Neural Components of Social Evaluation. William A. Cunningham 1, Marcia K. Johnson 1, J. Chris Gatenby 2, John C. Gore 12, & Mahzarin R.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Neural Components of Social Evaluation. William A. Cunningham 1, Marcia K. Johnson 1, J. Chris Gatenby 2, John C. Gore 12, & Mahzarin R."

Transcription

1 Social Evaluative Processing 1 Running Head: SOCIAL EVALUATIVE PROCESSING Neural Components of Social Evaluation William A. Cunningham 1, Marcia K. Johnson 1, J. Chris Gatenby 2, John C. Gore 12, & Mahzarin R. Banaji 3 1 Yale University, Department of Psychology 2 Yale Medical School, Department of Radiology 3 Harvard University, Department of Psychology William A. Cunningham Department of Psychology Yale University P.O. Box New Haven, CT william.cunningham@yale.edu

2 Social Evaluative Processing 2 Abstract Evaluative responses judgments along a good-bad dimension involve two seemingly dissociated processes: one that is relatively automatic and another that is more consciously controlled. In two studies, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) to investigate the different brain systems that are associated with controlled relative to automatic evaluative processing and their relationship to each other. Participants made either evaluative (good-bad) or nonevaluative (past-present) judgments about famous names (e.g., Adolph Hitler, O.J. Simpson, George Washington, Bill Cosby). Evaluative processing resulted in greater medial and ventrolateral prefrontal activity than nonevaluative processing. Furthermore, ventrolateral prefrontal activity was greater for evaluations where participants were ambivalent. Regarding the automatic component, greater amygdala activity was observed for names rated as bad than those rated as good whether the task directly involved an evaluative judgment (good/bad) or not (past/present). Together, these findings provide evidence for (a) the idea of automatic evaluation, and the role of the amygdala in producing it, (b) the involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex in explicit evaluative judgments, and (c) the role of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in processing evaluative ambivalence. Together, these findings help clarify the neural correlates of automatic and controlled evaluative processing. Evidence for the interaction between automatic and controlled evaluative processing is also discussed.

3 Social Evaluative Processing 3 Neural Components of Social Evaluation Arguably, among the most important cognitive processes are those involved in the constant evaluation of our physical and social environment, including inanimate objects, persons, and events. This seemingly ordinary act of assigning valence good and bad is crucial for survival, guiding behavior toward or away from an immediate significant object, or through the anticipation of future rewards and punishments in the development of goals. It is not surprising, then, that brain systems that underlie these processes have received considerable attention (see LeDoux, 1996). Yet, despite this interest, little attention has been devoted to decomposing neural activity into subcomponents of automatic and controlled evaluative judgment. Although the social cognitive conceptualization of attitudes has increasingly emphasized the dissociation of automatic and controlled evaluative processing, only recently have brain mechanisms involved in producing an evaluation begun to be investigated along the automatic-controlled dimension (see Cacioppo, Berntson, & Crites, 1996; LeDoux, 1996). In particular, although some brain regions have been associated with automatic evaluation, little attention has been devoted to the brain regions that may be involved in controlled evaluation, when people consciously reflect about whether a target is good or bad, friend or foe, to be approached or avoided. In this research, by manipulating the nature of judgments (evaluative or nonevaluative) performed on stimuli that clearly differ in valence, we examine the brain processes involved in controlled evaluation and contrast these with those involved in automatic aspects of evaluation. The Social Cognition of Evaluation The study of affect and emotion has always been an integral part of the study of social behavior. As early as 1935, Gordon Allport wrote about attitude as the most distinctive and important concept in American social psychology. Later, in their classic work on meaning in everyday language, Osgood, Suci, and Tannenbaum (1953) showed the importance of the evaluative dimension. Specifically, they demonstrated that evaluation explained most of the variance in ratings of word meaning, concluding that an evaluative factor is first in magnitude and in order of appearance (pg 44).

4 Social Evaluative Processing 4 More recently, indirect methods to tap evaluation have led to the discovery that evaluations can occur automatically, even in the absence of conscious intention or awareness. Building on latency-based semantic priming procedures, Fazio, Sanbonatsu, Powell, and Kardes (1986) found that binary good-bad responses to stimuli with strong positive and negative connotations (e.g., vomit, rainbow) were facilitated when preceded by brief presentations of evaluatively congruent stimuli. Participants were able to more quickly indicate that a target word (such as beautiful ) had a good meaning when preceded by a prime word with good meaning (such as triumph ) compared with a prime with bad meaning. Similarly, brief presentations of evaluatively negative words speeded up responses to subsequent negative words. This effect occurs even when the prime and target are not semantically associated, strongly suggesting an automatic evaluation of information. Extending these findings, Bargh Chaiken, Govender, and Pratto (1992) demonstrated the same evaluative facilitation effect for a task that required no explicit evaluation. Instead of having participants make evaluative (good-bad) judgments about target stimuli, Bargh et al. (1992) asked participants to pronounce evaluatively laden words that were preceded by evaluatively congruent or incongruent primes. Even when only pronouncing words, Bargh et al. (1992) found that the evaluative congruency of prime and target facilitated participants responses. Lastly, the automaticity of evaluation has been demonstrated in studies in which similar effects have been produced using subliminal primes (Draine & Greenwald, 1998) that more securely escape conscious reflection. It is of interest that another type of evaluation, evaluations that are consciously constructed, may not be based on the same representations as those that are activated automatically from the environment. Current models of attitude and the cognition-emotion relation more generally (Devine, 1989; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Johnson & Multhaup, 1992) propose that there are at least two dissociated systems that constitute the processes of evaluation. Evaluative/affective responses and experience are a function of both automatic/perceptual and controlled/reflective cognitive processes. Perceptual emotional processes are more primitive; they are relatively automatic responses to stimuli in the immediate environment. Reflective emotion is internally generated, typically involving controlled processing, and can operate in the

5 Social Evaluative Processing 5 absence of immediate stimuli. Evidence for dissociations in evaluative processing comes from numerous studies investigating attitudes toward social groups and individual members of those groups. When attitudes toward social groups are measured with both self-report measures and latency based measures, self-reported attitudes toward certain social groups (e.g., African- Americans, the elderly, foreigners) are substantially more positive than are found on the latency based measures (Cunningham, Nezlek, & Banaji, 2002; Devine, 1989; Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995; Rudman, Greenwald, Mellot, & Schwartz, 1999). The Neuroscience of Evaluation Typically explored in the context of fear conditioning, numerous animal studies have shown that the amygdala a small structure found in both hemispheres of the medial temporal lobe is critical for learning to pair negative outcomes with conditioned stimuli and the expression of such associations (see LeDoux, 2000 for a review). Recent work using neuroimaging has replicated the finding of a link between amygdala function and fear conditioning in humans (LaBar, Gatenby, Gore, LeDoux, & Phelps, 1998; Phelps, et al., 2001). Interestingly, it appears that the amygdala is not only involved in associative learning of rewards and punishments, but also in more abstract, conceptual representations of fear and threat. For instance, Phelps et al. (2001) told participants that they might receive mild shocks when particular visual stimuli were presented. Although no actual shock was administered, Phelps et al. found amygdala activity to stimuli verbally suggested to be threatening. Even more generally, amygdala activation was found to presented words with threatening connotations compared with neutral words (Isenberg, et al., 1999). Besides fear conditioning, the amygdala also appears to be involved in more general evaluation and valence detection. Imaging data have shown that the amygdala responds more strongly to unpleasant odors (Zald & Pardo, 1997), tastes (Zald, Lee, Fluegal, & Pardo, 1998), emotional pictures (Morris, et al., 1996), and words (Tabert et al., 2001) than to corresponding pleasant or neutral items. Further, patients with bilateral amygdala damage have difficulty judging emotional expressions, especially negative emotions such as fear, anger, and disgust (Adolphs, et al., 1999; Calder, Kreane, Manes, Antoun, & Young, 2000).

6 Social Evaluative Processing 6 Current neuroimaging work suggests that the amygdala is involved in the more automatic processes of evaluation. For example, conscious awareness of a valenced stimulus does not appear to be necessary to produce amygdala activation. In conceptual replications of previous research on supraliminal emotional face processing, Morris, Ohman, and Dolan (1998) and Whalen, et al. (1998) demonstrated that subliminal presentations of emotional faces (angry and fearful, respectively) led to significant amygdala activation. Moreover, in a study of automatic race bias, Phelps et al. (2000) demonstrated that automatic behavioral measures of racial evaluation (i.e., the Implicit Association Test and startle eye blink) correlated with amygdala activation to Black faces relative to White faces, whereas a self-report measure of racial bias (i.e., the Modern Racism Scale) did not. In contrast, the amygdala does not appear to be involved in other, more controlled, aspects of evaluation. For instance, while patients with bilateral amygdala damage show deficits in automatic visceral fear conditioning, they are able to accurately report the valence of objects (Adolphs et al., 1999; Bechara et al., 1995). Thus non-amygdala processes allow patients with amygdala damage to learn about, judge, evaluate, and report valence. Although less understood, several prefrontal regions appear to play a role in these additional affective/evaluative processes (Damasio, 1994). At a general level, it appears that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is involved in higher-order cognition, involving executive control, deliberative, conscious, or reflective processing (e.g., Stuss & Benson, 1984). Structures in the PFC are necessary to learn complex associations, and may be necessary for an awareness of emotional feeling states (Lane, et al., 1998). Neuroimaging experiments in which participants are asked to reflectively generate (Teasdale, et al., 1999), monitor (Henson, Rugg, Shallice, Josephs, & Dolan, 1999), anticipate (Porro, et al., 2002), attribute (Paradiso, et al., 1999) or report on (Gusnard, Akbudak, Shilman, & Raichle, 2001) emotional states typically show heightened prefrontal, especially medial prefrontal, activation. Prefrontal contributions to affective evaluation likely allow for flexible evaluations that can integrate information regarding current memories for past events, anticipation of future events (including rewards and punishments), context, higher order motivations, and personal

7 Social Evaluative Processing 7 values with information provided by more automatic perceptual evaluative systems (e.g., Johnson & Multhaup, 1992). Moreover, these systems likely are necessary when evaluations of objects must be updated as new information is learned. Patients with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, although normal on many standard cognitive neuropsychological tests, have difficulty with tasks that require higher order affective processing. For instance, patients have difficulty organizing future goal directed behavior, have diminished capacity to respond to punishment, present unrealistically positive evaluations of self, and display inappropriate emotional reactions (Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1990). 1 An Integration Research investigating the neural systems involved in evaluation has either been restricted to explorations of the automatic components of evaluation, or has not explicitly tried to disentangle the automatic from the controlled processes of evaluation and attitude. However, one line of research supports the idea that the amygdala and other subcortical structures are likely to be involved in automatic, unconscious evaluative processing and some of the prefrontal contributions to evaluation may reflect more deliberate and controlled processing. Using a gambling task, Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, and Lee (1999) demonstrated that damage to the amygdala resulted in a lack of skin conductance when receiving a reward or punishment, or while selecting stimuli with positive or negative consequences. In contrast, damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex resulted in a lack of skin conductance only when selecting stimuli that have positive or negative consequences. This result suggests that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is involved in the anticipatory function of evaluation. That is, whereas the amygdala is involved in the reaction to the valence of perceptual stimuli, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex may be involved in reflective processes of evaluation, especially when immediate rewards and punishments are not present. For the most part, research examining evaluative processing has relied on stimulus driven designs, where the neural activity to distinct positive versus negative stimuli is compared. A key weakness of such designs is that they do not allow investigators to distinguish the extent to which automatic and controlled processes are involved in judgments of the stimuli. Without

8 Social Evaluative Processing 8 manipulating evaluative strategies it is not possible to discern whether the differences in activity reflect intentional evaluative processes under the participants conscious control or automatic evaluative processes that would have occurred regardless of task. In the one fmri study (Zyseet, Huber, Ferstl, & von Cramon, 2002) that compared the activity during controlled evaluative versus nonevaluative judgments, the stimuli used were not identical between conditions. When the same stimuli do not serve in both conditions and are therefore not equated, it is unclear whether the effects obtained reflect processes of controlled evaluation, or differences in other processes (such as automatic evaluation) that might be elicited by the differences inherent in stimuli. Using ERP, Crites and Cacioppo (1998) compared evaluative (good-bad) and nonevaluative (vegetable or not) judgments of identical stimuli and found difference signals for the tasks, suggesting that different neural components are involved in explicit evaluative compared with nonevaluative judgments when stimuli are held constant. The present studies attempt to systematically distinguish brain activity associated with deliberate, intentional social evaluative judgment from those associated with more automatic social evaluative judgment. In two studies, we used identical stimuli for evaluative and nonevaluative judgment conditions: famous names (e.g. Adolph Hilter, Bill Cosby) were selected for stimuli that could easily either be judged to be good or bad (evaluative judgment) or a historical or present day figure (past or present, nonevaluative judgment). By holding stimuli constant and by using tasks that require differential operations to be performed on them evaluative and non-evaluative brain activation associated with the automatic component of evaluation should be observed in both the evaluative and nonevaluative conditions. Thus, differences in these conditions primarily reflect differences in controlled evaluative processing. Moreover, using logic similar to Bargh et al. (1992), we can also examine automatic evaluative processing. Using a nonevaluative task allows us to see whether differences in activation are associated with good and bad stimuli even when participants are not deliberately engaged in evaluation. Brain activity reflecting the difference in processing good versus bad when consciously evaluating and especially when not is taken as activity associated with relatively automatic evaluation.

9 Social Evaluative Processing 9 In sum, the present research was designed to answer four primary questions. First, what brain systems are involved in conscious, deliberate acts of evaluation? Second, when examining the activity associated with the processing of valence, how are these areas modulated by task? Presumably, areas of activation that differentiate good and bad items regardless of task represent more automatic aspects of evaluation. Third, can the act of controlled evaluation influence areas of activity, such as the amygdala, that are thought to be involved in the automatic processing of evaluation? Finally, can neural markers of behavioral ambivalence in evaluation (i.e., high positive and negative evaluation of the same target) be detected? Methods Two experiments were conducted to examine the neural correlates of automatic and controlled social evaluation. Because these studies use new stimuli/task combinations and because the above questions have not been directly answered in prior research, we conducted two experiments that largely replicated each other. The main distinction was the presentation order of judgments. In the first experiment, participants responded to stimuli in blocks of similar judgment types (several evaluative or non-evaluative trials in succession). In the second experiment, judgments were randomly ordered using an event-related design. Because the two experiments are close replications of one another, with the second providing additional information not available in the first, the methods and results of both will be presented jointly. Both studies were approved by the Human Investigation Committee of the Yale Medical School. Study 1: Blocked Conditions To investigate the neural substrates underlying controlled evaluative relative to nonevaluative processing, fourteen participants were shown a series of famous names during fmri. In the evaluative condition, participants indicated whether names referred to good or bad people. In the non-evaluative condition, participants indicated whether names referred to historical figures (past) or current (present) figures. For example, Adolph Hilter would be classified by most if not all participants as bad in the evaluation task and as past in the nonevaluative task. Likewise, Bill Cosby would be classified by most people as good in the evaluative task and as present in the non-evaluative task.

10 Social Evaluative Processing 10 Participants Fourteen right-handed participants (mean age: 26 years; 8 females) consented to participate and were paid for their participation. Participants reported no abnormal neurological history and had normal or corrected to normal vision. fmri Parameters All imaging was conducted with a GE 1.5T (General Electric Medical Systems Signa, Milwaukee, Wisconsin) scanner. To cover the frontal lobe and the majority of the temporal and parietal lobes, fourteen parallel coronal slices (slice thickness: 6mm, skip = 2 mm) were prescribed perpendicular to the AC-PC line, with the fourth slice centered on the amygdala. Functional images were acquired from posterior to anterior using an interleaved single shot gradient echoplanar pulse sequence (TE = 60ms, TR = 1.5s, in-plane resolution = x mm, matrix size = 64 x 64, and FOV = 20 x 20 cm). Procedure During fmri, participants categorized famous names along one of two dimensions (good-bad or past-present) and indicated their categorization by making one of two button presses with their right hand. Using Psyscope for Macintosh (Cohen, MacWhinney, Flatt, & Provost, 1993), stimuli were forward projected using an LCD and overhead projector onto a clear screen at the base of the MRI bore. A prism mirror positioned over the participants eyes allowed them to view stimuli. All stimuli were presented in black letters against a white background. Each block began with either the cue good-bad, or past-present for 3 seconds, followed by a series of eight randomly presented names. Both first and last names were presented (see Appendix A for a complete list of stimuli). Each name appeared for 2 seconds, and was followed by a blank screen for 1 second. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible, and before the next stimulus appeared. The 60 names used in the study were repeated in both conditions so that participants made a good-bad and past-present judgment on each name. Each run consisted of six of each block type, with block type randomly ordered. To synchronize stimulus presentations with functional scanning, all trials were initiated by a trigger sent by the MRI scanner at the beginning of each scan.

11 Social Evaluative Processing 11 Preprocessing Data were corrected for slice acquisition time and motion using SPM99, then coregistered to in-plane anatomical images and transformed to conform to the SPM99 default T1 MNI brain interpolated to 3x6x3 mm. Functional data were smoothed using a 9mm FWHM (fullwidth-half-maximum) kernel, and a high pass filter removed effects of scanner draft. Study 2: Event Related (Randomized Conditions) To better understand the processes involved in evaluative and nonevaluative judgments, Study 2 conceptually replicated Study 1 using a rapid event-related design. This design allowed us to randomly present trial types, and importantly to generate time courses of the neural activity for good and bad stimuli under evaluative and nonevaluative task processing. Participants Fifteen participants were paid for their participation. Participants reported no abnormal neurological history and had normal or corrected to normal vision. All participants provided informed consent. One participant was excluded for head motion greater than 2mm, and two additional participants were excluded for knowing the identity of fewer than 70% of the famous names presented. Twelve participants (mean age: 22 years; 6 females) remained for analysis. fmri Parameters To get whole brain functional coverage, twenty-six parallel axial slices (slice thickness: 4.5mm, no skip) were prescribed parallel to the AC-PC line, with the eleventh slice centered on the AC-PC line. Functional images were acquired from inferior to superior using a single shot gradient echoplanar pulse sequence (TE = 30ms, TR = 2s, in-plane resolution = 3.75 x 3.75 mm, matrix size = 64 x 64, and FOV = 24 x 24 cm). Procedure As in Study 1, participants were presented with a series of famous names and made either good-bad or past-present judgments during fmri. Stimulus presentation and response collection were as in Study 1, except that stimuli were presented in white letters against a black background. For each trial, a cue indicated whether the trial required a good-bad or a pastpresent response for 500ms, immediately followed by a famous name for 1.5 seconds. Between

12 Social Evaluative Processing 12 trials, a fixation cross remained on the screen for two to six seconds to space out trials. To synchronize stimulus presentations with functional scanning, all trials were initiated by a trigger sent by the MRI scanner at the beginning of each scan. Each of four runs contained 24 of each trial type (evaluative task: bad names, evaluative task: good names, nonevaluative task: bad names, nonevaluative task: good names). Because good-bad evaluations of target names are inherently subjective, after scanning, participants completed a questionnaire in which all names were rated along four dimensions. Participants indicated on a 0 to 9 scale the extent to which the target names represented good or bad individuals. Separate ratings of good and bad were obtained so that a measure of ambivalence could be obtained. In addition, they rated their degree of knowledge about each target person. Finally, participants indicated whether the person was a historical (past) or current (present day) figure. These ratings were used for an ideographic sorting the fmri data. An analysis of participant responses indicated a high degree of evaluative (good-bad) inter-rater consistency (α =.98). With this high degree of consistency, analyses using normative instead of idiographic ratings yielded nearly identical results. Preprocessing Data were corrected for slice acquisition time and motion using SPM99, then transformed to conform to the SPM99 default EPI MNI brain interpolated to 4x4x4 mm. Functional data were smoothed using a 8mm FWHM (full-width-half-maximum) kernel, and a high pass filter removed effects of scanner drift. To additionally condition the data, a low pass filter removed frequencies greater than 17Hz, a cutoff that represents the frequency after which hemodynamic signals as a function of experimental effects are no longer expected. Results and Discussion Data Analysis Study 1 Data in both studies were analyzed using the general linear model as implemented by SPM99 (Friston et al., 1995). To examine brain activity associated with evaluative (good-bad) judgments relative to nonevaluative (past-present) judgments, we constructed regressors that corresponded to the expected BOLD signal for the two condition types. Contrast t-maps for the

13 Social Evaluative Processing 13 two conditions were generated for each participant by comparing the estimated hemodynamic signal for the evaluative and nonevaluative conditions. The contrasts tested (a) greater activity for evaluative than nonevaluative judgments, and (b) greater activity for nonevaluative than evaluative judgments. Random effects composite t-maps were generated by averaging these individual participant t-maps. Regions of activation were defined as those areas in which 10 contiguous voxels were significant at p <.005 (uncorrected). Data Analysis Study 2 As in Study 1, a series of regressors 2 were constructed to examine brain activity to each of the four trial types: evaluative-bad, evaluative-good, nonevaluative-bad, nonevaluative-good. Whether a particular name was assigned as a good or bad trial was determined by the rating participants made after scanning. 3 Trials in which a participant did not know the person were dropped in these analyses. On average, fewer than 10% of names were dropped for any given participant. Two sets of contrast t-maps were generated for each participant by comparing the estimated hemodynamic signal for (a) the evaluative versus nonevaluative conditions, and (b) the good versus bad names. The first set of contrasts tested for task related differences that is, activity associated with the deliberate act of evaluation (or the converse). The second set tested for areas of activation that responded to the valence of the stimuli, regardless of task goals. Presumably, this second set of contrasts reflect relatively more automatic evaluative processing. Random effects composite t-maps were generated by averaging these individual participant t- maps. Regions of activation were defined as those areas in which 10 contiguous voxels were significant at p <.005 (uncorrected), or a priori regions identified in Study 1 with 5 contiguous voxels at p <.01 (uncorrected). Using the idiosyncratic ratings of names provided by participants, we conducted a second series of analyses. For some names, participants simultaneously indicated both positivity and negativity on the independent post scan ratings of good and bad (on 0 to 9 scales), a psychological state known as attitudinal ambivalence (Priester & Petty, 1996). For the purposes of analysis, we defined ambivalent names individually for each participant as names that exceeded a cutoff of 15 when the good rating was multiplied by the bad rating. Compared with

14 Social Evaluative Processing 14 the agreement ratings found for the evaluation ratings, the ambivalence ratings were more variable (α =.67). The BOLD effect for each of the four effects of interest (good-bad judgments for ambivalent names, good-bad judgments for nonambivalent names, past-present judgments of ambivalent names, and past-present judgments of nonambivalent names) were modeled with three regressors for each participant: a canonical hemodynamic response, and then two derivatives to model the onset and duration of the BOLD response. Random effect t-maps were generated using individual participant t-maps as input. Time lines for neural activity were generated using the SPM ROI toolbox (Poldrack et al., Regions of activation were defined functionally from each of the contrast images. Time lines were estimated for each of the four conditions for each of the functionally defined regions of interest. Time lines for each condition were adjusted for the residual effects of the other conditions and were subjected to a high pass filter to account for scanner drift. Analyses of response latencies indicated that participants responded more quickly during good-bad than past-present trials (1856ms vs. 1921ms), F(1,11) = 6.04, p <.05, and more quickly for good names than for bad names (1849ms vs.1927ms), F(1,11) = 9.32, p <.05. Response latencies for names rated ambivalently in the good-bad condition were found to be significantly longer than those rated as nonambivalent (2056ms vs. 1843ms), a pattern that was not observed for trials in the past-present condition (1996ms vs. 1950ms), F(1,9) = 20.48, p < Controlled Evaluation A primary contrast of interest was the evaluative > nonevaluative contrast, the contrast that compares controlled evaluative processing to controlled nonevaluative processing. As noted in the introduction, previous research has used different stimuli in the evaluative and nonevaluative conditions, making it difficult to determine whether obtained results are the function of automatic or controlled evaluation. As can be seen in Tables 1 and 2, several areas of activation differentiated evaluative from non-evaluative judgments. We will limit our discussion

15 Social Evaluative Processing 15 to three theoretically relevant regions that replicated across both studies: the medial prefrontal cortex, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and the amygdala. Medial Prefrontal Cortex (mpfc): Whether examining the extent of activity, or the statistical significance of activity, the most dramatic difference between evaluative and nonevaluative processing in both studies occurred in the medial prefrontal cortex (Figure 1, p <.001, corrected). As can be seen in the time lines from Study 2 presented in Figure 1b, evaluative judgments resulted in increases in activity, whereas nonevaluative judgments resulted in significant decreases in activity in mpfc. Defining fixation trials as baseline mental activity, these activations can be understood as increases in processing relative to a default mental state, and deactivation as decreases in processing relative to a default mental state (see Gusnard et al., 2001). In a recent meta-analysis on the neuroimaging of emotion, Phan, Wager, Taylor, and Liberzon (2002) found that the medial prefrontal cortex was involved in many aspects of affective processing, regardless of the valence and sensory modality of the triggering stimulus. Interestingly though, the greatest percentage of studies finding medial prefrontal activation were those in which stimuli were imagined compared with those in which stimuli were presented (nearly 70% for reflectively generated emotion, compared with about 45% for perceptually generated emotion). Further evidence for a reflective rather than perceptual role of mpfc in affect/evaluation comes from work examining anticipatory anxiety (Simpson, Drevets, Snyder, Gusnard, & Raichle, 2001). Anxiety can be contrasted from fear in that fear often is exhibited in the presence of a threatening stimulus, whereas anxiety is exhibited in the anticipation of a threatening stimulus. That reflective processing activates this region more so than perceptual processing further implicates this region in controlled aspects of emotion and evaluation. It is unclear whether the activations observed in medial prefrontal cortex are specific to social evaluations, or whether they occur during evaluation regardless of domain. A review of imaging studies in which mpfc activation has been found indicates that these studies share a feature in addition to reflective aspects of emotion. Studies that require reflective social processing, broadly defined, also appear to be associated with mpfc activity. Most notably,

16 Social Evaluative Processing 16 tasks that require making attributions of beliefs, intentions, and goals of others (Castelli et al., 2000; Gallagher et al., 2000; Goel et al., 1995) or self (S. C. Johnson et al. 2002; Kelley et al., 2000) have been shown to activate the mpfc relative to control tasks. Ventrolateral PFC (vlpfc): In addition to areas in the medial prefrontal cortex, areas of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex showed more activity for evaluative than nonevaluative judgments. As can be seen in Figures 2a and 2b, activation for the good-bad trials was significantly greater than for the past-present trials in vlpfc, a difference that could reflect memory selection. In memory retrieval and judgment, relevant information needs to be separated from irrelevant information. Consistent with a proposed inhibitory function of the prefrontal cortex (e.g., Hasher & Zacks, 1988; Shimamura, 1995), Thompson-Shill, D'Esposito, Aguirre, and Farah (1997) suggested that the vlpfc is involved in inhibiting irrelevant information during memory tasks. In the context of the present evaluative and nonevaluative tasks, the evaluative task may require more selection, or at least more complex selection, than does the nonevaluative task. For example, information regarding both good and bad behaviors of former President Bill Clinton may be stored in memory, and this information needs to be selected and weighed to make a binary good-bad judgment. Although both the evaluative and nonevaluative tasks require semantic processing to identify the stimulus person and activate relevant information to make a judgment, the evidence for past or present may involve fewer conflicting cues than the evidence for good or bad. If it is true that the ventrolateral prefrontal activation observed for the evaluative trials is a function of resolving multiple memory representations in evaluation, or the online weighing of evaluative information, then we should expect the greatest ventrolateral PFC activation to occur for trials in which the most ambivalent or evaluatively opposing judgments are made. Here, these ambivalent judgments would be ones in response to target names that represent those individuals who elicit both positive and negative evaluation. Fortunately, from the ratings obtained from each participant on independent good and bad dimensions, we can compute the subset of stimulus individuals who scored high on both positive and negative evaluation, to be compared to those who rated at one or the other extreme on the two

17 Social Evaluative Processing 17 dimensions.. As an example, names like Bill Clinton and Yassar Arafat fell into the high ambivalent category, whereas Adolf Hilter (only negative) and Mahatma Gandhi (only positive) fell into the unambivalent category. To test this prediction, we compared the activity for high ambivalent names relative to unambivalent names in both the good-bad and past-present judgment conditions. As can be seen in Figure 2c, activity in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex was indeed greatest for ambivalent names in the good-bad evaluative task. No difference was observed for the pastpresent nonevaluative task. This finding reinforces the idea that controlled evaluations are formed online from available memory representations, and that to form an evaluation, a weighing process assesses this information for relevance. For evaluative judgments, when there is little evaluative conflict, less of this processing is needed to form a judgment. Automatic Evaluation Amygdala: In previous work, the amygdala has been shown to be more sensitive to negative than positive information (Iseberg et al., 1999; Morris et al., 1996), though activation has been observed to positive information relative to neutral information as well (Hamann & Mao, 2002). As can be seen in the timelines from Study 2 presented in Figure 3b, we demonstrate both findings. Collapsing across tasks, left amygdala activity was observed for both good and bad names, but greater left amygdala activity was observed for bad names than good names. Importantly, as can be seen in Figure 3c, this difference between good and bad names was observed in both the good-bad (p <.05) and the past-present (p <.01) tasks. In other words, amygdala activation appeared whether or not participants consciously attended to the evaluative dimension, lending support to the idea that the amygdala is involved in the automatic processing of evaluation. Interestingly, our data suggest that consciously focusing on the nonevaluative aspects of a person may reduce amygdala activation, but only after several judgments have been made. Although we found no effect of judgment type in Study 2, we found greater left amygdala activity for evaluative blocks of trials than nonevaluative blocks of trials in Study 1. This apparent discrepancy can be resolved by plotting the average time courses of the amygdala for

18 Social Evaluative Processing 18 both the evaluative and nonevaluative blocks of Study 1. As can be seen in Figure 3a, for the first half of each block type, evaluative and nonevaluative judgments showed identical activation. It was only in the second half of the block that evaluative and nonevaluative blocks differed. Specifically, whereas the nonevaluative blocks showed the standard amygdala habituation effect (Breiter et al., 1996; Phelps et al., 2001; Wright et al., 2001), no such habituation was found for the evaluative blocks. Attention to nonevaluative aspects of stimuli may result in habituation whereas attention to evaluative aspects may prolong (and perhaps even increase) affective responses. In short, the amygdala is involved in the automatic processing of valence, but this automatic processing may not be impervious to conscious control. Nonevaluative Processing Although not the main focus of this paper, it is relevant to note that several regions showed greater activation in response to the nonevaluative compared to the evaluative task. That is, non-evaluative judgments did not simply produce lower activation than evaluative judgments. Most notably, several areas of dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal cortex showed greater activity associated with nonevaluative than evaluative judgments, areas that are associated with memory processing. It should be noted that the differences in these regions reflect differences in activation. That is, both evaluative and nonevaluative judgments involve processing in these areas, but nonevaluative processing recruited these to a greater extent. General Discussion This is among the first work that disentangles the neural components of controlled/reflective social evaluative processing from those associated with automatic/perceptual processing. This distinction is most readily observed in the differential patterns of activity in the medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Explicit good-bad evaluations of social stimuli activated, whereas past-present judgments of the same stimuli deactivated the medial prefrontal cortex. These results demonstrate a brain region that may underlie the ERP differences between evaluative and nonevaluative judgments of identical stimuli reported by Crites & Cacioppo (1998). Further support for this suggestion comes from two previous studies that required participants to make explicit good-bad judgments (Gusnard et al., 2001; Zysset et

19 Social Evaluative Processing 19 al., 2002). Although stimuli in the Zysset et al. study were not identical in the evaluative and nonevaluative conditions, making it difficult to draw out distinctions between automatic and controlled components, they did report activation in the mpfc when answering evaluative questions (e.g., I like Berlin ). In addition, Gusnard et al. found activation in the mpfc when participants reflected on whether a picture created pleasant or unpleasant feelings. In contrast, we found greater amygdala activation to bad names than to good names whether or not participants were asked to evaluate those names. Conceptually supporting Bargh et al. s (1992) behavioral work, we found that the amygdala was sensitive to valence whether participants were attending to this dimension or not. This finding is consistent with previous work on the automaticity of amygdala processing (see Morris et al., 1998; Whalen et al., 1998). The pattern of activation observed in the amygdala, in combination with the pattern observed in the medial prefrontal cortex, echoes and substantiates at the neural level, the proposed dissociation between automatic and controlled evaluative processing. This dissociation speaks to the nature of evaluative processing. First, that amygdala activity was observed in the evaluative condition as well as the non-evaluative condition indicates that automatic evaluative processes likely play a role in controlled evaluative processes. Prefrontal activation does not replace amygdala activation in controlled evaluation; instead the two are present simultaneously. The association between medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala processing is further demonstrated when looking at within participant correlations between these areas. Contrary to the idea of a complete separation of processes, we found that medial prefrontal activity and left amygdala activity are substantially correlated, r =.45. These medial prefrontal processes may integrate various current affective and memory sources in the service of building conscious evaluations, and one of these sources likely is the relatively more automatic evaluation provided by the amygdala. A relationship between amygdala and medial prefrontal activation may partially explain observed correlations between automatic and controlled attitude measures, and may provide additional support for claims that automatic processes may bias consciously formed evaluations (see Cunningham et al., 2002).

20 Social Evaluative Processing 20 In addition to the medial prefrontal cortex, another area of PFC, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, appears to play a role in controlled evaluative processing. The pattern of activity in this region provides support for suggestions that conscious evaluations reflect a constructed online process rather than a controlled activation of a stored memory representation (see Wilson & Hodges, 1992). When evaluatively opposing information was brought to bear on a judgment (the more equally and strongly positive and negative the target was), greater activity was observed in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Interpreting our findings in terms of the suggestions provided by Thompson-Shill et al. (1997) regarding vlpfc, we suggest that complex evaluations are built online, activating various relevant information, whether positive or negative. Processes in the vlpfc contribute to determining the extent to which the information is pertinent to making good-bad judgments. Interestingly, it appears that what one is reflecting about may influence amygdala activity, although this influence may not be instantaneous but may require time to build up. We found no difference in amygdala processing for evaluative and nonevaluative judgments when participants made alternating judgments (Study 2), or in the first of several judgments of a particular type (the first half of the Study 1 blocks). Yet, after making many evaluative or nonevaluative judgments in a series (after about 15 seconds), amygdala activation in Study 1 differed between judgments such that greater activity was observed for evaluative judgments than for nonevaluative judgments. This suggests that conscious attention to nonevaluative features of a stimulus may influence automatic evaluation, but only by maintaining such a set over time. From Figure 3a, it appears that attending to valence sustains the evaluative function of the amygdala during evaluative blocks and that repeated efforts to focus away from that dimension in nonevaluative blocks habituates amygdala activation. That a controlled attentional focus toward nonevaluation leads to reductions in amygdala processing, but only after repeated nonevaluative judgments, has implications for the control of unwanted automatic evaluations, such as those targeting social groups known to elicit automatic prejudice (see Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne, Thorn, & Castelli, 1997). A single attempt to focus attention away from evaluation to another nonevaluative feature may result in increased

21 Social Evaluative Processing 21 prejudicial behavior. The amygdala time lines from Study 2 show a negative automatic evaluation regardless of focus, and without bringing to bear conscious values and attitudes, this automatic evaluation may guide behavior. Yet, a strategy for focusing on nonevaluative features may become effective after some time. Continuously focusing attention away from evaluation may reduce the sensitivity of the amygdala, and may reduce automatic attitude effects. In sum, we found that separate brain systems were involved in the controlled/reflective aspects of evaluation as compared with those involved in the automatic/perceptual aspects, supporting theoretical distinctions that have been proposed largely on behavioral observations (e.g., Devine, 1989; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Johnson & Multhaup, 1992). We provide both additional support for the suggestion that the amygdala is involved in automatic evaluation, and provide evidence for distinct prefrontal contributions to controlled evaluation. Moreover, these data suggest that there is a connection between automatic and controlled evaluative processes (see also Cunningham et al., 2002). The fact that (a) amygdala processing was observed during both evaluative and nonevaluative judgments, and that (b) there was a correlation between medial prefrontal and amygdala activation, suggests that the output of a consciously generated evaluation may have elements of both automatic and controlled processes (as does consciously experienced memory, e.g., see Jacoby et al., 1992). This interrelation is additionally demonstrated in that controlled processing was shown to affect amygdala activation. After multiple trials, the amygdala was shown to habituate during nonevaluative trials whereas no such evaluation was observed for the evaluative trials. Together, these findings illustrate the ways that social psychology, cognitive psychology, and neuroscience may mutually help clarify the concept of evaluation that is central to the brain, mind and behavioral sciences.

22 Social Evaluative Processing 22 References Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Hamann, S., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., Phelps, E. A., Anderson, A., Lee, G. P., & Damasio, A. R. (1999). Recognition of facial emotion in nine individuals with bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 37, Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A handbook of social psychology (pp ). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 62, Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A. R., & Lee, G. P. (1999). Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A. R.. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269, Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L., Strauss, M. M., Hyman, S. E., & Rosen, B. R. (1996). Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron, 17, Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Crites, S. L. (1996). Social neuroscience: Principles of psychophysiological arousal and response. E. T., Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds). Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. (pp ). Guilford Press: New York, NY. Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manes, F., Antoun, N., Young, A. W. (2000). Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nature Neuroscience, 3,

23 Social Evaluative Processing 23 Castelli, F., Happe, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage, 12, Cohen, J. D., WacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J. (1993). PsyScope: a new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 25, Crites, S. L., Caciopo, J. T. (1996). Electrocortical differentiation of evaluative and nonevaluative categorizations. Psychological Science, 7, Cunningham, W. A., Nezlek, J. B., & Banaji, M. R. (2002). Implicit and explicit ethnocentrism: Revisiting the ideologies of prejudice. Manuscript in preparation: Yale University. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. Putnam: New York. Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H. (1990). Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behavioural Brain Research, 41, 81-94, Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, Draine, S. C. & Greenwald, A. G. (1998). Replicable unconscious semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,

24 Social Evaluative Processing 24 Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 50, Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Worsley, K. J., Poline, J. P., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1995). Statistic parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Human Brain Mapping, 2, Gallagher, H. L., Happe, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fmri study of 'theory of mind' in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, Goel, V., Grafman, J., Sadato, N., & Hallett, M. (1995). Modeling other minds. NeuroReport, 6, Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, Hamann, S., & Mao, H. (2002). Positive and negative emotional verbal stimuli elicit activity in the left amygdala. NeuroReport, 13, Hasher, L. & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower, Gordon (Ed). The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 22. (pp ). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.

25 Social Evaluative Processing 25 Henson, R. N., Rugg, M. D., Shallice, T., Josephs, O., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, , Isenberg, N., Silbersweig, D., Engelien, A., Emmerich, S., Malavade, K., Beattie, B., Leon, A. C., & Stern, E. (1999). Linguistic threat activates the human amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, , Jacoby, L. L., Yonelinas, A. P., & Jennings, J. M. (1997). The relation between conscious and unconscious (automatic) influences: A declaration of independence. In J. D. Cohen & J. W. Schooler (Eds), Scientific approaches to consciousness. Carnegie Mellon Symposia on cognition. (pp ). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Johnson, M. K. & Multhaup, K. S. (1992). Emotion and MEM. In S. Christianson (Ed). The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory. (pp ). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ. Johnson, S. C., Baxter, L. C., Wilder, L. S., Pipe, J. G., Heiserman, J. E., & Prigatano, G. P. (2002). Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain, 125, Kelley, W. M, Macrae, C. N, Wyland, C. L, Caglar, S, Inati, S, Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self?: An event-related fmri study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Axelrod, B., Yun, L. S., Holmes, A., & Schwartz, G. E. (1998). Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness. Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10,

26 Social Evaluative Processing 26 LaBar, K. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., LeDoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (1998). Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinction: a mixed-trial fmri study. Neuron, 20, LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: the mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon & Schuster: New York. LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., Thorn, T. M. J., & Castelli, L. (1997). On the activation of social stereotypes: The moderating role of processing objectives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1996). A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, 383, Morris, J. S., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and Unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393, Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1954). The Measurement of Meaning. University of Illinois Press: Urbana, IL. Paradiso, S., Johnson, D. L., Andreasen, N. C., O'Leary, D. S., Watkins, G. L., Ponto, L. L., & Hichwa, R. D. (1999). Cerebral blood flow changes associated with attribution of emotional valence to pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral visual stimuli in a PET study of normal subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156,

27 Social Evaluative Processing 27 Phelps E. A., O'Connor K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, Phelps E. A., O'Connor, K. J., Gatenby J. C., Gore, J. C., Grillon C., & Davis, M. (2001). Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 4, Phan K. L., Wager, T., & Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fmri. Neuroimage, 16, Porro, C. A., Baraldi, P., Pagnoni, G., Serafini, M., Facchin, P., Maieron, M., & Nichelli, P. (2002). Does anticipation of pain affect cortical nociceptive systems? Journal of Neuroscience, 22, Priester, J. R. & Petty, R. E. (1996). The gradual threshold model of ambivalence: Relating the positive and negative bases of attitudes to subjective ambivalence. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 71, Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., Mellott, D. S., Schwartz, L. K. (1999). Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: Flexibility and generality of the Implicit Association Test. Social Cognition, 17, Shimamura, A. P. (1995). Memory and the prefrontal cortex. In J. Grafman, Jordan & K. J. Holyoak (Eds). Structure and functions of the human prefrontal cortex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol (pp ). New York, NY; New York Academy of Sciences; New York Academy of Sciences.

28 Social Evaluative Processing 28 Simpson, J. R., Drevets, W. C., Snyder, A. Z., Gusnard, D. A., & Raichle M. E. (2001). Emotion-induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex: II. During anticipatory anxiety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98, Stuss, D. T. & Benson, D. F. (1984). Neuropsychological studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin, 95, Tabert, M. H., Borod, J. C., Tang, C. Y., Lange, G., Wei, T. C., Johnson, R., Nusbaum, A. O., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (2001). Differential amygdala activation during emotional decision and recognition memory tasks using unpleasant words: an fmri study. Neuropsychologia, 39, Teasdale, J. D., Howard, R. J., Cox, S. G., Ha, Y., Brammer, M. J., Williams, S. C., & Checkley, S. A. (1999). Functional MRI study of the cognitive generation of affect. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, Thompson-Schill, S. L., D'Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., & Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94, Tranel D. (1994). "Acquired sociopathy": the development of sociopathic behavior following focal brain damage. Progress in Experimental Personality & Psychopathology Research, Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 18,

29 Social Evaluative Processing 29 Wilson, T. D. & Hodges, S. D. (1992). Attitudes as temporary constructions. L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds). The construction of social judgments. (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wright, C. I., Fischer, H., Whalen, P. J., McInerney, S. C., Shin, L. M., & Rauch, S. L. (2001). Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatly presented emotional stimuli. NeuroReport, 12, Zald, D. H., Lee, J. T., Fluegel, K. W., & Pardo, J. V. (1998). Aversive gustatory stimulation activates limbic circuits in humans. Brain, 121, Zald, D. H. & Pardo, J. V. (1997). Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94, , Zysset, S., Huber, O., Ferstl, E., von Cramon, D. Y. (2002) The anterior frontomedian cortex and evaluative judgment: an fmri study. Neuroimage, 15,

30 Social Evaluative Processing 30 1 It is important to note that, although the prefrontal cortex is likely involved in the controlled aspects of evaluation, it is likely that areas of PFC also respond to evaluation at an automatic level. For instance, Kawasaki, Adolphs et al. (2001) demonstrated using single neuron recordings that valence is detected in ventral areas of the human prefrontal cortex within 160ms of stimulus presentation, a delay that suggests automatic evaluative processing. 2 Two regressors were used for each trial type: a canonical hemodynamic response, and then a derivatives to model the onset of the BOLD response 3 Idiosyncratic name ratings of were not collected for the first two participants. Names were assigned to condition for these participants using button presses recorded for good-bad trials recorded during fmri scanning. In addition, these participants were not included in the analyses of ambivalence.

31 Social Evaluative Processing 31 Table 1: Significant areas of BOLD signal (Study 1) Good-Bad > Past-Present Size Area BA R/L T x y z 904 Medial Prefrontal 9 R Medial Prefrontal 9 L Medial Prefrontal 10 L Ventrolateral Prefrontal 47 L Ventrolateral Prefrontal 47 L Ventrolateral Prefrontal 47 R Amygdala L Amygdala R Superior Temporal Sulcus L Superior Temporal Sulcus R Superior Temporal 22 L Superior Temporal 22 R Insula R Insula R Inferior Temporal 21 R Hypothalmus L Hippocampus R Past-Present > Good-Bad Size Area BA R/L T x y z 31 Dorsolateral Frontal 46 L Dorsolateral Frontal 6 L Dorsolateral Frontal 8 R Dorsolateral Frontal 9 R Dorsolateral Frontal 6 R Note: Tables show local maxima > 8.00mm apart per cluster. BA= Brodmann s Area; R/F = Right or Left hemisphere; T =Maximal t-statistic for the statistical difference; x, y, & z: the 3D coordinates of the activation

32 Social Evaluative Processing 32 Table 2: Significant areas of BOLD signal (Study 2) Contrast 1: Evaluative > Nonevaluative Size Area BA R/L T x y z 396 Medial Prefrontal 9 L Medial Prefrontal 9 L Ventrolateral Prefrontal 47 R Ventrolateral Prefrontal 47 R Ventrolateral Prefrontal 10 R Ventrolateral Prefrontal 47 L Ventrolateral Prefrontal 47 L Inferior Temporal 20 L Brain Stem Cerebellum R Contrast 2: Nonevaluative > Evaluative Size Area BA R/L T x y z 355 Dorsolateral Prefrontal 46 L Dorsolateral Prefrontal 6 R Dorsolateral Prefrontal 9 R Dorsolateral Prefrontal 8 R Posterior Cingulate 31 R Superior Parietal 7 R Middle Temporal 21 L Superior Temporal 22 L Inferior Temporal 37 R Inferior Temporal 37 L Inferior Temporal 37 L Cuneus 19 R Occipital 17 L Contrast 3: Bad Names > Good Names Size Area BA R/L T x y z 7 Amygdala L Ventrolateral Prefrontal 45 R Ventrolateral Prefrontal 47 L Anterior Cingulate 32 L Insula R Substantia Nigra R Posterior Cingulate 31 L Cerebellum R Note: Tables show local maxima > 8.00mm apart per cluster. BA= Brodmann s Area; R/F = Right or Left hemisphere; T =Maximal t-statistic for the statistical difference; x, y, & z: the 3D coordinates of the activation

33 Social Evaluative Processing 33 Figure 1: Activation in dorsal medial prefrontal cortex for good-bad > past-present a: Blocked Design (Study 1) Dorsal Medial Prefrontal Cortex b: Event Related Design (Study 2) Dorsal Medial Prefrontal Cortex % Signal Change Good-Bad vs. Past-Present Seconds Good-Bad Past-Present

34 Social Evaluative Processing 34 Figure 2: Activation in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex for good-bad > past-present a: Blocked Design (Study 1) Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex b: Event Related Design (Study 2) Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex % Signal Change Good-Bad vs. Past-Present Seconds Good-Bad Past-Present c: High vs. Low Ambivalent Names (Study 2) % Signal Change Good-Bad Trials Seconds % Signal Change Past-Present Trials Seconds NonAmb Ambivalent NonAmb Ambivalent

35 Social Evaluative Processing 35 Figure 3: Activations the amygdala to judgment task and stimulus valance a: Blocked Design (Study 1) Left Amygdala % Signal Change Good-Bad vs. Past-Present Seconds b: Event Related Design (Study 2) Good-Bad Past-Present Left Amygdala % Signal Change Bad vs. Good Seconds Bad Good c: Valence effects in both the past-present and good-bad tasks (Study 2) % Signal Change Good-Bad Judgments Seconds % Signal Change Past-Present Judgments Seconds Bad Good Bad Good

Implicit and Explicit Evaluation: fmri Correlates of Valence, Emotional Intensity, and Control in the Processing of Attitudes

Implicit and Explicit Evaluation: fmri Correlates of Valence, Emotional Intensity, and Control in the Processing of Attitudes 16.10 Implicit and Explicit Evaluation: fmri Correlates of Valence, Emotional Intensity, and Control in the Processing of Attitudes William A. Cunningham, Carol L. Raye, and Marcia K. Johnson Abstract

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

UNCORRECTED PROOF. Research Article. Psychology, Harvard University

UNCORRECTED PROOF. Research Article. Psychology, Harvard University PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Separable Neural Components in the Processing of Black and White Faces William A. Cunningham, 1 Marcia K. Johnson, 1 Carol L. Raye, 1 J. Chris Gatenby, 2 John C.

More information

Malleability in Implicit Stereotypes and Attitudes. Siri J. Carpenter, American Psychological Association Mahzarin R. Banaji, Yale University

Malleability in Implicit Stereotypes and Attitudes. Siri J. Carpenter, American Psychological Association Mahzarin R. Banaji, Yale University Malleability in Implicit Stereotypes and Attitudes Siri J. Carpenter, American Psychological Association Mahzarin R. Banaji, Yale University Poster presented at the 2nd annual meeting of the Society for

More information

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of Placebo effects in fmri Supporting online material 1 Supporting online material Materials and Methods Study 1 Procedure and behavioral data We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

The Neural Basis of Implicit Attitudes Damian Stanley, 1 Elizabeth Phelps, 1 and Mahzarin Banaji 2

The Neural Basis of Implicit Attitudes Damian Stanley, 1 Elizabeth Phelps, 1 and Mahzarin Banaji 2 CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE The Neural Basis of Implicit Attitudes Damian Stanley, 1 Elizabeth Phelps, 1 and Mahzarin Banaji 2 1 New York University and 2 Harvard University ABSTRACT Evidence

More information

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 SPECIAL ISSUE WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TE US ABOUT AND DIS? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 By: Angelika Dimoka Fox School of Business Temple University 1801 Liacouras Walk Philadelphia, PA

More information

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and 77 5 Discussion The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and implicit learning systems. Chapter 2 described the effects of performing a working memory task

More information

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion Kevin N. Ochsner, a, * Rebecca D. Ray, b Jeffrey C. Cooper, b Elaine R. Robertson, b Sita Chopra,

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

Neuroimaging vs. other methods

Neuroimaging vs. other methods BASIC LOGIC OF NEUROIMAGING fmri (functional magnetic resonance imaging) Bottom line on how it works: Adapts MRI to register the magnetic properties of oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin, allowing

More information

Exploring Reflections and Conversations of Breaking Unconscious Racial Bias. Sydney Spears Ph.D., LSCSW

Exploring Reflections and Conversations of Breaking Unconscious Racial Bias. Sydney Spears Ph.D., LSCSW Exploring Reflections and Conversations of Breaking Unconscious Racial Bias Sydney Spears Ph.D., LSCSW Race the Power of an Illusion: The Difference Between Us https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=b7_yhur3g9g

More information

Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion. Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) S.M. s ratings of emotional intensity of faces (fig 9.

Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion. Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) S.M. s ratings of emotional intensity of faces (fig 9. Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion Readings: Gazzaniga Chapter 9 Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) SM began experiencing seizures at age 20 CT, MRI revealed amygdala atrophy, result of genetic

More information

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Available online at www.sciencedirect.com R NeuroImage 18 (2003) 806 810 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Technical Note Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Andrea Mechelli,

More information

Supplementary Study A: Do the exemplars that represent a category influence IAT effects?

Supplementary Study A: Do the exemplars that represent a category influence IAT effects? Supplement A to Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method Variables and Construct Validity. Personality and Social Psychology

More information

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Communication Science

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Communication Science Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Communication Science JASON C. CORONEL The Ohio State University, USA EMILY B. FALK University of Pennsylvania, USA Theories of message and interpersonal processing

More information

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective MILITARY PSYCHOLOGY, 21:(Suppl. 1)S108 S112, 2009 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0899-5605 print / 1532-7876 online DOI: 10.1080/08995600802554748 Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future:

More information

Affective Priming: Valence and Arousal

Affective Priming: Valence and Arousal Affective Priming: Valence and Arousal Dr. Mark Ashton Smith Introduction Affect: Its hypothesized 3-D structure: valence, arousal and control It is widely held that emotion can be defined as a coincidence

More information

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory

More information

Importance of Deficits

Importance of Deficits Importance of Deficits In complex systems the parts are often so integrated that they cannot be detected in normal operation Need to break the system to discover the components not just physical components

More information

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview Investigations in Resting State Connectivity Scott FMRI Laboratory Overview Introduction Functional connectivity explorations Dynamic change (motor fatigue) Neurological change (Asperger s Disorder, depression)

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer Science & Technology (IJIRCST) ISSN: 2347-5552, Volume-2, Issue-6, November-2014 Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/324/5927/646/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmpfc Valuation System Todd A. Hare,* Colin F. Camerer, Antonio

More information

Empathy is a complex psychological response in which observation, memory,

Empathy is a complex psychological response in which observation, memory, CHAPTER 14 The Social Neuroscience of Empathy Jean Decety University of Washington Sara D. Hodges University of Oregon Empathy is a complex psychological response in which observation, memory, knowledge,

More information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information The Journal of Neuroscience, 2000, Vol. 20 RC108 1of5 Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information Charan Ranganath, 1 Marcia K. Johnson, 2 and Mark

More information

Controlling Racial Prejudice

Controlling Racial Prejudice PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Controlling Racial Prejudice Social-Cognitive Goals Affect Amygdala and Stereotype Activation Mary E. Wheeler and Susan T. Fiske Princeton University ABSTRACT The

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Implicit Attitude. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia. Mahzarin R. Banaji. Harvard University

Implicit Attitude. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia. Mahzarin R. Banaji. Harvard University 1 Implicit Attitude Brian A. Nosek University of Virginia Mahzarin R. Banaji Harvard University Contact Information Brian Nosek 102 Gilmer Hall; Box 400400 Department of Psychology University of Virginia

More information

VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification

VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification CYRIC Annual Report 2009 VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification Abe N. 1, Fujii T. 1, Ueno A. 1, Shigemune Y. 1, Suzuki M. 2, Tashiro

More information

Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers

Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers Dan Goodwin, Tom Bleymaier, Shipra Bhal Advisor: Dr. Amit Etkin M.D./PhD, Stanford Psychiatry Department Abstract We present a supervised learning approach to

More information

HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR

HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR Applicants Principal Investigator Student ID 4039921 Collaborators Name(s) Institution(s) Title of project: Neural basis of verbal and non-verbal false

More information

Made you look! Consciously perceived, irrelevant instructional cues can hijack the. attentional network

Made you look! Consciously perceived, irrelevant instructional cues can hijack the. attentional network Made you look! Consciously perceived, irrelevant instructional cues can hijack the attentional network Katherine Sledge Moore, Clare B. Porter, and Daniel H. Weissman Department of Psychology, University

More information

Hierarchically Organized Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition: The Functional Neuroanatomy of Empathy

Hierarchically Organized Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition: The Functional Neuroanatomy of Empathy Hierarchically Organized Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition: The Functional Neuroanatomy of Empathy Jaime A. Pineda, A. Roxanne Moore, Hanie Elfenbeinand, and Roy Cox Motivation Review the complex

More information

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest Experimental Design Kate Watkins Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford With thanks to: Heidi Johansen-Berg Joe Devlin Outline Choices for experimental paradigm Subtraction / hierarchical

More information

Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD. Lei Zhang. Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center

Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD. Lei Zhang. Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center Running head: NEUROBEHAVIORAL MECHANISMS OF FEAR GENERALIZATION 1 Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD Lei Zhang Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center Running head:

More information

The Impact of Emotion on Perception Bias or Enhanced Processing?

The Impact of Emotion on Perception Bias or Enhanced Processing? PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Impact of Emotion on Perception Bias or Enhanced Processing? René Zeelenberg, 1 Eric-Jan Wagenmakers, 2 and Mark Rotteveel 2 1 Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam,

More information

Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective

Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.3 Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective William A. Cunningham 1 and Philip David Zelazo 2 1 Department of Psychology, Ohio

More information

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Brain electrical activity methods description and explanation of method state effects trait effects Positron emission tomography

More information

Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal

Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal BBS-D-15-00893_ Mather_ Montagrin & Sander Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal Alison Montagrin 1,2,3 * & David Sander 1,2 1 Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, 2 Department of Psychology

More information

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition Gloria Wong 1,2, Sanda Dolcos 1,3, Ekaterina Denkova 1, Rajendra A. Morey 4,5, Lihong Wang 4,5, Nicholas Coupland 1, Gregory

More information

Action and Emotion Understanding

Action and Emotion Understanding Action and Emotion Understanding How do we grasp what other people are doing and feeling? Why does it seem so intuitive? Why do you have a visceral reaction when you see a wound or someone in a physically

More information

Chapter 2 Knowledge Production in Cognitive Neuroscience: Tests of Association, Necessity, and Sufficiency

Chapter 2 Knowledge Production in Cognitive Neuroscience: Tests of Association, Necessity, and Sufficiency Chapter 2 Knowledge Production in Cognitive Neuroscience: Tests of Association, Necessity, and Sufficiency While all domains in neuroscience might be relevant for NeuroIS research to some degree, the field

More information

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Neuroscience of Consciousness II 1 C83MAB: Mind and Brain Neuroscience of Consciousness II Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Consciousness State of consciousness - Being awake/alert/attentive/responsive Contents

More information

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate Supplementary Results Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate behavioral experiment was conducted (n = 16) to verify (a) that retrieval-induced forgetting is observed

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Affective Flexibility: Evaluative Processing Goals Shape Amygdala Activity

Affective Flexibility: Evaluative Processing Goals Shape Amygdala Activity University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications: Political Science Political Science, Department of 2008 Affective Flexibility: Evaluative Processing

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Lesions in Humans Eliminate Implicit Gender Stereotyping

Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Lesions in Humans Eliminate Implicit Gender Stereotyping The Journal of Neuroscience, 2001, Vol. 21 RC150 1of6 Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Lesions in Humans Eliminate Implicit Gender Stereotyping Elizabeth Milne and Jordan Grafman Cognitive Neuroscience Section,

More information

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni VISUAL AGNOSIA -damage to the extrastriate visual regions (occipital, parietal and temporal lobes) disrupts recognition of complex visual stimuli

More information

THE PREFRONTAL CORTEX. Connections. Dorsolateral FrontalCortex (DFPC) Inputs

THE PREFRONTAL CORTEX. Connections. Dorsolateral FrontalCortex (DFPC) Inputs THE PREFRONTAL CORTEX Connections Dorsolateral FrontalCortex (DFPC) Inputs The DPFC receives inputs predominantly from somatosensory, visual and auditory cortical association areas in the parietal, occipital

More information

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions NeuroImage 10, 642 657 (1999) Article ID nimg.1999.0500, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential

More information

Functional dissociation of amygdala-modulated arousal and cognitive appraisal, in Turner syndrome

Functional dissociation of amygdala-modulated arousal and cognitive appraisal, in Turner syndrome doi:10.1093/brain/awh562 Brain (2005), 128, 2084 2096 Functional dissociation of amygdala-modulated arousal and cognitive appraisal, in Turner syndrome D. H. Skuse, 1 J. S. Morris 1 and R. J. Dolan 2 1

More information

Detecting and Indexing Emotion

Detecting and Indexing Emotion Detecting and Indexing Emotion Brian Lickel University of Southern California Department of Psychology When Measuring Emotion It is Useful to have a Theory of Emotion Dimensional Positive versus Negative

More information

Neural Basis of Decision Making. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Neural Basis of Decision Making. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Neural Basis of Decision Making Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Phineas Gage: Sept. 13, 1848 Working on the rail road Rod impaled his head. 3.5 x 1.25 13 pounds What happened

More information

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Supplemental Information Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Roselinde H. Kaiser, Ph.D., Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli, Ph.D., Daniel G. Dillon, Ph.D., Franziska Goer, B.S., Miranda

More information

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Outline Neuroanatomy of emotion Critical conceptual, experimental design, and interpretation issues in neuroimaging research Fear and anxiety Neuroimaging research

More information

Experimental Design I

Experimental Design I Experimental Design I Topics What questions can we ask (intelligently) in fmri Basic assumptions in isolating cognitive processes and comparing conditions General design strategies A few really cool experiments

More information

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining 8 the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining items was included. Participants were asked to decide whether a different item was included in the array,

More information

Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear

Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear Elizabeth A. Phelps 1, Kevin J. O Connor 2, J. Christopher Gatenby 3, John C. Gore 3, Christian Grillon 4 and Michael Davis 5 1 Department

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Event-Related Activation in the Human Amygdala Associates with Later Memory for Individual Emotional Experience

Event-Related Activation in the Human Amygdala Associates with Later Memory for Individual Emotional Experience The Journal of Neuroscience, 2000, Vol. 20 RC99 1of5 Event-Related Activation in the Human Amygdala Associates with Later Memory for Individual Emotional Experience Turhan Canli, 1 Zuo Zhao, 1 James Brewer,

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations What is Fear Conditioning? Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning Information and prediction: Animals use environmental signals to predict the occurrence of biologically significant events. Similar

More information

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M. Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making Johanna M. Jarcho 1,2 Elliot T. Berkman 3 Matthew D. Lieberman 3 1 Department of Psychiatry

More information

Categorical Cognition: A Psychological Model of Categories and Identification in Decision Making: Extended Abstract

Categorical Cognition: A Psychological Model of Categories and Identification in Decision Making: Extended Abstract Categorical Cognition: A Psychological Model of Categories and Identification in Decision Making: Extended Abstract Roland G. Fryer, Jr. and Matthew O. Jackson* This paper introduces a notion of categorization

More information

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 341 356 Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Joseph B. Sala a,, Pia Rämä a,c,d, Susan M.

More information

fmri Evidence for Modality-Specific Processing of Conceptual Knowledge on Six Modalities

fmri Evidence for Modality-Specific Processing of Conceptual Knowledge on Six Modalities fmri Evidence for Modality-Specific Processing of Conceptual Knowledge on Six Modalities Simmons, W.K. 1, Pecher, D. 2, Hamann, S.B. 1, Zeelenberg, R. 3, & Barsalou, L.W. 1 1 Emory University, 2 Erasmus

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 women and 16 men. One female had to be excluded from brain data analyses because of strong movement

More information

Cognitive Level of Analysis - Cognition and Emotions

Cognitive Level of Analysis - Cognition and Emotions Camden E. Gaultney Year 12 IB Higher Level Psychology Ms. Cruz The 16th of April, 2012 Cognitive Level of Analysis - Cognition and Emotions The Learning Outcome #30: To what extent do cognitive and biological

More information

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Human Brain Mapping 6:373 377(1998) Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Randy L. Buckner 1,2,3 * 1 Departments of Psychology, Anatomy and Neurobiology, and Radiology, Washington University,

More information

The Importance of the Mind for Understanding How Emotions Are

The Importance of the Mind for Understanding How Emotions Are 11.3 The Importance of the Mind for Understanding How Emotions Are Embodied Naomi I. Eisenberger For centuries, philosophers and psychologists alike have struggled with the question of how emotions seem

More information

The Methods of Cognitive Neuroscience. Sensory Systems and Perception: Auditory, Mechanical, and Chemical Senses 93

The Methods of Cognitive Neuroscience. Sensory Systems and Perception: Auditory, Mechanical, and Chemical Senses 93 Contents in Brief CHAPTER 1 Cognitive Neuroscience: Definitions, Themes, and Approaches 1 CHAPTER 2 The Methods of Cognitive Neuroscience CHAPTER 3 Sensory Systems and Perception: Vision 55 CHAPTER 4 CHAPTER

More information

Research Assistant at Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging Harvard Medical School Advisor: Moshe Bar, Ph.D.

Research Assistant at Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging Harvard Medical School Advisor: Moshe Bar, Ph.D. Updated August 2013 Maital Neta, Ph.D. Department of Neurology Washington University School of Medicine 4525 Scott Ave., Room 2220 St. Louis, MO 63110 Phone: (314) 362-8898 Fax: (314) 362-2186 Email: maital@npg.wustl.edu

More information

Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes

Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes Dr. Susan Boland Lock Haven University of PA Presented at AAUW-PA 88 th Annual Meeting Explicit vs. Implicit Evidence of implicit processes Are we all

More information

Academic year Lecture 16 Emotions LECTURE 16 EMOTIONS

Academic year Lecture 16 Emotions LECTURE 16 EMOTIONS Course Behavioral Economics Academic year 2013-2014 Lecture 16 Emotions Alessandro Innocenti LECTURE 16 EMOTIONS Aim: To explore the role of emotions in economic decisions. Outline: How emotions affect

More information

Formation of Stereotypes

Formation of Stereotypes Special Issue: Social Cognition and Self. Citation: Gilmour, J. (2015). Formation of Stereotypes. Behavioural Sciences Undergraduate Journal, 2(1), 67-73. Author: Jillian Gilmour, Psychology, Mount Royal

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia 1 Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia Christine I. Hooker, Taylor L. Benson, Anett Gyurak, Hong Yin,

More information

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related?

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related? Shackman, Afterword Q2 1 Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related? Alexander J. Shackman Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, and Maryland Neuroimaging

More information

Myers Psychology for AP*

Myers Psychology for AP* Myers Psychology for AP* David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2010 *AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board, which

More information

Supplementary Materials for

Supplementary Materials for Supplementary Materials for Folk Explanations of Behavior: A Specialized Use of a Domain-General Mechanism Robert P. Spunt & Ralph Adolphs California Institute of Technology Correspondence may be addressed

More information

COGS Advanced Topics in Cognitive Science Emotion, Control of Cognition, & Dynamic Decision Making

COGS Advanced Topics in Cognitive Science Emotion, Control of Cognition, & Dynamic Decision Making COGS-6962 Advanced Topics in Cognitive Science Emotion, Control of Cognition, & Dynamic Decision Making Week 02 Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298(5596), 1191-1194. Simon,

More information

Evaluative versus Conceptual Judgments in Automatic Stereotyping and Prejudice

Evaluative versus Conceptual Judgments in Automatic Stereotyping and Prejudice Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 37, 244 252 (2001) doi:10.1006/jesp.2000.1456, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Evaluative versus Conceptual Judgments in Automatic Stereotyping

More information

Facial Emotion Processing in Paranoid and Non-Paranoid Schizophrenia

Facial Emotion Processing in Paranoid and Non-Paranoid Schizophrenia FACIAL EMOTION PROCESSING IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 1 Running head: FACIAL EMOTION PROCESSING IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Facial Emotion Processing in Paranoid and Non-Paranoid Schizophrenia Sophie Jacobsson Bachelor Degree

More information

Experimental design. Experimental design. Experimental design. Guido van Wingen Department of Psychiatry Academic Medical Center

Experimental design. Experimental design. Experimental design. Guido van Wingen Department of Psychiatry Academic Medical Center Experimental design Guido van Wingen Department of Psychiatry Academic Medical Center guidovanwingen@gmail.com Experimental design Experimental design Image time-series Spatial filter Design matrix Statistical

More information

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (1), 159-165 Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions SOWON HAHN AND SCOTT D. GRONLUND University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma Using a visual search

More information

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Paul E. Hemeren (paul.hemeren@his.se) University of Skövde, School of Humanities and Informatics

More information

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex in dual-task performance Fabienne Collette a,b, Laurence 01ivier b,c, Martial Van der Linden a,d, Steven Laureys b, Guy Delfiore b, André Luxen

More information

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Alexa Morcom Edinburgh SPM course 2017 Thanks to Rik Henson, Thomas Wolbers, Jody Culham, and the SPM authors for slides Overview Categorical designs Factorial designs

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/324/5929/900/dc1 Supporting Online Material for A Key Role for Similarity in Vicarious Reward Dean Mobbs,* Rongjun Yu, Marcel Meyer, Luca Passamonti, Ben Seymour, Andrew

More information

Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala. California Institute of Technology

Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala. California Institute of Technology Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala Daniel P. Kennedy 1, Jan Gläscher 1, J. Michael Tyszka 2 & Ralph Adolphs 1,2 1 Division of Humanities and Social Sciences 2 Division of Biology California

More information

Exploring the Neural Substrates of Self-Ownership and Memory. Dave Turk University of Aberdeen

Exploring the Neural Substrates of Self-Ownership and Memory. Dave Turk University of Aberdeen Exploring the Neural Substrates of Self-Ownership and Memory Dave Turk University of Aberdeen Evaluative and Non-Evaluative Self Temporary ownership also leads to selfmemory biases. What are the neural

More information

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times CHAPTER 4 Lars Kuchinke, Melissa L.-H. Võ, Lars Michael, and Arthur M. Jacobs 5 Abstract In a lexical decision experiment emotional valence and emotional

More information

Distinguishing informational from value-related encoding of rewarding and punishing outcomes in the human brain

Distinguishing informational from value-related encoding of rewarding and punishing outcomes in the human brain European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 39, pp. 2014 2026, 2014 doi:10.1111/ejn.12625 Distinguishing informational from value-related encoding of rewarding and punishing outcomes in the human brain Ryan

More information

The effects of skin tone on race-related amygdala activity: an fmri investigation

The effects of skin tone on race-related amygdala activity: an fmri investigation doi:10.1093/scan/nsl043 SCAN (2007) 2, 39 44 The effects of skin tone on race-related amygdala activity: an fmri investigation Jaclyn Ronquillo, 1 Thomas F. Denson, 1 Brian Lickel, 1 Zhong-Lin Lu, 1 Anirvan

More information

Orbitofrontal cortex provides cross-modal valuation of self-generated stimuli

Orbitofrontal cortex provides cross-modal valuation of self-generated stimuli Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Advance Access published May 7, 2010 doi:10.1093/scan/nsq038 SCAN (2010) 1 of 8 Orbitofrontal cortex provides cross-modal valuation of self-generated stimuli

More information