Neural Correlates of Anxiety Associated with Obsessive-Compulsive Symptom Dimensions in Normal Volunteers

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Neural Correlates of Anxiety Associated with Obsessive-Compulsive Symptom Dimensions in Normal Volunteers"

Transcription

1 Neural Correlates of Anxiety Associated with Obsessive-Compulsive Symptom Dimensions in Normal Volunteers David Mataix-Cols, Sarah Cullen, Kezia Lange, Fernando Zelaya, Christopher Andrew, Edson Amaro, Michael J. Brammer, Steven C.R. Williams, Anne Speckens, and Mary L. Phillips Background: The neural correlates of anxiety associated with obsessive-compulsive symptomlike provocation in normal volunteers are unknown. Methods: Ten healthy volunteers participated in four functional magnetic resonance experiments. Subjects were scanned while viewing alternating blocks of emotional (normally aversive, washing-relevant, checking-relevant, or hoarding-relevant pictures) and neutral pictures, and imagining scenarios related to the content of each picture type. Nonparametric brain mapping analyses were used. Results: In response to the provocative pictures in all experiments, increases in subjective anxiety and activation in bilateral ventral prefrontal, limbic, dorsal prefrontal, and visual regions were demonstrated. Anxiety related to different symptom dimensions was associated with different patterns of activation: provocation of washing-relevant anxiety predominantly activated dorsal and ventral prefrontal regions; checking-relevant anxiety predominantly activated dorsal prefrontal regions; and hoardingrelevant anxiety predominantly activated ventral prefrontal regions and the left amygdala. Conclusions: Our findings support a dimensional model of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) whereby 1) the brain systems implicated in the mediation of anxiety in response to symptom-related material in normal subjects are similar to those identified in OCD patients during symptom provocation, and 2) anxiety associated with different symptom dimensions is associated with differential patterns of activation of these neural systems. Further investigation of the neural basis of OCD symptom dimensions is required. Biol Psychiatry 2003 Society of Biological Psychiatry From the Division of Psychological Medicine (DM-C, SC, KL, MLP), Departments of Neuroimaging Research (FZ, CA, EA, SCRW), Biostatistics (MJB), and Psychology (AS), GKT School of Medicine and Institute of Psychiatry, London, United Kingdom. Address reprint requests to David Mataix-Cols, Ph.D., Department of Psychological Medicine, GKT School of Medicine and Institute of Psychiatry, 5th Floor Thomas Guy House, Guy s Hospital, London SE1 9RT United Kingdom. Received March 26, 2002; revised June 12, 2002; accepted June 21, Key Words: Obsessive-compulsive disorder, symptom dimensions, hoarding, neuroimaging, fmri, emotion Introduction Functional neuroimaging studies have substantially increased our understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Symptom provocation paradigms have used positron emission tomography (PET), single photon emission tomography (SPECT), or functional resonance imaging (fmri) to compare, in a single session, the patterns of brain activation during OCD symptom provocation and control states (Saxena and Rauch 2000). Although findings are discrepant, they strongly associate OCD symptoms with activation of the orbitofrontal cortex, with less consistent involvement of lateral frontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, temporal cortex, striatum, thalamus, amygdala, and insular cortex (Adler et al 2000; Breiter et al 1996; McGuire et al 1994; Rauch et al 1994; Saxena and Rauch 2000). Obsessive-compulsive disorder is heterogeneous, with symptoms that can be summarized in a few consistent, temporally stable, dimensions (Baer 1994; Leckman et al 1997; Mataix-Cols et al 1999a, 2002a; Summerfeldt et al 1999). Despite this phenotypic heterogeneity, most previous neuroimaging studies of OCD have grouped together patients with diverse symptom patterns, although they have included patients with predominantly contamination/ washing or mixed symptoms. To our knowledge, only three functional neuroimaging studies have examined the neural correlates of different symptoms in OCD. In one PET study (Cottraux et al 1996), all patients were predominantly checkers, and symptoms were provoked using an individually tailored script-driven paradigm that prompted the urge to check. They found significant increases in orbitofrontal and superior temporal cortex activity, and-,contrary to most previous findings, lower activity in the basal ganglia compared with control subjects. In another PET study, Rauch et al (1998) found significant correla Society of Biological Psychiatry /03/$30.00 doi:10/1016/s (03)

2 Obsessive-Compulsive Symptom Dimensions BIOL PSYCHIATRY 483 tions between the obsessions and checking dimension (Leckman et al 1997) and activations in bilateral striatum and between the contamination/washing dimension and activations in bilateral anterior cingulate, left orbitofrontal, and other cortical areas. Symmetry/ordering symptoms tended to be negatively correlated with regional cerebral blood flow in right striatum; however, these patients were examined while performing a continuous performance task rather than during the presentation of relevant stimuli. More recently, Phillips et al (2000) compared OCD patients who had mainly washing or checking symptoms with normal control subjects while viewing pictures of either normally disgusting scenes or washer-relevant pictures using fmri. When viewing washing-related pictures, only washers demonstrated activations in regions implicated in disgust perception, that is, visual regions and the anterior insula (Phillips et al 1997, 1998; Sprengelmeyer et al 1998). Checkers demonstrated activations in frontostriatal regions and the thalamus. Taken together, these studies suggest that the discrepancy within findings of previous functional imaging studies of OCD may be the result of relatively small sample sizes and phenotypic variations among their subject groups. Most individuals in the general population experience normal obsessive and compulsive phenomena at some time that resemble the form and content of abnormal, pathologic phenomena (Muris et al 1997; Rachman and de Silva 1978). Furthermore, studies involving nonclinical samples have demonstrated that normal individuals with high scores on self-administered scales of obsessions and compulsions show striking similarities to OCD patients regarding clinical and personality characteristics, as well as cognitive functioning (Gibbs 1996; Mataix-Cols et al 1999b, 1999c; Sher et al 1983). To date, however, little is known about the neural correlates of anxiety associated with symptom provocation in the normal population. To our knowledge, only three symptom provocation studies in OCD have used normal control groups (Breiter et al 1996; Cottraux et al 1996; Phillips et al 2000). All three of these studies found that symptom provocation induced some degree of anxiety not only among patients but also among normal control subjects, although to a lower extent. Breiter et al (1996) found that no brain areas were significantly related to symptom provocation (contaminants) in five normal subjects. Cottraux et al (1996) found significant increases in orbitofrontal cortex in both patients and control subjects and, interestingly, greater activations in thalamus and putamen in normal subjects than in patients during symptom provocation (aggressive obsessions and checking compulsions). Phillips et al (2000) found that normal subjects activated predominantly visual and frontal regions, cerebellum, middle and inferior temporal gyri and the hippocampus in response to washing-related pictures. Differences in the technology used (fmri vs. PET), the modality of stimuli presentation (exposure in vivo vs. script-driven auditory stimulation vs. visual stimulation), and the type of provocative symptom (mainly contaminants vs. mainly aggressive obsessions) could account for the differences among these three studies. Our study aimed to examine the neural correlates of experimentally induced anxiety associated with obsessivecompulsive symptomlike stimuli in a sample of normal subjects. It also aimed to validate a novel symptom provocation paradigm that combines two modalities of stimulus presentation, verbal instructions and visual presentation of symptom-related material. We hypothesized that, in normal volunteers, 1) anxiety would be provoked in response to all types of symptom-related material (Breiter et al 1996; Cottraux et al 1996), 2) anxiety provocation would activate regions previously identified during symptom provocation in OCD patients (orbitofrontal cortex, lateral frontal cortex, anterior cingulate, temporal cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus, amygdala, insula), and 3) distinct patterns of neural response would be associated with the anxiety related to each symptom dimension. Specifically, we hypothesized that 1) provocation of contamination/ washing-related anxiety would activate predominantly areas involved in emotion and disgust perception (i.e., ventral prefrontal and limbic regions; Phillips et al 2000; Rauch et al 1998); 2) provocation of checking-related anxiety would predominantly activate regions associated with attention but not emotion perception (i.e., dorsal prefrontal cortex, thalamus, and striatal regions, but not paralimbic and limbic regions; Phillips et al 2000; Rauch et al 1998); 3) although there are no previous studies on the neural correlates of hoarding symptoms, we hypothesized that hoarding-related anxiety would also activate areas involved in emotion and disgust perception; and 4) areas involved in visual processing would be activated in response to all types of symptom-related anxiety. Methods and Materials Subjects Ten right-handed healthy volunteers (five men, five women) were recruited among colleagues and ancillary staff at the Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College London. Mean age was 27.6 (SD 5.5; range 20 36). They reported no history of neurologic or psychiatric disorder by clinical screen (DM-C) and were on no medications at the time of the study. The Maudsley Hospital ethics committee approved the study protocol, and all subjects signed an informed consent form before their participation. Stimuli Fifty color pictures of scenes rated as aversive/disgusting by normal subjects (e.g., insects, mutilated bodies, decaying food)

3 484 BIOL PSYCHIATRY D. Mataix-Cols et al and 50 pictures of neutral scenes (e.g., furniture, nature scenes, household items) were selected from a standard set of stimuli (Lang et al 1997). These stimuli were carefully chosen to avoid resembling common triggers of OCD symptoms. In addition, pictures of washing-, checking-, and hoarding-related material (50 of each type) were obtained with a standard digital camera (Canon PhotoShot S20). For each symptom type, three clinicians with experience in OCD had previously listed the most common items that were provocative of anxiety and the urge to ritualize in OCD patients. Examples of washing-related pictures included a public telephone/toilet, money, a syringe, and an ashtray. Examples of checking-related pictures included electric appliances (computer, kettle, light switch), an open door, and a purse. Examples of hoarding-related scenes included old newspapers/ magazines, old clothes/toys, empty bottles/cans, and trash bins. Two hundred and fifty scenes were selected after an independent group of nine normal volunteers (unrelated to the study) had rated an originally larger pool of pictures according to their level of visual complexity, anxiety, and disgust on a 0 3 scale (0 nil;3 a lot). Pictures that were too simple or too complex were excluded, and an effort was made to avoid using washing-related pictures that could be perceived as very disgusting by normal individuals. The final 250 stimuli were well matched with regard to visual complexity, and, as intended, the normally aversive/ disgusting pictures induced more anxiety and disgust than the other three types of pictures (data not shown). One neutral picture of the World Trade Center in New York City was substituted with another neutral picture in the middle of the study following the terrorist attacks of September 11, Symptom Provocation Paradigm All subjects participated in four, 6-min experiments consisting of alternating blocks of emotional (normally aversive/disgusting, washing-, checking-, or hoarding-related pictures) and neutral pictures. The order in which the four experiments were conducted was fully counterbalanced, as was the order of the emotional and neutral conditions within each experiment. Before the presentation of each set of pictures, subjects were played a prerecorded voice file by means of high fidelity pneumatic headphones, instructing them to imagine being in a particular situation while looking at the scenes they were about to see (there were five slightly different recordings per type of stimuli in each experiment, to avoid monotony). Some examples of these instructions are the following: For normally aversive/disgusting pictures: Imagine that you must touch or stand by the following objects. For washing-related pictures: Imagine that you must come into contact with what s shown in the following pictures without washing yourself afterward. For checking-related pictures: Imagine that you are not sure whether you switched off or locked the following objects and it is impossible for you to go back and check. For hoarding-related pictures: Imagine that the following objects belong to you and that you must throw them away forever. For neutral scenes: Imagine that you are completely relaxed while looking at the following scenes. After each set of pictures, another prerecorded sound file of the question How anxious do you feel? was played to the subjects. Subjects had to rate their subjective anxiety by answering the question verbally with a number from 0 (no anxiety) to8 (extreme anxiety). Scanning was interrupted while both of the sound files mentioned earlier were played and while subjects gave their responses, so that scanning occurred only during picture presentation. The investigators heard these responses through the speaker system and noted them. Only one such scale was used because in previous studies, OCD and general anxiety analog scales were highly intercorrelated (Rauch et al 1994). These procedures were carefully explained and also provided in writing to all subjects before entering the scanner to make sure they fully understood the experimental procedure. Rating Scales Each participant s visual imagery ability was assessed with the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VIVQ; Marks 1989) before the scan. The VIVQ is a 16-item questionnaire requesting a response on a 1 5 scale. Scores range from 0 80; the lower the score, the better the ability to imagine visual scenes. In this sample, scores on the VVIQ suggested good visual imagery abilities (mean 36.3, SD 13.7, range 16 55). The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al 1961) and the state subscale of the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S; Spielberger et al 1983) were also administered immediately before the scan to account for psychologic state variables. As expected, the BDI (mean 2.1, SD 2.5, range 0 7) and STAI-S (mean 32.4, SD 3.9, range 27 40) scores suggested minimal depression and moderate state anxiety levels. The presence and degree of obsessive compulsive symptoms in this sample was assessed using the Padua Inventory (PI; Sanavio 1988). The PI is a 60-item self-administered questionnaire requesting a response on a 0 4 scale regarding degree of disturbance. Scores range from Its British adaptation (Macdonald and de Silva 1999) has four subscales: impaired control over mental activities/doubting (28 items; score range: 0 112), contamination (10 items; score range: 0 40), checking (12 items; score range: 0 48), and worries about losing control over motor behavior (11 items; score range: 0 44). Scores on the PI suggested low levels of obsessive compulsive symptoms in this sample (total score: mean 14.4, SD 13.5, range 2 47; impaired mental control: mean 7.6, SD 7.4, range 1 23; contamination: mean 4.1, SD 3.0; range 1 10; Checking: mean 3.5, SD 6.8, range 0 22; worries about loosing control: mean.8, SD 1.2, range 0 3). Because the PI does not cover hoarding symptoms, the Saving Inventory Revised (SI-R; Frost et al, unpublished report) was also administered. The SI-R is a revised version of the wellvalidated Hoarding Scale (Frost and Gross 1993) and consists of 26 items, scored from 0 4, that assess various components of hoarding behavior (score range 0 104). Preliminary studies on the SI-R suggest that its psychometric properties are sound (Frost et al, unpublished report). Mean scores on the SI-R were low (mean 6.2, SD 3.8, range 0 11). Finally, sensitivity to disgust was assessed with the Disgust Scale (DS; Haidt et al 1994). It consists of 32 items, and its

4 Obsessive-Compulsive Symptom Dimensions BIOL PSYCHIATRY 485 scores range from Scores on the DS (mean 41, SD 10, range 30 58) were comparable to the published norms (Haidt et al 1994). Image Acquisition Gradient echo echoplanar (EPI) images were acquired on a GE Sigma 1.5T Neurovascular system (General Electric, Milwaukee, WI) at the Maudsley Hospital, London. One hundred T2*- weighted whole-brain volumes depicting blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) contrast (Ogawa et al 1990) and consisting of 16 slices oriented according to the bicommissural plane (thickness 7 mm, 0.7 mm gap) were acquired over 6 min for each of the four experiments (TR 2.0 sec; TE 40 msec; flip angle 70; matrix). In each 20-sec stimulus presentation block, subjects viewed either 10 provocative or 10 neutral pictures. Each picture was presented for 1950 msec, with an interstimulus interval of 50 msec. Ten whole-brain volumes were acquired during each stimulus presentation block. Each stimulus block was followed by 1) an 8-sec period of complete silence during which subjects were asked to rate their level of anxiety; 2) a further 8-sec period during which the subjects listened to a sound file containing instructions pertinent to the next stimulus block. Four dummyvolumes were excited during this 8-sec period using exactly the same radio frequency (RF) pulses and gradient slice selection parameter, with the same repetition time of 2 sec to allow the magnetization to reach an equilibrium amplitude before the next period of data acquisition. The frequency-encoding gradient was turned off during this period to minimize acoustic noise and to ensure that the subjects heard the instructions clearly (Van De Moortele et al 1998). The four dummy volumes were later discarded from the time series. Individual brain activation maps were coregistered to a whole head gradient-recalled echo planar imaging scan of superior spatial resolution acquired on all subjects. This structural scan had the following acquisition parameters: TE 40 msec, TR 3000 msec, field of view 24 cm, image resolution , number of slices 43, slice thickness 3.0 mm, interslice gap.3 mm, number of signal averages 8. Figure 1. Differences in neural response to normally aversive, washing-, checking-, and hoarding-related stimuli. The left side of the brain is shown on the right side of the image and vice versa. OFC, orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex (BA11); MTG, middle temporal gyrus (BA37); IFG, inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44 and 45); A, amygdala; ACG, anterior cingulate gyrus (BA24/32); VLP, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (BA47). Statistical Analyses The effects of symptom provocation on subjective ratings of anxiety were assessed using repeated-measures analyses of variance to compare the provoked and neutral conditions in all four experiments. Following motion correction (Bullmore et al 1999), periodic change in T2*-weighted signal intensity at the (fundamental) experimentally determined frequency of alternation between A and B conditions ( 1/60 Hz in all four experiments) was estimated by an iterated least squares fit of a sinusoidal regression model to the fmri time series observed at each voxel (Bullmore et al 1996). This model included sine and cosine waves at the fundamental AB frequency of the experimental input function, parameterized by coefficients {g, d}. The power of periodic response to the input function was estimated by (g2 d2); and this fundamental power divided by its SE yielded a

5 486 BIOL PSYCHIATRY D. Mataix-Cols et al Table 1. Demographic Characteristics and Mean Subjective Anxiety Ratings of 10 Healthy Volunteers Subject No. Sex Age Disgust Experiment a Washing Experiment b Checking Experiment c Hoarding Experiment d Disgust Neutral Difference Washing Neutral Difference Checking Neutral Difference Hoarding Neutral Difference 1 Male Female Female Female Male Male Male Male Female Female Mean (SD) (5.5) (1.2) (.9) (1.1) (1.5) (.6) (1.1) (1.5) (.5) (1.3) (.7) (.5) (.3) a paired t (9) 14.1, p.001 (2-tailed) b paired t (9) 5.8, p.001 (2-tailed) c paired t (9) 6.1, p.001 (2-tailed) d paired t (9) 5.6, p.001 (2-tailed) standardized test statistic, the fundamental power quotient (FPQ), at each voxel. Parametric maps representing FPQ observed at each intracerebral voxel were constructed. To sample the distribution of FPQ under the null hypothesis that observed values of FPQ were not determined by experimental design (with few assumptions), all images observed in each anatomic plane were randomly permuted, and FPQ was estimated exactly as described earlier in each permuted time series. This process was repeated 10 times, resulting in 10 permuted parametric maps of FPQ at each plane for each subject. Observed and randomized FPQ maps were transformed into the standard space of Talairach and Tournoux (1988) and smoothed by a two-dimensional Gaussian filter with full width half maximum 11 mm. This filter size was chosen to accommodate regional differences in brain anatomy between subjects (Clark et al 1996) and has been employed in previous studies examining neural responses to emotional stimuli (e.g., Phillips et al 1997, 1999). The median observed FPQ at each intracerebral voxel in standard space was tested against a critical value of the permutation distribution for median FPQ ascertained from the permuted FPQ maps (Brammer et al 1997). To estimate differences in mean power of functional activation between the four experiments, we conducted six pairwise repeated measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) at each intracerebral voxel of the standardized power maps after their coregistration in standard (Talairach) space. We used a nonparametric model of inference on spatially informed test statistics to identify clusters of activation within brain regions that showed a significant difference (p.01) in mean power of response between experiments; for full details of this method and its validation see Bullmore et al (1999). Results Subjective Ratings Analog scale ratings reflected increased anxiety during provoked versus neutral conditions in all four experiments (Table 1). These differences were large for the normally aversive experiment and smaller, but highly significant, for the washing, checking, and hoarding experiments. Generic Brain Activation Maps ACTIVATION ASSOCIATED WITH NORMALLY AVER- SIVE STIMULI. Compared with neutral pictures, normally aversive pictures activated predominantly bilateral ventral prefrontal cortical regions (ventral anterior cingulate gyrus, orbitofrontal and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices), limbic regions (hippocampus, insula, parahippocampal gyrus, and amygdala) and regions involved in visual processing. Additional regions included bilateral dorsal and dorsomedial frontal regions (pre- and postcentral gyri, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus), cerebellum, thalamus, caudate nucleus, and putamen (see Table 2). ACTIVATION ASSOCIATED WITH WASHING-RE- LATED STIMULI. Compared with neutral pictures, washingrelated pictures activated predominantly bilateral visual regions, bilateral dorsal and dorsomedial prefrontal regions (medial and superior frontal gyri, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, precentral gyrus, dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus), and bilateral ventral prefrontal and limbic regions (ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, medial frontal gyrus (BA10), ventral anterior cingulate, insula, and parahippocampal gyrus). Additional regions included bilateral cerebellum and right putamen (Table 3). ACTIVATION ASSOCIATED WITH CHECKING-RE- LATED STIMULI. Compared with neutral pictures, checkingrelated pictures activated predominantly bilateral visual regions and dorsal prefrontal regions (medial frontal gyrus, preand postcentral gyri, dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex). There was little activation in

6 Obsessive-Compulsive Symptom Dimensions BIOL PSYCHIATRY 487 Table 2. Generically Activated Brain Regions in the Normally Aversive Experiment Brain Region Brodmann s Area(s) Side x a y a z a No. of Voxels p Value Ventral Prefrontal and Limbic Ventral anterior cingulate/ 25/11 R orbitofrontal gyrus 25 L Ventrolateral prefrontal 47 L R Ventral anterior cingulate 25/32 R L Hippocampus L R Insula R L Parahippocampal gyrus 35 L R Amygdala 34 R Visual Processing Fusiform gyrus 19 L R Inferior parietal lobe 40 L R Inferior temporal gyrus 37 R L Middle occipital gyrus 19/39 R L Precuneus 7 L R Superior parietal lobe 7 R L Dorsal/Dorsomedial Frontal Precentral gyrus 4/6 R /6 L Inferior frontal gyrus 44 L Inferior frontal gyrus 45 L R Postcentral gyrus 1/2 L Medial frontal gyrus 9 R L Anterior cingulate gyrus 24 R L Other Cerebellum L R Thalamus R L Caudate nucleus L R Putamen L R a The cluster with the largest number of voxels within each region. Talairach coordinates refer to the voxel with the maximum fundamental power quotient (FPQ) in each cluster. paralimbic and limbic regions (ventrolateral prefrontal gyrus, orbitofrontal cortex, insula, amygdala). Additional activated areas included bilateral cerebellum (Table 4). ACTIVATION ASSOCIATED WITH HOARDING-RE- LATED STIMULI. Compared with neutral pictures, hoarding-related pictures activated predominantly ventral prefrontal and limbic regions (orbitofrontal cortex, ventral anterior cingulate cortex, medial/superior frontal gyrus, anterior insula, ventrolateral prefrontal gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, and amygdala) and visual regions. There were few dorsal prefrontal activations (middle and superior frontal gyrus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, precentral gyrus). Additional areas included bilateral cerebellum (Table 5).

7 488 BIOL PSYCHIATRY D. Mataix-Cols et al Table 3. Generically Activated Brain Regions in the Washing Experiment Brain Region Brodmann s Area(s) Side x a y a z a No. of Voxels p Value Visual Processing Precuneus 7/31 R L Fusiform gyrus 37 R Cuneus 19 L R Occipital gyrus 19 R L Middle occipital gyrus 18/19 L R Inferior temporal gyrus 19 L R Dorsal/Dorsomedial Frontal Medial frontal gyrus 6 R Inferior frontal gyrus 44 L R Inferior frontal gyrus 45 R L Middle frontal gyrus 6 L R Precentral gyrus 6 L R Precentral gyrus 4 L R Middle frontal gyrus 9 R L Anterior cingulate gyrus 24/32 R Medial frontal gyrus 8 L R Ventral Prefrontal and Limbic Ventrolateral prefrontal 47 R L Orbitofrontal gyrus 11 R L Medial frontal gyrus 10 L R Ventral anterior cingulate 24/32 L R R Insula L Parahippocampal gyrus L Other Cerebellum L R Putamen R a The cluster with the largest number of voxels within each region. Talairach coordinates refer to the voxel with the maximum fundamental power quotient (FPQ) in each cluster. Differences in Neural Response to Normally Aversive, Washing-, Checking-, and Hoarding- Related Material Results of the repeated measures ANCOVAs comparing all possible combinations of two experiments are shown in Table 6 and Figure 1. Normally aversive pictures induced significantly more activation in right middle temporal gyrus and right orbitofrontal cortex than washing-related pictures and in those same areas plus right inferior frontal gyrus compared with checking-related pictures. There were no significant differences between normally aversive pictures and hoarding-related pictures. Hoarding-related pictures induced significantly greater activation in the left amygdala and right inferior frontal gyrus compared with checking-related pictures. Checking-related pictures induced significantly greater activation in the left dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus compared with hoarding-related pictures. Washing-related pictures induced significantly greater activation in bilateral

8 Obsessive-Compulsive Symptom Dimensions BIOL PSYCHIATRY 489 Table 4. Generically Activated Brain Regions in the Checking Experiment Brain Region Brodmann s Area(s) Side x a y a z a No. of Voxels p Value Visual Processing Fusiform gyrus 19 L R Precuneus 7 R L Cuneus 18/19 L Inferior temporal gyrus 19 L Middle occipital gyrus 37/19 L R Inferior occipital gyrus 18 R Middle temporal gyrus 21 L Occipital gyrus 19 L R Superior temporal gyrus 22 L Superior temporal gyrus 38 L Superior occipital gyrus 19 R Cuneus 31 R Dorsal/Dorsomedial Frontal Medial frontal gyrus 6 R Precentral gyrus 6/4 L /4 R Anterior cingulate gyrus 24 R L Inferior frontal gyrus 44 L R Middle frontal gyrus 46 R L Postcentral gyrus 40 L R Ventral Prefrontal and Limbic Ventrolateral prefrontal 47 R Insula L Orbitofrontal gyrus 11 R Amygdala R Ventrolateral prefrontal 47 R L Other Cerebellum R L a The cluster with the largest number of voxels within each region. Talairach coordinates refer to the voxel with the maximum fundamental power quotient (FPQ) in each cluster. inferior frontal gyri compared with checking-related pictures. On the right side, this activation extended to the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Finally, washing-related pictures induced significantly greater activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus compared with hoarding-related pictures. In these analyses, the expected number of false positive activated clusters was 2.91 and the observed number of activated clusters, 12. a z score but is used in this context because in nonparametric tests, none of the data distributional assumptions are made that underlie the use of a z score. There were no significant correlations between fmri signal intensity within the clusters of interest and subjective anxiety ratings (data not shown). Likewise, there were no significant correlations with scores on any of the questionnaire measures (VIVQ, BDI, STAI-S, PI, SI-R, DS). Correlations between Clinical Scales and Patterns of Neural Response Post hoc analyses were conducted correlating the intensity of activation within the two largest clusters of activation in each of the four experiments. In these analyses, the FPQ was averaged over each cluster. Note that the FPQ is analogous to Discussion Our findings demonstrate that OCD symptomlike anxiety can be reliably induced by appropriate, provocative stimuli in normal volunteers. As expected, presentation of normally aversive stimuli was associated with significant

9 490 BIOL PSYCHIATRY D. Mataix-Cols et al Table 5. Generically Activated Brain Regions in the Hoarding Experiment Brain Regions Brodmann s Area(s) Side x a y a z a No. of Voxels p Value Ventral Prefrontal and Limbic Orbitofrontal gyrus 11 R L Ventral anterior cingulate 24/32 R L Medial/superior frontal gyrus 10 L Insula R Parahippocampal gyrus 28 L Ventrolateral prefrontal 47 R L Amygdala L Visual Processing Precuneus 7/18 L R Cuneus 19 L R Fusiform gyrus 37/19 R /19 L Middle occipital gyrus 18/19 L R Lingual gyrus 18 L Superior temporal gyrus 38 R L Dorsal/Dorsomedial Frontal Middle frontal gyrus 9 L Inferior frontal gyrus 44 L R Inferior frontal gyrus 45 R L Precentral gyrus 4 L Other Cerebellum R L a The cluster with the largest number of voxels within each region. Talairach coordinates refer to the voxel with the maximum fundamental power quotient (FPQ) in each cluster. increases in subjective anxiety. In addition, the presentation of OCD symptomlike material (washing, checking, and hoarding experiments) was also associated with significant increases in anxiety. Previous OCD studies that included normal control groups have reported similar findings (Breiter et al 1996; Cottraux et al 1996). Overall, anxiety in response to all types of provocative stimuli was associated with activation in three main neural systems: 1) bilateral ventral prefrontal (orbitofrontal and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, ventral anterior cingulate gyrus) and limbic regions (anterior insula, amygdala, parahippocampal gyri), 2) dorsal prefrontal regions (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, pre- and postcentral gyri), and 3) regions involved in visual processing (fusiform gyrus, cuneus, precuneus, middle occipital gyrus, middle and superior temporal regions). Additional areas included cerebellum, thalamus, putamen, and caudate nucleus. These activations were most significant in response to normally aversive pictures but were also present in response to the washing-, checking-, and hoarding-related pictures. These areas have also been consistently activated in previous neuroimaging studies of patients with OCD (for a review, see Saxena and Rauch 2000). Few previous studies have examined the neural correlates of anxiety associated with OCD symptomlike provocation in normal individuals. Breiter et al (1996) failed to report activations in any brain regions in five healthy volunteers, but their study may have been insufficiently powered. In support of findings from other studies (Cottraux et al 1996; Phillips et al 2000), areas previously identified as dysfunctional in OCD (including the orbitofrontal cortex, lateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, and limbic regions) were activated during OCD symptomlike anxiety in our normal subjects. Many of these areas have also been associated with the induction of emotional states in normal subjects (e.g., George et al 1995; Mayberg et al 1999; Pardo et al 1993; Reiman et al

10 Obsessive-Compulsive Symptom Dimensions BIOL PSYCHIATRY 491 Table 6. Repeated Measures Analyses of Covariance Comparing Generic Brain Activation Maps among the Normally Aversive, Washing, Checking, and Hoarding Experiments in 10 Normal Volunteers Comparison Region (Brodmann s Area) Side x y z No. of Voxels p Normally Aversive Washing-related Middle temporal gyrus (37) R Orbitofrontal gyrus (11) R Normally Aversive Checking-related Middle temporal gyrus (37) R Orbitofrontal gyrus (11) R Inferior frontal gyrus (45) R Normally Aversive Hoarding-related No significant differences Hoarding- Checking-related Amygdala L Inferior frontal gyrus (45) R Checking- Hoarding-related Anterior cingulate gyrus (24/32) L Washing- Checking-related Inferior frontal gyrus/ventrolateral R prefrontal (45/47) Inferior frontal gyrus (44) L Washing- Hoarding-related Inferior frontal gyrus (44) L ; Teasdale et al 1999). Our findings suggest that the anxiety associated with both pathologic and normal obsessional states involves a pattern of neural response previously shown to be important for the perception and generation of emotional states. Therefore, our findings indicate the presence of a dysfunctional emotional regulatory system in OCD whereby exaggerated emotional responses occur in inappropriate contexts. The second important finding of this study is that different OCD symptom dimensions are associated with differential patterns of activation in these neural systems. Provocation of washing-related anxiety was predominantly associated with a pattern of activation within bilateral dorsomedial prefrontal cortices, ventral prefrontal cortex/limbic regions, and visual regions. Checking-related pictures induced activations mainly in dorsal prefrontal and visual regions, with less activation in ventral prefrontal or limbic regions. These findings are consistent with those of Phillips et al (2000), who found a differential response to washing-related pictures between OCD washers and checkers; only the former perceived these pictures as disgusting and activated areas normally involved in disgust perception (i.e., insula and visual regions; Phillips et al 1997, 1998; Sprengelmeyer et al 1998). Our findings of predominant activation within dorsal prefrontal cortical regions, regions that previously have been associated with performance of attentional, nonemotional tasks (for a review, see Drevets and Raichle 1998), during checkingrelated anxiety suggests that this type of anxiety and checking symptoms of OCD may be associated with increased attention to detail rather than an exaggerated perception of emotion. The provocation of hoarding-related anxiety was associated with activation predominantly in ventral prefrontal, limbic, and visual structures, with less activation in dorsal prefrontal areas. In fact, there were no significant differences in activation within any regions in response to normally aversive and hoarding-related pictures. Interestingly, hoarding-related pictures were associated with significantly greater activation in the left amygdala compared with checking-related pictures. Our findings suggest that hoarding symptoms are associated with strong emotional reactions. Studies with both nonclinical and clinical populations have suggested that hoarding is associated with a fear of loosing important things that may be needed later and overemotional attachment to possessions (Frost and Steketee 1998). In addition, recent studies have suggested that patients with hoarding symptoms present a higher prevalence of personality disorders, especially from the anxious-fearful cluster, compared with patients with other OCD symptoms (Frost et al 2000; Mataix-Cols et al 2000) and also have poorer treatment outcomes with serotonergic agents and behavior therapy (Black et al 1998; Mataix- Cols et al 1999a, 2002b; Saxena et al 2002; Winsberg et al 1999). If our demonstration of increased involvement of the amygdala during hoarding-related anxiety is replicated in OCD patients with predominantly hoarding symptoms, this may provide a neurobiological basis for these previous findings. We speculate that hoarding and checking symptoms represent two extremes of a continuum of activation in two neural systems: a ventral prefrontal/limbic system and a dorsal frontal system. We predict that hoarding symptoms would be associated with most significant activation within ventral prefrontal cortex and limbic regions but not within dorsal prefrontal cortical regions, whereas checking symptoms would be associated with most significant activation within the dorsal prefrontal cortex, rather than

11 492 BIOL PSYCHIATRY D. Mataix-Cols et al ventral prefrontal cortex and limbic regions. We predict that washing symptoms would be associated both with significant ventral prefrontal cortical/limbic activation but also with activation within dorsal prefrontal cortical regions, the latter possibly associated with attention to symptom-provoking material or attempts to suppress the anxiety. Limitations of our study include the lack of a structured screening instrument and the relatively small sample size. We found no correlations between fmri magnitudes and anxiety scores or scores on any of the questionnaires. It is possible that these negative results are due to small score ranges and standard deviations in these measures among our healthy volunteers. In OCD patients, it will be interesting to correlate scores on each of the quantitative symptom dimensions derived from the Yale Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Mataix-Cols et al 1999a) and patterns of brain activity. Another limitation is that only anxiety ratings were obtained after each block of pictures. Other forms of discomfort (e.g., disgust) may have been induced by the images; however, our design did not permit longer silence periods between blocks of stimuli. This study has important methodologic differences compared with previous symptom provocation studies in OCD. First, it combined two modalities of stimulus presentation, verbal instructions and symptom-related stimuli. Second, it used a priori selected provocative stimulus material instead of stimuli individually tailored to each subject s symptoms. Although OCD is heterogeneous, there are only a limited number of themes that normally trigger symptoms in these patients, and these symptoms tend to cluster in a consistent manner, as factor-analytical studies have shown (Baer 1994; Leckman et al 1997; Mataix-Cols et al 1999a; Summerfeldt et al 1999). The employment of a large number of pictures representative of each symptom dimension therefore allowed the inclusion of the triggers relevant to most patients (and also normal control subjects). One obvious advantage of this paradigm is that identical experimental stimuli are presented to all subjects, so that interindividual differences in patterns of activation are unable to be related to the experience of different stimuli. The results of our study provide support for a dimensional model of OCD whereby 1) the brain systems implicated in the mediation of anxiety in response to symptom material in normal subjects are similar to those previously identified during symptom provocation in OCD and 2) anxiety associated with different symptom dimensions is associated with differential patterns of activation of these neural systems. If these findings were replicated in OCD patients they would suggest that previous inconsistencies in neuroimaging studies of OCD patients might be in part due to the recruitment of patients with different predominant symptom dimensions. This study was funded by a project grant from the Wellcome Trust to Drs. Phillips, Mataix-Cols, and Speckens. Dr. Mataix-Cols was supported by a Marie Curie Fellowship from the European Union. References Adler CB, McDonough-Ryan P, Sax KW, Holland SK, Arndt SA, Strakowski SM (2000): FMRI of neuronal activation with symptom provocation in unmedicated patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. J Psychiatr Res 34: Baer L (1994): Factor analysis of symptom subtypes of obsessive compulsive disorder and their relation to personality and tic disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 55: Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J (1961): An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4: Black DW, Monahan P, Gable J, Blum N, Clancy G, Baker P (1998): Hoarding and treatment response in 38 nondepressed subjects with obsessive compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 59: Brammer M, Bullmore ET, Simmons A, Williams SCR, Grasby PM, Howard RJ, et al (1997): Generic brain activation mapping in functional magnetic resonance imaging: A nonparametric approach. Magn Reson Imaging 15: Breiter HC, Rauch SL, Kwong KK, Baher JR, Weisskoff RM, Kennedy DN, et al (1996): Functional magnetic resonance imaging of symptom provocation in obsessive compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 53: Bullmore ET, Brammer M, Rabe-Hesketh S, Curtis V, Morris R, Williams SCR, et al (1999): Methods for the diagnosis and treatment of stimulus correlated motion in generic brain activation studies using fmri. Hum Brain Mapp 7: Bullmore ET, Brammer M, Williams SCR, Rabe-Hesketh S, Janot N, David AS, et al (1996): Statistical methods of estimation and inference for functional MR image analysis. Magn Reson Med 3: Clark VP, Keil K, Maisog JM, Courtney S, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (1996): Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human visual cortex during face-matching: A comparison with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage 4:1 15. Cottraux J, Gerard D, Cinotti L, Froment J-C, Deiber M-P, Bars DL, et al (1996): A controlled positron emission tomography study of obsessive and neutral auditory stimulation in obsessive compulsive disorder with checking rituals. Psychiatry Res 60: Drevets WC, Raichle ME (1998): Reciprocal suppression of regional cerebral blood flow during emotional versus higher cognitive processes: Implications for interactions between emotion and cognition. Cogn Emotion 12: Frost RO, Gross RC (1993): The hoarding of possessions. Behav Res Ther 31: Frost RO, Steketee G. (1998) Hoarding: Clinical aspects and treatment strategies. In Jenike MA, Baer L, Minichiello WE, editors. Obsessive Compulsive Disorders: Practical Management, 3rd ed. St Louis: Mosby-Year Book, Frost RO, Steketee G, Williams LF, Warren R (2000): Mood, personality disorder symptoms and disability in obsessive compulsive hoarders: A comparison with clinical and nonclinical controls. Behav Res Ther 38:

12 Obsessive-Compulsive Symptom Dimensions BIOL PSYCHIATRY 493 George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PI, Horwitz B, Herscovitch P, Post RM (1995): Brain activity during transient sadness and happiness in healthy women. Am J Psychiatry 152: Gibbs NA (1996): Nonclinical populations in research on obsessive-compulsive disorder: A critical review. Clin Psychol Review 16: Haidt J, McCauley C, Rozin P (1994): Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Pers Indiv Diff 16: Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (1997): International Affective Picture System (IAPS). New York: NIMH Center for Study of Emotion and Attention. Leckman JF, Grice DE, Boardman J, Zhang H, Vitale A, Bondi C, et al (1997): Symptoms of obsessive compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 154: Macdonald AM, de Silva P (1999): The assessment of obsessionality using the Padua Inventory: Its validity in a British non-clinical sample. Pers Indiv Diff 27: Marks DF (1989): Bibliography of research utilizing the vividness of visual imagery questionnaire. Percept Mot Skills 69: Mataix-Cols D, Baer L, Rauch SL, Jenike MA (2000): Relation of factor-analyzed dimensions of obsessive compulsive disorder to personality disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 102: Mataix-Cols D, Barrios M, Sánchez-Turet M, Vallejo J, Junqué C (1999c): Reduced design fluency in sub-clinical obsessive compulsive subjects. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 11: Mataix-Cols D, Junqué C, Sánchez-Turet M, Vallejo J, Verger K, Barrios M (1999b): Neuropsychological functioning in a sub-clinical obsessive sample. Biol Psychiatry 45: Mataix-Cols D, Marks IM, Greist JH, Kobak KA, Baer L (2002b): Obsessive compulsive symptom dimensions as predictors of compliance with and response to behaviour therapy: Results from a controlled trial. Psychother Psychosom 71: Mataix-Cols D, Rauch SL, Baer L, Shera D, Eisen J, Goodman WK, et al (2002a): Symptom stability in adult obsessive compulsive disorder: Data from a two-year naturalistic study. Am J Psychiatry 159: Mataix-Cols D, Rauch SL, Manzo PA, Jenike MA, Baer L (1999a): Use of factor-analyzed symptom dimensions to predict outcome with serotonin reuptake inhibitors and placebo in the treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 156: Mayberg HS, Liotti M, Brannan SK, McGinnis S, Mahurin RK, Jarabek PA, et al (1999): Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: Converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness. Am J Psychiatry 156: McGuire PK, Bench CJ, Frith CD, Marks IM, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ (1994): Functional anatomy of obsessive compulsive phenomena. Br J Psychiatry 164: Muris P, Merckelbach H, Clavan M (1997): Abnormal and normal compulsions. Behav Res Ther 35: Ogawa S, Lee TM, Kay AR, Tank DW (1990): Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent blood oxygenation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 87: Pardo JV, Pardo PJ, Raichle ME (1993): Neural correlates of self-induced dysphoria. Am J Psychiatry 150: Phillips ML, Marks IM, Senior C, Lythgoe D, O Dwyer A-M, Meehan O, et al (2000): A differential neural response in obsessive compulsive disorder patients with washing compared with checking symptoms to disgust. Psychol Med 30: Phillips ML, Williams L, Senior C, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Andrew C, et al (1999): A differential neural response to threatening and non-threatening negative facial expressions in paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenics. Psychiatry Res Neuroim 92: Phillips ML, Young AW, Scott SK, Calder AJ, Andrew C, Giampietro V, et al (1998): Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust. Proc R Soc Lond B 265: Phillips ML, Young AW, Senior C, Calder AJ, Perret D, Brammer M, et al (1997): A specific neural substrate for perception of facial expressions of disgust. Nature 389: Rachman S, de Silva P (1978): Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behav Res Ther 16: Rauch SL, Dougherty DD, Shin LM, Alpert NM, Manzo P, Leahy L, et al (1998): Neural correlates of factor-analyzed OCD symptom dimensions: A PET study. CNS Spectrums 3: Rauch SL, Jenike MA, Alpert NM, Baer L, Breiter HCR, Savage CR, et al (1994): Regional cerebral blood flow measured during symptom provocation in obsessive compulsive disorder using oxygen 15-labeled carbon dioxide and positron emission tomography. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51: Reiman EM, Lane RD, Ahern GL, Scwartz GE, Davidson RJ, Friston KJ, et al (1997): Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. Am J Psychiatry 154: Sanavio E (1988): Obsessions and compulsions: The Padua Inventory. Behav Res Ther 26: Saxena S, Maidment KM, Vapnik T, Golden G, Rishwain T, Rosen RM, et al (2002): Obsessive compulsive hoarding: Symptom severity and response to multimodal treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 63: Saxena S, Rauch SL (2000): Functional neuroimaging and the neuroanatomy of obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 23: Sher KJ, Frost RO, Otto R (1983): Cognitive deficits in compulsive checkers: An exploratory study. Behav Res Ther 21: Spielberger CD (1983): Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Sprengelmeyer R, Rausch M, Eysel UT, Przunte H (1998): Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions. Proc R Soc Lond B 265: Summerfeldt LJ, Richter MA, Antony MM, Swinson RP (1999): Symptom structure in obsessive compulsive disorder: A confirmatory factor-analytic study. Behav Res Ther 37: Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988): Co-Planar Stereotactic Atlas of the Human Brain. Stuttgart: Thieme. Teasdale JD, Howard RJ, Cox SG, Ha Y, Brammer MJ, Williams SC, et al (1999): Functional MRI study of the cognitive generation of affect. Am J Psychiatry 156: Van De Moortele PF, Le Clec HG, Dehaene S, Le Bihan D (1998): Improving auditory comprehension in fmri: Insertion of silent intervals in multi-slice EPI. Neuroimage 7:S554. Winsberg ME, Cassic KS, Koran LM (1999): Hoarding in obsessive compulsive disorder: Report of 20 cases. J Clin Psychiatry 60:

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Brain electrical activity methods description and explanation of method state effects trait effects Positron emission tomography

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. Authors Year Patients Male gender (%) Mean age (range) Adults/ Children

More information

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Supplemental informations Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Stéphanie Lefebvre, PhD^1,2, Guillaume Baille, MD^4, Renaud Jardri MD, PhD 1,2 Lucie Plomhause, PhD

More information

This is a repository copy of Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust.

This is a repository copy of Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust. This is a repository copy of Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/1320/ Article: Phillips,

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Cover Page. The handle  holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/32078 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Author: Pannekoek, Nienke Title: Using novel imaging approaches in affective disorders

More information

Functional MRI Study of the Cognitive Generation of Affect

Functional MRI Study of the Cognitive Generation of Affect Functional MRI Study of the Cognitive Generation of Affect John D. Teasdale, Ph.D., Robert J. Howard, M.D., Sally G. Cox, B.Sc., Yvonne Ha, M.B., B.S., Michael J. Brammer, Ph.D., Steven C.R. Williams,

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

The effect of negative emotional context on neural and behavioural responses to oesophageal stimulation

The effect of negative emotional context on neural and behavioural responses to oesophageal stimulation DOI: 10.1093/brain/awg065 Brain (2003), 126, 669±684 The effect of negative emotional context on neural and behavioural responses to oesophageal stimulation Mary L. Phillips, 1 Lloyd J. Gregory, 2,4 Sarah

More information

Biological Risk Factors

Biological Risk Factors Biological Risk Factors Ms Angelina Crea Provisional Psychologist Academic Child Psychiatry Unit Royal Children s Hospital Professor Alasdair Vance Head Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics

More information

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Table S1: Brain regions involved in the adapted classification learning task Brain Regions x y z Z Anterior Cingulate

More information

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Overview of the SIIPS1 development. The development of the SIIPS1 consisted of individual- and group-level analysis steps. 1) Individual-person

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/324/5927/646/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmpfc Valuation System Todd A. Hare,* Colin F. Camerer, Antonio

More information

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention Goldsmiths Research Online. The Authors. Originally published: Science vol.291 2 March 2001 1803-1806. http://www.sciencemag.org. 11 October 2000; accepted 17 January 2001 The Role of Working Memory in

More information

Passport control a bit carried away. appreciated the advice forgot to talk to the manager, next thing I know my fmri thankfully, when aroused things back to normal Inattentive impaired children and adolescents:

More information

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Online supplementary information for: Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Joel L. Voss, Brian D. Gonsalves, Kara D. Federmeier,

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Green SA, Hernandez L, Tottenham N, Krasileva K, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. The neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Published

More information

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group 1 Supplemental Data Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group Additional inclusion criteria for the major depressive disorder group were: age of onset of

More information

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Human Brain Mapping 6:373 377(1998) Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Randy L. Buckner 1,2,3 * 1 Departments of Psychology, Anatomy and Neurobiology, and Radiology, Washington University,

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Redlich R, Opel N, Grotegerd D, et al. Prediction of individual response to electroconvulsive therapy via machine learning on structural magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA

More information

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri Human Brain Mapping 5:293 297(1997) Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri Vincent P. Clark, 1 * Raja Parasuraman, 2 Katrina Keil, 1 Rachel Kulansky, 1 Sean Fannon, 2 Jose Ma.

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

Facial Emotion Processing in Paranoid and Non-Paranoid Schizophrenia

Facial Emotion Processing in Paranoid and Non-Paranoid Schizophrenia FACIAL EMOTION PROCESSING IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 1 Running head: FACIAL EMOTION PROCESSING IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Facial Emotion Processing in Paranoid and Non-Paranoid Schizophrenia Sophie Jacobsson Bachelor Degree

More information

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Supplementary Information Supplementary Figure 1. The mean parameter

More information

Supplementary Materials for

Supplementary Materials for Supplementary Materials for Folk Explanations of Behavior: A Specialized Use of a Domain-General Mechanism Robert P. Spunt & Ralph Adolphs California Institute of Technology Correspondence may be addressed

More information

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory

More information

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M. Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making Johanna M. Jarcho 1,2 Elliot T. Berkman 3 Matthew D. Lieberman 3 1 Department of Psychiatry

More information

Supplemental information online for

Supplemental information online for Supplemental information online for Sleep contributes to the strengthening of some memories over others, depending on hippocampal activity at learning. Géraldine Rauchs (1,2), Dorothée Feyers (1), Brigitte

More information

Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of

Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of Introduction Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of scientific inquiry for over 100 years. In particular, this topic has had significant interest in the past

More information

doi: /brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease

doi: /brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease doi:10.1093/brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; 1165 1179 1165 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease Marianne J. U. Novak, 1 Jason D.

More information

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia 1 Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia Christine I. Hooker, Taylor L. Benson, Anett Gyurak, Hong Yin,

More information

DIMENSIONAL AND SUBTYPE MODELS OF OCD

DIMENSIONAL AND SUBTYPE MODELS OF OCD Chapter 2 DIMENSIONAL AND SUBTYPE MODELS OF OCD Steven Taylor Although obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is recognized in DSM-IV as a unitary syndrome (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000),

More information

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 SPECIAL ISSUE WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TE US ABOUT AND DIS? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 By: Angelika Dimoka Fox School of Business Temple University 1801 Liacouras Walk Philadelphia, PA

More information

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Available online at www.sciencedirect.com R NeuroImage 18 (2003) 806 810 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Technical Note Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Andrea Mechelli,

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Supplemental Information Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Roselinde H. Kaiser, Ph.D., Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli, Ph.D., Daniel G. Dillon, Ph.D., Franziska Goer, B.S., Miranda

More information

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations What is Fear Conditioning? Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning Information and prediction: Animals use environmental signals to predict the occurrence of biologically significant events. Similar

More information

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Outline Neuroanatomy of emotion Critical conceptual, experimental design, and interpretation issues in neuroimaging research Fear and anxiety Neuroimaging research

More information

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri Magnetic Resonance Imaging 24 (2006) 181 185 Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri Birgit Abler a, 4, Alard Roebroeck b, Rainer Goebel b, Anett

More information

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition Gloria Wong 1,2, Sanda Dolcos 1,3, Ekaterina Denkova 1, Rajendra A. Morey 4,5, Lihong Wang 4,5, Nicholas Coupland 1, Gregory

More information

An f MRI study of facial emotion processing in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome.

An f MRI study of facial emotion processing in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland e-publications@rcsi Psychiatry Articles Department of Psychiatry 1-1-2015 An f MRI study of facial emotion processing in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion

More information

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Neuron, Volume 70 Supplemental Information Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Luke J. Chang, Alec Smith, Martin Dufwenberg, and Alan G. Sanfey Supplemental Information

More information

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease Andrey V. Sokolov 1,3, Sergey V. Vorobyev 2, Aleksandr Yu. Efimtcev 1,3, Viacheslav S. Dekan 1,3, Gennadiy E. Trufanov

More information

2017, Joule Inc. or its licensors Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors.

2017, Joule Inc. or its licensors Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors. Results Validation: Reproducibility Figure S1. Reproducibility of the results of small-world parameters. Differences in topological properties of functional brain networks between bulimia nervosa (BN)

More information

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer Science & Technology (IJIRCST) ISSN: 2347-5552, Volume-2, Issue-6, November-2014 Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Carhart-Harris et al. 10.1073/pnas.1119598109 Fig. S1. Slices for arterial spin labeling (ASL) result. Lightbox display of slices showing regions where there were significant decreases

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA The Effect of Cognitive Functioning, Age, and Molecular Variables on Brain Structure Among Carriers of the Fragile X Premutation: Deformation Based Morphometry Study Naomi J. Goodrich-Hunsaker*, Ling M.

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

The functional anatomy of the McCollough contingent colour aftereffect

The functional anatomy of the McCollough contingent colour aftereffect Brain imaging NeuroReport 10, 195±199 (1999) WE report two functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) experiments which reveal a cortical network activated when perceiving coloured grids, and experiencing

More information

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018)

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) 1 / 22 Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) Jérôme Dockès, ussel Poldrack, Demian Wassermann, Fabian Suchanek, Bertrand

More information

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Supplemental Information Table of Contents 2 Behavioral Data 2 Table S1. Participant demographics

More information

Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion. Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) S.M. s ratings of emotional intensity of faces (fig 9.

Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion. Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) S.M. s ratings of emotional intensity of faces (fig 9. Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion Readings: Gazzaniga Chapter 9 Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) SM began experiencing seizures at age 20 CT, MRI revealed amygdala atrophy, result of genetic

More information

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain 1 Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain Emily Falk, Elliot Berkman, Traci Mann, Brittany Harrison, Matthew Lieberman This document contains: Example

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Gregg NM, Kim AE, Gurol ME, et al. Incidental cerebral microbleeds and cerebral blood flow in elderly individuals. JAMA Neurol. Published online July 13, 2015. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.1359.

More information

Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers

Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers Dan Goodwin, Tom Bleymaier, Shipra Bhal Advisor: Dr. Amit Etkin M.D./PhD, Stanford Psychiatry Department Abstract We present a supervised learning approach to

More information

VIII. 8. Visceral Perception and Emotion are Correlated to Brain Activity during Colonic Distention in Human

VIII. 8. Visceral Perception and Emotion are Correlated to Brain Activity during Colonic Distention in Human CYRIC Annual Report 2003 VIII. 8. Visceral Perception and Emotion are Correlated to Brain Activity during Colonic Distention in Human Hamaguchi T. *, Kano M. *, Rikimaru H. **, Kanazawa M. *, Itoh M. **,

More information

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion Kevin N. Ochsner, a, * Rebecca D. Ray, b Jeffrey C. Cooper, b Elaine R. Robertson, b Sita Chopra,

More information

Statistical parametric mapping

Statistical parametric mapping 350 PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY HOW TO UNDERSTAND IT Statistical parametric mapping Geraint Rees Wellcome Senior Clinical Fellow,Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Institute of Neurology, University College

More information

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex in dual-task performance Fabienne Collette a,b, Laurence 01ivier b,c, Martial Van der Linden a,d, Steven Laureys b, Guy Delfiore b, André Luxen

More information

Supplementary Digital Content

Supplementary Digital Content Supplementary Digital Content Contextual modulation of pain in masochists: involvement of the parietal operculum and insula Sandra Kamping a, Jamila Andoh a, Isabelle C. Bomba a, Martin Diers a,b, Eugen

More information

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner Cerebral Cortex 1 Sarah Heilbronner heilb028@umn.edu Want to meet? Coffee hour 10-11am Tuesday 11/27 Surdyk s Overview and organization of the cerebral cortex What is the cerebral cortex? Where is each

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of Placebo effects in fmri Supporting online material 1 Supporting online material Materials and Methods Study 1 Procedure and behavioral data We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was

More information

Neuroimaging. BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT. Human Brain Mapping

Neuroimaging. BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT. Human Brain Mapping 11/8/2013 Neuroimaging N i i BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT 2 Human Brain Mapping H Human m n brain br in m mapping ppin can nb

More information

Biomarkers Workshop In Clinical Trials Imaging for Schizophrenia Trials

Biomarkers Workshop In Clinical Trials Imaging for Schizophrenia Trials Biomarkers Workshop In Clinical Trials Imaging for Schizophrenia Trials Research focused on the following areas Brain pathology in schizophrenia and its modification Effect of drug treatment on brain structure

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 2012, 2:11

Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 2012, 2:11 Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders This Provisional PDF corresponds to the article as it appeared upon acceptance. Fully formatted PDF and full text (HTML) versions will be made available soon. Neural

More information

Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla

Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla Atsushi Miki*, Grant T. Liu*, Sarah A. Englander, Jonathan Raz, Theo G. M. van Erp,

More information

The Nervous System. Neuron 01/12/2011. The Synapse: The Processor

The Nervous System. Neuron 01/12/2011. The Synapse: The Processor The Nervous System Neuron Nucleus Cell body Dendrites they are part of the cell body of a neuron that collect chemical and electrical signals from other neurons at synapses and convert them into electrical

More information

positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes

positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 9212-9217, August 1996 Neurobiology Memory for object features versus memory for object location: A positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval

More information

Final Report 2017 Authors: Affiliations: Title of Project: Background:

Final Report 2017 Authors: Affiliations: Title of Project: Background: Final Report 2017 Authors: Dr Gershon Spitz, Ms Abbie Taing, Professor Jennie Ponsford, Dr Matthew Mundy, Affiliations: Epworth Research Foundation and Monash University Title of Project: The return of

More information

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Figure I. Changes in connectivity with the right anterior insular cortex. (A) wake > mild sedation, showing a reduction in connectivity between the anterior

More information

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 341 356 Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Joseph B. Sala a,, Pia Rämä a,c,d, Susan M.

More information

Face encoding and recognition in the human brain

Face encoding and recognition in the human brain Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 922-927, January 1996 Neurobiology Face encoding and recognition in the human brain (positron emission tomography/cerebral blood flow) JAMES V. HAXBY*t, LESLIE G.

More information

DIFFERENT BOLD RESPONSES TO EMOTIONAL FACES AND EMOTIONAL FACES AUGMENTED BY CONTEXTUAL INFORMATION. Kyung Hwa Lee

DIFFERENT BOLD RESPONSES TO EMOTIONAL FACES AND EMOTIONAL FACES AUGMENTED BY CONTEXTUAL INFORMATION. Kyung Hwa Lee DIFFERENT BOLD RESPONSES TO EMOTIONAL FACES AND EMOTIONAL FACES AUGMENTED BY CONTEXTUAL INFORMATION by Kyung Hwa Lee B.A., Chungnam National University, 1997 M.A., Chungnam National University, 1999 Submitted

More information

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety brain research 1481 (2012) 13 36 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship

More information

Methods of Visualizing the Living Human Brain

Methods of Visualizing the Living Human Brain Methods of Visualizing the Living Human Brain! Contrast X-rays! Computerized Tomography (CT)! Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI)! Positron Emission Tomography (PET)! Functional MRI! Magnetoencephalography

More information

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Outline Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Michael A. Yassa, B.A. Division of Psychiatric Neuro-imaging Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Why fmri? fmri - How it works Research

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 women and 16 men. One female had to be excluded from brain data analyses because of strong movement

More information

Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala. California Institute of Technology

Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala. California Institute of Technology Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala Daniel P. Kennedy 1, Jan Gläscher 1, J. Michael Tyszka 2 & Ralph Adolphs 1,2 1 Division of Humanities and Social Sciences 2 Division of Biology California

More information

Study No: Title : Rationale: Phase: Study Period: Study Design: Centres: Indication: Treatment: Objectives: Statistical Methods: Sample Size

Study No: Title : Rationale: Phase: Study Period: Study Design: Centres: Indication: Treatment: Objectives: Statistical Methods: Sample Size The study listed may include approved and non-approved uses, formulations or treatment regimens. The results reported in any single study may not reflect the overall results obtained on studies of a product.

More information

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy Methods Participants We examined a group of twelve male pathological gamblers (ten strictly right handed, all smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy males,

More information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information The Journal of Neuroscience, 2000, Vol. 20 RC108 1of5 Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information Charan Ranganath, 1 Marcia K. Johnson, 2 and Mark

More information

Outline. Biological Psychology: Research Methods. Dr. Katherine Mickley Steinmetz

Outline. Biological Psychology: Research Methods. Dr. Katherine Mickley Steinmetz Biological Psychology: Research Methods Dr. Katherine Mickley Steinmetz Outline Neuroscience Methods Histology Electrophysiological Recordings Lesion Neuroimaging Neuroanatomy Histology: Brain structure

More information

fmri: What Does It Measure?

fmri: What Does It Measure? fmri: What Does It Measure? Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/02/2018: Lecture 02-1 Note: This Powerpoint presentation may contain macros that I wrote to help me create

More information

Hierarchically Organized Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition: The Functional Neuroanatomy of Empathy

Hierarchically Organized Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition: The Functional Neuroanatomy of Empathy Hierarchically Organized Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition: The Functional Neuroanatomy of Empathy Jaime A. Pineda, A. Roxanne Moore, Hanie Elfenbeinand, and Roy Cox Motivation Review the complex

More information

Experimental Design I

Experimental Design I Experimental Design I Topics What questions can we ask (intelligently) in fmri Basic assumptions in isolating cognitive processes and comparing conditions General design strategies A few really cool experiments

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Devenney E, Bartley L, Hoon C, et al. Progression in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a longitudinal study. JAMA Neurol. Published online October 26, 2015. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.2061.

More information

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate Supplementary Results Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate behavioral experiment was conducted (n = 16) to verify (a) that retrieval-induced forgetting is observed

More information

4 Brain activation. during cognitive planning in twins discordant or concordant for obsessive-compulsive symptoms

4 Brain activation. during cognitive planning in twins discordant or concordant for obsessive-compulsive symptoms 4 Brain activation during cognitive planning in twins discordant or concordant for obsessive-compulsive symptoms This chapter is published as: A. den Braber, D. van t Ent, D.C. Cath, J. Wagner, D.I. Boomsma,

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Huijser, C. (2011). Neuroimaging studies in pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder

Citation for published version (APA): Huijser, C. (2011). Neuroimaging studies in pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Neuroimaging studies in pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder Huijser, C. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Huijser, C. (2011). Neuroimaging

More information

Giuseppina Rota Graduiertenkolleg Sprachliche Repräsentation und ihre Interpretation Klausurtagung, July 2003

Giuseppina Rota Graduiertenkolleg Sprachliche Repräsentation und ihre Interpretation Klausurtagung, July 2003 Physiological Self-Regulation of Anterior Cingulate Cortex Brain Activity Using fmri: implications for Ambiguity Processing in Sentence Interpretation. Giuseppina Rota Graduiertenkolleg Sprachliche Repräsentation

More information

Background Paper: Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Kristen Thomas. University of Pittsburgh

Background Paper: Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Kristen Thomas. University of Pittsburgh Background Paper: Obsessive Compulsive Disorder Kristen Thomas University of Pittsburgh December 2011 2 According to the DSM-IV, obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) will be diagnosed when the child displays

More information

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Outline Neuroanatomy of emotion Fear and anxiety Brain imaging research on anxiety Brain functional activation fmri Brain functional connectivity fmri Brain structural

More information

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience School of Psychological Sciences Faculty of Medical & Human Sciences Module Information *Please note that this is a sample guide to modules. The exact

More information

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning John Gabrieli Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Institute for Medical Engineering & Sciences McGovern Institute for Brain Sciences MIT Levels of Analysis

More information