Comparison of patients with ParkinsonÕs disease or cerebellar lesions in the production of periodic movements involving event-based or emergent timing

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Comparison of patients with ParkinsonÕs disease or cerebellar lesions in the production of periodic movements involving event-based or emergent timing"

Transcription

1 Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) Comparison of patients with ParkinsonÕs disease or cerebellar lesions in the production of periodic movements involving event-based or emergent timing Rebecca M.C. Spencer *, Richard B. Ivry Department of Psychology and Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California at Berkeley, 3210 Tolman Hall #1650, Berkeley, CA 94720, United States Accepted 16 September 2004 Available online 18 November 2004 Abstract We have hypothesized a distinction between the processes required to control the timing of different classes of periodic movements. In one class, salient events mark successive cycles. For these movements, we hypothesize that the temporal goal is a requisite component of the task representation, what we refer to as event-based timing. In the other class, the successive cycles are produced continuously. For these movements, alternative control strategies can optimize performance, allowing timing to be emergent. In a previous study, patients with cerebellar lesions were found to be selectively impaired on event-based timing tasks; they were unimpaired on a continuously produced task. In the present study, patients with ParkinsonÕs disease were tested on repetitive movement tasks in which timing was either event-based or emergent. Temporal variability on either type of task did not differ between on- and off-medication sessions for the ParkinsonÕs patients nor did patient performance differ from that of controls. These results suggest that the basal ganglia play a minimal role in movement timing and that impairments on event-based timing tasks are specific to cerebellar damage. Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: ParkinsonÕs disease; Basal ganglia; Timing; Cerebellum; Rhythmic movements 1. Introduction On a daily basis, we are capable of tracking time; we can do this explicitly by watching a clock or we can track time less directly. For instance, you could be aware that it is 9 a.m. by referencing the clock on the wall, or because you have brushed your teeth, showered, walked the dog, and biked to work. On a more minute time scale we are also capable of tracking time. While driving, you must release the clutch at a precise time relative to stepping on the gas. Likewise, for accuracy when throwing a ball, you must open the fist at a precise time within the swing of the arm (Timmann, Watts, & Hore, 2000). * Corresponding author. Fax: address: rspencer@socrates.berkeley.edu (R.M.C. Spencer). Recent studies have revealed that timing in the millisecond range can also be explicit or indirect. In the former case, timing occurs through the operation of an internal clock-like process in which the temporal goal is explicitly represented. By explicit, we do not mean conscious; to avoid confusion with the manner in which this term is used in studies of memory, we will refer to this process as event-based timing. For other tasks, timing is indirect; in these tasks timing is hypothesized to be emergent, resulting from the operation of non-temporal parameters such as muscle stiffness. The distinction between event-based and emergent timing was motivated by a series of studies by Zelaznik and colleagues (Robertson et al., 1999; Zelaznik, Spencer, & Doffin, 2000). They compared the performance of healthy young adults on two tasks that have been /$ - see front matter Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi: /j.bandc

2 R.M.C. Spencer, R.B. Ivry / Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) widely used to study motor timing, finger tapping and continuous circle drawing. Previous correlation studies had suggested a common timing process was engaged across a range of rhythmic, motor tasks (Franz, Zelaznik, & Smith, 1992; Keele & Hawkins, 1982; Keele, Pokorny, Corcos, & Ivry, 1985). Based on these results, significant correlations were expected between temporal variability on the continuous circle drawing task and the tapping task. However, timing variability of these two types of movements did not correlate. A key distinction between the continuous drawing task and the finger tapping task is that optimal performance on the circle drawing task requires the movements to be made in a smooth, continuous manner. In contrast, finger tapping is best viewed as the concatenation of a series of distinct cycles: Not only does the tapping surface define each cycle, but people tend to impose a slight pause prior to each downstroke. To assess the importance of the continuous/discontinuous distinction, Zelaznik, Spencer, and Ivry (2002) introduced a hybrid circle drawing task. In this task, subjects were instructed to pause between each circling cycle. In spite of the similarity between this task and continuous circle drawing, temporal variability on the intermittent circle drawing task correlated with tapping and neither task correlated with continuous circle drawing. We hypothesized that for the discontinuous movements an explicit event-based representation of the temporal goal is required, specifying the timing of critical events such as the onset of each cycle. In contrast, for continuous movements, timing is emergent. People are, of course, able to match the rate of continuous movements to an externally-defined temporal goal (i.e., movement rate), but this can be achieved by varying a non-temporal control parameter such as joint stiffness (see Ivry, Spencer, Zelaznik, & Diedrichsen, 2003). These individual difference studies pointed towards a distinction between tasks in which timing was either event-based or emergent. Further support was obtained in a series of studies involving patients with acquired cerebellar lesions. The patients exhibited increased temporal variability on the finger tapping and intermittent circle drawing tasks. However, they performed comparable to control participants on the continuous circle drawing task (Spencer, Zelaznik, Diedrichsen, & Ivry, 2003). The deficits on the event-based timing tasks are consistent with neuropsychological (e.g., Ackermann et al., 2001; Ivry, Keele, & Deiner, 1988) and neuroimaging studies (e.g., Jueptner et al., 1995; Kawashima et al., 2000; Penhune, Zatorre, & Evans, 1998) indicating that the cerebellum is essential for the precise representation of temporal information. The lack of an impairment on the continuous circle drawing task, despite the added complexity of this task in comparison to tapping, suggests that the cerebellum is not essential for tasks in which timing is emergent. The basal ganglia have also been associated with temporal processing. Manipulation of dopamine levels in rats and humans has been shown to alter the rate of perceived time, an effect attributed to the slowing or speeding of an internal clock (e.g., Meck, 1983, 1996, 2003; Meck & Benson, 2002). Neuroimaging studies have also associated basal ganglia activity during timing tasks with temporal processing requirements (Coull, Vidal, Nazarian, & Macar, 2004; Harrington, Haaland, & Hermanowitz, 1998a; Hinton & Meck, 2004; Nenadic et al., 2003; Rao et al., 1997). The results from patient studies have been less consistent. While some studies have reported that patients with ParkinsonÕs disease (PD) exhibit similar deficits as those observed in patients with cerebellar lesions (CE) (Harrington, Haaland, & Knight, 1998b; OÕBoyle, Freeman, & Cody, 1996; Pastor, Jahanshahi, Artieda, & Obeso, 1992), other studies have found that the PD patients perform similar to neurologically healthy controls (Duchek, Balota, & Ferraro, 1994; Ivry & Keele, 1989). In the current study, we tested PD patients on the tapping, intermittent circle drawing, and continuous circle drawing tasks. The study provides another assessment of the performance of PD patients on motor timing tests, adding to the empirical base for what has turned out to be a problematic issue. Of greatest interest, the study provides a test of whether the eventbased/emergent timing distinction can be dissociated neuropsychologically. It may be that deficits on the tapping and intermittent circle drawing tasks are observed in various patient groups with motor impairments. That is, these two tasks may be more sensitive to the effects of neuropathology than the continuous drawing task. On the other hand, finding that the PD patients were unimpaired on the tapping and intermittent drawing tasks would provide new support for the hypothesis that the cerebellum is specialized for event-based timing. Two other results are possible. First, the PD patients might be selectively impaired on the continuous circle drawing task, a result that would constitute a double dissociation when combined with the CE patient findings. Second, PD patients may be impaired on all three tasks. 2. Method 2.1. Participants Eight patients with a diagnosis of idiopathic ParkinsonÕs disease and eight age- and education-matched controls volunteered in exchange for payment. Patient volunteers were given a neurological exam to assess motor function as well as a series of neuropsychological tests to evaluate cognitive status. Those exhibiting signs of dementia or marked cognitive impairment were excluded. Neuropsychological and demographic

3 86 R.M.C. Spencer, R.B. Ivry / Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) Table 1 Descriptive variables across tasks PD patients Controls On-meds Off-meds D ND D ND D ND Finger Tapping (22.9) (20.2) (11.6) (14.0) (15.7) (10.3) Intermittent Circles Total (48.2) (58.7) (37.3) (95.0) (37.7) (41.9) Movement (30.2) (33.8) (42.2) (77.7) (29.3) (32.3) Pause (33.0) (39.2) (38.1) (35.2) (29.0) (36.7) Continuous Circles (21.4) (19.9) (17.7) (69.8) (21.0) (44.4) Note. Standard errors (across participants) appear in parentheses. (D, dominant limb; ND, non-dominant limb). data are presented in Table 1. The PD patients participated in two sessions. During the first session, the patients were instructed to maintain their normal medication regimen ( On session). For the second session, the patients skipped their morning medication, allowing the testing to take place at least 12 h after their last medication ( Off session). At least 1 week elapsed between the two sessions. Control participants participated in a single experiment session. These participants reported and exhibited no history of neurological damage in a neurological battery performed prior to testing. This work was approved by the University of California, Berkeley Committee on the Usage of Human Research Participants. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior to testing Procedure Participants were seated in front of a 36-in. high table. Kinematic data were collected with an Ascension Technology minibird magnetic tracking system (136 Hz sampling rate) controlled by a computer which also generated the pacing tones. One mm kinematic marker was attached to the tip of the index finger. A synchronization-continuation paradigm was used for all three tasks. During the synchronization phase a 20-ms tone was played every 550 ms. The tones ceased during the continuation phase. A template mounted on the table served as a spatial goal for the circle drawing tasks. The template consisted of two circles. One circle was 6 cm in diameter and the subject was instructed to trace this pattern; the other circle was 1 cm in diameter and intersected the larger circle at the point farthest from the subject. For the continuous circle drawing task, the subject was instructed to complete one cycle for every beat of the metronome, moving continuously from cycle to cycle. The finger tip was to pass through the smaller circle coincident with the metronome beat. For the intermittent circle task, the subject was instructed to trace a complete circle during one 550-ms interval and then pause on the smaller circle for the subsequent 550-ms interval. For the finger tapping task, the subject rested his or her forearm on the table. With the index finger fully extended, the subject was instructed to tap the table coincident with each beat of the metronome. For the tapping task no instructions were given regarding movement continuity. Based on previous studies, we expected that there would be a significant pause between the successive taps. A trial began when the experimenter triggered the metronome. The subjects were instructed to begin moving in synchrony with the metronome. After 10 paced cycles (5.5 s), the metronome was disengaged and data were collected for an additional 11 s, an interval in which approximately 20 movement cycles could be completed. The end of a trial was signaled by a series of four tones. Five test trials were collected for each hand for each of the three timed movement conditions. Prior to the test trials, three practice trials were completed. In addition to the neurological exam, maximal movement rate was measured to assess motor competence. In both sessions, maximal tapping rate was measured by having the participant tap on the table surface as fast as possible for 10 s. After some of the participants had been tested, it was apparent that the patients had performed as well as the controls on this task. As a further test, the maximum rate test was also performed with arm movements by six of the control participants, two of the PD patients in the On session, and five of the PD patients in the Off session. In this condition, participants made arm movements between two targets that were spaced 15 cm apart. The instructions emphasized speed over accuracy. As with the tapping task, data were collected for 10 s. In each session, four trials were collected for each hand on the maximal rate tasks.

4 R.M.C. Spencer, R.B. Ivry / Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) Each of the 10 conditions, five tasks by two limbs, was tested in separate trial blocks with the order of the blocks randomized across participants. Patients performed the blocks in the same order in the On and Off sessions. At the start of each block, the experimenter provided instructions and demonstrated the task for that block. Each session lasted approximately 1 h Data Analysis Only the data from the continuation phase of the test trials were analyzed. For finger tapping, the beginning of each cycle was defined as the point when the finger touched the table. For both drawing tasks, the beginning of a cycle was defined as the maximum displacement in the y-dimension, the point at which the marker was farthest from the subject. The beginning of the pause portion of each intermittent circle cycle was defined as the onset of the plateau in the y-dimension. Thus, we were able to partition the intermittent circle cycle into pause and movement time. 3. Results Trajectories of patient and control performance are presented in Fig. 1. Similar to trajectories produced by unimpaired adults when tapping, the PD patients exhibited a pause between tapping cycles. Furthermore, although not at the instructed ratio (discussed below), there were distinct pauses inserted between cycles in the intermittent circle drawing task. No pause is evident in the trajectories produced during continuous circle drawing. Thus, as expected, the movements of the PD patients are similar to the controls in that tapping and intermittent drawing entail discontinuities, a prerequisite for event-based timing Cycle duration Mean cycle durations for the timed movement conditions are presented in Table 1. Although, as may be expected, PD patients moved slower than controls on average, this was not a significant difference (F(1,95) =.36, p =.55) nor were timed movements slower in the Off session relative to the On session (F(1,95) = 1.76, p =.19) Table 1 also lists the movement and pause durations for the intermittent circle drawing task. Overall, the patients and controls were able to approximate the target cycle duration. Note that the goal duration for a complete cycle for the intermittent circle drawing task was 1100 ms (550 ms movement ms pause). While the overall duration was close to this goal, all of the partic- Fig. 1. Kinematic traces as produced by (A) a PD patient in the On session, (B) the same PD patient in the Off session, and (C) a control.

5 88 R.M.C. Spencer, R.B. Ivry / Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) ipants distorted the target 1:1 movement:pause ratio. The patients performed approximately 3:1 while in the On session and 4:1 in the Off session. Control participants performed approximately 2.5: Cycle variability Of primary interest was the variability of the cycle durations. A normalized measure, the coefficient of variability (CoV), defined as the standard deviation of cycle duration divided by mean cycle duration, was used as illustrated in Fig. 2. The CoV is useful for comparing temporal consistency on tasks with different cycle durations since, within the range studied here, the standard deviation is linearly related to cycle duration (see Robertson et al., 1999). The CoV also accounts for differences in cycle duration between individuals within each task Cycle variability: ParkinsonÕs patientsõ on- versus off-performance Our first analysis was the comparison of variability within-subjects, the performance of the patients tested On and Off medication. Given our interest in seeing if PD produces similar deficits as observed in CE patients, we focused on the two event-based timing tasks, finger tapping and intermittent drawing. A t-test of the combined variability from the intermittent circle drawing and finger tapping tasks was conducted, comparing performance for the On and Off sessions. There was no difference in performance on these tasks between sessions (t(7) =.8518, p =.79). Likewise, a t-test comparing performance on the continuous task between the On and Off sessions was also not significant (t(7) = 1.30, p =.88). Previous On Off comparisons of variability on repetitive movement tasks have only used the tapping task. In the current data set, no difference was found on this task when evaluated alone (dominant limb t(7) =.23, p =.41; non-dominant limb t(7) = 1.11, p =.85). This null result is in accord with the previous report of Ivry and Keele (1989), but at odds with that of OÕBoyle et al. (1996), who reported increased variability in patients when off medication Cycle variability: ParkinsonÕs patientsõ performance versus controls Next, we compared temporal variability of the PD patients to controls. While our medication manipulation failed to produce an increase in temporal variability, it is possible that the patients are more variable than appropriately matched control participants. Based on the post hoc observation that the patients were actually more variable when On medication, we decided to use the data from this session in the comparisons with the controls. Note that because all of the PD patients were tested in the On state first, the two groups have comparable experience with the tasks for these data sets. No group difference was found on the event-based timing tasks (pooled comparison of tapping and intermittent circle drawing, t(7) = 1.05, p =.16). We also looked at the tapping data separately given previous research with this task. Although the patientsõ means were higher than that observed for the controls with both Fig. 2. The average within-subject variability, the coefficient of variation, for all tasks, groups, and sessions. Error bars represent the standard error (the variability in the mean coefficient of variation values across subjects). On is performance of PD patients in the On session; Off is performance of PD patients in the Off session.

6 R.M.C. Spencer, R.B. Ivry / Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) hands, neither effect was significant (dominant limb t(7) = 1.13, p =.15; non-dominant limb t(7) = 1.40, p =.10). Surprisingly, patients tended to be more variable than the controls on the continuous drawing task (t(7) = 1.70, p =.07), an effect that neared significance Relationship of ParkinsonÕs symptoms to temporal performance Maximal tapping rate was measured to assess motor competence of the patients in the On and Off medication sessions. Surprisingly, performance on this task was not mediated by medication state (F(1,31) =.45, p =.51). Furthermore, the maximal tapping rate for PD patients was greater than that produced by controls (F(1,31) = 5.13, p =.03). This effect is illustrated in Fig. 3A. In spite of this, there is evidence that the PD patients in this study exhibited Parkinsonian symptoms. First, the neurological assessments (see Table 2) provide evidence that the patients exhibited mild to moderate impairments. It is regrettable that, due to the limited time of testing in the Off session, the neurological assessments were not performed during the Off session. However, results from the maximal rate reaching task are reassuring of additive deficits in the Off session. When tested on the maximal rate arm movement task, PD patients in the Off session were slower than controls (F(1,21) = 8.92, p <.01; as illustrated in Fig. 3B). Additional evidence that the patients exhibited Parkinsonian symptoms is evident in the top row of Fig. 1. In the finger tapping task, the task performed without a spatial template, the PD patients produced movements Table 2 Demographic data for the participants. PD Patients Control Subjects Age (yrs) 65.4 (SD = 8.2) 65.0 (SD = 7.8) Education (yrs) (SD = 2.5) (SD = 2.5) Gender (male/female) 5/3 4/4 Years post diagnosis 9.4 (SD = 5.6) Hoehn and Yahr Scale a Stage 1 = 2 Stage 2 = 2 Stage 2.5 = 1 Stage 3 = 2 Stage 3 = 1 Schwab and England Scale b 95% = 2 90% = 1 80 % = 2 70 % = 2 65% = 1 UPDRS c 29.8 SD = 19.2 a Hoehn and Yahr (1967). b Schwab and England (1997). c Fahn et al. (1987). of smaller amplitude than controls. This difference was significant (t(7) = 4.48, p =.001; as illustrated in Fig. 4). While the comparison of temporal variability produced by PD patients relative to controls did not reveal a timing deficit associated with PD, disease severity across the patients may predict the variability observed in patient performance. To examine this, PearsonÕs correlations (one-tailed) were calculated for severity ratings on the UPDRS relative to timing performance. Disease severity did not predict performance on the repetitive tapping task (r =.41; r >.56 indicates significance a p <.05) nor the intermittent drawing task (r =.18). However, the UPDRS did predict temporal performance on the continuous drawing task (r =.72). Fig. 3. The range of maximal rates produced across groups on the (A) maximal rate tapping task and (B) the maximal rate arm movement task (note that data was not collected on all participants for this task). Thin lines indicate the median within that group/limb; thick lines represent the mean maximal rate.

7 90 R.M.C. Spencer, R.B. Ivry / Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) event-based tasks, finger tapping and intermittent circle drawing. This null finding was obtained in both the On and Off medication sessions, although as evidenced by the maximum tapping assay, the effects of skipping a medication cycle were minimal, at least for finger movements. For the emergent timing task, continuous circle drawing, the PD patients exhibited a marginally significant increase in variability compared to the controls. As with the event-based tasks, performance did not differ between the On and Off sessions Comparing the effects of ParkinsonÕs disease and cerebellar lesions Fig. 4. Average tap amplitude for the timed tapping task. 4. Discussion In the present study, we examined the performance of PD patients on repetitive movement tasks in which the timing has been hypothesized to be either event-based or emergent. The results clearly indicate that for this group of patients, no deficits were observed on the These results stand in contrast to those observed previously in a similar study involving patients with lesions of the cerebellum. As depicted in Fig. 5, cerebellar patients were impaired on the event-based timing tasks (Ivry et al., 2003; Spencer et al., 2003). For patients with unilateral cerebellar lesions, increased temporal variability was observed during finger tapping and intermittent circle drawing when performed with the ipsilesional hand compared to the contralesional hand. In a second study, patients with bilateral cerebellar degeneration were impaired with either hand in comparison to matched controls on tapping and intermittent circle drawing tasks. Critically, in both the unilateral and bilateral cerebellar patients, performance was unimpaired on the continuous circle drawing task. When considered in conjunction with the current results, two important conclusions can be made. First, the results fail to support the hypothesis that the Fig. 5. Comparison of results across patient groups and across studies. CE patients had unilateral damage. Thus the solid lighter-gray bars are for the ipsilesional impaired limb; hatched darker-gray bars are for the contralesional unimpaired limb.

8 R.M.C. Spencer, R.B. Ivry / Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) event-based timing tasks are more sensitive to the effects of neurological damage than the emergent timing task. The PD patients were not impaired on the event-based timing tasks, finger tapping and intermittent drawing. Second, the results are consistent with the hypothesis that event-based timing deficits are selective to cerebellar damage, at least for patient groups tested on these two classes of movements. Indeed, the results suggest a possible double dissociation given that the PD patients were marginally impaired on the continuous circle drawing task. This effect requires further study Are ParkinsonÕs patients impaired on tasks requiring precise timing? While cerebellar patients consistently exhibit increased temporal variability on event-based timing tasks, the picture for PD patients remains unclear. Some studies have reported normal performance on the repetitive tapping task (Duchek et al., 1994) even when the patients are tested off-medication (Ivry & Keele, 1989). The current results are in accord with these studies. However, other studies have reported that PD patients are more variable than controls (Harrington et al., 1998a, 1998b; OÕBoyle et al., 1996). There are various reasons for this ambiguous set of results, none of which provide a satisfactory explanation. One possible explanation is that our patients may exhibit less severe deficits relative to those reported by Harrington et al. (1998a, 1998b), OÕBoyle et al. (1996) and their respective colleagues. The PD patients in the current study tapped as fast, if not faster than the control participants. Consistent with this, the patients showed little clinical impairment on the UPDRS measures of distal involvement; seven of the PD participants were rated at 0, normal performance, and the other patient was rated at 1, minimal impairment. However, it should be noted that our patients fall within the same range on the UPDRS (mean = 29.8, SD = 19.2) as the patients studied by Harrington et al. (mean = 29.4, SD = 13.6). Furthermore, the reduction in tap amplitude in the PD patients is indicative of a spatial deficit of distal movements in spite of the lack of temporal deficit. This latter note is consistent with research by Margolin and Wing (1983) which suggests that micrographia, reduced writing size, is associated with reduced amplitude or force rather than reduced movement time. Moreover, the PD patients in the current study did exhibit clinical signs of the disease for more proximal effectors. Six scored 1 or higher on the UPDRS measures of hand and wrist movements (opening and closing of the hand; rapid alternation of the wrist) and the patients were slower than the controls on the maximal arm movement assay. We also observed a significant correlation between disease severity and variability on the continuous circle drawing task and a near significant correlation with variability on the intermittent circle drawing task. These results suggest that, even if our patients exhibited little problem with distal movements, evidence of their Parkinsonism was evident with more proximal movements. Nonetheless, they were not impaired on the intermittent circle drawing task, a task that required movement about the elbow and shoulder. In conclusion, the presence or absence of PD symptoms does not appear to account for whether or not the patients show a timing impairment Event and emergent timing Three sets of results support the distinction between event-based and emergent timing. First, the correlational studies with neurologically healthy individuals show that a common process underlies temporal acuity on tapping and intermittent circle drawing which is separable from that associated with continuous circle drawing. Individual differences on the two event-based timing tasks are also correlated with the ability to judge the duration of brief events (Zelaznik et al., 2002). Second, in a study of bimanual coordination, split-brain patients exhibit a striking difference in the coordination between the two classes of movements. The two hands remain temporally coupled during tapping (Ivry & Hazeltine, 1999; Tuller & Kelso, 1989) and intermittent finger movements (Kennerley, Diedrichsen, Hazeltine, Semjen, & Ivry, 2002). In contrast, this coupling is absent during continuous circle drawing (Kennerley et al., 2002). Third, patients with cerebellar lesions are selectively impaired on event-based timing tasks. Taken together, these results indicate that the event-based/emergent timing distinction is not only psychologically valid, but that these two classes of movements engage distinct neural systems. The difference between event-based and emergent timing can also be seen in a micro-analysis of the temporal structure of tapping and continuous circle drawing (Spencer & Zelaznik, 2003). For the tapping task, cycle duration was computed based on the point of table contact, the point of flexion-onset, the point of maximum velocity, and the point of minimum velocity. The variability in cycle time differed greatly depending on the point at which the cycle duration was measured. For circle drawing, when cycle duration was computed based on the reversal point in the y-dimension, the reversal point in the x-dimension, the point of maximum velocity, and the point of minimum velocity, there was no difference in temporal variability with respect to where the cycle duration was measured. This analysis suggests that timing is an emergent property during such movements, reflecting the continuous variation in a control parameter such as joint stiffness. In contrast, salient events are timed during tapping. The Spencer and Zelaznik study suggests that this salient point is the contact point with

9 92 R.M.C. Spencer, R.B. Ivry / Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) the table surface, consistent with the task instruction to touch the table coincident with the beat in the paced portion of the trial Conclusions In sum, the cerebellum has a clear role in the temporal control of event-based timing and contributes minimally, if at all, to the temporal control of emergent timing. Based on the present results, the basal ganglia appear to contribute minimally to the temporal control of event-based timing (see also, Aparicio and Ivry, this volume). The present results suggest a possible role for the basal ganglia in emergent timing. How timing emerges in continuous movements and the underlying subcortical and cortical neural correlates remains an important problem for future investigation. References Ackermann, H., Riecker, A., Mathiak, K., Erb, M., Grodd, W., & Wildgruber, D. (2001). Rate-dependent activation of prefrontalinsular-cerebellar network during passive listening to trains of click stimuli: An fmri study. Neuroreport, 12, Coull, J. T., Vidal, F., Nazarian, B., & Macar, F. (2004). Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Science, 303, Duchek, J. M., Balota, D. A., & Ferraro, F. R. (1994). Component analysis of a rhythmic finger tapping task in individuals with senile dementia of the AlzheimerÕs type and in individuals with ParkinsonÕs disease. Neuropsychology, 8, Fahn, S. et al. (1987). The Unified ParkinsonÕs Disease Rating Scale. In S. Fahn, C. D. Marsden, D. B. Calne, & M. Goldstein (Eds.), Recent developments in ParkinsonÕs disease (pp ). Florham Park, NJ: Macmillan Health Care Information. Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N., & Smith, A. (1992). Evidence of common timing processes in the control of manual, orofacial, and speech movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 24, Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y., & Hermanowitz, N. (1998a). Temporal processing in the basal ganglia. Neuropsychology, 12, Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y., & Knight, R. T. (1998b). Cortical networks underlying mechanisms of time perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, Hinton, S. C., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Frontal-striatal circuitry activated by human peak-interval timing in the supra-seconds range. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, Hoehn, M. M., & Yahr, M. D. (1967). Parkinsonism: Onset, progression, and mortality. Neurology, 17, Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, E. (1999). Subcoritcal locus of temporal coupling in the bimanual movements of a callosotomy patient. Human Movement Science, 18, Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, Ivry, R. B., Keele, S. W., & Deiner, H. C. (1988). Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution. Experimental Brain Research, 73, Ivry, R. B., Spencer, R. M. C., Zelaznik, H. N., & Diedrichsen, J. (2003). The cerebellum and event timing. In S. M. Highstein & W. T. Thach (Eds.). The Cerebellum: Recent developments in cerebellar research, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 978, pp ). New York: New York Academy of Sciences. Jueptner, M., Rijntjes, M., Weiller, C., Faiss, J. H., Timmann, D., Mueller, S. P., et al. (1995). Localization of a cerebellar timing process using PET. Neurology, 45, Kawashima, R. et al. (2000). Human cerebellum plays an important role in memory-timed finger movement: An fmri study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 83, Keele, S. W., & Hawkins, H. L. (1982). Explorations of individual differences relevant to high level skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, Keele, S. W., Pokorny, R. A., Corcos, D. M., & Ivry, R. B. (1985). Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, Kennerley, S. W., Diedrichsen, J., Hazeltine, E., Semjen, A., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Callosotomy patients exhibit temporal and spatial uncoupling during continuous bimanual movements. Nature Neuroscience, 5, Margolin, D. I., & Wing, A. M. (1983). Agraphia and micrographia: Clinical manifestations of motor programming and performance disorders. Acta Psychologica, 54, Meck, W. H. (1983). Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory storage processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, Meck, W. H. (1996). Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, Meck, W. H. (2003). Introduction: Persistence of time. In W. H. Meck (Ed.), Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. (pp. xvii xli). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Meck, W. H., & Benson, A. M. (2002). Dissecting the brainõs internal clock: How frontal-striatal circuitry keeps time and shifts attention. Brain and Cognition, 48, Nenadic, I., Gaser, C., Volz, H.-P., Rammsayer, T., Hager, F., & Sauer, H. (2003). Processing of temporal information and the basal ganglia: New evidence from fmri. Experimental Brain Research, 148, OÕBoyle, D. J., Freeman, D. J., & Cody, F. W. (1996). The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with ParkinsonÕs disease. Brain, 119, Pastor, M. A., Jahanshahi, M., Artieda, J., & Obeso, J. A. (1992). Performance of repetitive wrist movements in ParkinsonÕs disease. Brain, 115, Penhune, V. B., Zatorre, R. J., & Evans, A. C. (1998). Cerebellar contributions to motor timing: A PET study of auditory and visual rhythm reproduction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, Timmann, D., Watts, S., & Hore, J. (2000). Causes of left-right ball inaccuracy in overarm throws made by cerebellar patients. Experimental Brain Research, 130, Rao, S. M., Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y., Bobholz, J. A., Cox, R. W., & Binder, J. R. (1997). Distributed neural systems underlying the timing of movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, Robertson, S. D., Zelaznik, H. N., Lantero, D., Bojczyk, K. G., Spencer, R. M., Doffin, J. G., et al. (1999). Correlations for timing consistency among tapping and drawing tasks: Evidence against a single timing process for motor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, Schwab, R. S., & England, A. C. (1997). Schwab and England activities of daily living scale (1969). In R. M. Herndon (Ed.), Handbook of clinical neurologic scales (pp ). New York: Dernos Vermande. Spencer, R. M. C., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2003). Weber (slope) analyses of timing variability in tapping and drawing tasks. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, Spencer, R. M. C., Zelaznik, H. N., Diedrichsen, J., & Ivry, R. B. (2003). Disrupted timing of discontinuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science, 300,

10 R.M.C. Spencer, R.B. Ivry / Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) Tuller, B., & Kelso, J. A. (1989). Environmentally-specified patterns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 75, Zelaznik, H. N., Spencer, R. M., & Doffin, J. (2000). Temporal precision in tapping and circle drawing movements at preferred rates is not correlated: Further evidence against timing as a general purpose ability. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, Zelaznik, H. N., Spencer, R. M., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Evidence in support of the dissociation of timing processes in tapping and drawing movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28,

Timing Variability in Circle Drawing and Tapping: Probing the Relationship Between Event and Emergent Timing

Timing Variability in Circle Drawing and Tapping: Probing the Relationship Between Event and Emergent Timing Journal of Motor Behavior, 2005, Vol. 37, No. 5, 395 403 Timing Variability in Circle Drawing and Tapping: Probing the Relationship Between Event and Emergent Timing Howard N. Zelaznik Department of Health

More information

Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements

Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements HOWARD N. ZELAZNIK 1, REBECCA M. SPENCER 1, AND RICHARD B. IVRY 2 1 Department of Health, Kinesiology and Leisure

More information

Role of the cerebellum in movements: control of timing or movement transitions?

Role of the cerebellum in movements: control of timing or movement transitions? Exp Brain Res (2005) 161: 383 396 DOI 10.1007/s00221-004-2088-6 RESEARCH ARTICLE Rebecca M. C. Spencer. Richard B. Ivry. Howard N. Zelaznik Role of the cerebellum in movements: control of timing or movement

More information

Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception

Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception Sundeep Teki Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception Auditory Cognition Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging SENSORY CORTEX SMA/PRE-SMA HIPPOCAMPUS BASAL

More information

On the nature of Rhythm, Time & Memory. Sundeep Teki Auditory Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London

On the nature of Rhythm, Time & Memory. Sundeep Teki Auditory Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London On the nature of Rhythm, Time & Memory Sundeep Teki Auditory Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London Timing substrates Timing mechanisms Rhythm and Timing Unified timing

More information

Effects of focal basal ganglia lesions on timing and force control

Effects of focal basal ganglia lesions on timing and force control Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) 62 74 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Effects of focal basal ganglia lesions on timing and force control Paul Aparicio, Joern Diedrichsen, Richard B. Ivry * Department of Psychology,

More information

The distinction between tapping and circle drawing with and without tactile feedback: An examination of the sources of timing variance

The distinction between tapping and circle drawing with and without tactile feedback: An examination of the sources of timing variance THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2012, ifirst, 1 15 The distinction between tapping and circle drawing with and without tactile : An examination of the sources of timing variance Breanna

More information

Cortical and subcortical contributions to the representation of temporal information

Cortical and subcortical contributions to the representation of temporal information Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 1461 1473 Cortical and subcortical contributions to the representation of temporal information Todd C. Handy a,, Michael S. Gazzaniga a, Richard B. Ivry b a Department of Psychological

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article MOVING TO DIRECTLY CUED LOCATIONS ABOLISHES SPATIAL INTERFERENCE DURING BIMANUAL ACTIONS Jörn Diedrichsen, 1 Eliot Hazeltine, 2 Steven Kennerley, 1 and Richard B. Ivry 1 1 University of

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[cdl Journals Account] [CDL Journals Account] On: 29 May 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 770849124] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

Different Brain Circuits Underlie Motor and Perceptual Representations of Temporal Intervals

Different Brain Circuits Underlie Motor and Perceptual Representations of Temporal Intervals Different Brain Circuits Underlie Motor and Perceptual Representations of Temporal Intervals Domenica Bueti, Vincent Walsh, Chris Frith, and Geraint Rees Abstract & In everyday life, temporal information

More information

Cerebellum and Timing

Cerebellum and Timing Cerebellum and Timing 52 Rebecca M. C. Spencer and Richard B. Ivry Abstract A functional characterization of the cerebellum centers on the hypothesis that this structure is essential for the representation

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

The Role of the Basal Ganglia in Beat Perception

The Role of the Basal Ganglia in Beat Perception THE NEUROSCIENCES AND MUSIC III DISORDERS AND PLASTICITY The Role of the Basal Ganglia in Beat Perception Neuroimaging and Neuropsychological Investigations Jessica A. Grahn MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences

More information

Intermanual interactions during initiation and production of rhythmic and discrete movements in individuals lacking a corpus callosum

Intermanual interactions during initiation and production of rhythmic and discrete movements in individuals lacking a corpus callosum DOI 10.1007/s00221-006-0640-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Intermanual interactions during initiation and production of rhythmic and discrete movements in individuals lacking a corpus callosum Dagmar Sternad Æ Kunlin

More information

Form B3L: UPDRS Part III Motor Examination 1

Form B3L: UPDRS Part III Motor Examination 1 Initial Visit Packet NACC Uniform Data Set (UDS) LBD MODULE Form B3L: UPDRS Part III Motor Examination 1 ADC name: Subject ID: Form date: / / Visit #: Examiner s initials: INSTRUCTIONS: This form is to

More information

Rhythm Categorization in Context. Edward W. Large

Rhythm Categorization in Context. Edward W. Large Rhythm Categorization in Context Edward W. Large Center for Complex Systems Florida Atlantic University 777 Glades Road, P.O. Box 39 Boca Raton, FL 3343-99 USA large@walt.ccs.fau.edu Keywords: Rhythm,

More information

The Coupled Oscillator Model of Between-Hand Coordination in Alternate-Hand Tapping: A Reappraisal

The Coupled Oscillator Model of Between-Hand Coordination in Alternate-Hand Tapping: A Reappraisal Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 200, Vol. 27, No. 2, 25-265 Copyright 200 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-523/0/$5.0O DOI: 0.037//0096-523.27.2.25

More information

Peripheral facial paralysis (right side). The patient is asked to close her eyes and to retract their mouth (From Heimer) Hemiplegia of the left side. Note the characteristic position of the arm with

More information

Two different processes for sensorimotor synchronization in continuous and discontinuous rhythmic movements

Two different processes for sensorimotor synchronization in continuous and discontinuous rhythmic movements Exp Brain Res (2009) 199:157 166 DOI 10.1007/s00221-009-1991-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Two different processes for sensorimotor synchronization in continuous and discontinuous rhythmic movements Kjerstin Torre

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

Subcortical locus of temporal coupling in the bimanual movements of a callosotomy patient

Subcortical locus of temporal coupling in the bimanual movements of a callosotomy patient Human Movement Science 18 (1999) 345±375 www.elsevier.com/locate/humov Subcortical locus of temporal coupling in the bimanual movements of a callosotomy patient Richard B. Ivry *, Eliot Hazeltine Department

More information

The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson's disease

The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson's disease Brain (1996), 119,51-70 The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson's disease Donald J. O'Boyle, 1 John S. Freeman 2 and Frederick W. J. Cody 2 1

More information

The Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on the Kinematics of Reach-to-Pinch Function

The Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on the Kinematics of Reach-to-Pinch Function The Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on the Kinematics of Reach-to-Pinch Function Raviraj Nataraj, Peter J. Evans, MD, PhD, William H. Seitz, MD, Zong-Ming Li. Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH, USA. Disclosures:

More information

Planning ahead through space and time: from neuropsychology to motor control

Planning ahead through space and time: from neuropsychology to motor control Planning ahead through space and time: from neuropsychology to motor control Mariama C.Dione (mariama.dione@univ-lille3.fr) Laurent Ott (laurent.ott@univ-lille3.fr) Yvonne Delevoye-Turrell (yvonne.delevoye@univ-lille3.fr)

More information

Continuous and Discontinuous Drawing: High Temporal Variability Exists Only in Discontinuous Circling in Young Children

Continuous and Discontinuous Drawing: High Temporal Variability Exists Only in Discontinuous Circling in Young Children Journal of Motor Behavior, 2008, Vol. 40, No. 5, 391 399 Copyright 2008 Heldref Publications Continuous and Discontinuous Drawing: High Temporal Variability Exists Only in Discontinuous Circling in Young

More information

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2002, Vol. 28, No. 3, 445 457 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.445

More information

ID # COMPLETED: YES.. 1 DATE NO... 5 NEUROLOGICAL EXAM

ID # COMPLETED: YES.. 1 DATE NO... 5 NEUROLOGICAL EXAM ID # COMPLETED: YES.. 1 DATE NO... 5 NEUROLOGICAL EXAM VIDEOTAPED: YES.. 1 NO... 5 COMMENT: NEUROLOGICAL EXAM "Normal, Abnormal, Other, Can't execute or Missing for each question. Always complete specify

More information

Psychological Research Springer-Verlag 1989

Psychological Research Springer-Verlag 1989 Psychol Res (1989) 50:251-256 Psychological Research Springer-Verlag 1989 Mechanisms of perceptual timing: Beat-based or interval-based judgements? Steven W. Keele, Roberto Nicoletti, Richard I. lvry,

More information

Time perception: Manipulation of task difficulty dissociates clock functions from other cognitive demands

Time perception: Manipulation of task difficulty dissociates clock functions from other cognitive demands Neuropsychologia xxx (2006) xxx xxx Time perception: Manipulation of task difficulty dissociates clock functions from other cognitive demands Alexandra C. Livesey, Matthew B. Wall, Andrew T. Smith Department

More information

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Laboratory Equipment & Test Procedures. Motor learning and control historically has built its discipline through laboratory research. This has led to the

More information

Keeping time: Effects of focal frontal lesions

Keeping time: Effects of focal frontal lesions Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 1195 1209 Keeping time: Effects of focal frontal lesions Terence W. Picton a,b,, Donald T. Stuss a,b, Tim Shallice a,c,d, Michael P. Alexander a,e,f, Susan Gillingham a a The

More information

Research Article Parkinsonian Rigidity Shows Variable Properties Depending on the Elbow Joint Angle

Research Article Parkinsonian Rigidity Shows Variable Properties Depending on the Elbow Joint Angle Parkinson s Disease Volume, Article ID 587, 5 pages http://dx.doi.org/.55//587 Research Article Parkinsonian Rigidity Shows Variable Properties Depending on the Elbow Joint Angle Takuyuki Endo, Toshimitsu

More information

Measuring sensorimotor synchronization abilities

Measuring sensorimotor synchronization abilities TIMELY School on «Timing and Time Perception: Procedures, Measures, & Applications» Corfu, February 4-8, 2013 Measuring sensorimotor synchronization abilities Simone Dalla Bella EuroMov, M2H Laboratory,

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS Simon Grondin, Vincent Laflamme, Nicolas Bisson, Emi Hasuo, Tsuyoshi Kuroda Université Laval, Québec, Canada simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca Abstract

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Benefits of Weight bearing increased awareness of the involved side decreased fear improved symmetry regulation of muscle tone

Benefits of Weight bearing increased awareness of the involved side decreased fear improved symmetry regulation of muscle tone From the information we have gathered during our Evaluation, the Clinical Reasoning we used to identify key problem areas and the Goals Established with functional outcomes we now have enough information

More information

Voluntary Timing and Brain Function: An Information Processing Approach

Voluntary Timing and Brain Function: An Information Processing Approach Brain and Cognition 48, 7 30 (2002) doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1301, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Voluntary Timing and Brain Function: An Information Processing Approach Alan M. Wing Behavioural

More information

The neural representation of time Richard B Ivry and Rebecca MC Spencer

The neural representation of time Richard B Ivry and Rebecca MC Spencer The neural representation of time Richard B Ivry and Rebecca MC Spencer This review summarizes recent investigations of temporal processing. We focus on motor and perceptual tasks in which crucial events

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback

Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback Exp Brain Res (2006) 168: 307 311 DOI 10.1007/s00221-005-0280-y RESEARCH NOTE Daniel J. Goble Æ Colleen A. Lewis Æ Susan H. Brown Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback Received:

More information

Processing of temporal information and the basal ganglia: new evidence from fmri

Processing of temporal information and the basal ganglia: new evidence from fmri Exp Brain Res (2003) 148:238 246 DOI 10.1007/s00221-002-1188-4 RESEARCH ARTICLE Igor Nenadic Christian Gaser Hans-Peter Volz Thomas Rammsayer Frank Häger Heinrich Sauer Processing of temporal information

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) FOCUSED QUESTION For stroke patients, in what ways does robot-assisted therapy improve upper extremity performance in the areas of motor impairment, muscle power, and strength?

More information

Multidimensional sequence learning in patients with focal basal ganglia lesions

Multidimensional sequence learning in patients with focal basal ganglia lesions Brain and Cognition 58 (2005) 75 83 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Multidimensional sequence learning in patients with focal basal ganglia lesions Jacqueline C. Shin a, *, Paul Aparicio b, Richard B. Ivry

More information

Home Exercise Program Progression and Components of the LTP Intervention. HEP Activities at Every Session Vital signs monitoring

Home Exercise Program Progression and Components of the LTP Intervention. HEP Activities at Every Session Vital signs monitoring Home Exercise Program Progression and Components of the LTP Intervention HEP Activities at Every Session Vital signs monitoring Blood pressure, heart rate, Borg Rate of Perceived Exertion (RPE) and oxygen

More information

EVALUATION AND MEASUREMENTS. I. Devreux

EVALUATION AND MEASUREMENTS. I. Devreux EVALUATION AND MEASUREMENTS I. Devreux To determine the extent and degree of muscular weakness resulting from disease, injury or disuse. The records obtained from these tests provide a base for planning

More information

Spatial deficits in ideomotor limb apraxia A kinematic analysis of aiming movements

Spatial deficits in ideomotor limb apraxia A kinematic analysis of aiming movements Spatial deficits in ideomotor limb apraxia A kinematic analysis of aiming movements Kathleen Y. Haaland, 1 Deborah L. Harrington 1 and Robert T. Knight 2,3 Brain (1999), 122, 1169 1182 1 Psychology and

More information

Strick Lecture 1 March 22, 2006 Page 1

Strick Lecture 1 March 22, 2006 Page 1 Strick Lecture 1 March 22, 2006 Page 1 Motor Planning and Programming The point of this lecture is to reveal important features about the operation of our motor system by studying motor behavior. Figures

More information

Full Body (medicine ball) Saggital Front Reach

Full Body (medicine ball) Saggital Front Reach Saggital Front Reach Saggital Front Reach 1) Start position: Stand with feet hip width apart. Hold medicine ball or dumbbell at waist. 2) Step forward 2-3 feet with the heel striking first and lean torso

More information

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE Isabelle Guay and Simon Grondin Université Laval, Québec, Canada Email: simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca ABSTRACT

More information

Prism adaptation in Parkinson's disease

Prism adaptation in Parkinson's disease Reprinted from Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1988;51: 1584-1587 Copyright 1988 Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry All rights of reproduction of this reprint are reserved

More information

THE EFFECTS OF INTERACTIVE METRONOME TRAINING ON FEMALE SOCCER PLAYERS TIMING ABILITY

THE EFFECTS OF INTERACTIVE METRONOME TRAINING ON FEMALE SOCCER PLAYERS TIMING ABILITY Umeå University Department of Psychology Master thesis, Spring 2012 THE EFFECTS OF INTERACTIVE METRONOME TRAINING ON FEMALE SOCCER PLAYERS TIMING ABILITY Ronja Frimalm Supervisor: Louise Rönnqvist, professor

More information

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Exp Brain Res (1998) 123:346±350 Springer-Verlag 1998 RESEARCH NOTE Jinsung Wang George E. Stelmach Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Received: 1 April

More information

MECHANISMS OF TIMING ACROSS TASKS AND TEMPORAL INTERVALS. Ashley S. Bangert

MECHANISMS OF TIMING ACROSS TASKS AND TEMPORAL INTERVALS. Ashley S. Bangert MECHANISMS OF TIMING ACROSS TASKS AND TEMPORAL INTERVALS by Ashley S. Bangert A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (Psychology) in

More information

Clinical Learning Exercise #1

Clinical Learning Exercise #1 Clinical Learning Exercise #1 Exercise: We are going to assume nothing is wrong with the peripheral nervous system and attempt to identify the central nervous system anatomical location for the following

More information

Two Types of TMS-Induced Movement Variability After Stimulation of the Primary Motor Cortex

Two Types of TMS-Induced Movement Variability After Stimulation of the Primary Motor Cortex Two Types of TMS-Induced Movement Variability After Stimulation of the Primary Motor Cortex Timothy Verstynen, Talia Konkle and Richard B. Ivry J Neurophysiol 96:1018-1029, 2006. First published 3 May

More information

Cerebellar Structures and the Programming of Movement Sequences

Cerebellar Structures and the Programming of Movement Sequences Behavioural Neurology, 1990,3,87-97 Cerebellar Structures and the Programming of Movement Sequences ALBRECHT WERNER INHOFF 1 and ROBERT RAFAU 1 State University of New York at Binghamton, USA 2 University

More information

Time perception, cognitive correlates, age and emotions

Time perception, cognitive correlates, age and emotions Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 187 ( 2015 ) 695 699 PSIWORLD 2014 Time perception, cognitive correlates, age and emotions Cristian Vasile*

More information

14 - Central Nervous System. The Brain Taft College Human Physiology

14 - Central Nervous System. The Brain Taft College Human Physiology 14 - Central Nervous System The Brain Taft College Human Physiology Development of the Brain The brain begins as a simple tube, a neural tube. The tube or chamber (ventricle) is filled with cerebrospinal

More information

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen Narrowing wide-field optic flow affects treadmill gait in left-sided Parkinson's disease van der Hoorn, Anouk; Hof, At L.; Leenders, Klaus; de Jong, Bauke M. Published in: Movement

More information

Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns

Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns Perception & Psychophysics 1986, 40 (2), 69-73 Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns PETER J. ESSENS TNO Institute for Perception, Soesterberg, The Netherlands In two reproduction experiments,

More information

Parkinson s Disease in the Elderly A Physicians perspective. Dr John Coyle

Parkinson s Disease in the Elderly A Physicians perspective. Dr John Coyle Parkinson s Disease in the Elderly A Physicians perspective Dr John Coyle Overview Introduction Epidemiology and aetiology Pathogenesis Diagnosis and clinical features Treatment Psychological issues/ non

More information

Janet M. Duchek, David A. Balota, and F. Richard Ferraro

Janet M. Duchek, David A. Balota, and F. Richard Ferraro Neuropsychology 1994, Vol. 8. No. 2, 218-226 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0894-4105/94/S3.00 Component Analysis of a Rhythmic Finger Tapping Task in Individuals With Senile

More information

COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT IN PARKINSON S DISEASE

COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT IN PARKINSON S DISEASE 1 GENERAL INTRODUCTION GENERAL INTRODUCTION PARKINSON S DISEASE Parkinson s disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative movement disorder, named after James Parkinson who described some of its characteristic

More information

Neurophysiology of systems

Neurophysiology of systems Neurophysiology of systems Motor cortex (voluntary movements) Dana Cohen, Room 410, tel: 7138 danacoh@gmail.com Voluntary movements vs. reflexes Same stimulus yields a different movement depending on context

More information

Long-Term Effects of Thalamic Deep Brain Stimulation on Force Control in a Patient with Parkinson s Disease-Driven Action Tremor

Long-Term Effects of Thalamic Deep Brain Stimulation on Force Control in a Patient with Parkinson s Disease-Driven Action Tremor Long-Term Effects of Thalamic Deep Brain Stimulation on Force Control in a Patient with Parkinson s Disease-Driven Action Tremor Karen L. Francis, PhD* Waneen W. Spirduso, EdD Tim Eakin, PhD Pamela Z.

More information

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical Short Communication TheScientificWorldJOURNAL (2006) 6, 1805 1809 ISSN 1537-744X; DOI 10.1100/tsw.2006.291 Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical V. Frak 1,2, *, D. Bourbonnais 2, I. Croteau

More information

ID # COMPLETED: YES 1 DATE NO 2

ID # COMPLETED: YES 1 DATE NO 2 ID # COMPLETED: YES 1 DATE NO 2 NEUROLOGICAL EXAM "Normal, Abnormal, Other, Can't execute or Missing for each question. If you circle "Abnormal" or Other, also check the appropriate reason why or explain

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 3, Number 1 Submitted: August 10, 2007 Revisions: August 20, 2007 Accepted: August 27, 2007 Publication Date: September 10, 2007 The Effect of Cell Phone

More information

Cerebellar Involvement in Clumsiness and Other Developmental Disorders

Cerebellar Involvement in Clumsiness and Other Developmental Disorders NEURAL PLASTICITY VOLUME 10, NO. 1 2, 2003 Cerebellar Involvement in Clumsiness and Other Developmental Disorders Richard B. Ivry Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, California,

More information

1) Drop off in the Bi 150 box outside Baxter 331 or to the head TA (jcolas).

1) Drop off in the Bi 150 box outside Baxter 331 or  to the head TA (jcolas). Bi/CNS/NB 150 Problem Set 5 Due: Tuesday, Nov. 24, at 4:30 pm Instructions: 1) Drop off in the Bi 150 box outside Baxter 331 or e-mail to the head TA (jcolas). 2) Submit with this cover page. 3) Use a

More information

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly Auditory Temporal Processes 1 Ben-Artzi, E., Babkoff, H., Fostick, L. (2011). Auditory temporal processes in the elderly. Audiology Research, 1, 21-23 ***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully

More information

Test Administration Instructions for the Fullerton Advanced Balance (FAB) Scale 10

Test Administration Instructions for the Fullerton Advanced Balance (FAB) Scale 10 The Interactive Health Partner Wellness Program addresses fall prevention with assessments, outcomes tracking in an easy to use, comprehensive online system. Website: www.interactivehealthpartner.com Email:

More information

Timing Impairments and Neural Dysfunction in Basal Ganglia Disorders

Timing Impairments and Neural Dysfunction in Basal Ganglia Disorders Timing Impairments and Neural Dysfunction in Basal Ganglia Disorders Deborah L. Harrington University of California, San Diego Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System Co-Investigators University of

More information

Bimanual Interference Associated With the Selection of Target Locations

Bimanual Interference Associated With the Selection of Target Locations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 1, 64 77 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.64

More information

Designing a scientific poster

Designing a scientific poster Designing a scientific poster Similar to a platform (PowerPoint) presentation - fonts, tables/graphic, color scheme - lots of visuals, low word-count Differences - Personal interaction (therefore shower,

More information

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview Investigations in Resting State Connectivity Scott FMRI Laboratory Overview Introduction Functional connectivity explorations Dynamic change (motor fatigue) Neurological change (Asperger s Disorder, depression)

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

Perception and Production of Brief Durations: Beat-Based Versus Interval-Based Timing

Perception and Production of Brief Durations: Beat-Based Versus Interval-Based Timing Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2001, Vol. 27, No. 2, 485-493 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/01/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.2.485

More information

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Asymmetry in learning in the reverse direction Full recovery from UP using DOWN: initial return to naïve values within 10 minutes,

More information

Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy

Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy Eliot Hazeltine 1, Andrea Weinstein 2, and Richard B. Ivry 2 Abstract & Two studies [Ivry, R. B., Franz, E. A., Kingstone, A., & Johnston, J. C. The psychological

More information

Therapy Manual DO NOT PRINT

Therapy Manual DO NOT PRINT Therapy Manual Contents 1. Shoulder 2. Shoulder and elbow a. Protraction: 1 DoF 1 b. Flexion: 1 DoF 1-6 c. Extension: 1 DoF 1-2 d. Abduction: 1 DoF 1-4 e. External rotation: 1 DoF 1-14 a. Combined shoulder

More information

Cerebellar Involvement in Anticipating the Consequences of Self-Produced Actions During Bimanual Movements

Cerebellar Involvement in Anticipating the Consequences of Self-Produced Actions During Bimanual Movements Cerebellar Involvement in Anticipating the Consequences of Self-Produced Actions During Bimanual Movements Jörn Diedrichsen, Timothy Verstynen, Steven L. Lehman and Richard B. Ivry J Neurophysiol 93:801-812,

More information

Original Articles. Calne, resting tremor. Mortimer, Pirozzolo, Hansch, & Webster, postural disturbance III

Original Articles. Calne, resting tremor. Mortimer, Pirozzolo, Hansch, & Webster, postural disturbance III 2004 97-106 Original Articles 1 2 3 1 1 2 3 47 22 III I II muscular rigidity postural disturbance resting tremor bradykinesia Calne, 2001 Mortimer, Pirozzolo, Hansch, & Webster, 1982 Tel: 02-23627076 E-mail:

More information

Recognizing one s own clapping: The role of temporal cues

Recognizing one s own clapping: The role of temporal cues Psychological Research (2004) 69: 147 156 DOI 10.1007/s00426-003-0165-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Ru diger Flach Æ Gu nther Knoblich Æ Wolfgang Prinz Recognizing one s own clapping: The role of temporal cues Received:

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Brain Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 24.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Brain Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 24. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Brain Res. 2011 October 24; 1419: 19 33. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.08.039. TIMING VARIABILITY OF REACH TRAJECTORIES IN LEFT VERSUS

More information

UPPER BODY STANDING 12. March in place (hand to opposite knee) For more intensity raise arms above head if your balance is GOOD. 13.

UPPER BODY STANDING 12. March in place (hand to opposite knee) For more intensity raise arms above head if your balance is GOOD. 13. LOW IMPACT EXERCISES SITTING 1. Breathe 2. Half circles with head 3. Neck movements (Chin to chest, ear to shoulder) 4. Neck Stretch Sitting in your chair, reach down and grab the side of the chair with

More information

A Multimodal Paradigm for Investigating the Perisaccadic Temporal Inversion Effect in Vision

A Multimodal Paradigm for Investigating the Perisaccadic Temporal Inversion Effect in Vision A Multimodal Paradigm for Investigating the Perisaccadic Temporal Inversion Effect in Vision Leo G. Trottier (leo@cogsci.ucsd.edu) Virginia R. de Sa (desa@cogsci.ucsd.edu) Department of Cognitive Science,

More information

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Hearing-aids Induce Plasticity in the Auditory System: Perspectives From Three Research Designs and Personal Speculations About the

More information

Time Perception: Beyond Simple Interval Estimation

Time Perception: Beyond Simple Interval Estimation Time Perception: Beyond Simple Interval Estimation Niels Taatgen 12 (taatgen@cmu.edu), Hedderik van Rijn 12 (hedderik@ai.rug.nl) and John Anderson 1 (ja@cmu.edu) 1 Carnegie Mellon University, Department

More information

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific?

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Temporal Binding and Internal Clocks: Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Richard Fereday (FeredayR@cardiff.ac.uk), Marc J. Buehner (BuehnerM@cardiff.ac.uk) Cardiff University, School of Psychology,

More information

Overview: An Image of Human Neural Timing

Overview: An Image of Human Neural Timing 1109_frame_MASTER.book Page 515 Wednesday, October 23, 2002 10:01 AM 21 Overview: An Image of Human Neural Timing Penelope A. Lewis and R. Chris Miall CONTENTS 21.1 Introduction...515 21.2 Are There Multiple

More information

What goes wrong with balance in Parkinson s Disease? Fay B Horak, PhD, PT Professor of Neurology Oregon Health and Science. CoM

What goes wrong with balance in Parkinson s Disease? Fay B Horak, PhD, PT Professor of Neurology Oregon Health and Science. CoM What goes wrong with balance in Parkinson s Disease? Fay B Horak, PhD, PT Professor of Neurology Oregon Health and Science CoM CoM Course Objectives Understand different types of balance systems affected

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

Clinical Study A New Approach for the Quantitative Evaluation of the Clock Drawing Test: Preliminary Results on Subjects with Parkinson s Disease

Clinical Study A New Approach for the Quantitative Evaluation of the Clock Drawing Test: Preliminary Results on Subjects with Parkinson s Disease Neurology Research International Volume 2010, Article ID 283890, 6 pages doi:10.1155/2010/283890 Clinical Study A New Approach for the Quantitative Evaluation of the Clock Drawing Test: Preliminary Results

More information

Visuomotor Learning Generalizes Between Bilateral and Unilateral Conditions Despite Varying Degrees of Bilateral Interference

Visuomotor Learning Generalizes Between Bilateral and Unilateral Conditions Despite Varying Degrees of Bilateral Interference Visuomotor Learning Generalizes Between Bilateral and Unilateral Conditions Despite Varying Degrees of Bilateral Interference Jinsung Wang, J. Toby Mordkoff and Robert L. Sainburg J Neurophysiol 104:2913-2921,

More information

Two types of phase error correction mechanisms in synchronization tapping

Two types of phase error correction mechanisms in synchronization tapping Two types of phase error correction mechanisms in synchronization tapping Kouji Takano 1 Yoshihiro Miyake 1 1 Dept. of Computational Intelligence and Systems Science, Tokyo Institute of Technology, 4259

More information