Hippocampal theta activity related to elicitation and inhibition of approach locomotion

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Hippocampal theta activity related to elicitation and inhibition of approach locomotion"

Transcription

1 Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Research report Hippocampal theta activity related to elicitation and inhibition of approach locomotion Harry M. Sinnamon Neuroscience and Behavior Program, Wesleyan University, Judd Hall, 207 High Street, Middletown, CT , USA Received 22 September 2004; received in revised form 4 December 2004; accepted 6 December 2004 Abstract This study determined if the hippocampal theta rhythm showed phase relationships or changes in amplitude and frequency with the onset of stimuli and locomotion in a task in which auditory cues initiated and suppressed approach locomotion. Rats with electrodes in the dorsal hippocampus lapped at a milk dipper and were presented a tone which predicted the delivery of a food pellet. In some trials the pellet cue tone was negated by 60-Hz clicks beginning 0.3 s after onset, and no pellet was delivered. A video capture system (20-ms sampling) synchronized to the hippocampal recording system (10-ms sampling) was used to determine the onset of locomotor approach to the pellet area. The findings failed to support proposals that phase-related mechanisms play a role in encoding and retrieval of movement-related information. Neither the pellet cue nor the negating cue reset the theta rhythm, and they did not produce differential evoked potentials. During milk lapping, theta amplitude increased in the 1/2 s prior to all pellet cues regardless of their locomotor effect. Frequency also rose but only when a nonnegated pellet elicited short-latency locomotion. During locomotor execution, theta peak amplitude peaked earlier than theta frequency by approximately one period. In general during performance of this task, increasing theta amplitude reflected a general preparation to process the cue and increasing theta frequency reflected the readiness to respond to the cue with locomotion Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Theta; Hippocampus; Approach; Locomotion; Inhibition; Reward; Attention There is general agreement that behavioral states characterized by prominent hippocampal theta activity are associated with increased levels of information processing related to the control of behavior [4,14,38 40]. However, the mechanisms used by the processes, the types of information processed, and the circuitry implementing behavioral control all remain elusive. Particular interest has developed in the temporal and phase dynamics of the theta rhythm because of the correspondence between theta frequencies and optimal parameters for long term potentiation [19,20,24]. Proposals have been made for theta phase-related mechanisms both for the encoding [21] and for the retrieval of movement-related information [19]. If the theta rhythm does operate in this way, it would be expected that phase-related patterns in theta Tel.: ; fax: address: hsinnamon@wesleyan.edu. would be present for stimuli that are being encoded, or that initiate retrieval of conditioned movements. Consistent with this idea, resetting of the theta rhythm was found in some cases with the presentation of an auditory cue that elicited approach behavior in a classical conditioning situation [9]. Consistent resetting has been found for auditory cues that required processing in working memory and that controlled differential instrumental behavior [16]. The general relationship between theta activity and movement is well-established. Theta activity is prominent during the performance of locomotor and orienting behaviors and is minimal during immobility and performance of repetitive consummatory and instrumental behaviors [5,15,36,41] in the absence of postural adjustments [36]. Theta activity of a lower frequency also appears during fear-related immobility in the rat [30]. Synchronization of theta to movements could represent either dependency on either the motor act or the /$ see front matter 2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi: /j.bbr

2 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) implicit sensory events related to it. Such movement-related phasing has been reported for sniffing [14,22,23]; and instrumental bar pressing behavior [8,31]. Synchronization of theta rhythm to the onset of locomotion has not been reported, but less time-locked modulations of theta activity prior to locomotion have been found. Theta frequency increases prior to the rapid onset of locomotor behaviors in the behaving rat [25,37,41]. In the anesthetized rat, low frequency theta activity increases prior to the initiation of locomotion [32,34]. It also appears prior to the initiation of a locomotor aversive movement in competitive feeding situation [26]. The purpose of the present study was to specify the temporal dynamics of the relationship between theta activity and sensory and locomotor events in a task in which auditory cues initiate and inhibit approach. Specifically, the question was whether a theta response to a cue was time-locked and could be described as rhythm resetting or synchronization, or alternately had a more graded modulated pattern. An analogous question was asked of theta patterns related to the onset of approach locomotion. The focus of the study was the theta response to cues that inhibit approach locomotion. Although the hippocampus appears to be involved in the processing of simple appetitive cues eliciting approach locomotion [7], a recent study [33] found no phase-related responses to auditory cues that elicited spatially directed locomotor approach. One of the earliest proposed functions [12] for the hippocampus is the inhibition of behavior. Recent considerations of this idea have implicated the hippocampus in latent inhibition [18], pre-pulse inhibition [3], the inhibition of attention to interfering stimuli [10,29], and have reinterpreted the wellestablished spatial function of the hippocampus in terms of inhibition [11]. Therefore, a cue suppressing well-learned approach locomotion would be expected to engage the encoding and control mechanisms of the hippocampus, and affect the theta generation processes related to locomotion. Rats were pre-trained in a classical conditioning task to associate an auditory cue with the delivery of a food pellet until the cue reliably produced approach locomotion to the pellet location. Next, in certain trials a delayed negating cue was superimposed on the pellet cue, and the pellet was withheld. This presentation procedure is a challenging variant of the stopsignal paradigm [13] intended to maximize active encoding. Because of its difficulty, the inhibition procedure allowed the comparison of trials in which the same negating cue was either effective or not effective in suppressing approach locomotion. 1. Methods and materials 1.1. Subjects and surgery All surgical and testing procedures were approved by the Wesleyan Animal Welfare Committee. Five Male Sprague Dawley rats bred at Wesleyan University were housed on a 12:12 reversed light cycle in individual cages with ad libitum access to water. They were provided a daily food pellet ration to produce a weekly weight gain of g up to a weight of 400 g. After preliminary training in the test chamber, the rats were anesthetized by intraperitoneal injections of Nembutal (40 mg/kg). During surgery, anesthesia was supplemented as needed by 10 mg/kg intraperitoneal injections of Nembutal. Eight 1-mm holes were drilled into the skull to receive anchor screws and electrodes. The electrodes were twisted pairs of Teflon-insulated stainless steel wires (125- m diameter) with a vertical tip separations of 1.5 mm. They were placed bilaterally in the dorsal hippocampus at approximately 4.0 mm posterior to bregma, 3.0 mm lateral to the midline, and at various depths but with the superficial pole always dorsal to CA1 cell layer. An uninsulated copper wire was wrapped around the anchor screws to serve as ground connection. The electrode wire terminated in Amphenol pins that were inserted into a plastic strip secured to the skull with dental cement. The incision was infiltrated with Marcaine (0.5%), treated with topical antibiotics, and closed with a wound clip. The rat was returned to ad libitum feeding for 5 days before resuming training Apparatus Test chamber The test chamber, illustrated in Fig. 1, had a floor 61 cm 25 cm and sidewalls 35 cm high that slanted outward 15. The front wall (panel B) contained an acrylic window for the video camera (Hitachi Denshi KP-M2U, 6-mm lens). A stainless steel tube protruded from a lower corner of the window to deliver a 45-mg food pellet to a circular tray recessed in the floor. The rat initiated the trial by using its forepaw to depress a clear acrylic treadle (3.5 cm 24 cm) centered in the floor 24 cm in front of the front wall (panel A). In the up position, the treadle protruded 1.3 cm above the floor, and an embedded light emitting diode (LED) was lit. Depression of the treadle extinguished the LED, and raised a dipper through an access hole in the floor to present 0.1 cc of sweetened condensed milk and water (1:1 by volume) for 1.36 s. When the dipper retracted, a 4- khz tone at 65 db was produced for 0.7 s by a speaker located above the camera window (panel B). On pellet trials (panel C), the pellet dispenser was activated at the offset of the tone resulting in the presentation of the pellet approximately 0.7 s later. On negation trials, the speaker above the pellet cue speaker produced clicks (60 Hz) beginning 0.3 s after the onset and continuing for its duration; no pellet was delivered on these trials (panel D). The rat restarted the trial by turning away from the pellet area and entering the rear of stall formed by two acrylic walls that lead to the treadle (panel A) Video recording A frame grabber board (Data Translation 3152) captured single monochrome video fields of pixels at rate of 50 fields/s. The video acquisition and storage was controlled by a custom program (BEProbe) running on a standard PC (Dell Optiplex GT110). The program is available in binary executable and Visual Basic 6 source code form at Each 5.12-s trial was associated with 256 frames Analog recordings A cable equipped with two dual operational amplifiers (LMC 6482) configured as voltage followers was connected to the strip on the rat s head. The other end terminated at a 9-channel slip-ring mounted on a counterbalanced arm. The paired outputs of the operational amplifiers were led to Grass P15 differential amplifiers

3 238 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Fig. 1. Schematic illustration of the test apparatus. In the self-paced trial, the rat entered the stall to depress an illuminated treadle and lapped at a milk dipper presented for 1.36 s at a hole in the floor. On pellet trials (panel C), the retraction of the dipper was simultaneous with the onset of a 0.7-s, 4-kHz tone from the lower speaker. At the offset of the tone, a dispenser was activated and a food pellet exited the delivery tube to rest in the recessed tray approximately 0.7 s later. The rat could initiate locomotor approach to the pellet either during or after the pellet cue. After obtaining the pellet, the rat entered the rear of the stall to start another trial. On approximately half of the trials, a negating cue (clicks from the upper speaker) was superimposed on the pellet cue starting at 0.3 (panel D) and no pellet was dispensed. Correct behavior on these trials was suppression of locomotor approach to the pellet area and a direct turn to the rear of the stall. The depression of the treadle triggered two synchronized computers to acquire the data. One stored the current 64 video frames in a circulating buffer and then stored the subsequent 192 frames; the other acquired the parallel analog measures at a sampling rate of 1 khz, and stored them at a sampling interval of 100 ms. and low and high half-amplitude filter settings at 1 and 30 Hz. A Microstar DAP 2400 A/D board mounted in a separate computer sampled these signals at 1 khz, and to reduce the data storage demands, the digitized values were averaged over 10 samples to yield an effective sampling rate of 100 Hz. An Analog Devices ADXL05 accelerometer was mounted near the rat end of the recording cable. The single axis was oriented so that forward and upward movements produced upward deflections. Two force transducers (WP Instruments) mounted under a movable section of the floor in the stall registered the rat s presence in the stall. These signals and the various event markers were sampled similarly to the hippocampal activity. The video and A/D recording acquisition systems used continuously updated buffers containing 1.28 s of data. When the rat depressed the treadle, the two acquisition systems stored the data from their buffers and then stored the next 3.84 s of their respective data. For each 20-ms video frame there were two values for each recording channel. Each was the mean of 101-kHz samples, one corresponded to the first 10-ms period and the other corresponded to the second 10-ms period.

4 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Procedure After surgery, the rats were pre-trained in multiple (range 9 19), 20-min sessions, each approximately 40 trials, to enter the stall, depress the treadle, lap the milk, and approach the pellet area on pellet cue trials. To strengthen the association between the pellet cue and the pellet presentation, a portion of the trials were doubledipper trials in which the pellet cue was omitted, and instead of a pellet delivery, the dipper was presented again. The number and density of double dipper trials was customized for each rat. Pretraining was complete when the rats consistently approached the pellet area on trials with the pellet cues and consistently remained at the dipper during its absence on the double dipper trials. Subsequent recording sessions (range 12 19) of trials with both pellet cues and negated pellet cues provided the data for the study. The rats found it difficult to withhold approach to the pellet area on negated trials. To facilitate training, trials with negated pellet cues after the first session were presented consecutively until locomotion to the pellet area was suppressed. Negated cue trials were limited to approximately 1/3 of the total trials in a session to avoid generally extinguishing the approach behavior to the pellet cue. Recordings from approximately 24 trials were stored for each session; half were from negated cue trials and included trials in which the rat both incorrectly approached the pellet area and correctly suppressed locomotion Histology The rat was given a lethal dose of Nembutal and perfused through the heart with normal saline followed by 10% Formalin. After several weeks of additional fixation, the brain was sectioned transversely every 100 m with a vibratome. Unstained sections were viewed with a microscope at 40 magnification. Recording sites were localized with reference to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson [28] Analysis of hippocampal activity Peak-by-peak measures of amplitude and frequency were used to relate theta activity to the onset of locomotor and stimulus events. The hippocampal record was transformed into standard scores, band pass filtered without phase lag between 4 and 14 Hz, and twice smoothed with a period 3 running average. Filtering, without phase lag, was implemented on acquired signals by performing Fourier transforms on the records for each s trial, setting the appropriate coefficients to 0, and performing the inverse transform. Positive peaks (relative positivity at the deeper electrode) above a selectable threshold were detected and their amplitudes determined. Detection accuracy was checked visually and corrected manually. The peak amplitude and inter-peak interval values were aperiodic and to make these records compatible with 10-ms sampling period of the analog records, they were interpolated. For the interpolation, the amplitude and interval values of a peak were replicated for each 10-ms sampling period up to the next peak. The onsets and offsets of behavioral events were determined (time marked) primarily by inspecting replays of video frame sequences, and as needed by inspecting the trajectories of manually digitized points representing the nose, eyes and forepaws of the rat. The onset times provided indexes for excerpting the analog records and generating peri-event averages. Averages of hippocampal slow wave activity were considered to show phasing relationships or synchronization if the peaks of the average exceeded 2 standard errors of the mean (S.E.M.). Because averages of non-synchronous signals approach 0, plots of the average along with ±2S.E.M. provides a graphic approximation of consecutive 1-sided t-tests of the null hypothesis of no synchrony at a significance level <0.05. Averages of peak-by-peak measures were evaluated for trends around locomotor initiation by repeated measures analysis of variance comparing values at 400, 200 ms, and the values at +200 and +400 to the values at 0 ms. For trends around the onset of the cues where more resolution was needed, comparisons were made at intervals of 100 ms. Comparisons at specific time points were made by paired t tests. 2. Results 2.1. Overview Each of the five rats had bilateral electrodes which had a superficial pole dorsal to the CA1 cell layer of the dorsal hippocampus, and a deeper pole at variable depths, including the hippocampal fissure, CA4, CA3 dendritic area, and dendritic region of the dentate gyrus. The analysis here was based on one bipolar electrode selected for each rat as the most consistent theta through the trial. Representative hippocampal activity is shown in panel A of Fig. 2 which also shows the key behavioral and stimulus events in a pellet cue trial. Positive peaks (deeper electrode relatively positive) of hippocampal activity filtered in the theta band are indicated in panel B, and the interpolated peak amplitudes and interpeak intervals are shown in panels C and D. The 5.12-s trial included 256 frames each of 20-ms duration. In the first frame shown (50) the rat entered the rear of the stall (note depression in force plate record, panel J), and began the approach to the treadle to start a trial. At frame 62, the rat s right forelimb (Rt FL) contacted the treadle (panel K) and the rat oriented to begin lapping (panel E) at the milk dipper (panel N). The pellet cue occurred (panel M) at the retraction of the dipper. The rat initiated locomotor approach to the pellet with an upward head movement (frame 152) that was apparent in the single frame inspection and well reflected in the accelerometer record (panel F). The initiation was not well reflected in the horizontal movement (panels G and H). Within few frames, the accelerating rightward head movement appeared in the horizontal velocity (panel H) and later in the lower resolution horizontal displacement (panel G). Approximately 200 ms later, the rat lifted the left forelimb (arrow, frame 162) to begin the stepping phase of the approach to the pellet (frame 167). Fig. 3 shows a representative trial with a negated pellet cue in which the rat suppressed locomotion to the pellet area. In addition the rat showed withdrawal from the treadle, a pattern appearing only in negated cue trials. After contacting (frame 60) and depressing the treadle, the rat lapped (panel E) at the milk dipper (panel N). After the dipper retracted and the pellet cue occurred (panel M), the rat continued to lap at the access hole (panel E). The rat responded to the negating

5 240 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Fig. 2. Representative pellet trial showing behavioral measures, hippocampal activity measures, and stimulus events. Five video frames selected from the 256 frames in the 5.12-s trial show behaviors leading to the approach to the pellet (Rt FL, right forelimb). (Panel A) Hippocampal activity high pass filtered at 4 Hz. (Panel B) positive peaks in smoothed band pass (4 14 Hz) activity. (Panels C and D) Peak-by-peak amplitude of positive peaks and inter-peak interval, values interpolated from left to right. (Panel E) Manually digitized record of tongue protrusions at the milk dipper. (Panel F) Accelerometer trace, upward and forward movements produce upward deflections. (Panels G and H) Manually digitized trajectory and velocity of the left eye on the horizontal plane. (Panel J) Force plate in stall, note the depression when the rat enters the stall prior to frame 50 and the elevation when the rat has left to approach the pellet around frame 167. (Panels K P) Stimulus event markers. cue (panel P) by making a rightward head movement (HM, frame 158), followed by a succession of steps (frames 187, 199, 220) back into the stall reflected in the depression in the force plate record (panel J). Suppressing locomotion to the pellet area on the negated pellet cue trials was difficult, and no rat showed complete mastery of the task. In pretraining the rats had been presented with pellet cues exclusively associated with the pellets, and in the negated cue recording sessions, the majority of trials involved non-negated pellet cues followed by pellet delivery. Despite the difficulty, all rats showed indications that they were attending to the negating cue and responding appropriately with suppression of approach locomotion on some trials. Fig. 4, panel A shows the proportion of trials with negated pellet cues in which the rats approached the pellet area. In the first session, the rats were generally unresponsive to the negating cue, persisting in approach, but by the last three sessions, all had increased the proportion of trials without approach (median = 0.51, range: ). When the pellet cue was not negated, all rats continued to approach the pellet area on virtually every trial. Panel B shows that four of the five rats showed an increase in backward locomotion from the treadle on negated cue trials. Withdrawal behavior never occurred on non-negated trials. It persisted

6 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Fig. 3. Representative negated cue trial. The selected video frames show suppression of locomotor approach to the pellet and withdrawal from the treadle during the negating cue (Rt FL, right forelimb; HM, head movement, Lt FL, left forelimb). Other panels as in Fig. 2 except panel (P) which indicates a negating cue. despite the delay it caused in the starting of the next trial, as the rat could enable the treadle only by returning to the front of the stall, locomoting around the stall, and entering it from the rear. Panel C shows redirected locomotion, another behavior that only appeared on negated pellet cue trials. It was a short-latency approach that veered away from the pellet area during the negating cue. Redirected locomotion increased in frequency for all rats with continued experience with the negating cue. The absence of complete locomotor suppression with the negated cue was useful because it provided trials in which the negating cue was not effective for comparison to trials in which the cue suppressed locomotion. The qualitative indexes illustrated in Fig. 4 were more effective in showing the development of locomotor suppression by the negating cue than was the latency of locomotor initiation. It was anticipated that after pretraining with the pellet cue alone, the rats would initiate locomotion within 0.3 ms of the cue and then after experience with the negating cue, they would progressively delay locomotion until the negating period had passed. Contrary to expectation, all rats showed a variety of latencies throughout the training with the negating cue Hippocampal activity related to pellet and negating cues To relate hippocampal activity to locomotion and cues in this task, trials with similar locomotor behaviors were combined across sessions. Thus, pellet cues were classified in terms of the time that the locomotion to the pellet occurred.

7 242 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Fig. 4. Increase in behaviors related to locomotor suppression in five subjects with experience with a negating cue superposed on a pellet cue. Comparison to behavior in the first session with the negating cue to the mean of the last three sessions. (A) Approach locomotion to the pellet area. (B) Backward locomotion from the treadle (withdrawal). (C) Redirected locomotion, shortlatency approach that veered away from the pellet area during the negating cue. Locomotion initiated within 0.3 s of the onset of the pellet cue, i.e., prior to the time that the negating cue could have occurred, represents maximal locomotor activation by the pellet cue. In Fig. 5, pellet cues with this short latency locomotion are represented by the thick lines. In panels E and F, the accelerometer (Accel) (thick lines) show the rise associated with the locomotor head movement first appearing during the period before negation period (arrow) and becoming more prominent during the negation period. The force plate traces rose later as the subsequent stepping of the hindlimbs moved the rat out of the stall. The short latency trials were compared to trials in which locomotion appeared at longer latencies, i.e., during the negation period (panels A and C), and after the negation period (panels B and D). Locomotion occurring during the negation period (in the absence of the negating cue) reflects weaker activation of locomotor initiation; it is represented on the left side of Fig. 5. Locomotion after the offset of the pellet cue reflects the weakest activation of locomotor activation; it is represented on the right side of Fig. 5. The values at 100-ms intervals for each of the slower locomotion conditions were compared to the fast locomotion condition by two-way analysis of variance, with repeated measures over time, followed by individual paired t tests. Theta amplitude progressively rose within the 0.5 s period prior to all pellet cues. During this time, the dipper was present and the rats continued to lap the milk. As shown in Fig. 5, panel A, when locomotion started prior to the negating period, the pre-cue amplitude rise continued steeply into the pellet cue period to reach a maximum associated with rapid phase of the locomotion. When the locomotion was initiated during the negation period, the pre-cue rise continued more slowly (panel A), and when the locomotion was initiated after the pellet cue, the rise in amplitude stopped and maintained a lower level throughout the pellet cue (panel B). Inter-peak intervals decreased prior to onset of the cues that elicited shorter latency locomotion either before or during the negation period (Fig. 5, panel C). The pre-onset decline was not present when the locomotion occurred after the cue offset (panel D). The inter-peak intervals prior to cues which elicited locomotion during the negation period (panel C) were generally lower but the differences were small and not significant for any of the individual comparisons. After the onset of the pellet cue, inter-peak interval decreased according to the elicited locomotor patterns. When locomotion started prior to the negating period (panel C), the decline that begun prior to cue onset continued. When locomotion started during the negating period, the decline reversed and then resumed (panel C). When locomotion started after the pellet cue offset, the decline was not apparent during the pellet cue. These patterns indicate that theta amplitude generally, and theta frequency more selectively, increased prior to an expected cue that elicited locomotion. After onset of the cue, the time course of theta amplitude and frequency tracked the execution of locomotion. For negated pellet cues, trials in which locomotion to the pellet area was absent reflects maximal suppression of locomotor initiation. They provide the reference and are represented by heavy lines in Fig. 6. Trials with locomotion starting after the negating cue reflects lesser locomotor suppression and are represented on the right side of Fig. 6. Trials with locomotion starting during the negating cue reflect least locomotor suppression and are represented on the left side of Fig. 6. Similar to the pattern found for non-negated pellet cues, amplitude rose during the 0.5-s period prior to the negated cues regardless of whether locomotion was suppressed, or whether it started during the negating cue (panel A) or after it (panel B). When locomotion was suppressed, the rise stopped prior to the negating cue and remained level throughout the negating cue. When locomotion was incorrectly initiated during the negating cue, the pattern was similar but there was small continuation of the rise into the negation period (panel A). When incorrect locomotion occurred after the offset of the negating cue, amplitude maintained a level throughout the negation cue that was lower than the suppressed locomotion case. Note that this pattern is a reverse of the general positive association between amplitude and locomotion. Inter-peak interval did not show pre-onset trends prior to the negated pellet cues, but it was generally lower when locomotion was suppressed (panels C and D). The magnitude of the difference was small, with only one of the individual comparisons significant (panel D). However, the pre-onset inter-peak intervals on suppressed trials were also low compared to the trials with non-negated cues eliciting short latency locomotion (F(1,76) = 17.70, P < 0.001; Fig. 6). This pattern is a reversal of the general positive association of lower inter-peak intervals with locomotion.

8 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Fig. 5. Theta peak amplitude and inter-peak interval averaged around the onset of non-negated pellet cues that elicited locomotion at different latencies. Means of five rats. The reference condition, represented by thick lines in each panel, is the cue eliciting locomotion within 0.3 s of onset, i.e., prior to the negating period. Circles and triangles, 100-ms points of comparison. Filled, significant difference (paired t-test, P < 0.05) from the corresponding mean for the reference condition. The dashed vertical lines represent, respectively, the onset of the pellet cue, and the onset and offset of the negation period. (A) Thin line, mean theta amplitude around a pellet cue eliciting locomotion within the potential negation period at latencies ( s). Pre-onset increase over time for both cues was significant (F(9,76) = 4.21, P < 0.001) and similar in pattern (F(9,76) = 0.51, P = 0.87). Post-onset time course of two conditions differed (F(9,76) = 9.67, P < 0.001), (B) Thin line, mean theta amplitude for trials in which locomotion to the pellet area appeared after the offset of the pellet cue (after the potential negating cue). Pre-onset increase over time for both cues was significant (F(9,76) = 2.61, P = 0.01) and similar in pattern (F(9,76) = 1.06, P = 0.40). Post-onset time course of two conditions differed (F(9,76) = 4.35, P < 0.001). (C) Mean theta inter-peak intervals for cues eliciting locomotion during the negation period (thin line) compared to cues eliciting locomotion earlier (thick line). Pre-onset, the cue eliciting locomotion during the negation period was generally lower (F(1, 76) = 21.22, P < 0.001) but none of the individual differences were significant. Both cues showed a decrease leading up to onset (F(9,76) = 2.45, P = 0.01) that was similar (F(9,76) = 0.54, P = 0.83). Post-onset, the time course for the two cues differed (F(9,76) = 9.67, P < 0.001). (D) Mean inter-peak intervals for cues eliciting post-offset locomotion compared to earlier locomotion. Pre-onset, no differences between cues (F(1,76) = 0.68, P < 0.41), and no trend over time (F(9,76) = 1.60, P = 0.13). Post-onset, time course for the two cues differed (F(9,76) = 3.21, P = 0.002). (E and F) Mean accelerometer (Accel) and force plate measures for a representative rat for the three cue conditions. Arrows indicate start of rise of accelerometer trace associated with the onset of locomotion. Hippocampal slow wave activity was averaged to determine if cues which controlled locomotion produced differential evoked responses or synchronization of theta activity. Fig. 7 shows the averages for the five recording sites around the onset of pellet cues eliciting short latency locomotion (left panels) and pellet cues with a superimposed negating cue which suppressed locomotion (right panels). The vertical dashed lines indicate the pellet cue onset and the boundaries of the negating period. Consistent evoked responses to the onset of any cue were infrequent and none differentiated the cues with different locomotor responses. For example, one site (vt 52-2, panels C and D) in which the deep electrode was located in the dendritic region of the dentate gyrus showed a prominent evoked response to the onset of the pellet cue. It showed similar waveforms in response to non-negated pellet cues that elicited locomotion at various latencies and to effective and non-effective negated pellet cues. Averages for effective negating cues are shown in the right panels of Fig. 7. Synchronization of theta activity, indicated by peaks exceeding the 2S.E.M. limits, was found but relations to the properties of the cues showed no consistent pattern among the sites. Of particular interest, there were no

9 244 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Fig. 6. Theta peak amplitude and inter-peak interval averaged around the onset of a pellet cue with a superimposed negating cue. Means of five rats, format similar to Fig. 5.The reference condition represented by the thick lines includes trials in which the negating cue effectively suppressed locomotion to the pellet area. (A) Averaged peak amplitude around cues in which incorrect locomotion occurred during the negating cue (thin line) compared to cues with correctly suppressed locomotion (thick line). Pre-onset, cues did not differ (F(1,76) = 0.62, P = 0.43), and showed increasing trends (F(9,76) = 2.47, P = 0.01) that were similar (F(9,76) = 1.48, P = 0.17). Post-onset, overall differences between cues not significant (F(1,76) = 0.07, P = 0.80), and the trend over time (F(9,76) = 2.47, P = 0.01) was similar (F(9,76) = 1.48, P = 0.17). (B) Averaged peak amplitude averaged around cues in which incorrect locomotion occurred after the offset of the negated cue (thin line) compared to cues with correctly suppressed locomotion (thick line). Pre-onset, cues did not differ (F(1,76) = 0.44, P = 0.51), and showed increasing trends (F(9,76) = 8.47, P < 0.001) that were similar (F(9,76) = 0.35, P = 0.96). Post-onset, amplitude generally lower when locomotion occurred after the cue (F(1,76) = 21.15, P < 0.001), and no significant trend over time (F(9,76) = 1.53, P = 0.15). (C) Averaged inter-peak interval for cues with incorrect locomotion during the negating cue (thin line). Pre-onset, inter-peak interval generally lower when locomotion was suppressed (F(1,76) = 12.95, P < 0.001) and neither cue showed significant trend (F(9,76) = 1.52, P = 0.16). Post-onset, the trend over time for two conditions differed (F(9,76) = 4.93, P < 0.001). (D) Averaged inter-peak interval for cues with incorrect locomotion after the negated cue (thin line). Pre-onset, inter-peak interval generally lower when locomotion was suppressed (F(1,76) = 5.38, P = 0.02) and neither cue showed significant trend (F(9,76) = 0.65, P = 0.75). Post-onset, inter-peak interval generally lower when locomotion was suppressed (F(1,76) = 7.04, P = 0.009), and both conditions showed a decline (F(9,76) = 2.35, P = 0.02) that was similar (F(9,76) = 0.42, P = 0.92). (E and F) Averaged accelerometer (Accel) and force plate traces for one rat. Arrows indicate start of rise of accelerometer traces associated with the onset of locomotion. indications that the onset of the effective negating cue produced a resetting or synchronization of the theta pattern. The right panels of Fig. 7 show these averages. One site (vt62-1, panel K) showed synchronization during the locomotor suppression by the negating cue. However, the synchronization started prior to the onset of the negating cue, and also appeared prior to correct and incorrect locomotion. Finally, averages around the negating cues producing withdrawal from the treadle, arguably the most extreme suppressive response, showed no indication of phase relations. These patterns indicate that the cues controlling the initiation and suppression of locomotion in this task did not produce differential evoked responses or synchronization of theta activity Hippocampal activity related to locomotor initiation Locomotor approaches to the pellet area were classified according the type of cue (non-negated and negated) and according to latency (prior to, during and following the negating period). The various locomotor bouts produced simi-

10 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Fig. 7. Averaged hippocampal activity (high pass filtered 4 Hz) around the onset of pellet cues eliciting short latency locomotion (left panels) and pellet cues with a superimposed negating cue that suppressed locomotion (right panels). The points above and below the averages indicate 2 standard errors of the mean (S.E.M.) above and below 0. (A K) Each row represents averages for one recording site for the two conditions. (L and M) Accelerometer (Accel) and force plate averages for rat vt62. The vertical dashed lines indicate the onset of the pellet cue, and the negating period. lar accelerometer and force plate recordings (Fig. 8, panels L Q). The leftmost panels of Fig. 8 show theta amplitude and inter-peak intervals around the shortest latency locomotion which was initiated prior to the negating period. Theta amplitude increased (panel A) and inter-peak interval decreased (panel F) in the 0.5 s period prior to the onset of this rapid onset locomotion, and the trends continued during the execution. All types of locomotion showed the general pattern of an increase in amplitude (panels B E) and a decrease in inter-peak interval (panels G K) during execution. These patterns indicate that amplitude and frequency of theta activity did not differentiate between correct and incorrect locomotion in this task. The pre-locomotor decrease in inter-peak interval was found for only the short-latency locomotion elicited prior to the negation period. Note that the period prior to this short-latency locomotion overlapped the period prior to the onset of the pellet cue. As shown in Fig. 5, the amplitude rise and inter-peak interval decline prior to this type of approach began in the period prior to the pellet cue. In Fig. 8, the time courses of the changes in amplitude and inter-peak interval were similar for the various types of locomotion. Amplitude peaked closely in time with the maximal acceleration of the locomotor approach and interpeak interval reached a minimum at a later point. This lag of inter-peak interval relative to amplitude was tested by comparing the times of the peaks in the accelerometer record, theta amplitude, and theta inter-peak interval. For each rat, the three values were averaged for the five types of locomotor bouts. Fig. 9 shows that the maximum in the accelerometer record appeared at approximately 150 ms after the onset of the locomotor head movement. Theta amplitude reached its maximum less than 50 ms later which was not significantly longer (t(4) = 1.19, P = 0.30). Inter-peak interval reached a minimum approximately 300 ms after the start of locomotion which was later than the accelerometer maximum (t(4) = 6.97, P = 0.002) and the amplitude maximum (t(4) = 2.62, P = 0.06). The differences were consistent for all of the recording sites. Hippocampal slow wave activity averaged around the onsets of the various classes of locomotor approach were examined for indications of phase relations, synchronization or resetting of theta activity. Fig. 10 provides examples for two of the recording sites. Site vt52-2 which displayed the largest evoked response to the pellet cue onset is illustrated in panels A E. It showed a synchronized pattern following the onset of correct approach (panel D) and incorrect approach (panel E) to the pellet area but no sustained pattern in the other conditions. Site vt62-1 which had theta records illustrated in Figs. 1 and 2 showed no indication of synchronization during any of the locomotor types. Overall, initiation of locomotion in this task was not associated with phase-related changes in theta activity that were consistent across recording sites or consistently differentiated between the classes of locomotion. 3. Discussion This study determined how hippocampal theta activity related to cue events and locomotion in a task involving both

11 246 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Fig. 8. Theta peak amplitude and inter-peak interval averaged around onset of the onset of the locomotor head movement initiating the approach to the pellet area. Mean of five recording sites. The left panels (A, F, and L) represent short-latency (<0.3 s) locomotion initiated prior to the potential negating cue. Panels B, G, and M: locomotion on pellet trials initiated correctly during the period when the negating cue could have occurred but did not. Panels C, H, and N: incorrect locomotion initiated during the negating cue. Panels D, J and P: locomotion correctly initiated after the offset of non-negated pellet cue. Panels E, K, and Q: incorrect locomotion initiated after a negated pellet cue. The average traces are bracketed by ±1 standard error traces. Filled circles indicate significant differences before or after the onset tested by analysis of variance at 200-ms intervals. (A) Pre-onset: F(2,8) = 8.50, P = 0.01; (B) pre-onset: F(2,8) = 5.84, P = 0.03; (C) pre-onset: F(2,8) = 8.46, P = 0.01; (D) pre-onset: F(2,8) = 11.82, P = 0.004; (E) pre-onset: F(2,8) = 6.21, P = 0.024; (F) pre-onset: F(2,8) = 4.38, P = 0.05; post-onset, F(2,8) = 20.95, P = 0.001; (G) post-onset: F(2,8) = , P < 0.001; (H) post-onset: F(2,8) = 24.28, P < 0.001; (J) post-onset: F(2,8) = 30.36, P < 0.001; (K) Post-onset: F(2,8) = 21.58, P = (Panels L Q) Accelerometer and force plate records averaged for one rat (vt62). Number of trials for the five rats in each condition: (A and F) 47, 73, 59, 108, 37; (B and G) 95, 45, 57, 21, 39; (C and H) 47, 56, 35, 19, 55; (D and J) 34, 25, 13, 11, 24; (E and K) 23, 40, 29, 14, 21. Fig. 9. Comparison of times following the onset of locomotion for the maximum in the accelerometer record, the maximum in theta peak amplitude and the minimum in the theta inter-peak interval. Each data point is the mean (±1S.E.M.) of five rats. The value for each rat was the mean collapsed over the five types of locomotor bouts. The number of trials for each case is given in the caption for Fig. 8. the initiation and suppression of well-learned approach locomotion. The absence of specific responses to the cue consistently associated with a food pellet was not surprising. However, the putative role of the hippocampus in behavioral inhibition suggested that theta activity would selectively respond if the pellet cue was negated by a delayed superimposed cue never associated with the pellet. The theta rhythm did not synchronize either to the pellet cue when it could be negated or to the negating cue itself. Theta activity was present throughout the trial although amplitude and frequency were relatively low during milk lapping. During milk lapping, starting at approximately 0.5 s prior to the onset of pellet cues, theta amplitude and frequency showed anticipatory changes. Average amplitude increased prior to all cue conditions but frequency increased only when a nonnegated pellet elicited locomotion at shorter latencies. Following the onset of the cues, both theta peak amplitude and frequency depended on whether and when locomotion was executed or suppressed. Both amplitude and frequency increased with the execution of locomotion, but increases in

12 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) Fig. 10. Averaged hippocampal activity (high pass filtered 4 Hz) around the onset of locomotion. Locomotion conditions same as Fig. 8. Recording sites and format same as Fig. 7. theta peak amplitude peaked earlier. These results indicate that changes in theta patterns in this inhibition task were not closely time-locked to the stimulus or behavioral events. Rather, they appeared to be graded modulations in amplitude and frequency related to the preparation for, and execution of, movement. Consistent with related work [33], theta activity was nearly continuous throughout the trial with modulations of amplitude and frequency during the various behavioral sequences in the trial, which included approach to the treadle, lapping of the milk, orientation and locomotor approach to the pellet area. Although the theta rhythm did not reset or consistently synchronize with any of the stimulus or locomotor events, amplitude and frequency changed prior to the cue onsets, and therefore theta activity was sensitive to the features of the task. A recent study [42] that found changes in theta power corresponding to transitions in instrumental behavior generally similar to the present patterns also did not report cue related phasic changes in theta activity. The factors required for a cue to reset or synchronize theta activity appear not to be present in this situation even though it incorporated several of the information processing functions proposed for the hippocampus. When optimally performing the task, the rat withheld approach at the onset of the pellet cue, waited for a possible negating cue, and selected either an approach or an alternative to it. Performance would seem to involve attention, working memory, inhibitory control, and response selection. Moreover, the absence of complete mastery of this difficult task makes it reasonable to infer that active information processing occurred throughout the recording sessions. It seems that these factors are not sufficient for an approach cue or a negating cue to reset or synchronize the theta rhythm. The specification of factors that differentiate tasks in which cues produce resetting ([16,17,24] and tasks in which cues do not [33,42] will further understanding the function of theta activity. The negating cue used here was behaviorally effective and the absence of resetting can not be due to its lack of salience. Other types of inhibitory cues, perhaps those requiring working memory [17], would produce a resetting of theta activity. A factor that could work against producing theta resetting tendency to develop response predispositions. The rats frequently appeared to enter a trial with a movement program pre-selected on the basis of the outcomes of recent trials. Tasks structured to minimize the opportunity to pre-program responses might minimize theta activity prior to the cue onset and accordingly increase the likelihood of resetting. Vinogradova [39] has proposed that sustained theta associated with focused attention is resistant to resetting and represents the filtering out of distracting information. Another factor possibly working against resetting is that a rise in theta amplitude (and in some trials frequency) occurred prior to the onset of the predictable pellet cue. If cue onset were unpredictable, the increase in theta amplitude or frequency might be sufficiently abrupt to be characterized as resetting. Until research supports these conjectures, the available evidence leads to the conclusion that theta activity during performance of a behavioral inhibition task is modulated by processes that have relatively low time resolution. At least in this task, theta activity did not seem to reflect the

13 248 H.M. Sinnamon / Behavioural Brain Research 160 (2005) processing of higher time resolution phasic events like cue onset. Theta amplitude rose prior to the onset of the pellet cues that elicited locomotion at short and long latencies and prior to negated pellet cues which suppressed locomotion. Therefore, the pre-onset amplitude trends did not predict the behavior evoked by the cue. The rise in amplitude appeared during continuous milk lapping, and appears to reflect expectancy of cue onset rather than specific movements. The half-second period in which it occurred corresponds to approximately two to three theta cycles at the frequency typical during lapping of milk. This finding is consistent with the idea that the theta rhythm is involved in the coding of sensory information that is relevant to preparation for movement. In cats tested in an omitted stimulus paradigm, rising expectancy was associated with an amplitude increase in the theta components of the response to a cue that predicted the onset of a significant period [2]. In rats, theta power increases appeared prior to a bar press which initiated discriminatory cues to which the rats were uncertain of the appropriate response but not when the cues were well learned [42]. In the present study, theta frequency also rose during the pre-onset period but only in trials in which a nonnegated pellet elicited locomotion at shorter latencies. Frequency did not rise prior to the cue onset in trials in which the cue elicited locomotion at longer latencies or when the negating cue effectively suppressed approach. It may therefore reflect the activation of initiation processes that would antagonize suppression. Overall, the patterns indicate that the rise in theta amplitude reflected a general preparation to process the cue [42], and the rise in theta frequency reflected the readiness to respond to the cue with locomotion [4]. The changes in theta amplitude and frequency that appeared prior to the onset of the pellet cue reflected both the structure and the ordering of the trials. The trials were structured so that a fixed period of milk lapping preceded the onset of the pellet cue. It was effective in producing relative immobility and a low baseline level of theta activity. With the dipper fixed in duration and always terminating with the onset a pellet cue, the rats could predict the onset of the cue. The detection of rises in theta amplitude and frequency that correlated with the anticipation of the cue onset was facilitated by the low baseline theta activity during milk lapping. In a related study [33] that also used predictable locomotor cues but did not find these pre-onset trends, overt orienting behavior was present and baseline theta activity was higher than the present study. Following the onset of the cues, the patterns of theta peak amplitude and frequency depended on whether locomotion was executed or suppressed. Both amplitude and frequency increased with the execution of locomotion. When the latency was short, the locomotor-related trends continued those begun prior to the onset of the pellet cue. When the locomotion was delayed, the pre-onset trends leveled until locomotor onset. One finding did not fit this straightforward pattern: amplitude was maintained at a higher level on trials with locomotor suppression compared to trials with incorrect locomotion after the offset of the cue. With the inhibition of conditioned approach, various search behaviors emerge [35], and it is likely that these alternatives to locomotion also would be associated with theta activity. Further studies of locomotor suppression, should differentiate the suppression of approach locomotion associated with immobility versus that associated with alternative active behaviors such as orienting and the redirected locomotion which restarts the trial. With the execution of locomotor approach, the time courses of increases in theta peak amplitude and forward acceleration were similar whereas the increase in theta frequency peaked later. The lag of frequency relative to amplitude corresponded to approximately one theta cycle. Different sensitivity and recovery time courses for theta amplitude and frequency after reversible lesions have been described [6,27]. Differences in the short-term time courses of the two theta parameters apparently has not been explicitly reported, but a similar pattern is apparent in the time course of theta activity with locomotor onset described in the cat [1]. Several features of the amplitude frequency difference need to be clarified. One is the degree to which the times of the peaks are determined by the kinematics of the locomotion. In this study, the locomotor episodes were of short duration, involving only a few steps to reach the pellet, and therefore the apparent earlier peak in amplitude could reflect a greater sensitivity of amplitude to deceleration. Another possibility is that amplitude reduction corresponds to the arrival at the goal of the approach [42]. A fundamental question for further work is how amplitude and frequency covary during the execution of a range of behavior patterns. In this study, amplitude increases appeared prior to the onset of cues regardless of their behavioral effects, whereas pre-onset increases in frequency were related to the subsequent behavior. Combined with this finding, the temporal priority of amplitude over frequency with the execution of locomotion suggests different functions for amplitude and frequency modulation. Consistent with sensory-oriented theories [29,39] and recent findings [42], the anticipatory changes in amplitude are consistent with a role in attention and in processing antecedent information such as feed-forward signals. Consistent with the sensorimotor theory of Bland and Oddie [4], the lagging changes in frequency are consistent with a role in processing movement-related information such as reafference, feedback, and consequences. In general, the patterns observed suggest that theta amplitude and frequency might differentiate along a cognitive-motor dimension. Acknowledgements Supported by a Wesleyan University Program grant. Thanks to Esther Schlegel, Leah Pransky, Seth Shipman, Bruce Strickland, David Boule, and Greg Pare for contributions to this work.

Hippocampal Theta Activity and Behavioral Sequences in a Reward-Directed Approach Locomotor Task

Hippocampal Theta Activity and Behavioral Sequences in a Reward-Directed Approach Locomotor Task Hippocampal Theta Activity and Behavioral Sequences in a Reward-Directed Approach Locomotor Task H.M. Sinnamon* HIPPOCAMPUS 15:518 534 (2005) ABSTRACT: Hippocampal rhythmic slow wave activity (theta) has

More information

Summary of behavioral performances for mice in imaging experiments.

Summary of behavioral performances for mice in imaging experiments. Supplementary Figure 1 Summary of behavioral performances for mice in imaging experiments. (a) Task performance for mice during M2 imaging experiments. Open triangles, individual experiments. Filled triangles,

More information

Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals

Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals Nissl stained coronal or horizontal sections were made from kainate injected (right) and saline injected (left) animals at different

More information

Supplementary Figure 1

Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Miniature microdrive, spike sorting and sleep stage detection. a, A movable recording probe with 8-tetrodes (32-channels). It weighs ~1g. b, A mouse implanted with 8 tetrodes in

More information

CROSSMODAL PLASTICITY IN SPECIFIC AUDITORY CORTICES UNDERLIES VISUAL COMPENSATIONS IN THE DEAF "

CROSSMODAL PLASTICITY IN SPECIFIC AUDITORY CORTICES UNDERLIES VISUAL COMPENSATIONS IN THE DEAF Supplementary Online Materials To complement: CROSSMODAL PLASTICITY IN SPECIFIC AUDITORY CORTICES UNDERLIES VISUAL COMPENSATIONS IN THE DEAF " Stephen G. Lomber, M. Alex Meredith, and Andrej Kral 1 Supplementary

More information

Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements

Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements Y. Isomura et al. 1 Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements Yoshikazu Isomura, Rie Harukuni, Takashi Takekawa, Hidenori Aizawa & Tomoki Fukai

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more strongly when an image was negative than when the same

More information

Implanting an Adult Rat with the Single-Channel Epoch Transmitter for Recording Electrocardiogram in the Type II electrode configuration.

Implanting an Adult Rat with the Single-Channel Epoch Transmitter for Recording Electrocardiogram in the Type II electrode configuration. Implanting an Adult Rat with the Single-Channel Epoch Transmitter for Recording Electrocardiogram in the Type II electrode configuration. Recommended Surgical Tools A. Scalpel handle B. Scalpel blade (#15)

More information

Book 3: Lab Procedures Book 3: Ch. 1: The Hypothesis and Overview

Book 3: Lab Procedures Book 3: Ch. 1: The Hypothesis and Overview Book 3: Lab Procedures Book 3: Ch. 1: The Hypothesis and Overview 13 Introduction This experiment will investigate how cocaine acts on dopamine neurons in the brain. Cocaine is a drug of abuse that increases

More information

Supplementary Methods

Supplementary Methods 1 Supplementary Methods Social Preference Test Subjects Seventy-four Long-Evans, male rats served as subjects (S-rats) in the foodpreference test, with 40 assigned to the CXT-Same (CXT-S) Condition and

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Reward rate affects the decision to begin work. (a) Latency distributions are bimodal, and depend on reward rate. Very short latencies (early peak) preferentially occur when a greater

More information

PRIMING PATTERN DETERMINES THE CORRELATION BETWEEN HIPPOCAMPAL THETA ACTIVITY AND LOCOMOTOR STEPPING ELICITED BY STIMULATION IN ANESTHETIZED RATS

PRIMING PATTERN DETERMINES THE CORRELATION BETWEEN HIPPOCAMPAL THETA ACTIVITY AND LOCOMOTOR STEPPING ELICITED BY STIMULATION IN ANESTHETIZED RATS Pergamon www.elsevier.com/locate/neuroscience Theta and locomotion Neuroscience Vol. 98, No. 3, pp. 459 470, 2000 459 2000 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001

Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001 Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001 An Investigation into the Mechanisms of Memory through Hippocampal Microstimulation In rodents, the hippocampus

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Yingxue Wang, Sandro Romani, Brian Lustig, Anthony Leonardo, Eva Pastalkova Supplementary Materials Supplementary Modeling

More information

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT ACTA NEUROBIOL: EXP. 1988, 48: 117-121 Short communication LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT W. Jeffrey WILSON and Jennifer C. HALL Department of Psychological

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

The individual animals, the basic design of the experiments and the electrophysiological

The individual animals, the basic design of the experiments and the electrophysiological SUPPORTING ONLINE MATERIAL Material and Methods The individual animals, the basic design of the experiments and the electrophysiological techniques for extracellularly recording from dopamine neurons were

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Large-scale calcium imaging in vivo.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Large-scale calcium imaging in vivo. Supplementary Figure 1 Large-scale calcium imaging in vivo. (a) Schematic illustration of the in vivo camera imaging set-up for large-scale calcium imaging. (b) High-magnification two-photon image from

More information

Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque,

Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) 62 76 Differential contributions of hippocampus, amygdala and perirhinal cortex to recognition of novel objects, contextual stimuli and stimulus relationships Sandra N.

More information

Section 4. Intro to Neurophysiology

Section 4. Intro to Neurophysiology Section 4. Intro to Neurophysiology 4.1 Action potentials at work (Cockroach Receptive Fields) Overview The goals of this unit are to: 1) introduce you to the basic concepts, equipment, and methodology

More information

Quick Guide - eabr with Eclipse

Quick Guide - eabr with Eclipse What is eabr? Quick Guide - eabr with Eclipse An electrical Auditory Brainstem Response (eabr) is a measurement of the ABR using an electrical stimulus. Instead of a traditional acoustic stimulus the cochlear

More information

Protocol for Rat Sleep EEG

Protocol for Rat Sleep EEG Protocol for Rat Sleep EEG Subjects Male Spraue Dawley rats weihin 250-300 rams at the time of surery are used. Food and water are available ad libitum throuhout the experiment. Rats are roup housed prior

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1 SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Figure 1 The supralinear events evoked in CA3 pyramidal cells fulfill the criteria for NMDA spikes, exhibiting a threshold, sensitivity to NMDAR blockade, and all-or-none

More information

Nature Methods: doi: /nmeth Supplementary Figure 1. Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse.

Nature Methods: doi: /nmeth Supplementary Figure 1. Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse. Supplementary Figure 1 Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse. a. Probability distribution of firing rates across the population (notice log scale) in our data. The range of firing rates is wide but

More information

Assessment of anti-seizure properties of two proprietary compounds in the electrical kindling model of epilepsy. Date

Assessment of anti-seizure properties of two proprietary compounds in the electrical kindling model of epilepsy. Date Assessment of anti-seizure properties of two proprietary compounds in the electrical kindling model of epilepsy Date This study was conducted under the terms of a Laboratory Services Agreement between

More information

Nov versus Fam. Fam 1 versus. Fam 2. Supplementary figure 1

Nov versus Fam. Fam 1 versus. Fam 2. Supplementary figure 1 a Environment map similarity score (mean r ).5..3.2.1 Fam 1 versus Fam 2 Nov versus Fam b Environment cofiring similarity score (mean r ).7.6.5..3.2.1 Nov versus Fam Fam 1 versus Fam 2 First half versus

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

KneeAlign System Surgical Technique Guide

KneeAlign System Surgical Technique Guide KneeAlign System Surgical Technique Guide Table of Contents Step 1 System Assembly... 1 Step 2 System Assembly... 2 Step 3 System Assembly... 2 Step 4 System Assembly... 2 Step 5 Sensor Pairing... 2 Step

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Laboratory Equipment & Test Procedures. Motor learning and control historically has built its discipline through laboratory research. This has led to the

More information

Neuro-MS/D DIAGNOSTICS REHABILITATION TREATMENT STIMULATION. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulator. of motor disorders after the stroke

Neuro-MS/D DIAGNOSTICS REHABILITATION TREATMENT STIMULATION. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulator. of motor disorders after the stroke Neuro-MS/D Transcranial Magnetic Stimulator DIAGNOSTICS of corticospinal pathway pathology REHABILITATION of motor disorders after the stroke TREATMENT of depression and Parkinson s disease STIMULATION

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Forsyth et al. 10.1073/pnas.1509262112 SI Methods Inclusion Criteria. Participants were eligible for the study if they were between 18 and 30 y of age; were comfortable reading in

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

Classical Conditioning II. Generalization and Discrimination. Generalization Example. Generalization/Discrimination; Time and the Order of Events

Classical Conditioning II. Generalization and Discrimination. Generalization Example. Generalization/Discrimination; Time and the Order of Events Classical Conditioning II Generalization/Discrimination; Time and the Order of Events Generalization and Discrimination Generalization responding to a stimulus because of its similarity to the CS. Discrimination

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

TIP CARD. Model Temporary External Pacemaker

TIP CARD. Model Temporary External Pacemaker TIP CARD Model 5392 Temporary External Pacemaker PRE-USE What to do... What NOT to do... Battery Replace the battery for each new patient. Do NOT reuse battery. Physical Check case for cracks/damage.**

More information

CAREFULLY READ ALL INSTRUCTIONS PRIOR TO USE

CAREFULLY READ ALL INSTRUCTIONS PRIOR TO USE CAREFULLY READ ALL INSTRUCTIONS PRIOR TO USE INDICATIONS FOR USE The LATERA Absorbable Nasal Implant is indicated for supporting upper and lower lateral nasal cartilage. CAUTION: Federal law restricts

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Representation of negative motivational value in the primate

Representation of negative motivational value in the primate Representation of negative motivational value in the primate lateral habenula Masayuki Matsumoto & Okihide Hikosaka Supplementary Figure 1 Anticipatory licking and blinking. (a, b) The average normalized

More information

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l Journal oj Experimental Psychology 12, Vol. 63, No. 3, 289-293 REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l MAURICE HERSHENSON 2 Brooklyn College In measuring reaction time (RT) to simultaneously

More information

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT 1 Courtesy of Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Alexa Morcom Edinburgh SPM course 2017 Thanks to Rik Henson, Thomas Wolbers, Jody Culham, and the SPM authors for slides Overview Categorical designs Factorial designs

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training. Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral training. a, Mazes used for behavioral training. Asterisks indicate reward location. Only some example mazes are shown (for example, right choice and not left choice maze

More information

Supplementary Material for

Supplementary Material for Supplementary Material for Selective neuronal lapses precede human cognitive lapses following sleep deprivation Supplementary Table 1. Data acquisition details Session Patient Brain regions monitored Time

More information

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest Experimental Design Kate Watkins Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford With thanks to: Heidi Johansen-Berg Joe Devlin Outline Choices for experimental paradigm Subtraction / hierarchical

More information

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey 3.6.QH Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey Versnel, Huib; Zwiers, Marcel; Van Opstal, John Department of Biophysics University of Nijmegen Geert Grooteplein 655

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization Acoust. Sci. & Tech. 24, 2 (23) PAPER The role of low components in median plane localization Masayuki Morimoto 1;, Motoki Yairi 1, Kazuhiro Iida 2 and Motokuni Itoh 1 1 Environmental Acoustics Laboratory,

More information

Posner s Attention Test

Posner s Attention Test iworx Physiology Lab Experiment Experiment HP-18 Posner s Attention Test Background Setup Lab Note: The lab presented here is intended for evaluation purposes only. iworx users should refer to the User

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Neurobiology: The nerve cell. Principle and task To use a nerve function model to study the following aspects of a nerve cell:

Neurobiology: The nerve cell. Principle and task To use a nerve function model to study the following aspects of a nerve cell: Principle and task To use a nerve function model to study the following aspects of a nerve cell: INTRACELLULAR POTENTIAL AND ACTION POTENTIAL Comparison between low and high threshold levels Comparison

More information

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction Instructor: Paul Wolfgang Faculty sponsor: Yuan Shi, Ph.D. Andrey Mavrichev CIS 4339 Project in Computer Science May 7, 2009 Research work was completed in collaboration with Michael Tobia, Kevin L. Brown,

More information

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview Investigations in Resting State Connectivity Scott FMRI Laboratory Overview Introduction Functional connectivity explorations Dynamic change (motor fatigue) Neurological change (Asperger s Disorder, depression)

More information

CHASING WITH A MODEL EYE

CHASING WITH A MODEL EYE J. exp. Biol. 137, 399-409 (1988) 399 Printed in Great Britain The Company of Biologists Limited 1988 CHASING WITH A MODEL EYE BY STEPHEN YOUNG Department of Pure and Applied Biology, Imperial College,

More information

Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 13 Golcbia St., ?

Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 13 Golcbia St., ? The evoked cardiac response as 0.0 1 1. a runction or cognitive load in subjects differing on the individual difference variable of reaction time Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener Institute

More information

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala 11 December 1997 Nature 390, 604-607 (1997) Macmillan Publishers Ltd. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala MICHAEL T. ROGAN, URSULA V. STÄUBLI & JOSEPH E. LEDOUX

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Journal of Neuroscience Methods

Journal of Neuroscience Methods Journal of Neuroscience Methods 187 (2010) 171 182 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Neuroscience Methods journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jneumeth Intrinsic signal optical

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Attentional Blink Paradigm Attentional Blink Paradigm ATTENTIONAL BLINK 83 ms stimulus onset asychrony between all stimuli B T D A 3 N P Z F R K M R N Lag 3 Target 1 Target 2 After detection of a target in a rapid stream of visual

More information

Nord.cTracICA.Cr USER'S MANUAL A CAUTION QUESTIONS? .. Visit our website'at. new products, prizes, fitness tips, and much more!

Nord.cTracICA.Cr USER'S MANUAL A CAUTION QUESTIONS? .. Visit our website'at. new products, prizes, fitness tips, and much more! Nord.cTracICA.Cr Model No. NTCW90907.1 Serial No. Write the serial number in the space above for reference. _ USER'S MANUAL Serial Number Decal (under frame) QUESTIONS? As a manufacturer, we are committed

More information

Characterization of Sleep Spindles

Characterization of Sleep Spindles Characterization of Sleep Spindles Simon Freedman Illinois Institute of Technology and W.M. Keck Center for Neurophysics, UCLA (Dated: September 5, 2011) Local Field Potential (LFP) measurements from sleep

More information

Multipower. User Guide E S S E N T I A L S T R E N G T H

Multipower. User Guide E S S E N T I A L S T R E N G T H E L E M E N T E S S E N T I A L S T R E N G T H User Guide The identification plate of and manufacturer, affixed to the frame behind the barbell rack, gives the following details: A B C D E Name and address

More information

The Exercise Wheelchair

The Exercise Wheelchair NeuroGym Technologies Inc. The Exercise Wheelchair Product Manual NeuroGym Technologies Inc. 2012 1644 Bank Street, Suite 103 Ottawa, Ontario, Canada K1V 7Y6 Toll Free: 1-877-523=4148 www.neurogymtech.com

More information

A new method of sonograph lateral resolution measurement using PSF analysis of received signal

A new method of sonograph lateral resolution measurement using PSF analysis of received signal A new method of sonograph lateral resolution measurement using PSF analysis of received signal L. Doležal, J. Hálek Faculty of Medicine Palacký University in Olomouc, Czech Republic E-mail: ladol@tunw.upol.cz

More information

Humans make voluntary decisions to talk, walk, stand up, or sit down. The

Humans make voluntary decisions to talk, walk, stand up, or sit down. The 2 E X E R C I S E Skeletal Muscle Physiology O B J E C T I V E S 1. To define motor unit, twitch, latent period, contraction phase, relaxation phase, threshold, summation, tetanus, fatigue, isometric contraction,

More information

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal A. Project Description Objective of research: The objective of this study is to determine if hippocampal dopamine D1 receptors facilitate novel object

More information

The Technical Model: an Overview Explanation of the Technical Model

The Technical Model: an Overview Explanation of the Technical Model A Technical Model for Pole Vault Success Michael A. Young Louisiana State University ****************This is a modified version of an article published in Track Coach Pole vaulting is perhaps the most

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

A SYSTEM FOR CONTROL AND EVALUATION OF ACOUSTIC STARTLE RESPONSES OF ANIMALS

A SYSTEM FOR CONTROL AND EVALUATION OF ACOUSTIC STARTLE RESPONSES OF ANIMALS A SYSTEM FOR CONTROL AND EVALUATION OF ACOUSTIC STARTLE RESPONSES OF ANIMALS Z. Bureš College of Polytechnics, Jihlava Institute of Experimental Medicine, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Prague

More information

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions Behavioral Neuroscience 1996, Vol. 110, No. 5, 933-945 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/96/S3.00 Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal

More information

ABR assesses the integrity of the peripheral auditory system and auditory brainstem pathway.

ABR assesses the integrity of the peripheral auditory system and auditory brainstem pathway. By Prof Ossama Sobhy What is an ABR? The Auditory Brainstem Response is the representation of electrical activity generated by the eighth cranial nerve and brainstem in response to auditory stimulation.

More information

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques C O N F I D E N T I A L Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques DATE This study was conducted under the terms of a Materials Transfer and Services Agreement between NeuroDetective International and

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

TEMPERATURE and HUMIDITY TRANSDUCER SUPPLIED P18L TYPE USER S MANUAL. from a CURRENT LOOP

TEMPERATURE and HUMIDITY TRANSDUCER SUPPLIED P18L TYPE USER S MANUAL. from a CURRENT LOOP TEMPERATURE and HUMIDITY TRANSDUCER SUPPLIED from a CURRENT LOOP P18L TYPE USER S MANUAL 1 2 Contents 1. Application... 2 2. Basic requirements, operational safety... 2 3. Installation... 2 3.1. Assembly...

More information

The mammalian cochlea possesses two classes of afferent neurons and two classes of efferent neurons.

The mammalian cochlea possesses two classes of afferent neurons and two classes of efferent neurons. 1 2 The mammalian cochlea possesses two classes of afferent neurons and two classes of efferent neurons. Type I afferents contact single inner hair cells to provide acoustic analysis as we know it. Type

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Trial structure for go/no-go behavior

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Trial structure for go/no-go behavior Supplementary Figure 1 Trial structure for go/no-go behavior a, Overall timeline of experiments. Day 1: A1 mapping, injection of AAV1-SYN-GCAMP6s, cranial window and headpost implantation. Water restriction

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Correlation between Membrane Potential Responses and Tentacle Movement in the Dinoflagellate Noctiluca miliaris

Correlation between Membrane Potential Responses and Tentacle Movement in the Dinoflagellate Noctiluca miliaris ZOOLOGICAL SCIENCE 21: 131 138 (2004) 2004 Zoological Society of Japan Correlation between Membrane Potential Responses and Tentacle Movement in the Dinoflagellate Noctiluca miliaris Kazunori Oami* Institute

More information

Neuro-Audio Version 2010

Neuro-Audio Version 2010 ABR PTA ASSR Multi-ASSR OAE TEOAE DPOAE SOAE ECochG MLR P300 Neuro-Audio Version 2010 one device for all audiological tests Auditory brainstem response (ABR)/Brainstem evoked response audiometry (BERA)

More information

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS Subjects Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had previous exposure to a tone language. Subjects were divided into two groups based on musical

More information

Early Learning vs Early Variability 1.5 r = p = Early Learning r = p = e 005. Early Learning 0.

Early Learning vs Early Variability 1.5 r = p = Early Learning r = p = e 005. Early Learning 0. The temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts individual differences in motor learning ability Howard Wu *, Yohsuke Miyamoto *, Luis Nicolas Gonzales-Castro, Bence P.

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex

Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex Koji Yashiro, Thorfinn T. Riday, Kathryn H. Condon, Adam C. Roberts, Danilo R. Bernardo, Rohit Prakash, Richard J. Weinberg, Michael

More information

Lab #3: Electrocardiogram (ECG / EKG)

Lab #3: Electrocardiogram (ECG / EKG) Lab #3: Electrocardiogram (ECG / EKG) An introduction to the recording and analysis of cardiac activity Introduction The beating of the heart is triggered by an electrical signal from the pacemaker. The

More information

Psychological Research

Psychological Research Psychol Res (1984) 46:121-127 Psychological Research Springer-Verlag 1984 Research note: Peak velocity timing invariance Alan M. Wing I and Ed Miller 2 1 Medical Research Council Applied Psychology Unit,

More information

Original user manual REDCORD AXIS. «Get ready to rotate!» axis_usermanual_may2013_v3 AXIS. Page 1

Original user manual REDCORD AXIS. «Get ready to rotate!» axis_usermanual_may2013_v3 AXIS. Page 1 Original user manual axis_usermanual_may2013_v3 REDCORD AXIS «Get ready to rotate!» Page 1 AXIS Congratulations with your new Redcord AXIS Redcord AXIS is a revolutionary exercise device that takes suspension

More information

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object Supplemental materials for: The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object David Whitney*, David A. Westwood, & Melvyn A. Goodale* *Group on Action and Perception, The

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information