period during which no stimuli were administered, given 10 startle stimuli at each of three intensities, 95, 105, and 115 db, for a total of 30 startle stimuli. This was followed immediately by five startle-stimulus-alone test trials, five light test trials, and five noise --, light test trials at each of three startle stimulus intensities. For light test trials, the startle stimulus occurred 3.5 s after the onset of the light (i.e., the time at which the shock unconditioned stimulus would have occurred). On noise --* light test trials the startle stimulus occurred 3.5 s after the onset of the light (i.e., 3.5 s after the offset of the noise). All trial types were presented in a pseudorandom sequence with the constraint that each trial type occur only once in each consecutive three-trial block. The interval between successive startle stimuli was 30 s. Surgery. One to 2 days after the presurgery test, rats were given bilateral lesions of the perirhinal cortex (n = 10) or sham operations (n = 10). The rats were anesthetized with chloral hydrate (400 mg/kg ip) and placed in a Kopf stereotaxic instrument. The skin was retracted, and the muscles attached to either side of the skull were cut and retracted. Holes were drilled in the skull, and the lateral surface of the skull was chipped away. Kopf Model NE-300 electrodes (0.25 mm diameter) insulated to within 0.5 mm of the tip were used to make DC anodal lesions. Campeau and Davis (1995a) reported that Prh lesions interfered with fear-potentiated startle only when the lesion included most of the rostrocaudal extent of the Prh. To accomplish this, we made three lesions along the rhinal sulcus on each side of the brain. The anterior-posterior axis was referenced from bregma, the lateral coordinates from the midline, and the dorsoventral coordinates from the surface of the skull measured at bregma. The coordinates were (1) AP - 1.6, ML _+ 6.2, DV -7.8; (2)AP -4, ML _+ 6.5, DV -7.7; and (3)AP -5, ML , DV Each lesion was made by passing 1.0 ma of current for 12 s. For sham-operated rats the electrode was lowered to the same coordinates, but no current was passed. Postsurgery testing. Conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle was assessed 7 to 10 days after surgery. The test was identical to the presurgery test. Retraining. Light+ retraining began 1 day after the postsurgery test. On five consecutive days, the rats were placed in the stabilimeters and 5 min later given the first of 15 light+ trials identical to the trials given in Phase 1. Post-retraining testing. Conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle was assessed 1 day after retraining. The test was identical to the previous two tests. Table 1 Experimental Designs Training phase Post light+ 1 2 Presurgery test Lesion Postsurgery test Retraining retraining test Experiment 1 Perirhinal cortex light+ light+, noise --~ light- light, noise --* light ~ ~ light, noise ~ light light+ Sham light, noise ---, light Experiment 2 Perirhinal cortex light+ light+ light, noise --* light ~ ~ / light, noise ~ light light+ light, noise --~ light Sham Note. Noise ---* light denotes serial presentation of stimuli; plus sign denotes shock; minus sign denotes the omission of shock. In testing, fear-potentiated startle was assessed in the presence and absence of the light when it was and was not preceded by the noise.

4 Histology. At the end of the experiment, animals were overdosed with chloral hydrate and perfused intracardially with 0.9% saline followed by 10% buffered formalin phosphate. The brains were removed and stored for at least 48 hr in 20% (wt/vol) sucrose. Frozen sections 60 Ima thick were taken through the lesion site and stained with cresyl violet. The lesions were transcribed onto atlas plates (Paxinos & Watson, 1986). Data reduction and statistical analyses. The mean startle amplitudes from the three startle stimulus intensities for the no-cs (i.e., startle-stimulus-alone test trials), light, noise ~ light, and noise test trials were pooled to form a single mean for each rat for each of these trial types. Difference scores were then calculated for each rat by subtracting the mean startle amplitude obtained on no-cs test trials from the mean startle amplitude obtained on trials in which a CS was presented (i.e., light, noise ~ light, and noise test trials). The resulting difference scores reflect the magnitude of fear-potentiated startle in the presence of each CS. Because statistical differences observed in the difference scores might be the result of differences in startle amplitude on no-cs test trials, mean startle amplitudes on no-cs test trials were analyzed separately with an analysis of variance (ANOVA). The mean startle amplitudes for no-cs test trials are presented in the captions for Figures 1 and 2. No significant differences were found in no-cs test trials. Consequently, difference scores for each of the three tests were computed and analyzed with an ANOVA with trial type (light or noise ~ light) as a within-subjects factor and group (i.e., lesion or sham) as a between-subjects factor. Subsequent comparisons were made with t tests. The difference scores calculated on noise test trials are not required for the assessment of conditioned inhibition and were analyzed separately to determine whether the lesion produced any unexpected changes in responding to the noiseconditioned inhibitor alone. PERIRHINAL CORTEX LESIONS 479 Histology Results and Discussion The data from I rat with unilateral sparing of the Prh were excluded from the data analysis. The remaining 9 lesioned rats had bilateral destruction of the Prh that extended along the rostrocaudal extent of the rhinal sulcus from bregma -1.8 to bregma -5.3 (Paxinos & Watson, 1986). The lesions typically encroached upon parietal cortex dorsal to the Prh and piriform cortex and entorhinal cortex ventral to the Prh. Lesions did not extend beyond the external capsule, and in no case was there identifiable damage to the lateral or basolateral nuclei of the amygdala. Figure 1 shows a serial reconstruction of the smallest and largest lesion. Behavioral Data One sham-operated rat died during surgery, so its presurgery data were excluded from the statistical analysis. Figure 2 shows the results of the presurgery, postsurgery, and postretraining tests for conditioned inhibition. Prior to surgery the noise inhibited fear-potentiated startle to the light in both groups, F(I, 16) = 30.32, p <.001. Following surgery, rats sustaining lesions of the Prh no longer showed fear-potentiated startle to the light. The ANOVA conducted on the postsurgery data revealed a significant effect of group, F(1, 16) = 11.79, p <.01, and trial type, F(1, 16) = 4.86, p <.05, but no group by trial type interaction, F(1, 16) = Figure 1. A serial reconstruction showing the extent of the largest (hatched) and smallest (solid) perirhinal cortex lesion. Plates follow Paxinos and Watson (1986). Coordinates are relative to bregma. 1.23, p >.05. Sham-lesioned rats showed fear-potentiated startle, t(8) = 2.87, p <.05, whereas Prh-lesioned rats did not, t(8) = 1.67, p <.05, leading to a significant difference between the two groups on light test trials, t(16) = 4.54, p <.00 I. Closer inspection of Figure 2 shows that the magnitude of fear-potentiated startle to the light was reduced from the presurgery to the postsurgery test in sham-operated rats. A similar reduction in fear-potentiated startle has been reported under conditions of repeated testing (Falls & Davis, 1997) and is most likely a consequence of extinction that occurs over the course of repeated testing (Falls, 1993; Falls & Davis, 1997). Despite this, the noise inhibited this

5 480 FALLS, BAKKEN, AND HELDT Presurgery Post,surgery [] Light Noise~Light [] Difference Post-retraining r~ 100 o e~ -100 ~ ~ Perirhinal Cortex Sham Lesions Lesions Perirhinal Cortex Sham Lesions Lesions Perirhinal Cortex Sham Lesions Lesions Figure 2. Conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle measured before surgery, after surgery, and after light + shock retraining in rats sustaining perirhinal cortex (Prh) lesions or sham lesions. The data shown are the mean startle difference scores calculated by subtracting the amplitude of startle obtained on no-cs (conditioned stimulus) test trials from the amplitude of startle obtained on trials in which a CS was presented (i.e., light and serial noise --, fight test trials). Also shown are the differences between the two trial types. A negative difference represents conditioned inhibition. Mean startle amplitudes on no-cs test trials were as follows: Prh lesions: 383, 395, and 313 for presurgery, postsurgery, and post-retraining, respectively. Sham lesions: 471, 363, and 367 for presurgery, postsurgery, and post-retraining, respectively. smaller magnitude of fear-potentiated startle in shamlesioned rats, t(8) = 2.34, p <.05. The postretraining data show that following light + retraining, Prh-lesioned rats could still show fear-potentiated startle to the light. These data are consistent with those reported by Campeau and Davis (1995a) showing that posttraining lesions of the Prh did not interfere with the acquisition of fear-potentiated startle. An ANOVA on the postretraining data revealed a significant effect of trial type, F(1, 16) = 44.18, p <.001, and no effect of group, F(1, 16) = 1.46, p >.05, or group by trial type interaction, F < 1. Most importantly, the noise still significantly inhibited fear-potentiated startle to the retrained light in lesioned rats, t(8) = 4.44, p <.01, even though there was no further noise ---* light- retraining. The retention of conditioned inhibition in Prh lesions cannot be attributed to rapid reacquisition of conditioned inhibition during testing. Analysis of the first three trials of the postretraining test revealed a significant effect of trial type, F(1, 16) = 17.28,p <.001, and no effect of group, F(1, 16) = 2.78, p >.05, or group by trial type interaction, F < 1. This is strong evidence that conditioned inhibition was retained following Prh lesions. There were no differences between the groups in responding to the noise-conditioned inhibitor alone, F(1, 16) = 1.24, p >.05 (data not shown). Hence, the lesion did not produce any unexpected responding to the noise-conditioned inhibitor. The results of the present experiment suggest that Prh lesions interfere with the expression of conditioned fear but do not interfere with the expression of conditioned inhibition of fear. The results of several behavioral experiments suggest that following light+, noise --, light- training the noise acquires the ability to inhibit fear. Neither a novel noise nor a noise nonreinforced separately from the light (i.e., light+, noise-) significantly inhibits fear-potentiated startle to the light (Falls, 1993; Falls & Davis, 1997). However, it is possible that in animals sustaining lesions of the Prh, the nonassociative inhibitory effect of the noise is somehow enhanced. If true, this nonassociative inhibition could mask any effect the lesion might have had on conditioned inhibition. The purpose of Experiment 2 was to examine the nonassociative inhibitory effect of the noise in rats sustaining lesions of the Prh. Experiment 2 Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1 in all respects except that the rats were given light+ training only. Hence, the noise was neutral at the time of testing. Subjects Method A total of 19 experimentally naive, male, albino Sprague- Dawley rats were obtained from the Northern Illinois University Psychology Department rat colony. The rats weighed between 350 and 400 g at the start of the experiment. The rats were housed and maintained as described in Experiment 1.

6 PERIRHINAL CORTEX LESIONS ' ,2., 100 Presurgery Postsurgery Post-retraining I [] Light ] II Noise---~Light [] Difference V~ ] ~ i r~ -200 t Perirhinal Cortex Sham Lesions Perirhinal Cortex Sham Lesions Perirhinal Cortex Lesions Lesions Lesions Sham Lesions Figure 3. Nonassociative inhibition of fear-potentiated startle measured before surgery, after surgery, and after light + shock retraining in rats sustaining perirhinal cortex (Prh) lesions or sham lesions. Prior to surgery these rats were given light + shock training only. Hence, the noise was novel at the time of testing. The data shown are the mean startle difference scores calculated by subtracting the amplitude of startle obtained on no-cs (conditioned stimulus) test trials from the amplitude of startle obtained on trials in which a CS was presented (i.e., light and serial noise ----, light test trials). Also shown are the differences between the two trial types. A negative difference represents nonassociative inhibition. Mean startle amplitudes on no-cs test trials were as follows: Prh lesions: 444, 487, and 613 for presurgery, postsurgery, and post-retraining, respectively. Sham lesions: 454, 395, and 410 for presurgery, postsurgery, and post-retraining, respectively. Apparatus The apparatus was the same as that used in Experiment 1. Procedure Training. The basic experimental procedure is outlined in Table 1. Following the initial startle test, rats were given pairings of a light+ in two phases. In Phase 1 the rats were given 2 days of light+ training consisting of 15 light+ trials. In Phase 2 the rats were given 5 additional days of light+ training consisting of 15 light+ trials each day. Testing, surgery, and retraining. Nonassociative inhibition of fear-potentiated startle was assessed 1 day after the last training day. The test was identical to the tests described in Experiment 1. One to two days after the presurgery test, rats were given bilateral lesions of the Prh (n = 10) or sham operations (n = 9) as described for Experiment 1. Nonassociative inhibition of fear-potentiated startle was assessed 7 to 10 days after surgery and was followed by light+ retraining consisting of 15 light+ trials on each of 5 days. Finally, postretraining nonassociative inhibition was assessed 1 day after the last retraining day. Histology Results The 10 lesioned rats had complete bilateral lesions of the Prh similar to those described in Experiment 1. The lesions typically encroached upon parietal cortex, but in no case did the lesions extend medially beyond the external capsule. Behavioral Data Figure 3 shows the results of the presurgery, postsurgery and postretraining tests for nonassociative inhibition. The presurgery data show that the neutral noise did not inhibit fear-potentiated startle, F(1, 17) = 2.27, p >.05. As in Experiment 1, lesions of the Prh interfered with the expression of fear-potentiated startle to the light, t(17) = 4.30,p <.001. Following light+ retraining, Prh-lesioned rats again showed fear-potentiated startle to the light. Most importantly, in the postretraining test the neutral noise did not inhibit fear-potentiated startle in either Prh-lesioned, t(9) < 1, or sham-lesioned rats, t(8) = 1.99,p >.05. There were no differences between the groups in responding on noise-alone trials, F(1, 17) = 2.20, p >.05 (data not shown). Consistent with previous studies (Falls & Davis, 1995b; Falls & Davis, 1997), the neutral noise produced little nonassociative inhibition of fear-potentiated startle to the light CS. Hence, the retention of inhibition in Prh-lesioned animals of Experiment 1 cannot be attributed to a lesionproduced increase in nonassociative inhibition. Recall that behavioral experiments have indicated that light+, noise ----, light- training results in the noise acquiring the ability to inhibit conditioned fear (Falls, 1993; Falls & Davis, 1997). Because rats sustaining lesions of the Prh in Experiment 1 were given conditioned-inhibition training prior to the lesion, the noise must have acquired the ability to inhibit fear in these animals as well. The results of Experiment 2 suggest

7 482 FALLS, BAKKEN, AND HELDT that nonassociative inhibition is not enhanced following Prh lesions. Therefore, Prh-lesioned animals in Experiment 1 must have retained conditioned inhibition, suggesting that areas of the Prh that are important for the initial expression of fear-potentiated startle are not important for the expression of conditioned inhibition. General Discussion The results of this study confirm the importance of the Prh in the initial expression of conditioned fear (cf. Campeau & Davis, 1995a; Corodimas & LeDoux, 1995; Rosen et al., 1991). More importantly, they indicate that the Prh is not critical for the expression of previously acquired conditioned inhibition of fear. This strongly suggests that a conditioned inhibitor does not reduce fear by some action in the Prh, nor is the Prh a structure through which the conditioned inhibitor is processed en route to affecting conditioned fear elsewhere in the conditioned fear circuit. The role that the Prh plays in the expression of conditioned fear is still unclear. One view is that the Prh is included in the normal pathway through which auditory and visual information reaches the amygdala (Campeau & Davis, 1995a; Rosen et al., 1991). Posttraining lesions of the Prh interfere with the expression of conditioned fear because they disrupt this pathway. Reacquisition is thought to take place through alternative CS pathways that do not normally participate in the expression of conditioned fear (Campeau & Davis, 1995a). Under this hypothesis, the present results suggest that a conditioned inhibitor does not reduce fearpotentiated startle by altering the processing of the CS at this sensory convergent zone. In addition, the present results suggest that a conditioned inhibitor, unlike the CS, is not processed through the perirhinal cortex. Therefore, a conditioned inhibitor must act elsewhere in the fear-potentiated startle circuit, and it must use sensory pathways different from those used by the CS en route to its insertion point in the fear-potentiated startle circuit. However, it is possible that as a result of conditionedinhibition training, inhibition develops at the alternative CS pathway as well as at the Prh. For an auditory CS these alternative CS pathways may include direct projections from the auditory thalamus and auditory association areas to the amygdala (LeDoux, Cicchetti, Xagoraris, & Romanski, 1990). Analogous visual pathways to the amygdala have not been worked out. Nevertheless, this could explain how conditioned inhibition is retained following Prh lesions. However, it seems unlikely that inhibition would develop at structures that do not normally participate in the expression of fear-potentiated startle. Therefore, under the hypothesis that the Prh is included in the normal CS pathway to the amygdala, the present results suggest that a conditioned inhibitor affects fear-potentiated startle either afferent or efferent to the Prh. Furthermore, because the inhibition was retained, the present results suggest that the alternative CS pathway shares some of the same afferent or efferent circuitry with the Prh. This shared circuitry may include thalamic sensory structures, the amygdala, or structures efferent to the amygdala. Corodimas and LeDoux (1995) have suggested an alternative view of the role of the Prh in the expression of conditioned fear. In their view the Prh is important for a postacquisitional memory process. Lesions of the Prh interfere with the expression of conditioned fear because they disrupt either memory storage or retrieval (Coover & Mertes, 1995; Corodimas & LeDoux, 1995). Consistent with this, studies conducted in both primates and rats suggest that the Prh plays an important role in memory processes in non-fear-related tasks (Meunier, Bachevalier, Mishkin, & Murray, 1993; Nagahara, Otto, & Gallagher, 1995; Wiig & Bilkey, 1994, 1995; Zola-Morgan, Squire, Amaral, & Suzuki, 1989; Zola-Morgan, Squire, & Ramus, 1994). Under this hypothesis, the present results suggest that the conditioned inhibitor does not affect storage or retrieval by some action at the Prh. In addition, the memory-storage or retrieval function of the Prh necessary for the expression of conditioned fear is not necessary for the expression of conditioned inhibition. Corodimas and LeDoux (1995) suggested that the memory deficit induced by Prh lesions may be specifically related to an inability of animals to process contextual stimuli or use these stimuli to retrieve CS information. The present results suggest that this contextual information is not essential for conditioned inhibition of fear. This issue deserves further study, especially in light of behavioral data that indicate that inhibition can be context dependent (Bouton, 1991; Bouton & Bolles, 1979; Bouton & Nelson, 1994). For example, if conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle is context dependent, the present results suggest that other areas of the brain control the contextual aspects of conditioned inhibition of fear. As previously mentioned, little is known about the neural systems responsible for conditioned inhibition of fear. There are several strategies one could use to identify candidate neural systems. One strategy would be to propose a conceptual definition of the inhibition of fear and then investigate the role of brain systems that might fit with this definition. For example, theorists have long thought that conditioned inhibition is the consequence of conditioning of some process that is antagonistic to the conditioned response of fear. Konorski (1948) argued that conditioned inhibition was the result of the accrual of inhibitory associations between nodal representations of the CS and US. These inhibitory associations would exist side by side with the excitatory, conditioned response-producing associations. Using this hypothesis, one might attempt to determine whether conditioned inhibition is intrinsic to the fear-potentiated startle pathway. Alternatively, its been argued that conditioned inhibition of fear is a consequence of incompatible affect elicited by the conditioned inhibitor (i.e., conditioning of a pleasure or "relief" response; Denny, 1971; McAllister & McAllister, 1992, 1995). Using this hypothesis, one might investigate the role of structures implicated in reward or in appetitive conditioning in conditioned inhibition of fear. Finally, there are behavioral data to suggest that conditioned inhibition might involve an interaction between the sensory representations of the CS and conditioned inhibitor (cf. Bouton & Nelson, 1994; Holland, 1985). Using this hypothesis, one might consider the possibility that conditioned

8 PERIRHINAL CORTEX LESIONS 483 inhibition results from an interaction between thalamic or subthalamic visual and auditory structures. An alternative strategy would be to consider the fact that a conditioned inhibitor of fear must inhibit fear by interacting with the known neural pathways responsible for the production of fear. Hence, it should be possible to evaluate where along these neural pathways a conditioned inhibitor affects fear-potentiated startle (Falls & Davis, 1995b). Once this site is identified, it should then be possible to determine those areas activated by a conditioned inhibitor by identifying the brain areas that project to this site in the fearpotentiated startle circuit. The advantage of this approach is that it does not require commitment to a specific conceptual definition of conditioned inhibition. Instead, it leaves open to empirical identification the nature of conditioned inhibition. Following the latter strategy, Falls and Davis (1995b) showed that a conditioned inhibitor does not affect fearpotentiated startle at the central nucleus of the amygdala. Other components of the fear-potentiated startle circuit where a conditioned inhibitor could act include (a) sensory structures afferent to the amygdala, (b) the lateral nucleus or basolateral nucleus of the amygdala, and (c) points along the pathway from the central nucleus of the amygdala to the startle-reflex pathway. It is possible that a conditioned inhibitor acts somewhere in the sensory system to affect processing of the CS. Falls and Davis (1995b) argued that insertion of inhibition in the CS pathway was unlikely given the demonstration of stimulus transfer. They argued that to account for stimulus transfer, a noise-conditioned inhibitor would have to act simultaneously at every sensory system. As a consequence, Falls and Davis favored the idea that the conditioned inhibitor acts at a more central site where stimuli from different modalities converge (e.g., the lateral or basolateral nuclei of the amygdala). However, as mentioned, behavioral data suggest that conditioned inhibition might involve an interaction between the sensory representations of the CS and conditioned inhibitor (cf. Bouton & Nelson, 1994; Holland, 1985). Recent neuroanatomical data suggest that conditioned inhibition may involve functional interactions between sensory systems. Mclntosh and Gonzalez-Lima (1995) measured fluorodeoxyglucose incorporation in rats given tone+, light/noise- conditioned inhibition training and found lower incorporation in the ventral division of medial geniculate in animals for which the light was inhibitory. This raises the possibility that the light inhibitor affects the processing of the tone CS by reducing activity in the lemniscal auditory pathways. The lateral and basolateral nuclei are critical for the acquisition and expression of conditioned fear: Lesions of these nuclei interfere with conditioned fear to auditory (Campeau & Davis, 1995b; LeDoux et al., 1990; Maren, Aharonov, & Fanselow, 1996), visual (Campeau & Davis, 1995b; Lee, Walker, & Davis, 1996; Sananes & Davis, 1992), and contextual CSs (Maren et al., 1996). It is possible that a conditioned inhibitor acts at the level of the lateral or basolateral nuclei. However, evaluating the role of these nuclei in the expression of conditioned inhibition is compli- cated by the fact that both pretraining and posttraining lesions of the lateral and basolateral nuclei eliminate fearpotentiated startle (Campeau & Davis, 1995b; Lee et al., 1996). And it is unlikely that conditioned fear can be reacquired following posttraining lesions (Maren et al., 1996). Therefore, alternatives to the lesion technique must be used. For example, GABA receptors are abundant in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (Carlsen, 1988; Nitecka & Ben-Ari, 1987; Rainnie, Asprodini, & Shinnick-Gallagher, 1991). It is possible that a conditioned inhibitor reduces fear-potentiated startle by causing the release of GABA in the basolateral nucleus. If so, direct infusion of a GABA antagonist into the basolateral nucleus might reduce conditioned inhibition. A conditioned inhibitor could act efferent to the central nucleus of the amygdala. The central nucleus projects to the caudal pontine reticular nucleus (Campeau & Davis, 1995b; Hitchcock & Davis, 1991), an essential sensorimotor interface of the acoustic startle reflex pathway (Davis, Gendelman, Tischler, & Gendelman, 1982; Koch & Ebert, 1993; Koch, Lingenhohl, & Pilz, 1992; Lingenhohl & Friauf, 1992, 1994). This projection may be both direct (Campeau & Davis, 1995b; Hitchcock & Davis, 1991) and indirect via the midbrain (Fendt, Koch, & Schnitler, 1994; Frankland & Yeomans, 1995; Yeomans & Polard, 1993). Although it does not appear that the conditioned inhibitor directly reduces the startle reflex (Falls & Davis, 1993), a conditioned inhibitor might affect the ability of the central nucleus to modulate the startle reflex either in the midbrain or at the caudal pontine reticular nucleus. If so, a conditioned inhibitor should reduce startle facilitated by either electrical (Rosen & Davis, 1988) or chemical (Koch & Ebert, 1993) stimulation of the central nucleus of the amygdala. Once the site where a conditioned inhibitor acts to inhibit fear-potentiated startle is identified, it will be possible to identify the structures that project to that site that may potentially inhibit it. At the present time there is no direct information concerning the identity of the brain areas that might be activated by a conditioned inhibitor. However, a few brain areas are noteworthy (see Falls & Davis, 1995a). Work by Yadin and Thomas has consistently implicated the lateral septal nucleus in the reduction of fear-related behavior (Thomas, 1988; Yadin & Thomas, 1981, 1992). There have also been suggestions that the inhibition of fear may involve primary sensory cortex (LeDoux, Romanski, & Xagoraris, 1989; Teich et al., 1989) and infralimbiccingulate cortex (Holson, 1986; Jaskiw & Weinberger, 1992; Morgan & LeDoux, 1995; Morgan, Romanski, & LeDoux, 1993). However, there are data to suggest that these areas may not be involved in the reduction of fear in all situations (Falls & Davis, 1993; Gewirtz, Falls, & Davis, in press). For example, Gewirtz et al. found that rats sustaining lesions of the infralimbic-cingulate cortex showed normal conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle suggesting that the infralimbic-cingulate cortex may not be activated by a conditioned inhibitor. In an attempt to begin to identify the brain areas activated by a conditioned inhibitor, Campeau et al. (in press) measured the induction of the mrna for the early immedi-

9 484 FALLS, BAKKEN, AND HELDT ate gene c-fos in rats that were trained for conditioned inhibition. In rats that were presented with the conditioned inhibitor immediately before being killed, specific induction was observed in the ventral nucleus of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and associated septohypothalamic nucleus, the locus coeruleus, and the pedunculopontine nucleus. The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis projects to many of the same brainstem areas as the central nucleus of the amygdala (Holstedge, Meiners, & Tan, 1985), but in several instances it seems to have a functional effect on fear and stress opposite to that of the central nucleus of the amygdala (Henke, 1984; Morrow, Grijalca, Geiselman, & Novin, 1993). The pedunculopontine nucleus, like the central nucleus of the amygdala, projects to the caudal pontine reticular nucleus and has been implicated in the modulation of the startle reflex (Koch et al., 1993). The fact that a conditioned inhibitor specifically activates these structures suggests that the conditioned inhibitor might affect the ability of the central nucleus to modulate the startle reflex. Studies are underway to evaluate this possibility as well as to evaluate whether lesions of these areas will interfere with the expression of conditioned inhibition. In summary, the present data show that lesions of the Prh interfere with the expression of conditioned fear but do not affect the expression of conditioned inhibition of fear. Although the present data do not directly address the debate over whether the perirhinal cortex is involved in the transmission of stimulus information to the amygdala or in some postacquisitional process such as memory storage or retrieval, they do suggest that not all fear-related stimulus information or memory processes involve the perirhinal cortex. In addition, the present results suggest that the perirhinal cortex is not the site in the fear-potentiated startle circuit where a conditioned inhibitor acts to reduce conditioned fear. Together with previous results (Falls & Davis, 1995b), these data suggest that a conditioned inhibitor is more likely to act elsewhere in the fear-potentiated startle circuit, either at sensory structures that process the CS, the lateral or basolateral nuclei of the amygdala, or at some point along the pathway connecting the central nucleus of the amygdala to the startle-reflex pathway. Identification of this site will be important for identifying the neural circuits responsible for the inhibition of fear. References Bouton, M. E. (1991). A contextual analysis of fear extinction. In P. R. Martin (Ed.), Handbook of behavior therapy and psychological science: An integrative approach (pp ). New York: Pergamon Press. Bouton, M. E., & Bolles, R. C. (1979). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learning and Motivation, 10, Bouton, M. E., & Nelson, J. B. (1994). Context-specificity of target versus feature inhibition in a feature negative discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, Campeau, S., & Davis, M. (1995a). Involvement of subcortical and cortical afferents to the lateral nucleus of the amygdala in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle in rats trained concurrently with auditory and visual conditioned stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15, Campeau, S., & Davis, M. (1995b). Involvement of the central nucleus and basolateral complex of the amygdala in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle in rats trained concurrently with auditory and visual conditioned stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15, Campeau, S., Falls, W. A., Cullinan, W. E., Helmreich, D. L., Davis, M., & Watson, S. J. (in press). The elicitation and reduction of fear: Behavioral and endocrinological indices and brain induction of the immediate-early gene c-fos. Neuroscience. Carlsen, J. (1988). Immunocytochemical localization of glutamate decarboxolase in the rat basolateral arnygdaloid nucleus, with special reference to GABAergic innervation of amygdalostriatal projection neurons. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 273, Coover, G. D., & Mertes, S. J. (1995). Posttralning lesions of the perirhinal cortex decrease passive avoidance of punished drinking but not its reinstatement. Socie~. for Neuroscience, Abstracts, 21, Corodimas, K. E, & LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Disruptive effects of posttraining perirhinal cortex lesions on conditioned fear: Contributions of contextual cues. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, Davis, M., & Astrachan, D. I. (1978). Conditioned fear and startle magnitude: Effects of different footshock or backshock intensities used in training. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 4, Davis, M., Campeau, S., Kim, M., & Falls, W. A. (1995). Neural systems of emotion: The amygdala's role in fear and anxiety. In J. L. McGaugh, N. M. Weinberger, & G. Lynch (Eds.), Brain and memory: Modulation and mediation of neuroplasticity (pp. 3-40). New York: Oxford University Press. Davis, M., Gendelman, D. S., Tischler, M. D., & Gendelman, E M. (1982). A primary acoustic startle circuit: Lesions and stimulation studies. Journal of Neuroscience, 6, Denny, M. R. (1971). Relaxation theory and experiments. In E R. Brush (Ed.), Aversive conditioning and learning (pp ). New York: Academic Press. Falls, W. A. (1993). Conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle. Doctoral dissertation, Yale University, New Haven, CT. Falls, W. A., & Davis, M. (1993). Visual cortex ablations do not prevent extinction of conditioned fear to a visual conditioned stimulus. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 60, Falls, W. A., & Davis, M. (1995a). Behavioral and physiological analysis of fear inhibition. In M. J. Freidman, D. S. Charney, & A. Y. Deutch (Eds.), Neurobiological and clinical consequences of stress: From normal adaptation to PTSD (pp ). Raven Press. Falls, W. A., & Davis, M. (1995b). Lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala block conditioned excitation, but not conditioned inhibition of fear as measured with the fear-potentiated startle effect. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, Falls, W. A., & Davis, M. (1997). Inhibition of fear-potentiated startle can be detected after the offset of a feature trained in a serial feature negative discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23, Fendt, M., Koch, M., & Schnitler, H. U. (1994). Lesions of the central gray block the sensitization of the acoustic startle response in rats. Brain Research, 61, Frankland, E W., & Yeomans, J. S. (1995). Fear-potentiated startle and electrically evoked startle mediated by synapses in rostral rnidbrain. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, Gewirtz, J. C., Falls, W. A., & Davis, M. (in press). Normal conditioned inhibition and extinction of freezing and fear-

10 PERIRHINAL CORTEX LESIONS 485 potentiated startle following lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex. Behavioral Neuroscience. Henke, P. G. (1984). The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and immobilization stress: Unit activity, escape behavior, and gastric pathology. Behavioural Brain Research, 11, Hitchcock, J. M., & Davis, M. (1986). Lesions of the amygdala, but not of the cerebellum or red nucleus, block conditioned fear as measured with the potentiated startle paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, Hitchcock, J. M., & Davis, M. (1991). The efferent pathway of the amygdala involved in conditioned fear as measured with the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, Holland, P. C. (1985). The nature of conditioned inhibition in serial and simultaneous feature negative discriminations. In R. R. Miller & N. E. Spear (Eds.), Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Holson, R. R. (1986). Medial prefrontal cortical lesions and timidity in rats: I. Reactivity to aversive stimuli. Physiology and Behavior, 37, Holstedge, G., Meiners, L., & Tan, K. (1985). Projections of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis to the mesencephalon, pons, and medulla oblongata in the cat. Experimental Brain Research, 58, Jaskiw, G. E., & Weinberger, D. R. (1992). lbotenic acid lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex augment swim-stress induced locomotion. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 41, Kim, M., & Davis, M. (1993). Electrolytic lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala block acquisition and expression of fear-potentiated startle even with extensive training but do not prevent reacquisition. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, Koch, M., & Ebert, U. (1993). Enhancement of acoustic startle response by stimulation of an excitatory pathway from the central amygdala/basal nucleus of Meynert to the pontine reticular formation. Experimental Brain Research, 93, Koch, M., Kungel, M., & Herbert, H. (1993). Cholinergic neurons in the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus are involved in the mediation of prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response in the rat. Experimental Brain Research, 97, Koch, M., Lingenhohl, K., & Pilz, E K. D. (1992). Loss of the acoustic startle response following neurotoxic lesions of the caudal pontine reticular formation: Possible role of giant neurons. Neuroscience, 49, Konorski, J. (1948). Conditioned reflexes and neuronal organization. London: Cambridge University Press. LeDoux, J. E. 0987). Emotion, In E Plum (Ed.), Handbook of physiology: Nervous system (pp ). Washington, DC: American Physiological Society. LeDoux, J. E., Cicchetti, E, Xagoraris, A., & Romanski, L. M. (1990). The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: Sensory interface of the amygdala in fear conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience, 10, LeDoux, J. E., Romanski, L., & Xagoraris, A. (1989). Indelibility of subcortical memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, Lee, Y., Walker, D., & Davis, M. (1996). Lack of a temporal gradient of retrograde amnesia following NMDA-induced lesions of the basolateral amygdala assessed with the fearpotentiated startle paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, Lingenhohl, K., & Friauf, E. (1992). Giant neurons in the caudal pontine reticular formation receive short latency acoustic input: An intracellular recording and HRP-study in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 325, Lingenhohl, K., & Friauf, E. (1994). Giant neurons in the rat reticular formation: A sensory interface in the elementary acoustic startle circuit? Journal of Neuroscience, 14, LoLordo, V. M., & Fairless, J. L. (1985). Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: The literature since In R. R. Miller & N. E. Spear (Eds.), Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition (pp. 1-49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Maren, S., Aharonov, G., & Fanselow, M. S. (1996). Retrograde abolition of conditioned fear after excitotoxic lesions in the basolateral amygdala of rats: Absence of a temporal gradient. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, McAllister, W. R., & McAllister, D. E. (1992). Fear determines the effectiveness of a feedback stimulus in aversively motivated instrumental learning. Learning and Motivation, 23, McAllister, W. R., & McAllister, D. E. (1995). Two factor theory: Implications for understanding anxiety based clinical phenomena. In W. O'Donohue & L. Krasner (Eds.), Theories of behavior therapy: Exploring behavior change (pp ). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Mclntosh, A. R., & Gonzalez-Lima, E (1995). Functional network interactions between parallel auditory pathways during Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Brain Research, 683, Meunier, M., Bachevalier, J., Mishkin, M., & Murray, E. A. (1993). Effects on visual recognition of combined and separate ablations of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortex in monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience, 13, Mishkin, M., & Appenzeller, T. (1987). The anatomy of memory. Scientific American, 256(6), Morgan, M. A., & LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Differential contributions of dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex to the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, Morgan, M. A., Romanski, L. M., & LeDoux, J. E. (1993). Extinction of emotional learning: Contribution of medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience Letters, 163, Morrow, N. S., Gftjalca, C. V., Geiselman, P. J., & Novin, D. (1993). Effects of amygdaloid lesions on gastric erosion formation during exposure to activity-stress. Physiology and Behavior, 53, Nagahara, A. H., Otto, T., & Gallagher, M. (1995). Entorhinalperirhinal lesions impair performance of rats on two versions of place learning in Morris water maze. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 3-9. Nitecka, L., & Ben-Aft, Y. (1987). Distribution of GABA-like immunoreactivity in the rat amygdaloid complex. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 266, Papini, M. R., & Bitterman, M. E. (1993). The two-test strategy in the study of inhibitory conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19, Paxinos, G., & Watson, C. (1986). The rat brain. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Rainnie, D. G., Asprodini, E. K., & Shinnick-Gallagher, P. (1991). Inhibitory transmission in the basolateral amygdala. Journal of Neurphysiology, 66, Rescorla, R. A. (1969). Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 72, Rosen, J. B., & Davis, M. (1988). Temporal characterisitcs of enhancement of startle by electrical stimulation of the amygdala. Physiology and Behavior, 44, Rosen, J. B., Hitchcock, J. M., Miserendino, M. J. D., Falls, W. A., Campeau, S., & Davis, M. (1991). Lesions of the perirhinal cortex, but not of the frontal, medial pre-frontal, visual or insular cortex block fear-potentiated startle using a visual conditioned stimulus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 12, Sananes, C. B., & Davis, M. (1992). N-methyl-D-asparate lesions

Supplementary Methods

Supplementary Methods 1 Supplementary Methods Social Preference Test Subjects Seventy-four Long-Evans, male rats served as subjects (S-rats) in the foodpreference test, with 40 assigned to the CXT-Same (CXT-S) Condition and

More information

Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral Amygdala Lesions in Rats?

Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral Amygdala Lesions in Rats? Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 76, 268 283 (2001) doi:10.1006/nlme.2001.4042, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear Historical views that thought and emotion were processed separately in the brain Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: So far, fear is the best understood What is fear? Dictionary: A feeling of agitation and

More information

The role of amygdala glutamate receptors in fear learning, fear-potentiated startle, and extinction

The role of amygdala glutamate receptors in fear learning, fear-potentiated startle, and extinction Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 71 (2002) 379 392 www.elsevier.com/locate/pharmbiochembeh The role of amygdala glutamate receptors in fear learning, fear-potentiated startle, and extinction David

More information

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala 11 December 1997 Nature 390, 604-607 (1997) Macmillan Publishers Ltd. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala MICHAEL T. ROGAN, URSULA V. STÄUBLI & JOSEPH E. LEDOUX

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

Younglim Lee and Michael Davis Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut 06508

Younglim Lee and Michael Davis Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut 06508 The Journal of Neuroscience, August 15, 1997, 17(16):6434 6446 Role of the Hippocampus, the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis, and the Amygdala in the Excitatory Effect of Corticotropin- Releasing Hormone

More information

Neuroscience Letters 374 (2005)

Neuroscience Letters 374 (2005) Neuroscience Letters 374 (2005) 113 118 Glutamate and GABA B transmissions in lateral amygdala are involved in startle-like electromyographic (EMG) potentiation caused by activation of auditory thalamus

More information

Animal Behavior Processes /89/$ , Vol. 15, No. 1, 54-64

Animal Behavior Processes /89/$ , Vol. 15, No. 1, 54-64 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/89/$00.75 1989, Vol. 15, No. 1, 54-64 Influence of Long-Term Sensitization

More information

Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not the expression, of step-down inhibitory avoidance

Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not the expression, of step-down inhibitory avoidance Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research (2) 33: 829-834 Amygdaloid NMDA receptors and fear memory ISSN 1-879X 829 Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not

More information

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT ACTA NEUROBIOL: EXP. 1988, 48: 117-121 Short communication LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT W. Jeffrey WILSON and Jennifer C. HALL Department of Psychological

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5983/1288/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Induction of Fear Extinction with Hippocampal-Infralimbic BDNF Jamie Peters, Laura M. Dieppa-Perea, Loyda M. Melendez,

More information

Are Different Parts of the Extended Amygdala Involved in Fear versus Anxiety?

Are Different Parts of the Extended Amygdala Involved in Fear versus Anxiety? Are Different Parts of the Extended Amygdala Involved in Fear versus Anxiety? Michael Davis Although there is a close correspondence between fear and anxiety, and the study of fear in animals has been

More information

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats Learning and Memory PREVIOUS work has provided evidence of a role for the hippocampal formation in contextual as opposed to cue conditioning. Similar deficits have been observed after transection of the

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

Contributions of Postrhinal and Perirhinal Cortex to Contextual Information Processing

Contributions of Postrhinal and Perirhinal Cortex to Contextual Information Processing Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2000, Vol, 114, No. 5, 882-894 0735-7044/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.5.882 Contributions of Postrhinal and

More information

Deficits in Attentional Orienting Following Damage to the Perirhinal or Postrhinal Cortices

Deficits in Attentional Orienting Following Damage to the Perirhinal or Postrhinal Cortices Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 118, No. 5, 1117 1122 0735-7044/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.1117 Deficits in Attentional Orienting

More information

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction Behavioral Neuroscience 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 123, No. 2, 303 314 0735-7044/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014988 Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on

More information

Abstract. R. Roesler 1, J. Quevedo 1, C. Rodrigues 1, M. Madruga 1, M.R.M. Vianna 1 and M.B.C. Ferreira 2

Abstract. R. Roesler 1, J. Quevedo 1, C. Rodrigues 1, M. Madruga 1, M.R.M. Vianna 1 and M.B.C. Ferreira 2 Brazilian Amygdaloid Journal non-nmda of Medical receptors and Biological and memory Research expression (1999) 32: 349-353 ISSN 0100-879X Short Communication 349 Increased training prevents the impairing

More information

ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon Limbic System II. David P. Wolfer MD

ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon Limbic System II. David P. Wolfer MD ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon 05.05.2014 Limbic System II David P. Wolfer MD Institute of Anatomy, University of Zurich Institute for Human Movement Sciences and Sport, ETH Zurich http://www.dpwolfer.ch

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

David L. Walker and Michael Davis Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut 06508

David L. Walker and Michael Davis Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut 06508 The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1, 1997, 17(23):9375 9383 Double Dissociation between the Involvement of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis and the Central Nucleus of the Amygdala in Startle

More information

Partial Disruption of Fear Conditioning in Rats With Unilateral Amygdala Damage: Correspondence With Unilateral Temporal Lobectomy in Humans

Partial Disruption of Fear Conditioning in Rats With Unilateral Amygdala Damage: Correspondence With Unilateral Temporal Lobectomy in Humans Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1996, Vol. 110, No. 5, 991-997 0735-7044/96/$3.00 Partial Disruption of Fear Conditioning in Rats With Unilateral

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival Threats are much better reinforcers Fear is a prime

More information

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting Animal Learning & Behavior 1989, 17 (3), 269-279 Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting PETER C. HOLLAND Duke University, Durham, North Carolina In three experiments, transfer effects

More information

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965, Vol. 59, No. 1, 13-17 AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 GORDON BOWER, RONALD STARR, AND LEAH LAZAROVITZ Stanford

More information

Retrograde Abolition of Conditional Fear After Excitotoxic Lesions in the Basolateral Amygdala of Rats: Absence of a Temporal Gradient

Retrograde Abolition of Conditional Fear After Excitotoxic Lesions in the Basolateral Amygdala of Rats: Absence of a Temporal Gradient Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1996, Vol. I10, No. 4, 718-726 0735-7044/96/$3.00 Retrograde Abolition of Conditional Fear After Excitotoxic Lesions

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Behavioural Brain Research 110 (2000) 97 108 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Stephen Maren *, William Holt Department of Psychology

More information

Supplemental Information. A Visual-Cue-Dependent Memory Circuit. for Place Navigation

Supplemental Information. A Visual-Cue-Dependent Memory Circuit. for Place Navigation Neuron, Volume 99 Supplemental Information A Visual-Cue-Dependent Memory Circuit for Place Navigation Han Qin, Ling Fu, Bo Hu, Xiang Liao, Jian Lu, Wenjing He, Shanshan Liang, Kuan Zhang, Ruijie Li, Jiwei

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

Contextual Fear Discrimination Is Impaired by Damage to the Postrhinal or Perirhinal Cortex

Contextual Fear Discrimination Is Impaired by Damage to the Postrhinal or Perirhinal Cortex Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 116, No. 3, 479 488 0735-7044/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.3.479 Contextual Fear Discrimination

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn.4642

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn.4642 Supplementary Figure 1 Recording sites and example waveform clustering, as well as electrophysiological recordings of auditory CS and shock processing following overtraining. (a) Recording sites in LC

More information

Standard Operating Procedure

Standard Operating Procedure 1.0 Purpose: 1.1 An acoustic startle model of sensorimotor gating, in which a weak acoustic stimulus (the pre-pulse) is used to decrease the reflex response (startle) produced by a second, more intense,

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/330/6009/1408/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Paradoxical False Memory for Objects After Brain Damage Stephanie M. McTighe, Rosemary A. Cowell, Boyer D. Winters,

More information

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 121, No. 5, 1043 1052 0735-7044/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.5.1043 Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA LESIONS DIFFERENTIALLY AFFECT RESPONSES CONTROLLED BY CS-US CONTIGUITY AND RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTINGENCY IN THE RAT

VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA LESIONS DIFFERENTIALLY AFFECT RESPONSES CONTROLLED BY CS-US CONTIGUITY AND RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTINGENCY IN THE RAT ACTA NEUROBIOL. EXP. 1987, 47: 83-91 VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA LESIONS DIFFERENTIALLY AFFECT RESPONSES CONTROLLED BY CS-US CONTIGUITY AND RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTINGENCY IN THE RAT W. Jeffrey WILSON and Ericka

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

Multiple Roles for Synaptic Plasticity in Pavlovian Fear Conditioning

Multiple Roles for Synaptic Plasticity in Pavlovian Fear Conditioning Maren Synaptic Plasticity and Fear Conditioning 1 To appear in Neuronal Mechanisms of Memory Formation (ed. C. Hölscher) Multiple Roles for Synaptic Plasticity in Pavlovian Fear Conditioning Stephen Maren

More information

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats Physiology & Behavior 76 (2002) 117 129 The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats Peter C. Holland a, *, Gorica D. Petrovich b, Michela Gallagher b a Department

More information

N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptors in the Basolateral Amygdala Are Required for Both Acquisition and Expression of Conditional Fear in Rats

N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptors in the Basolateral Amygdala Are Required for Both Acquisition and Expression of Conditional Fear in Rats Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1996, Vol. l 10, No. 6, 1365-1374 0735-7044/96/$3.00 N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptors in the Basolateral Amygdala Are

More information

Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque,

Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) 62 76 Differential contributions of hippocampus, amygdala and perirhinal cortex to recognition of novel objects, contextual stimuli and stimulus relationships Sandra N.

More information

Neurons of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis (BNST)

Neurons of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis (BNST) Neurons of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis (BNST) Electrophysiological Properties and Their Response to Serotonin DONALD G. RAINNIE a Harvard Medical School and Department of Psychiatry, Brockton

More information

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (3), 230-245 doi:10.3758/lb.37.3.230 Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning JUSTIN A. HARRIS, SABA A GHARA EI, AND CLINTON A. MOORE

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may have as many as 200,000

More information

AMPA Receptor Facilitation Accelerates Fear Learning without Altering the Level of Conditioned Fear Acquired

AMPA Receptor Facilitation Accelerates Fear Learning without Altering the Level of Conditioned Fear Acquired The Journal of Neuroscience, August 1, 1997, 17(15):5928 5935 AMPA Receptor Facilitation Accelerates Fear Learning without Altering the Level of Conditioned Fear Acquired Michael T. Rogan, Ursula V. Stäubli,

More information

Limbic system outline

Limbic system outline Limbic system outline 1 Introduction 4 The amygdala and emotion -history - theories of emotion - definition - fear and fear conditioning 2 Review of anatomy 5 The hippocampus - amygdaloid complex - septal

More information

The Amygdala Modulates Memory Consolidation of Fear-Motivated Inhibitory Avoidance Learning But Not Classical Fear Conditioning

The Amygdala Modulates Memory Consolidation of Fear-Motivated Inhibitory Avoidance Learning But Not Classical Fear Conditioning The Journal of Neuroscience, September 15, 2000, 20(18):7059 7066 The Amygdala Modulates Memory Consolidation of Fear-Motivated Inhibitory Avoidance Learning But Not Classical Fear Conditioning Ann E.

More information

Fornix Lesions Impair Context-Related Cingulothalamic Neuronal Patterns and Concurrent Discrimination Learning in Rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus)

Fornix Lesions Impair Context-Related Cingulothalamic Neuronal Patterns and Concurrent Discrimination Learning in Rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 118, No. 6, 1225 1239 0735-7044/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.6.1225 Fornix Lesions Impair Context-Related

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Research Paper. Christopher W. Butler, 1 Yvette M. Wilson, 1 Jenny M. Gunnersen, and Mark Murphy

Research Paper. Christopher W. Butler, 1 Yvette M. Wilson, 1 Jenny M. Gunnersen, and Mark Murphy Research Paper Tracking the fear memory engram: discrete populations of neurons within amygdala, hypothalamus, and lateral septum are specifically activated by auditory fear conditioning Christopher W.

More information

DISSOCIATING SPACE AND TRACE IN DORSAL AND VENTRAL HIPPOCAMPUS. Jennifer Lee Czerniawski. A thesis submitted to the. Graduate School-New Brunswick

DISSOCIATING SPACE AND TRACE IN DORSAL AND VENTRAL HIPPOCAMPUS. Jennifer Lee Czerniawski. A thesis submitted to the. Graduate School-New Brunswick DISSOCIATING SPACE AND TRACE IN DORSAL AND VENTRAL HIPPOCAMPUS By Jennifer Lee Czerniawski A thesis submitted to the Graduate School-New Brunswick Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey in partial

More information

Contextual and Auditory Fear Conditioning are Mediated by the Lateral, Basal, and Central Amygdaloid Nuclei in Rats

Contextual and Auditory Fear Conditioning are Mediated by the Lateral, Basal, and Central Amygdaloid Nuclei in Rats Research Contextual and Auditory Fear Conditioning are Mediated by the Lateral, Basal, and Central Amygdaloid Nuclei in Rats Ki A. Goosens 1 and Stephen Maren 1,2,3 1 Department of Psychology and 2 Neuroscience

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0039-1 Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Cody W. Polack & Mario A. Laborda & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 July 2011 # Psychonomic

More information

Ventral medial prefrontal cortex and emotional perseveration: the memory for prior extinction training

Ventral medial prefrontal cortex and emotional perseveration: the memory for prior extinction training Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 121 130 Research report Ventral medial prefrontal cortex and emotional perseveration: the memory for prior extinction training Maria A. Morgan a,c,, Jay Schulkin c,d,e,

More information

Dorsomedial thalamic lesions and amphetamine: Acquisition and retention of a visual pattern discrimination escape task

Dorsomedial thalamic lesions and amphetamine: Acquisition and retention of a visual pattern discrimination escape task Physiological Psychology 1978, Vol. 6 (3),288-293 Dorsomedial thalamic lesions and amphetamine: Acquisition and retention of a visual pattern discrimination escape task KARL L. WUENSCH, BELINDA BROOME,

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

CROSSMODAL PLASTICITY IN SPECIFIC AUDITORY CORTICES UNDERLIES VISUAL COMPENSATIONS IN THE DEAF "

CROSSMODAL PLASTICITY IN SPECIFIC AUDITORY CORTICES UNDERLIES VISUAL COMPENSATIONS IN THE DEAF Supplementary Online Materials To complement: CROSSMODAL PLASTICITY IN SPECIFIC AUDITORY CORTICES UNDERLIES VISUAL COMPENSATIONS IN THE DEAF " Stephen G. Lomber, M. Alex Meredith, and Andrej Kral 1 Supplementary

More information

Functional Inactivation of the Amygdala before But Not after Auditory Fear Conditioning Prevents Memory Formation

Functional Inactivation of the Amygdala before But Not after Auditory Fear Conditioning Prevents Memory Formation The Journal of Neuroscience, 1999, Vol. 19 RC48 1of5 Functional Inactivation of the Amygdala before But Not after Auditory Fear Conditioning Prevents Memory Formation Ann E. Wilensky, Glenn E. Schafe,

More information

Visual Pathways Involved in Fear Conditioning Measured with Fear-Potentiated Startle: Behavioral and Anatomic Studies

Visual Pathways Involved in Fear Conditioning Measured with Fear-Potentiated Startle: Behavioral and Anatomic Studies The Journal of Neuroscience, December 15, 2001, 21(24):9844 9855 Visual Pathways Involved in Fear Conditioning Measured with Fear-Potentiated Startle: Behavioral and Anatomic Studies Changjun Shi and Michael

More information

Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6

Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6 Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6 I. WHAT IS AN EMOTION? A. Three components (Oately & Jenkins, 1996) 1. caused by conscious or unconscious evaluation of an event as relevant to a goal that is important

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function

Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function ARTICLE IN PRESS Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function Bernard W. Balleine 1 and Simon Killcross 2 1 Department of Psychology and the Brain Research Institute, University

More information

Thalamocortical Dysrhythmia. Thalamocortical Fibers. Thalamocortical Loops and Information Processing

Thalamocortical Dysrhythmia. Thalamocortical Fibers. Thalamocortical Loops and Information Processing halamocortical Loops and Information Processing 2427 halamocortical Dysrhythmia Synonyms CD A pathophysiological chain reaction at the origin of neurogenic pain. It consists of: 1) a reduction of excitatory

More information

Pattern Memory Involves Both Elemental and Configural Processes: Evidence From the Effects of Hippocampal Lesions

Pattern Memory Involves Both Elemental and Configural Processes: Evidence From the Effects of Hippocampal Lesions Behavioral Neuroscience 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 125, No. 4, 567 577 0735-7044/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0023762 Pattern Memory Involves Both Elemental and Configural Processes:

More information

Representation of negative motivational value in the primate

Representation of negative motivational value in the primate Representation of negative motivational value in the primate lateral habenula Masayuki Matsumoto & Okihide Hikosaka Supplementary Figure 1 Anticipatory licking and blinking. (a, b) The average normalized

More information

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (2), 213-219 Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning THOMAS HIGGINS and ROBERT A. RESCORLA University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING. Felipe Schiffino

ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING. Felipe Schiffino ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING by Felipe Schiffino A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in partial

More information

Associative learning

Associative learning Introduction to Learning Associative learning Event-event learning (Pavlovian/classical conditioning) Behavior-event learning (instrumental/ operant conditioning) Both are well-developed experimentally

More information

Opioid Receptors Regulate the Extinction of Pavlovian Fear Conditioning

Opioid Receptors Regulate the Extinction of Pavlovian Fear Conditioning Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 117, No. 6, 1292 1301 0735-7044/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1292 Opioid Receptors Regulate the

More information

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition Denniston, James C., Robert P. Cole, and Ralph R. Miller. (1998). "The role of temporal relationships in the transfer of conditioned inhibition." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

More information

Acquisition and retention of visual discrimination learning after ablation of perirhinal cortex in the rat

Acquisition and retention of visual discrimination learning after ablation of perirhinal cortex in the rat Psychobiology 1998.26 (1).36-41 Acquisition and retention of visual discrimination learning after ablation of perirhinal cortex in the rat M.J.EACOTT University oj Durham, Durham, England Eight Dark Agouti

More information

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi LT Memory and the Amygdala 1 Running Head: Role of the amygdala in long term memory What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment University of Mississippi LT Memory and the Amygdala

More information

Tracking the Fear Engram: The Lateral Amygdala Is an Essential Locus of Fear Memory Storage

Tracking the Fear Engram: The Lateral Amygdala Is an Essential Locus of Fear Memory Storage 10010 The Journal of Neuroscience, October 26, 2005 25(43):10010 10015 Brief Communication Tracking the Fear Engram: The Lateral Amygdala Is an Essential Locus of Fear Memory Storage Glenn E. Schafe, 1

More information

The Dorsal Hippocampus Is Essential for Context Discrimination but Not for Contextual Conditioning

The Dorsal Hippocampus Is Essential for Context Discrimination but Not for Contextual Conditioning Behavioral Neuroscience 1998, Vol. 112, No. 4, 863-874 Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/98/$3.00 The Dorsal Hippocampus Is Essential for Context Discrimination but

More information

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(3),363-367 An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock WLLAM C. GORDON and ROBERT R. MOWRER University ofnew Mexico, Albuquerque, New

More information

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Pain Pathways Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Pain Pathways MAP TO NEUROSCIENCE CORE CONCEPTS 1 NCC1. The brain is the body's most complex organ. NCC3. Genetically determined circuits are the foundation

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Failure to defend against an environmental threat such as

Failure to defend against an environmental threat such as Persistence of fear memory across time requires the basolateral amygdala complex Andrew M. Poulos 1, Veronica Li, Sarah S. Sterlace, Fonda Tokushige, Ravikumar Ponnusamy, and Michael S. Fanselow Department

More information

Dissociable Effects of Lesions to the Perirhinal Cortex and the Postrhinal Cortex on Memory for Context and Objects in Rats

Dissociable Effects of Lesions to the Perirhinal Cortex and the Postrhinal Cortex on Memory for Context and Objects in Rats Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 119, No. 2, 557 566 0735-7044/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.2.557 Dissociable Effects of Lesions to the

More information

Fear Extinction in Rodents

Fear Extinction in Rodents Fear Extinction in Rodents Chun-hui Chang, 1 Ewelina Knapska, 1 Caitlin A. Orsini, 1 Christine A. Rabinak, 1 Joshua M. Zimmerman, 1 andstephenmaren 1 UNIT 8.23 1 University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Behav Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Behav Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Behav Neurosci. 2010 April ; 124(2): 294 299. doi:10.1037/a0018911. Role of Corticosterone in Trace and Delay Conditioned Fear- Potentiated

More information

nucleus accumbens septi hier-259 Nucleus+Accumbens birnlex_727

nucleus accumbens septi hier-259 Nucleus+Accumbens birnlex_727 Nucleus accumbens From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Brain: Nucleus accumbens Nucleus accumbens visible in red. Latin NeuroNames MeSH NeuroLex ID nucleus accumbens septi hier-259 Nucleus+Accumbens birnlex_727

More information

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1992. 20 (3). 233-239 The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats JAMES S. MILLER and JOYCE A.

More information

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions Behavioral Neuroscience 1996, Vol. 110, No. 5, 933-945 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/96/S3.00 Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal

More information

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts By Erin Marie Fleming

More information

AD-A OCUMENTATION PAGE P I JM ý-o-0e 6

AD-A OCUMENTATION PAGE P I JM ý-o-0e 6 AD-A278 414 OCUMENTATION PAGE P I JM5. 140 0ý-o-0e 6 ~our 341", -. ~ COc com elsflo and rev~e-'m itne cooemion of 0'l &I'O, Send Cc~n v et '~ t- le 0 ý at."a c,^ -'c.qag suggestlc's for reducng ini t ivs-go

More information

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery L132 NT MJA/cla Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 60-73 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.60 Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery Gonzalo P. Urcelay University of Cambridge, Cambridge,

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Context and Pavlovian conditioning

Context and Pavlovian conditioning Context Brazilian conditioning Journal of Medical and Biological Research (1996) 29: 149-173 ISSN 0100-879X 149 Context and Pavlovian conditioning Departamento de Psicologia, Pontifícia Universidade Católica

More information

Theme 2: Cellular mechanisms in the Cochlear Nucleus

Theme 2: Cellular mechanisms in the Cochlear Nucleus Theme 2: Cellular mechanisms in the Cochlear Nucleus The Cochlear Nucleus (CN) presents a unique opportunity for quantitatively studying input-output transformations by neurons because it gives rise to

More information