Individual differences in the fan effect and working memory capacity q

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Individual differences in the fan effect and working memory capacity q"

Transcription

1 Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) Journal of Memory and Language Individual differences in te fan effect and working memory capacity q Micael F. Bunting a, *, Andrew R.A. Conway b, Ricard P. Heitz c a Department of Psycological Sciences, University of Missouri, 207 McAlester Hall, Columbia, MO 65211, USA b Department of Psycology, Princeton University, Green Hall, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA c Scool of Psycology, Georgia Institute of Tecnology, Atlanta, GA, USA Received 18 April 2004; revision received 22 July 2004 Available online 17 September 2004 Abstract In opposition to conceptualizing working memory (WM) in terms of a general capacity, we present four experiments tat favor te view tat individual differences in WM depend on attentional control. Hig- and low-wm participants, as assessed by te operation span task, learned unrelated sentences for wic te subject and predicate of te sentences sared concepts (fan). Sentences were learned in sets organized by subjects (Experiments 1A and 1B) or predicates (Experiments 2A and 2B). WM predicted accuracy and reaction times on a subsequent speeded verification task, but not learning. In Experiments 1A and 2A, low-wm participants ad a steeper, positively sloped fan effect for reaction times to studied items tan ig-wm participants. In Experiments 1B and 2B, fan was eliminated across but not witin memory sets, wic eliminated individual differences but not slope to te fan effect. Tese effects suggest te crux of WM is attentional control, and competition across sets causes individual differences. Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rigts reserved. Keywords: Attentional control; Fan effect; General capacity; Individual differences; Inibition; Working memory q Experiments 1A and 1B of tis researc were submitted to te University of Illinois at Cicago in partial fulfillment of te requirements for te Master of Arts degree by Micael F. Bunting. We tank Bennetta Fairfax, Cristina Gabe, Antony Kamis, Dimitri Perivoliotis, and Bradley Poole for testing participants in te prescreening, and Stellan Olsson and Gary Raney for offering elpful comments on an earlier draft of te manuscript. We especially tank Stepen Tuolski for providing elp in planning and interpreting tese experiments. Micael Bunting is supported by a postdoctoral fellowsip at te University of Missouri from te Missouri Reabilitation Researc Training Program (Kristofer Hagglund, P.I.), te National Institute of Cild Healt and Human Development, and te National Institutes of Healt (Grant 2 T32 HD ). * Corresponding autor. Fax: address: buntingm@missouri.edu (M.F. Bunting). Introduction Rapidly and accurately retrieving familiar information from semantic memory sometimes requires working memory capacity. Cantor and Engle (1993, see Experiment 1 of teir report) demonstrated as muc wen tey observed tat te magnitude (or slope) of fan effects for retrieval from long-term semantic memory varied as a function of individual differences in working memory capacity. Fan effects occur wen te time required to verify a proposition encoded in long-term memory increases concomitantly wit te number of propositions collectively committed to memory (Anderson, 1983). Fan effects are te product of interference among sared concepts. Te critical result in Cantor and Engle (1993) X/$ - see front matter Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rigts reserved. doi: /j.jml

2 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) was tat participants wit less working memory capacity revealed more dramatic fan effects tan tose wit more working memory capacity, and te slopes of individual fan effects statistically accounted for te relationsip between working memory capacity and reading compreension, as measured by te Verbal Scolastic Aptitude Test (VSAT). Tis result as been used to promote te idea of working memory capacity as amount of activation. According to Cantor and EngleÕs (1993) teoretical framework, working memory is te activated portion of long-term memory, and working memory capacity is te total amount of activation available for cognition (a similar position was taken in Anderson & Lebiere, 1998; Engle, Cantor, & Carullo, 1992; Lovett, Reder, & Lebiere, 1999). Four experiments are reported ere; te results of wic are inconsistent wit te notion of working memory capacity as amount of activation and inconsistent wit Cantor and EngleÕs (1993) interpretation of individual differences in te fan effect. Te current experiments suggest tat individual differences in working memory capacity and te fan effect are better explained by a model in wic working memory capacity refers to te ability to resist interference rater tan a limited amount of activation. Working memory as capacity Tere is some consensus among cognitive psycologists tat working memory is a system, or set of processes, tat allows for te active maintenance of information in te face of concurrent processing and/ or distraction (Baddeley & Hitc, 1974; Baddeley & Logie, 1999; Engle, Kane, & Tuolski, 1999; Miyake & Sa, 1999). Working memory span tasks, including counting span, operation span, and reading span, were developed in accordance wit tis view of working memory (for examples of eac see Case, Kurland, & Goldberg, 1982; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Turner & Engle, 1989, respectively). Altoug different in surface level features, tese tasks are structurally similar, for some sort of secondary processing (be it counting sapes, solving matematical operations, or reading sentences) is imposed during encoding to detract from memory for some memoranda. For example, in te operation span task used in te present set of experiments and described more torougly in te Prescreening, participants verify te veracity of simple matematical equations wile attempting to remember strings of unrelated words for later recall. Eac trial consists of a matematical operation followed by a memoranda (e.g., Is (9 * 2) 2 = 15? Road), and recall is cued after sets of 2 5 operationword strings. Working memory span is te number of words correctly recalled. Assessed as suc, working memory capacity reliably and strongly predicts more complex cognitive beaviors suc as reasoning, problem solving, and reading compreension (Engle, 2001). Working memory span was once commonly described as te capacity to sare resources among competing goals (Cantor & Engle, 1993; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Just & Carpenter, 1992; Turner & Engle, 1989). Daneman and Carpenter (1980), in te tradition of Baddeley and HitcÕs (1974) working memory model, proposed tat te processing and storage components of teir reading span task made competing demands for a sared resource, and tey ypotesized tat more efficient processing permits more room for storage. Oters, including Turner and Engle (1989), took a similar capacity view of working memory, but tey differed from Daneman and Carpenter in te extent to wic tey conceived of tis resource as a domainspecific. Turner and Engle made a convincing argument for te domain-generality of working memory wen tey demonstrated tat operation span, wic does not involve reading, accounted for as muc variance in multiple measures of reading ability as reading span. Reading span and operation span account for te same variance in reading compreension, and tey load on te same factor in factor-analytic studies (Conway, Cowan, Bunting, Terriault, & Minkoff, 2002; Engle, Kane et al., 1999; Kane et al., 2004). Terefore, te processing component need not be reading in order to predict reading compreension. According to te general capacity model of working memory capacity, working memory is a domain-free memory capacity (Engle et al., 1992). Te dual components of operation span, as in oter span tasks, require active maintenance (i.e., remembering words for later recall) in te face of concurrent processing (i.e., solving matematical operations). Working memory as attentional control Te general capacity model was closely associated wit a size metapor for memory capacity and empasized te measurement of memory ability, but tis model as not eld up to empirical investigation (e.g., Conway & Engle, 1994; for a review see Engle, 2001). Engle and colleagues, wo were largely responsible for proposing and advancing te general capacity model, ave found tat pure memorial processes fail to account for te covariation between working memory span and iger-order cognition. Tey ave instead argued tat working memory is equivalent to sort-term memory plus a general, controlled-attention ability (Engle, 2001; Engle, Tuolski, Lauglin, & Conway, 1999; Kane & Engle, 2003). It is tis attentional control component tat is responsible for te predictive utility of working memory span tasks. According to Engle and colleaguesõ controlled attention teory of working memory, working memory capacity refers to te attentional ability to maintain activation for goal-relevant information and ignore goal-irrelevant or distracting information via control mecanisms suc as goal maintenance and inibition.

3 606 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) In support of tis view, Engle and colleagues ave produced a line of correlation-based studies in wic were found positive and predictive relationsips between working memory capacity and measures of attentional control. Tis researc includes Conway, Cowan, and BuntingÕs (2001) demonstration tat individual differences in working memory capacity are predictive of sadowing errors in a dicotic listening ( cocktail party ) paradigm. Kane and Engle (2003) demonstrated a similar relationsip but wit te Stroop paradigm. Te Stroop effect, a quintessential marker of te failure of attentional control, emerges in a color naming task wen te to-be-named color appears in te form of a color word and wen te ink color and color name do not matc (e.g., te word blue written in te color red wen te goal is to name te color red). Kane and Engle (2003) sowed tat tose wit less working memory capacity are more likely to make Stroop errors tan tose wit more working memory capacity, but only wen te Stroop task was composed of mostly congruent trials (e.g., te word blue sown in te color blue). Wen te task was so biased, tose wit less working memory capacity could rely on te easier prepotent response of reading te word sown. Adopting tis strategy, owever, is detrimental to performance on te occasional incongruent trial in wic te to-be-named color and color name do not matc. Kane and Engle suggested tat individual differences in te Stroop effect are evidence of te ig spanõs ability to remain focused on te task goal, wic is someting tat takes attentional control. Haser and colleagues also implicate attentional control, specifically te ability to suppress proactive interference, in teir view of working memory capacity (Haser & Zacks, 1988; Haser, Zacks, & May, 1999). Lustig, May, and Haser (2001; see also May, Haser, & Kane, 1999) sowed tat te reading span task is susceptible to te effects of proactive interference (PI). Lustig, May et al. were motivated by te ypotesis tat PI builds over many consecutive sets in most any memory span task but especially in te reading span task. Terefore, tey manipulated set order, presenting sets eiter in ascending (smallest to largest) or descending order (largest to smallest), and ypotesized tat te descendingorder condition would pose less difficulty even for tose prone to interference effects, suc as older adults in teir study. Arguably, te effect of PI sould be minimized if te largest (and most difficult) sets are presented first, before te effects of PI ave mounted. Not only did tis manipulation improve span scores, but te relationsip between span and reading compreension was attenuated. From tis perspective, te crux of working memory span tasks is interference, and te ability te control interference from prior episodes is te source of individual differences (Haser, Tonev, Lustig, & Zacks, 2003; Lustig, Haser, & Tonev, 2001; Lustig, May et al., 2001; May et al., 1999). Te relevant distinction for us is not between tese two teories of attentional control but ow te concept of attentional control is an important sift from a focus on memory capacity and te size metapor for memory. For tat reason, we will use te term attentional control to refer collectively to EngleÕs teory and HaserÕs teory, wic ave more commonalities tan differences and wic would make identical predictions in our studies. We use te term capacity view to refer to te opposing view of working memory capacity tat is focused on quantifying memory capacity. Working memory and te fan effect Te sift from memorial processes or memory ability to attentional control in contemporary accounts of working memory brougt us to reevaluate Cantor and EngleÕs (1993) experiment wit te fan paradigm and teir primary explanation of teir effect. In Cantor and Engle (1993) and in te basic fan paradigm (Anderson, 1983), participants first learned many facts about various people in various locations (e.g., te artist is in te ouse, te plumber is in te park, te teacer is in te boat). In a subsequent speeded recognition task, tey verified te verity of some statements; some were learned target facts and some were unstudied foils (e.g., te teacer is in te train). Cantor and EngleÕs materials were also used in Experiment 1 of tis report and are listed in Table 1. In Cantor and Engle, an interference effect was manifest in reaction time to te queried facts; te more facts tat were associated wit a person or a location, te longer it took participants to verify just one fact about tat person or location. In te basic fan paradigm, a fan effect on retrieval can appen for accuracy measures, too. Cantor and Engle (1993) found tat te slope of te fan effect for reaction times was greater for tose wit low working memory capacity tan tose wit ig working memory capacity. (Working memory span was assessed wit te operation span task, and te low and ig groups were tose in te lower and upper quartiles of te distribution for operation span, respectively.) Cantor and Engle (1993) advocated for te general capacity view of working memory and attributed individual differences in te fan effect to differences in te activation capacity of working memory. Tey argued tat working memory is equivalent to te activated portion of semantic long-term memory and tat working memory capacity is equivalent to source activation. Tis view is consistent wit oter capacity-limited models of spreading activation (e.g., Anderson, 1983). To account for te fan effect, Anderson (1983) suggested tat learned facts are stored in networks of semantic-associates. Wen a test sentence is presented in a fan paradigm like tat used by Cantor and Engle (1993), te memory

4 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) Table 1 Studied and non-studied Person and Location pairs in Experiments 1A (d) 1B() Location-terms Person-terms Lawyer Artist Plumber Fireman Teacer Doctor (1) Studied Person and Location pairs Boat d d Park d d Curc d d Bank d d Diner Hotel Museum Airport House d d Store d d Zoo d d Train d d Stadium Jail Teater Camper (2) Non-studied Person and Location pairs Boat d d Park d d Curc d d Bank d d Diner Hotel Museum Airport House d d Store d d Zoo d d Train d d Stadium Jail Teater Camper Note. Propositions were of te form, Te person is in te location. nodes corresponding to te subject and predicate of te sentence are activated, and tis activation spreads evenly to related nodes. For example, if given te sentence, a plumber is in te park, activation sould spread to oter locations associated wit te person (e.g., plumber) and to oter people associated wit te location (e.g., park). Participants correctly verified facts in te recognition test wen activation spreading from one node intersected wit activation spreading from anoter node. Because activation spreads equally and evenly to all related nodes, te number of oter associative links limits te speed at wic activation spreads. Greater source activation, owever, speeds te spread of activation. Hence, arguing from Cantor and EngleÕs vantage, one could attribute faster reaction times and smaller fan effects for tose wit more working memory capacity to more source activation, or capacity. An attentional control view of working memory, consistent wit EngleÕs controlled attention view and HaserÕs inibition view, can also account for Cantor and EngleÕs (1993) effect and an additional prediction tat te general capacity view cannot. Individual differences in te fan effect may be due to te ability to avoid making spurious connections (cf. Lustig, Haser et al., 2001, 2001). Tose wit low working memory capacity may keep irrelevant propositions unnecessarily active, tereby overloading working memory. Te inibition view was in its infancy at te time te Cantor and Engle paper was publised, but Cantor and Engle offered tis as an untested ypotesis and callenge to teir own interpretation: If low-span subjects are more vulnerable to interference tan are ig-span subjects, te fan task could reflect te difference. It remains to be seen weter te differential fan effect observed between ig-

5 608 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) and low-span subjects in te present studies would be found wen te fact-retrieval task involved minimal interference (p. 1111). Two kinds of interference are apparent wen one considers carefully ow participants learned and were later tested on te fan propositions in Cantor and Engle (1993). Cantor and Engle employed a blocked design for te learning procedure. Participants were exposed to te propositions in sets organized by te person-term of te sentences, and te duration of te study time was proportionate to te set size. Referring to te stimuli in Table 1, one can see tat te tree propositions for plumber (te plumber is in te park... curc... bank) were studied togeter and were terefore episodically unique. Altoug tere was interference wit te person-term plumber as a result of te fact tat it was associated wit multiple locations, tat interference was limited witin a single learning episode. Te same was not true for interference from multiple instances of te location-terms. Te location-terms were repeated across blocks witin te learning procedure. For example, te location-term, park, was sared wit two person-terms, plumber and teacer. Tis particular form of cross-episode interference sould be especially difficult for tose wit low working memory capacity. An attentional control view of working memory would predict tat interference from irrelevant episodes is detrimental to low spans but not ig spans (Lustig, May et al., 2001). Our goal was to test te interference ypotesis wit te manipulation of response competition between Experiments 1A and 1B. We replicated Cantor and EngleÕs (1993) design in Experiment 1A and sougt one critical effect: a fan size-by-span interaction, wic would indicate tat te slope of te fan effect for te low-span group was greater tan te slope for te ig-span group. (Note. Cantor and Engle reported te fan effect as a function of person-fan, rater tan location-fan or total-fan. Because te purpose of Experiment 1A was to replicate Cantor and Engle, te same approac was taken ere for all experiments). In Experiment 1B, we eliminated interference from across episodes but not interference witin episodes. Location-fan was eld at one (i.e., interference or overlap among locations in te propositions was eliminated). Person-fan was greater tan one to cause a fan effect. According to a capacity-limited view of working memory capacity, individual differences in te fan effect sould remain because activation is still divided as person-fan increases and igand low-span participants differ in te total amount of activation. In contrast and according to attentional control views of working memory capacity, te elimination of overlap among locations (Experiment 1B) sould reduce response competition from irrelevant learning episodes and, terefore, eliminate or attenuate individual differences in te fan effect. A main effect of fan was ypotesized in Experiments 1A and 1B. Participant screening Participants were screened for working memory capacity on te operation-word span task. Turner and Engle (1989) originally reported tis score, and it consistently correlates wit performance on tests of cognitive ability (e.g., VSAT and RavenÕs Progressive Matrices; see Cantor & Engle, 1993; Conway et al., 2002; Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Engle, Kane et al., 1999). Estimates of internal consistency reliability, suc as coefficient as and split-alf correlations, are typically in te range of (Conway et al., 2002; Engle, Kane et al., 1999; Kane et al., 2004). Operation span scores are also stable over time (e.g., over a period of tree monts in Klein & Fiss, 1999). Participants were tested individually and solved algebraic equations wile trying to remember series of unrelated words. A pool of 66 matematical operations (stated in te form of a question) was randomly paired wit an equal number of memoranda (ig-frequency, one-syllable, and semantically unrelated words; e.g., Is (8 * 3) 3 = 21? CALF). Te stimuli were previously reported in LaPointe and Engle (1990). Eac operation began wit multiplication or division of two integers, and a tird integer was eiter added to or subtracted from te result. A solution, wic was correct alf of te time, was provided for eac equation. Participants read eac operation aloud, said yes or no to verify its veracity, and read a word. Te experimenter initiated presentation of eac operation-word string by key press and advanced to te stimuli immediately after te participant read te word. Tree question marks followed eac set, wic ad 2 6 memoranda, and cued participants to recall and write te words in serial order on a response seet. Te response seet contained an equal number of blanks for eac trial. Participants were neiter told ow many words to expect on an upcoming trial, nor were tey reminded ow many words occurred on a previous trial. However, guessing was encouraged, and recall was not timed. Tree series of eac set size (for a total of 15 sets) were presented in a random order tat was te same for all participants. Tree additional series of two items eac were practice. Eac participantõs working memory span was te sum of te correctly recalled words for trials tat were recalled in te correct order and witout error (maximum score was 60). Accuracy at solving te matematical operations was monitored but was not taken into account in te calculation of working memory span. However, participants below 85% matematical accuracy were excluded. A large number of participants were prescreened on te operation span task for use in tese experiments and oter researc in our laboratory. Only tose in te upper or lower quartiles were eligible for tese experiments. Experiments 1A and 1B were conducted in one semester, and quartile boundaries were

6 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) determined by operation span scores from 340 participants. Quartiles for Experiments 2A and 2B were determined by operation span scores from 400 participants. In bot cases, te cutoff score for te lower quartile was 10, and upper quartile scores began at 19. Participants were undergraduates from te subject pool of te Psycology Department at te University of Illinois at Cicago and received course credit in excange for participating in te screening or any of te reported experiments. Participants ad Englis as a first language, normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and normal use of te dominant and to make rapid keyboard and written responses. Experiment 1A: Interference from location-terms, a replication of Cantor and Engle (1993) Metod Participants Forty-two participants (23 ig-span and 19 lowspan; M(SD) span scores = (5.14) and 6.79 (2.12), respectively) were selected from a pool of 340 participants prescreened on operation span. Eigt additional participants (2 ig-span and 6 low-span) were excluded for not acieving 63% accuracy on te recognition component of te fan task. Tere were 8, 24, and 32 verification trials per level of fan, respectively, so participants could miss 3 responses at Fan 1, 9 responses at Fan 3, and 12 responses at Fan 4. Materials and procedure Te materials and procedure for te fan task (acquisition and recognition) came from Cantor and Engle (1993). Te tasks were computerized and te experimental session lasted min. In tis and eac subsequent experiment, te time between te Prescreening and te fan task was different for eac participant, but most participants completed bot tasks witin a week and all participants completed bot tasks witin a six week period. Acquisition and retention task. Participants learned 16 sentences of te form, te person is in te location. See Table 1 for te materials. Eac location-term was associated wit two person-terms, and eac person-term was associated wit one, tree, or four location-terms. Hence, person-fan was 1, 3, or 4, and location-fan was 2. Sentences were grouped by te person term (e.g., all sentences for plumber were presented togeter). Te algoritm n(10s) + 10s, were n equals te number of sentences to be displayed, determined te presentation duration per group. Oral recall was tested after eac set was presented once (presentation of a person-term wit a question mark cued recall). A set of propositions was scored as correct if all of te appropriate sentences were recalled in any order and witout te generation of any additional incorrect sentences. Te experimenter monitored recall and indicated by key press weter a set was recalled correctly or not. Tis memorization-recall procedure was repeated until a set was correctly recalled on tree consecutive cycles. Wen tis criterion was met, eac was presented once more. Te number of memorization-test cycles per fan set was recorded for data analysis. Recognition task. Te speeded recognition test ad four blocks of 32 trials eac (16 studied and 16 non-studied). Non-studied (foil) sentences were generated by switcing person-terms between sets of studied sentences so tat tey still reflected te same fan size. Participants verified te veracity of eac sentence by key press (keys 1 and 3 represented yes and no, respectively). Reaction time and errors were recorded for data analysis. Results Data were analyzed from te acquisition and recognition tasks. Te dependent measure for te acquisition task was te number of memorization-recall cycles required to reac criterion. Te dependent measures for te recognition task were reaction time and accuracy. Te independent measures for bot were working memory capacity (i.e., span, ig, and low) and propositional fan size (fan: 1, 3, and 4). Trial type (studied and nonstudied sentences) was an additional independent measure in te recognition task. Acquisition and retention task Te low-span group did not require more memorization-test cycles tan te ig-span group to meet te acquisition criterion. Te mean number of memorization-recall cycles (see Table 2) for eac level of span was submitted to a fan span mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA). Tere was a significant main effect of fan, F(2,80) = 12.42, MSE = 0.25, p <.001. Ortogonal pairwise comparisons sowed tat fewer memorization-recall cycles were required on average to reac criterion at Fan 1 (M = 3.12) tan at Fan 3 (M = 3.51) or Fan 4 (M = 3.63). Te difference between Fan 3 and Fan 4 was not significant. (Note: alpa was adjusted downwards by means of te Bonferroni correction for multiple analyses). Tere was neiter a main effect of span nor an interaction between fan and span, F(1,40) < 1.0, MSE = 0.55, ns, and F(2,80) = 1.51, MSE = 0.25, ns, respectively. Recognition task Reaction time. In tis and all experiments, reaction times are for correct responses, and reaction times on incorrect response were omitted from data analysis.

7 610 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) Table 2 Means (and SD) for te number of memorization-test cycles as a function of fan size and span group for all experiments Span group Fan size Experiment 1A Hig-span 3.13 (.43) 3.37 (.51) 3.63 (.61) Low-span 3.11 (.21) 3.68 (.84) 3.63 (.76) M 3.12 (.35) 3.51 (.69) 3.63 (.67) Experiment 1B Hig-span 3.11 (.26) 3.41 (.80) 3.61 (.87) Low-span 3.27 (.59) 3.79 (.72) 3.89 (.53) M 3.19 (.46) 3.60 (.77) 3.75 (.73) Experiment 2A Hig-span 3.05 (.15) 3.27 (.48) 3.48 (.66) Low-span 3.12 (.27) 3.38 (.52) 3.38 (.57) M 3.08 (.22) 3.33 (.50) 3.43 (.61) Experiment 2B Hig-span 3.05 (.15) 3.36 (.58) 3.52 (.61) Low-span 3.22 (.33) 3.39 (.45) 3.74 (.64) M 3.13 (.27) 3.38 (.51) 3.63 (.63) Note. Perfect recall would result in tree memorization-test cycles. Reaction times greater tan 3.0 SDs from eac participantõs mean were replaced by te value at 3.0 SD. Tis adjustment affected less tan 1.0% of scores in tis and eac remaining experiment. We tested te tree-way interaction among fan, span, and trial type, but based on Cantor and Engle (1993),we did not expect tat te fan span interaction would vary by trial type. Terefore, mean reaction times (in milliseconds; see Table 3) were submitted to a fan span trial type mixed-design ANOVA. As predicted and as found in Cantor and Engle (1993), tere was an interaction between fan and span, F(2,80) = 3.30, MSE = 146,983, p <.04. Based on KeppelÕs (1991) formula for repeated measures, te effect size (x 2 ) was.09, wic is a medium Table 3 Means (and SD) for correct response times and error rates to studied and non-studied items on te recognition test of Experiments 1A and 1B for ig- and low-span participants Probe type Response times (in ms) Mean errors Fan 1 Fan 3 Fan 4 Fan 1 Fan 3 Fan 4 Experiment 1A Hig span Studied 1437 (268) 1791 (389) 1809 (375).04 (.21).74 (1.01) 1.57 (1.41) Non-studied 1737 (447) 2011 (460) 1998 (372).39 (.50) 1.22 (1.35) 1.17 (1.15) M 1587 (328) 1901 (406) 1903 (359).22 (.29).98 (.94) 1.37 (.91) Low span Studied 1545 (355) 2144 (671) 2216 (734).11 (.46) 1.63 (1.64) 3.16 (2.22) Non-studied 1985 (401) 2503 (511) 2509 (576).63 (.90) 2.63 (1.98) 3.16 (2.79) M 1765 (329) 2324 (560) 2362 (633).37 (.52) 2.13 (1.41) 3.16 (1.71) Experiment 1B Hig span Studied 1407 (509) 1761 (515) 1814 (430).32 (.72).91 (.97) 1.36 (1.22) Non-studied 1719 (455) 1896 (456) 1901 (439).45 (.67) 1.32 (2.01).91 (1.38) M 1563 (454) 1828 (468) 1857 (396).39 (.49) 1.11 (1.05) 1.14 (.88) Low span Studied 1379 (358) 1819 (476) 1938 (363).09 (.29) 1.00 (1.11) 1.41 (1.71) Non-studied 1758 (463) 1923 (371) 1902 (330).73 (.88) 1.82 (2.34) 1.32 (1.73) M 1568 (383) 1871 (409) 1920 (305).41 (.53) 1.41 (1.53) 1.36 (1.57)

8 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) to large effect (Coen, 1977). A follow-up test to tis interaction confirmed tat te slope of te fan effect for reaction time was greater for low-spans (M = 211, SE = 40) tan ig-spans (M = 113, SE = 24), t(df = 40) = 2.17, p < Tis comparison of slopes for ig- and low-spans is in keeping wit our primary ypotesis tat ig- and low-spans would differ in te magnitude (or slope) of teir fan effects. Cantor and Engle sowed tat te fan effect slope accounted for te relationsip between working memory and verbal abilities (as assessed by te verbal scolastics aptitude test, VSAT) and argued tat te magnitude of te fan effect appears to reflect te same mecanism as te working memory spans. Te pattern of te fan span interaction was similar for bot targets and foils, and tus te test of te full tree-way interaction was not significant, F(2,80) < 1.0, MSE = 42,796, ns. In te absence of any indication tat te results are different for targets and foils, we did not analyze tese items separately. Additional analyses are reported in Appendix A. Error rate. Mean errors during verification (see Table 3) were submitted to an analysis like tat for te reaction time data. A significant interaction between fan and span indicated tat te slope of te fan effect of errors was greater for low- tan ig-span participants, F(2,80) = 8.50, MSE = 1.67, p <.001. Te effect size (x 2 ) was.26, a large effect (Coen, 1977). A follow-up test to tis interaction confirmed tat te slope of te fan effect for errors was greater for low-spans (M = 1.39, SE =.11) tan ig-spans (M =.38, SE =.06), t(df = 40) = 4.52, p <.001. We did not analyze targets and foils separately because te pattern of te fan effect was similar for bot, as indicated by te fact tat te tree-way interaction of fan span trial type was not statistically significant, F(2,80) < 1.0, MSE = 1.92, ns. Additional analyses are reported in Appendix A. Discussion Te low- and ig-span groups acquired te materials in an equal amount of time, but important individual differences emerged in te recognition task. Te lowand ig-span groups revealed fan effects for reaction times and errors wit te increasing number of propositions associated wit te person-terms of te sentences, but te slopes of te fan effects were larger for te low-span group. Tese results are similar to tose of Cantor and Engle (1993, see Experiment 1 of teir 1 By slope, we mean te slope of te linear regression line troug te data points at eac fan level, wic takes into account te fact tat te distance between Fan 1 and Fan 3 is greater tan te distance between Fan 3 and Fan 4. report). Cantor and Engle reported tat individual differences in working memory capacity predicted te fan effect for reaction times, but tey did not find additional differences in te fan effect for errors. It sould be noted tat te studied propositions in Cantor and EngleÕs (1993) experiment were atypical in one respect. In teir experiment and te current Experiment 1A, te propositions at Fan 1 and Fan 3 were nested subsets of te Fan 4 propositions. For example, as seen in Table 1, te locations for lawyer and plumber togeter completely overlap wit te locations for teacer. In oter versions of te fan paradigm (e.g., Anderson, 1983), tere is overlap in te items from different fans, but tese overlaps do not result in complete subset relationsips. Suc subset relationsips could affect te way in wic Fan 4 propositions were represented in memory. In particular, any probe involving a Fan 4 person could be answered by reference to te corresponding Fan 1 and 3 items (e.g., using te above example, any probe involving teacer could be answered by retrieving te locations associated wit lawyer and plumber instead of retrieving te teacer locations directly). Tis implies tat te participants need not store four locations wit te Fan 4 person-terms at all; rater, tey need only store te two related person terms (and rely on te location-fans from tere). Tus, te representation tat participants may be constructing essentially turns te Fan 4 items into Fan 2 items. Te materials in te remaining experiments were constructed to avoid suc subset relationsips. Te results of Experiment 1A are consistent wit te view of working memory capacity as amount of activation, but susceptibility to response competition from te overlap among fan materials, specifically location-terms, may account for te individual differences observed ere. In Experiment 1A, location-fan was eld constant at two wile person-fan varied from one, tree, and four. In Experiment 1B, eac person-term was associated wit a unique location (i.e., location-fan = 1). Two alternative outcomes were foreseen. First, according to a capacity-limited perspective (Cantor & Engle, 1993), te span effects sould remain because personfan as not canged. Tat is, one would predict tat more working memory capacity sould permit faster spreading activation to te multiple location-terms associated wit te person-terms at Fan 3 and Fan 4. An alternative outcome is tat eliminating tis specific form of interference (overlapping location-terms) will at least attenuate, if not eliminate, any effect of span. An attentional control view of working memory would make tis prediction based on researc suggesting tat low spans are prone to interference from irrelevant episodes and spurious connections (Kane & Engle, 2003; Lustig, May et al., 2001). Reports from te participants also lead us to expect te second outcome. Many participants reported tat

9 612 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) tey used mnemonics to elp tem learn te fan propositions. For example, some reported trying to remember just te first letter of eac location associated wit a person-term. So, plumber PCB migt ave elped a participant to remember tat te plumber was in te park, curc, and bank. Based on attentional control teories of working memory capacity, we would not expect igand low-span participants to differ in integration and mnemonic strategies suc as tis (cf. Engle et al., 1992). But, we would expect tem to differ in teir susceptibility to interference from competing responses tat do not fit teir mnemonic cunk. Experiment 1B: No interference from location-terms Metod Participants Forty-four new participants (22 ig-span and 22 low-span; M(SD) span scores = (6.15) and 6.77 (2.56), respectively) were selected from te same pool of participants wo completed operation span for Experiment 1A. Two additional participants (1 igspan and 1 low-span) were excluded from analyses for not acieving 63% accuracy on te recognition task. Materials and procedure Te materials and procedure were uncanged from Experiment 1A, wit te exception tat tere was no overlap among location-terms (location-fan = 1). Studied and non-studied person- and location-terms are sown in Table 1. Results Te independent and dependent variables were te same as tose for Experiment 1A. Acquisition and retention task As in Experiment 1A, te low-span group did not require more memorization-test cycles tan te ig-span group to meet te acquisition criterion. Te mean number of memorization-recall cycles (see Table 2) for eac level of span was submitted to a fan span mixed-design ANOVA. Tere was a fan effect, F(2,84) = 17.19, MSE =.213, p <.001. Ortogonal pairwise comparisons sowed tat Fan 1 (M = 3.19) propositions were acquired faster tan Fan 3 (M = 3.60) or Fan 4 (M = 3.75) propositions. Te difference between Fan 3 and Fan 4 was not statistically significant. (Note: alpa was adjusted downwards by means of te Bonferroni correction for multiple analyses). Tere was neiter a main effect of span nor an interaction between fan and span, F(1,42) = 2.79, MSE =.88, ns, and F(2,84) < 1.0, MSE =.22, ns, respectively. Recognition Task Reaction time. Mean reaction times for correct responses (see Table 3) were submitted to a fan span trial type mixed-design ANOVA. Individual differences in working memory capacity were unrelated to te slope of te fan effect for reaction times. Neiter te test of te full treeway interaction nor te interaction between fan and span were significant, F(2, 84) = 1.02, MSE = 49,226, ns, and F(2,84) < 1.0, MSE = 66,794, ns, respectively. Te effect size for te fan span interaction (wic was te critical effect in Experiment 1A) was.01, and te power (by carts from Pearson & Hartley, 1972) was.88 to detect an effect size (x 2 ) of.09, or te effect size in Experiment 1A. Terefore, we cannot attribute te failure to detect te critical effect to a lack of power. However, tere was a fan effect for reaction times, and te slope of te fan effect was significantly greater tan zero, F(2,84) = 40.96, MSE = 66,794, p <.001. Additional analyses are reported in Appendix B. Error rate. Mean errors (see Table 3) were submitted to a fan span trial type mixed-design ANOVA. As for te reaction time analysis, individual differences in working memory capacity were unrelated to te fan effect slope for errors. Neiter te test of te full tree-way interaction nor te interaction between fan and span were significant, F(2,84) < 1.0, MSE = 1.49, ns, and F(2,84) < 1.0, MSE = 1.83, ns. Te effect size for te fan span interaction was.01, and te power (by carts from Pearson & Hartley, 1972) was greater tan.98 to detect an effect size (x 2 ) of.26, or te effect size in Experiment 1A. As in te case of te analysis of reaction times, we cannot attribute te failure to detect te critical effect to a lack of power. Tere was a significant, positively sloped fan effect for errors, F(2,84) = 11.81, MSE = 1.83, p <.001. Additional analyses are reported in Appendix B. Discussion Te outcome of tis experiment was consistent wit our predictions about working memory capacity and attentional control. Te ig- and low-span groups revealed nearly identical fan effects for reaction times and errors wit increasing person-fan, but te absence of location-based interference eliminated te relationsip between individual differences in working memory and fan. Tat is, te low-span group performed like te ig-span group wen te materials did not contain overlapping location-terms. Te fact tat tere is still a main effect of fan sould come as no surprise. Radvansky, Spieler, and Zacks (1993, see also Radvanksy, 1999) demonstrated tat fan effects can be attenuated and even eliminated wen te materials can be organized into real world situations, or situation models. In Radvansky et al. (1993, Experiment 1), participants did not sow fan effects for

10 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) reaction times or errors wen multiple inanimate objects were represented in a single location (i.e., a single situation model), but tey sowed fan effects wen multiple objects were in multiple locations (i.e., multiple situation models). However, te same was not true wen Radvansky et al. used person-terms in place of inanimate objects (see teir Experiment 3). In tis case, Radvansky et al. sowed tat facts about people being in locations do not readily give rise to person-based or location-based organization, and in fact participants did not spontaneously use situation models to represent people in general locations. Our current experiment is not a direct replication of Radvansky et al. (1993, Experiment 3), but our person- and location-terms are similar, as are our results. Participants in our study were asked to represent a single person in multiple locations. Tis not only does not conform to a real-world situation or an easily conceivable situation model, but our materials were not suggestive of one type of situation over anoter. Power analyses offered reassurance tat we ad sufficient power to detect effects of te same magnitude as te critical effects in Experiment 1A. Noneteless, our conclusions for Experiment 1B are contingent upon furter analysis of te between-experiments effect of interference. Wat we really need to know is weter te differences in slope between ig- and low-span participants canges depending on te interference condition. If an attentional control account of working memory is correct, ten we sould expect to see a difference in te performance for low-span, but not ig-span participants between experiments. Experiments 1A and 1B: Between-experiments analyses Te critical effect in Experiment 1A te fan span interaction was te same for targets and foils, and terefore we collapsed across te trial type variable in te between-experiments analysis of te recognition data. Te dependent measures are reaction time and accuracy in recognition. Te independent measures are working memory span and location-based interference, wic was present in Experiment 1A but not in Experiment 1B. Because we did not observe any relevant differences in learning rates for ig- and low-span participants, furter analysis of te acquisition data were not conducted. Recognition task Reaction time Reaction times for correct responses were submitted to a fan interference span mixed-design ANOVA. Te test of te full tree-way interaction was not significant, F(2,164) = 1.56, MSE = 52,955, ns. However, even in te absence of tat effect, te trends in te data are meaningful. In Fig. 1, ig-spans clearly did not differ between experiments and ad nearly identical reaction times in Experiments 1A and 1B. Low-spans performed muc like ig-spans wen we controlled for location-based interference in Experiment 1B, but reaction times for te low-spans in Experiment 1A are clearly different from te rest. A significant interference span interaction provides statistical support for tese claims as well, F(1,82) = 3.70, MSE = 435,869, p =.05. Tere is a simple effect of interference for low-spans but not ig-spans. Collapsing across fan, low-spans were significantly faster in Experiment 1B witout location-based interference (M = 1786 ms, SE = 72) tan in Experiment 1A (M = 2150 ms, SE = 105), F(1,39) = 8.53, MSE = 158,051, p <.006. Te mean reaction times for ig-spans were statistically equivalent in Experiments 1A (M = 1797 ms, SE = 66) and 1B (M = 1750 ms, SE = 88), F(1, 43) < 1.0, MSE = 133,716, ns. Additional analyses are reported in Appendix C. Error rate Mean errors (see Fig. 1) were submitted to a fan interference span mixed-design ANOVA, and te test of te full tree-way interaction was significant, F(2, 164) = 3.14, MSE =.875, p <.046. For low-spans, te fan effect slope was significantly greater in Experiment 1A, te interference experiment (M =.92, SE =.11), tan in te absence of location-based interference in Experiment 1B (M =.34, SE =.11), F(1,39) = 14.53, MSE =.24, p <.001. Te fan effect slopes for ig-spans were statistically equivalent in Experiments 1A (M =.38, SE =.06) and 1B (M =.27, SE =.08), F(1, 43) = 1.31, MSE =.11, ns. Additional analyses are reported in Appendix C. Discussion and interim conclusion Te preceding analyses of errors and reaction times provide collaborating evidence tat te difference in slopes for ig- and low-span participants depends on te presence of location-based interference. Altoug we probably lacked te statistical power to detect te interaction among fan interference span for reaction times, we argue tat a trend toward te desired effect is apparent in Fig. 1 and supported by oter significant effects. Te significant, between-subjects interaction between interference and span sows tat low-spans responded faster in te absence of location-based interference (Experiment 1B) tan in its presence (Experiment 1A). Critical, too, is te fact tat ig-spans performed equivalently between experiments and like te low-spans in Experiment 1B. Experiments 1A and 1B togeter support te ypotesis tat te ability to control attention in te service of preventing attention to irrelevant episodes, not te amount of LTM activation, accounts for individual

11 614 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) Fig. 1. Between-experiments effects for Experiments 1A and 1B. Mean verification times (in ms) and mean errors for ig- and lowspan participants as a function of fan size (1, 3, and 4) and te presence (Experiment 1A) or absence (Experiment 1B) of interference from overlapping location-terms. Fan is collapsed across trial type (studied and non-studied items). Bars represent standard error of te mean (RT, reaction time). differences in te fan effect. All forms of interference were not eliminated in Experiment 1B, but interference from competing learning episodes was removed. Tis result is consistent wit control accounts of working memory. Lustig, May et al. (2001) argued tat susceptibility to interference from irrelevant episodes is te crux for wy te reading span works te way it does. Te results of Experiment 1B seem consistent wit tat interpretation. Te implications of tis finding for teories of working memory are discussed furter in te General discussion. Experiment 2A: Interference from person-terms Location-fan was manipulated between Experiments 1A and 1B (but was eld constant witin eac experiment) wile person-fan varied from one, tree, and four. We next tested if a similar pattern of results would attain from te manipulation of te presence or absence of overlapping person-terms. Experiment 2A was similar to Experiment 1A wit respect to te fact tat participants experienced interference bot witin a learning episode and across episodes. Tere were significant canges as well. First, te design of Experiment 2A was adjusted to prevent subset relationsips. As discussed in Experiment 1A, Cantor and EngleÕs (1993) smaller fan propositions (Fan 1 and Fan 3) were completely nested witin te largest set (Fan 4). Tis is atypical and may ave inadvertently affected ow te Fan 4 items were represented. Te stimuli for Experiment 2A did not contain complete subset relationsips. To accommodate tis, fan size was canged from 1, 3, and 4 in Experiments 1A and 1B to 1, 2, and 3 in Experiments 2A and 2B. Tere was a second significant cange wit respect to ow te stimuli were grouped during learning. In Experiments 1A and 1B, te propositions were presented for study according to te personterms of te sentences, so tat all of te propositions for a person-term were presented togeter. In Experiments 2A and 2B, te propositions were organized for acquisition according to te location-terms. Location fan varied from 1 to 3, and all of te person-terms associated wit a single location were studied togeter. Te person-terms, but not location-terms, overlapped across sets in Experiment 2A. Te critical difference between Experiments 2A and 2B was te absence of overlapping person-terms in Experiment 2B (person-terms were associated wit just one location). Metod Participants Forty-tree new participants (22 ig-span and 21 low-span; M(SD) span scores = (3.20) and 6.90 (2.11), respectively) were selected from a pool of 400 participants wo completed operation span. Five additional participants (2 ig-span and 3 low-span) were excluded from analyses for not maintaining 63% accuracy on te recognition task.

12 M.F. Bunting et al. / Journal of Memory and Language 51 (2004) Materials and procedure Te experimental session lasted min. Te tasks were computerized, and participants were tested individually. Acquisition and retention task. Participants learned 12 sentences of te form, te person is in te location (see Table 4 for te materials list). Eac person was associated wit two locations, and eac location was associated wit one, two, or tree people. Hence, person-fan was 1, 2, or 3, and location-fan was 2. Sentences were grouped by te location term (e.g., all sentences for train were presented togeter). Te procedure was oterwise uncanged from Experiment 1A. Recognition task. Te speeded recognition test ad four blocks of 24 randomly-ordered trials eac (12 studied and 12 non-studied). Tere was one added criterion for te construction of te fan propositions and nonstudied foils. In order to avoid subset relationsips, te Fan 1 and Fan 2 items were not completely nested witin Fan 3, as was te case in te current Experiment 1A and Experiment 1 of Cantor and Engle (1993). In Experiments 1A and 1B, people were switced between sets of studied sentences so tat tey still reflected te same fan size. In tis experiment, te non-studied items do not reflect te same fan size. Te procedure was oterwise uncanged from Experiment 1A. Results Data were analyzed from te acquisition and recognition tasks. Te dependent measure for te acquisition task was te number of memorization-recall cycles required to meet criterion. Te dependent measures for te recognition task were reaction time and accuracy. Te independent measures for bot were working memory capacity (i.e., span; ig and low) and propositional fan size (fan: 1, 2, and 3). Trial type (studied and nonstudied sentences) was an additional independent measure in te recognition task. Acquisition and retention task As in Experiments 1A and 1B, te low-span group did not require more memorization-recall cycles tan te ig-span group to meet te acquisition criterion. Table 4 Studied and non-studied Person and Location pairs in Experiments 2A (d) 2B() Person-terms Location-terms Boat Park Curc Zoo Store Train (1) Studied Person and Location pairs Lawyer d d Artist d d Plumber d d Teacer d d Fireman d d Coac Carpenter Doctor d d Journalist Cook Realtor Musician (2) Non-studied Person and Location pairs Lawyer d d Artist d d Plumber d d Teacer d d Fireman d d Coac Carpenter Doctor d d Journalist Cook Realtor Musician Note. Propositions were of te form, Te person is in te location.

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 4, 354-373 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association Inc 0096-3445/94/S3.00 Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent

More information

Sickle Cell. Scientific Investigation

Sickle Cell. Scientific Investigation Scientific Investigation Red blood cells are oval and ave a biconcave sape, giving tem te appearance of an inner tube witout te ole. Teir sape gives tem flexibility as tey pass into small capillaries.

More information

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 699-703 Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening GREGORY J. H. COLFLESH University of Illinois, Chicago,

More information

Each year is replete with occasions to give gifts. From

Each year is replete with occasions to give gifts. From Journal of Consumer Researc Advance Access publised December 29, 2016 Experiential Gifts Foster Stronger Social Relationsips tan Material Gifts CINDY CHAN CASSIE MOGILNER Eac year is replete wit occasions

More information

Propensity score analysis with hierarchical data

Propensity score analysis with hierarchical data Section on Statistics in Epidemiology Propensity score analysis wit ierarcical data Fan Li, Alan M. Zaslavsky, Mary Bet Landrum Department of Healt Care Policy, Harvard Medical Scool 180 Longwood Avenue,

More information

Name: Key: E = brown eye color (note that blue eye color is still represented by the letter e, but a lower case one...this is very important)

Name: Key: E = brown eye color (note that blue eye color is still represented by the letter e, but a lower case one...this is very important) MONOHYBRID CROSS v2 Name: Introduction A gamete is te egg or sperm cell tat is produced by meiosis. A gamete contains te aploid number of cromosomes (in a uman tis number is 23). In eac of tese cromosomes

More information

White Rose Research Online URL for this paper:

White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: Tis is an autor produced version of Meta-analysis of absolute mean differences from randomised trials wit treatment-related clustering associated wit care providers. Wite Rose Researc Online URL for tis

More information

Running head: SEPARATING DECISION AND ENCODING NOISE. Separating Decision and Encoding Noise in Signal Detection Tasks

Running head: SEPARATING DECISION AND ENCODING NOISE. Separating Decision and Encoding Noise in Signal Detection Tasks Running ead: 1 Separating Decision and Encoding Noise in Signal Detection Tasks (Psycological Review, in press) Carlos Alexander Cabrera1, Zong-Lin Lu1, and Barbara Anne Doser2 Te Oio State University1

More information

EXPERTISE, UNDERUSE, AND OVERUSE IN HEALTHCARE * Amitabh Chandra Harvard and the NBER. Douglas O. Staiger Dartmouth and the NBER

EXPERTISE, UNDERUSE, AND OVERUSE IN HEALTHCARE * Amitabh Chandra Harvard and the NBER. Douglas O. Staiger Dartmouth and the NBER PRELIMINARY & INCOMPLETE DO NOT CITE OR DISTRIBUTE EXPERTISE, UNDERUSE, AND OVERUSE IN HEALTHCARE * Amitab Candra Harvard and te NBER Douglas O. Staiger Dartmout and te NBER Version: Marc 27, 2011 Abstract

More information

Memory retrieval and interference: Working memory issues q

Memory retrieval and interference: Working memory issues q Journal of Memory and Language 55 (2006) 33 46 Journal of Memory and Language www.elsevier.com/locate/jml Memory retrieval and interference: Working memory issues q Gabriel A. Radvansky a, *, David E.

More information

Lothian Palliative Care Guidelines patient information

Lothian Palliative Care Guidelines patient information Lotian Palliative Care Guidelines patient information Q. How will I know if te morpine is not going to work for some of my pain? A. You may still ave pain despite taking bigger doses of morpine and may

More information

Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Episodic Retrieval: Examining the Dynamics of Delayed and Continuous Distractor Free Recall

Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Episodic Retrieval: Examining the Dynamics of Delayed and Continuous Distractor Free Recall Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 6, 1020 1034 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.6.1020

More information

Proactive interference and practice effects in visuospatial working memory span task performance

Proactive interference and practice effects in visuospatial working memory span task performance MEMORY, 2011, 19 (1), 8391 Proactive interference and practice effects in visuospatial working memory span task performance Lisa Durrance Blalock 1 and David P. McCabe 2 1 School of Psychological and Behavioral

More information

Effects of working memory capacity on mental set due to domain knowledge

Effects of working memory capacity on mental set due to domain knowledge Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (6), 1456-1462 Effects of working memory capacity on mental set due to domain knowledge TRAVIS R. RICKS University of Illinois, Chicago, Illinois KANDI JO TURLEY-AMES Idaho

More information

Unbiased MMSE vs. Biased MMSE Equalizers

Unbiased MMSE vs. Biased MMSE Equalizers Tamkang Journal of Science and Engineering, Vol. 12, No. 1, pp. 45 56 (2009) 45 Unbiased MMSE vs. iased MMSE Equalizers Rainfield Y. Yen Department of Electrical Engineering, Tamkang University, Tamsui,

More information

Homophily and minority size explain perception biases in social networks

Homophily and minority size explain perception biases in social networks 3 5 6 7 8 9 3 5 Homopily and minority size explain perception biases in social networks Eun Lee,+,*, Fariba Karimi,3,+,*, Claudia Wagner,3, Hang-Hyun Jo,5,6, Markus Stromaier,7, and Mirta Galesic 8,9 Department

More information

Locomotor and feeding activity rhythms in a light-entrained diurnal rodent, Octodon degus

Locomotor and feeding activity rhythms in a light-entrained diurnal rodent, Octodon degus Locomotor and feeding activity rytms in a ligt-entrained diurnal rodent, Octodon degus R. GARCÍA-ALLEGUE, 1 P. LAX, 1 A. M. MADARIAGA, 2 AND J. A. MADRID 1 1 Department of Pysiology and Parmacology (Animal

More information

Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control

Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2011, 64 (6), 1048 1055 Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control Thomas S. Redick and Randall

More information

Working memory and flexibility in awareness and attention

Working memory and flexibility in awareness and attention Psychological Research (2005) 69: 412 419 DOI 10.1007/s00426-004-0204-7 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Michael F. Bunting Æ Nelson Cowan Working memory and flexibility in awareness and attention Received: 18 October

More information

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (6), 1067-1073 Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources MOSHE NAVEH-BENJAMIN University of Missouri,

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity

The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2001, 8 (2), 331-335 The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity ANDREW R. A. CONWAY University of Illinois, Chicago, Illinois NELSON

More information

A GEOMETRICAL OPTIMIZATION PROBLEM ASSOCIATED WITH FRUITS OF POPPY FLOWER. Muradiye, Manisa, Turkey. Muradiye, Manisa, Turkey.

A GEOMETRICAL OPTIMIZATION PROBLEM ASSOCIATED WITH FRUITS OF POPPY FLOWER. Muradiye, Manisa, Turkey. Muradiye, Manisa, Turkey. Matematical and Computational Applications, Vol. 14, No. 3, pp. 169-175, 009. Association for Scientific Researc A GEOMETRICAL OPTIMIZATION PROBLEM ASSOCIATED WITH FRUITS OF POPPY FLOWER Gözde Değer 1,

More information

Performance of Fractured Horizontal Wells in High-Permeability Reservoirs P. Valkó, SPE and M. J. Economides, SPE, Texas A&M University

Performance of Fractured Horizontal Wells in High-Permeability Reservoirs P. Valkó, SPE and M. J. Economides, SPE, Texas A&M University SPE 349 Performance of Fractured Horizontal Wells in Hig-Permeability Reservoirs P. Valkó, SPE and M. J. Economides, SPE, Texas A&M University Copyrigt 995, Society of Petroleum Engineers, Inc. Tis paper

More information

Widespread use of pure and impure placebo interventions by GPs in Germany

Widespread use of pure and impure placebo interventions by GPs in Germany Family Practice 2012; 29:79 85 doi:10.1093/fampra/cmr045 Advance Access publised on 1 August 2011 Ó Te Autor 2011. Publised by Oxford University Press. All rigts reserved. For permissions, please e-mail:

More information

Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Acute Hamstring Injury: Can We Provide a Return to Play Prognosis?

Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Acute Hamstring Injury: Can We Provide a Return to Play Prognosis? Sports Med (2015) 45:133 146 DOI 10.1007/s40279-014-0243-1 SYSTEMATIC REVIEW Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Acute Hamstring Injury: Can We Provide a Return to Play Prognosis? Gustaaf Reurink Elisabet G.

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (3), 490-494 Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness GINO CAMP, DIANE PECHER, and HENK G. SCHMIDT Erasmus University Rotterdam,

More information

Correcting for Lead Time and Length Bias in Estimating the Effect of Screen Detection on Cancer Survival

Correcting for Lead Time and Length Bias in Estimating the Effect of Screen Detection on Cancer Survival American Journal of Epidemiology ª Te Autor 2008. Publised by te Jons Hopkins Bloomberg Scool of Public Healt. All rigts reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org.

More information

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN WORKING MEMORY AND INTELLIGENCE: DECONSTRUCTING THE WORKING MEMORY TASK

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN WORKING MEMORY AND INTELLIGENCE: DECONSTRUCTING THE WORKING MEMORY TASK THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN WORKING MEMORY AND INTELLIGENCE: DECONSTRUCTING THE WORKING MEMORY TASK By YE WANG A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

Dual n-back training increases the capacity of the focus of attention

Dual n-back training increases the capacity of the focus of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:135 141 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0335-6 BRIEF REPORT Dual n-back training increases the capacity of the focus of attention Lindsey Lilienthal & Elaine Tamez & Jill Talley Shelton

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

A Platoon-Level Model of Communication Flow and the Effects on Operator Performance

A Platoon-Level Model of Communication Flow and the Effects on Operator Performance A Platoon-Level Model of Communication Flow and te Effects on Operator Performance by Patricia W. Kilduff, Jennifer C. Swoboda, and Josua Katz ARL-MR-0656 November 2006 Approved for public release; distribution

More information

International Journal of Health Sciences and Research ISSN:

International Journal of Health Sciences and Research   ISSN: International Journal of ealt Sciences and Researc www.ijsr.org ISSN: 2249-9571 Review rticle ombay lood Group: Distinct Descent Samarpita Pramanik Senior Tutor, Scool of Nursing, Cristian ospital erampur,

More information

Effects of Adult Age on Structural and Operational Capacities in Working Memory

Effects of Adult Age on Structural and Operational Capacities in Working Memory Psychology and Aging 1991, Vol. 6, No. 1.118-127 Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, nc. 0882-7974/91/S.00 Effects of Adult Age on Structural and Operational Capacities in Working

More information

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Nart Bedin Atalay (natalay@selcuk.edu.tr) Selcuk University, Konya, TURKEY Mine Misirlisoy

More information

Yuka Kotozaki Cognitive Psychology, Graduate School of Information Sciences Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan

Yuka Kotozaki Cognitive Psychology, Graduate School of Information Sciences Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan Working Memory and Persistent Inhibitory Set: An Individual Differences Investigation Syoichi Iwasaki (siwasaki@cog.is.tohoku.ac.jp) Cognitive Psychology, Graduate School of Information Sciences Tohoku

More information

Derivation of Nutrient Prices from Household level Consumption Data: Methodology and Application*

Derivation of Nutrient Prices from Household level Consumption Data: Methodology and Application* Derivation of Nutrient Prices from Houseold level Consumption Data: Metodology and Application* by Dipankor Coondoo Economic Researc Unit, Indian Statistical Institute, Kolkata, India dcoondoo@isical.ac.in

More information

Three-dimensional simulation of lung nodules for paediatric multidetector array CT

Three-dimensional simulation of lung nodules for paediatric multidetector array CT Te Britis Journal of Radiology, 82 (2009), 401 411 Tree-dimensional simulation of lung nodules for paediatric multidetector array CT 1,2 X LI, MS, 1,2,3 E SAMEI, PD, 4 D M DELONG, PD, 5 R P JONES, BA,

More information

Journal of Theoretical Biology

Journal of Theoretical Biology Journal of Teoretical Biology 269 (2011) 131 137 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Teoretical Biology journal omepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yjtbi Life istory and mating systems select

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Working Memory Capacity and the Antisaccade Task: Individual Differences in Voluntary Saccade Control

Working Memory Capacity and the Antisaccade Task: Individual Differences in Voluntary Saccade Control Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2004, Vol. 30, No. 6, 1302 1321 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.6.1302

More information

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The spacing and lag effect in free recall The spacing and lag effect in free recall Michael J. Kahana, Bradley R. Wellington & Marc W. Howard Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology Brandeis University Send correspondence to: Michael

More information

Allergy: the unmet need

Allergy: the unmet need Allergy: te unmet need A blueprint for better patient care A report of te Royal College of Pysicians Working Party on te provision of allergy services in te UK Royal College of Pysicians June 2003 Royal

More information

Public Assessment Report. Scientific discussion. Carbidopa/Levodopa Bristol 10 mg/100 mg, 12.5 mg/50 mg, 25 mg/100 mg and 25 mg/250 mg tablets

Public Assessment Report. Scientific discussion. Carbidopa/Levodopa Bristol 10 mg/100 mg, 12.5 mg/50 mg, 25 mg/100 mg and 25 mg/250 mg tablets Public Assessment Report Scientific discussion Carbidopa/Levodopa Bristol 10 mg/100 mg, 12.5 mg/50 mg, 25 mg/100 mg and 25 mg/250 mg tablets (carbidopa/levodopa) NL/H/3044/001-004/DC Date: 25 February

More information

Automatic activation of attribute knowledge in heuristic inference from memory

Automatic activation of attribute knowledge in heuristic inference from memory Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:372 377 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0334-7 BRIEF REPORT Automatic activation of attribute knowledge in heuristic inference from memory Patrick H. Khader & Thorsten Pachur & Kerstin

More information

arxiv: v2 [cs.ro] 31 Jul 2018

arxiv: v2 [cs.ro] 31 Jul 2018 Design and Development of Effective Transmission Mecanisms on a Tendon Driven Hand Ortosis for Stroke Patients arxiv:182.6131v2 [cs.ro] 31 Jul 218 Sangwoo Park1, Lynne Weber2, Lauri Bisop2, Joel Stein2,3

More information

Summary. Introduction. Methods

Summary. Introduction. Methods Osteoartritis and Cartilage (2001) 9, 771 778 2001 OsteoArtritis Researc Society International 1063 4584/01/080771+08 $35.00/0 doi:10.1053/joca.2001.0474, available online at ttp://www.idealibrary.com

More information

ABSTRACT. The Mechanisms of Proactive Interference and Their Relationship with Working Memory. Yi Guo Glaser

ABSTRACT. The Mechanisms of Proactive Interference and Their Relationship with Working Memory. Yi Guo Glaser ABSTRACT The Mechanisms of Proactive Interference and Their Relationship with Working Memory by Yi Guo Glaser Working memory (WM) capacity the capacity to maintain and manipulate information in mind plays

More information

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

HCN channels enhance spike phase coherence and regulate the phase of spikes and LFPs in the theta-frequency range

HCN channels enhance spike phase coherence and regulate the phase of spikes and LFPs in the theta-frequency range HCN cannels enance spike pase coerence and regulate te pase of spikes and LFPs in te teta-frequency range Manisa Sina and Risikes Narayanan 1 Cellular Neuropysiology Laboratory, Molecular Biopysics Unit,

More information

THEORETICAL AND REVIEW ARTICLES. Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user s guide

THEORETICAL AND REVIEW ARTICLES. Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user s guide Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (5), 769-786 THEORETICAL AND REVIEW ARTICLES Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user s guide ANDREW R. A. CONWAY University of Illinois, Chicago,

More information

the risk of heart disease and stroke in alabama: burden document

the risk of heart disease and stroke in alabama: burden document te risk of eart disease and stroke in alabama: burden document finding te Pat to CardioVasCUlar ealt 21 table of Contents executive Summary... 1 demograpic caracteristics of Alabama... 2 Leading causes

More information

Citation Knight J, Andrade M (2018) Genes and chromosomes 4: common genetic conditions. Nursing Times [online]; 114: 10,

Citation Knight J, Andrade M (2018) Genes and chromosomes 4: common genetic conditions. Nursing Times [online]; 114: 10, Genetics Keywords Nondisjunction/Aneuploidy/ Trisomy/Autosomal/Recessive Tis article as been double-blind peer reviewed In tis article... Definitions of nondisjunction and aneuploidy Clinical features

More information

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Anna V. Fisher (fisher.449@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University

More information

Cuing Effects in Short-term Recall. Gerald Tehan. University of Southern Queensland. Michael S. Humphreys. University of Queensland

Cuing Effects in Short-term Recall. Gerald Tehan. University of Southern Queensland. Michael S. Humphreys. University of Queensland Cuing Effects in Short-term Recall Cuing in Short-term Recall 1 Gerald Tehan University of Southern Queensland Michael S. Humphreys University of Queensland Mailing Address: Gerry Tehan Faculty of Sciences

More information

ABSTRACT CAN IRRELEVANT ALTERNATIVES AFFECT PROBABILITY JUDGMENT? THE DISCRIMINATION BIAS. Professor Michael Dougherty Department of Psychology

ABSTRACT CAN IRRELEVANT ALTERNATIVES AFFECT PROBABILITY JUDGMENT? THE DISCRIMINATION BIAS. Professor Michael Dougherty Department of Psychology ABSTRACT Title of thesis: CAN IRRELEVANT ALTERNATIVES AFFECT PROBABILITY JUDGMENT? THE DISCRIMINATION BIAS Amber Sprenger, Master of Science, 2005 Thesis directed by: Professor Michael Dougherty Department

More information

Public Assessment Report. Scientific discussion. Ramipril Teva 1.25 mg, 2.5 mg, 5 mg and 10 mg tablets Ramipril DK/H/2130/ /DC.

Public Assessment Report. Scientific discussion. Ramipril Teva 1.25 mg, 2.5 mg, 5 mg and 10 mg tablets Ramipril DK/H/2130/ /DC. Public Assessment Report Scientific discussion Ramipril Teva 1.25 mg, 2.5 mg, 5 mg and 10 mg tablets Ramipril DK/H/2130/001-004/DC 3 April 2014 Tis module reflects te scientific discussion for te approval

More information

Working-Memory Capacity as a Unitary Attentional Construct. Michael J. Kane University of North Carolina at Greensboro

Working-Memory Capacity as a Unitary Attentional Construct. Michael J. Kane University of North Carolina at Greensboro Working-Memory Capacity as a Unitary Attentional Construct Michael J. Kane University of North Carolina at Greensboro Acknowledgments M. Kathryn Bleckley Andrew R. A. Conway Randall W. Engle David Z. Hambrick

More information

Spiral of Silence in Recommender Systems

Spiral of Silence in Recommender Systems Spiral of Silence in Recommender Systems Dugang Liu, Cen Lin Department of Computer Science, Xiamen University Xiamen, Cina ABSTRACT Yangua Xiao Scool of Computer Science, Fudan University Sangai, Cina

More information

Working Memory (Goal Maintenance and Interference Control) Edward E. Smith Columbia University

Working Memory (Goal Maintenance and Interference Control) Edward E. Smith Columbia University Working Memory (Goal Maintenance and Interference Control) Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Goal Maintenance Interference resolution: distraction, proactive interference, and directed forgetting

More information

Retrieval of Concepts in Script-Based Texts and Narratives: The Influence of General World Knowledge

Retrieval of Concepts in Script-Based Texts and Narratives: The Influence of General World Knowledge Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2002, Vol. 28, No. 4, 780 790 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.4.780

More information

INTRODUCTION METHODS

INTRODUCTION METHODS INTRODUCTION Deficits in working memory (WM) and attention have been associated with aphasia (Heuer & Hallowell, 2009; Hula & McNeil, 2008; Ivanova & Hallowell, 2011; Murray, 1999; Wright & Shisler, 2005).

More information

Two optimal treatments of HIV infection model

Two optimal treatments of HIV infection model SSN 746-733 England UK World Journal of Modelling and Simulation Vol. 8 () No. pp. 7-35 Two optimal treatments of HV infection model Kalid Hattaf Noura Yousfi Laboratory Analysis Modeling and Simulation

More information

Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z

Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 10, 522-527 (1971) Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z JAMES F. JUOLA AND R. C. ATKINSON Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305 Two groups

More information

Public Assessment Report. Scientific discussion. Orlyelle 0.02 mg/3 mg and 0.03 mg/3 mg film-coated tablets. (Ethinylestradiol/Drospirenone)

Public Assessment Report. Scientific discussion. Orlyelle 0.02 mg/3 mg and 0.03 mg/3 mg film-coated tablets. (Ethinylestradiol/Drospirenone) Public Assessment Report Scientific discussion Orlyelle 0.02 mg/3 mg and 0.03 mg/3 mg film-coated tablets (Etinylestradiol/Drospirenone) NL/H/2890/001-002/DC Date: 18 June 2014 Tis module reflects te scientific

More information

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2, No.3, 533 538 Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Ss were required to learn linear

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

Age differences in item manipulation span: The case of letter-number sequencing

Age differences in item manipulation span: The case of letter-number sequencing Emery, L., Myerson, J., & Hale, S. (2007). Age differences in item-manipulation span: The case of letter-number sequencing. Psychology and Aging, 22(1), 75-83. (Mar 2007) Published by the American Psychological

More information

Intelligence 38 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Intelligence

Intelligence 38 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Intelligence Intelligence 38 (2010) 255 267 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence Interference control, working memory capacity, and cognitive abilities: A latent variable analysis Nash Unsworth University

More information

Is the influence of working memory capacity on high-level cognition mediated by complexity or resource-dependent elementary processes?

Is the influence of working memory capacity on high-level cognition mediated by complexity or resource-dependent elementary processes? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (3), 528-534 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.3.528 Is the influence of working memory capacity on high-level cognition mediated by complexity or resource-dependent elementary

More information

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Psychon Bull Rev (2017) 24:1651 1657 DOI 10.3758/s13423-017-1226-7 BRIEF REPORT Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Sébastien De Schrijver 1 & Pierre Barrouillet 1 Published

More information

Automaticity of Number Perception

Automaticity of Number Perception Automaticity of Number Perception Jessica M. Choplin (jessica.choplin@vanderbilt.edu) Gordon D. Logan (gordon.logan@vanderbilt.edu) Vanderbilt University Psychology Department 111 21 st Avenue South Nashville,

More information

Public Assessment Report Scientific discussion. Kagitz (quetiapine) SE/H/1589/01, 04-05/DC

Public Assessment Report Scientific discussion. Kagitz (quetiapine) SE/H/1589/01, 04-05/DC Public Assessment Report Scientific discussion Kagitz (quetiapine) SE/H/1589/01, 0405/DC Tis module reflects te scientific discussion for te approval of Kagitz. Te procedure was finalised on 20161221.

More information

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Unsworth, Nash] On: 13 January 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 918847458] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Spontaneous Trait Inferences Are Bound to Actors Faces: Evidence From a False Recognition Paradigm

Spontaneous Trait Inferences Are Bound to Actors Faces: Evidence From a False Recognition Paradigm Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 83, No. 5, 1051 1065 0022-3514/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.5.1051 Spontaneous

More information

Cognition. Mid-term 1. Top topics for Mid Term 1. Heads up! Mid-term exam next week

Cognition. Mid-term 1. Top topics for Mid Term 1. Heads up! Mid-term exam next week Cognition Prof. Mike Dillinger Mid-term 1 Heads up! Mid-term exam next week Chapters 2, 3, of the textbook Perception, Attention, Short-term memory The lectures are to help you digest the chapters; exams

More information

A Mathematical Model for Assessing the Control of and Eradication strategies for Malaria in a Community ABDULLAHI MOHAMMED BABA

A Mathematical Model for Assessing the Control of and Eradication strategies for Malaria in a Community ABDULLAHI MOHAMMED BABA ttp://www.sciencepub.net/report A Matematical Model for Assessing te Control of and Eradication strategies for Malaria in a Community ABDULLAHI MOHAMMED BABA DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS, NIGER STATE COLLEGE

More information

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES Journal of Experimental Vol. 51, No. 4, 1956 Psychology CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES KENNETH H. KURTZ AND CARL I. HOVLAND Under conditions where several concepts are learned concurrently

More information

Modeling H1N1 Vaccination Rates. N. Ganesh, Kennon R. Copeland, Nicholas D. Davis, National Opinion Research Center at the University of Chicago

Modeling H1N1 Vaccination Rates. N. Ganesh, Kennon R. Copeland, Nicholas D. Davis, National Opinion Research Center at the University of Chicago Modeling H1N1 Vaccination Rates N. Ganes, Kennon R. Copeland, Nicolas D. Davis, National Opinion Researc Center at e University of Cicago James A. Singleton, Tammy Santibanez, Centers for Disease Control

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

Differently confident: Susceptibility to bias in perceptual judgments of size interacts with working memory capacity

Differently confident: Susceptibility to bias in perceptual judgments of size interacts with working memory capacity DOI 10.3758/s13414-016-1071-1 Differently confident: Susceptibility to bias in perceptual judgments of size interacts with working memory capacity Christopher A. Sanchez 1 # The Psychonomic Society, Inc.

More information

A LEGACY OF SERVICE, LOVE AND SOCIAL JUSTICE

A LEGACY OF SERVICE, LOVE AND SOCIAL JUSTICE MQp2K#2`S:.lzS4 r?biib2`bi`22i?2hi?+2mi2`- #mbh/bm;/2/b+ibqmmkbm;+2`2kqmv 7`2/2`B+KYrBHHBKb#mBH/BM; SlOzi`2KQMibi`22i `Qt#m`v.KzlSlz I3

More information

Cognitive control, automaticity, and working memory: A dual-process analysis. Karen A. Daniels. Washington University in St. Louis

Cognitive control, automaticity, and working memory: A dual-process analysis. Karen A. Daniels. Washington University in St. Louis Running title: Cognitive control and working memory. Cognitive control, automaticity, and working memory: A dual-process analysis Karen A. Daniels Washington University in St. Louis Please do not quote

More information

The role of interference in memory span

The role of interference in memory span Memory & Cognition 1999, 27 (5), 759-767 The role of interference in memory span CYNTHIA P. MAY University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona LYNN HASHER Duke University, Durham, North Carolina and MICHAEL J.

More information

2/27/2017. Modal Model of Memory. Executive Attention & Working Memory. Some Questions to Consider (over the next few weeks)

2/27/2017. Modal Model of Memory. Executive Attention & Working Memory. Some Questions to Consider (over the next few weeks) Executive Attention & Working Memory Memory 1 Some Questions to Consider (over the next few weeks) Why can we remember a telephone number long enough to place a call, but then we forget it almost immediately?

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

DISSOCIATION BETWEEN DECLARATIVE AND PROCEDURAL MECHANISMS IN LONG-TERM MEMORY

DISSOCIATION BETWEEN DECLARATIVE AND PROCEDURAL MECHANISMS IN LONG-TERM MEMORY DISSOCIATION BETWEEN DECLARATIVE AND PROCEDURAL MECHANISMS IN LONG-TERM MEMORY A dissertation submitted to the Kent State University College of Education, Health, and Human Services in partial fulfillment

More information

Developmental Evidence for Working Memory as Activated Long-Term Memory

Developmental Evidence for Working Memory as Activated Long-Term Memory Commentary on: Ruchkin D.S., Grafman J., Cameron K., Berndt R.S. (2003). Working Memory Retention Systems: A State of Activated Long-Term Memory. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, vol. 26, p. 250. Developmental

More information

Illusory Correlation and Group Impression Formation in Young and Older Adults

Illusory Correlation and Group Impression Formation in Young and Older Adults Journal of Gerontology: PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES 2000, Vol. 55B, No. 4, P224 P237 Copyright 2000 by The Gerontological Society of America Illusory Correlation and Group Impression Formation in Young and

More information

Unexpected costs of high working memory capacity following directed forgetting and contextual change manipulations

Unexpected costs of high working memory capacity following directed forgetting and contextual change manipulations Unexpected costs of high working memory capacity following directed forgetting and contextual change manipulations By: PETER F. DELANEY AND LILI SAHAKYAN Delaney, P. F., & Sahakyan, L. (2007). Unexpected

More information

Searching for serial refreshing in working memory: Using response times to track the content of the focus of attention over time For Review Only

Searching for serial refreshing in working memory: Using response times to track the content of the focus of attention over time For Review Only Searching for serial refreshing in working memory: Using response times to track the content of the focus of attention over time Journal: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Manuscript ID PBR-BR--.R Manuscript

More information

Comparison of four scoring methods for the reading span test

Comparison of four scoring methods for the reading span test Behavior Research Methods 2005, 37 (4), 581-590 Comparison of four scoring methods for the reading span test NAOMI P. FRIEDMAN and AKIRA MIYAKE University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado This study compared

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Separating Habit and Recollection: Memory Slips, Process Dissociations, and Probability Matching

Separating Habit and Recollection: Memory Slips, Process Dissociations, and Probability Matching Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1996, Vol. 22, No. 6,1323-1335 Copyright 19% by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/96/$3.00 Separating Habit and

More information