OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING INDUCED BY US-UNPREDICTABILITY IN A HUMAN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION PARADIGM

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING INDUCED BY US-UNPREDICTABILITY IN A HUMAN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION PARADIGM"

Transcription

1 psycho.belg.2013_1.book Page 39 Tuesday, March 12, :42 PM Psychologica Belgica 2013, 53/1, DOI: 39 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING INDUCED BY US-UNPREDICTABILITY IN A HUMAN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION PARADIGM Ann Meulders(1), Jelle Mampaey(2), Yannick Boddez(1), Fernando Blanco(3), Debora Vansteenwegen(1), & Frank Baeyens(1)* (1) University of Leuven, (2) University of Hasselt, & (3) Universidad de Deusto, Spain We investigated whether offset-control of the unconditioned stimulus (US) reduces context conditioning induced by US-unpredictability within a human conditioned suppression preparation. We also examined lack of control vs. loss of control. Three groups (No Controllability, NC; Controllability, C; Loss of Controllability, LC) received unsignaled USs during two learning phases (ACQ1-2). The NC group, never had offset-control, whereas the C group, always had offset-control. The LC group, had offset-control during ACQ1, but not during ACQ2. Results indicated that US-unpredictability led to contextual conditioned suppression during ACQ1, only when participants did not have offset-control; when they did, no context conditioning was established. From ACQ1 to ACQ2, contextual conditioned suppression increased in the LC group, but it was not more pronounced than in the NC group. These data suggest that offset-control attenuates context conditioning induced by US-unpredictability and at least in this paradigm loss of control is not worse than lack of control. Introduction In general, the ability to predict important life events allows an organism to prepare adequately for upcoming events and sometimes even control (e.g., escape/avoid or approach) them (Dickinson, 1980; Hollis, 1997). When important life events are unpredictable, however, an organism cannot initiate proper preparatory responses. A large number of animal studies demonstrates that exposure to unpredictable stressors evokes more negative emotional consequences, such as generalised anxiety and enhanced autonomic responding, than exposure to identical predictable stressors (Abbott, Schoen, & Badia, * Ann Meulders, Research Group on Health Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Leuven; Jelle Mampaey, Institute for Behavioural Sciences, University of Hasselt; Yannick Boddez, Debora Vansteenwegen, and Frank Baeyens, Centre for the Psychology of Learning and Experimental Psychopathology, Department of Psychology, University of Leuven; Fernando Blanco, Laboratorio de Psicología del Aprendizaje, Universidad de Deusto, Bilbao, Spain. This research is supported by a University of Leuven grant no. GOA/2001/01. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ann Meulders, Research Group on Health Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, box 3726, 3000 Leuven. ann.meulders@ppw.kuleuven.be

2 40 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING 1984; Mineka, Cook, & Miller, 1984; Rosellini, Warren, & DeCola, 1987). Furthermore, impaired performance after exposure to unpredictable stressors and preference for predictability have been often reported both in animals (Fanselow, 1980) and in humans (Craske, Glover, & DeCola, 1995; Lejuez, Eifert, Zvolensky, & Richards, 2000). Contemporary models of classical conditioning provide a rich theoretical framework to understand the emotional and behavioural/motivational disruption induced by unpredictable stressors. In particular, these effects can be attributed to context conditioning (Fanselow, 1980; Odling-Smee, 1975). In a typical cued fear conditioning experiment, an unconditioned stimulus (US) and a conditioned stimulus (CS) are repeatedly presented together. After a few paired CS-US presentations, the initially neutral CS will start to elicit conditioned fear responding in anticipation of the US. Associative learning models assume that the formation of an excitatory CS-US association underlies such cued fear responses (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972). Likewise, a context in which unpredictable shocks are administered acquires associative strength and therefore evokes more contextual fear and thus chronic (i.e., long-lasting) conditioned fear than a context in which predictable shocks are presented (Balsam & Tomie, 1985; Meulders, Vervliet, Fonteyne, Baeyens, Hermans, & Vansteenwegen, 2012; Vansteenwegen, Iberico, Vervliet, Marescau, & Hermans, 2008). Experimental research devoted to context conditioning in humans is rather scant and is largely restricted to fear conditioning experiments. Important pioneer work by Grillon stimulated the research interest in this topic (Grillon, 2002). In a between-subjects paradigm (Grillon & Davis, 1997), one group of participants received paired presentations of a light-cs and a shock- US (predictable group), whereas another group received explicitly unpaired CS-US presentations (unpredictable group). Basically, results corroborated the animal conditioning findings, that is, the unpredictable experimental context elicited more contextual fear responding compared to the predictable context in an immediate test as well as in a test after a retention interval (Grillon & Davis, 1997). Vansteenwegen and co-workers (Iberico, Vansteenwegen, Vervliet, Dirikx, Marescau, & Hermans, 2008; Vansteenwegen et al., 2008) subsequently replicated and extended these findings in an elegant within-subjects design. Recently, Meulders, Vervliet, Vansteenwegen, Hermans, and Baeyens (2011) replicated the basic findings of Grillon and Vansteenwegen in a human conditioned suppression paradigm and they added an important feature to the procedure, more precisely, a US-only group. A USonly manipulation typically comprises the presentation of temporally unpredictable, unsignaled USs in absence of any predictors for their occurrence or absence. In other words, USs are presented temporally unpredictable and without any cue signalling their occurrence just like in an explicitly unpaired

3 A. MEULDERS et al. 41 manipulation, but also safety signals indicating the non-occurrence of the US are omitted. In human fear conditioning research, unpredictable aversive experiences are commonly modelled by delivering shocks and CSs in an explicitly unpaired manner (Fonteyne, Vervliet, Hermans, Baeyens, & Vansteenwegen, 2010; Grillon, 2002; see Meulders et al., 2012, for an exception). In animal research, however, US-only presentations are typically used to produce US-unpredictability (e.g., Fanselow, 1980). According to the safety signal hypothesis (Seligman & Binik, 1977), the identification of safety signals (present in the unpaired procedure, but not in the US-only procedure) might be able to reduce context conditioning because they offer a short period of relief. In line with this hypothesis, Meulders et al. (2011) showed that the USonly manipulation yielded higher levels of context conditioning as compared with the unpaired manipulation at least in a between-subjects design. Given the empirical evidence that has accumulated to support the notion that US-unpredictability induces context conditioning and a myriad of negative emotional consequences, one might wonder how these emotional disturbances might be alleviated. One possible way to counter the debilitating effects of US-unpredictability might be, to offer the opportunity to terminate the negative effects of the US (i.e., offset-control). Previous research indeed showed that offset-control over a series of unsignaled 20% CO 2 inhalations reduced self-reported anxiety and intense panic experiences in individuals with high levels of suffocation fear (Zvolensky, Lejuez, & Eifert, 1998). Therefore, in the present paper, we focus on the intriguing question of what happens when USs are presented unpredictable (US-only procedure) but their negative consequences can be controlled (offset-control). More specifically, we address the question whether unpredictable USs still produce context conditioning if their adverse consequences can be terminated. Research also indicates that a lack of control over aversive stimuli often results in negative emotional responding evidenced by elevated psychological arousal and catastrophic cognitions (Sanderson, Rapee, & Barlow, 1989). Catastrophic cognitions refer to chronic anticipation and expectations of catastrophic outcomes, that is, continuously having the idea that something bad is going to happen, impeding the ability to relax and feel safe. Loss of control over aversive events is even more detrimental (in terms of elevated autonomic responding, emotional disturbance and impaired performance) than lack of control (Crombez, Eccleston, De Vlieger, Van Damme, & De Clercq, 2008; Zvolensky, Eifert, Lejuez, & McNeil, 1999). Because the few studies on this topic applied biologically potent USs (e.g., painful shock and 20% CO 2 inhalation) our second aim was to explore whether loss of control would also have a larger impact on performance than mere lack of control when biologically non-significant, instructional USs are used.

4 42 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING We predicted that: a) US-unpredictability induces context conditioning in the group without offset-control, but not in the groups having offset-control, and that b) loss of offset-control in the group that previously had control leads to more context conditioning compared with the group that never had any control at all. Furthermore, the response patterns for the group that always had control or never had control are not expected to change during the experiment. Method Participants Thirty undergraduate psychology students (14 males and 16 females, mean age in years = 19, ranging from 18-23) participated in this study in return for course credit. None of them had previous experience with the Martians preparation, and they were all uninformed with respect to the purpose of the experiment. All participants were randomly assigned to one of the three experimental groups: No Controllability (NC) group, Loss of Controllability (LC) group or Controllability (C) group. Apparatus, software, and stimuli The experimental procedure (referred to as the Martians task) was implemented within a flexible Windows 95 environment by Baeyens and Clarysse (1998) using Microsoft Visual C The experiment was run on a Pentium III 730 MHz, 128 Mb RAM multimedia PC (Dell Optiplex GX110); participants responded on the spacebar and the escape button of the computer keyboard. The US consisted of the simultaneous presentation of a 0.5-s white flashing screen (5 flashes at a rate of 10 flashes/s; flash-time = 50 ms, inter-flashtime = 50 ms) accompanied by a metallic sound (Windows TM 95 In the computer program error.wav ). Auditory stimuli were played back in continuous looping and presented in stereo at 85 db through a Typhoon TM Bass Vibration Headset. Following the procedure of Meulders et al. (2011), context was manipulated by changing the background colour of the computer screen. During the pretraining, the baseline measurement, the instructional US phase, and the control instruction phase the background colour remained black, but during the context conditioning phase it was coloured pink.

5 A. MEULDERS et al. 43 Procedure In the present study, we employed the US-only procedure described by Meulders et al. (2011) to establish US-unpredictability in all experimental groups. In order to manipulate offset-control, a control button [1] was introduced as a means to terminate the negative consequences of the US. Conditioned suppression was assessed as an index of context conditioning. Three between-subjects groups were included: the No Controllability (NC), the Controllability (C), and the Loss of Controllability (LC) groups. The experiment consisted of five phases: the pretraining, the baseline assessment, the instructional US phase, offset-control instruction phase and the context conditioning phase. Note that the pretraining phase, the instructional US phase, and offset-control instruction phase served to practice the operant task (i.e., bar-pressing) and to instruct participants about the effects of the US and the control button, whereas the context conditioning phase was the actual learning phase with intermittent test trials after each block. During the first part of the context conditioning phase (ACQ1), unpredictable and uncontrollable USs were delivered in the NC group, whereas in the C and the LC groups USs were unpredictable but their consequences could be terminated. During the second part of the context conditioning phase (ACQ2), loss of control was operationalized by making the use of the control button ineffective in the LC group, whereas the C and the NC groups continued the training that they received during the first part of this phase. Pretraining The purpose of the pretraining phase was to teach participants to continuously press the spacebar of their computer keyboard. Participants were told that Martians were trying to invade Earth and that their landing could be prevented by pressing the spacebar ( shooting a laser gun at each of the Martians that would try to land ). Instructions told that in case of a hit an explosion rather than a Martian would be depicted on the screen. We further emphasised the importance of emitting a regular bar-pressing pattern because a new Martian would appear about every 0.25 s and they just had one single missile per Martian. Martians appeared one by one in rows on the screen, from left to right and from top to bottom, at intervals of 0.25 s, with a space of 0.8 cm in between each Martian (or explosion). If the participant pressed the spacebar before a new Martian was displayed, an explosion appeared at that position. The main 1. Note that the control button was available in all groups during the entire experiment, but that pressing this button was effective only when participants were provided with offsetcontrol.

6 44 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING goal was to have as few Martians and as many explosions as possible on the screen. Only one bar-press per Martian was allowed. If more than one barpress was recorded (that is, if either the participant s bar-pressing rate exceeded 4/s, or she/he held the space bar down), a Martian was displayed. The screen was filled when 70 Martians or explosions (10 in each of 7 rows; inter-row distance was 2 cm) had been presented. At the time the screen was filled, it scrolled up, one line at a time, to make room for new Martians. The pretraining phase lasted for 50 s. At the end of this phase, participants received feedback about the total hit rate and the number of killed and missed Martians. Baseline assessment This phase was identical to the pretraining phase except that it lasted only for 25 s. At the end of this phase, during a 3-s period bar-pressing behaviour was recorded to serve as a baseline test, during which participants did not notice any changes i.e., Martians just kept landing at the same rate and the background colour of the computer screen (i.e., context) remained the same. Instructional US phase In this phase, the instructional US was introduced. Instructions explained that the Martians developed a powerful anti-laser shield (US = white flashing screen accompanied by a metallic sound). If participants continued to fire their laser gun (press the spacebar) when this anti-laser shield is activated, their gun shots would bounce back and an inescapable invasion of Martians would be triggered. An invasion lasted for 20 s and consisted of a white flashing screen accompanied by a complex sound pattern (Windows TM 95 Robotz~2.wav ). Martians now invaded the screen and bar-pressing was ineffective during the invasion (no explosions appeared consequently after bar-pressing). Participants were told that they can avoid invasions, if they cease firing their laser gun during the activation of the anti-laser shield or in other words, if they suppress their bar-pressing behaviour during US presentation. Basically, if no key press was registered during the US, the Martians kept appearing on the computer screen and nothing else happened, but if a response was recorded, the US was followed by an invasion of Martians. During the instructional US phase, four USs were presented and the experimenter explicitly illustrated the impact of bar-pressing during the US. On the first trial, the experimenter refrained from pressing the spacebar before the presentation of the US; at the second trial, he kept pressing the spacebar during the US in order to trigger an invasion; the third trial, the experimenter again ceased bar-pressing during the US and demonstrated that there was no danger of starting to press the spacebar again immediately after

7 A. MEULDERS et al. 45 the US had disappeared from the computer screen; and at the fourth trial, the experimenter demonstrated that it was essential to stop firing before the US was actually presented, by showing the impossibility of avoiding the invasion when stopping firing once the US appeared. Offset-control instruction phase In this phase, a new weapon to terminate invasions was introduced. The newly developed weapon could be activated by pressing the control button (escape button on the keyboard) in case an invasion is triggered. Furthermore, instructions told that this modern weapon is a prototype and that it is not reliably tested yet. It is possible that during the experiment the weapon a) works effectively all the time, b) is never operational, or c) sometimes works and sometimes fails to work effectively. When participants finished reading the instructions, the experimenter showed them the use of this control button. Context conditioning phase This phase was divided in two learning stages (ACQ1 and ACQ2), each comprising four 4-trial blocks (B1-B2 for ACQ1 and B3-4 for ACQ2). All participants received four US-only trials per block in which the USs were scheduled temporally unpredictable (mean ITI = 10 s, range = 7.5 s-12.5 s). As a consequence, participants could not avoid invasions. After each block a context assessment trial (3 s) was run. The three experimental groups were distinguished based on the crucial controllability manipulation: in the No Controllability (NC) group, pressing the control button never terminated the invasion of Martians once it had been triggered, whereas in the Controllability (C) group, participants could always successfully terminate the invasion by using the control button. For the Loss of Controllability (LC) group, pressing the control button reliably ended the invasion during the first part of the context conditioning phase (ACQ1), but during the second part (ACQ2) participants could no longer terminate the invasion by pressing the control button. Response definition: conditioned suppression of bar-pressing behaviour As mentioned before, after each acquisition block (Block 1-8), a context assessment trial was run. During a context assessment trial the total number of bar-presses was recorded for each participant during a 3-s period. Obviously, no USs were presented during the context assessment. Because a Martian was about to land every 0.25 s, the optimal response rate is 12 barpresses/3 s, resulting in the elimination of all Martians and thus providing an indication of optimal operant responding. Following Meulders et al. (2011)

8 46 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING suppression of bar-pressing during these context assessment trials, indicated by lower bar-pressing levels than during the baseline assessment, will be used as an index of context conditioning [2]. Due to US-unpredictability in the present set-up, the US can be expected to occur at any moment. This chronic anticipation of US is thought to result in a more general contextual suppression compared with the suppression of bar-pressing behaviour in response to the CS in the classical Martian task. Based on the fact that the unsignaled USs are actually contingent upon the presence of the experimental background context, this context should gain some associative strength and start to elicit expectancy of the US, resulting in a true conditioned response (i.e., suppression of bar-pressing compared with the baseline). Note for the statistical analyses, means were calculated over two blocks (i.e., eight trials). Descriptive statistics Results Use of the control button We expected that participants would use the control button more frequently when it effectively terminated the negative consequences of the US i.e., the invasion of Martians. Although the control button was only effective in the groups with offset-control, the majority of participants in each group pressed this button on every trial in which an invasion was triggered; 100%, 80% and 70% of the participants, in the C, LC and the NC groups, respectively. More specifically, only three participants in the NC group did not use the control button on every trial with a triggered invasion, respectively on 43%, 15%, and 11% of these trials. Further, two participants in the LC group did not use the control button on every trial with a triggered invasion (always during the training without offset-control, i.e., ACQ2), on 38% and 19% of the trials, respectively. 2. The standard Martian preparation was mostly used to assess conditioned responding to discrete CSs (Baeyens, Vansteenwegen, Beckers, Hermans, Kerkhof, & De Ceulaer, 2005). Consequently, suppression ratios were calculated to analyse bar-pressing data, using the following formula: A/(A+B), with A being the number of bar-presses during the CS and B being the number of bar-presses in absence of the CS. Because the main interest of the present study was not cued (CS) conditioning but contextual conditioned responding, we followed the suggestion of Meulders et al. (2011). As no clear-cut equivalent for B is at hand (the context is omnipresent), they analysed the absolute bar-pressing behaviour and used a pre-treatment baseline measurement as a comparison.

9 A. MEULDERS et al. 47 Triggered invasions Invasions were triggered on at least half of the US-only trials in all experimental groups during the entire experiment, suggesting that participants could not predict the US and avoid the invasions merely on the basis of temporal conditioning. Invasions were triggered on 91%, 88%, and 61% of the trials, respectively in the C, LC and NC group. It seems reasonable that participants without offset-control after a while would elicit less invasions compared to participants with offset-control. Because more context conditioning is established in the groups without control, indexed by more suppression of bar-pressing behaviour, evidently more invasions will be avoided compared with the groups with control. Moreover, we acknowledge that invasions were typically longer in the groups without offset-control since they were not able to terminate them once they were triggered like in the groups with control. Conditioned suppression of bar-pressing behaviour We analysed the mean number of bar-presses during the context assessment trials (3 s) averaged over two conditioning blocks. As a result, the two learning phases (ACQ1 and ACQ2) were each divided in two blocks of eight trials, respectively referred to as B1/B2 and B3/B4. Baseline differences A simple one-way ANOVA was carried out to assure that no baseline differences were present between the respective conditions before the experimental treatment was applied. The analysis showed that baseline responding did not differ between groups, F(2, 27) = 1.31, MSE = 2.63, p =.29. Test of context conditioning: ACQ1 As can be seen in Figure 1, by the end of the first learning phase (B2), barpressing behaviour was more suppressed during the context in the NC group than in the C group and the LC group. A 3 x 3 [Block (Baseline, B1, and B2) x Group (NC, LC, and C)] repeated measures ANOVA was performed on the absolute bar-pressing data. This analysis showed a significant main effect of Group, F(2, 27) = 9.20, MSE = 6.09, p <.001, and a significant main effect of Block, F(2, 54) = 6.44, MSE = 3.82, p <.01. The Block x Group interaction, however, was not significant, F(4, 54) = 1.86, MSE = 3.82, p =.13. Planned comparisons confirmed that bar-pressing behaviour in the NC group became more suppressed from baseline to B2 as compared to the LC group and the C group, F(1, 27) = 4.22, MSE = 5.54, p <.05. At B2, the barpressing behaviour between the LC group and the C group did not differ, F <

10 48 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING 1, whereas more contextual conditioned suppression of bar-pressing behaviour was present in the NC group compared with the C group, F(1, 27) = 8.01, MSE = 7.21, p <.01, as well as compared with the LC group, F(1, 27) = 10.54, MSE = 7.21, p <.01. Next, within-group contrasts calculated from baseline to B2 also were in line with our predictions. In the C group and the LC group, no differences in bar-pressing behaviour occurred from baseline to B2, both F < 1, whereas bar-pressing during the context in the NC group became more suppressed from baseline to B2, F(1, 27) = 9.83, MSE = 5.54, p <.01. Taken together, these data suggest that the experience with unpredictable USs enhances context conditioning as indicated by contextual conditioned suppression of bar-pressing, only when participants are not able to terminate the negative consequences of the US (NC group). In contrast, when participants are able to control these adverse consequences (C and LC group), no context conditioning is established. In brief, offset-control seems to attenuate context conditioning as induced by onset-unpredictability of the US. 14 Mean number of bar-presses during the contexxt (3 s) C LC NC B1 B2 B3 B4 BASELINE ACQ 1 ACQ 2 Figure 1 Mean number of bar-presses (and SE s) during baseline assessment and both parts of the context conditioning phase (ACQ1-2) for the three experimental groups: the Control (C) group, the No Control (NC) group, and the Loss of Control (LC) group. Note that B1, B2, B3, and B4 respectively refer to the mean triggered invasions for Block 1-2, Block 3-4, Block 5-6, and Block 7-8

11 A. MEULDERS et al. 49 Test of context conditioning: ACQ2 As can be seen in Figure 1, bar-pressing levels in the C and in the NC groups remained stable, that is, more contextual conditioned suppression was observed in the NC group than in the C group. Moreover, bar-pressing during the context became more suppressed in the LC group when comparing the Baseline-B2 difference and the Baseline-B4 difference, but this conditioned suppression was still relatively smaller than in the NC group (Baseline-B4 difference). To evaluate the effect of loss of control on context conditioning, we carried out a 3 x 3 [Block (B2, B3, and B4) x Group (NC, LC, and C)] repeated measures ANOVA on the bar-pressing data. This analysis yielded a significant main effect of Group, F(2, 27) = 6.11, MSE = 17.06, p <.01, whereas the main effect of Block and the Block x Group interaction did not turn out significant, respectively, F < 1, and, F(4, 54) = 1.91, MSE = 2.56, p =.12. Although, the data-pattern in Figure 1 seems to suggest that bar-pressing behaviour in the respective experimental groups evolved differently from ACQ1 to ACQ2, this difference was not statistically significant for C group and the LC group, F(1, 27) = 2.58, MSE = 4.08, p =.11, nor for the C group and the NC group, Fs < 1. Bar-pressing behaviour did tend to become more suppressed in the LC group as compared to the NC group from B2 to B4, F(1, 27) = 3.53, MSE = 4.08, p <.07. Pairwise contrasts calculated at B4 showed that no differences were present between the LC group and the C group, F(1, 27) = 1.32, MSE = 9.08, p =.26, and the LC group and the NC group, F(1, 27) = 1.24, MSE = 9.08, p =.28. As expected the difference between the C group and the NC group was still present at the end of ACQ2 (B4), F(1, 27) = 5.12, MSE = 9.08, p <.05. Finally, within-group contrasts from the end of ACQ1 (B2) to the end of ACQ2 (B4) were calculated to evaluate how contextual conditioned suppression of bar-pressing behaviour evolved within each group. As expected, for the C group and the NC group, no differences occurred from B2 to B4, both F < 1. For the LC group, barpressing behaviour during the context tended to become more suppressed from B2 to B4, F(1, 27) = 3.34, MSE = 4.08, p =.07, and compared with baseline, significantly more contextual conditioned suppression was observed at B4, F(1, 27) = 5.53, MSE = 4.18, p <.05. These results provide only minimal evidence for the hypothesis that in the case of US-unpredictability, loss of offset-control is manifested in context conditioning. No evidence was found to support the notion that loss of control is worse than lack of control in terms of performance.

12 50 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING Discussion Unsignaled presentations of unconditioned stimuli are known to produce context conditioning in animals (e.g., Bouton, 1984; Fanselow, 1980) as well as in humans (Fonteyne, Vervliet, Hermans, Baeyens, & Vansteenwegen, 2009; Grillon, 2002; Meulders et al., 2012; Meulders et al., 2011; Vansteenwegen et al., 2008). The present paper addresses the empirical question of whether context conditioning induced by US-unpredictability can be attenuated by providing the opportunity to terminate the negative consequences of the US, that is, by offset-control. Furthermore, we also wanted to explore whether losing control is more debilitating than lacking control per se. In a human conditioned suppression preparation (e.g., Arcediano, Ortega, & Matute, 1996; Meulders et al., 2011), three experimental groups received unsignaled, temporally unpredictable USs. During the context conditioning phase, we manipulated the opportunity to control the offset of the US-consequences in these three conditions. During the entire context conditioning phase, participants in the No Controllability (NC) group never had offset-control, whereas participants in the Controllability group always had offset-control. The Loss of Controllability (LC) group, had offset-control during the first part of the context conditioning phase (ACQ1), but lost it in the second part (ACQ2). Suppression of bar-pressing behaviour compared with the baseline bar-pressing level was assessed to index context conditioning. First and most importantly, results indicated that offset-control (the ability to terminate the negative consequences of the US) reduces context conditioning as induced by US-unpredictability. More specifically during ACQ1, contextual conditioned suppression of bar-pressing was more substantial in the group without offset-control (NC group) than in both groups having offsetcontrol (LC and C groups). These data can be interpreted as follows: in general, two different behavioural responses optimise performance on the experimental task in the present set-up: a) suppression of bar-pressing behaviour in order to avoid invasions (avoidance behaviour), and b) pressing the control button in order to terminate invasions once triggered (escape behaviour). In this experiment, escape behaviour is driven by the operant response-outcome contingency whereas avoidance behaviour is driven by the Pavlovian context- US pairings [3]. A functional analysis of these behaviours demonstrates that 3. This is mainly a procedure-specific argument and is not intended to explain complex and persistent avoidance behaviour in e.g., anxiety disorders. In the Martians task, due to the pairings of the context and the US (temporal US-unpredictability) contextual suppression will develop. Because a negative outcome is avoided due to this behaviour (i.e., a possible invasion) this can be labelled as avoidance behaviour. On the other hand, the contingency between the operant (pushing the control button) and the ending of an ongoing negative outcome (i.e., ending an invasion) can be labelled as escape behaviour.

13 A. MEULDERS et al. 51 for participants in the offset-control groups, the avoidance behaviour has a higher cost than the escape behaviour, since the latter is always effective, but the former is not. In fact, if they use the control button when an invasion is already triggered, it will reliably terminate the invasion. If participants in the offset-control groups, however, suppress their bar-pressing behaviour and no US is presented, Martians will be able to land and therefore this has a relative higher cost in terms of poorer task performance in the specified game context. In contrast, for participants in the group without offset-control, the escape behaviour is not costly but also not effective. Therefore, in order to perform well, they have to engage in the more costly avoidance behaviour. Second, loss of control did not seem to have a more detrimental impact on performance than just lacking control. In particular, although loss of control resulted in more contextual conditioned suppression of bar-pressing from ACQ1 to ACQ2, participants in the Loss of Controllability (LC) group did not have higher levels of contextual conditioned suppression than the group that never had offset-control (NC group) during ACQ2. These data suggest that at least in this paradigm, using a non-biologically significant, instructional US, loss of control is not worse than mere lack of control. These results seem to diverge from those reported with biologically significant USs such as pain (electrocutaneous shock) or panic (20% CO 2 inhalation) (Crombez et al., 2008; Zvolensky et al., 1999). Nonetheless, we do acknowledge that the lack of a statistical significant difference between the lack of control and loss of control groups should be interpreted with caution and claims about the genuine absence of differences at this moment is preliminary. Future research should investigate whether prolonged training within the present set-up might still generate the predicted differences between these conditions. Third, an observation that needs further attention is the fact that even when participants had no offset-control at all, they used the control button frequently and quite persistently. That is, also the Loss of Controllability (LC) group kept pressing the control button as much as the group that never had controllability (NC), although, participants in the former group factually experienced the disconfirmation of offset-control (i.e., the fact that the control button was no longer effective). These results can be tentatively interpreted as an illusion of control effect (Langer, 1975; Matute, 1995; Matute, Vadillo, Vegas, & Blanco, 2007). The persistent use of the control button in the groups without offset-control might suggest that participants overestimate the potential effectiveness of their own behaviour to terminate the invasions and thus demonstrate wrongful perceived control beliefs (possibly due to delayed operant trace conditioning). Alternatively, it might be that participants acted following the adage better safe than sorry. As no additional punishment followed if the control button was pressed when it was ineffec-

14 52 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING tive, the cost of the escape behaviour (i.e., pressing the control button) always remained relatively low. It is also possible that participants without offsetcontrol kept trying to use the control button because they did not fully trust its (non-)functioning i.e., a lack of inhibitory learning of the response-no outcome contingency. Future research may address the possible mechanisms underlying this peculiar observation. Fourth, the implications of these findings for the clinical practice should be considered as well. To date, several procedures to reduce contextual anxiety (i.e., context conditioning) have been advanced, such as repeated nonreinforced context exposure (Fonteyne et al., 2010) and signalling procedures (i.e., offering predictive cues in an unpredictable context) (Fonteyne et al., 2009). The latter procedure dovetails with the psychotherapeutic treatment protocols for panic disorder (PD), that is, perceived unpredictable panic attacks are put under control of internal (bodily) cues that occur before a typical full-blown panic attack (Craske & Barlow, 2008; Craske et al., 1995). Next to enhancing predictability of aversive events, eliminating the negative consequences of aversive events (offset-control) might be a pathway to reduce contextual anxiety and emotional distress. For example, asthma attacks are also often perceived as unpredictable due to inaccurate perception of asthma triggers (Janssens, Verleden, De Peuter, Van Diest, & Van den Bergh, 2009), leading to high comorbidity with PD (Lehrer, Karavidas, Lu, Feldman, Kranitz, Abraham et al., 2008) and poor asthma control (Put, Van den Bergh, Lemaigre, Demedts, & Verleden, 2003). Hence, from this point of view, providing offset-control over the negative effects of an asthma attack, for example by the use of an asthma inhaler, might reduce chronic anxiety and thus might reduce chronic apprehension and worrying about future unpredictable asthma attacks. Future research might further disentangle the benefits of offset-control vs. prediction in the treatment of clinical anxiety. Finally, a limitation that is intrinsically entangled with the offset-control manipulation in the present set-up is that although the number of presentations of the primary (unpredictable) US (i.e., activation of the anti-laser screen) was kept constant across the experimental groups, the number and length of the invasions (i.e., the consequences of the US) was not optimally controlled for. Illusion of control (e.g., Matute, 1994; Matute, 1995) as well as learned helplessness paradigms (e.g., Maier & Seligman, 1976; Seligman & Beagley, 1975) typically use yoked designs to overcome this problem. Therefore, further research should consider using such yoked designs. Notwithstanding this limitation, the results clearly demonstrate that the controllability procedure attenuates context conditioning (effect). For example, from a clinical perspective, it would be interesting to have a way to reduce context conditioning i.e., to prevent spreading of chronic fear in an asthma patient by providing offset-control over unpredictable asthma attacks (e.g., through

15 A. MEULDERS et al. 53 medication), and that irrespective of the possible mechanisms underlying this effect. Another limitation might involve the context manipulation in the present set-up. Context typically refers to long-lasting cues as compared to discrete cues (CSs) with a clear on- and offset. Formulating a strict definition of context, however, is difficult and continuously under debate (e.g., Balsam & Tomie, 1985). A definition of context might be based on its perceptual characteristics (e.g., complexity, foreground/background), spatial characteristics (e.g., three-dimensionality), temporal characteristics (e.g., longer duration, see Grillon, 2002), and/or by its functional characteristics (e.g., modulatory properties, see Bouton, 2002). We believe that the contexts used in this experimental set-up (i.e., the background colour of the computer screen) comprise some of these features, such as background-foreground differentiation and the longer lasting duration of the context presentation than the presentation of the cues (i.e., US). However, the contexts used in our study contain no strict modulatory functions and are not three-dimensional. Therefore, the contexts used in this study might lack some ecological validity. However, changing the background colour of the computer screen has been used before to manipulate contexts in the Martians task (e.g., renewal studies, Havermans, Keuker, Lataster, & Jansen, 2005). The background colour of the computer screen seems a highly task relevant contextual feature since it is embedded in the broader game context of the Martians task. To recapitulate, the present study provided evidence for the notion that offset-control reduces context conditioning as induced by exposure to unpredictable USs. Contrary to previous research with biologically potent USs, loss of control did not turn out to have worse effects on performance than mere lack of control. References Abbott, B.B., Schoen, L.S., & Badia, P. (1984). Predictable and unpredictable shock: Behavioral measures of aversion and physiological measures of stress. Psychological Bulletin, 96, Arcediano, F., Ortega, N., & Matute, H. (1996). A behavioural preparation for the study of human Pavlovian conditioning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, Baeyens, F., & Clarysse, J. (1998). Martians for Windows 95 [Computer Program], Leuven: University of Leuven. Baeyens, F., Vansteenwegen, D., Beckers, T., Hermans, D., Kerkhof, I., & De Ceulaer, A. (2005). Extinction and renewal of Pavlovian modulation in human sequential feature positive discrimination learning. Learning & Memory, 12, Balsam, P.D., & Tomie, A. (1985). Context and learning. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

16 54 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING Bouton, M.E. (1984). Differential control by context in the inflation and reinstatement paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, Bouton, M.E. (2002). Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry, 52, Craske, M.G., & Barlow, D.H. (2008). Panic disorder and agoraphobia. In D.H. Barlow (Ed.), Clinical handbook of psychological disorders (pp. 1-64). New York: The Guilford Press. Craske, M.G., Glover, D., & DeCola, J. (1995). Predicted versus unpredicted panic attacks: Acute versus general distress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., De Vlieger, P., Van Damme, S., & De Clercq, A. (2008). Is it better to have controlled and lost than never to have controlled at all? An experimental investigation of control over pain. Pain, 137, Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Fanselow, M.S. (1980). Signaled shock-free periods and preference for signaled shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal behavior Processes, 6, Fonteyne, R., Vervliet, B., Hermans, D., Baeyens, F., & Vansteenwegen, D. (2009). Reducing chronic anxiety by making the threatening event predictable: An experimental approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, Fonteyne, R., Vervliet, B., Hermans, D., Baeyens, F., & Vansteenwegen, D. (2010). Exposure to the context and removing the unpredictability of the US: Two methods to reduce contextual anxiety compared. Biological Psychology, 85, Grillon, C. (2002). Startle reactivity and anxiety disorders: aversive conditioning, context, and neurobiology. Biological Psychiatry, 52, Grillon, C., & Davis, M. (1997). Fear-potentiated startle conditioning in humans: Explicit and contextual cue conditioning following paired versus unpaired training. Psychophysiology, 34, Havermans, R.C., Keuker, J., Lataster, T., & Jansen, A. (2005). Contextual control of extinguished conditioned performance in humans. Learning and Motivation, 36, Hollis, K.L. (1997). Contemporary research on Pavlovian conditioning. The American Psychologist, 52, Iberico, C., Vansteenwegen, D., Vervliet, B., Dirikx, T., Marescau, V., & Hermans, D. (2008). The development of cued versus contextual conditioning in a predictable and an unpredictable human fear conditioning preparation. Acta Psychologica, 127, Janssens, T., Verleden, G., De Peuter, S., Van Diest, I., & Van den Bergh, O. (2009). Inaccurate perception of asthma symptoms: A cognitive-affective framework and implications for asthma treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, Langer, E.J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32,

17 A. MEULDERS et al. 55 Lehrer, P.M., Karavidas, M.K., Lu, S.E., Feldman, J., Kranitz, L., Abraham, S. et al. (2008). Psychological treatment of comorbid asthma and panic disorder: A pilot study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, Lejuez, C.W., Eifert, G.H., Zvolensky, M.J., & Richards, J.B. (2000). Preference between onset predictable and unpredictable administrations of 20% carbondioxide-enriched air: Implications for better understanding the etiology and treatment of panic disorder. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, Maier, S.F., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1976). Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, Matute, H. (1994). Learned helplessness and superstitious behavior as opposite effects of uncontrollable reinforcements in humans. Learning and Motivation, 25, Matute, H. (1995). Human reactions to uncontrollable outcomes: Further evidence for superstitions rather than helplessness. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, Matute, H., Vadillo, M.A., Vegas, S., & Blanco, F. (2007). Illusion of control in internet users and college students. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10, Meulders, A., Vervliet, B., Fonteyne, R., Baeyens, F., Hermans, D., & Vansteenwegen, D. (2012). Preexposure to (un)predictable shock modulates discriminative fear learning between cue and context: An investigation of the interaction between fear and anxiety. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 84, Meulders, A., Vervliet, B., Vansteenwegen, D., Hermans, D., & Baeyens, F. (2011). A new tool for assessing context conditioning induced by US-unpredictability in humans: The Martians task restyled. Learning and Motivation, 42, Mineka, S., Cook, M., & Miller, S. (1984). Fear conditioned with escapable and inescapable shock: Effects of a feedback stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 10, Odling-Smee, F.J. (1975). The role of background stimuli in Pavlovian conditioning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, Put, C., Van den Bergh, O., Lemaigre, V., Demedts, M., & Verleden, G. (2003). Evaluation of an individualised asthma programme directed at behavioural change. European Respiratory Journal, 21, Rescorla, R.A., & Wagner, A.R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A.H. Black & W.F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II (pp ). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Rosellini, R.A., Warren, D., & DeCola, J. (1987). Predictability and controllability: Differential effects upon contextual fear. Learning and Motivation, 18, Sanderson, W.C., Rapee, R.M., & Barlow, D.H. (1989). The influence of an illusion of control on panic attacks induced via inhalation of 5.5% carbon dioxideenriched air. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, Seligman, M.E.P., & Beagley, G. (1975). Learned helplessness in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88,

18 56 OFFSET-CONTROL ATTENUATES CONTEXT CONDITIONING Seligman, M., & Binik, Y. (1977). The safety signal hypothesis. In H. Davis & H. Hurwiwtz (Eds.), Operant Pavlovian interactions (pp ). Hillsdale: NJ: Erlbaum. Vansteenwegen, D., Iberico, C., Vervliet, B., Marescau, V., & Hermans, D. (2008). Contextual fear induced by unpredictability in a human fear conditioning preparation is related to the chronic expectation of a threatening US. Biological Psychology, 77, Zvolensky, M.J., Eifert, G.H., Lejuez, C.W., & McNeil, D.W. (1999). The effects of offset control over 20% carbon dioxide-enriched air on anxious responding. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, Zvolensky, M.J., Lejuez, C.W., & Eifert, G.H. (1998). The role of offset control in anxious responding: An experimental test using repeated administrations of 20% CO2-enriched air. Behavior Therapy, 29, Received February 27, 2012 Revision received June 1, 2012 Accepted June 8, 2012

The resistance of renewal to instructions that devalue the role of contextual cues in a conditioned suppression task with humans

The resistance of renewal to instructions that devalue the role of contextual cues in a conditioned suppression task with humans The resistance of renewal to instructions that devalue the role of contextual cues in a conditioned suppression task with humans Author Neumann, David Published 2007 Journal Title Learning and Motivation

More information

Stimulus competition in the absence of compound conditioning

Stimulus competition in the absence of compound conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (1), 3-14 Stimulus competition in the absence of compound conditioning HELENA MATUTE and OSKAR PINEÑO Universidad de Deusto, Bilbao, Spain Most associative theories

More information

Evaluative conditioning: a review and a model

Evaluative conditioning: a review and a model Evaluative conditioning: a review and a model Remco C. Havermans and Anita Jansen Evaluative conditioning refers to the transfer of affective value to an initially neutral stimulus by pairing the neutral

More information

Context and Pavlovian conditioning

Context and Pavlovian conditioning Context Brazilian conditioning Journal of Medical and Biological Research (1996) 29: 149-173 ISSN 0100-879X 149 Context and Pavlovian conditioning Departamento de Psicologia, Pontifícia Universidade Católica

More information

A Unique Safety Signal: Social-Support Figures Enhance Rather Than Protect From Fear Extinction

A Unique Safety Signal: Social-Support Figures Enhance Rather Than Protect From Fear Extinction 743002CPXXXX10.1177/2167702617743002Hornstein et al.social-support Figures Enhance Fear Extinction research-article2017 Brief Empirical Report A Unique Safety Signal: Social-Support Figures Enhance Rather

More information

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet University of Amsterdam Supplementary Figures and Legends Day 1 Day 2 Day 3

More information

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation.

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation. Unit 06 - Overview How We Learn and Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Operant Conditioning s Applications, and Comparison to Classical Conditioning Biology, Cognition, and Learning Learning By

More information

Conducting extinction in multiple contexts does not necessarily attenuate the renewal of shock expectancy in a fear-conditioning procedure with humans

Conducting extinction in multiple contexts does not necessarily attenuate the renewal of shock expectancy in a fear-conditioning procedure with humans Conducting extinction in multiple contexts does not necessarily attenuate the renewal of shock expectancy in a fear-conditioning procedure with humans Author Neumann, David, V. Lipp, Ottmar, E. Cory, Storm

More information

Abundant research has been devoted to understanding anxiety

Abundant research has been devoted to understanding anxiety Fear Conditioning in Virtual Reality Contexts: A New Tool for the Study of Anxiety Johanna M. Baas, Monique Nugent, Shmuel Lissek, Daniel S. Pine, and Christian Grillon Background: Context conditioning

More information

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ISSN: 1747-0218 (Print) 1747-0226 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20 Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive

More information

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms Q0667 QJEP(B) si-b03/read as keyed THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 56B (1), 43 55 Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative

More information

Chapter 6. Learning: The Behavioral Perspective

Chapter 6. Learning: The Behavioral Perspective Chapter 6 Learning: The Behavioral Perspective 1 Can someone have an asthma attack without any particles in the air to trigger it? Can an addict die of a heroin overdose even if they ve taken the same

More information

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events.

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. behaviorism - the view that psychology (1) should be an objective science

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback acquisition associative learning in classical conditioning the initial stage when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery L132 NT MJA/cla Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 60-73 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.60 Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery Gonzalo P. Urcelay University of Cambridge, Cambridge,

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. What is Learning? Learning is the process that allows us to adapt (be flexible) to the changing conditions

More information

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Chapter 5 Study Guide Chapter 5 Study Guide Practice Exam Questions: Which of the following is not included in the definition of learning? It is demonstrated immediately Assuming you have eaten sour pickles before, imagine

More information

Panic Control Therapy (PCT) 1 Barlow model

Panic Control Therapy (PCT) 1 Barlow model Problem-specific competences describe the knowledge and skills needed when applying CBT principles to specific conditions. They are not a stand-alone description of competences, and should be read as part

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010 Notes Textbook used Week 1-3: Bouton, M.E. (2016). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. 2nd Ed. Sinauer Week 4-6: Rieger, E. (Ed.) (2014) Abnormal Psychology:

More information

Biological Psychology

Biological Psychology Biological Psychology 92 (2013) 51 58 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Biological Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Extinction, generalization, and return

More information

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Jennifer A. Kaminski (kaminski.16@osu.edu) Center for Cognitive Science, Ohio State University 10A

More information

Recency and primacy in causal judgments: Effects of probe question and context switch on latent inhibition and extinction

Recency and primacy in causal judgments: Effects of probe question and context switch on latent inhibition and extinction Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (6), 1087-1093 doi: 10.3758/MC.36.6.1087 Recency and primacy in causal judgments: Effects of probe question and context switch on latent inhibition and extinction STEVEN GLAUTIER

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Basic characteristics

Basic characteristics Learning Basic characteristics The belief that the universe is lawful and orderly The occurrence of phenomena as a function of the operation of specific variables Objective observation Controlled experiments

More information

Generalization of Fear Effects in Reinstatement to a Discrete Stimulus

Generalization of Fear Effects in Reinstatement to a Discrete Stimulus University of South Carolina Scholar Commons USC Aiken Psychology Theses Psychology Department 8-2014 Generalization of Fear Effects in Reinstatement to a Discrete Stimulus Lauren Best University of South

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events acquisition associative learning In classical conditioning, the initial stage, when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

Learning and Anxiety 1

Learning and Anxiety 1 Learning and Anxiety 1 Childhood Learning Experiences in the Development and Maintenance of Anxiety Disorders Margo C. Watt a,b & Samantha DiFrancescantonio a Submitted to Strides October 2010 a Department

More information

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives Theoretical Perspectives REBT (1955) Albert Ellis Action & Result Oriented Teaches how to identify self-defeating thoughts Replaces thoughts w/ life enhancing ones 1 A B C s of personality formation: A

More information

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn?

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Defining Learning A relatively permanent change in behavior or the potential for behavior that results from experience Results from many life experiences, not just structured

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

The renewal of extinguished conditioned fear with fearrelevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli by a context change after extinction

The renewal of extinguished conditioned fear with fearrelevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli by a context change after extinction The renewal of extinguished conditioned fear with fearrelevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli by a context change after extinction Author Neumann, David, Longbottom, Paula Published 2008 Journal Title Behaviour

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning Operant Conditioning The nature of reinforcement: Premack s probability differential theory Response deprivation

More information

Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning

Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning Paulo F. Carvalho (pcarvalh@indiana.edu) Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University 1101 East Tenth Street Bloomington,

More information

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher.

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher. 1. A stimulus change that increases the future frequency of behavior that immediately precedes it. 2. In operant conditioning, a reinforcement schedule that reinforces a response only after a specified

More information

The relevance of recent developments in classical conditioning to understanding the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders

The relevance of recent developments in classical conditioning to understanding the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Acta Psychologica 127 (2008) 567 580 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The relevance of recent developments in classical conditioning to understanding the etiology

More information

Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning i

Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning i Gallistel & Gibbon Page 1 In press Current Directions in Psychological Science Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning i C. R. Gallistel University of California, Los Angeles John

More information

I. Classical Conditioning

I. Classical Conditioning Learning Chapter 8 Learning A relatively permanent change in an organism that occur because of prior experience Psychologists must study overt behavior or physical changes to study learning Learning I.

More information

Generalization of Human Fear Acquisition and Extinction within a Novel Arbitrary Stimulus Category

Generalization of Human Fear Acquisition and Extinction within a Novel Arbitrary Stimulus Category Generalization of Human Fear Acquisition and Extinction within a Novel Arbitrary Stimulus Category Ellen Vervoort*, Bram Vervliet, Marc Bennett, Frank Baeyens Centre for the Psychology of Learning and

More information

Learning Theories. Dr. Howie Fine INTRODUCTION. Learning is one of the most researched and discussed area in Psychology.

Learning Theories. Dr. Howie Fine INTRODUCTION. Learning is one of the most researched and discussed area in Psychology. Learning Theories Dr. Howie Fine 1 INTRODUCTION Learning is one of the most researched and discussed area in Psychology. Learning What? Vs. How? Laymen view learning generally in terms of what is being

More information

Developmental Psychology and Learning SOCIAL EDUCATION COURSE Academic year 2014/2015

Developmental Psychology and Learning SOCIAL EDUCATION COURSE Academic year 2014/2015 Developmental Psychology and Learning SOCIAL EDUCATION COURSE Academic year 2014/2015 EXTENDED SUMMARY Lesson #6 Monday, Oct. 27 th 2014: 19:00/21:00 Friday, Oct. 31 th 2014, 09:00/11:00 LESSON PLAN: HUMAN

More information

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli. BACB 4 th Edition Task List s Content Area E: Specific Behavior-Change Procedures E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Flexible Use of Recent Information in Causal and Predictive Judgments

Flexible Use of Recent Information in Causal and Predictive Judgments Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2002, Vol. 28, No. 4, 714 725 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.4.714

More information

Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online

Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online The use of an unpleasant sound as the unconditional stimulus in aversive Pavlovian conditioning experiments that involve children and adolescent participants Author Neumann, David, Waters, Allison, Westbury

More information

CATS IN SHORTS. Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions. Holger Ursin March Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen.

CATS IN SHORTS. Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions. Holger Ursin March Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen. CATS IN SHORTS Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions Holger Ursin March 2009 Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen Alarm Activation Load Stress Response Stress Stressor Stimuli Stimulus expectancy

More information

The Mechanics of Associative Change

The Mechanics of Associative Change The Mechanics of Associative Change M.E. Le Pelley (mel22@hermes.cam.ac.uk) I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Site Cambridge CB2 3EB, England Abstract

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction 1 2 3 4 5 Myers Psychology for AP* Unit 6: Learning Unit Overview How Do We Learn? Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Learning by Observation How Do We Learn? Introduction Learning Habituation

More information

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1 The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Lecture 5.1 David S. Touretzky Based on notes by Lisa M. Saksida November, 2015 Outline Classical and instrumental conditioning The Rescorla

More information

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (2), 213-219 Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning THOMAS HIGGINS and ROBERT A. RESCORLA University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) Classical vs. Instrumental The modern view is that these two types of learning involve similar

More information

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Chapter 6/9: Learning Chapter 6/9: Learning Learning A relatively durable change in behavior or knowledge that is due to experience. The acquisition of knowledge, skills, and behavior through reinforcement, modeling and natural

More information

an ability that has been acquired by training (process) acquisition aversive conditioning behavior modification biological preparedness

an ability that has been acquired by training (process) acquisition aversive conditioning behavior modification biological preparedness acquisition an ability that has been acquired by training (process) aversive conditioning A type of counterconditioning that associates an unpleasant state (such as nausea) with an unwanted behavior (such

More information

The contribution of ABA and psychiatry to the emerging model of positive behaviour support

The contribution of ABA and psychiatry to the emerging model of positive behaviour support The contribution of ABA and psychiatry to the emerging model of positive behaviour support Royal College of Psychiatrists Faculty of Intellectual Disability Psychiatry Annual Conference 2016 Dr Ashok Roy

More information

Signals of Reward and Non Reward

Signals of Reward and Non Reward Signals of Reward and Non Reward Signals of reward and non reward predict: Pleasure or disappointment Success or failure 1 Reward Training By definition, this means that the animal carries out a response/action

More information

City Research Online. Permanent City Research Online URL:

City Research Online. Permanent City Research Online URL: Mondragon, E., Alonso, E., Fernandez, A. & Gray, J. (01). An extension of the Rescorla and Wagner Simulator for context conditioning. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 1(), pp. -0. doi:.1/j.cmpb.01.01.01

More information

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/85/$00.75 1985, VOl. 11, No. 4, 548-564 Predictive Accuracy and the

More information

Short article Backward blocking: The role of within-compound associations and interference between cues trained apart

Short article Backward blocking: The role of within-compound associations and interference between cues trained apart THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2008, 61 (2), 185 193 Short article Backward blocking: The role of within-compound associations and interference between cues trained apart Miguel A. Vadillo

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

Outline. History of Learning Theory. Pavlov s Experiment: Step 1. Associative learning 9/26/2012. Nature or Nurture

Outline. History of Learning Theory. Pavlov s Experiment: Step 1. Associative learning 9/26/2012. Nature or Nurture Outline What is learning? Associative Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning History of Learning Theory Nature or Nurture BEHAVIORISM Tabula Rasa Learning: Systematic,

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Initial Tests of the Learned Helplessness Explanation Innoculation against helplessness If dogs are given escapable pre-preshock, then subsequent expo

Initial Tests of the Learned Helplessness Explanation Innoculation against helplessness If dogs are given escapable pre-preshock, then subsequent expo Aversive Conditioning Learned Helplessness Punishment Learned Helplessness Dogs were exposed to inescapable shocks delivered to the paw via an electrode. When given a chance to learn to escape from or

More information

Psychology in Your Life

Psychology in Your Life Sarah Grison Todd Heatherton Michael Gazzaniga Psychology in Your Life FIRST EDITION Chapter 6 Learning 2014 W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. Section 6.1 How Do the Parts of Our Brains Function? 6.1 What Are

More information

Mini symposium on pain and suffering

Mini symposium on pain and suffering Mini symposium on pain and suffering in the honor of Prof. Dr. Geert Crombez (UGent) Francqui Chair* 2010 2011 Leuven, May 13 th 2011 10:00 10:30 Welcome and short introduction 10:30 11:00 Presentation

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION Except where reference is made to the works of others, the work described in this dissertation is my own or was done in collaboration

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Classical Conditioning Learning through Association Ivan Pavlov discovered the form of learning called Classical

More information

Associative Learning

Associative Learning Learning Learning Associative Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Biological Components of Learning Cognitive Components of Learning Behavioral Therapies Associative

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

Goal: To identify the extent to which learning and cognition might be important in the development of different forms of psychopathology

Goal: To identify the extent to which learning and cognition might be important in the development of different forms of psychopathology Key Dates TU Apr 18 Unit 24 TH Apr 20 Unit 25; Psychological Perspective Assignment in class TU Apr 25 Begin Social Perspectives, Unit IIIC and 26 TH Apr 27 (last class) Unit 27; Social Perspective Assignment

More information

THANK YOU! Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) 1/30/2015

THANK YOU! Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) 1/30/2015 THANK YOU! Fear Potentiated Startle and Fear Extinction in a Sample of Undergraduate Women Exposed to a Campus Mass Shooting Susan M. Hannan, M.A. Antonia V. Seligowski, M.A. Holly K. Orcutt, Ph.D. Our

More information

Looking out for danger: An attentional bias towards spatially predictable. threatening stimuli.

Looking out for danger: An attentional bias towards spatially predictable. threatening stimuli. 1 Looking out for danger: An attentional bias towards spatially predictable threatening stimuli. Lies Notebaert*, Geert Crombez, and Stefaan Van Damme, Jan De Houwer Ghent University, Belgium Jan Theeuwes

More information

Flavor evaluative conditioning and contingency awareness

Flavor evaluative conditioning and contingency awareness Learning & Behavior 7, 35 (), 33-1 Flavor evaluative conditioning and contingency awareness SUSAN G. WARDLE, CHRIS J. MITCHELL, AND PETER F. LOVIBOND University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales,

More information

GCSE PSYCHOLOGY UNIT 2 LEARNING REVISION

GCSE PSYCHOLOGY UNIT 2 LEARNING REVISION GCSE PSYCHOLOGY UNIT 2 LEARNING REVISION GCSE PSYCHOLOGY UNIT 2 CLASSICAL CONDITIONING LEARNING LEARNING= Is a relatively permanent change in behaviour due to experience Some behaviours are learnt, but

More information

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0039-1 Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Cody W. Polack & Mario A. Laborda & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 July 2011 # Psychonomic

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Stephen E. Brock, Ph.D., NCSP 1

Stephen E. Brock, Ph.D., NCSP 1 EDS 245: Stephen E. Brock, Ph.D., NCSP California State University, Sacramento Behavioral Interventions Behavioral Consultation and Intervention: A Credentialing Standard Standards for Graduate Preparation

More information

Chapter 6: Learning The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

Chapter 6: Learning The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Chapter 6: Learning Learning A relatively permanent change in behavior brought about by experience Distinguishes between changes due to maturation and changes brought about by experience Distinguishes

More information

Chapter Six. Learning. Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning

Chapter Six. Learning. Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Chapter Six Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Part One: Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning is conditioning by association, such as a pet getting excited

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 10 A Synthetic Perspective on Instrumental Learning

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 10 A Synthetic Perspective on Instrumental Learning PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 10 A Synthetic Perspective on Instrumental Learning Avoidance Learning Avoidance learning is puzzling because the reinforcer is not obvious. What sustains running to

More information

STUDY GUIDE ANSWERS 6: Learning Introduction and How Do We Learn? Operant Conditioning Classical Conditioning

STUDY GUIDE ANSWERS 6: Learning Introduction and How Do We Learn? Operant Conditioning Classical Conditioning STUDY GUIDE ANSWERS 6: Learning Introduction and How Do We Learn? 1. learning 2. associate; associations; associative learning; habituates 3. classical 4. operant 5. observing Classical Conditioning 1.

More information

Computers in Human Behavior

Computers in Human Behavior Computers in Human Behavior 29 (2013) 552 558 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Computers in Human Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh Conditioned suppression

More information

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE 이자료는이바로바스교수의응용행동수정강의를리차드손임상심리학박사가요약해서 정리한것입니다. Lovaas Method Philosophy Children stay with family at home If not working (no positive changes

More information

Representation and Generalisation in Associative Systems

Representation and Generalisation in Associative Systems Representation and Generalisation in Associative Systems M.E. Le Pelley (mel22@hermes.cam.ac.uk) I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Site Cambridge CB2 3EB,

More information

A PROCESS MODEL OF TRUST IN AUTOMATION: A SIGNAL DETECTION THEORY BASED APPROACH

A PROCESS MODEL OF TRUST IN AUTOMATION: A SIGNAL DETECTION THEORY BASED APPROACH A PROCESS MODEL OF TRUST IN AUTOMATION: A SIGNAL DETECTION THEORY BASED APPROACH Jorge Zuniga 1, Malcolm McCurry 2, J. Gregory Trafton 3 George Mason University 1 Fairfax, VA Exelis, Inc 2 Alexandria,

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Proactive Interference in Human Predictive Learning

Proactive Interference in Human Predictive Learning International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 2002, 15, 55-68. Copyright 2002 by the International Society for Comparative Psychology Proactive Interference in Human Predictive Learning Leyre Castro,

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Chapter Title Attention and Pavlovian Conditioning Copyright Year 2011 Copyright Holder Springer Science+Business Media, LLC Corresponding Author

More information

Higher-order retrospective revaluation in human causal learning

Higher-order retrospective revaluation in human causal learning THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (2), 137 151 Higher-order retrospective revaluation in human causal learning Jan De Houwer University of Southampton, Southampton, UK Tom Beckers

More information

2. Hull s theory of learning is represented in a mathematical equation and includes expectancy as an important variable.

2. Hull s theory of learning is represented in a mathematical equation and includes expectancy as an important variable. True/False 1. S-R theories of learning in general assume that learning takes place more or less automatically, and do not require and thought by humans or nonhumans. ANS: T REF: P.18 2. Hull s theory of

More information

Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans

Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans Research Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans Daniela Schiller, 1,2,3 Christopher K. Cain, 2,3 Nina G. Curley, 1 Jennifer S. Schwartz, 1 Sarah A. Stern, 2 Joseph

More information

Myers PSYCHOLOGY. (7th Ed) Chapter 8. Learning. James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University. Worth Publishers

Myers PSYCHOLOGY. (7th Ed) Chapter 8. Learning. James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University. Worth Publishers Myers PSYCHOLOGY (7th Ed) Chapter 8 Learning James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University Worth Publishers Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience Association

More information

Panic Stations Module 2

Panic Stations Module 2 PANIC STATIONS Panic Stations Module 2 More about Panic Introduction 2 How do panic attacks happen? 2 How does panic disorder develop? 3 How is panic disorder maintained? 4 What can be done about panic

More information