Relative Contributions of Expressive Behavior and Contextual Information to the Judgment of the Emotional State of Another

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Relative Contributions of Expressive Behavior and Contextual Information to the Judgment of the Emotional State of Another"

Transcription

1 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1990, Vol. 59, No. 5, /90/$00.75 Relative Contributions of Expressive Behavior and Contextual Information to the Judgment of the Emotional State of Another Makoto Nakamura, Ross Buck, and David A. Kenny University of Connecticut This study used a technique for assessing the relative impact of facial-gestural expressions, as opposed to contextual information regarding the elicitor and situation, on the judgment of emotion. In Study 1, 28 undergraduates rated videotapes of spontaneous facial-gestural expressions and separately rated the emotionally loaded color slides that elicited those expressions. The source clarities of the expressions and slides were matched using correlation and distance measures, and 18 expressions and 9 slides were selected. In Study 2, 72 undergraduate receivers were shown systematic pairings of these expressions and slides and rated the emotional state of the expresser, who was supposedly watching that slide under public or private situational conditions. Expressions were found to be more important sources for all emotion judgments. For female receivers slides were relatively more important in the public than in the private situation. One of the fundamental areas for debate in the study of emotional communication is the question of the relative roles of expressive behavior on the part of the individual versus knowledge of the context in which the expression occurs in determining the judgment of an individual's emotional state. It was once widely believed that expressive behavior is an unreliable indicator of emotional state and that context (i.e, the eliciting stimulus and situation) contributes more than expression in emotion judgments (e.g, Bruner & Tagiuri, 1954; Hunt, 1941; Tagiuri, 1968). The detailed critique of this belief by Ekman, Friesen, and Ellsworth (1972) did much to make the study of emotional expression scientifically respectable. The critique by Ekman et al. (1972) was based on conceptual distinctions that forced a reevaluation of previous experimental designs and past data. For example, these authors pointed out that the relative importance of expressive behavior and contextual information (i.e, about the elicitor and situation) would be determined in great part by the clarity or ambiguity of the information from each of these sources. They defined source clarity as the "amount or type of information available to observers when they are exposed to a single source" (p. 138). Ekman et al. noted the need systematically to measure and match source clarity in order to study meaningfully the relative importance of expressive behavior and contextual information in the judgment of emotion. This research was supported in part by National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01 MH to Ross Buck and R01 MH to David A. Kenny and by the University of Connecticut Research Foundation. Thanks go to Deborah A. Kashy, who provided critical comments on an earlier version of this article. Makoto Nakamura is now at the General Psychology Laboratory, Faculty of Human Sciences, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ross Buck, Department of Communication Sciences, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut Context In general, contexts are defined by senders, elicitors, situations, and receivers. Senders are stimulus persons whose fixed characteristics (e.g, age, gender, and physical features) provide information. Elicitors are stimuli that evoke emotion in senders. Situations are environments or fields that surround senders, elicitors, and sometimes receivers, and are characterized by the physical setting and social setting. Finally, receivers are people who are exposed to and receive information from senders, elicitors, or situations, or any combination thereof. One might think that receivers are usually observers and do not play important parts in characterizing contexts. Although this is generally true in experimental situations, and is in the present study, in everyday life the relationships between senders and receivers are interactive (Kenny & La Voie, 1984). In interactive settings, receivers are not mere observers but positive interactants who influence the contextual information) Why is a particular emotion expected to be associated with a certain pattern of contextual information? To answer this question, the effects of display and decoding rules must be examined. Display rules were originally defined by Ekman and Friesen (1969; Ekman, 1973, 1977) to refer to the conventions, norms, and habits that develop regarding the management of emotional responses. For example, Ekman, Friesen, and Malmstrom (1970; Ekman & Friesen, 1975) found that whereas American students expressed in an interview their negative emotion toward a tension-evoking movie, Japanese students tried to make positive expressions. This result is consistent with Contextual information is discussed in a general and interactive sense. Although it is important to take interactions between senders and receivers into account, as Kenny and La Vole (1984) suggested, in the following discussion it is assumed that the relationship between senders and receivers is static and noninteractive. Thus, in this study receivers do not affect contextual information, although they may be affected by that information. 1032

2 CONTRIBUTIONS OF BEHAVIOR AND INFORMATION 1033 the fact that in Japan there exists a taboo (i.e, a display rule) against expressing negative emotions in public situations. Shimoda, Argyle, and Ricci-Bitti (1978) also reported that in comparison to English and Italian students, Japanese students tended not to express clearly emotions and attitudes. Decoding rules are the opposite side of the same coin: They are defined as cultural rules or expectations concerning the attention to and interpretation of emotional information (Buck, 1984). It is possible to interpret the ability to detect deception as a kind of ability to manage decoding rules. Blanck and Rosenthal (1982) and DePaulo and Rosenthal (1979) reviewed and studied the developmental changes in decoding discrepant messages from multiple sources. It was found that the ability to decode discrepant messages increases with age; that is, as they get older, children begin to attend to less controllable channels, which are more likely to "leak" or reveal the "true feelings," when they are confronted with inconsistent messages. Both display and decoding rules are learned through developmental processes and play important roles in interpersonal and social communication (Saarni, 1984). They come into play in the handling of the complex pattern of contextual information when making judgments of emotion. Expressive Behavior In contrast to context, expressive behavior includes information from different expressive channels of the sender (e.g, facial expressions, body movement, paralanguage, and verbal behavior). Emotional expressions are linked to judgments of emotion both directly (i.e, through spontaneous communication; Buck, 1984) and through decoding rules. Many studies have investigated the relative importance of different channels of emotional expression. The relative importance of channels is often defined in terms of the relative controllability or strength of inhibition of emotion expression in the channels. There seems to be a leakage hierarchy of nonverbal channels within a sender (cf. Blanck & Rosenthal, 1982; DePaulo & Rosenthal, 1979; Ekman & Friesen, 1974): Discrepancies between different channels are the most leaky (uncontrollable); micromomentary facial/bodily cues are next; and tone of voice and body movement follow. Facial expressions constitute the most controllable nonverbal channel. In regard to the need to control the expressive display, it is well-known that situations vary along a public-private (or formal-informal) dimension (cf. Knapp, 1984) and that the privacy of a situation may be critical to the expression or inhibition of true emotion. Indeed, one would expect that a common decoding rule is that expressive behavior is usually more inhibited in a public situation than in a private situation. In judging the relative contributions of expressive behavior and context (elicitors and situations) to emotion judgments, the relative importance of the information sources should be related to the clarity of the expression relative to the clarity of the elicitor, as well as the controllability of the emotional expression relative to the need to control such expression (e.g, in a public vs. private situation). Previous Research Previous studies in this area have produced conflicting results (Frijda, 1958; Goldberg, 1951; Goodenough & Tinker, 1931; Knudsen & Muzekari, 1983; Munn, 1940; Spignesi & Shor, 1981; Vinacke, 1949; Wallbott, 1988; Watson, 1972). Only one study (Watson, 1972) found that facial cues contribute to emotion judgments more than contextual cues, and no study found context primacy. The controversy about the relative importance of face and context was renewed by Russell and Fehr (1987), who demonstrated that judgments of target facial expression can be influenced by the previous viewing of facial expression anchors, thus suggesting the importance of context. In reply, Ekman and O'Sullivan (1988) argued that Russell and Fehr's target faces may have been emotionally ambiguous or neutral (see also Russell & Fehr, 1988). Also, recently Wallbott (1988) found evidence that expression information was dominant when the expresser was female, whereas situation information was dominant when the expresser was male. Contextual information was not often systematically considered in these studies, and the relationship between elicitors and situations was often not stated. For example, Watson (1972) used the following sentences as contextual information: "He has just asked his girl to marry him and she says'yes' "and "At a party, he overhears an acquaintance making an insulting remark about his girl:' In the latter case, receivers can get information both of an elicitor (insulting remark about his girl) and a situation (at a party). However, the former sentence provides only information of an elicitor, that is, her answer"yes; which will presumably evoke happy feelings in the man. Situational information is not available in the sentence, and this may lead to inconsistent results. If the situation were private, judges may expect that the person will express emotion freely, whereas if the situation were public, facial expressions may be misleading. The Present Study One of the most critical problems with previous studies is in the definition and control of contextual information (e.g, elicitors, situations, and their relationship). The main purpose of the present study was to investigate the relative contributions of expressive behaviors and elicitors to emotion judgment when situational information is both specified and manipulated. As noted above, Ekman et al. (1972) emphasized the necessity of considering the clarity of information provided by both sources, face and context. If the clarifies of the sources are not known, it is impossible to compare the relative contributions of the information sources in a meaningful way. For example, if an informative face is combined with an ambiguous context and if the results show more contribution of face than context, it is not valid to conclude that facial expressions compose a more important source of information than contexts. The results may reflect only the difference of the amount of information available in those sources. The slide viewing technique (Buck, 1978) is useful in the study of this topic. This technique has been developed to study the spontaneous communication of emotion through facialgestural expressions. Sender subjects are shown a series of emotionally loaded slides and asked to describe their reactions to the slides verbally while, unknown to them, their facial-gestural reactions (including the face, shoulders, and upper torso) are videotaped by a hidden camera. The slide viewing technique makes it possible to specify not

3 1034 M. NAKAMURA, R. BUCK, AND D. KENNY only the clarity of information provided by the expression and the elicitor (the slide), but also to define and manipulate the situation. In the present study, receivers were provided with explanations about the slide viewing situation that led them to view it as either a public or private (spontaneous) expressive situation. Instructing receivers that senders were watching slides by themselves and were recorded by a hidden camera should encourage the receiver to regard the situation as more informal and spontaneous, for it implies relatively freely expressed behavior on the part of senders. On the other hand, instructing receivers that senders were watching slides with several experimenters in a laboratory and were being recorded by a visible camera should encourage receivers to regard the situation as more formal and public, and it should imply that the expressive behavior on the part of the sender is more influenced by display rules, On the basis of the preceding considerations, the following hypotheses were developed: First, we expected that facial expressions would generally have a larger effect on the emotion judgments than elicitors. This is based on the evidence of the universality of facial display of emotion (Darwin, 1972; Ekman & Friesen, 1975), which would suggest that all else being equal, a sender's facial expression should be a more reliable indicator of his or her particular emotional response than would the elicitor. For this reason, we expected that there would be a general tendency for facial expressions to be more important than contextual information in emotion judgments. Second, we expected that facial expressions would be relatively more important in the private situation than in the public situation, whereas elicitors would be relatively more important in the public situation than in the private situation. In the public situation, receivers should expect that the level of inhibition of expressive behavior is high relative to the private situation. On the other hand, the effect of elicitors may be rather stable across the situations. Two studies were conducted. In Study 1, the source clarities of the facial-gestural expressions and slides were measured and equated. In Study 2 the above hypotheses were examined by using the pairings of the expressions and slides that were selected in Study 1, and decoding rules were also analyzed. Study 1 The purpose of Study I was to measure and equate the source clarities of facial expressions and slides. Method Subjects. A total of 28 undergraduates (14 men and 14 women) enrolled in an introductory communication sciences course at the University of Connecticut participated in Study 1. Stimuli. In all, 18 videotaped facial-gestural expressions (3 expressions for each of 6 senders) and nine slides were used as stimuli. The expressions and slides were selected in pilot studies from a pool of 232 videotaped facial expressions of undergraduates at the University of Connecticut taken by Wagner, Buck, and Winterbotham (1989) and the pool of the slides used in the slide viewing technique (Buck, 1978). Procedure. Subjects were told that the expressions were taken in a private situation; that is, the expressions were recorded by a hidden video camera while senders were watching slides alone. 2 Subjects were shown all 18 expressions and all nine slides one-by-one by using a 20-in. TV monitor with a videotape recorder (for facial expressions) and a slide projector with a cm back-lighted screen (for slides). When viewing expressions, subjects were asked to rate how they thought the sender felt; when viewing slides, they rated how they thought people, in general, would feel about the slide. In both cases, the ratings were made along eight 7-point emotion scales. The eight scales were Pleasant-Unpleasant, Strong-Weak, Not at all Happy-Very Happy, Not at all Sad-Very Sad, Not at all Angry-Very Angry, Not at all Afraid-Very Afraid, Not at all Surprised-Very Surprised, and Not at all Disgusted- Very Disgusted. The mean ratings of the stimuli on these scales constitute the measure of the source clarities of the stimuli. Results and Discussion Distance and correlation measures among stimuli were obtained to match the source clarities of the stimuli. To measure the correlation between two stimuli, the eight scales were correlated for each subject. These correlations were then averaged across the 28 subjects. The distance measure was the square root of the sum of squared differences between the means averaged across 28 subjects' eight scales. By examining the distance and correlation measures and also profiles of ratings of each slide and facial expression across the eight scales, 18 facial expressions (3 expressionsmpleasant, neutral, and unpleasant-- each from 3 different male and 3 different female senders) and 9 slides (3 pleasant, 3 unpleasant, and 3 neutral) were selected. Those slides/expressions that provided closer distance and stronger correlation with one another and that met additional criteria were chosen. These additional criteria were as follows: Pleasant face/slide. The mean rating scores on the Pleasant and Happy scales were greater than the midpoint of the 7-point scales; the mean rating scores for negative emotions were less than the midpoint; and the differences of the mean rating scores between the positive and negative emotions scales were more than one unit on the scales. Neutral face/slide. Mean rating scores (except for the Pleasant scale) were less than the midpoint and the ranges of the ratings were within two units on the scales. Unpleasant face/slide. Mean rating scores for Pleasant and Happy scales were less than the midpoint; mea. n ratings for negative emotions were greater than midpoints; and the differences of the rating scores between the positive and negative emotion scales were more than one unit on the scales. Table 1 shows the average correlation (the top number in each cell) and distance measures (the bottom number) for each cell. The value in each cell is the mean across subjects of the correlations and the distances between stimuli. Recall that the larger the correlation and the smaller the distance, the more similar the two stimuli are perceived. The results suggest that the neutral facial expressions were rated closer to the pleasant slides than to the neutral slides and that the neutral slides were rated closer to the pleasant facial expressions than to the neutral facial expressions. Therefore, both the neutral facial expressions 2 Earlier pilot studies varied the contextual information presented to subjects. We had public and private conditions as in Study 2. Because no differences were found between these two conditions in the pilot studies, only the private condition was used in this study. This supports the expectation that the effects of elicitors are stable across situations.

4 m CONTRIBUTIONS OF BEHAVIOR AND INFORMATION 1035 Table 1 Means of Correlation and Distance Scores Measure Neutral face Pleasant face Unpleasant face Neutral slide Pleasant slide Unpleasant slide B.99 m Note. The top and bottom numbers in each cell are correlation and distance scores, respectively.. Higher correlations and smaller distances indicate greater similarity. and slides tended to be rated as pleasant. However, with the exception of the neutral stimuli, the ratings of the same types (pleasant and unpleasant) of facial expressions and slides were highly consistent with each other, indicating that their source clarities were matched, and these facial expressions and slides served as the stimuli in Study 2. Despite our attempts to equate the source clarities of the slides and facial expressions, there were some differences both within the set of slides or expressions of the same type and across the two. To further equate the source clarities, we adjusted the ratings in Study 2 for these differences. For example, for ratings of a pleasant facial expression, we computed the mean rating of all pleasant facial expressions and slides. The adjustment consisted of subtracting from the ratings in Study 2 for this pleasant facial expression the average rating for that expression from Study 1 minus the mean for all pleasant slides and facial expressions from Study 1. The same type of adjustment was made to the slide ratings. Study 2 The goal of Study 2 was to examine the hypotheses proposed in the introduction by using the stimuli whose source clarities were determined and matched in Study 1. Method Design of the study. Independent variables were situation (situation information: public vs. private), order (the order of stimulus presentation: forward vs. backward), set (sets of pairings of facial expressions and a slide: three sets), and Rsex (receivers' sex) as between-subjects variables; therefore, there were 234 ( ) between-subjects cells. The within-subjects variables were Ftype (type of facial expressions: pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant), Stype (types of slides: pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant), and Ssex (senders' sex). Dependent variables were the emotion scales used in both the pilot studies and Study 1. The within-subjects variables were combined by using three Greco- Latin squares: Ftype, Stype, and sender were counterbalanced. The six senders were divided randomly into three groups, each composed of one male and one female sender. Across all subjects, each sender's facial expression was paired with all three types of slides, and each sender was paired with all nine slides. Each subject viewed all nine cells of Ftype Stype. Subjects. A total of 72 subjects (36 men and 36 women) enrolled in an introductory communication sciences course at the University of Connecticut pffrticipated in this study. Subjects were run in groups ranging from a minimum of 2 to a maximum of 12. Stimuli. The facial expressions and slides in which source elarities were examined and matched in Study 1 served as stimuli. There were 3 facial expressions (pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant) for each of six senders (3 men and 3 women; a total ofl 8 facial expressions) and three slides of three types (pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant; a total of 9 slides). Apparatus, A tape recorder was used to give instructions and to manipulate situational information. A 20-in. TV monitor, a videotape recorder, a slide projector, and a back-lighted screen (30 25 cm) were used to show facial expressions and slides. Booklets for the experimental session contained the instructions and the eight 7-point emotion scales for 2 practice and 18 experimental sequences. The eight 7-point scales were the same as those of Study 1. Six 7-point scales were used to examine the effects of the situational information (manipulation cheek): Public-Private, Formal-Informal, Natural-Artificial, Tense-Relaxed, Intentional-Spontaneous, and Nonexpressive-Expressive. These scales were selected to describe the general characteristics of situations in which facial expressions may occur. After the experimental session, a questionnaire was presented that contained a credibility cheek to examine whether subjects suspected that the pairing of facial expressions and a slide were not the actual ones. Finally, a Decoding Rule scale was given that included 36 sets of the following three 7-point scales: Do Not Express True Feelings At All-Express lhte Feelings Very Much, Do Not Make Positive Expressions At All-Make Positive Expressions Very Much, and Do Not Make Negative Expressions-Make Negative Expressions Very Much. These scales were rated to reply to the following description: "When a true feeling is (happy, sad, anger, fear, surprise, or disgus0 and a situation is (public, private, formal, informal, tense, or relaxed), people in general (followed by three scales): In this description, the emotions (e.g, happy) and situations (e.g, public) were systematically combined. Procedure. First, the following general instructions were provided by using both a written format and a tape recorder: This is a study of emotion. This study has two sessions: an experimental session and a questionnaire session. In the first session, you will see 18 pairs of a facial expression and a slide from a previous study. In the previous study, the facial expressions of 6 subjects (target persons) were recorded while they were watching various kinds of slides. The process of recording will be explained at the beginning of this experiment. There were nine slides, and each target person watched three of the nine. Therefore, some of the nine slides were watched by more than one target person. As you might expect, the reactions of the target persons are not exactly the same even to the same slides. In this experiment, we would like you to rate how the target persons felt about the slides along eight emotion scales provided. In the following session, you will get a questionnaire and answer several questions concerning emotion, which are not related to the first session. The manipulation oftbe situational information (public vs. private) followed. To do this, the setting and recording process of a previous study (i.e, slide viewing technique; Buck, 1978) was explained by using an explanation sheet, tape-recorded explanation, and three slides that illustrate the setting. In the public condition, subjects were informed that the facial expressions were recorded by a video camera visible to the sender while the sender was watching a slide in a laboratory with three experimenters (professors). In the private condition, subjects were informed that the facial expressions were recorded by a hidden

5 1036 M. NAKAMURA, R. BUCK, AND D. KENNY video camera while a sender was watching slides alone in a room. Following the explanations, the first manipulation cheek was performed; that is, subjects were asked to rate the setting of the previous study along six situation scales (e.g, Public-Private). The main purpose of the manipulation check was to examine how subjects perceived the situation in which facial expressions were taken, that is, the effect of the situational information on the subjects' perception of the situations. The second purpose was to ensure that the subjects were explicitly aware of the public or private characteristics of the situation. In the experimental session, after two practice sequences and ensuring that subjects understood the procedure, 18 experimental sequences were presented. First, the videotaped facial expressions of a sender and a slide were simultaneously shown to subjects for about 5 s. After the pairing was turned off, subjects rated the emotional state of the sender along the eight emotion scales for about 30 s (the duration varied slightly among sequences and subjects). The next pairing was provided after all subjects had finished the rating, and this procedure was repeated until 18 experimental sequences were completed. Following the experiment, the second manipulation cheek (same as above) was performed. After the experimental session, a questionnaire session was conducted. At the beginning of the session, the credibility check was performed. Then subjects answered questions concerning decoding rules. There was no time limitation in the questionnaire session. Analyses. Repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed. Independent variables were Ftype, Stype, Ssex, situation, order, set, and Rsex. Dependent variables were the rating scores for the six situation scales (in the manipulation check), the eight emotion scales (in the experimental session), the credibility scale (in the credibility check), and the three expression scales (in the Decoding Rule scale). To measure the relative effects of the facial expressions versus the slides, a difference score was also used as a dependent variable. The difference score was calculated from emotion scale rating scores by the following equation: D = (PF - UF) - (PS - US), where D is a difference score, PF is a pleasant facial expression rating score, UF is an unpleasant facial expression rating score, PS is a pleasant slide rating score, and US is an unpleasant slide rating score. The PF, UF, PS, and US scores were obtained by averaging the rating scores for each category Source (facial expression or slide) x Pleasantness of Source (pleasant or unpleasant) for each receiver. A positive difference score for positive emotion scales and a negative difference score for negative emotion scales means that the effect of facial expressions is larger than that of slides. Results Manipulation checks. Four-way (Situation x Order x Set x Rsex) repeated measures ANOVAs were performed with the mean scores of the first and second manipulation checks as dependent variables. A very large situation effect was obtained, F(1, 48) = 40.41, p <.001. That is, in the public condition the situation in which facial expressions were taken was rated to be more formal, public, artificial, tense, intentional, and nonexpressive than in the private condition. Therefore, the intended effect of the manipulation of situational information on the subjects' perception of the situation was obtained. No other effects were significant except for the interaction between order and set, F(2, 48) = 4.85, p <.01. This interaction was further examined by analyzing the data of the first and second manipulation checks separately. The Order x Set inter- action was significant only in the first manipulation check. Because the first manipulation check was performed before the experimental session, the interaction must be attributed to chance differences between particular groups of subjects. Credibility check. The average rating score of credibility was 4.57 (7-point scale, minimum l to maximum 7, high score means high credibility). A four-way (Situation x Order Set x Rsex) ANOVA was performed with the credibility rating score as a dependent variable. No between-subjects variable affected the credibility other than the significant interaction between set and Rsex, F(2, 48) = 5.18, p <.0 I. As set represents the three Greco-Latin squares, that is, the different pairings of a facial expression and a slide, the interaction means that the different pairings affect differently the credibility rating of male and female receivers. However, as will be shown later, this Set Rsex interaction seems not to affect the qualitative aspect of the emotion scale ratings in the actual experiment. Accordingly, the entire set of data was analyzed in the following sections. Decoding Rule scale. Repeated measures ANOVAs were performed with three independent variables, emotion (six emotions), situation (public and private), and Rsex, for the three scale rating scores as dependent variables (express true feelings, express positive expressions, express negative expressions). All effects were highly significant (p <.00 l). The ratings of happy and surprise were significantly different from the negative emotions of sadness, anger, fear, and disgust. In the public situations, facial expressions were rated to be more inhibited than in the private situations, supporting the existence of a decoding rule to this effect. Emotion x Situation interactions occurred, so that in the private situations all emotions are judged to be expressed very much, whereas, in the public situations, negative emotions (especially sad) were judged to be less expressed than positive emotions. There was an interaction between Rsex and situation for the express true feeling rating scale. The effect of situation is larger for women than for men; thus, the decoding rule that one changes one's expression of true feelings in public versus private situations is apparently stronger for women. Contributions offacialexpressions andslides. Six-way (Situation x Rsex Ftype X Stype x Set X Order) repeated measures ANOVAs were performed with the eight emotion scales as dependent variables. The main effects of Ftype and Stype were highly significant in all scales but one, the Stype main effect for the Angry scale. The main effects indicate that for the positive emotions scales, pleasant facial expressions and slides were rated higher than unpleasant ones. For negative emotion scales, including the Strong and Surprise scales, the relationship was the opposite. The Ftype X Stype interaction was significant for five scales--pleasant, Strong, Happy, Sad, and Disgust. Table 2 presents the mean rating scores for the Pleasant and Sad scales on each type of facial expression and slide. On the Pleasant scale, facial expressions had a smaller effect when paired with unpleasant slides than with pleasant slides. The Happy scale showed a similar pattern. On the other hand, on the Sad scale (as well as disgust), the relationship was opposite: The effect of facial expressions was weakest for pleasant slides and strongest for unpleasant slides. The pattern for the Strong scale was quite different. When both slides and facial expressions were consistent and not neutral, the ratings were the highest.

6 CONTRIBUTIONS OF BEHAVIOR AND INFORMATION 1037 Table 2 Mean Rating Scores for the Pleasant and Sad Scales Facial expression Scale/slide Pleasant Neutral Unpleasant Pleasant Pleasant Neutral Unpleasant Sad Pleasant Neutral Unpleasant Note. A rating score varies from I to 7. Table 4 Mean Rating Scores for Receiver Sex, Situation, and Slide Type on the Pleasant Scale Type of slide Receiver sex/situation Pleasant Neutral Unpleasant Male Public Private Female Public Private Note. A rating score varies from i to 7. In order to investigate directly the relative effects of facial expressions and slides, the difference scores were analyzed. As stated above, a positive difference score for positive emotion scales and a negative score for negative scales indicate that the effect of facial expressions is larger than the effect of slides. A series of t tests was performed, and the results are shown in Table 3. For all eight scales, mean difference scores were significantly different from zero. All of these effects indicate that facial expressions had larger effects than those of slides. For strong and surprise, negative information was seen as stronger and more surprising, which explains the negative signs in Table 3. Summarizing the results of this section, it appears that facial expressions generally tended to have a larger effect on emotion scale ratings than did slides. Effect of situational information. The six-way repeated measures ANOVA described above was examined to analyze the effect of situational information. A significant Stype x Situation interaction was found for the Angry and Disgust scales. On these two scales, slides had a larger effect in the public situation than in the private situation. Also significant was the Ftype x Situation interaction for the Strong scale. The effect of facial expressions was relatively larger in the private situation. It was also found that Stype and situation interacted with Rsex. This three-way interaction was significant for five Table 3 Results oft Tests of Difference Scores Scale M t(48) Pleasant "* Strong ** Happy "* Sad ** Angry " Afraid ** Surprise ** Disgust " Note. A positive difference score for positive emotion scales and a negative difference score for negative emotion scales means that the effect of facial expressions is larger than that of slides. *p <.01. ** p <.001. scales--pleasant, Happy, Sad, Afraid, and Disgust. Table 4 provides the mean rating scores on the Pleasant scale for Stype, situation, and Rsex. On this scale and on the other four scales, the Stype x Situation X Rsex interaction means that the effect of Stype was smaller in the private situation than in the public situation for female receivers. On the other hand, for male receivers the effect of Stype did not change as much across the situations. Repeated measures ANOVAs were performed separately for male and female receivers. Consistent with the preceding analyses, significant Stype x Situation interactions were found in the same five scales as above, as well as the Angry scale, for female but not male receivers. These results indicate that Stype had a relatively smaller effect in the private situation than in the public situation for female receivers, but this was not the case for male receivers. Similar results were found in the analyses of the difference scores. A four-way (Situation x Rsex x Order Set) ANOVA was performed with the difference score as a dependent variable. A significant Situation X Rsex interaction was found for three scales--pleasant, Happy, and Disgust--and a similar trend was found for the Sad and Afraid scales (see Table 5). These five scales are exactly the same as above. The mean difference scores on the Pleasant scale for each situation and Rsex are presented in Table 6. Slides had a larger effect in the public situation than in the private situation for female receivers. This tendency was opposite for male receivers. As can be seen in Table 5, Rsex was significant for five of the eight scales. In each case, women attributed more importance to the slide than men. Thus, men seemed to value facial information more than women did. Summarizing the results of this section, women attributed less importance than did men to the facial expression as opposed to the slide, and women were more sensitive to the situational information in that they paid relatively more attention to the slides in the public situation than in the private situation. General Discussion On all of the emotion rating scales, facial expressions had a larger effect on emotion ratings than did slides. This result can be interpreted as suggesting that facial expressions are more important information sources than elicitors (i.e~ slides) in the judgment of emotion in this study. This generalization is, to

7 1038 M. NAKAMURA, R. BUCK, AND D. KENNY Table 5 F Test Results ofano VAs of Difference Scores Receiver's Situation Scale Situation sex receiver's sex Pleasant * 4.39* Strong Happy * Sad " 3.89 Angry Afraid 4.79" ** 4.19* Surprise Disgust * ** * p <.05. **p <.01. some extent, consistent with the results of previous studies. That is, although only one study (Watson, 1972) found face primacy, no study found context primacy. At least, contextual information did not overwhelm facial expressions in the previous studies. The present results suggest that this face primacy is especially strong for male receivers. The second hypothesis of this study was that facial expressions would be relatively more important in private as opposed to public situations. Significant effects of situational information were found only for female receivers. That is, female receivers considered facial expressions to be relatively more important in the private situation and slides to be relatively more important in the public situation. On the other hand, for male receivers the situational information did not affect the emotion rating. Therefore, this hypothesis was supported by the results of only female receivers. Although this gender difference was not expected, we will examine some possible explanations of this result. One explanation is that there was a gender difference in the perception of the situational information. This possibility is rejected because in the manipulation check there were no gender differences. Both male and female receivers rated the public situation to be more formal, public, and artificial than the private situation. There was also no gender difference in the credibility check of the convincingness of the pairings of facial expressions and slides. A third possible explanation is that there may be a gender difference in decoding rules; that is, female receivers may be more likely to use decoding rules that consider the formality of situations. This may, in fact, be the case. According to the Decoding Rule scale, both male and female receivers had a rule to distinguish public and private situations, but the Rsex Situation interaction indicated that this was stronger for women than men. Female receivers had a stronger expectation that in private situations people generally express true emotion more than in public situations. This explanation for the gender difference is compatible with that suggested by Hall (1979). She reviewed gender difference studies and examined some hypotheses to explain the female superiority in nonverbal communication. One hypothesis was that the apparent female superiority results from women's ability to consider broader information in communicating with others. In this study, female receivers used a more efficient strategy of combining the information from facial expressions, elici- tors, and situations in judging the emotional state of others. Therefore, the result suggests that women are more skillful receivers in emotion communication than men because they apply situational decoding rules in an appropriate and effective way. As argued in the preceding section, the relative importance of slides and facial expressions can be defined as the relative strength of spontaneous expression versus inhibition of emotion in the face. The results suggest that, in female receivers, the association between facial expressions and emotion was stronger in the private situation relative to the public situation. This can also be explained by the relative strength of the expected inhibition of facial expressions of emotion in the different situations. The implication is that in public situations facial expressions are perceived to be more inhibited than in private situations. This interpretation is consistent with the results of the Decoding Rule scale. Summarizing the arguments, the greater overall contribution of facial expression as opposed to context was affected for female receivers by how public versus private the situation was. The conflicting results of previous studies may be due in part to the failure to define and control contextual information and to measure the source clarity of the expressive and contextual information. The gender difference--that only female receivers took the situational information into account--may reflect the fact that women use more appropriate decoding rules in emotion communication. The limitations of the present study include the limited range of both the senders' and receivers' age and their lack of personal relationship. It is expected that the strategies and skills of decoding the emotional state of others will change with the age of receivers and also with the particular combination of the sex and dominance of a sender and a receiver. For example, the strategies that are used by younger children will differ from those of adults (Blanck & Rosenthal, 1982; Blanck, Rosenthal, Snodgrass, DePaulo, & Zuckerman, 1981; Saarni, 1979, 1982, 1984). Socially subordinate people may be more sensitive to changing situations than socially dominant people (Eagly, 1987; Snodgrass, 1985). Further studies should be conducted to investigate the effects of the above-mentioned developmental factors and contextual information on the relative contributions of facial expressions and an elicitor. A further limitation is that only a single culture was investigated. We are now replicating this study with a Japanese sample. Despite limitations, the results of this study are consistent Table 6 Mean Difference Scores for Receiver Sex and Situation on the Pleasant Scale Situation Receiver sex Public Private Male Female Note. Positive scores indicate that facial expressions are given more importance than slides.

8 CONTRIBUTIONS OF BEHAVIOR AND INFORMATION 1039 with the emphasis on the communicative importance of facial expression recognized by Darwin (1972) and reemphasized by Ekman et al. (1972; Ekman & O'Sullivan, 1988). Although it is true that ratings of facial expressions may be altered to some extent by manipulations of context (Russell & Fehr, 1987, 1988), the present study, using systematic combinations of contextual and expressive information matched for source clarity, found strong evidence for the importance of expressive behavior. References Blanck, P. D, & Rosenthal, R. (1982). Developing strategies for decoding "leaky" messages: On learning how and when to decode discrepant and consistent social communications. In R. W Feldman (Ed.), The development of nonverbal behavior in children (pp ). New York: Springer-Verlag. Blanck, P. D, Rosenthal, R, Snodgrass, S. E, DePaulo, B. M, & Zuckerman, M. (1981). Longitudinal and cross-sectional age effects in nonverbal decoding skill and style. Developmental Psychology, 18, Bruner, J, & Tagiuri, R. (1954). The perception of people. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, 2nd ed., pp ). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Buck, R. (1978). The slide-viewing technique for measuring nonverbal sending accuracy: A guide for replication. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 8, 63. Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York: Guilford Press. Darwin, C. (1972). Expression of the emotions in man and animals. New York: Philosophical Library. DePaulo, B. M, & Rosenthal, R. (1979). Age changes in nonverbal decoding skills: Evidence for increasing differentiation. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly, 25, Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ekman, R (1973). Darwin and facial expression. New York: Academic Press. Ekman, R (1977). Biological and cultural contributions to body and facial movement. In J. Blacking (Ed.), The anthropology of the body (ASA. Monograph 15). London: Academic Press. Ekman, P, & Friesen, W V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, Ekman, P, & Friesen, W. V. (1974). Detecting deception from the body or face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face. Englewood Cliffs, N J: Prentice-Hall. Ekman, P, Friesen, W V, & Ellsworth, R (1972). Emotion in the human face. New York: Pergamon Press. Ekman, P, Friesen, W V, & Malmstrom, E. J. (1970). Facial behavior and stress in two cultures. Unpublished manuscript, Langley Porter Neuropsychiatric Institute, San Francisco. Ekman, P, & O'Sullivan, M. (1988). The role of context in interpreting facial expression: Comment on Russell and Fehr (1987). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, Frijda, N. H. (1958). Facial expression and situational cues. Journalof Abnormal Social Psychology, 57, Goldberg, H. D. (1951). The role of"cutting" in the perception of the motion picture. Journal of Applied Psychology, 35, Goodenough, E L, & Tinker, M. A. (1931). The relative potency of facial and verbal description of stimulus in the judgment of emotion. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12, Hall, J. A. (1979). Gender, gender roles, and nonverbal communication skills. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.), Skill in nonverbal communication (pp ). Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn & Hain. Hunt, W A. (1941). Recent developments in the field of emotion. Psychological Bulletin, 38, Kenny, D. A, & La Vole, L. (1984). The social relations model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 18, pp ). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Knapp, M. L. (1984). Interpersonal communication and human relationship. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Knudsen, H. R., & Muzekari, L. H. (1983). The effects of verbal statements of context on facial expressions of emotion. JournalofNonverbal Behavior, 7, Munn, N. L. (1940). The effect of knowledge of the situation upon judgment of emotion from facial expression. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 35, Russell, J. A, & Fehr, B. (1987). Relativity in the perception of emotion in facial expression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, Russell, J. A, & Fehr, B. (1988). Reply to Ekman and O'Sullivan. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, Saarni, C. (1979). Children's understanding of display rules for expressive behavior. Developmental Psychology, 15, Saarni, C. (1982). Social and affective functions of nonverbal behavior: Developmental concerns. In R. W Feldman (Ed.), Development of nonverbal behavior in children (pp ). New York: Springer- Verlag. Saarni, C. (1984). An observational study of children's attempts to monitor their expressive behavior. Child Development, 55, Shimoda, K., Argyle, M, & Ricci-Bitti, P. (1978). The intercultural recognition of emotional expressions by three national racial groups: English, Italian, and Japanese. European Journal of Social Psychology, 8, Snodgrass, S. E. (1985). Women's intuition: The effect of subordinate role on interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, Spignesi, A, & Shot, R. E. (1981). The judgment of emotion from facial expressions, contexts, and their combination. The Journal of General Psychology, 104, Tagiuri, R. (1968). Person perception. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology(vol. 3, 2nd ed, pp ). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Vinacke, W. E. (1949). The judgment of facial expressions by three national-racial groups in Hawaii: I. Caucasian faces. Journal of Personality, 17, Wagner, H, Buck, R., & Winterbotham, M. (1989). Sex differences in the communication of specific emotions. Unpublished study, University of Connecticut. Wallbott, H. G. (1988). In and out of context: Influences of facial expression and context information on emotion attributions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 27, Watson, S. G. (1972). Judgment of emotion from facial and contextual cue combinations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, Received October 24, 1988 Revision received January 2, 1990 Accepted March 17, 1990

Sex differences in emotion recognition by observing body movements: A case of American students

Sex differences in emotion recognition by observing body movements: A case of American students Japanese Pvycholokneat Research 1987, Vol.29, No.2, 89-93 Short Report Sex differences in emotion recognition by observing body movements: A case of American students SHUNYA SOGON1 Human Emotion Laboratory,

More information

Face and Tone of Voice in the Communication of Deception

Face and Tone of Voice in the Communication of Deception Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1982, Vol. 43, No. 2, 347-357 Copyright 1982 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. OQ22-3514/82/4302-0347$00.75 and Tone of Voice in the Communication

More information

Neuropsychosocial Factors in Emotion Recognition: Facial Expressions

Neuropsychosocial Factors in Emotion Recognition: Facial Expressions Neuropsychosocial Factors in Emotion Recognition: Facial Expressions 1 It has often been said that the eyes are the window to the soul. This statement may be carried to a logical assumption that not only

More information

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, 785-789. Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 EXAMINATION OF THE PASSIVE FACIAL FEEDBACK HYPOTHESIS USING AN IMPLICIT MEASURE: WITH A FURROWED BROW, NEUTRAL OBJECTS

More information

The Effect of Contextual Information and Emotional Clarity on Emotional Evaluation

The Effect of Contextual Information and Emotional Clarity on Emotional Evaluation American International Journal of Social Science Vol. 6, No. 4, December 2017 The Effect of Contextual Information and Emotional Clarity on Emotional Evaluation Fada Pan*, Leyuan Li, Yanyan Zhang, Li Zhang

More information

Social Context Based Emotion Expression

Social Context Based Emotion Expression Social Context Based Emotion Expression Radosław Niewiadomski (1), Catherine Pelachaud (2) (1) University of Perugia, Italy (2) University Paris VIII, France radek@dipmat.unipg.it Social Context Based

More information

Misinterpretation of facial expression:a cross-cultural study

Misinterpretation of facial expression:a cross-cultural study Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences (1999), 53, 45 50 Regular Article Misinterpretation of facial expression:a cross-cultural study TOSHIKI SHIOIRI, md, phd, 1,3 TOSHIYUKI SOMEYA, md, phd, 2 DAIGA HELMESTE,

More information

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials for Wisneski and Skitka Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction Table of Contents 2 Pilot Studies 2 High Awareness

More information

CHAPTER THIRTEEN Managing Communication

CHAPTER THIRTEEN Managing Communication CHAPTER THIRTEEN Managing Communication 1 Effective Management 3 rd Edition Chuck Williams What Would You Do? JetBlue Headquarters Forest Hills, New York JetBlue offers direct flights, low fares, and great

More information

Running head: CULTURES 1. Difference in Nonverbal Communication: Cultures Edition ALI OMIDY. University of Kentucky

Running head: CULTURES 1. Difference in Nonverbal Communication: Cultures Edition ALI OMIDY. University of Kentucky Running head: CULTURES 1 Difference in Nonverbal Communication: Cultures Edition ALI OMIDY University of Kentucky CULTURES 2 Abstract The following paper is focused on the similarities and differences

More information

Fukuoka University of Education

Fukuoka University of Education Tomoko Sugimura sugitomo@fukuoka-edu.ac.jp Fukuoka University of Education 18 5 6 facial perception, gender-discrimination, young children Developmental studies have demonstrated that young children inaccurately

More information

Running head: FACIAL EXPRESSION AND SKIN COLOR ON APPROACHABILITY 1. Influence of facial expression and skin color on approachability judgment

Running head: FACIAL EXPRESSION AND SKIN COLOR ON APPROACHABILITY 1. Influence of facial expression and skin color on approachability judgment Running head: FACIAL EXPRESSION AND SKIN COLOR ON APPROACHABILITY 1 Influence of facial expression and skin color on approachability judgment Federico Leguizamo Barroso California State University Northridge

More information

MATSUMOTO AND EKMAN'S JAPANESE AND CAUCASIAN FACIAL EXPRESSIONS OF EMOTION (JACFEE): RELIABILITY DATA AND CROSS-NATIONAL DIFFERENCES

MATSUMOTO AND EKMAN'S JAPANESE AND CAUCASIAN FACIAL EXPRESSIONS OF EMOTION (JACFEE): RELIABILITY DATA AND CROSS-NATIONAL DIFFERENCES MATSUMOTO AND EKMAN'S JAPANESE AND CAUCASIAN FACIAL EXPRESSIONS OF EMOTION (JACFEE): RELIABILITY DATA AND CROSS-NATIONAL DIFFERENCES Michael Biehl, David Matsumoto, Paul Ekman, Valerie Meant, Karl Heider,

More information

Comparison of Multisensory Display Rules. in Expressing Complex Emotions between Cultures

Comparison of Multisensory Display Rules. in Expressing Complex Emotions between Cultures ISCA Archive http://www.isca-speech.org/archive FAAVSP - The 1 st Joint Conference on Facial Analysis, Animation, and Auditory-Visual Speech Processing Vienna, Austria, September 11-13, 2015 Comparison

More information

Lecture 6: Social Cognition (Seeking Accuracy) Part I: Non-verbal communication Part II: Attribution

Lecture 6: Social Cognition (Seeking Accuracy) Part I: Non-verbal communication Part II: Attribution Lecture 6: Social Cognition (Seeking Accuracy) Part I: Non-verbal communication Part II: Attribution Social Perception A fundamental process through which people come to understand their social world.

More information

Emotion Elicitation Effect of Films in a Japanese

Emotion Elicitation Effect of Films in a Japanese SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 00, (), - Society for Personality Research (Inc.) Emotion Elicitation Effect of Films in a Japanese Sample Wataru Sato, Motoko Noguchi, and Sakiko Yoshikawa Kyoto University,

More information

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu Person Perception Forming Impressions of Others Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu Person Perception person perception: how we come to know about others temporary states, emotions, intentions and desires impression

More information

Valence and Gender Effects on Emotion Recognition Following TBI. Cassie Brown Arizona State University

Valence and Gender Effects on Emotion Recognition Following TBI. Cassie Brown Arizona State University Valence and Gender Effects on Emotion Recognition Following TBI Cassie Brown Arizona State University Knox & Douglas (2009) Social Integration and Facial Expression Recognition Participants: Severe TBI

More information

DO INFANTS EXPRESS DISCRETE EMOTIONS? ADULT JUDGMENTS OF FACIAL, VOCAL, AND BODY ACTIONS

DO INFANTS EXPRESS DISCRETE EMOTIONS? ADULT JUDGMENTS OF FACIAL, VOCAL, AND BODY ACTIONS DO INFANTS EXPRESS DISCRETE EMOTIONS? ADULT JUDGMENTS OF FACIAL, VOCAL, AND BODY ACTIONS By: Linda A. Camras, Jean Sullivan, and George Michel Camras, LA, Sullivan, J, & Michel, GF. Adult judgments of

More information

Culture and Emotion THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN EMOTION. Outline

Culture and Emotion THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN EMOTION. Outline Outline Culture and Emotion The Evolution of Human Emotion Universality in Emotion- The Basic Emotions Perspective Cultural Differences in Emotion Conclusion Chapter 8 THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN EMOTION Emotion:

More information

PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions

PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions Session 6 The Concept of Emotion Lecturer: Dr. Annabella Osei-Tutu, Psychology Department Contact Information: aopare-henaku@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing

More information

Does It Matter Where We Meet? The Role of Emotional Context in Evaluative First Impressions

Does It Matter Where We Meet? The Role of Emotional Context in Evaluative First Impressions Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 2010 Canadian Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 64, No. 2, 107 116 1196-1961/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019139 Does It Matter Where We Meet? The Role of Emotional

More information

Emotions of Living Creatures

Emotions of Living Creatures Robot Emotions Emotions of Living Creatures motivation system for complex organisms determine the behavioral reaction to environmental (often social) and internal events of major significance for the needs

More information

The innate hypothesis

The innate hypothesis The innate hypothesis DARWIN (1872) proposed that the facial expression of emotion evolved as part of the actions necessary for life: Anger: Frowning (to protect eyes in anticipation of attack) Surprise:

More information

Running head: GENDER DIFFERENCES IN EMOTION JUDGMENT

Running head: GENDER DIFFERENCES IN EMOTION JUDGMENT Running head: GENDER DIFFERENCES IN EMOTION JUDGMENT Gender Differences for Speed and Accuracy in the Judgment of the Six Basic Emotions Samantha Lumbert Rochester Institute of Technology 256 Abstract

More information

Drive-reducing behaviors (eating, drinking) Drive (hunger, thirst) Need (food, water)

Drive-reducing behaviors (eating, drinking) Drive (hunger, thirst) Need (food, water) Instinct Theory: we are motivated by our inborn automated behaviors that generally lead to survival. But instincts only explain why we do a small fraction of our behaviors. Does this behavior adequately

More information

University of Groningen. Imitation of emotion Velde, Sytske Willemien van der

University of Groningen. Imitation of emotion Velde, Sytske Willemien van der University of Groningen Imitation of emotion Velde, Sytske Willemien van der IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check

More information

Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification. and. Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect. Nila K Leigh

Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification. and. Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect. Nila K Leigh 1 Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification and Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect Nila K Leigh 131 Ave B (Apt. 1B) New York, NY 10009 Stuyvesant High School 345 Chambers

More information

An Introduction to the Study of Nonverbal Communication p. 1 Nonverbal Communication: Basic Perspectives p. 3 Defining Nonverbal Communication p.

An Introduction to the Study of Nonverbal Communication p. 1 Nonverbal Communication: Basic Perspectives p. 3 Defining Nonverbal Communication p. An Introduction to the Study of Nonverbal Communication p. 1 Nonverbal Communication: Basic Perspectives p. 3 Defining Nonverbal Communication p. 5 Encoding p. 5 Decoding p. 6 Classifying Nonverbal Behavior

More information

Outline. Emotion. Emotions According to Darwin. Emotions: Information Processing 10/8/2012

Outline. Emotion. Emotions According to Darwin. Emotions: Information Processing 10/8/2012 Outline Emotion What are emotions? Why do we have emotions? How do we express emotions? Cultural regulation of emotion Eliciting events Cultural display rules Social Emotions Behavioral component Characteristic

More information

Scalar Ratings of Contempt 1 Running Head: Scalar Ratings of Contempt. Scalar Ratings of Contempt Expressions. David Matsumoto

Scalar Ratings of Contempt 1 Running Head: Scalar Ratings of Contempt. Scalar Ratings of Contempt Expressions. David Matsumoto 1 Running Head: Scalar Ratings of Contempt Scalar Ratings of Contempt Expressions David Matsumoto San Francisco State University 2 Abstract This article reports two studies examining the recognition of

More information

Mothers' ability to identify nonverbal expressions of four affects in their own children.

Mothers' ability to identify nonverbal expressions of four affects in their own children. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1980 Mothers' ability to identify nonverbal expressions of four affects in their own children. Joel Alan

More information

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (6), 1067-1073 Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources MOSHE NAVEH-BENJAMIN University of Missouri,

More information

MY RIGHT I: DECEPTION DETECTION AND HEMISPHERIC DIFFERENCES IN SELF-AWARENESS

MY RIGHT I: DECEPTION DETECTION AND HEMISPHERIC DIFFERENCES IN SELF-AWARENESS SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2003, 31(8), 767-772 Society for Personality Research (Inc.) MY RIGHT I: DECEPTION DETECTION AND HEMISPHERIC DIFFERENCES IN SELF-AWARENESS SARAH MALCOLM AND JULIAN PAUL

More information

Deception Detection Accuracy Using Verbal or Nonverbal Cues

Deception Detection Accuracy Using Verbal or Nonverbal Cues The Journal of Undergraduate Research Volume 9 Journal of Undergraduate Research, Volume 9: 2011 Article 9 2011 Deception Detection Accuracy Using Verbal or Nonverbal Cues Caroline Hicks South Dakota State

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis'

Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis' Japanese Psychological Rescarch 1994, Vol.36, No. 1. 41-48 Special lssue: Decision Making Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis' KAZUHISA

More information

Facial Signs of Emotional Experience

Facial Signs of Emotional Experience Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1980, Vol. 39, No. 6, 1125-1134 Facial Signs of Emotional Experience Paul Ekman and Wallace V. Friesen University of California, San Francisco Sonia Ancoli

More information

C. Identify gender differences in communication. 9. Men and women communicate in different ways, primarily because of socialization and status.

C. Identify gender differences in communication. 9. Men and women communicate in different ways, primarily because of socialization and status. ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR: AN EXPERIENTIAL APPROACH EIGHTH EDITION Joyce S. Osland PART 2 CREATING EFFECTIVE WORK GROUPS Chapter 8 Interpersonal Communication OBJECTIVES: A. Understand the transactional

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Overview. Basic concepts Theories of emotion Universality of emotions Brain basis of emotions Applied research: microexpressions

Overview. Basic concepts Theories of emotion Universality of emotions Brain basis of emotions Applied research: microexpressions Emotion Overview Basic concepts Theories of emotion Universality of emotions Brain basis of emotions Applied research: microexpressions Definition of Emotion Emotions are biologically-based responses

More information

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 BLL R. BROWN AND THOMAS J. REBBN2 UNlVERSTY OF LOUSVLLE This experiment required Ss to make Markov-generated histoforms that were

More information

Looking at You or Looking Elsewhere: The Influence of Head Orientation on the Signal Value of Emotional Facial Expressions

Looking at You or Looking Elsewhere: The Influence of Head Orientation on the Signal Value of Emotional Facial Expressions Motiv Emot (2007) 31:137 144 DOI 10.1007/s11031-007-9057-x ORIGINAL PAPER Looking at You or Looking Elsewhere: The Influence of Head Orientation on the Signal Value of Emotional Facial Expressions Ursula

More information

Emotion Lecture 26 1

Emotion Lecture 26 1 Emotion Lecture 26 1 The Trilogy of Mind Immanuel Kant (1791); Hilgard (1980) There are three absolutely irreducible faculties of mind: knowledge, feeling, and desire. Cognition Knowledge and Beliefs Emotion

More information

training & research for academic newcomers A project of the King Baudouin Foundation

training & research for academic newcomers A project of the King Baudouin Foundation training & research for academic newcomers A project of the King Baudouin Foundation Communication and Presentation skills Hanna Mamzer, Bernd-Friedrich Voigt, Thomas Gebhardt Structure of the day: 9.00-10.45

More information

How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations

How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations Motoyuki Saito (m-saito@kwansei.ac.jp) Department of Psychological Science, Kwansei Gakuin University Hyogo 662-8501, JAPAN Abstract

More information

FACIAL EXPRESSION RECOGNITION FROM IMAGE SEQUENCES USING SELF-ORGANIZING MAPS

FACIAL EXPRESSION RECOGNITION FROM IMAGE SEQUENCES USING SELF-ORGANIZING MAPS International Archives of Photogrammetry and Remote Sensing. Vol. XXXII, Part 5. Hakodate 1998 FACIAL EXPRESSION RECOGNITION FROM IMAGE SEQUENCES USING SELF-ORGANIZING MAPS Ayako KATOH*, Yasuhiro FUKUI**

More information

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Honors Scholar Theses Honors Scholar Program Spring 5-1-2008 Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible Juemei Yang

More information

The Multi-modal Nature of Trustworthiness Perception

The Multi-modal Nature of Trustworthiness Perception The Multi-modal Nature of Trustworthiness Perception Elena Tsankova 1, Eva Krumhuber 2, Andrew J. Aubrey 3, Arvid Kappas 1, Guido Möllering 1, David Marshall 3, Paul L. Rosin 3 1 Jacobs University Bremen,

More information

LESSON OBJECTIVES LEVEL MEASURE

LESSON OBJECTIVES LEVEL MEASURE DEOMI SYLLABUS/ NOTETAKER 740 O'Malley Rd Revised: 20 November 2000 Patrick AFB, FL 32925 PERCEPTIONS LESSON OBJECTIVES LEVEL MEASURE A. Identify the perception process Knowledge Written B. Explain perceptual

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

Interpersonal skills are defined as everyday skills employed for. communication and interaction with individuals. These skills include all

Interpersonal skills are defined as everyday skills employed for. communication and interaction with individuals. These skills include all Interpersonal skills essay 1 Definition: Interpersonal skills are defined as everyday skills employed for communication and interaction with individuals. These skills include all methods of communication,

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E. In D. Algom, D. Zakay, E. Chajut, S. Shaki, Y. Mama, & V. Shakuf (Eds.). (2011). Fechner Day 2011: Proceedings of the 27 th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 89-94). Raanana,

More information

Brain and Cognition, 48(2-3), (2002) Evaluation of nonverbal emotion in face and voice: some preliminary findings on a new battery of tests

Brain and Cognition, 48(2-3), (2002) Evaluation of nonverbal emotion in face and voice: some preliminary findings on a new battery of tests Brain and Cognition, 48(2-3), 499-514 (2002) Evaluation of nonverbal emotion in face and voice: some preliminary findings on a new battery of tests Marc David Pell McGill University, Montréal Abstract

More information

How the Experience of Emotion is Modulated by Facial Feedback

How the Experience of Emotion is Modulated by Facial Feedback J Nonverbal Behav (2018) 42:129 151 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0264-1 ORIGINAL PAPER How the Experience of Emotion is Modulated by Facial Feedback Sven Söderkvist 1 Kajsa Ohlén 1 Ulf Dimberg 1

More information

Nonverbal Communication of Affect in Children

Nonverbal Communication of Affect in Children Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1975, Vol. 31, No. 4, 644-653 Nonverbal Communication of Affect in Children Ross Buck University of Connecticut A paradigm was tested for measuring the tendency

More information

Heavy Smokers', Light Smokers', and Nonsmokers' Beliefs About Cigarette Smoking

Heavy Smokers', Light Smokers', and Nonsmokers' Beliefs About Cigarette Smoking Journal of Applied Psychology 1982, Vol. 67, No. 5, 616-622 Copyright 1982 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 002I-9010/82/6705-0616S00.75 ', ', and Nonsmokers' Beliefs About Cigarette Smoking

More information

Black 1 White 5 Black

Black 1 White 5 Black PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Black 1 White 5 Black Hypodescent in Reflexive Categorization of Racially Ambiguous Faces Destiny Peery and Galen V. Bodenhausen Northwestern University ABSTRACT Historically,

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Temporal Context and the Recognition of Emotion from Facial Expression

Temporal Context and the Recognition of Emotion from Facial Expression Temporal Context and the Recognition of Emotion from Facial Expression Rana El Kaliouby 1, Peter Robinson 1, Simeon Keates 2 1 Computer Laboratory University of Cambridge Cambridge CB3 0FD, U.K. {rana.el-kaliouby,

More information

Effects of Arousal on Judgments of Others' Emotions

Effects of Arousal on Judgments of Others' Emotions Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1984, Vol. 46, No. 3, 551-560 Copyright 1984 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Effects of Arousal on Judgments of Others' Emotions Margaret S.

More information

PERCEPTUAL CONDITIONS AFFECTING EASE OF ASSOCIATION

PERCEPTUAL CONDITIONS AFFECTING EASE OF ASSOCIATION Journal of Experimental Psychology 1972, Vol. 93, No. 1, 176-180 PERCEPTUAL CONDITIONS AFFECTING EASE OF ASSOCIATION PETER G. ARNOLD AND GORDON H. BOWER 2 Stanford University Four experiments replicated

More information

What is Emotion? Emotion is a 4 part process consisting of: physiological arousal cognitive interpretation, subjective feelings behavioral expression.

What is Emotion? Emotion is a 4 part process consisting of: physiological arousal cognitive interpretation, subjective feelings behavioral expression. What is Emotion? Emotion is a 4 part process consisting of: physiological arousal cognitive interpretation, subjective feelings behavioral expression. While our emotions are very different, they all involve

More information

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Michael Romano (mrr2@nyu.edu) Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place New York, NY 1000 USA Abstract Traditional

More information

Test Reactivity: Does the Measurement of Identity Serve as an Impetus for Identity Exploration?

Test Reactivity: Does the Measurement of Identity Serve as an Impetus for Identity Exploration? 86 Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis. JASNH, 2003, Vol. 2, No. 2 Test Reactivity: Does the Measurement of Identity Serve as an Impetus for Identity Exploration? Kristine S. Anthis Southern

More information

Understanding Emotions. How does this man feel in each of these photos?

Understanding Emotions. How does this man feel in each of these photos? Understanding Emotions How does this man feel in each of these photos? Emotions Lecture Overview What are Emotions? Facial displays of emotion Culture-based and sex-based differences Definitions Spend

More information

The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items

The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items Kenn Konstabel Department of Psychology, Estonian Centre of Behavioural and Health Sciences University

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Volume 7, Number 20 Submitted: April 26, 2002 First Revision: August 4, 2002 Second Revision: August 28, 2002 Accepted: August 28, 2002 Publication Date: August 28,

More information

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Mem Cogn (2011) 39:1222 1231 DOI 10.3758/s13421-011-0105-0 Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Dawn M. McBride & Jaclyn K. Beckner & Drew

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

Emotions and Moods. Robbins & Judge Organizational Behavior 13th Edition. Bob Stretch Southwestern College

Emotions and Moods. Robbins & Judge Organizational Behavior 13th Edition. Bob Stretch Southwestern College Robbins & Judge Organizational Behavior 13th Edition Emotions and Moods Bob Stretch Southwestern College 2009 Prentice-Hall Inc. All rights reserved. 8-0 Chapter Learning Objectives After studying this

More information

Gender Differences in Nonverbal, Interpersonal Sensitivity. Across Three Cultures: Japan, India, and the United States

Gender Differences in Nonverbal, Interpersonal Sensitivity. Across Three Cultures: Japan, India, and the United States Gender Differences in Nonverbal, Interpersonal Sensitivity Across Three Cultures: Japan, India, and the United States Prerna Sud Abstract The study examined how, and to what extent, gender and cultural

More information

Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes

Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes Abstract Background: As a consequence of insufficient evidence on the safety and efficacy of e- cigarettes, there has been

More information

Artificial Emotions to Assist Social Coordination in HRI

Artificial Emotions to Assist Social Coordination in HRI Artificial Emotions to Assist Social Coordination in HRI Jekaterina Novikova, Leon Watts Department of Computer Science University of Bath Bath, BA2 7AY United Kingdom j.novikova@bath.ac.uk Abstract. Human-Robot

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

Effects of "Mere Exposure" on Learning and Affect

Effects of Mere Exposure on Learning and Affect Journal ot Personality and Social Psychology 1975, Vol. 31, No. 1, 7-12 Effects of "Mere Exposure" on Learning and Affect David J. Stang Queens College of the City University of New York The mediating

More information

Structural Differences of Physical and Mental Events and Processes

Structural Differences of Physical and Mental Events and Processes Structural Differences of Physical and Mental Events and Processes Xu Xu (XXU@NIU.EDU) Katja Wiemer-Hastings (KATJA@NIU.EDU) Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University, De Kalb, IL 60115 USA

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance 1 Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Erica Behrens Winona State University Abstract This study examines the effects of self-consciousness

More information

Public Speaking Chapter 1. Speaking in Public

Public Speaking Chapter 1. Speaking in Public Public Speaking Chapter 1 Speaking in Public FATE OR OPPORTUNITY? Will you faint, shake, sweat, sway, look down, feel your mouth go dry, or perhaps worse even die? Will you blush, flush, stammer and trip

More information

Sociable Robots Peeping into the Human World

Sociable Robots Peeping into the Human World Sociable Robots Peeping into the Human World An Infant s Advantages Non-hostile environment Actively benevolent, empathic caregiver Co-exists with mature version of self Baby Scheme Physical form can evoke

More information

Group Dynamics and Emotional Expression

Group Dynamics and Emotional Expression Group Dynamics and Emotional Expression The study of emotional expressions has a long tradition in psychology. Although research in this domain has extensively studied the social context factors that influence

More information

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Hidehito Honda (hito@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Kimihiko Yamagishi (kimihiko@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Graduate School of Decision Science

More information

Chapter 5: Research Language. Published Examples of Research Concepts

Chapter 5: Research Language. Published Examples of Research Concepts Chapter 5: Research Language Published Examples of Research Concepts Contents Constructs, Types of Variables, Types of Hypotheses Note Taking and Learning References Constructs, Types of Variables, Types

More information

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution 1 Robert D. Mather University of Central Oklahoma Charles M. Mather University of Science

More information

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1995 The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the

More information

Emotion Theory. Dr. Vijay Kumar

Emotion Theory. Dr. Vijay Kumar Emotion Theory Dr. Vijay Kumar Emotions Just how many emotions are there? Basic Emotions Some have criticized Plutchik s model as applying only to English-speakers Revised model of basic emotions includes:

More information

LIMITATIONS OF COMMUNICATION: MECHANISMS OF IMAGE MAINTENANCE IN FORM OF SELECTIVE PERCEPTION, SELECTIVE MEMORY AND SELECTIVE DISTORTION

LIMITATIONS OF COMMUNICATION: MECHANISMS OF IMAGE MAINTENANCE IN FORM OF SELECTIVE PERCEPTION, SELECTIVE MEMORY AND SELECTIVE DISTORTION LIMITATIONS OF COMMUNICATION: MECHANISMS OF IMAGE MAINTENANCE IN FORM OF SELECTIVE PERCEPTION, SELECTIVE MEMORY AND SELECTIVE DISTORTION HANS SEBALD* Students of human behavior have become aware of the

More information

Judgments of Facial Expressions of Emotion in Profile

Judgments of Facial Expressions of Emotion in Profile Emotion 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 11, No. 5, 1223 1229 1528-3542/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0024356 BRIEF REPORT Judgments of Facial Expressions of Emotion in Profile David Matsumoto

More information

Conversations Without Words: Using Nonverbal Communication to Improve the Patient-Caregiver Relationship

Conversations Without Words: Using Nonverbal Communication to Improve the Patient-Caregiver Relationship Conversations Without Words: Using Nonverbal Communication to Improve the Patient-Caregiver Relationship Judith A. Hall, PhD University Distinguished Professor of Psychology Northeastern University j.hall@neu.edu

More information

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not?

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not? The Effects of Voice Pitch on Attractiveness 1 The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not? Lead Author Katie Leaderbrand Co-Researchers Josh Dekam, and Ashley

More information

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Jean-Pierre Thibaut (jean-pierre.thibaut@univ-poitiers.fr) University of Poitiers, CeRCA, CNRS UMR 634, 99 avenue du recteur Pineau

More information

Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not...

Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not... Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not... Taylor, AJ, Bendall, RCA and Thompson, C Title Authors Type URL Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not... Taylor, AJ, Bendall,

More information

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The spacing and lag effect in free recall The spacing and lag effect in free recall Michael J. Kahana, Bradley R. Wellington & Marc W. Howard Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology Brandeis University Send correspondence to: Michael

More information

An Implicit Assessment of the Effect of Artificial Cheek Raising: When Your Face Smiles, the World Looks Nicer

An Implicit Assessment of the Effect of Artificial Cheek Raising: When Your Face Smiles, the World Looks Nicer To appear in Perceptual and Motor Skills, Vol. 116 (2013) An Implicit Assessment of the Effect of Artificial Cheek Raising: When Your Face Smiles, the World Looks Nicer Hideko MORI Bunka Gakuen Nagano

More information

Inferences: What inferences about the hypotheses and questions can be made based on the results?

Inferences: What inferences about the hypotheses and questions can be made based on the results? QALMRI INSTRUCTIONS QALMRI is an acronym that stands for: Question: (a) What was the broad question being asked by this research project? (b) What was the specific question being asked by this research

More information