AN IMAGING STUDY OF WORKING MEMORY: THE EFFECTS OF CONCURRENT ARTICULATION ON PHONOLOGICAL TASKS AND THE CEREBELLUM S ROLE. David A.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "AN IMAGING STUDY OF WORKING MEMORY: THE EFFECTS OF CONCURRENT ARTICULATION ON PHONOLOGICAL TASKS AND THE CEREBELLUM S ROLE. David A."

Transcription

1 AN IMAGING STUDY OF WORKING MEMORY: THE EFFECTS OF CONCURRENT ARTICULATION ON PHONOLOGICAL TASKS AND THE CEREBELLUM S ROLE by David A. Raboy BPhil, University of Pittsburgh, 2010 Submitted to the Faculty of University Honors College and the School of Arts and Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Philosophy University of Pittsburgh 2010

2 UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH University Honors College This thesis was presented by David A. Raboy It was defended on April 21, 2010 and approved by Natasha Tokowicz, Professor, Department of Psychology Mark Wheeler, Professor, Department of Psychology Lori Holt, Professor, Department of Psychology Thesis Advisor: Julie Fiez, Professor, Department of Psychology ii

3 Copyright by David A. Raboy 2010 iii

4 AN IMAGING STUDY OF WORKING MEMORY: THE EFFECTS OF CONCURRENT ARTICULATION ON PHONOLOGICAL TASKS AND THE CEREBELLUM S ROLE David A. Raboy, BPhil University of Pittsburgh, 2010 Recent work has shown that cerebellar patients have difficulty with particular types of rhyme judgments (pairs with phonological and orthographic mismatch), as well as working memory tasks (Ben-Yehudah and Fiez, 2008). Both working memory and rhyme judgment tasks can be disrupted by concurrent articulation (Gathercole and Baddeley, 1994; Besner 1987), indicating a possible set of common regions in the cerebellum. In a review investigating the effects of concurrent articulation on phonological judgment tasks, Besner (1987) found differential effects for rhyme judgments compared to homophone and non-word homophone judgments. Interestingly, there may be differences even within rhyme judgments where word pairs with mixed phonological and orthographic (visual) similarity are more affected by concurrent articulation (Johnston and McDermott, 1986). We performed a behavioral experiment (Experiment 1) to replicate the effects found by both Besner and Johnston and McDermott. Our behavioral experiment found similar results. Concurrent articulation decreased the accuracy of rhyme judgments but not homophone and non-word homophone judgments; within the rhyme judgment task, word pairs with mixed phonology and orthography were the most affected. In order to elucidate the potential role of the cerebellum in these tasks, we designed a neuroimaging experiment (Experiment 2) with both a working memory component and a phonological tasks component. We identified a set of ten regions that were positively active during the working iv

5 memory task and used this set of regions to explore potential regions of overlap between working memory and rhyme judgment, but not homophone and non-word homophone judgments. Of the ten regions active for the phonological tasks, only one bilateral region in the superior cerebellum showed a significant task effect. Counter to what we had hypothesized, it showed greater activation for the homophone and non-word homophone judgments than the rhyme judgments. While our results show a separation of the tasks in this bilateral region, further study is necessary to help explain why we saw lower activation for rhyming in this region and why we were unable to identify any rhyme specific areas within the cerebellum. v

6 TABLE OF CONTENTS PREFACE... X 1.0 INTRODUCTION EXPERIMENT 1 BEHAVIORAL EXPERIMENT METHODS Participants Stimuli Procedure RESULTS Reaction Time Accuracy DISCUSSION EXPERIMENT 2 NEUROIMAGING EXPERIMENT METHODS Participants Stimuli Procedure fmri Data Acquisition fmri Data Pre-processing vi

7 3.1.6 fmri Localizer Regions of Interest Analysis RESULTS Behavioral Results Imaging Results DISCUSSION GENERAL DISCUSSION APPENDIX. STIMULI BIBLIOGRAPHY vii

8 LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Reaction Time±SE for Rhyme Component Table 2. Reaction Time±SE for Multi-Task Component Table 3. Percent Error±SE for Rhyme Component Table 4. Percent Error±SE for Multi-Task Component Table 5. ROIs with Negative Activation Table 6. ROIs with Positive Activation Table 7. Reaction Time±SE for Multi-Task Component in Experiment Table 8. Percent Error±SE for Multi-Task Component in Experiment viii

9 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Format of Presentation Screens for Experiment Figure 2. Block design for Experiment Figure 3. Format of Presentation Screens for Experiment Figure 4. Block Design for Multi Task Component in Experiment Figure 5. Block Design for Working Memory Component in Experiment Figure Regions from Working Memory Component with Positive Activation Figure 7. Percent Signal Change by Task in ROI Figure 8. Percent Signal Change by Task for ROI ix

10 PREFACE I owe sincere gratitude to Corrine Durisko who committed much time and effort in guiding me through all the steps leading up to this thesis. Heartfelt thanks are also due to Dr. Julie Fiez who gave me the opportunity to call this project my own and whose intellectual pursuits motivated and helped to shape this work. x

11 1.0 INTRODUCTION The cerebellum has long been known for its important role in motor control. Besides its role in movement, more recent studies have begun to elucidate a role for the cerebellum in what are considered to be higher order cognitive functions, such as language. (Stoodley and Schmahmann, 2009; Stoodley and Schmahmann, 2008; Strick, Dum, and Fiez, 2009; Schmahmann, 1996). These studies have found many connections between the cerebellum and cognition, revealing a more complicated role for the cerebellum than what was long thought. A study by Ben-Yehudah and Fiez (2008) compared cerebellum lesion patients and controls on reading tasks to determine the relationship between cerebellar function and reading. Patients did not differ from controls on basic reading skills such as word identification, comprehension, and fluency. However, the patients were significantly worse at deciding if a pair of words with mismatched orthography and phonology rhymed (ex. thigh and fly, fear and bear). Patients with anterior and superior cerebellar lobular damage had severe difficulty on this task whereas patients with damage in the inferior lobules performed normally. In verbal working memory tasks, patients displayed memory spans equivalent to controls on words and digits recalled in the order of presentation but had difficulty with recalling digits in reverse order and non-words. The authors propose that verbal working memory and rhyme judgment are both impaired in patients because they rely on a common articulatory monitoring process in the anterior and superior lobules of the cerebellum. 11

12 In examining behavioral studies to find a common articulatory process for rhyming and verbal working memory, the work with concurrent articulation in working memory emerged as a possible explanation. The seminal model of working memory proposed by Alan Baddeley provides one theoretical perspective. At its core, the model posits three key components necessary for maintaining and manipulating information in working memory: the visuo-spatial sketchpad that serves as a temporary store with limited capacity for visual and spatial features of a stimuli, the phonological loop that executes a similar function for verbal information, and a control system known as the central executive that is a flexible coordinator of the maintenance systems, responsible for shifting attention and binding all of the information into a working representation (Baddeley, 2003). The phonological loop is further divided into two parts: a short term store (the phonological store) which holds phonological information of the to-beremembered items, and an articulatory rehearsal mechanism that acts to prevent the decay of verbal information in the phonological store by keeping it active through a repetitive, silent inner speech process (Gathercole and Baddeley, 1994). In other words, through an inner speech like mechanism, the phonological loop functions to maintain stimuli in working memory. In order to demonstrate the validity of his model, Baddeley used concurrent articulation, the repetitive utterance of irrelevant material such as counting digits or repeatedly saying the same word. Having participants concurrently articulate while trying to remember a short list of words decreased performance. Baddeley attributed this finding to the fact that concurrent articulation occupies the needed articulatory rehearsal mechanism, thus preventing items in the store from being refreshed (Gathercole and Baddeley, 1994). In a series of experiments, Baddeley and Lewis (1981) applied the concepts they developed in the domain of verbal working memory to the study of reading. Using a variety of 12

13 non-word stimuli to prevent subjects from using any existing lexical information, the authors did not find an expected impairment in reading performance when participants engaged in concurrent articulation. This result is difficult to reconcile with Baddeley s model of working memory which posits that all phonological tasks should be impaired by concurrent articulation since articulation should prevent stimuli from reaching the phonological store. In order to make sense of their results, the authors conclude that there must be several information stores involved in reading. However, Baddeley has never updated his model of working memory to account for his own findings. Even in a review of his most salient findings 22 years later, Baddeley (2003) wrote of only a singular phonological store for all phonological information. Other groups of researchers specifically interested in what underlies reading processes looked at concurrent articulation effects to see if phonology must always be accessed (Besner, 1987; Johnston and McDermott, 1986). Their work provides reason to question Baddeley s ideas. In a review examining the widely held conclusions of concurrent articulation literature at the time, Besner (1987) reviewed experiments that explored participants responses to homophones to determine if there is a general phonological code for all similar reading tasks or possible differences between superficially similar phonological tasks. Besner included the Baddeley and Lewis (1981) study, in which participants were asked to decide whether a pair of non-words sounded the same with and without concurrent articulation. After introducing the various studies, Besner concluded that only rhyme judgments are affected by concurrent articulation while homophone and non-word homophone judgments are not. Besner therefore argued that there must be different underlying mechanisms responsible for the varying impact of concurrent articulation, and that the widely held conclusion that concurrent articulation affects all phonological decisions is unjustifiably generalized. To explain these differential effects, 13

14 Besner suggested that rhyme judgments require post-assembly phonemic segmentation and deletion processes that are affected by concurrent articulation and which are not necessary for other phonological decisions such as homophones. A study by Johnston and McDermott (1986) looking at the effect of concurrent articulation on different types of rhyme decisions provides evidence that even Besner (1987) may be over generalizing. Johnston and McDermott created four types of rhyming word pairs by mixing phonological and orthographic (visual) similarity. Type 1 pairs were visually similar, non-rhyming words (e.g. bone, none). Type 2 pairs were visually similar rhyming words (e.g. full, pull). Type 3 pairs were dissimilar non-rhyming words (e.g. chair, reel). Type 4 pairs were visually dissimilar rhyming words (e.g. dare, hair). Using these four types of word pairs, the authors performed four experiments manipulating the speed of articulation and stimuli presentation mode. Concurrent articulation required participants to count from one to eight at a pace of 3 per second or as quickly as possible. Word pairs were presented either simultaneously with one word above the other or successively with the second word displayed 1500 ms after the first word display ended. These two conditions of each variable were mixed to create the four experiments. Johnston and McDermott found that concurrent articulation affected the accuracy of responses regardless of the mode of presentation or articulation speed. This effect was greatest for the type 1 (bone, none) word pairs. Participants also had more errors on the type 4 (dare, hair) word pairs compared to the type 2 (full, pull) and type 3 (chair, reel). Overall, participants performed much worse on type 1 (bone, none) pairs than type 4 (dare, hair) pairs. The authors concluded that type 1 (bone, none) pairs would most benefit from encoding the words into an articulatory form, a process blocked by concurrent articulation. This finding calls into question the conclusion by Besner (1987), in which all rhyme decisions are treated similarly. 14

15 The current literature leaves us wondering what common mechanism might explain a common contribution of the cerebellum for rhyme decisions and verbal working memory tasks. This study aims to test and validate the findings of Johnston and McDermott (1986) that different rhyme judgments are differentially affected by concurrent articulation and to examine the cerebellum s role in various phonological and working memory tasks. The methods and results from Experiment 2 (using simultaneous presentation and rapid articulation) in Johnston and McDermott s study were used to motivate the present experimental design, which included the four types of rhyme word pairs in the rhyming task of this study. Also, this study incorporates a non-word homophone task that is similar to the task used by Baddeley and Lewis (1981) The conclusion of Besner (1987), that rhyme judgments are affected by concurrent articulation, but other phonological judgments (such as homophone and non-word homophone) are not affected, was combined with the evidence from Ben-Yehudah and Fiez (2008) which indicated a common region for rhyme and recall in the cerebellum to generate specific predictions. Based on the results of Ben-Yehudah and Fiez, we hypothesize that control participants should have similar activation patterns for rhyming and working memory tasks in the cerebellum whereas homophone and non-word homophone tasks should have different activation sites. 15

16 2.0 EXPERIMENT 1 BEHAVIORAL EXPERIMENT 2.1 METHODS Participants Twenty undergraduate students (8 females, 12 males) enrolled in an introductory psychology course at the University of Pittsburgh served as participants for the behavioral portion of the experiment. All of the participants were native English speakers and given credit for participation. All subjects provided informed consent on a University of Pittsburgh consent form approved by the Institutional Review Board Stimuli The stimuli for the rhyme component were 80 pairs of words of four different types (20 word pairs of each type). Type 1 (bone, none) word pairs look alike but do not rhyme, type 2 (full, pull) word pairs look alike and do rhyme, type 3 (chair, reel) word pairs do not look alike and do not rhyme, and type 4 (dare, hair) word pairs do not look alike but do rhyme (see Appendix A.1). Three tasks comprised the multi-task component, a rhyme judgment task, a homophone judgment task, and a non-word homophone judgment task. Each of these tasks had 64 pairs of 16

17 words. In the rhyme judgment task, word pairs were split into the same four types as the rhyme component (see Appendix A.2). For the homophone and non-word homophone tasks, there were 64 word triplets (see Appendix A.3, A.4). Each of these triplets had a main word that was either paired with a word that created a homophone match half of the time or a word that did not make a homophone match the other half. The non-word homophone stimuli also consisted of 64 word triplets of non-real words. Words for this task were created by morphing the words from the homophone task using the following rules; only one letter could be added to a word, only two letters in a word could be changed, and the vowel sounds had to stay constant. These non-words were created to avoid creating homophones of actual words while maintaining the construction of a real English word by avoiding unusual sequences of letters. All of the stimuli were single syllable words/non-word and presented in all capital letters. All stimuli were matched for frequency, word length, number of phonemes, and number of phonological and orthographic neighbors. There was a practice phase for each component of the experiment. All stimuli for the practice were completely different than those used in the main experiment Procedure The behavioral experiment was designed using E-Prime software (Schneider et al., 2002). It consisted of two parts; a rhyme only component that consisted only of rhyme judgments, and a multi-task component that consisted of rhyme judgments as well as homophone and non-word homophone judgment tasks. The order of presentation for these two components and all tasks within the multi-task component were presented randomly. During a portion of each component, subjects were asked to perform a secondary task, concurrent articulation (e.g. repeating the word 17

18 the out loud as quickly as possible). Rapid articulation was used in this study because it was easier for participants to maintain. During the task epochs when no secondary task was performed, subjects made word pair judgments without any distraction. Participants were provided with both detailed written instructions and verbal instructions and any additional questions were addressed before the study began. For both components, subjects were presented with a 3 s instruction screen and a preparatory 1500 ms fixation screen. The fixation screen was followed by a block of eight trials. Each trial consisted of a word pair display screen for 500 ms and a 2500 ms response screen. The stimuli were presented one above the other at the center of the screen with a fixation cross in between the two words (see Figure 1). During the response screen, the fixation cross remained in the center of the screen. Participants were instructed to press 1 if the word pair rhymed or were a homophone pair or 2 if the word pair did not rhyme or form a homophone pair. Responses were to be made as quickly as possible and were valid if made both during word presentation as well as during the response screen afterwards. To mimic the imaging experiment, a 15 sec fixation baseline screen was displayed which had an asterisk at the center of the screen. All presentation screens had a white background with black, bold, Courier New, size 18 font (see Figure 1). A button press began the next block so that participants could rest in between if they wished (see Figure 2 for block design) 18

19 WORD + WORD Figure 1. Format of Presentation Screens for Experiment 1 Button Press Instruction Fixation Display Response Baseline User Defined 3000 ms 1500 ms 500 ms 2500 ms ms. 8 trials Figure 2. Block design for Experiment 1 The rhyme only component consisted of 10 blocks, 5 with articulation and 5 with no articulation, with the blocks presented in random order. The instruction screen for the articulation blocks said Articulation in the center of the screen and for the no articulation blocks it said No Articulation. In the articulation trials, participants were to articulate from the instruction screen until the baseline screen. For each block, two of each of the four rhyme pair types were presented in random order in each block. Participants were to press 1 if the pair of words rhymed and 2 if they did not as quickly as possible. 19

20 The multi-task component consisted of four sets of six blocks each. Each set had two blocks of a rhyme task, two blocks of a homophone task, and two blocks of a non-word homophone tasks. Each task had one block with articulation and one block with no articulation. All of the blocks in each set were presented in random order. The instruction screen for each block consisted of the type of block and either articulation or no articulation written in the center of the screen (e.g. Homophone No Articulation ). For rhyme task blocks, the block proceeded exactly as described above with two words from each of the four types presented in random order. For the homophone and non-word homophone trials, a main word (or non-word) was paired with two possible items, either the word (or non-word) which made a homophone or the word (or non-word) making a non-homophone pair. Half of all trials in the task resulted in 1 responses and half 2 responses, with the order of the trials randomized. Participants were instructed to press 1 if the pair of words rhymed or were homophones and 2 if they were not. The participants first completed a practice experiment which was a shortened version of the main experiment to become familiar with the protocol. The word stimuli used in the practice session were different than those used in the main experiment, but the procedure and timing of the blocks were identical to the main experiment. The same written and verbal instructions were given and participants were encouraged to ask any questions just as in the main experiment. The rhyme component and multi-task component were presented in random order. The practice session of the rhyme component consisted of two blocks, articulation and no articulation, presented in random order. The practice multi-task component consisted of one set of the six blocks presented randomly. 20

21 2.2 RESULTS Reaction Time Only correct trials were used to calculate average response times for each subject. A main effect of rhyme pair type, F(3,57) = 37.0, p< was found for reaction time (see Table 1). Responses for type 1 (bone, none) pairs were significantly slower than for all other pair types, p< Also, responses to type 4 (dare, hair) pairs were significantly slower than for type 2 (full, pull) and type 3 (chair, reel) pairs, p<.005. No significant main effect was found for concurrent articulation in the rhyme component, F(1,19) = 0.577, p<0.46. However, there was a significant interaction between pair type and articulation, F(3,57) = 4.07, p<0.01. There was a main effect of task type on reaction time in the multi-task component, F(2,38) = 8.95, p<0.001 with participants responding significantly slower for non-word homophone trials than rhyme or homophone (see Table 2). No main effect of concurrent articulation on reaction time was found between the tasks in the multi-task component, F(1,19) = 0.797, p<0.38. There was no significant interaction between task type and articulation, F(2,38) = 2.27, p<0.12. Table 1. Reaction Time±SE for Rhyme Component Type 1 Type 2 Type 3 Type 4 Overall ± ± ± ±12.21 Articulation ± ± ± ±19.89 No Articulation ± ± ± ±

22 Table 2. Reaction Time±SE for Multi-Task Component Homophone Non-Word Homophone Rhyme Overall ± ± ±8.10 Articulation ± ± ±13.44 No Articulation ± ± ± Accuracy There was a main effect of pair type on the percent error (percentage of incorrect responses) in the rhyme component, F(3,57) = 26.2 p< (see Table 3). Comparing pair types using paired t-tests, participants missed significantly more rhyme judgments of type 1 (bone, none) pairs compared to all other pair types, p< In addition, participants performed significantly worse on the type 4 (dare, hair) pairs compared to the type 2 (full, pull) and type 3 (chair, reel) pairs, p<.03. More errors also occurred for type 2 pairs than type 3 pairs, p<.01. Looking at the effects of concurrent articulation, a main effect of articulation was found between pair types, F(1,19) = 13.5 p< Using paired t-tests, only type 1 (bone, none) pairs showed a significant effect of articulation with more errors occurring in the concurrent articulation condition, t(19)=2.98 p< However, there was not a significant interaction between pair type and articulation, F(3,57) = 1.86, p<0.15. No main effect was found for task type in the multi-task component, F(2,38) = 1.99, p<0.15 (see Table 4). However, participants responded less accurately on the non-word homophone task than on the homophone task, p< A main effect of articulation was also found for the multi-task component, F(1,19) = 4.83 p<0.05. Again, rhyme decisions were significantly more effected by concurrent articulation, p< There was no interaction between task type and articulation, F(2,38) = 2.32, p<

23 Table 3. Percent Error±SE for Rhyme Component Type 1 Type 2 Type 3 Type 4 Overall 31.04± ± ± ±1.72 Articulation 35.29± ± ± ±2.31 No Articulation 26.72± ± ± ±1.88 Table 4. Percent Error±SE for Multi-Task Component Homophone Non-Word Homophone Rhyme Overall 9.87± ± ±1.14 Articulation 10.81± ± ±1.83 No Articulation 8.95± ± ± DISCUSSION Our results support the argument by Besner (1986) that concurrent articulation impairs rhyme judgments but not homophone and non-word homophone judgments. While the reaction times for all three tasks were not significantly affected by concurrent articulation, articulation significantly increased errors for the rhyme task without affecting the other tasks. Furthermore, the results mostly verify the findings by Johnston and McDermott in Experiment 2 (1987). We similarly found type 1 (bone, none) pairs resulted in the slowest responses. Whereas Johnston and McDermott only found significantly slower responses for type 1 pairs compared to type 2 (full, pull) and 3 (chair, reel) pairs and type 4 (dare, hair) pairs compared to type 2 pairs, we found type 1 pairs to be significantly slower than all other types and significantly slower responses for type 4 pairs compared to type 2 and 3 pairs. Our finding that reaction time was not significantly affected by concurrent articulation also matches Johnston and McDermott s results. We also replicated their finding that significantly more errors occurred for type 1 and 4 pairs than on type 2 and type 3. However, only type 1 (bone, none) words 23

24 showed a significant effect of articulation on accuracy in our study whereas Johnston and McDermott found this effect for type 2 (full, pull) and type 4 (dare, hair) word pairs as well. While our results demonstrated this pattern (see Table 3), the differences were not significant. These results verify the findings by Johnston and McDermott (1987) and Besner (1986) which intimate that different processes are involved in rhyme judgments compared to homophone and non-word homophone judgments and that there are differences even within rhyme judgments. Replicating their results gives us confidence that our imaging results for the multi-task component can be used to look at areas involved in a possible articulatory mechanism even when no concurrent articulation is used, using the basic logic that tasks that are disrupted by concurrent articulation are likely to share a common neural substrate within the cerebellum. 24

25 3.0 EXPERIMENT 2 NEUROIMAGING EXPERIMENT 3.1 METHODS Participants Twenty participants took part in the neuroimaging experiment. Because of a scanning error resulting in poor cerebellar coverage, four participants were excluded from the analysis. The remaining participants (11 females, 5 males) were between the ages of 18 and 38 (average of 23.5) and were native English speakers. They received monetary compensation for participating Stimuli All of the stimuli used in the rhyme and multi-task components were identical to those used in Experiment 1. In addition, a third component involving a working memory task was added to the experimental design. For this task, the items on each trial were drawn from a pool of nine words and nine letters (see Appendix A.5). The letters were all dissimilar sounding consonants. The words were one-syllable, four letter, dissimilar words, each beginning with a different consonant which were matched for frequency. 25

26 3.1.3 Procedure The neuroimaging portion of the experiment used an adapted version of the behavioral experiment with necessary timing modifications for use in the scanner. The scanning sessions included a rhyme-only task component, but because of a design oversight, details of this component will not be discussed further. A working memory portion of the experiment was also added. There were no articulation trials in the neuroimaging experiment, but the number of trials and stimuli for each task remained the same. As with the behavioral experiment, participants first completed a practice experiment. Participants were provided with both written and verbal instructions before each component in the practice experiment. To minimize brightness in the scanner, all presentation screens had a grey background with white text. The text remained bold, Courier New, size 18 font (see Figure 3). WORD + WORD Figure 3. Format of Presentation Screens for Experiment 2 26

27 In the main experiment conducted in the scanner, participants were given one of eight counterbalanced experimental designs, in which the order of presentation for the rhyme component and multi-task component was mixed. The working memory component was always presented after the rhyme and multi-task components because of movement concerns. A start screen first notified participants which component was about to begin. Following the trigger pulse, a two second fixation cross at the center of the screen was displayed. This was followed by a set of six blocks for the multi-task component and five blocks for the working memory component. The design of a block for the multi-task component (see Figure 4) was the same as in the behavioral experiment with the following exceptions: There were no concurrent articulation blocks. In the multi-task component, the instruction screen lasted for 2000 ms. The fixation before each block was 2000 ms instead of 1500 ms so that it would take up 1 TR. The baseline was changed from 15 sec to 14 sec so that it would also take up an even number of TRs. Each block progressed automatically after the baseline screen without a button press by the participant. Responses were recorded using a response glove attached to the participants hand. Instruction Fixation Display Response Baseline 2000 ms 2000 ms 500 ms 2500 ms ms 8 trials Figure 4. Block Design for Multi Task Component in Experiment 2 The multi-task component consisted of four sets of six blocks each. Each set had two blocks of a rhyme task, two blocks of a homophone task, and two blocks of a non-word 27

28 homophone tasks. All of the blocks in each set were presented in random order. Participants were instructed to press the button attached to their index finger when the pair of words rhymed or were homophones and their middle finger when the words were not a rhyming or homophone pair. The working memory component consisted of four counterbalanced sets of trials, across which the type of stimuli (words, letters) and output modality (written, spoken) was varied. Each set consisted of five blocks each. All sets began with a four second instruction screen which said the set type in the center of the screen. Then, the trigger pulse was followed by a two second fixation cross in the center of the screen. This was followed by the five trials in the set. Each trial consisted of the presentation of six stimuli, with stimulus presented in all capital letters at the center of the screen for 750 ms, followed by a 250 ms fixation cross in the center of the screen followed by the. Once all six stimuli were presented, there was a ten second delay with five pound signs in the center of the screen. The delay period was followed by a six second response period indicated by five question marks at the center. A 12 second baseline then concluded each block (see Figure 5 for block design). Fixation Display Delay Response Baseline 250 ms 750 ms ms 6000 ms ms 6 trials Figure 5. Block Design for Working Memory Component in Experiment 2 28

29 For written sets, one of the investigators went into the scanning room to take off the response glove and put a pad of paper on the participant s stomach and a pen in the participant s hand. The investigator put the participant s hand with the pen in the proper starting position on the page. Before going into the scanner, participants were told to write the six words or letters of each block across the page and then move their hand down a little bit on the page for the next trial. When participants finished one of the written runs, the investigator turned the page on the writing pad and again placed the participant s hand with the pen at the top of the page. For spoken trials, participants were instructed to speak normally into a microphone that was attached to headphones. The practice experiment was performed on a computer outside of the scanner in order for the participants to become familiar with the procedure and timing of the tasks. All of the timing was the same as for the scanning experiment besides the removal of the 2 s fixation screen which followed the trigger pulse. The stimuli used in the practice were all completely different than those used in the main experiment. All participants first practiced the rhyme component, then the multi-task component, and finally the working memory component. Participants were provided detailed written and verbal instructions before each task and were asked if they had any questions. To mimic using the response glove, they were instructed to respond using their index finger if the word pair rhymed or were a homophone pair and their middle finger if the word pair did not rhyme or form a homophone pair. For the multi-task component, participants practiced one block of the rhyme task, one block of the homophone task, and one block of the non-word homophone task. The three blocks were presented in random order. They then practiced one block for each type of the working memory component in the following order: written words, written letters, spoken words, and 29

30 spoken letters. In order to simulate the testing conditions of the scanner, participants were instructed to hold the writing pad across their stomach and write their responses while looking at the screen fmri Data Acquisition All fmri data were collected using a 3.0T Siemens Magnetom Allegra (Siemens AG, USA) head-only research scanner with a circularly polarized transmit/receive head coil and a projection mirror at the Brain Imaging and Research Center (University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pa). Prior to the functional scans, high resolution and T2-weighted in-plane (38 slice) structural scans using Echo-planar imaging pulse sequence were obtained. For the functional scanning, 38, 3.2 mm thick, oblique slices were taken parallel to the plane of the anterior commissure and posterior commissure (TR time = 2000ms, echo time = 25 ms, flip angle = 70, field of view = 200). The scanning slice prescription was changed based on subject anatomy to cover the entire cerebellum. While activations in all covered brain regions were obtained, only activations in the cerebellum were analyzed in the current study fmri Data Pre-processing The scanning data were reconstructed and preprocessed using Neuroimaging Software Package, NIS 3.6 (University of Pittsburgh, Princeton University) and an in-house software package called Fiswidgets (Functional Imaging Software Widgets, Fissell et al., 2003). After performing quality checks, the data were corrected for motion with Automated Image Registration, AIR (Woods et al., 1993) Data from tasks in which movement in any 30

31 direction exceeded four mm or degrees was excluded from analysis. Data were then corrected for linear trends adjusting for any possible scanner drift. A reference brain was chosen from among the subjects, and all extraneous matter was removed each subject s T2 structural image. The stripped structural scans were co-registered to the first functional scan, and then transformed into the reference brain space. Functional images were scaled to a global mean and then smoothed using a three dimensional Gaussian filter (8 mm full width at half maximum). The reference brain and all functional data were then converted into Talairach space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) using AFNI (AFNI_2008_07_18_1710, Cox, 1996) in order to perform statistical analysis fmri Localizer In order to localize regions associated with working memory, we performed a voxel-wise analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the data from the working memory component. We identified two sets of regions that were active during the encoding and retrieval epochs of the working memory component. In order to obtain BOLD (blood oxygen level depending) signal in each epoch, a portion of the task window was contrasted with baseline fixation. For the encoding epoch, we contrasted the last 2 s of encoding and the first 4 s of the maintenance interval versus the last 4 s of baseline. For the retrieval epoch, we contrasted the last 2 s of retrieval and the first 4 s of baseline with the last 4 s of baseline. We used a threshold criterion of p =.005 and a voxel contiguity threshold of three voxels. From this localizer, we were able to identify a set of 18 regions in the cerebellum (9 from each working memory epoch). 31

32 3.1.7 Regions of Interest Analysis The set of 18 regions was then applied to the multi-task component using a time series analysis. Of these 18 regions, 8 (5 from the encoding epoch, 3 from the retrieval epoch) showed negative patterns of activation for both the working memory component as well as the multi-task component. These eight regions are noted in Table 5 and will not be discussed further. For the remaining 10 regions (Figure 6, Table 6), data from the time series analysis were used to calculate percent signal change in the regions during the multi-task component. To compute percent signal change, we calculated the average activation across the 24 sec of each task (rhyme, homophone, non-word homophone) minus the average activation for the last four seconds of baseline across all three tasks, and then divided this value by the average activation across each task and then multiplied the resulting number by 100. We then performed a repeatedmeasures ANOVA to look at differences between the tasks in each ROI. Table 5. ROIs with Negative Activation Regions with negative activation for encoding versus baseline and response versus baseline in the working memory component at p = 0.005, voxel contiguity = 3. Contrast Hemisphere Talairach coordinate Cerebellar region Working Memory Retrieve L -39, -55, -44 VIIIB L -26, -33, -47 VIIIB R 23, -74, -40 CRI/CRII Working Memory Encode L -16, -45, -50 IX R -26, -46, -44 IX R 23, -39, -54 VIIIB R -26, -32, -43 X R -20, -36, -29 IV 32

33 Figure Regions from Working Memory Component with Positive Activation (Blue=Retrieve, Red=Encode, Purple=Overlap) Table 6. ROIs with Positive Activation Regions with positive activation for encoding versus baseline and response versus baseline in the working memory component at p = 0.005, voxel contiguity = 3. Contrast Hemisphere Talairach coordinate Task Effect? Cerebellar region Working Memory L -26, -56, -47 No VIIIA Retrieve R 19, -56, -50 No VIIIA L -26,-58, -22 No VI L -32, -43, -29 No VI L -13, -55, -18 No V R 13, -58, -18 No V Working Memory Encode L -7, -65, -22 (ROI 1) Yes VI R 6, -61, -29 (ROI 2) Yes VI/VIII R 36, -58, -26 No VI R 13, -45, -15 No IV 33

34 3.2 RESULTS Behavioral Results As with Experiment 1, there was a main effect of task type for the response time in the multi-task component, F(2,30) = 6.49 p< Participants responded significantly slower on non-word homophone judgments than homophone judgments, p< Unlike Experiment 1, this effect was not significant for non-word homophone versus rhyme judgments. Also, rhyme judgments were performed significantly more slowly than homophone judgments, p< 0.04 (see Table 7). Table 7. Reaction Time±SE for Multi-Task Component in Experiment 2 Homophone Non-Word Homophone Rhyme ± ± ±14.24 Also coinciding with the results from Experiment 1, there was no significant main effect of task type on accuracy for the multi-task component F(2,30) = 2.82 p<0.08. However, nonword homophone judgments were responded to incorrectly significantly more than homophone judgments, p< 0.05 (see Table 8). Table 8. Percent Error±SE for Multi-Task Component in Experiment 2 Homophone Non-Word Homophone Rhyme 7.39± ± ±

35 3.2.2 Imaging Results Of the 10 ROIs analyzed, only two showed a significant main effect of task type (rhyme vs. homophone vs. non-word homophone) for percent signal change. These bilateral regions were both active in the encoding contrast. The ROI at Tailarach coordinates -7, -65, -22 (from now on referred to as ROI 1) (F(2,30) = 5.01 p<0.02) was significantly more active for the nonword homophone task than the rhyme task, p< (Figure 7). The ROI at coordinates 6, -61, -29 (from now on referred to as ROI 2) (F(2,30) = 6.04 p<0.006) was significantly more active for the homophone (p< 0.02) and non-word homophone (p< 0.02) tasks compared to the rhyme task (Figure 8). % Signal Change ROI 1 Homophone Rhyme Non-Word Homophone Figure 7. Percent Signal Change by Task in ROI 1 35

36 % Signal Change ROI 2 Homophone Rhyme Non-Word Homophone Figure 8. Percent Signal Change by Task for ROI DISCUSSION The results are opposite what we had expected and difficult to explain. Regions identified as active for the working memory component showed the most activation for homophone and non-word homophone tasks. Results from the study by Ben-Yehudah and Fiez (2008) suggest that we should have found the opposite effect, with common activity for recall and rhyming tasks. There are a number of reasons why we may not have gotten the result that we expected. In the working memory task, patients with cerebellar damage in Ben-Yehudah and Fiez (2008) performed within the range of intact controls for digits and words recalled in the order of presentation. Their deficit was evident on tasks with non-word recall and backwards recall of digits. In our study, participants recalled words and stimuli in the order of presentation. It is possible that only more difficult working memory tasks recruit cerebellar areas that are common 36

37 with rhyme judgments tasks. We might not have found significant percent signal change for the rhyme task in the ROIs for this reason. It may also be that collapsing across multiple recall modalities (written and spoken) and stimulus types (letters and words) in the working memory component to identify ROIs cancelled out the activation of some cerebellar regions involved in working memory. However, we did identify regions that were found by Durisko and Fiez (2010) in a study specifically meant to identify cerebellar regions involved in working memory. Interestingly, one of the regions in which we found a significant effect of task type on percent signal change (ROI 1) was identified by Durisko and Fiez as a region involved in overt speech but not working memory. In our study, activation in this ROI may have been particularly boosted by the spoken recall sets (the Durisko and Fiez study used only written recall). It is possible that participants anticipated a spoken response and began to internally practice this output while the stimuli were being encoded. Using a paired t-test, we found a significant effect of recall mode in ROI 1 with significantly more activation for the spoken blocks, t(16)=98.24 p< The associated bilateral region, ROI 2, also showed this same effect, t(16)=84.61 p< Another possibility is that activation was influenced by the difficulty of the tasks. The behavioral results support this. The non-word homophone task, which displayed the most signal change in ROIs 1 and 2, had the slowest response times in both Experiments. Specifically, responses for the non-word homophone task were significantly slower compared to both the rhyme and homophone tasks in Experiment 1, but only significantly slower than the homophone task in Experiment 2. Accuracy was also significantly worse on the non-word homophone task compared to the homophone task in Experiment 2 and this same effect approached significance in Experiment 1. In Experiment 2 however, responses for the rhyme task were significantly 37

38 slower than the homophone task although the homophone task showed a greater percent signal change in ROI 2. Also, despite the higher percent signal change for the non-word homophone task compared to the rhyme task, there was no significant difference of accuracy between these tasks. This task difficulty explanation cannot account for all of the effects seen between the tasks or how the increased activation for the homophone and particularly non-word homophone tasks relate to the working memory component used to identify the ROIs. 38

39 4.0 GENERAL DISCUSSION The current data are difficult to make sense of within the framework of concurrent articulation and models of working memory. One possibility is that our localizer did not accurately identify areas involved in working memory for some of the reasons mentioned above. Instead, it seems that ROI 1 and 2 are likely regions identified because of their strong activation for spoken recall. We found that both ROIs in this bilateral region were significantly more active for spoken recall in the working memory component after realizing that ROI 1 is the same region labeled by Durisko and Fiez (2010) as an area for overt speech. Therefore, the higher activation in these regions for homophone and non-word homophone judgments may indicate that these tasks require more internal verbal practice in anticipation of verbal recall than the rhyme task. This would seem particularly true for the non-word homophone task which requires participants to make phonological decisions for stimuli with no existing lexical information. Interestingly, non-word homophone judgments had the most activation in the region corresponding to Durisko and Fiez overt speech area (ROI 1). Further analysis might want to parse out the activation for spoken and written recall in the working memory component to see if this finding is consistent even in the ROIs which did not show a significant task effect. Due to the difficulty of non-word homophone judgments, why is this task not affected by concurrent articulation? In both Experiment 1 and 2 of our study, non-word homophone judgments resulted in the slowest and least accurate responses. Consistent with previous studies 39

40 (Besner 1987), we found that despite the apparent difficulty of the task, there are no effects of concurrent articulation on responses. It may be that concurrent articulation does not affect nonword homophone judgments because there are no existing phonological representations to disrupt when the unfamiliar stimuli are first presented. Therefore, an effect of concurrent articulation would likely be found in responses to subsequent presentations of the non-words. In other words, participants should have greater difficulty with non-word homophone judgments for stimuli that were first presented during concurrent articulation since this distracting task should prevent participants from creating a phonological representation of the new stimuli. This process should not be disrupted for non-word homophone pairs that were presented without concurrent articulation and these stimuli should therefore be responded to more accurately on subsequent presentations. Another consistent finding for the effects of concurrent articulation is its effect on accuracy but not reaction time (Johnston and McDermott, 1986; Baddeley, Eldridge, and Lewis, 1981; Baddeley and Lewis, 1981). This effect is obvious for the rhyme judgments in Experiment 1. In the multi-task component, the average reaction time for the rhyme task is almost the same with and without concurrent articulation but there is a large effect of articulation on the percent error. Also, type 1 pairs (bone, none) in the rhyme component displayed a significant effect of articulation on accuracy but were actually responded to quicker with concurrent articulation. The idea of an articulatory monitoring process proposed by Ben-Yehudah and Fiez (2008) can help to explain this effect. Articulatory monitoring would benefit phonological decisions with conflict such as the type 1 (bone, none) and 4 rhyme pairs which mix orthographic and phonological similarity (dare, hair). If concurrent articulation affects the articulatory monitoring process, difficult phonological decisions such as these should suffer from decreased accuracy due to the 40

41 inability to check for errors in pronunciation. However, occupying this monitoring process should not influence the reaction time. In fact, the quicker responses for type 1 pairs during concurrent articulation in Experiment 1 might be due to the disruption of the monitoring process removing a step of processing leading up to a response. The articulatory monitoring process should also help for unfamiliar stimuli such as in the non-word homophone task since participants have no previous experience with the stimuli. In fact, the cerebellar patients in the study by Ben-Yehudah and Fiez did have difficulty with recalling non-words in a working memory task. Looking for activation of these difficult tasks may help to locate a site of the articulatory monitoring process in the cerebellum. Future studies could look at the overlap of activation for backwards recall, which cerebellar patients had difficulty with (Ben-Yehudah and Fiez (2008), non-word homophone judgments, and rhyme judgments using rhyme pairs with mixed orthography and phonology (type 1 and 4). In the current study, it is possible that pulling out the effects of these difficult rhyme judgments may result in different effects. It would also be beneficial to look at active areas across the brain rather than a focus in the cerebellum given the complicated pattern of results. Although opposite in direction from what we had expected, we do see a separation of activation for the rhyme task versus the other tasks in the multi-task component. The percent signal change for the rhyme task is significantly different in ROI 1 (Figure 7) then the homophone and non-word homophone tasks. While the effect is puzzling, it is worth further attention. It may be that the decreased activation for rhyme judgments in the cerebellum corresponds with an increase in the cortex. A widened search could help to clarify this possibility. 41

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience Working memory 1 What is working memory? Brief, immediate memory for information we are currently processing. Closely related to attention: attending to something is often

More information

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Memory. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Memory. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin AQA A Level Psychology Topic Companion Memory Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin AQA A LEVEL Psychology topic companion: MEMORY Page 2 Contents Memory The multi-store model 3 Types of long-term memory 9 The working

More information

Cognition. Mid-term 1. Top topics for Mid Term 1. Heads up! Mid-term exam next week

Cognition. Mid-term 1. Top topics for Mid Term 1. Heads up! Mid-term exam next week Cognition Prof. Mike Dillinger Mid-term 1 Heads up! Mid-term exam next week Chapters 2, 3, of the textbook Perception, Attention, Short-term memory The lectures are to help you digest the chapters; exams

More information

Dissociation of Verbal Working Memory System Components Using a Delayed Serial Recall Task

Dissociation of Verbal Working Memory System Components Using a Delayed Serial Recall Task Dissociation of Verbal Working Memory System Components Using a Delayed Serial Recall Task Jason M. Chein and Julie A. Fiez Department of Psychology, Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, University

More information

Information and cue-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states

Information and cue-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states Information and cue-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states Psycholinguistics 2 Contents: Introduction... 1 Pilot Experiment... 2 Experiment... 3 Participants... 3 Materials... 4 Design... 4 Procedure...

More information

ASHI 712. The Neuroscience of Human Memory. Dr. Olave E. Krigolson LECTURE 2: Short Term Memory and Sleep and Memory

ASHI 712. The Neuroscience of Human Memory. Dr. Olave E. Krigolson LECTURE 2: Short Term Memory and Sleep and Memory ASHI 712 The Neuroscience of Human Memory Dr. Olave E. Krigolson krigolson@uvic.ca LECTURE 2: Short Term Memory and Sleep and Memory Working / Short Term Memory Sunglasses Chair Dress Earrings Boots Bed

More information

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 SPECIAL ISSUE WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TE US ABOUT AND DIS? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 By: Angelika Dimoka Fox School of Business Temple University 1801 Liacouras Walk Philadelphia, PA

More information

Verbal Working Memory. The left temporoparietal junction in verbal working memory: Storage or attention. Baddelely s Multiple-Component Model

Verbal Working Memory. The left temporoparietal junction in verbal working memory: Storage or attention. Baddelely s Multiple-Component Model Verbal Working Memory The left temporoparietal junction in verbal working memory: Storage or attention Susan Ravizza LTM vs WM Focusing on the storage component of WM Maintenance of words, pictures, goals

More information

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS. Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS. Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney Sonification Lab, School of Psychology Georgia Institute of Technology, 654 Cherry

More information

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview Investigations in Resting State Connectivity Scott FMRI Laboratory Overview Introduction Functional connectivity explorations Dynamic change (motor fatigue) Neurological change (Asperger s Disorder, depression)

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT

THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT Tajuk Bab/Chapter title 1 THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT Teo Sieak Ling and Yeo Kee Jiar shireleneteoh@hotmail.my kjyeo_utm@yahoo.com Abstract Extensive scientific studies in human

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Andy C.H. Lee a,b,, Trevor W. Robbins b, Stephen Smith c, Gemma A. Calvert c, Irene Tracey c, Paul Matthews c, Adrian M. Owen a. 1.

Andy C.H. Lee a,b,, Trevor W. Robbins b, Stephen Smith c, Gemma A. Calvert c, Irene Tracey c, Paul Matthews c, Adrian M. Owen a. 1. Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 2420 2437 Evidence for asymmetric frontal-lobe involvement in episodic memory from functional magnetic resonance imaging and patients with unilateral frontal-lobe excisions Andy

More information

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Braver et al. 10.1073/pnas.0808187106 SI Methods Participants. Participants were neurologically normal, righthanded younger or older adults. The groups did not differ in gender breakdown

More information

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning GSTF Journal of Psychology (JPsych) Vol. No., August 5 Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning Hironori Oto 79 Received 6 Jul 5 Accepted 9 Aug 5 Abstract This paper

More information

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Parkland College A with Honors Projects Honors Program 2015 Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Hannah Li Parkland College Recommended Citation Li, Hannah, "Social and Pragmatic Language

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

AS Module 1. Cognitive & Developmental Psychology. Cognitive Human Memory. The nature and structure of memory Thought to be three types

AS Module 1. Cognitive & Developmental Psychology. Cognitive Human Memory. The nature and structure of memory Thought to be three types AS Module 1 Cognitive & Developmental Psychology Cognitive Human Memory The nature and structure of memory Thought to be three types Sensory memory Short term memory Long term memory Short Term Memory

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

AGE-RELATED DIFFERENCES IN VERBAL, VISUAL. AND SPATIAL MEMORY: The Same or Different?

AGE-RELATED DIFFERENCES IN VERBAL, VISUAL. AND SPATIAL MEMORY: The Same or Different? i AGE-RELATED DIFFERENCES IN VERBAL, VISUAL AND SPATIAL MEMORY: The Same or Different? Thesis submitted by Rhonda M Shaw (B.Psych [Hons]) February 1 2007 For the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy School of

More information

Effects of Cognitive Load on Processing and Performance. Amy B. Adcock. The University of Memphis

Effects of Cognitive Load on Processing and Performance. Amy B. Adcock. The University of Memphis Effects of Cognitive Load 1 Running Head: Effects of Cognitive Load Effects of Cognitive Load on Processing and Performance Amy B. Adcock The University of Memphis Effects of Cognitive Load 2 Effects of

More information

Chapter 5 Short-term/Working Memory

Chapter 5 Short-term/Working Memory Chapter 5 Short-term/Working Memory Original Information Processing Model rehearsal transfer Sensory Memory Working Memory Long-term Memory input from the world attention retrieval Characterizing Memories

More information

Sex influences on material-sensitive functional lateralization in working and episodic memory: Men and women are not all that different

Sex influences on material-sensitive functional lateralization in working and episodic memory: Men and women are not all that different www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 32 (2006) 411 422 Sex influences on material-sensitive functional lateralization in working and episodic memory: Men and women are not all that different Kristen

More information

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications. Dr. Peter J. Fiester November 14, 2012 Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications. Briefly discuss a few examples

More information

Developmental Evidence for Working Memory as Activated Long-Term Memory

Developmental Evidence for Working Memory as Activated Long-Term Memory Commentary on: Ruchkin D.S., Grafman J., Cameron K., Berndt R.S. (2003). Working Memory Retention Systems: A State of Activated Long-Term Memory. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, vol. 26, p. 250. Developmental

More information

Running head: THE EFFECT OF STIMULUS LENGTH AND TYPE

Running head: THE EFFECT OF STIMULUS LENGTH AND TYPE 1 Running head: THE EFFECT OF STIMULUS LENGTH AND TYPE Stimuli Recognition: The Effect of Stimulus Length and Type Karla Roberts Hanover College 2 Stimuli Recognition: The Effect of Stimulus Length and

More information

Does Wernicke's Aphasia necessitate pure word deafness? Or the other way around? Or can they be independent? Or is that completely uncertain yet?

Does Wernicke's Aphasia necessitate pure word deafness? Or the other way around? Or can they be independent? Or is that completely uncertain yet? Does Wernicke's Aphasia necessitate pure word deafness? Or the other way around? Or can they be independent? Or is that completely uncertain yet? Two types of AVA: 1. Deficit at the prephonemic level and

More information

Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there an effect on all trials?

Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there an effect on all trials? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (1), 171-177 Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there an effect on all trials? JEAN SAINT-AUBIN and DENIS OUELLETTE Université de Moncton, Moncton,

More information

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 699-703 Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening GREGORY J. H. COLFLESH University of Illinois, Chicago,

More information

Introduction Persons with aphasia who are trained to generate abstract words (e.g., justice) in a specific context-category (e.g.

Introduction Persons with aphasia who are trained to generate abstract words (e.g., justice) in a specific context-category (e.g. Introduction Persons with aphasia who are trained to generate abstract words (e.g., justice) in a specific context-category (e.g., courthouse) have been shown to improve not only on the trained items,

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Proactive interference plays a role in the word-length effect

Proactive interference plays a role in the word-length effect Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (4), 541-545 Proactive interference plays a role in the word-length effect JAMES S. NAIRNE and IAN NEATH Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana and MATT SERRA

More information

Coding. The format in which information is stored in the various memory stores.

Coding. The format in which information is stored in the various memory stores. Coding The format in which information is stored in the various memory stores. Coding The format in which information is stored in the various memory stores. Capacity The amount of information that can

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Selective Interference With the Maintenance of Location Information in Working Memory

Selective Interference With the Maintenance of Location Information in Working Memory Neuropsychology Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1996, Vol. I0, No. 2, 228-240 08944105/96/$3.00 Selective Interference With the Maintenance of Location Information in Working

More information

Controlled Experiments

Controlled Experiments CHARM Choosing Human-Computer Interaction (HCI) Appropriate Research Methods Controlled Experiments Liz Atwater Department of Psychology Human Factors/Applied Cognition George Mason University lizatwater@hotmail.com

More information

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information:

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: WORKING MEMORY Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: bamponsah@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education 2014/2015 2016/2017

More information

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions NeuroImage 10, 642 657 (1999) Article ID nimg.1999.0500, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential

More information

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Experimental Design Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Overview Design of functional neuroimaging studies Categorical designs Factorial designs Parametric

More information

Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala. California Institute of Technology

Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala. California Institute of Technology Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala Daniel P. Kennedy 1, Jan Gläscher 1, J. Michael Tyszka 2 & Ralph Adolphs 1,2 1 Division of Humanities and Social Sciences 2 Division of Biology California

More information

fmri Acquisition: Temporal Effects

fmri Acquisition: Temporal Effects Functional MRI Data Acquisition: Temporal fmri Acquisition: Temporal Effects Session length Repetition time Fixed vs. distributed temporal sampling Sparse temporal sampling Noise source recording Prospective

More information

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer Science & Technology (IJIRCST) ISSN: 2347-5552, Volume-2, Issue-6, November-2014 Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using

More information

Incorporation of Imaging-Based Functional Assessment Procedures into the DICOM Standard Draft version 0.1 7/27/2011

Incorporation of Imaging-Based Functional Assessment Procedures into the DICOM Standard Draft version 0.1 7/27/2011 Incorporation of Imaging-Based Functional Assessment Procedures into the DICOM Standard Draft version 0.1 7/27/2011 I. Purpose Drawing from the profile development of the QIBA-fMRI Technical Committee,

More information

Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction

Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction Global aging poses significant burdens as age-related impairments in cognitive function affect quality

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Functional specializations in lateral prefrontal cortex associated with the integration and segregation of information in working memory

Functional specializations in lateral prefrontal cortex associated with the integration and segregation of information in working memory Running Title: INTEGRATION AND SEGREGATION IN ANTERIOR PFC Functional specializations in lateral prefrontal cortex associated with the integration and segregation of information in working memory Nicola

More information

Magnetic Resonance Angiography

Magnetic Resonance Angiography Magnetic Resonance Angiography 1 Magnetic Resonance Angiography exploits flow enhancement of GR sequences saturation of venous flow allows arterial visualization saturation of arterial flow allows venous

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Types and Structure of Memory Types of Memory Type of Memory Time Course Capacity Conscious Awareness Mechanism of Loss Sensory Short-Term and Working Long-Term Nondeclarative

More information

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES by John W. Brelsford, Jr. and Richard C. Atkinson TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 July 21, 1967 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES!, Reproduction

More information

Memory Development. Cognitive Development

Memory Development. Cognitive Development Memory Development Cognitive Development Memory as information storage Memory Why does our memory sometimes fail us? Memory Schachter s Seven Sins of Memory 1. Transience 2. Absent-Mindedness 3. Blocking

More information

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The spacing and lag effect in free recall The spacing and lag effect in free recall Michael J. Kahana, Bradley R. Wellington & Marc W. Howard Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology Brandeis University Send correspondence to: Michael

More information

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 341 356 Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Joseph B. Sala a,, Pia Rämä a,c,d, Susan M.

More information

The Working Memory Model (WMM) By Karys, Emily V, Dan C, Liam

The Working Memory Model (WMM) By Karys, Emily V, Dan C, Liam The Working Memory Model (WMM) By Karys, Emily V, Dan C, Liam So what is the Working Memory Model? The Working Memory Model (WMM) is one explanation for how the short term memory (STM) functions and is

More information

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/24/2018: Lecture 05-2 Note: This Powerpoint presentation

More information

How should you study for Friday's exam?

How should you study for Friday's exam? How should you study for Friday's exam? re-read textbook? re-read lecture slides? study guide? NO! these are passive. Use active study. Test yourself by Take the practice quizzes in Moodle Create your

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Outline Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Michael A. Yassa, B.A. Division of Psychiatric Neuro-imaging Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Why fmri? fmri - How it works Research

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

Experimental Design I

Experimental Design I Experimental Design I Topics What questions can we ask (intelligently) in fmri Basic assumptions in isolating cognitive processes and comparing conditions General design strategies A few really cool experiments

More information

Beyond the Psychologist s Report. Nancy Foster, PhD Institute for Brain-Behavior Integration

Beyond the Psychologist s Report. Nancy Foster, PhD Institute for Brain-Behavior Integration Beyond the Psychologist s Report human mental processes are complex functional systems that are not localized in narrow, circumscribed areas of the brain, but take place through the participation of groups

More information

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 1 The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning Anila D Mello Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 2 Abstract Learning and changing

More information

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (6), 1067-1073 Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources MOSHE NAVEH-BENJAMIN University of Missouri,

More information

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information:

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: bamponsah@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education 2014/2015 2016/2017 Session Overview

More information

Phonological and Orthographic Processing: One or Both Hemispheres? Jeffrey R. Mathesius. University of British Columbia Okanagan

Phonological and Orthographic Processing: One or Both Hemispheres? Jeffrey R. Mathesius. University of British Columbia Okanagan 1 Phonological and Orthographic Processing: One or Both Hemispheres? Jeffrey R. Mathesius University of British Columbia Okanagan A Directed Studies Project Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

Critical Review: Inner and overt speech post-stroke: Is there a dissociation?

Critical Review: Inner and overt speech post-stroke: Is there a dissociation? Critical Review: Inner and overt speech post-stroke: Is there a dissociation? Caitlin Farrar M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate Western University: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders This critical review

More information

Primacy and recency effects as indices of the focus of attention

Primacy and recency effects as indices of the focus of attention Primacy and recency effects as indices of the focus of attention Alexandra B. Morrison, Andrew R. A. Conway and Jason M. Chein Journal Name: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience ISSN: - Article type: Original

More information

Users Manual for BrainMap Scribe 1.1.1

Users Manual for BrainMap Scribe 1.1.1 Users Manual for BrainMap Scribe 1.1.1 http://brainmap.org Angela R. Laird, Ph.D. Research Imaging Center, UT Health Science Center San Antonio BrainMap Development Team: Peter T. Fox, M.D. Jack L. Lancaster,

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

Functional Elements and Networks in fmri

Functional Elements and Networks in fmri Functional Elements and Networks in fmri Jarkko Ylipaavalniemi 1, Eerika Savia 1,2, Ricardo Vigário 1 and Samuel Kaski 1,2 1- Helsinki University of Technology - Adaptive Informatics Research Centre 2-

More information

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Neuroethics Publications Center for Neuroscience & Society 1-1-2009 The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy Andrea L. Glenn University

More information

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 4, 354-373 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association Inc 0096-3445/94/S3.00 Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent

More information

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the U.S. Office of Naval Research 1 Introduction During

More information

Working memory for musical and verbal material under conditions of irrelevant sound

Working memory for musical and verbal material under conditions of irrelevant sound Honors Theses Psychology Spring 2014 Working memory for musical and verbal material under conditions of irrelevant sound Kristi M. Von Handorf Penrose Library, Whitman College Permanent URL: http://hdl.handle.net/10349/1267

More information

Optical Illusions 4/5. Optical Illusions 2/5. Optical Illusions 5/5 Optical Illusions 1/5. Reading. Reading. Fang Chen Spring 2004

Optical Illusions 4/5. Optical Illusions 2/5. Optical Illusions 5/5 Optical Illusions 1/5. Reading. Reading. Fang Chen Spring 2004 Optical Illusions 2/5 Optical Illusions 4/5 the Ponzo illusion the Muller Lyer illusion Optical Illusions 5/5 Optical Illusions 1/5 Mauritz Cornelis Escher Dutch 1898 1972 Graphical designer World s first

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 197 (2009) 186 197 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Top-down attentional

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

Prof. Greg Francis 1/4/19

Prof. Greg Francis 1/4/19 PSY 200 Greg Francis Lecture 15 Modal Model of Memory Atkinson & Shiffrin (196) Today we focus on the Short-term store (Short term memory) Why there is a gate at the first floor stairway in the Psych building.

More information

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES Journal of Experimental Vol. 51, No. 4, 1956 Psychology CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES KENNETH H. KURTZ AND CARL I. HOVLAND Under conditions where several concepts are learned concurrently

More information

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION INTRODUCTION Research on the Deaf Brain is beginning to provide a new evidence base for policy and practice in relation to intervention with deaf children. This talk outlines the multi-channel nature of

More information

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest Experimental Design Kate Watkins Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford With thanks to: Heidi Johansen-Berg Joe Devlin Outline Choices for experimental paradigm Subtraction / hierarchical

More information

CHAPTER 6: Memory model Practice questions at - text book pages 112 to 113

CHAPTER 6: Memory model Practice questions at - text book pages 112 to 113 QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS CHAPTER 6: Memory model Practice questions at - text book pages 112 to 113 1) Which of the following sequences reflects the order in which the human brain processes

More information

Analogical Inference

Analogical Inference Analogical Inference An Investigation of the Functioning of the Hippocampus in Relational Learning Using fmri William Gross Anthony Greene Today I am going to talk to you about a new task we designed to

More information

Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming

Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming Brain (2000), 123, 620 640 Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming Randy L. Buckner, 1,3 Wilma Koutstaal, 2 Daniel L. Schacter 2 and Bruce

More information

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (1), 154-158 Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation JOO-SEOK HYUN AND STEVEN J. LUCK University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa In mental rotation, a

More information

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and 77 5 Discussion The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and implicit learning systems. Chapter 2 described the effects of performing a working memory task

More information

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Psychon Bull Rev (2017) 24:1651 1657 DOI 10.3758/s13423-017-1226-7 BRIEF REPORT Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Sébastien De Schrijver 1 & Pierre Barrouillet 1 Published

More information

Memory (1) Visual Sensory Store. Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory

Memory (1) Visual Sensory Store. Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory Memory (1) Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory Visual Sensory Store It appears that our visual system is able to hold a great deal of information but that if we do not attend to this information

More information

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes Brain (1998), 121, 1985 2002 Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes Anthony D. Wagner, 1 John E. Desmond, 1,2 Gary H. Glover 2 and John

More information

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses? Mental Imagery What is Imagery? What is nature of the images? Exact copy of original images? Represented in terms of meaning? If so, then how does the subjective sensation of an image arise? What is the

More information

Sperling conducted experiments on An experiment was conducted by Sperling in the field of visual sensory memory.

Sperling conducted experiments on An experiment was conducted by Sperling in the field of visual sensory memory. Levels of category Basic Level Category: Subordinate Category: Superordinate Category: Stages of development of Piaget 1. Sensorimotor stage 0-2 2. Preoperational stage 2-7 3. Concrete operational stage

More information

TITLE:&The"organisation"of"working"memory"networks"is"shaped"by"early"sensory"experience"

TITLE:&Theorganisationofworkingmemorynetworksisshapedbyearlysensoryexperience TITLE:&Theorganisationofworkingmemorynetworksisshapedbyearlysensoryexperience VeliaCardin 1,2,3,MaryRudner 1,RitaFDeOliveira 4,JosefineAndin 1,MerinaSu 5,LilliBeese 2,Bencie Woll 2 andjerkerrönnberg 1

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information