Talking genes the molecular basis of language impairment

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Talking genes the molecular basis of language impairment"

Transcription

1 the molecular basis of language impairment Dianne F Newbury and Anthony P Monaco University of Oxford, UK Many children acquire language so smoothly that it appears to be an innate ability. If this is true, then it should be possible to identify genes that underlie variations in linguistic abilities. For most children the process of understanding and using language seems to occur with little effort and minimal instruction. The typical five-year-old has a vocabulary of words a feat that requires an average lexical growth of nine words per day. Furthermore, in parallel to the completion of this task, they are also required to master other, more complex, aspects of language (e.g., grammar, pragmatics). The ease with which most children fulfil these expectations has led to the proposal that language is an innate instinct, enabling the acquisition of language in the absence of explicit tuition, and must, at least in part, be genetically determined (Pinker, 1994). Specific Language Impairment (SLI) For some children, the acquisition of language can prove to be a much more difficult task. In many cases these difficulties are attributed to additional developmental or medical problems (e.g., cleft lip, autism, cerebral palsy). However, there remain a small, but significant, number of children who appear to be developing normally in all other respects, but yet encounter an inexplicable disorder in the acquisition of language. These children are said to have Specific Language Impairment or SLI. Complexities of the SLI phenotype When presented in this manner the definition of SLI appears relatively straightforward. However, a diagnosis is often applied in terms of exclusionary criteria rather than by a defined set of distinct clinical characteristics (see Table 1). A diagnosis implies that a child s language skills are disproportionately worse than their non-verbal abilities and that they are not affected by any additional disorders that might affect language development (e.g., deafness, autism). However, the exact nature of the language problem often shows great variation from child to child with differences both in the range of linguistic domains involved (phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics and pragmatics), and in the language modality affected (expressive or receptive). Furthermore, affected individuals may present with associated developmental delays (e.g., dyslexia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder), and additional non-verbal deficits (e.g., in coordination) are not uncommon. Although it has been proposed that the language impairments may underlie many of these additional problems, the exact relationship between the factors remains unclear, and their importance in diagnosis continues to be a matter of debate. Genetics of SLI Lenneberg (1967) was perhaps the first to document the familial nature of language impairments, an observation that has since been consistently endorsed by many independent studies, suggesting that SLI may be genetic in nature. First-degree relatives of language-impaired individuals are at an increased risk of developing SLI them- Biologist (2002) 49 (6) 1

2 Table 1. Current diagnostic research criteria for Specific Language Impairment (SLI) ICD-10 Research Diagnostic Criteria for Specific Developmental Disorders of Speech and Language (World Health Organisation, 1993): 1) language skills that fall outside the two standard deviation limit for the child's age (although this may not be the case in older subjects); 2) the language delay should not be directly attributable to neurological or speech mechanism abnormalities, sensory impairments, mental retardation or environmental factors; 3) the language delay should not be directly attributable to hearing loss in childhood. Cases of partial hearing loss may be included if the hearing loss is considered to be a complicating factor but not a sufficient direct cause for the language delay; 4) the language delay should not form part of a pervasive mental retardation or global developmental delay; 5) the language delay may be accompanied by associated problems and is often followed by difficulties in reading and spelling, abnormalities in interpersonal relationships, and emotional and behavioural disorders. DSM-IV Criteria for Mixed Expressive-Receptive Language Disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 1994): 1) receptive and expressive language skills that are substantially below those obtained from standardised measures of nonverbal intellectual capacity; 2) language difficulties interfere with academic or occupational achievement or with social communication; 3) the language delay should not be directly attributable to mental retardation, a speech-motor sensory deficit, or environmental deprivation; 4) the language delay should not form part of a pervasive mental retardation; 5) the language delay may co-occur with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder, developmental coordination disorder and enuresis. NOTE: Both the ICD-10 and DSM-IV make a distinction between expressive language disorder (in which a child's ability to use expressive spoken language is delayed, but language comprehension is within the normal limits) and receptive language disorder (in which the child's understanding of language is below the appropriate level for their mental age). Figure 1. The genetics of simple and complex disorders A. Simple genetic disorders ñ e.g., the KE family Father's pair of chromosome 7 Mother's pair of chromosome 7 Affected Unaffected B. Complex genetic disorders ñ e.g., SLI Mother Father Possible combinations of parental chromosome 7 in children XX } } Affected Unaffected Figure 1. The genetics of simple and complex disorders A. Simple Monogenic Disorders e.g., the KE family. Monogenic disorders are caused by a mutation in a single gene (which lies on chromosome 7 in the above diagram). Each individual carries two variants (or alleles) of each gene, one on each chromosome 7. Children may inherit a normal allele (yellow on diagram above) or a mutated allele (red on diagram above). Children who inherit a mutated allele always develop the disorder. For monogenic disorders, it is possible to predict the probability that two individuals will have an affected child. The couple shown above have a 50% chance of having an affected child. B. Complex Genetic Disorders e.g., SLI. The above diagram represents a hypothetical model of a complex disorder (such as SLI), which is caused by interactions between several genes and the environment. In the hypothetical situation depicted above, it is assumed that there are eight genes (i.e., 16 alleles) that interact to produce a risk factor for SLI (Red lines = alleles which make you susceptible to SLI, yellow lines = alleles which have no impact upon language status). If the hypothetical model assumes that, out of the 16 alleles, an individual needs to inherit more than four risk alleles to have a high risk of developing SLI. Then in the above diagram, the mother will probably have language problems because she has seven susceptibility variants whereas the father is unlikely to develop SLI because he only has three susceptibility alleles. Because of the number of genes involved, it is difficult to predict the probability that this couple will have a child with SLI. In the above hypothetical model any child with more than four risk alleles will have a high chance of developing the disorder (whether they inherit the risk alleles on chromosome 1, 3, 6, 11, 12, 16 or 19). XY selves, and often report a history of other related disorders such as dyslexia and hyperactivity. Moreover, investigations of twin pairs affected by SLI indicate that monozygotic twins, who are genetically identical, are more similar in terms of their language profile than dizygotic (non-identical) twins, who share only half their genes. These investigations provide strong evidence for the role of genes in the aetiology of SLI (Bishop, 2001). However, for generalised cases of language impairment (as opposed to specific impairments in particular components of language e.g., Van der Lely and Stollwerck, 1996), no clear inheritance pattern has ever been observed. It is therefore considered likely that the onset of these forms of SLI involves several genes that interact, both with each other and the linguistic environment, to produce an overall susceptibility to the development of the disorder (i.e., a complex disease see Figure 1). The KE family Historically, there is only one case of a speech and language disorder for which the above statement has been proved to be untrue. Since the early 1990s, scientists interested in language impairment have been studying a special three-generation pedigree known as the KE family (see Figure 2). 2 Biologist (2002) 49 (6)

3 Figure 2. The KE pedigree Squares represent males. Circles represent females. Black symbols represent individuals affected by a severe speech and language disorder. Symbols with lines through them represent those individuals are deceased. This family are affected by a severe form of a speech and language impairment that is not inherited like a complex disease, but instead involves a change (or mutation) within a single gene (i.e., a monogenic disorder see figure 1). The speech and language impairment in the KE family affects similar numbers of males and females, indicating that the gene involved does not lie on a sex chromosome (X or Y). It affects approximately 50% of the family members, indicating that it follows a dominant pattern of inheritance (i.e., a single mutated copy of the gene is sufficient to cause the disorder). Furthermore, there is no evidence to suggest the presence of complicating factors such as decreased penetrance (i.e., when individuals with the mutation do not develop the disorder) and phenocopies (i.e., when individuals with the disorder do not possess the mutation). One or both of these are generally expected to be widespread in more common and genetically complex forms of SLI. The KE phenotype It should be noted that many researchers consider certain aspects of the KE phenotype to be atypical of specific language impairment and would therefore exclude them from a strict diagnosis of SLI. Affected individuals present with a severe orofacial dyspraxia, which impedes both verbal and non-verbal articulatory movement, and thus might be predicted to directly underlie, or at least contribute to, their speech impediment. However, studies of mute children and children affected by cerebral palsy have indicated that even a complete absence of expressive linguistic abilities does not necessarily result in an impairment in the receptive domain. Thus, since the KE deficits are not limited to the expressive domain, we might expect the aetiology to be more complex than that of a speech impairment caused by orofacial dyspraxia. An additional problem is found when one considers the non-verbal abilities of the KE family. A number of affected members have non-verbal IQ scores that are low enough to place them outside of the normal range. Since these deficits preclude the existence of a discrepancy between verbal and non-verbal abilities some researchers argue that the KE language problems may form part of more-general learning difficulties. However, it has been found that these non-verbal difficulties do not co-segregate with the phenotype (i.e., some affected individuals have normal non-verbal IQs and some unaffected individuals have non-verbal IQs outside the normal range). Therefore, no clear association can be made between the deficit and the speech and language impairment. Thus, although the affected members of the KE family members clearly have speech and language difficulties, it remains a matter of debate whether these difficulties have a similar, or different, aetiology to those found in SLI. Linkage analysis For simple monogenic disorders, genes are traditionally mapped using the positional cloning approach. This Figure 3. Recombination and linkage A given chromosome from child 1 Biologist (2002) 49 (6) 3 a b c The corresponding chromosome from child 2 (sibling of child 1) Figure 3. Recombination and linkage The above diagram shows a comparison of a single chromosome between two siblings. The parent carried one light blue and one dark blue chromosome, and recombination between these chromosomes resulted in the mosaic patterns seen in each of the siblings (see text). Because DNA is inherited in these mosaic blocks, if it is known that the two siblings share the same DNA at point a then it is likely that they will also be sharing at point b. However, we are unable to infer anything about the sharing at point c from the information at a because it is likely that a recombination will have occurred between these two points.

4 Figure 4. Linkage analysis using microsatellite markers Microsatellite markers are amplified, tagged with fluorescent labels and separated on polyacrylamide gels, according to size. In the gel above each lane represents a single individual. The blue, green and yellow markers represent different microsatellites within each individual. The use of three different colours allows the running of similar sized microsatellites upon the same gel. The differences in sizes between individuals allows us to follow the way in which chromosomal segments are inherited within families. The red bands are a size standard, which allow the sizing of each microsatellite. process basically involves the reconstruction of inheritance patterns for the entire genome within families affected by the disorder, thus allowing the identification of regions that are passed onto affected children more often than would be expected by chance alone. Such an approach allows the mapping of disease genes to specific chromosomes when little, or nothing, is known about the biochemical basis of the disorder and is very powerful for simple disease, when the pattern of inheritance is clear. Although positional cloning theoretically involves scanning of the entire genome, it does not require the sequencing of every base or the study of every gene. Instead it exploits the fact that DNA is inherited as mosaic blocks from parents to offspring. During the production of gametes, chromosomes become entangled with each other and can cross over swapping parts of genetic material between chromosome pairs (i.e., recombination). Recombination is a highly random process and therefore, even two cells that have inherited the same array of parental chromosomes are likely to differ. As a consequence of the recombination process, the closer together that two genes are on a chromosome, the more likely that they will be inherited within the same block unit of DNA (see Figure 3). Thus by sampling regions of DNA along each chromosome, it is possible to infer the origin of the majority of the genome without the need to sequence every base. This process is known as a genome screen. A genome screen typically involves the genotyping of hundreds of microsatellite markers spread evenly across the genome (Figure 4). These are selectively neutral, noncoding regions of DNA that consist of repeat stretches of sequence (e.g., CACACACA). The repetitive nature of these sequences means that they show great variability between individuals. For example, one individual may have five repeat units (CACACACACA) where another may have nine (CACACACACACACA- CACA). Using microsatellite information it is possible to identify regions where affected children are more similar at the DNA level than would be expected by chance alone. These are the regions where genes involved in the disorder are likely to lie. The probability that a given region is involved in a given disorder is measured by the logarithmic odds ratio (LOD score). This is the probability of the data arising if the given region is linked to a disorder, against the probability of the data arising if the given region is not linked to a disorder. As a general rule, any region with a LOD of above 3.0 (i.e., a ratio of 1000:1 in favour of linkage) is considered worthy of further investigation. In some cases, these regions may contain many genes, all of which need to be studied in detail to allow the evaluation of their involvement in the disorder under study. In cases where the biochemical basis of the disorder is known, it is possible to prioritise the study of genes, first investigating those which represent the best candidates (e.g., for language, a good candidate would be expressed in the brain during development). FOXP2 In October 2001, scientists in Oxford used a positional cloning approach to isolate the gene mutated in the KE family (Lai et al., 2001). The gene is found on chromosome 7 and is called FOXP2. This is a member of a large family of genes, all of which possess a conserved sequence known as a winged-helix or forkhead box (fox) domain. There are over 100 fox genes, all of which act as transcription factors, i.e., they are responsible for regulating the expression of other genes. FOXP2 has been shown to be expressed in many cell types but has a particularly strong expression in foetal brain. The KE mutation involves a change at a single DNA nucleotide from guanine (G) to adenine (A), which co-segregates with the speech and language phenotype (i.e., all affected individuals carry the mutation, but no unaffected individuals have the change). This single base change results in an alteration within the protein amino acid sequence (arginine to histidine). Although such a small change may not always be expected to alter the function of a protein, Lai et al. showed that this particular base falls within the fox domain and is conserved in the sequence of all FOX genes, not only in humans but also across species such as mice, fruit flies and yeast, indicating that it has an important function. Lai et al. provided further evidence for the involvement of FOXP2 in speech and language impairments by identifying another patient (known as CS) who is unrelated to the KE family, but has a similar deficit and carries a disruption in the FOXP2 gene. The CS patient did not have 4 Biologist (2002) 49 (6)

5 a mutation in the FOXP2 gene but instead had a translocation (i.e., a transfer of DNA between two chromosomes) involving chromosomes 5 and 7, in which the chromosome 7 breakpoint fell directly within the FOXP2 sequence and thereby disrupts the gene. FOXP2 in generalised SLI The evidence for the involvement of FOXP2 in these two cases of language impairment is clear. However, given the dissent regarding the KE phenotype, questions remained regarding its relevance in more common and genetically complex forms of SLI. A recent study screened the entire FOXP2 coding sequence within 43 individuals with SLI and 48 individuals with autism (which often presents with associated language delays; Newbury et al. 2002). All the variants found within these individuals were in non-coding sequence and were found to occur at equal frequencies within non-language-impaired controls. Thus, it appears that the role of FOXP2 may not generalise to SLI, or may only be relevant to limited numbers of individuals with a specific subtype of speech and language impairment. However, it remains possible that the characterisation of the FOXP2 pathway may reveal alternative upstream or downstream genes that do play a role in more common forms of SLI. Furthermore, a recent investigation indicated that the sequence of the FOXP2 gene is likely to extend beyond that originally characterised (Bruce & Margolis, 2002), and therefore additional mutation screens will be required before the gene can be fully excluded as a candidate in SLI. Complex SLI In contrast to the KE family, the study of genetically complex SLI continues to pose more of a challenge. Because these forms of language impairment are expected to involve several genes, the effect of each gene variant is reduced over that in a single-gene disorder and the individual genes are therefore harder to detect. Moreover, complex diseases are not necessarily caused by mutations in genes but may arise as a result of common genetic variations that even perfectly healthy people carry. The more of these susceptibility variants an individual carries the greater their risk of developing SLI. Thus, because the gene variations do not directly cause the language problems, the exact combination of genes underlying the disorder may vary from person to person (i.e., SLI is a heterogeneous disorder). Whilst the positional cloning approach can be applied to the study of complex diseases, heterogeneity necessitates larger sample sizes and often results in a loss of power, meaning that less clearly defined chromosomal regions may be identified, which tend to be much larger in size. Because of these complications the significance threshold for genome screens of complex diseases differs from that for a monogenic disorder. For complex disease, a LOD of above 2.2 is considered to be suggestive of linkage and a LOD of above 3.6 is considered a significant linkage. Genome screens for loci involved in SLI Recently, two studies have used the genome screen method to investigate which chromosomal regions might be involved in complex forms of SLI (SLIC, 2002; Bartlett et al., 2002). The SLI Consortium collected DNA from 98 families, all of which contained at least one individual affected by SLI. These samples were taken from two sources: clinical samples of severely affected children were collected from special language schools in the London area and more mainstream samples were taken from an epidemiological study in Cambridge. Using these cohorts, SLIC researchers performed a systematic genome screen using 500 microsatellite markers spaced evenly across the genome (approximately one every eight million base-pairs (Mb)). They investigated three language-related measures within these families: a composite score of expressive language abilities, a composite score of receptive language abilities and a putative measure of phonological short-term memory (which has been proposed to be a core deficit in the SLI phenotype). They identified a region on the long arm of chromosome 16 that was linked to the short-term memory score with a LOD of 3.55, and an area on the long arm of chromosome 19 linked to the expressive language score, also with a LOD of Bartlett et al. studied 86 individuals selected from branches of five large Canadian families. In contrast to the SLIC study, this investigation used a binary system under which family members were classified as affected or unaffected according to three alternative diagnostic schedules (language impairment, reading impairment and clinical impairment). Using this approach, two regions of linkage were found, one on chromosome 13q (Maximum LOD Score = 3.92) under a recessive model of reading impairment and a second on chromosome 2p under a recessive model of language impairment (Maximum LOD Score = 2.79). Note that neither of the above studies found evidence for linkage to chromosome 7 the site of the FOXP2 gene. The apparent dissociation between the results of these two genome screens is not unusual in the study of complex disorders and may have arisen for several reasons. Each study employed very different methods with respect to the selection of subjects, the measurement of language impairment and the detection of linkage within their samples. The divergence between the methods used and the results obtained further highlights the challenges involved in the genetic investigation of a complex disorder such as SLI. It should also be noted that the genome screen approach only highlights regions that are likely to be involved in the disorder under study and, given the complexity of SLI, it remains possible that these results may represent false positives. Thus replication within other groups of language-impaired children will be required to validate each of these regions of linkage. Furthermore, it is likely that all of the areas identified by the above studies contain hundreds of genes, each of which will need to be screened in order to evaluate their involvement in SLI. Conclusion In conclusion, this has been an exciting year in linguistic genetics. We have witnessed the identification of the first gene to be implicated in a speech and language disorder, and have taken the first step forward in the process of identifying gene variants that may affect language variation within a more general population. It is hoped that over the next few years the extension of these studies will provide us with a better understanding of the processes involved in language acquisition, and will pave the way for better diagnostic tests and potential therapies for disorders such as SLI. References Pinker S (1994) The Language Instinct. The New Science of Language and Mind. Penguin Books Biologist (2002) 49 (6) 5

6 Bartlett CW et al (2002) A major susceptibility locus for Specific Language Impairment is located on chromosome 13q21. American Journal of Human Genetics, 71, Bishop DVM (2001) Genetic and environmental risks for specific language impairment in children. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological sciences, 356, Bruce HA & Margolis RL (2002) FOXP2: Novel exons, splice variants, and CAG repeat length stability. Human Genetics, 111, Lai CSL et al. (2001) A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413, Newbury DF et al. (2002) FOXP2 is not a major susceptibility gene for autism or Specific Language Impairment (SLI) The American Journal of Human Genetics, 70, The SLI Consortium (2002) A genome-wide scan identifies two novel loci involved in specific language impairment (SLI). The American Journal of Human Genetics, 70, Van der Lely HK and Stollwerck L (1996) A grammatical specific language impairment in children: an autosomal dominant inheritance? Brain and Language, 52, Further reading For a review of the SLI phenotype please see: Bishop DVM (1997) Uncommon Understanding: Development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press For a review of the genetic analysis of complex traits please see: Lander ES and Schork NJ (1994) Genetic dissection of complex traits. Science, 265, For a review of the molecular genetics of speech and language disorders please see: Fisher SE (2002) Isolation of the genetic factors underlying speech and language disorders. In: Behavioural Genetics in the Postgenomic Era Plomin R, DeFries JC, Craig IW, McGuffin P (Eds) APA Books: Washington DC For a review of the genetics of language please see: Stromswold K (2001) The heritability of language: A review and metaanalysis of twin, adoption and linkage studies. Language, 77, Website The website for the Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics in Oxford, with details of the on-going research. Anthony Monaco received his undergraduate degree from Princeton University and his MD-PhD from Harvard Medical School. He was a post doctoral fellow in Hans Lehrach's laboratory at the ICRF followed by four years as an ICRF senior scientist and head of the Human Genetics Laboratory at the Institute of Molecular Medicine, Oxford. In 1995, he was awarded a Wellcome Trust Principal Research Fellowship and joined the Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics in Oxford working on the genetic basis of neurological and psychiatric disorders including autism and developmental language and reading disorders. In 1998 he was appointed as Director of the Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics. Dianne Newbury received her undergraduate degree from Nottingham University and her PhD from Oxford University. She has worked in Anthony Monaco s laboratory at the Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics since 1997and heads the SLI genome screen project. The Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics University of Oxford Roosevelt Drive Oxford OX3 7BN, UK anthony.monaco@well.ox.ac.uk dianne@well.ox.ac.uk 6 Biologist (2002) 49 (6)

What favorite organism of geneticists is described in the right-hand column?

What favorite organism of geneticists is described in the right-hand column? What favorite organism of geneticists is described in the right-hand column? Model Organism fruit fly?? Generation time 12 days ~ 5000 days Size 2 mm 1500-1800mm Brood size hundreds a couple dozen would

More information

Mapping of genes causing dyslexia susceptibility Clyde Francks Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics University of Oxford Trinity 2001

Mapping of genes causing dyslexia susceptibility Clyde Francks Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics University of Oxford Trinity 2001 Mapping of genes causing dyslexia susceptibility Clyde Francks Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics University of Oxford Trinity 2001 Thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Mapping

More information

Learning Abilities and Disabilities

Learning Abilities and Disabilities CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Learning Abilities and Disabilities Generalist Genes, Specialist Environments Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry,

More information

Genetics and Genomics in Medicine Chapter 8 Questions

Genetics and Genomics in Medicine Chapter 8 Questions Genetics and Genomics in Medicine Chapter 8 Questions Linkage Analysis Question Question 8.1 Affected members of the pedigree above have an autosomal dominant disorder, and cytogenetic analyses using conventional

More information

Dan Koller, Ph.D. Medical and Molecular Genetics

Dan Koller, Ph.D. Medical and Molecular Genetics Design of Genetic Studies Dan Koller, Ph.D. Research Assistant Professor Medical and Molecular Genetics Genetics and Medicine Over the past decade, advances from genetics have permeated medicine Identification

More information

Chapter 7: Pedigree Analysis B I O L O G Y

Chapter 7: Pedigree Analysis B I O L O G Y Name Date Period Chapter 7: Pedigree Analysis B I O L O G Y Introduction: A pedigree is a diagram of family relationships that uses symbols to represent people and lines to represent genetic relationships.

More information

The Chromosomal Basis of Inheritance

The Chromosomal Basis of Inheritance The Chromosomal Basis of Inheritance Factors and Genes Mendel s model of inheritance was based on the idea of factors that were independently assorted and segregated into gametes We now know that these

More information

Genome - Wide Linkage Mapping

Genome - Wide Linkage Mapping Biological Sciences Initiative HHMI Genome - Wide Linkage Mapping Introduction This activity is based on the work of Dr. Christine Seidman et al that was published in Circulation, 1998, vol 97, pgs 2043-2048.

More information

Pedigree Construction Notes

Pedigree Construction Notes Name Date Pedigree Construction Notes GO TO à Mendelian Inheritance (http://www.uic.edu/classes/bms/bms655/lesson3.html) When human geneticists first began to publish family studies, they used a variety

More information

Introduction to linkage and family based designs to study the genetic epidemiology of complex traits. Harold Snieder

Introduction to linkage and family based designs to study the genetic epidemiology of complex traits. Harold Snieder Introduction to linkage and family based designs to study the genetic epidemiology of complex traits Harold Snieder Overview of presentation Designs: population vs. family based Mendelian vs. complex diseases/traits

More information

Cognitive and Behavioral Genetics: An Overview. Steven Pinker

Cognitive and Behavioral Genetics: An Overview. Steven Pinker Cognitive and Behavioral Genetics: An Overview Steven Pinker What is Cognitive and Behavioral Genetics? Behavioral genetics = Genetic basis of behavior: How genes wire up a brain capable of seeing, moving,

More information

Pedigree Analysis. A = the trait (a genetic disease or abnormality, dominant) a = normal (recessive)

Pedigree Analysis. A = the trait (a genetic disease or abnormality, dominant) a = normal (recessive) Pedigree Analysis Introduction A pedigree is a diagram of family relationships that uses symbols to represent people and lines to represent genetic relationships. These diagrams make it easier to visualize

More information

Genetic Variation Junior Science

Genetic Variation Junior Science 2018 Version Genetic Variation Junior Science http://img.publishthis.com/images/bookmarkimages/2015/05/d/5/c/d5cf017fb4f7e46e1c21b874472ea7d1_bookmarkimage_620x480_xlarge_original_1.jpg Sexual Reproduction

More information

Today s Topics. Cracking the Genetic Code. The Process of Genetic Transmission. The Process of Genetic Transmission. Genes

Today s Topics. Cracking the Genetic Code. The Process of Genetic Transmission. The Process of Genetic Transmission. Genes Today s Topics Mechanisms of Heredity Biology of Heredity Genetic Disorders Research Methods in Behavioral Genetics Gene x Environment Interactions The Process of Genetic Transmission Genes: segments of

More information

Genes and Inheritance

Genes and Inheritance Genes and Inheritance Variation Causes of Variation Variation No two people are exactly the same The differences between people is called VARIATION. This variation comes from two sources: Genetic cause

More information

An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics I. Heather A Lawson Advanced Genetics Spring2018

An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics I. Heather A Lawson Advanced Genetics Spring2018 An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics I Heather A Lawson Advanced Genetics Spring2018 Outline What is Quantitative Genetics? Genotypic Values and Genetic Effects Heritability Linkage Disequilibrium

More information

Psychology of Language

Psychology of Language PSYCH 150 / LIN 155 UCI COGNITIVE SCIENCES syn lab Psychology of Language Prof. Jon Sprouse 01.24.13: Williams Syndrome and Specific Language Impairment 1 Is there a language faculty? Do the processes

More information

Section 8.1 Studying inheritance

Section 8.1 Studying inheritance Section 8.1 Studying inheritance Genotype and phenotype Genotype is the genetic constitution of an organism that describes all the alleles that an organism contains The genotype sets the limits to which

More information

Class XII Chapter 5 Principles of Inheritance and Variation Biology

Class XII Chapter 5 Principles of Inheritance and Variation Biology Question 1: Mention the advantages of selecting pea plant for experiment by Mendel. Mendel selected pea plants to carry out his study on the inheritance of characters from parents to offspring. He selected

More information

By Mir Mohammed Abbas II PCMB 'A' CHAPTER CONCEPT NOTES

By Mir Mohammed Abbas II PCMB 'A' CHAPTER CONCEPT NOTES Chapter Notes- Genetics By Mir Mohammed Abbas II PCMB 'A' 1 CHAPTER CONCEPT NOTES Relationship between genes and chromosome of diploid organism and the terms used to describe them Know the terms Terms

More information

Problem set questions from Final Exam Human Genetics, Nondisjunction, and Cancer

Problem set questions from Final Exam Human Genetics, Nondisjunction, and Cancer Problem set questions from Final Exam Human Genetics, Nondisjunction, and ancer Mapping in humans using SSRs and LOD scores 1. You set out to genetically map the locus for color blindness with respect

More information

Chapter 18 Genetics of Behavior. Chapter 18 Human Heredity by Michael Cummings 2006 Brooks/Cole-Thomson Learning

Chapter 18 Genetics of Behavior. Chapter 18 Human Heredity by Michael Cummings 2006 Brooks/Cole-Thomson Learning Chapter 18 Genetics of Behavior Behavior Most human behaviors are polygenic and have significant environmental influences Methods used to study inheritance include Classical methods of linkage and pedigree

More information

3. What law of heredity explains that traits, like texture and color, are inherited independently of each other?

3. What law of heredity explains that traits, like texture and color, are inherited independently of each other? Section 2: Genetics Chapter 11 pg. 308-329 Part 1: Refer to the table of pea plant traits on the right. Then complete the table on the left by filling in the missing information for each cross. 6. What

More information

Psych 3102 Lecture 3. Mendelian Genetics

Psych 3102 Lecture 3. Mendelian Genetics Psych 3102 Lecture 3 Mendelian Genetics Gregor Mendel 1822 1884, paper read 1865-66 Augustinian monk genotype alleles present at a locus can we identify this? phenotype expressed trait/characteristic can

More information

NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948

NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948 NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948 Achievement Criteria Biological ideas relating to genetic variation are limited to concepts and processes connected with: the continuity of life based on the

More information

MULTIFACTORIAL DISEASES. MG L-10 July 7 th 2014

MULTIFACTORIAL DISEASES. MG L-10 July 7 th 2014 MULTIFACTORIAL DISEASES MG L-10 July 7 th 2014 Genetic Diseases Unifactorial Chromosomal Multifactorial AD Numerical AR Structural X-linked Microdeletions Mitochondrial Spectrum of Alterations in DNA Sequence

More information

Lecture 6: Linkage analysis in medical genetics

Lecture 6: Linkage analysis in medical genetics Lecture 6: Linkage analysis in medical genetics Magnus Dehli Vigeland NORBIS course, 8 th 12 th of January 2018, Oslo Approaches to genetic mapping of disease Multifactorial disease Monogenic disease Syke

More information

Genetics All somatic cells contain 23 pairs of chromosomes 22 pairs of autosomes 1 pair of sex chromosomes Genes contained in each pair of chromosomes

Genetics All somatic cells contain 23 pairs of chromosomes 22 pairs of autosomes 1 pair of sex chromosomes Genes contained in each pair of chromosomes Chapter 6 Genetics and Inheritance Lecture 1: Genetics and Patterns of Inheritance Asexual reproduction = daughter cells genetically identical to parent (clones) Sexual reproduction = offspring are genetic

More information

A gene is a sequence of DNA that resides at a particular site on a chromosome the locus (plural loci). Genetic linkage of genes on a single

A gene is a sequence of DNA that resides at a particular site on a chromosome the locus (plural loci). Genetic linkage of genes on a single 8.3 A gene is a sequence of DNA that resides at a particular site on a chromosome the locus (plural loci). Genetic linkage of genes on a single chromosome can alter their pattern of inheritance from those

More information

Calculate the percentage of cytosine for the beetle. (2)

Calculate the percentage of cytosine for the beetle. (2) Questions Q1. (i) Figure 10 shows the percentages of bases for three organisms. Calculate the percentage of cytosine for the beetle.... % (ii) Explain why the information given about the Ebola virus indicates

More information

What causes language disorders in children? Dorothy Bishop University of Oxford

What causes language disorders in children? Dorothy Bishop University of Oxford What causes language disorders in children? Dorothy Bishop University of Oxford 1 Specific language impairment (SLI) Diagnosed in children when language does not follow normal developmental course Problems

More information

UNIT IV. Chapter 14 The Human Genome

UNIT IV. Chapter 14 The Human Genome UNIT IV Chapter 14 The Human Genome UNIT 2: GENETICS Chapter 7: Extending Medelian Genetics I. Chromosomes and Phenotype (7.1) A. Two copies of each autosomal gene affect phenotype 1. Most human traits

More information

Education Options for Children with Autism

Education Options for Children with Autism Empowering children with Autism and their families through knowledge and support Education Options for Children with Autism Starting school is a major milestone in a child s life, and a big step for all

More information

Principles of Inheritance and Variation

Principles of Inheritance and Variation Principles of Inheritance and Variation Question 1: Mention the advantages of selecting pea plant for experiment by Mendel. Answer Mendel selected pea plants to carry out his study on the inheritance of

More information

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives)

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Recognize that behavior is multi-factorial with genetic components Understand how multi-factorial traits are studied. Explain the terms: incidence, prevalence,

More information

Genetics Review. Alleles. The Punnett Square. Genotype and Phenotype. Codominance. Incomplete Dominance

Genetics Review. Alleles. The Punnett Square. Genotype and Phenotype. Codominance. Incomplete Dominance Genetics Review Alleles These two different versions of gene A create a condition known as heterozygous. Only the dominant allele (A) will be expressed. When both chromosomes have identical copies of the

More information

Chapter 1 : Genetics 101

Chapter 1 : Genetics 101 Chapter 1 : Genetics 101 Understanding the underlying concepts of human genetics and the role of genes, behavior, and the environment will be important to appropriately collecting and applying genetic

More information

Unit 8.1: Human Chromosomes and Genes

Unit 8.1: Human Chromosomes and Genes Unit 8.1: Human Chromosomes and Genes Biotechnology. Gene Therapy. Reality or fiction? During your lifetime, gene therapy may be mainstream medicine. Here we see a representation of the insertion of DNA

More information

Chapter 4 PEDIGREE ANALYSIS IN HUMAN GENETICS

Chapter 4 PEDIGREE ANALYSIS IN HUMAN GENETICS Chapter 4 PEDIGREE ANALYSIS IN HUMAN GENETICS Chapter Summary In order to study the transmission of human genetic traits to the next generation, a different method of operation had to be adopted. Instead

More information

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) DSM-5 (Criteria and Major Changes for SLP-Related Conditions) Individuals meeting the criteria will be given a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder with three levels of severity based on degree of support

More information

Mendelian Genetics. 7.3 Gene Linkage and Mapping Genes can be mapped to specific locations on chromosomes.

Mendelian Genetics. 7.3 Gene Linkage and Mapping Genes can be mapped to specific locations on chromosomes. 7 Extending CHAPTER Mendelian Genetics GETTING READY TO LEARN Preview Key Concepts 7.1 Chromosomes and Phenotype The chromosomes on which genes are located can affect the expression of traits. 7.2 Complex

More information

UNIT 6 GENETICS 12/30/16

UNIT 6 GENETICS 12/30/16 12/30/16 UNIT 6 GENETICS III. Mendel and Heredity (6.3) A. Mendel laid the groundwork for genetics 1. Traits are distinguishing characteristics that are inherited. 2. Genetics is the study of biological

More information

Diploma in Equine Science

Diploma in Equine Science The process of meiosis is summarised in the diagram below, but it involves the reduction of the genetic material to half. A cell containing the full number of chromosomes (two pairs) is termed diploid,

More information

12 Biology Revision Notes - Term 3

12 Biology Revision Notes - Term 3 QCE Biology Year 2016 Mark 0.00 Pages 21 Published Jan 24, 2017 Revision Notes - Term 3 By Sophie (1 ATAR) Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org) Your notes author, Sophie. Sophie achieved an ATAR of 1 in 2016

More information

Introduction to Genetics

Introduction to Genetics Introduction to Genetics Table of contents Chromosome DNA Protein synthesis Mutation Genetic disorder Relationship between genes and cancer Genetic testing Technical concern 2 All living organisms consist

More information

B-4.7 Summarize the chromosome theory of inheritance and relate that theory to Gregor Mendel s principles of genetics

B-4.7 Summarize the chromosome theory of inheritance and relate that theory to Gregor Mendel s principles of genetics B-4.7 Summarize the chromosome theory of inheritance and relate that theory to Gregor Mendel s principles of genetics The Chromosome theory of inheritance is a basic principle in biology that states genes

More information

Meiotic Mistakes and Abnormalities Learning Outcomes

Meiotic Mistakes and Abnormalities Learning Outcomes Meiotic Mistakes and Abnormalities Learning Outcomes 5.6 Explain how nondisjunction can result in whole chromosomal abnormalities. (Module 5.10) 5.7 Describe the inheritance patterns for strict dominant

More information

GENETICS - CLUTCH CH.2 MENDEL'S LAWS OF INHERITANCE.

GENETICS - CLUTCH CH.2 MENDEL'S LAWS OF INHERITANCE. !! www.clutchprep.com CONCEPT: MENDELS EXPERIMENTS AND LAWS Mendel s Experiments Gregor Mendel was an Austrian monk who studied Genetics using pea plants Mendel used pure lines meaning that all offspring

More information

1042SCG Genetics & Evolutionary Biology Semester Summary

1042SCG Genetics & Evolutionary Biology Semester Summary 1042SCG Genetics & Evolutionary Biology Semester Summary Griffith University, Nathan Campus Semester 1, 2014 Topics include: - Mendelian Genetics - Eukaryotic & Prokaryotic Genes - Sex Chromosomes - Variations

More information

Chapter 15 Notes 15.1: Mendelian inheritance chromosome theory of inheritance wild type 15.2: Sex-linked genes

Chapter 15 Notes 15.1: Mendelian inheritance chromosome theory of inheritance wild type 15.2: Sex-linked genes Chapter 15 Notes The Chromosomal Basis of Inheritance Mendel s hereditary factors were genes, though this wasn t known at the time Now we know that genes are located on The location of a particular gene

More information

14 1 Human Heredity. Week 8 vocab Chapter 14

14 1 Human Heredity. Week 8 vocab Chapter 14 Week 8 vocab Chapter 14 Vocab 1. Karyotype 5. sex-linked gene 2. sex chromosome 6. nondisjunction 3. pedigree 7. DNA fingerprinting 4. Polygenic 1 of 43 Biology Biology 2 of 43 14-1 Human Heredity 3 of

More information

Chapter 17 Genetics Crosses:

Chapter 17 Genetics Crosses: Chapter 17 Genetics Crosses: 2.5 Genetics Objectives 2.5.6 Genetic Inheritance 2.5.10.H Origin of the Science of genetics 2.5.11 H Law of segregation 2.5.12 H Law of independent assortment 2.5.13.H Dihybrid

More information

Ch 6.2 Inheritance in Sex Linked Genes.notebook April 19, 2018

Ch 6.2 Inheritance in Sex Linked Genes.notebook April 19, 2018 Name 3 "Big Ideas" from our last notebook lecture: * * * 1 Inheritance of Linked Genes 2 Learning Goals Understand Sex linked Genes How to use a Punnett Square to solve genetics problems and how to use

More information

CS2220 Introduction to Computational Biology

CS2220 Introduction to Computational Biology CS2220 Introduction to Computational Biology WEEK 8: GENOME-WIDE ASSOCIATION STUDIES (GWAS) 1 Dr. Mengling FENG Institute for Infocomm Research Massachusetts Institute of Technology mfeng@mit.edu PLANS

More information

Working memory in. development: Links with learning between. typical and atypical populations. TRACY PACKIAM ALLOWAY Durham University, UK

Working memory in. development: Links with learning between. typical and atypical populations. TRACY PACKIAM ALLOWAY Durham University, UK Working memory in 00 000 00 0 000 000 0 development: Links with learning between typical and atypical populations 4 2 5 TRACY PACKIAM ALLOWAY Durham University, UK Outline 00 000 00 0 000 000 0 Working

More information

Name Class Date. KEY CONCEPT The chromosomes on which genes are located can affect the expression of traits.

Name Class Date. KEY CONCEPT The chromosomes on which genes are located can affect the expression of traits. Section 1: Chromosomes and Phenotype KEY CONCEPT The chromosomes on which genes are located can affect the expression of traits. VOCABULARY carrier sex-linked gene X chromosome inactivation MAIN IDEA:

More information

Downloaded from

Downloaded from Chapter-5 Principles of Inheritance and Variations Chapter No. Chapter Name Concepts Degree of imp. Ref. NCERT text book.: page nos Common errors 5 Principles of inheritance and variations 1. Mendel s

More information

Chapter 3 Outline. I. Becoming Parents

Chapter 3 Outline. I. Becoming Parents Chapter 3 Outline I. Becoming Parents A. Conception 1. Changing Theories of Conception Two-seed theory: (Hippocrates) Joining of male and female seeds. There are no significant anatomical differences between

More information

QB365 Important Questions - Principles of Inheritance and Variation

QB365 Important Questions - Principles of Inheritance and Variation QB36 Important Questions - Principles of Inheritance and Variation th Standard CBSE Biology Reg.No. : Time : 0:00:00 Hrs Section - A ) Heterozygous purple flower is crossed with recessive white flower.

More information

IS IT GENETIC? How do genes, environment and chance interact to specify a complex trait such as intelligence?

IS IT GENETIC? How do genes, environment and chance interact to specify a complex trait such as intelligence? 1 IS IT GENETIC? How do genes, environment and chance interact to specify a complex trait such as intelligence? Single-gene (monogenic) traits Phenotypic variation is typically discrete (often comparing

More information

Convergent genetic linkage and associations to language, speech and reading measures in families of probands with Specific Language Impairment

Convergent genetic linkage and associations to language, speech and reading measures in families of probands with Specific Language Impairment J Neurodevelop Disord (2009) 1:264 282 DOI 10.1007/s11689-009-9031-x Convergent genetic linkage and associations to language, speech and reading measures in families of probands with Specific Language

More information

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives)

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Recognize that behavior is multi-factorial with genetic components Understand how multi-factorial traits are studied. Explain the terms: prevalence, incidence,

More information

Collated questions Demonstrate understanding of biological ideas relating to genetic variation DNA STRUCTURE

Collated questions Demonstrate understanding of biological ideas relating to genetic variation DNA STRUCTURE Collated questions Demonstrate understanding of biological ideas relating to genetic variation DNA STRUCTURE THE ROLE OF DNA IN INHERITANCE (2013:2) Use the diagram above to help you explain the relationship

More information

UNIT 2: GENETICS Chapter 7: Extending Medelian Genetics

UNIT 2: GENETICS Chapter 7: Extending Medelian Genetics CORNELL NOTES Directions: You must create a minimum of 5 questions in this column per page (average). Use these to study your notes and prepare for tests and quizzes. Notes will be stamped after each assigned

More information

2017 Version. Key Question types NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948

2017 Version. Key Question types NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948 2017 Version Key Question types NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948 Linking DNA, Alleles and Chromosomes Chromosomes are made up of DNA. DNA is a large molecule that is coiled into a double helix

More information

A. Incorrect! Cells contain the units of genetic they are not the unit of heredity.

A. Incorrect! Cells contain the units of genetic they are not the unit of heredity. MCAT Biology Problem Drill PS07: Mendelian Genetics Question No. 1 of 10 Question 1. The smallest unit of heredity is. Question #01 (A) Cell (B) Gene (C) Chromosome (D) Allele Cells contain the units of

More information

Pedigree Analysis Why do Pedigrees? Goals of Pedigree Analysis Basic Symbols More Symbols Y-Linked Inheritance

Pedigree Analysis Why do Pedigrees? Goals of Pedigree Analysis Basic Symbols More Symbols Y-Linked Inheritance Pedigree Analysis Why do Pedigrees? Punnett squares and chi-square tests work well for organisms that have large numbers of offspring and controlled mating, but humans are quite different: Small families.

More information

Assessment Schedule 2015 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of genetic variation and change (91157)

Assessment Schedule 2015 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of genetic variation and change (91157) NCEA Level 2 Biology (91157) 2015 page 1 of 6 Assessment Schedule 2015 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of genetic variation and change (91157) Assessment Criteria Achievement Achievement with Merit

More information

Example: Colour in snapdragons

Example: Colour in snapdragons Incomplete Dominance this occurs when the expression of one allele does not completely mask the expression of another. the result is that a heterozygous organism has a phenotype that is a blend of the

More information

GENETICS - NOTES-

GENETICS - NOTES- GENETICS - NOTES- Warm Up Exercise Using your previous knowledge of genetics, determine what maternal genotype would most likely yield offspring with such characteristics. Use the genotype that you came

More information

Jay M. Baraban MD, PhD January 2007 GENES AND BEHAVIOR

Jay M. Baraban MD, PhD January 2007 GENES AND BEHAVIOR Jay M. Baraban MD, PhD jay.baraban@gmail.com January 2007 GENES AND BEHAVIOR Overview One of the most fascinating topics in neuroscience is the role that inheritance plays in determining one s behavior.

More information

Human Genetics Notes:

Human Genetics Notes: Human Genetics Notes: Human Chromosomes Cell biologists analyze chromosomes by looking at. Cells are during mitosis. Scientists then cut out the chromosomes from the and group them together in pairs. A

More information

Genetics Unit Exam. Number of progeny with following phenotype Experiment Red White #1: Fish 2 (red) with Fish 3 (red) 100 0

Genetics Unit Exam. Number of progeny with following phenotype Experiment Red White #1: Fish 2 (red) with Fish 3 (red) 100 0 Genetics Unit Exam Question You are working with an ornamental fish that shows two color phenotypes, red or white. The color is controlled by a single gene. These fish are hermaphrodites meaning they can

More information

Downloaded from Chapter 5 Principles of Inheritance and Variation

Downloaded from  Chapter 5 Principles of Inheritance and Variation Chapter 5 Principles of Inheritance and Variation Genetics: Genetics is a branch of biology which deals with principles of inheritance and its practices. Heredity: It is transmission of traits from one

More information

Chapter 1 Heredity. Prepared by: GOAD s Team

Chapter 1 Heredity. Prepared by: GOAD s Team Chapter 1 Heredity Prepared by: GOAD s Team IMPORTANT VOCABULARY WORDS Traits Character Genes Allele Genotype homozygote heterozygote Dominant recessive phenotype WHAT IS HEREDITY? HEREDITY - is a passing

More information

LESSON 3.2 WORKBOOK. How do normal cells become cancer cells? Workbook Lesson 3.2

LESSON 3.2 WORKBOOK. How do normal cells become cancer cells? Workbook Lesson 3.2 For a complete list of defined terms, see the Glossary. Transformation the process by which a cell acquires characteristics of a tumor cell. LESSON 3.2 WORKBOOK How do normal cells become cancer cells?

More information

This fact sheet describes the condition Fragile X and includes a discussion of the symptoms, causes and available testing.

This fact sheet describes the condition Fragile X and includes a discussion of the symptoms, causes and available testing. 11111 Fact Sheet 54 FRAGILE X SYNDROME This fact sheet describes the condition Fragile X and includes a discussion of the symptoms, causes and available testing. In summary Fragile X is a condition caused

More information

Genetics Practice Test

Genetics Practice Test Name: ate: 1. Which genetic concept was proposed by Mendel?. chromosome nondisjunction. independent assortment. multiple alleles. sex linkage 4. Mendel s discovery that characteristics are inherited due

More information

Name Period. Keystone Vocabulary: genetics fertilization trait hybrid gene allele Principle of dominance segregation gamete probability

Name Period. Keystone Vocabulary: genetics fertilization trait hybrid gene allele Principle of dominance segregation gamete probability Name Period BIO B2 GENETICS (Chapter 11) You should be able to: 1. Describe and/or predict observed patterns of inheritance (dominant, recessive, co- dominant, incomplete dominance, sex- linked, polygenic

More information

Lesson Overview Human Chromosomes

Lesson Overview Human Chromosomes Lesson Overview 14.1 Human Chromosomes Human Genome To find what makes us uniquely human, we have to explore the human genome, which is the full set of genetic information carried in our DNA. This DNA

More information

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER What is the DSM-5? The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (the DSM) is developed by the

More information

Lecture 13: May 24, 2004

Lecture 13: May 24, 2004 Lecture 13: May 24, 2004 CH14: Mendel and the gene idea *particulate inheritance parents pass on discrete heritable units *gene- unit of inheritance which occupies a specific chromosomal location (locus)

More information

Human Chromosomes. Lesson Overview. Lesson Overview Human Chromosomes

Human Chromosomes. Lesson Overview. Lesson Overview Human Chromosomes Lesson Overview 14.1 THINK ABOUT IT If you had to pick an ideal organism for the study of genetics, would you choose one that produced lots of offspring, was easy to grow in the lab, and had a short life

More information

CHROMOSOMAL THEORY OF INHERITANCE

CHROMOSOMAL THEORY OF INHERITANCE AP BIOLOGY EVOLUTION/HEREDITY UNIT Unit 1 Part 7 Chapter 15 ACTIVITY #10 NAME DATE PERIOD CHROMOSOMAL THEORY OF INHERITANCE The Theory: Genes are located on chromosomes Chromosomes segregate and independently

More information

GAUTENG DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION SENIOR SECONDARY INTERVENTION PROGRAMME LIFE SCIENCES GRADE 12 SESSION 3 (LEARNER NOTES)

GAUTENG DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION SENIOR SECONDARY INTERVENTION PROGRAMME LIFE SCIENCES GRADE 12 SESSION 3 (LEARNER NOTES) TOPIC 1: MENDEL S 1 ST LAW, SEX AND BLOOD GROUP DETERMINATION Learner Note: Mendel s Laws are very important and you must understand the basic concepts of Genetics. You must understand the concepts of

More information

Quantitative genetics: traits controlled by alleles at many loci

Quantitative genetics: traits controlled by alleles at many loci Quantitative genetics: traits controlled by alleles at many loci Human phenotypic adaptations and diseases commonly involve the effects of many genes, each will small effect Quantitative genetics allows

More information

Genetics and Heredity Notes

Genetics and Heredity Notes Genetics and Heredity Notes I. Introduction A. It was known for 1000s of years that traits were inherited but scientists were unsure about the laws that governed this inheritance. B. Gregor Mendel (1822-1884)

More information

Multivariate Linkage Analysis of Specific Language Impairment (SLI)

Multivariate Linkage Analysis of Specific Language Impairment (SLI) doi: 10.1111/j.1469-1809.2007.00361.x Multivariate Linkage Analysis of Specific Language Impairment (SLI) Anthony P. Monaco THE SLI CONSORTIUM (SLIC) 1 SUMMARY Specific language impairment (SLI) is defined

More information

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. Autism Spectrum Disorder Neurodevelopmental disorder Reflects understanding of the etiology of disorder as related to alterations

More information

The Determination of the Genetic Order and Genetic Map for the Eye Color, Wing Size, and Bristle Morphology in Drosophila melanogaster

The Determination of the Genetic Order and Genetic Map for the Eye Color, Wing Size, and Bristle Morphology in Drosophila melanogaster Kudlac 1 Kaitie Kudlac March 24, 2015 Professor Ma Genetics 356 The Determination of the Genetic Order and Genetic Map for the Eye Color, Wing Size, and Bristle Morphology in Drosophila melanogaster Abstract:

More information

Pragmatic language impairments

Pragmatic language impairments Pragmatic language impairments Dorothy Bishop Wellcome Principal Research Fellow Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford 1 Oral communication involves: COMPREHENSION decoding speech

More information

Copyright The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Chapter 6 Patterns of Inheritance

Copyright The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Chapter 6 Patterns of Inheritance Chapter 6 Patterns of Inheritance Genetics Explains and Predicts Inheritance Patterns Genetics can explain how these poodles look different. Section 10.1 Genetics Explains and Predicts Inheritance Patterns

More information

Ch 8 Practice Questions

Ch 8 Practice Questions Ch 8 Practice Questions Multiple Choice Identify the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1. What fraction of offspring of the cross Aa Aa is homozygous for the dominant allele?

More information

100% were red eyed = red is dominant - He then bred 2 offspring from the F1 generation F1 = Rr x Rr

100% were red eyed = red is dominant - He then bred 2 offspring from the F1 generation F1 = Rr x Rr 7. Gene Linkage and Cross-over Thomas Hunt Morgan 1910 Working with fruit flies he proved that genes on the same chromosome tended to be inherited together. = Linked genes ie. Eye color and hair color

More information

2018 Version. NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948

2018 Version. NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948 2018 Version NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948 Achievement Criteria (Part ONE) AS 90948 S1.9 Biological concepts and processes relating to the inheritable nature of DNA will be selected from:

More information

Pre-AP Biology Unit 7 Genetics Review Outline

Pre-AP Biology Unit 7 Genetics Review Outline Unit 7 Genetics Review Outline Pre-AP Biology 2017-2018 LT 1 - I can explain the relationships among alleles, genes, chromosomes, genotypes, and phenotypes. This target covers application of the vocabulary

More information

A Genomewide Scan Identifies Two Novel Loci Involved in Specific Language Impairment *

A Genomewide Scan Identifies Two Novel Loci Involved in Specific Language Impairment * Am. J. Hum. Genet. 70:384 398, 2002 A Genomewide Scan Identifies Two Novel Loci Involved in Specific Language Impairment * The SLI Consortium * Approximately 4% of English-speaking children are affected

More information

Lecture 17: Human Genetics. I. Types of Genetic Disorders. A. Single gene disorders

Lecture 17: Human Genetics. I. Types of Genetic Disorders. A. Single gene disorders Lecture 17: Human Genetics I. Types of Genetic Disorders A. Single gene disorders B. Multifactorial traits 1. Mutant alleles at several loci acting in concert C. Chromosomal abnormalities 1. Physical changes

More information

Mendelian Genetics. Activity. Part I: Introduction. Instructions

Mendelian Genetics. Activity. Part I: Introduction. Instructions Activity Part I: Introduction Some of your traits are inherited and cannot be changed, while others can be influenced by the environment around you. There has been ongoing research in the causes of cancer.

More information