Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function"

Transcription

1 ARTICLE IN PRESS Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function Bernard W. Balleine 1 and Simon Killcross 2 1 Department of Psychology and the Brain Research Institute, University of California, Los Angeles, Box , CA , USA 2 School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Tower Building, Park Place, Cardiff CF10 3AT, UK The amygdala is a heterogeneous structure that has been implicated in a wide variety of functions, most notably in fear conditioning. From this research, an influential serial model of amygdala processes has emerged in which aversive learning is mediated by the amygdala basolateral nucleus whereas performance, in this case of various defensive reflexes, is mediated by the central nucleus. By contrast, recent evidence from appetitive conditioning studies suggests that the basolateral and central nuclei operate in parallel to mediate distinct incentive processes: the basolateral nucleus encodes emotional events with reference to their particular sensory-specific features, whereas the central nucleus encodes their more general motivational or affective significance. Given that there is little if any direct behavioral evidence for the serial model, we suggest that more attention should be given to the claims of the parallel view. Introduction Although it has long been known that the amygdala has a central role in emotional learning and memory, the exact nature of its involvement is still disputed. Several general theories of amygdala function have been advanced [1 3], with perhaps the most influential being the model developed from studies of aversive learning, such as Pavlovian fear conditioning [4,5]. This view proposes that the formation of associations between sensory perceptual cues and biologically significant events takes place in the lateral amygdala, where pathways carrying these sources of information converge, and that these associations then mediate the performance of various defensive responses via output through the central amygdala to diverse hypothalamic, midbrain and medullary nuclei. By this model, therefore, processing of emotional information within the amygdala proceeds in a serial manner between the lateral and central nuclei. This is in general agreement with recent anatomical studies suggesting that the amygdala is not a unitary structure but, rather, is composed of anatomically distinct nuclei Corresponding author: Balleine, B.W. (balleine@psych.ucla.edu). broadly divisible into two groups: one whose internal circuitry is more cortical, including the lateral, basolateral and basomedial nuclei (BLA), and another that is more striatal, involving the medial and central nuclei (CeN) [6 8]. Nevertheless, recent evidence from several sources challenges this serial view. Studies investigating the role of the amygdala in motivational processes other than fear, including studies assessing appetitive Pavlovian conditioning and reward-related learning in instrumental conditioning, suggest that the BLA and CeN function not serially but independently and in parallel to mediate distinct aspects of emotional processing [9]. When added to recent evidence questioning key assumptions of the serial model in fear conditioning, including the necessity of plasticity in the lateral amygdala for fear learning and the interaction of lateral and central nuclei in performance [5], these findings suggest that it is perhaps time to consider alternative models of amygdala functioning. This paper advances a parallel model of amygdala function in emotional learning, using three lines of argument. First, evidence from appetitive Pavlovian conditioning studies is considered, suggesting that the BLA and CeN function independently and in parallel to control distinct aspects of emotional learning. The BLA mediates associations between predictive stimuli and the sensory properties of biologically significant events, and the CeN mediates the association of predictive stimuli with the affective or emotional properties of those events. Second, recent evidence from Pavlovian fear conditioning is re-evaluated, suggesting that, whereas it is at least partially inconsistent with the serial model, it is wholly consistent with predictions from the parallel model. Finally, we consider evidence from studies of rewardrelated learning in instrumental conditioning that demonstrate that, whereas the BLA mediates the emotional significance of specific rewards, the CeN seems to establish the general affective response that underlies the nonspecific reinforcing effect of those events. Together, this evidence suggests that the amygdala subserves incentive learning, the process through which sensory-perceptual features of appetitive (or aversive), and rewarding (or punishing) events acquire affective significance and, hence, the ability to motivate or incite responses and actions. The amygdala as a whole controls /$ - see front matter Q 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi: /j.tins

2 2 ARTICLE IN PRESS both the associative significance (through Pavlovian incentive learning) and the reward value (through instrumental incentive learning) [10 13] of sensory perceptual events. In this respect, the different nuclei of the amygdala have a coordinating function: they establish the incentive value of motivationally significant events within specific functional domains. Establishing associative significance: Pavlovian incentive learning Numerous authors have suggested that Pavlovian conditional stimuli (CSs) that are initially motivationally neutral can acquire incentive properties through association with representations of biologically significant unconditional stimuli (USs) [10 13]. However, it has long been recognized that USs are represented in terms of multiple features or components and that these can enter into independent associations with CSs. Konorski [14] was the first to articulate this idea to explain his distinction between consummatory and preparatory conditioning. He argued that independent associations are formed between the representation of the CS and both the sensory features and the motivational properties of the US. In this model, the sensory associations mediate USspecific consummatory responses, such as chewing, licking, blinking and flinching, and the motivational associations mediate more general preparatory behavior, such as changes in heart rate, blood pressure, approach and withdrawal. Current evidence suggests that the amygdala is heavily involved in both of these aspects of Pavlovian incentive learning: that the BLA mediates the essential associative processes that support consummatory conditioning, whereas the CeN mediates processes that support preparatory conditioning (Figure 1). Appetitive incentives In the appetitive situation, lesions of the BLA produce a deficit in the association of CSs with sensory-specific features of US representations without affecting the production of preparatory conditioned responses (CRs), most notably conditioned approach responses motivated by the arousing nature of the US [15]. For example, whereas lesions of the BLA do not affect acquisition of conditioned approach towards food during a tone or light CS, they do abolish the sensitivity of approach responses elicited by that CS to post-training devaluation of its associated US [16]. In the past few years, it has become clear that BLA lesions are in fact affecting highly specific consummatory aspects of CS US associations. For example, in addition to other consummatory reflexes, CSs paired with food can elicit eating in non-deprived animals, an effect that depends on the integrity of the BLA and its connections with ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the lateral hypothalamus [17,18]. Pavlovian CSs can also exert specific effects on food seeking. Evidence from experiments employing Pavlovian instrumental transfer procedures (PIT; Box 1) suggests that Pavlovian CSs can signal highly specific features of, for example, a food US, and hence prime or facilitate performance of actions that have been trained with that food as an outcome relative to performance of other actions [12]. Several studies suggest (a) CS US (b) Sensory thalamus and cortices Visceral brainstem and hypothalamic afferents Affective CeN system CS US BLA Preparatory CRs (general CRs based on US valence) Consummatory CRs (US-specific CRs) Brainstem, midbrain and reticular nuclei Bed nucleus, hypothalamus, prefrontal cortex and n.accumbens TRENDS in Neurosciences Figure 1. Role of the amygdala in Pavlovian incentive learning. (a) Current model of the motivational and affective associations acquired by the conditioned stimulus (CS) during both appetitive and aversive Pavlovian conditioning (derived from [14]). Differential associations with the sensory-specific and general affective properties of the unconditioned stimulus (US) produce various conditioned responses (CRs) grouped generally into discrete consummatory reflexes (e.g. chewing, salivating, blinking, flinching or retraction) and more diffuse preparatory reflexes (e.g. heart rate, blood pressure, approach or withdrawal). Solid arrows reflect excitatory connections and the broken arrow represents the facilitatory effect of conditioned affective activation on the performance of consummatory CRs. (b) Plasticity involving segregated inputs to the lateral, basolateral and basomedial nuclei (BLA) and the medial and central nuclei (CeN) from sensory and visceral brainstem and hypothalamic afferents mediates the formation of stimulus-specific CS US associations in the BLA, and general CS affect associations in the CeN. Differential outputs to brainstem autonomic and hypothalamic nuclei mediate preparatory and consummatory CRs, whereas CeN control of afferents to the ventral striatum and direct BLA projections to the nucleus accumbens mediate the influence of Pavlovian CSs on instrumental actions in general [i.e. conditioned emotional responses (CERs)] and specific transfer paradigms. Overlapping inputs between CeN and BLA in the anterolateral bed nucleus of the stria terminalis are hypothesized to mediate the facilitatory effects of preparatory CSs on the performance of consummatory CRs, whereas CeN inputs to midbrain nuclei seem to control CS-specific CRs. These relationships are argued to be common to both appetitive and aversive conditioning paradigms.

3 ARTICLE IN PRESS 3 Box 1. Pavlovian instrumental transfer Addictive behavior readily illustrates the principle that rewardrelated cues exert a powerful excitatory influence on the performance of actions in animals and humans [59]. In fact, the first formal demonstration of this influence was reported in an early study by Estes [60]. He found that a tone paired with food increased the likelihood that rats would press a lever that had been previously reinforced with that food reward, even though the rats had not been trained to press the lever in the presence of the tone. As Estes s study makes clear, there are three components to experiments that assess effects of this kind: a Pavlovian phase, in which the signals for reward are established; an instrumental phase, in which the association between the reward and actions leading to it is learned; and a test phase, in which the impact of the signals for reward on the performance of instrumental actions is assessed. For this reason, the protocol for assessing the influence of reward-related cues on instrumental performance is often referred to as Pavlovian instrumental transfer (or PIT). Evidence suggests that PIT can be mediated by the representation of the specific sensory features of the Pavlovian reinforcer. For example in hungry rats, a CS paired with food pellets increases the performance of actions that the animals have learned to associate with the pellets, and a CS paired with sucrose increases the performance of actions the animals have learned to associate with sucrose even though both CSs predict a nutritive commodity [12]. PIT effects can also be produced by a general motivational influence of a CS rather than the specific sensory features of the reinforcer. A stimulus paired with food will increase the performance of lever pressing in hungry rats even if they were trained to press the lever for water when they were thirsty [12]. However, this motivational modulation seems to be specific to CSs that elicit appetitive arousal; CSs paired with food tend generally to increase food-related actions whereas CSs paired with aversive events result in the suppression of those actions [12]. that US-specific PIT is mediated by the BLA [19,20] and, indeed, recent evidence suggests that this function is dissociable from that of the CeN. Thus, whereas CeN lesions had no effect on US-specific PIT, BLA lesions completely abolished it [20]. Previous experiments examining the role of the amygdala in PIT had reported that lesions of the CeN, rather than of the BLA, abolished the general excitatory effect of CSs associated with food USs [21 23]. One crucial difference between these and the more recent studies is their design: whereas BLA lesions were shown to affect performance using a US-specific PIT protocol, the effects of CeN lesions were demonstrated using a protocol in which performance could be influenced by the general affective and motivational effects of food-related cues on instrumental performance. Therefore, these differences in the roles of the CeN and BLA in PIT could be reconciled if the US-specific effect were mediated by the BLA and the more general motivational effect by the CeN. To assess this claim directly, a procedure was developed in which both the US-specific and the general forms of PIT could be generated in the same animal [20]. In this case, three CSs were paired with three different food rewards: two with food rewards that could also be earned by performing distinct lever-press actions, and one with a food reward that was not available as an instrumental outcome. The CSs paired with foods also earned by the actions generated a US-specific PIT effect: each elevated performance specifically of the action that had earned the food paired with that CS and not the other action. By contrast, the third CS generated a general effect, elevating performance on both actions. Using this procedure, the effects of BLA and CeN lesions on the general and specific forms of PIT were compared, and it was shown that lesions of the BLA abolished US-specific but not general PIT, whereas lesions of the CeN abolished general but not US-specific PIT [20]. This finding confirmed the explanation proposed for the different effects of BLA and CeN lesions on PIT, and also provided evidence of a structural basis for the distinct consummatory and preparatory conditioning processes proposed by Konorski. In essence, the results of this study suggest that the BLA is involved in the formation of Pavlovian incentives involving the association of a CS with the specific sensory features of the US. By contrast, the CeN is involved in preparatory conditioning that is, in the association of a CS with the general affective properties of the US. Finally, this double dissociation provides evidence that the BLA and CeN can function independently in appetitive conditioning, in line with other reported dissociations between these structures [21 23]. Aversive incentives Perhaps the most thoroughly researched function of the amygdala is its role in fear conditioning, particularly as evaluated by freezing or the facilitation of unconditioned reflexes such as startle [3,5,24,25]. This research suggests that these defensive responses are produced by the association of CS-related inputs from primary sensory relays with US-related nociceptive inputs from the insular cortex and thalamic relays that project to the lateral nucleus of the BLA. Aspects of other aversive conditioning paradigms, notably those that are amenable to training of US-specific CRs (e.g. CS eyeshock associations and conditioned punishment), are also mediated by the BLA [26 28]. Similarly, because conditioned freezing is likely to be controlled by an association between the CS and the nociceptive sensory properties of the aversive US, it might be best characterized as a US-specific defensive reflex paralleling US-specific consummatory reflexes such as chewing, licking and swallowing. (Indeed, Bolles [29] described the marked differences in the responses produced by constant-current and constant-voltage shock sources, emphasizing the dependence of these distinct fear responses on the specific properties of the US.) However, in marked contrast to the dissociations reported in appetitive conditioning, contemporary students of fear conditioning have argued that, beyond the lateral nucleus, the BLA and CeN work serially in aversive situations to determine the aversive CR. According to the standard view, the BLA mediates the CS US association whereas the CeN mediates consequent performance of the defensive CR [3,5,24,25]. Recent evidence poses substantial problems for this serial theory. First, anatomical evidence suggests that the crucial site of plasticity in the lateral nucleus is not connected directly with the area of the CeN that controls important aspects of the CR [30]. Second, several studies have shown that BLA and CeN can mediate distinct fear responses. For example, BLA damage can abolish conditioned fear responses to a discrete cue but leave intact

4 4 ARTICLE IN PRESS aversive learning about contextual cues, as assessed by a place-preference measure [31]; it can also impair conditioned freezing without markedly impairing avoidance of the shocked side of a Y-maze [32]. Similarly, although lesions of the BLA reduce freezing, they are without substantial influence on conditioned suppression of lever pressing. This measure has much in common with general PIT effects, but it evaluates the suppression of ongoing lever pressing for food produced by a CS paired with shock, rather than facilitated responding produced by an appetitive CS [27]. Despite suggestions to the contrary [4,33], detailed analysis has shown that this dissociation is not due to the number or duration of suppression-training trials [13]. Nevertheless, although conditioned suppression does not necessarily reflect freezing (lesions of the ventral periaqueductal gray that abolish freezing have no effect on conditioned suppression [34]), in some procedures the observed suppression might be due in part to freezing. Indeed, evidence suggests that, when very few conditioning trials are used, BLA lesions can simultaneously affect correlated measures of freezing and conditioned suppression [33]. Here suppression is likely to reflect freezing rather than aversive PIT. When conditioned suppression reflects aversive PIT rather than freezing, BLA lesions affect freezing without affecting suppression [27]. Interestingly, and paralleling effects on appetitive general PIT, lesions of the CeN do abolish conditioned suppression, contributing to a double dissociation between BLA and CeN function in mediating conditioned punishment and suppression, respectively [27]. Furthermore, freezing can be generated in rats that have BLA lesions after overtraining of the CS shock association, and this non-bla-mediated fear learning has been argued to depend on the CeN [35]. Indeed, recent data suggest that the CeN is not simply a source of efferents descending to the brainstem but is itself a target both of sensory afferents involving the CS [36] and of nociceptive inputs from the parabrachial nucleus [37]. Clearly, such data question the necessity of the serial model of BLA and CeN involvement in aversive conditioning and have led some researchers to entertain the possibility that these systems contribute to fear learning in parallel, much as was argued earlier in this article for appetitive conditioning [5,30]. In fact, what evidence there is points to substantial similarities in function between the BLA and CeN in appetitive and aversive situations: in both cases, the BLA appears to mediate US-specific (in this case nociceptive) associations of the CS, whereas the CeN mediates associations with the more general affective (in this case aversive) aspects of the US. Consummatory preparatory interactions A further area of similarity in appetitive and aversive Pavlovian conditioning is the influence that general affective states have on the performance of US-specific consummatory CRs. Konorski [14] proposed that conditioned emotional states facilitate the performance of US-specific CRs and more recent evidence has largely confirmed this suggestion [38]. It has also been established that the amygdala, and in particular the CeN, has a central role in this effect both in appetitive conditioning, where it mediates attentional processing of the CS [39 41], and in aversive conditioning. The role in aversive conditioning is most obvious in the potentiation of startle responses by CSs associated with shock [42,43], but it is also found in eyeblink conditioning. In the latter case, although lesions of the interpositus nucleus in the cerebellum abolish acquisition of the eyeblink CR, they do not affect emotional responses to the CS such as changes in heart rate and ultrasonic vocalization (USV) [44,45]. Conversely, lesions of the amygdala abolish USVs and significantly delay acquisition and attenuate performance of the eyeblink CR even though the CR is ultimately acquired [44]. Therefore, in addition to functioning in parallel, consummatory and preparatory processes subserved by the BLA and CeN must also interact at a site of major convergence, and one area that has been implicated in this regard is the anterolateral region of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis [46,47]. To the extent that US-specific CRs are potentiated by CeN-mediated emotional states, CeN lesions should produce a deficit in CRs that are otherwise controlled by the BLA. In support of this prediction, it has been widely reported that lesions of the CeN produce deficits in conditioned freezing [48,49], an effect that recovers with continued training [33]. Although the observation that both BLA and CeN lesions disrupt freezing has often been interpreted as evidence for the serial model [49], it is interesting to note that it is no less consistent with the parallel incentive processing approach (Figure 1). Reward value: instrumental incentive learning The influence of Pavlovian incentives on the performance of goal-directed actions in various PIT and conditionedsuppression studies has been dissociated, both behaviorally and neurally, from the incentive processes that establish the reward value of a goal or outcome earned as a consequence of instrumental performance [10,11]. For example, after hungry rats have been trained to press a lever to obtain food, post-training shifts, such as from hunger to satiety, directly reduce the potency of Pavlovian incentives. By contrast, in instrumental conditioning, such shifts in motivational state often have little direct effect on the reward value of a food outcome until the effect of the shift on the incentive value of that food event has been made explicit through consummatory experience (i.e. through instrumental incentive learning) [50,51] (Figure 2). With regard to instrumental responding for food, states such as hunger and satiety thus affect performance not because they affect energetic conditions such as drive, but because they affect the incentive value of nutritive outcomes (Box 2). Primary reward Considerable evidence suggests that the representation of the reward value of food is mediated by chemosensory processing involving the gustatory cortex [52]. However, the gustatory cortex is not involved in encoding changes in incentive value. This requires the memory of the sensory properties of food, which involves the gustatory cortex, to be integrated with an affective signal mediated by a further component of the incentive system. Thus, changes

5 ARTICLE IN PRESS 5 (a) (c) Sensory features Motiv. system Drive Stimulus Response (Habit based) Emotional feedback Incent. learning Action outcome association, goal-directed actions General affective signal Reinforcement signal (b) Peripheral metabolic and humoral visceral signals (d) Sensory cortices Hypothalamic and medial thalamic nuclei Sensorimotor cortex Dorsolateral striatum, ventromedial prefrontal cortices Globus pallidus, motor cortex Insular and prefrontal cortices BLA Medial prefrontal cortex, dorsomedial striatum and accumbens core CeN SNr Box 2. Instrumental incentive learning The motivational state of animals is a major determinant of their instrumental performance; not surprisingly, hungry rats work more vigorously for a food reward than do sated ones. However, current evidence suggests that this increase in vigor occurs because food deprivation induces rats to assign a higher incentive value to nutritive outcomes when they are contacted in that state, and that this value then causes the more vigorous performance [61]. In fact, the role of incentive learning in instrumental performance, and the incentive value that it derives, is common across all motivational systems so far investigated. For example, incentive learning has been found to influence performance after: (i) sensory-specific satiety; (ii) shifts in food deprivation; (iii) shifts in water deprivation; (iv) changes in outcome value mediated by drug states; (iv) changes in the value of thermoregulatory and sexual rewards; and (v) outcome devaluation induced by taste aversion learning (reviewed in [12,50,61]). In all of these cases, it is clear that animals have to learn about changes in the incentive value of an instrumental outcome through consummatory contact with that outcome before this change affects performance of their goal-directed, instrumental actions [50]. Instrumental incentive learning involves two interacting associative processes (Figure 2). The first derives from evaluative conditioning and involves association of the representation of salient sensory features of the instrumental outcome with motivational systems that are sensitive to the detection of signals induced by factors such as nutrients, fluids or toxins. This connection opens a feedback loop to provide the basis for the second associative connection, between the outcome representation and emotional feedback (this is produced by the affective system through its activation by motivational inputs, and determines the value of the instrumental outcome). It is the formation of this second associative connection that constitutes instrumental incentive learning [10,12,14]. TRENDS in Neurosciences Figure 2. Role of the amygdala in instrumental incentive learning. (a) Current model of reward processes in instrumental conditioning based on two processes: one involving association between the sensory features of the instrumental outcome and specific motivational processes, and the other involving association between those sensory features and emotional feedback produced by motivational activity [10,50]. The strength of the incentive learning connection is determined by the degree of emotional feedback modulated by drive processes such as hunger and thirst. Instrumental incentive learning can control the direction and intensity of instrumental performance both directly and through integration with processes that mediate action outcome learning. Arrows represent excitatory connections; dots represent modulatory connections. (b) The amygdala specifically the BLA is in a position to establish the reward value of the specific goals or outcomes of goaldirected instrumental actions, through the association of cortical sensory inputs with motivational and emotional inputs to the BLA from the hypothalamus, thalamus, prefrontal cortex and insular cortex. BLA efferents to the medial prefrontal cortex and dorsomedial striatal circuitry, which are implicated in instrumental learning, can influence the acquisition of goal-directed actions and the BLA can influence instrumental performance directly through connections with the nucleus accumbens core [9]. (c) A general model of stimulus response learning. In this model, the motivational basis for the reinforcement signal that mediates the acquisition of habitual, as opposed to goal-directed, actions comes from the affective system. This signal acts as a catalyst to strengthen the connection between sensory and motor representations [11]. (d) A possible systems-level implementation of the stimulus response model. The affective signal mediated by the CeN controls dopaminergic afferents from the substantia nigra pars reticulata to the dorsolateral striatum. The CeN also makes direct projections to ventromedial prefrontal cortices (particularly the medial orbitofrontal and infralimbic areas), which have been implicated in the motivational control of habitual actions [62,63]. in the value of the sensory features of nutritive outcomes seem to be a function of emotional feedback that is, of the emotional response experienced contiguously with detection of the taste [10]. If the emotional response is pleasant, the value of the outcome is correspondingly increased, whereas if it is unpleasant the value is reduced (Figure 2). The gustatory cortex has strong reciprocal connections with the BLA, and two recent series of experiments have provided clear evidence for the involvement of the BLA in incentive learning. In one series, lesions of the BLA rendered the instrumental performance of rats insensitive to devaluation of the instrumental outcome by sensoryspecific satiety; they appeared no longer able to associate the sensory features of the instrumental outcome with its incentive value [53]. Similarly, although the acquisition of concurrent instrumental discriminations benefits from the use of different outcomes in each case, BLA lesions abolish this advantage [19]. Furthermore, BLA-lesioned animals appear unable to use the specific sensory features of the instrumental outcome as a discriminative cue to control choice performance [53]. Because animals with BLA lesions are not impaired in normal discrimination performance or sensory preconditioning [54], these findings suggest that they have a specific deficit representing the sensory features of motivationally significant events. More recently, the role of the BLA in instrumental incentive learning was confirmed using post-training infusions of the protein-synthesis inhibitor anisomycin [55]. It is well documented that the consolidation of stimulus affect associations that underlie conditioned freezing and the reconsolidation of these associations after retrieval both depend on the synthesis of new proteins in the BLA [56]. To assess whether the same is true of instrumental incentive learning, hungry rats were trained to press two levers, one earning food pellets and the other a sucrose solution. After this training, the rats were sated and given the opportunity for incentive learning that is, they were allowed to consume either the food pellets or the sucrose solution in the sated state.

6 6 ARTICLE IN PRESS Box 3. Outstanding questions (i) Can the functional disconnection of the BLA and CeN by asymmetrical lesions of these structures provide evidence for serial processes in either appetitive or aversive Pavlovian conditioning? (ii) Do BLA and CeN lesions affect CS-elicited eating by affecting sensory-specific or general affective influences on feeding, in parallel with the effects of these lesions on PIT? Does either structure mediate the effects on eating produced by autonomic arousal, such as that elicited by tail-pinch? (iii) What can disconnection studies involving the BLA and CeN reveal about their important output projections to the nucleus accumbens core and shell in PIT? (iv) What is the role of CeN midbrain connections in general affective PIT? (v) Might CeN lesions influence the acquisition and/or performance of habitual instrumental actions? (vi) What is the role in instrumental incentive learning of the connectivity of the BLA, including afferent sensory cortical and efferent prefrontal and striatal connections? Do these latter connections mediate action goal associations or estimates of goal value? Immediately after this consumption phase, half of the rats were given an infusion of anisomycin and the remainder received an infusion of vehicle into the BLA. A subsequent two-lever choice extinction test conducted when the rats were sated found that the rats in the vehicle group performed fewer responses on the lever that, in training, delivered the outcome to which they were exposed when sated prior to the test. Infusion of anisomycin into the BLA completely blocked this shift in preference. A similar logic revealed that incentive learning was also subject to reconsolidation involving the BLA, again revealing strong parallels between appetitive and aversive functions of the BLA [55]. Secondary reward and punishment Studies of conditioned reinforcement (and its aversive counterpart, conditioned punishment) enable assessment of the role of the amygdala in instrumental performance for secondary rather than primary reward. Such studies demonstrate further parallels between appetitive and aversive domains, in addition to parallel functions of the BLA and CeN. Lesions of the BLA attenuate the acquisition of a new instrumental response in conditioned reinforcement procedures [57]. By contrast, lesions of the CeN have no effect on conditioned reinforcement of lever presses, but do attenuate the response-enhancing effect of intra-accumbens infusions of amphetamine. Thus, once again, evidence suggests that the CeN modulates the arousing and energizing properties of Pavlovian cues, albeit in this instance in a response-contingent manner [58]. In direct parallel, lesions of the BLA abolish conditioned punishment, but CeN lesions are without effect in this particular aversively motivated task [27]. It is likely that the role of the BLA and CeN in these higherorder tasks depends on multiple associative processes. Undoubtedly, the reward-related stimulus in conditioned reinforcement studies is to some extent an instrumental incentive for responding. At the same time, the affective response elicited by this stimulus might also provide a reinforcement signal to strengthen stimulus response associations that control instrumental performance [9]. We suggest that the function of the reward-related stimulus as an instrumental incentive is mediated by the BLA, whereas its function as a reinforcement signal is mediated by the CeN. Certainly BLA lesions leave outcome-independent instrumental performance intact [53], which is consistent with the proposition that the BLA and CeN operate in parallel to mediate dissociable aspects of Pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning. Thus, the BLA establishes the incentive value of specific instrumental outcomes whereas the CeN mediates the general affective properties of instrumental reinforcers (Figure 2). Concluding remarks Although many details are still to be determined (Box 3), the evidence reviewed here suggests that two fundamental conclusions can be drawn. First, in line with the division first proposed in Konorski s incentive theory [14], it seems that the BLA is involved in encoding the incentive properties of Pavlovian USs and instrumental outcomes on the basis of their specific sensory features, and so has a direct role in consummatory conditioning; by contrast, the CeN mediates the influence of the general affective and reinforcing properties of these events and so seems to be more directly involved in preparatory conditioning. Second, and despite a long tradition of theorizing to the contrary, it does not seem to be necessary, nor is there any direct evidence to suggest, that the BLA and CeN must function serially. Rather, it seems likely that they function simultaneously and in parallel to encode associations of conditioned stimuli and instrumental actions with the sensory-specific and general affective incentive properties of both Pavlovian USs and instrumental outcomes. Although interactions between these parallel processes can occur, they do not necessitate a serial processing view. Therefore, we conclude that the evidence reviewed here is consistent with the suggestion that the BLA and CeN have a common function in both the appetitive and aversive domain. It has commonly been thought that the BLA and CeN function serially in aversive conditioning but in parallel in appetitive conditioning; however, we believe that the proposal that these structures function independently and in parallel in both appetitive and aversive situations is not only more parsimonious, but also more consistent with the available evidence. Acknowledgements Preparation of this manuscript was supported by the National Institute of Health, grant #56446 to B.W.B. and a UK MRC Career Establishment Grant to S.K. (G ). References 1 McGaugh, J.L. (2004) The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, Cardinal, R.N. et al. (2002) Emotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, Fanselow, M.S. and Gale, G.D. (2003) The amygdala, fear, and memory. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 985, LeDoux, J.E. (2000) The amygdala and emotion: A view through fear. In The Amygdala: A functional analysis (2nd edn) (Aggleton, J.P., ed.), pp , Oxford University Press

7 ARTICLE IN PRESS 7 5 Maren, S. (2005) Building and burying fear memories in the brain. Neuroscientist 11, Swanson, L.W. (2003) The amygdala and its place in the cerebral hemisphere. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 985, Swanson, L.W. and Petrovich, G.D. (1998) What is the amygdala? Trends Neurosci. 21, Swanson, L.W. (2000) Cerebral hemisphere regulation of motivated behavior. Brain Res. 886, Balleine, B.W. (2005) Neural; bases of food seeking: affect, arousal and reward in corticostriatolimbic circuits. Physiol. Behav. 86, Balleine, B.W. (2004) Incentive behavior. In The Behavior of the Laboratory Rat: a Handbook with Tests (Whishaw, I.Q. and Kolb, B., eds), pp , Oxford University Press 11 Dayan, P. and Balleine, B.W. (2002) Reward, motivation, and reinforcement learning. Neuron 36, Dickinson, A. and Balleine, B.W. (2002) The role of learning in the operation of motivational systems. In Learning, Motivation & Emotion, Volume 3 of Steven s Handbook of Experimental Psychology (3rd edn) (Gallistel, C.R., ed.), pp , John Wiley & Sons 13 Killcross, S. and Blundell, P. (2002) Associative representations of emotionally significant outcomes. In Emotional Cognition: From Brain to Behaviour. Advances in Consciousness Research (Moore, S. and Oaksford, M., eds), pp , Benjamins Publishing Company 14 Konorski, J. (1967) Integrative Activity of the Brain, University of Chicago Press 15 Holland, P.C. and Gallagher, M. (1999) Amygdala circuitry in attentional and representational processes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, Hatfield, T. et al. (1996) Neurotoxic lesions of basolateral, but not central, amygdala interfere with Pavlovian second-order conditioning and reinforcer devaluation effects. J. Neurosci. 16, Holland, P.C. et al. (2002) The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats. Physiol. Behav. 76, Petrovich, G.D. et al. (2005) Amygdalar and prefrontal pathways to the lateral hypothalamus are activated by a learned cue that stimulates eating. J. Neurosci. 25, Blundell, P. et al. (2001) Lesions of the basolateral amygdala disrupt selective aspects of reinforcer representation in rats. J. Neurosci. 21, Corbit, L.H. and Balleine, B.W. (2005) Double dissociation of basolateral and central amygdala lesions on the general and outcome-specific forms of Pavlovian instrumental transfer. J. Neurosci. 25, Hall, J. et al. (2001) Involvement of the central nucleus of the amygdala and nucleus accumbens core in mediating Pavlovian influences on instrumental behaviour. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, Holland, P.C. and Gallagher, M. (2003) Double dissociation of the effects of lesions of basolateral and central amygdala on conditioned stimulus-potentiated feeding and Pavlovian instrumental transfer. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, Everitt, B.J. et al. (2003) Appetitive behavior: impact of amygdaladependent mechanisms of emotional learning. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 985, LeDoux, J.E. (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, Davis, M. (1998) Anatomic and physiologic substrates of emotion in an animal model. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 15, Blair, H.T. et al. (2005) The lateral amygdala processes the value of conditioned and unconditioned aversive stimuli. Neuroscience 133, Killcross, S. et al. (1997) Different types of fear-conditioned behaviour mediated by separate nuclei within amygdala. Nature 388, Blair, H.T. et al. (2005) Unilateral storage of fear memories by the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 25, Bolles, R.C. (1967) Theory of Motivation, Harper & Row 30 Pare, D. et al. (2004) New vistas on amygdala networks in conditioned fear. J. Neurophysiol. 92, Selden, N.R.W. et al. (1991) Complementary roles for the amygdala and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience 42, Vazdarjanova, A. and McGaugh, J.L. (1998) Basolateral amygdala is not critical for cognitive memory of contextual fear conditioning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, Lee, J.L.C. et al. (2005) Conditioned suppression and freezing as measures of aversive Pavlovian conditioning: effects of discrete amygdala lesions and overtraining. Behav. Brain Res. 159, Amorapanth, P.et al. (1999) Lesions of periaqueductal gray dissociateconditioned freezing from conditioned suppression behavior in rats. Learn. Mem. 6, Maren, S. (1999) Neurotoxic basolateral amygdala lesions impair learning and memory but not the performance of conditional fear in rats. J. Neurosci. 19, Linke, R. et al. (2000) Differential projection of the posterior paralaminar thalamic nuclei to the amygdaloid complex in the rat. Exp. Brain Res. 134, Shi, C. and Davis, M. (1999) Pain pathways involved in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle: lesion studies. J. Neurosci. 19, Bombace, J.C. et al. (1991) Modulation of a conditioned eyeblink response by a putative emotive stimulus conditioned with hindleg shock. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 17, Han, J.S. et al. (1999) Disconnection of the amygdala central nucleus and substantia innominata/nucleus basalis disrupts increments in conditioned stimulus processing in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 113, Gallagher, M. and Holland, P.C. (1994) The amygdala complex: multiple roles in associative learning and attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 91, Lee, H.J. et al. (2005) Role of amygdalo nigral circuitry in conditioning of a visual stimulus paired with food. J. Neurosci. 25, Kim, M. and Davis, M. (1993) Electrolytic lesions of the amygdala block acquisition and expression of fear-potentiated startle even with extensive training but do not prevent reacquisition. Behav. Neurosci. 107, Campeau, S. and Davis, M. (1995) Involvement of the central nucleus and basolateral complex of the amygdala in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle in rats trained concurrently with auditory and visual conditioned stimuli. J. Neurosci. 15, Lee, T. and Kim, J.J. (2004) Differential effects of cerebellar, amygdalar, and hippocampal lesions on classical eyeblink conditioning in rats. J. Neurosci. 24, Lavond, D.G. et al. (1984) Effect of bilateral lesions of the dentate and interpositus cerebellar nuclei on conditioning of heart-rate and nictitating membrane/eyelid responses in the rabbit. Brain Res. 305, Dong, H.W. et al. (2001) Topography of projections from amygdala to bed nuclei of the stria terminalis. Brain Res. Rev. 38, Davis, M. and Shi, C. (1999) The extended amygdala: are the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis differentially involved in fear versus anxiety? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 877, Amorapanth, P. et al. (2000) Different lateral amygdala outputs mediate reactions and actions elicited by a fear-arousing stimulus. Nat. Neurosci. 3, Goosens, K.A. and Maren, S. (2001) Contextual and auditory fear conditioning are mediated by the lateral, basal, and central amygdaloid nuclei in rats. Learn. Mem. 8, Balleine, B.W. (2001) Incentive processes in instrumental conditioning. In Handbook of Contemporary Learning Theories (Klein, R.M.S., ed.), pp , LEA 51 Corbit, L.H. and Balleine, B.W. (2003) Instrumental and Pavlovian incentive processes have dissociable effects on components of a heterogeneous instrumental chain. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 29, Balleine, B.W. and Dickinson, A. (2000) The effect of lesions of the insular cortex on instrumental conditioning: evidence for a role in incentive memory. J. Neurosci. 20, Balleine, B.W. et al. (2003) The effect of lesions of the basolateral amygdala on instrumental conditioning. J. Neurosci. 23, Blundell, P. et al. (2003) Preserved sensitivity to outcome value after lesions of the basolateral amygdala. J. Neurosci. 23,

8 8 ARTICLE IN PRESS 55 Wang, S.H. et al. (2005) Consolidation and reconsolidation of incentive learning in the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 25, Nader, K. et al. (2000) The labile nature of consolidation theory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, Burns, L. et al. (1993) Differential effects of excitotoxic lesions of the basolateral amygdala, ventral subiculum and medial prefrontal cortex on responding with conditioned reinforcement and locomotor activity potentiated by intraaccumbens infusions of D-amphetamine. Behav. Brain Res. 55, Robledo, P. et al. (1996) Effects of excitotoxic lesions of the central amygdaloid nucleus on the potentiation of rewardrelated stimuli by intra-accumbens amphetamine. Behav. Neurosci. 110, Everitt, B.J. et al. (1999) Associative processes in addiction and reward. The role of amygdala ventral striatal subsystems. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 877, Estes, W.K. (1948) Discriminative conditioning II: Effects of a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus upon a subsequently established operant response. J. Exp. Psych 38, Dickinson, A.B. and Balleine, B.W. (1994) Motivational control of goaldirected action. Anim. Learn. Behav. 22, Yin, H.H. et al. (2004) Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, Killcross, S. and Coutureau, E. (2003) Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cereb. Cortex 13,

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

Still at the Choice-Point

Still at the Choice-Point Still at the Choice-Point Action Selection and Initiation in Instrumental Conditioning BERNARD W. BALLEINE AND SEAN B. OSTLUND Department of Psychology and the Brain Research Institute, University of California,

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival Threats are much better reinforcers Fear is a prime

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

Instrumental Conditioning VI: There is more than one kind of learning

Instrumental Conditioning VI: There is more than one kind of learning Instrumental Conditioning VI: There is more than one kind of learning PSY/NEU338: Animal learning and decision making: Psychological, computational and neural perspectives outline what goes into instrumental

More information

The orbitofrontal cortex, predicted value, and choice

The orbitofrontal cortex, predicted value, and choice Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. ISSN 0077-8923 ANNALS OF THE NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES Issue: Critical Contributions of the Orbitofrontal Cortex to Behavior The orbitofrontal cortex, predicted value, and choice

More information

The Contribution of the Amygdala to Reward-Related Learning and Extinction

The Contribution of the Amygdala to Reward-Related Learning and Extinction Chapter 14 The Contribution of the Amygdala to Reward-Related Learning and Extinction Rose Chesworth and Laura Corbit Additional information is available at the end of the chapter Abstract There has been

More information

ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon Limbic System II. David P. Wolfer MD

ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon Limbic System II. David P. Wolfer MD ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon 05.05.2014 Limbic System II David P. Wolfer MD Institute of Anatomy, University of Zurich Institute for Human Movement Sciences and Sport, ETH Zurich http://www.dpwolfer.ch

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Introduction to Systems Neuroscience. Nov. 28, The limbic system. Daniel C. Kiper

Introduction to Systems Neuroscience. Nov. 28, The limbic system. Daniel C. Kiper Introduction to Systems Neuroscience Nov. 28, 2017 The limbic system Daniel C. Kiper kiper@ini.phys.ethz.ch http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html LIMBIC SYSTEM The term limbic system mean

More information

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. Provided for non-commercial research and educational use. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This article was originally published in the Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference,

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala Disrupt Selective Aspects of Reinforcer Representation in Rats

Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala Disrupt Selective Aspects of Reinforcer Representation in Rats The Journal of Neuroscience, November 15, 2001, 21(22):9018 9026 Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala Disrupt Selective Aspects of Reinforcer Representation in Rats Pam Blundell, 1 Geoffrey Hall, 1 and

More information

nucleus accumbens septi hier-259 Nucleus+Accumbens birnlex_727

nucleus accumbens septi hier-259 Nucleus+Accumbens birnlex_727 Nucleus accumbens From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Brain: Nucleus accumbens Nucleus accumbens visible in red. Latin NeuroNames MeSH NeuroLex ID nucleus accumbens septi hier-259 Nucleus+Accumbens birnlex_727

More information

ISIS NeuroSTIC. Un modèle computationnel de l amygdale pour l apprentissage pavlovien.

ISIS NeuroSTIC. Un modèle computationnel de l amygdale pour l apprentissage pavlovien. ISIS NeuroSTIC Un modèle computationnel de l amygdale pour l apprentissage pavlovien Frederic.Alexandre@inria.fr An important (but rarely addressed) question: How can animals and humans adapt (survive)

More information

THE AMYGDALA AND REWARD

THE AMYGDALA AND REWARD THE AMYGDALA AND REWARD Mark G. Baxter* and Elisabeth A. Murray The amygdala an almond-shaped group of nuclei at the heart of the telencephalon has been associated with a range of cognitive functions,

More information

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi LT Memory and the Amygdala 1 Running Head: Role of the amygdala in long term memory What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment University of Mississippi LT Memory and the Amygdala

More information

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety C82NAB Neuroscience and Behaviour Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 1 Outline Emotion I (first part) Studying brain substrates of

More information

Emotion Explained. Edmund T. Rolls

Emotion Explained. Edmund T. Rolls Emotion Explained Edmund T. Rolls Professor of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford and Fellow and Tutor in Psychology, Corpus Christi College, Oxford OXPORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Contents 1 Introduction:

More information

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear Historical views that thought and emotion were processed separately in the brain Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: So far, fear is the best understood What is fear? Dictionary: A feeling of agitation and

More information

Limbic system outline

Limbic system outline Limbic system outline 1 Introduction 4 The amygdala and emotion -history - theories of emotion - definition - fear and fear conditioning 2 Review of anatomy 5 The hippocampus - amygdaloid complex - septal

More information

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title A Continuum of Learning and Memory Research Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9q3031wx Journal International Journal of Comparative Psychology,

More information

Effects of limbic corticostriatal lesions on a u t o shaping performance in rats

Effects of limbic corticostriatal lesions on a u t o shaping performance in rats Effects of limbic corticostriatal lesions on a u t o shaping performance in rats BJ Everitt, JA Parkinson, G Lachenal, KM Halkerston, N Rudarakanchana, RN Cardinal, J Hall, CH Morrison, JW Dalley, SR Howes,

More information

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in 1 NAME COURSE TITLE 2 TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in dopamine neurotransmitter, up-regulation

More information

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Behavioural Brain Research 110 (2000) 97 108 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Stephen Maren *, William Holt Department of Psychology

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and Gallo Taste Learning and Memory in Aging Milagros Gallo, PhD Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and in humans involves conscious recall. 1 Visual recognition memory is

More information

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Hippocampal Pyramidal Neuron of Mice and Rats Figure 1 Open Field Apparatus Open Field Behavior

More information

Associative learning

Associative learning Introduction to Learning Associative learning Event-event learning (Pavlovian/classical conditioning) Behavior-event learning (instrumental/ operant conditioning) Both are well-developed experimentally

More information

Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion

Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion NEUROBIOLOGY OF ADDICTION REVIEW Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion Barry J Everitt & Trevor W Robbins Drug addiction is increasingly viewed as the

More information

Decision neuroscience seeks neural models for how we identify, evaluate and choose

Decision neuroscience seeks neural models for how we identify, evaluate and choose VmPFC function: The value proposition Lesley K Fellows and Scott A Huettel Decision neuroscience seeks neural models for how we identify, evaluate and choose options, goals, and actions. These processes

More information

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and 77 5 Discussion The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and implicit learning systems. Chapter 2 described the effects of performing a working memory task

More information

Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - -

Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - - BRAIN PLASTICITY Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - - Recently, it was found that new neurons and glial cells are born in specific brain regions - reorganization. Brain plasticity occurs

More information

Overlapping neural systems mediating extinction, reversal and regulation of fear

Overlapping neural systems mediating extinction, reversal and regulation of fear Review Overlapping neural systems mediating extinction, reversal and regulation of fear Daniela Schiller 1,2 and Mauricio R. Delgado 3 1 Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003,

More information

BRAIN PLASTICITY. Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - -

BRAIN PLASTICITY. Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - - BRAIN PLASTICITY Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - - Recently, it was found that new neurons and glial cells are born in specific brain regions - reorganization. Brain plasticity occurs

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature21682 Supplementary Table 1 Summary of statistical results from analyses. Mean quantity ± s.e.m. Test Days P-value Deg. Error deg. Total deg. χ 2 152 ± 14 active cells per day per mouse

More information

Brain anatomy and artificial intelligence. L. Andrew Coward Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia

Brain anatomy and artificial intelligence. L. Andrew Coward Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia Brain anatomy and artificial intelligence L. Andrew Coward Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia The Fourth Conference on Artificial General Intelligence August 2011 Architectures

More information

For more information about how to cite these materials visit

For more information about how to cite these materials visit Author(s): Peter Hitchcock, PH.D., 2009 License: Unless otherwise noted, this material is made available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-commercial Share Alike 3.0 License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0/

More information

Basal Ganglia General Info

Basal Ganglia General Info Basal Ganglia General Info Neural clusters in peripheral nervous system are ganglia. In the central nervous system, they are called nuclei. Should be called Basal Nuclei but usually called Basal Ganglia.

More information

Stress and Emotion. Stressors are things that challenge homeostasis -- these challenges may be real or merely anticipated

Stress and Emotion. Stressors are things that challenge homeostasis -- these challenges may be real or merely anticipated Stress and Emotion 1 Stressors are things that challenge homeostasis -- these challenges may be real or merely anticipated Stress responses are what the body does about it 2 1 Two broad stressor categories

More information

Classical Conditioning

Classical Conditioning What is classical conditioning? Classical Conditioning Learning & Memory Arlo Clark-Foos Learning to associate previously neutral stimuli with the subsequent events. Howard Eichenbaum s Thanksgiving Pavlov

More information

Class 16 Emotions (10/19/17) Chapter 10

Class 16 Emotions (10/19/17) Chapter 10 Class 16 Emotions (10/19/17) Chapter 10 Notes By: Rashea Psych 302 10/19/17 Emotions The issues o Innate or learned? o Voluntary or involuntary? (conscious/unconscious) o Adaptive behavior or communication?

More information

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This chapter was originally published in the book Neuroeconomics: Decision Making and

More information

Prof. Saeed Abuel Makarem & Dr.Sanaa Alshaarawy

Prof. Saeed Abuel Makarem & Dr.Sanaa Alshaarawy Prof. Saeed Abuel Makarem & Dr.Sanaa Alshaarawy 1 Objectives By the end of the lecture, you should be able to: Describe the anatomy and main functions of the thalamus. Name and identify different nuclei

More information

Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6

Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6 Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6 I. WHAT IS AN EMOTION? A. Three components (Oately & Jenkins, 1996) 1. caused by conscious or unconscious evaluation of an event as relevant to a goal that is important

More information

Behavioral and Motivational mechanisms of Brain. Limbic system and the Hypothalamus

Behavioral and Motivational mechanisms of Brain. Limbic system and the Hypothalamus Behavioral and Motivational mechanisms of Brain Limbic system and the Hypothalamus 1 General functions 1. Control of behavior 2. Control level of activities in different parts of brain 3. Motivational

More information

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats Physiology & Behavior 76 (2002) 117 129 The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats Peter C. Holland a, *, Gorica D. Petrovich b, Michela Gallagher b a Department

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates

Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates Wen Cai, MD, Ph.D. Chief Medical Officer--La Frontera Arizona Clinical Associate Professor--Psychiatry and Psychology University of Arizona College

More information

9.14 Class 32 Review. Limbic system

9.14 Class 32 Review. Limbic system 9.14 Class 32 Review Limbic system 1 Lateral view Medial view Brainstem, sagittal section Sensory- Perceptual Motor Behavior Major functional modules of the CNS Motivation Courtesy of MIT Press. Used with

More information

ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING. Felipe Schiffino

ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING. Felipe Schiffino ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING by Felipe Schiffino A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in partial

More information

Anatomy of the basal ganglia. Dana Cohen Gonda Brain Research Center, room 410

Anatomy of the basal ganglia. Dana Cohen Gonda Brain Research Center, room 410 Anatomy of the basal ganglia Dana Cohen Gonda Brain Research Center, room 410 danacoh@gmail.com The basal ganglia The nuclei form a small minority of the brain s neuronal population. Little is known about

More information

A Computational Model of Emotional Conditioning in the Brain

A Computational Model of Emotional Conditioning in the Brain A Computational Model of Emotional Conditioning in the Brain Christian Balkenius Jan Morén Lund University Cognitive Science Kungshuset, Lundagård S-222 22 LUND christian.balkenius@fil.lu.se jan.moren@fil.lu.se

More information

THE BRAIN HABIT BRIDGING THE CONSCIOUS AND UNCONSCIOUS MIND

THE BRAIN HABIT BRIDGING THE CONSCIOUS AND UNCONSCIOUS MIND THE BRAIN HABIT BRIDGING THE CONSCIOUS AND UNCONSCIOUS MIND Mary ET Boyle, Ph. D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD How did I get here? What did I do? Start driving home after work Aware when you left

More information

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman BIOMED 509 Executive Control Cambridge University Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan UNM SOM JL Brigman 4-7-17 Symptoms and Assays of Cognitive Disorders Symptoms of Cognitive Disorders Learning & Memory

More information

Reward Systems: Human

Reward Systems: Human Reward Systems: Human 345 Reward Systems: Human M R Delgado, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, USA ã 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Introduction Rewards can be broadly defined as stimuli of positive

More information

Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling

Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling Research Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling Kate M. Wassum, 1,2,4 Sean B. Ostlund, 1,2 Bernard W. Balleine, 3

More information

Control of food consumption by learned cues: A forebrain hypothalamic network

Control of food consumption by learned cues: A forebrain hypothalamic network Physiology & Behavior 91 (2007) 397 403 Control of food consumption by learned cues: A forebrain hypothalamic network Gorica D. Petrovich, Michela Gallagher Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences,

More information

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review Jim Morrill, MD, PhD MGH Charlestown HealthCare Center Massachusetts General Hospital Disclosures Neither I nor my spouse/partner has a relevant financial relationship

More information

Memory. Psychology 3910 Guest Lecture by Steve Smith

Memory. Psychology 3910 Guest Lecture by Steve Smith Memory Psychology 3910 Guest Lecture by Steve Smith Note: Due to copyright restrictions, I had to remove the images from the Weschler Memory Scales from the slides I posted online. Wechsler Memory Scales

More information

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2004, Vol. 30, No. 2, 104 117 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.2.104

More information

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

The Adolescent Developmental Stage The Adolescent Developmental Stage o Physical maturation o Drive for independence o Increased salience of social and peer interactions o Brain development o Inflection in risky behaviors including experimentation

More information

The psychology department at Memorial University provided a supportive environment in which I was surrounded and encouraged by eminent scholars

The psychology department at Memorial University provided a supportive environment in which I was surrounded and encouraged by eminent scholars The highlight for November is by Steve Reilly from the Department of Psychology at the University of Illinois at Chicago. Dr. Reilly s interest in conditioned taste aversion (CTA) learning dates back to

More information

Computational cognitive neuroscience: 8. Motor Control and Reinforcement Learning

Computational cognitive neuroscience: 8. Motor Control and Reinforcement Learning 1 Computational cognitive neuroscience: 8. Motor Control and Reinforcement Learning Lubica Beňušková Centre for Cognitive Science, FMFI Comenius University in Bratislava 2 Sensory-motor loop The essence

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

Contributions of the striatum to learning, motivation, and performance: an associative account

Contributions of the striatum to learning, motivation, and performance: an associative account Review Contributions of the striatum to learning, motivation, and performance: an associative account Mimi Liljeholm and John P. O Doherty Division of the Humanities and Social Sciences, and Computation

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Chapter 5 Study Guide Chapter 5 Study Guide Practice Exam Questions: Which of the following is not included in the definition of learning? It is demonstrated immediately Assuming you have eaten sour pickles before, imagine

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

NEURAL CIRCUITRY OF ANXIETY AND STRESS DISORDERS

NEURAL CIRCUITRY OF ANXIETY AND STRESS DISORDERS 64 NEURAL CIRCUITRY OF ANXIETY AND STRESS DISORDERS MICHAEL DAVIS BRAIN SYSTEMS IN THE GENERATION OF FEAR AND ANXIETY Role of the Amygdala Many data now indicate that the amygdala, along with its many

More information

Eating beyond metabolic need: how environmental cues influence feeding behavior

Eating beyond metabolic need: how environmental cues influence feeding behavior Review Special issue: neural control of appetite Eating beyond metabolic need: how environmental cues influence feeding behavior Alexander W. Johnson Department of Psychology, Michigan State University,

More information

Chapter 3. Structure and Function of the Nervous System. Copyright (c) Allyn and Bacon 2004

Chapter 3. Structure and Function of the Nervous System. Copyright (c) Allyn and Bacon 2004 Chapter 3 Structure and Function of the Nervous System 1 Basic Features of the Nervous System Neuraxis: An imaginary line drawn through the center of the length of the central nervous system, from the

More information

THE CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. The Brain & Spinal Cord

THE CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. The Brain & Spinal Cord THE CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM The Brain & Spinal Cord Review: Nervous System Parallel Distributed Processing Composition of the CNS Nuclei: Clusters of neurons in the CNS ( neighborhoods ) Fiber Tracts/Pathways:

More information

REVIEW Neural and psychological mechanisms underlying compulsive drug seeking habits and drug memories indications for novel treatments of addiction*

REVIEW Neural and psychological mechanisms underlying compulsive drug seeking habits and drug memories indications for novel treatments of addiction* European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 40, pp. 2163 2182, 2014 doi:10.1111/ejn.12644 REVIEW Neural and psychological mechanisms underlying compulsive drug seeking habits and drug memories indications for

More information

Fear Models in Animals and Humans

Fear Models in Animals and Humans Catherine A. Hartley and Elizabeth A. Phelps Abstract While fear learning is an adaptive behavior critical to our survival, excessive fear can markedly impair one s ability to function and is a central

More information

Functional Neuroanatomy. IBRO ISN African Neuroscience School 4-13 th Dec 2014 Nairobi, Kenya

Functional Neuroanatomy. IBRO ISN African Neuroscience School 4-13 th Dec 2014 Nairobi, Kenya Functional Neuroanatomy IBRO ISN African Neuroscience School 4-13 th Dec 2014 Nairobi, Kenya What is/are the function(s) of the nervous system? Sensation Perception Visceral activities (Homeostasis) Behavior

More information

PARALLELS BETWEEN CEREBELLUM- AND AMYGDALA-DEPENDENT CONDITIONING

PARALLELS BETWEEN CEREBELLUM- AND AMYGDALA-DEPENDENT CONDITIONING PARALLELS BETWEEN CEREBELLUM- AND AMYGDALA-DEPENDENT CONDITIONING Javier F. Medina, J. Christopher Repa, Michael D. Mauk and Joseph E. LeDoux Recent evidence from cerebellum-dependent motor learning and

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may have as many as 200,000

More information

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala 11 December 1997 Nature 390, 604-607 (1997) Macmillan Publishers Ltd. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala MICHAEL T. ROGAN, URSULA V. STÄUBLI & JOSEPH E. LEDOUX

More information

Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making

Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making A Unifying Computational Neuroscience Approach Edmund T. Rolls University of Oxford Department of Experimental Psychology Oxford England OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Contents

More information

Insular Cortex-Amygdala Dialogue During Taste Recognition Memory Formation

Insular Cortex-Amygdala Dialogue During Taste Recognition Memory Formation The highlight for April is by Dr. Federico Bermudez-Rattoni from the Institute of Cellular Physiology, National University of Mexico in Mexico City, Mexico. Dr. Bermudez-Rattoni s work over the past 25

More information

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD in this talk what is marijuana? the brain on marijuana is the teen brain special? current research what is marijuana?

More information

Consolidation and Reconsolidation of Incentive Learning in the Amygdala

Consolidation and Reconsolidation of Incentive Learning in the Amygdala 830 The Journal of Neuroscience, January 26, 2005 25(4):830 835 Brief Communication Consolidation and Reconsolidation of Incentive Learning in the Amygdala Szu-Han Wang, 1 * Sean B. Ostlund, 2 * Karim

More information

Chapter 4. Role of the nucleus accumbens core and shell in Pavlovian instrumental transfer

Chapter 4. Role of the nucleus accumbens core and shell in Pavlovian instrumental transfer Chapter 4: Pavlovian instrumental transfer 128 Chapter 4. Role of the nucleus accumbens core and shell in Pavlovian instrumental transfer Abstract. When an initially neutral stimulus has been paired in

More information

Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral Amygdala Lesions in Rats?

Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral Amygdala Lesions in Rats? Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 76, 268 283 (2001) doi:10.1006/nlme.2001.4042, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral

More information

Teach-SHEET Basal Ganglia

Teach-SHEET Basal Ganglia Teach-SHEET Basal Ganglia Purves D, et al. Neuroscience, 5 th Ed., Sinauer Associates, 2012 Common organizational principles Basic Circuits or Loops: Motor loop concerned with learned movements (scaling

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

Basal Ganglia. Introduction. Basal Ganglia at a Glance. Role of the BG

Basal Ganglia. Introduction. Basal Ganglia at a Glance. Role of the BG Basal Ganglia Shepherd (2004) Chapter 9 Charles J. Wilson Instructor: Yoonsuck Choe; CPSC 644 Cortical Networks Introduction A set of nuclei in the forebrain and midbrain area in mammals, birds, and reptiles.

More information

The Methods of Cognitive Neuroscience. Sensory Systems and Perception: Auditory, Mechanical, and Chemical Senses 93

The Methods of Cognitive Neuroscience. Sensory Systems and Perception: Auditory, Mechanical, and Chemical Senses 93 Contents in Brief CHAPTER 1 Cognitive Neuroscience: Definitions, Themes, and Approaches 1 CHAPTER 2 The Methods of Cognitive Neuroscience CHAPTER 3 Sensory Systems and Perception: Vision 55 CHAPTER 4 CHAPTER

More information

Parsing reward. Kent C. Berridge and Terry E. Robinson

Parsing reward. Kent C. Berridge and Terry E. Robinson Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.26 No.9 September 2003 507 Parsing reward Kent C. Berridge and Terry E. Robinson Department of Psychology (Biopsychology Program), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor,

More information

Introduction the basics of psychological learning and memory theory. From Mechanisms of Memory by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Introduction the basics of psychological learning and memory theory. From Mechanisms of Memory by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Introduction the basics of psychological learning and memory theory. From Mechanisms of Memory by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Definitions Learning: The acquisition of an altered behavioral response due to an

More information

1) Drop off in the Bi 150 box outside Baxter 331 or to the head TA (jcolas).

1) Drop off in the Bi 150 box outside Baxter 331 or  to the head TA (jcolas). Bi/CNS/NB 150 Problem Set 5 Due: Tuesday, Nov. 24, at 4:30 pm Instructions: 1) Drop off in the Bi 150 box outside Baxter 331 or e-mail to the head TA (jcolas). 2) Submit with this cover page. 3) Use a

More information

Learning. Learning. Habituation. Sensitization. Habituation and Sensitization

Learning. Learning. Habituation. Sensitization. Habituation and Sensitization Learning Learning Adaptive change in behavior that results from past experiences Nonassociative Learning Habituation Sensitization Classical Conditioning Instrumental Learning/Operant Conditioning Ability

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010 Notes Textbook used Week 1-3: Bouton, M.E. (2016). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. 2nd Ed. Sinauer Week 4-6: Rieger, E. (Ed.) (2014) Abnormal Psychology:

More information

Impaired Outcome-Specific Devaluation of Instrumental Responding in Mice with a Targeted Deletion of the AMPA Receptor Glutamate Receptor 1 Subunit

Impaired Outcome-Specific Devaluation of Instrumental Responding in Mice with a Targeted Deletion of the AMPA Receptor Glutamate Receptor 1 Subunit The Journal of Neuroscience, March 2, 2005 25(9):2359 2365 2359 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Impaired Outcome-Specific Devaluation of Instrumental Responding in Mice with a Targeted Deletion of the AMPA

More information

Multiple Roles for Synaptic Plasticity in Pavlovian Fear Conditioning

Multiple Roles for Synaptic Plasticity in Pavlovian Fear Conditioning Maren Synaptic Plasticity and Fear Conditioning 1 To appear in Neuronal Mechanisms of Memory Formation (ed. C. Hölscher) Multiple Roles for Synaptic Plasticity in Pavlovian Fear Conditioning Stephen Maren

More information

Multiple Forms of Value Learning and the Function of Dopamine. 1. Department of Psychology and the Brain Research Institute, UCLA

Multiple Forms of Value Learning and the Function of Dopamine. 1. Department of Psychology and the Brain Research Institute, UCLA Balleine, Daw & O Doherty 1 Multiple Forms of Value Learning and the Function of Dopamine Bernard W. Balleine 1, Nathaniel D. Daw 2 and John O Doherty 3 1. Department of Psychology and the Brain Research

More information

THE PERSISTENCE OF MALADAPTIVE MEMORY: ADDICTION, DRUG

THE PERSISTENCE OF MALADAPTIVE MEMORY: ADDICTION, DRUG THE PERSISTENCE OF MALADAPTIVE MEMORY: ADDICTION, DRUG MEMORIES AND ANTI-RELAPSE TREATMENTS A.L. Milton 1 & B.J. Everitt 1 1 Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Experimental

More information

Basal nuclei, cerebellum and movement

Basal nuclei, cerebellum and movement Basal nuclei, cerebellum and movement MSTN121 - Neurophysiology Session 9 Department of Myotherapy Basal Nuclei (Ganglia) Basal Nuclei (Ganglia) Role: Predict the effects of various actions, then make

More information