Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory"

Transcription

1 Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.30 No.7 INMED/TINS special issue Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory Ole Jensen 1*, Jochen Kaiser 2* and Jean-Philippe Lachaux 3* 1 F.C. Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Kapittelweg 29, 6525 EN Nijmegen, the Netherlands 2 Institute of Medical Psychology, Johann Wolfgang Goethe-Universität, Heinrich-Hoffmann-Str. 10, Frankfurt am Main, Germany 3 INSERM, Unite 821, 95 Boulevard Pinel, BRON CEDEX, France Both theoretical and experimental animal work supports the hypothesis that transient oscillatory synchronization of neuronal assemblies at gamma frequencies ( Hz) is closely associated with sensory processing. Recent data from recordings in animals and humans have suggested that gamma-frequency activity also has an important role in attention and both working and long-term memory. The involvement of gammaband synchronization in various cognitive paradigms in humans is currently being investigated using intracranial and high-density electro- and magnetoencephalography recordings. Here, we discuss recent findings demonstrating human gamma-frequency activity associated with attention and memory in both sensory and non-sensory areas. Because oscillatory gamma-frequency activity has an important role in neuronal communication and synaptic plasticity, it could provide a key for understanding neuronal processing in both local and distributed cortical networks engaged in complex cognitive functions. This review is part of the INMED/TINS special issue Physiogenic and pathogenic oscillations: the beauty and the beast, based on presentations at the annual INMED/TINS symposium ( Introduction In recent years, fast cortical oscillatory activity in the gamma-frequency band ( Hz), as recorded in humans using electro- or magnetoencephalography (EEG or MEG, respectively), has received increasing interest because of its association with various cognitive functions in healthy humans [1 3] and a possible role in psychiatric and neurological dysfunctions [4,5]. Although the coordinated activity of large numbers of neurons is required to produce oscillatory gamma-frequency activity in EEG and MEG recordings, its functional role remains unclear. The current understanding of fast oscillations is that they reflect synchronous activity of large ensembles of rhythmically firing neurons, which can be observed at multiple spatial scales, from single-unit recordings to the scalp EEG (Box 1). Corresponding author: Jensen, O. (ole.jensen@fcdonders.ru.nl). * All authors contributed equally to this work. Available online 17 May From a physiological perspective, there are strong arguments for synchronization in the gamma-frequency band being important for neuronal communication. If a neuron receives input from several other neurons, this drive is enhanced if the spiking inputs are coincident because the synchronization enables the postsynaptic potentials to integrate [6] (Box 2, Figure Ia). The duration of an excitatory postsynaptic potential (PSP) is approximately t PSP 10 ms [7], which defines the timeframe of temporal integration. Thus, gamma-frequency oscillations with a duration of ms will provide a tighter synchronization than will oscillations in lower frequency bands (Box 2, Figure Ia). Because this principle enables a group of neurons to exert a stronger drive when synchronized on downstream networks, it has been proposed that long-distance neuronal communication is reflected by phase synchronization [6,8 10]. The hypothesis that synchronous firing of a group of cells increases their functional impact has been the cornerstone of various theories of the role of oscillatory gammafrequency activity. For instance, the binding problem addresses the physiological mechanisms responsible for combining different features in a visual scene to form a coherent percept. It has been proposed that the binding problem can be solved by synchronous firing of neurons encoding the features to be combined [11]. The synchronized neurons will additionally provide a stronger drive to downstream visual areas, enabling a coherent percept to propagate. Electrophysiological recordings in both humans and animals have provided experimental support for feature binding being achieved by neuronal synchronization in the gamma-frequency band [12 20]. Furthermore, in the locust, odour discrimination is impaired if oscillatory synchronization is blocked pharmacologically [21]. In agreement with the hypothesis that synchronization is important for neuronal communication, it has been proposed that gamma-frequency synchronization serves as a mechanism for gain control [6]. Because an increase in neuronal synchronization results in amplification of the drive exerted on downstream areas, the gain with respect to neuronal firing increases too (Box 2, Figure Ib). As addressed below, gain control by neuronal synchronization might provide the neuronal substrate for directed attention. Finally, it has been proposed that sustained neuronal /$ see front matter ß 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi: /j.tins

2 318 Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.30 No.7 firing in the gamma-frequency range is responsible for active maintenance of representations in working memory. This hypothesis has received support from animal recordings [22] and, as reviewed below, also human research [23 28]. All of the mechanisms addressed above pertain to dynamic changes in the pattern of neuronal firing. However, stable, long-lasting modifications of synaptic connections are another essential component of neuronal processing. Experimental work has demonstrated an intricate connection between neuronal firing and the phase of ongoing gamma-frequency oscillations in relation to synaptic plasticity (Box 1, Figure Id) [29]. As discussed below, oscillatory gamma-band activity has also been associated with encoding and recall of long-term memory. In summary, there are several theories suggesting that gamma-band synchronization has a pivotal role in attention and memory. These theories have received partial support from animal research; however, it remains to be determined whether the same mechanisms are present in humans when complex cognitive tasks are applied. Recent technical developments in human electrophysiological research have provided the tools for addressing this question. Beyond advances in recording techniques, these developments include better source-modelling methods for localizing oscillatory brain activity from EEG and MEG signals and the application of novel signal-processing techniques for detection of transient neural oscillations. Using these techniques, it has become possible to study human gamma-band neural synchronization in various cortical regions, such as the primary visual cortex, Broca s area and pars opercularis. This emerging line of research is reviewed here to better understand the role of gammafrequency synchronization in human attention and memory. Gamma-frequency activity during attentional selection and stimulus encoding Because of the limited processing capacities of human sensory systems, attention helps to select stimulus features relevant for behavioural goals from a rich and noisy environment. At the physiological level, attention appears to operate by increasing the relative ability of neurons Box 1. Detecting gamma-frequency activity: from pairs of neurons to cortical lobes The lowest level at which oscillatory gamma-frequency activity can be detected is in the firing pattern of individual neurons. This can be achieved by intracranial single- or multi-unit recordings in animals and, in rare cases, humans. Typically, the gamma-frequency activity is observed as spiking patterns that repeat every ms. A group of neurons is said to have oscillatory synchronous activity if their spiking co-occurs periodically. From single-unit recordings, oscillatory synchrony can be characterized from the spiking activity by simple analysis techniques, such as auto- and cross-correlograms. The example in Figure Ia (reprinted, with permission, from Ref. [59]) shows the cross-correlogram for two neurons with overlapping receptive fields 3 4 mm apart recorded in the primary visual cortex of the cat. The repeated correlations, each of 20 ms, are indicative of oscillatory synchronization at 50 Hz. The cross-correlogram is estimated between pairs of cells by calculating the histogram of the number of spikes fired by the second neuron as a function of time relative to spikes of the first neuron. Intracranially recorded local field potentials (LFPs) reflect the joint activity of a larger group of neurons. The LFP is thought to measure postsynaptic potentials resulting from neuronal spiking. Oscillatory gamma-frequency activity is observed as a periodic modulation of the ongoing LFP trace. The example in Figure Ib (adapted, with permission, from Ref. [60]. Copyright 2003 by the Society for Neuroscience) shows the relationship between neuronal firing, recorded as multi-unit activity, and the LFP, recorded simultaneously at the same site in cat area 18. The 200 ms epoch marked by the horizontal bar is expanded below. The vertical broken line indicates the onset of the stimulus (sine-wave gratings) presented to the cat. The spiking of the neurons (multi-unit activity) was phase-locked to the gamma-frequency oscillations in the LFP. Phase-locking between neuronal firing and the LFP implies that the LFP is produced by larger ensembles of neurons firing in synchrony. Often, oscillatory activity in the LFP is characterized by timefrequency representations of power. The time-frequency representation in Figure Ic (adapted, with permission, from Ref. [60]. Copyright 2003 by the Society for Neuroscience) is calculated from the LFP signals in Figure Ib. Approximately 50 ms after the onset of the sine-wave gratings, the power in the gamma-frequency band (50 70 Hz) increased and it is sustained while the stimulus is presented. Because the power is constrained to the gammafrequency band it reflects sustained oscillations in the LFP. Timefrequency representations of power were calculated using spectral analysis (Welch s method), applying a sliding 100 ms Hanningtapered time window. In humans, activity can be measured using ieeg recordings, by placing subdural electrodes directly at the brain surface. This is typically carried out in epileptic patients undergoing presurgical evaluations. The ieeg is related to LFP recordings; however, the spatial scale over which the two techniques integrate neuronal activity is different. The ieeg records the average electrical activity of patches of cortical tissue 1cm 2 in diameter (for a review, see Ref. [61]). The example in Figure Id (adapted, with permission, from Ref. [62]) shows the time-frequency representation of the gamma-band response recorded by ieeg in the human lateral occipital sulcus in response to face stimuli (Morlet wavelet analysis; units are in standard deviations of a prestimulus baseline). Finally, oscillatory gamma-frequency activity can be detected in healthy subjects using scalp EEG or MEG recordings. The signals measured by MEG and EEG represent the sum of activity from large ensembles of cells on a fairly crude spatial scale. Although the EEG and MEG signals are highly spatially smeared at the sensor level, they might still reflect local changes in neuronal synchronization, to be distinguished from changes in long-range synchronization visible in signals measured by two distant sensors. Evidence of this was obtained by recent simultaneous ieeg and MEG recordings in an epileptic patient performing cognitive tasks [63] and the comparison of ieeg, EEG and MEG studies of different subjects performing identical tasks [16,62]. The example in Figure Ie (adapted, with permission, from Ref. [64]) shows time-frequency representations of changes in power in response to visual stimulation measured by MEG (using an average of 28 axial gradiometers over the visual cortex). Changes are determined relative to the 1 s prestimulus baseline. The brain areas responsible for producing the gamma-frequency activity can be identified using source analysis and mapped onto the subject s magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan (Figure If; Adapted, with permission, from Ref. [64]). Consistent with animal findings, the sources of the gamma-frequency activity were identified in the primary visual cortex. In summary, those comparisons using different recording techniques have revealed strong similarities between the gamma-band responses recorded at different spatial scales, with respect to both timing and the response to experimental manipulation. The gap between micro- and macroscopic recordings is further bridged by recent methodological developments, for instance the ability to localize the neuronal sources of the gamma-frequency band responses. This suggests that gamma-band modulations recorded at the scalp level in humans probably reflect local changes in oscillatory synchronization, although definitive experimental and theoretical evidence is still partially lacking.

3 Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.30 No Figure I. representing attended stimuli in sensory cortices to influence downstream cortical areas [30]. One proposed mechanism involves neural synchronization: as already mentioned, a neuron receiving multiple synaptic inputs is more likely to fire if these inputs are synchronized [6]. Thus, a group of neurons representing a visually attended object has a stronger effect on receiving neurons in downstream areas if it fires synchronously (Box 2, Figure Ia,b). Indeed, several studies in monkeys have directly observed this phenomenon: visual or somatosensory neurons responding to a visual and somatosensory stimulus fire in synchrony if their target stimulus is attended [31 33].In humans, several EEG and MEG studies have found similar effects. Attended visual and somatosensory stimuli trigger stronger gamma-band responses than unattended stimuli [34,35] (Figure 1). A MEG study demonstrated that shifts of selective attention between visual and auditory modalities were accompanied by enhancements of the gammaband responses towards cortical areas, which were specific to the attended modality [36]. The amplification of gammaband activity by attention is not unique to sensory cortices, because intracranial EEG (ieeg) recordings have revealed similar effects in the premotor cortex in subjects shifting their visual attention towards the location of an upcoming target [37]. Those results complement the hypothesis that neural synchrony mediates feature binding and, in addition, underlies attentional mechanisms for encoding feature conjunctions [38]. In combination, these results suggest that attention relies on gamma-band synchronization to integrate neural activities related to a specific sensory object into a stable, salient and coherent representation, which would then be available for further maintenance in shortterm memory or encoding into long-term memory. Gamma-frequency activity during maintenance of working and short-term memory Short-term memory is defined as the capacity to retain and manipulate information that is no longer accessible in the environment. Following the hypothesis that the representation of a specific stimulus relies on a synchronously oscillating assembly of neurons, a memory trace could be established by sustained oscillations in the absence of external stimulation (Box 2, Figure Ic). This provides a putative mechanism for maintenance of short-term or working memory of neuronal representations in agreement with Hebb s [39] proposal that neuronal representations can be sustained by persistent firing of recurrently connected neurons. Studies of gamma-band activity during short-term or working memory tasks have yielded crucial support for this model. For instance, Tallon-Baudry et al. [26] found that active maintenance of abstract visual shapes in short-term memory was characterized by enhanced gamma-frequency power at occipital and bilateral temporal EEG electrode sites. Furthermore, the time course of this activity varied according to the duration of the retention interval [27]. However, the spatial resolution of the EEG was insufficient to tell whether the same neural populations were underlying both perception and memory. A recent MEG study identified sustained gamma-frequency activity in a spatial, delayed match-to-sample task [28]. Subjects were required to maintain the orientation of presented faces during a 3-s retention period. The sources accounting for the gammafrequency activity during the retention period were identified in occipital areas. The model and data presented above suggest that the memory trace of a visual stimulus is maintained by the neuronal representations in visual

4 320 Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.30 No.7 Box 2. The functional role of neuronal gamma-frequency synchronization Because oscillatory gamma-frequency activity reflects the spike timing of a large ensemble of neurons, it is bound to have a profound effect on neuronal processing. Consider a neuron receiving input from a set of neurons in a network (red traces; Figure Ia). When the firing of the neurons in the network is synchronized, their effect on the receiving neuron increases because their inputs will add up. In particular, oscillations at gamma frequencies provide a mechanism for synchronizing the input with high temporal precision relative to the time course of postsynaptic potentials (PSPs). Synchronization at lower frequencies (e.g. alpha-frequency oscillations) is more temporally smeared, resulting in a less-pronounced summation of the PSPs. Thus, neurons synchronized by alphafrequency oscillations probably have a smaller effect on target cells. As a consequence, neuronal synchronization in the gammafrequency band provides an important mechanism for neuronal communication. Gamma-frequency synchronization can be a physiological mechanism for directed attention (Figure Ib). According to the principle of binding by neuronal synchronization, groups of neurons encoding the red and blue faces, respectively, will fire synchronously. However, there will be little synchronization between the cells encoding the red and blue faces. When attention is directed to the red face, the synchronization of the respective cells will increase. Thus, this group of neurons (red cells) will provide a stronger drive to downstream targets compared with less-synchronized cells (blue cells). During working memory operations, a set of cells encoding a specific sample stimulus will fire in synchrony during presentation (Figure Ic). When the object disappears, the synchronous oscillatory firing is maintained during the retention interval, thus providing a mechanism for the maintenance of working memory. Note that this principle might also apply to extrastriate visual areas and association cortices because representations in working memory are not necessarily maintained in early sensory areas. Phase-specific synaptic input, with respect to the gamma rhythm, can facilitate synaptic plasticity and thus encoding of long-term memory (Figure Id) [29]. Consider a cell driven by subthreshold gamma-frequency oscillations. If incoming spikes are timed to the phase of the oscillations, this is likely to result in a stronger synaptic strengthening (top traces) compared with when the spikes are arriving independent of the phase (bottom trace). Figure I. cortex activated by the stimulus; however, higher-order areas are probably also involved. Several ieeg studies in patients have provided evidence for sustained gamma-frequency activity outside early sensory cortices. Two studies, in which patients were asked to memorize short, visual-letter sequences, reported a parametric increase in gamma-frequency activity, with memory load (the number of memorized letters) in nonvisual cortical areas (Figure 2a,b) [23,25]. Mainy et al. [25] found sustained gamma-frequency oscillations in regions associated with phonological processing, including Broca s area and auditory and prefrontal cortices. Modulations of gamma-frequency activity in areas along the visual ventral stream were also observed in intracranial recordings during encoding in a working-memory task, in which objects had to be either ignored or attended [40]. Pronounced differences were found between the lateral occipital cortex and the fusiform gyrus, reflecting enhancement of anticipation and stimulus-induced gamma-frequency activity, respectively. These studies indicate that synchronously oscillating neural assemblies in extrastriate visual cortex, and even nonvisual areas, are engaged in maintenance of the memory traces of visual stimuli. This suggests a mechanism of transmission or transformation between synchronous neural assemblies, from sensory cortices to higher-order cortical regions. Mainy et al. [25] have further demonstrated that such a transmission might start during stimulus presentation, because gamma-band responses to letter presentation occurred simultaneously in Broca s area and visual regions. Studies of other modalities have also suggested that the locus of oscillatory activity underlying short-term memory depends on the type of information that must be retained. Recordings using ieeg during maintenance of olfactory identity information revealed sustained gamma-frequency oscillations in the amygdala [41]. Gamma-frequency activity in auditory, short-term memory tasks was found in regions containing the putative auditory dorsal and ventral processing streams during maintenance of spatial, versus nonspatial, sound features [22,41]. An auditory, spatial, delayed match-to-sample task, in which subjects were asked to compare specific auditory features of sounds presented 800 ms apart, was associated with posterior temporo-parietal gamma-frequency activity during the delay phase [42], suggesting involvement of the auditory dorsal stream in encoding and maintenance of auditory spatial information (Figure 2c e). Conversely, memoryrelated increases in gamma-frequency power were

5 Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.30 No Figure 1. Selective attention results in increased gamma-frequency activity in the somatosensory system. Specific tactile stimuli were presented simultaneously to both index fingers and subjects were asked to identify, and thereby attend to, stimuli on either the left or the right finger. Short-lasting stimuli were delivered every 1 s using a Braille device. Brain activity was recorded using a whole-head MEG system. (a) The time-frequency representations of power show that gammaband activity in sensors over left-brain areas increased when attention was directed to the right finger compared to when attention was directed to the left finger. (b) The sensors showing this effect were located over left sensorimotor areas. (c) The regions accounting for the allocation of attention were identified in the left primary sensorimotor cortex using a source-modelling approach. Similar effects were identified in the right somatosensory system when attention was directed to the left finger. Adapted, with permission, from Ref. [34]. Copyright 2006 by the Society for Neuroscience. observed in the left inferior frontal and anterior temporal cortex during maintenance of short-term memory of auditory patterns [24]. In summary, these studies support the proposed model that maintenance of short-term and working memory involves sustained neural synchronization in the gamma-frequency band. However, it seems evident that the synchronous assemblies mediating perception and the maintenance of memory do not necessarily belong to the same brain regions. Apparently, in several situations, it is not the precise physical aspects of the stimulus being reflected in the gamma-frequency band but rather higher-level representations of relevant information. These higher-level representations could, for instance, represent a label, enabling retrieval of the actual stimuli. The next question is how representations in working memory can be transferred into long-term memory. Gamma-frequency activity and long-term memory From a physiological perspective, there are several reasons to believe that synchronized activity in the gamma-frequency band should have a role in encoding long-term memory through the modification of synaptic connections. First, it has been argued that the neuronal synchronization of representations in sensory areas results in a stronger synaptic drive to downstream areas [6]. As a consequence, the stronger drive results in stronger neuronal firing, promoting synaptic plasticity. Second, it has been demonstrated that the timing of synaptic discharges, with respect to the phase of ongoing gamma-frequency oscillation, modulates synaptic plasticity [29]. This could be explained by an increase in plasticity when a synaptic discharge coincides with depolarization provided by a peak in the gamma-frequency cycle (Box 2, Figure Id). Whereas there are few animal studies exploring gamma-frequency activity in paradigms of long-term memory, the relationship between long-term memory and gamma-frequency activity has been supported by several experimental studies in humans. Subsequent memory paradigms using EEG, MEG and ieeg recordings have shown that gamma-frequency activity during encoding predicts successful formation of long-term memory [43 46]. The gamma-frequency sources, reflecting the subsequent memory effect, were identified in early visual areas from MEG data [44] (Figure 3). It was argued that the gamma-frequency synchronization in visual areas results in a stronger drive, promoting synaptic changes in downstream association areas responsible for encoding long-term memory. Intracranial depth-electrode recordings in epileptic patients have demonstrated that local gamma-frequency activity in the hippocampus and synchronization between rhinal and hippocampal areas was correlated with successful encoding of long-term memory [47]. Furthermore, gamma-band activity has been shown to reflect successful retrieval of long-term memory too. Recognition of previously encoded items resulted in increased gamma-frequency activity compared with rejection of novel items [43,44]. Because the primary gamma-frequency sources have been identified in the occipital cortex, it was proposed that retrieval from long-term memory involves a top-down drive, reactivating the actual visual representations [44]. Long-term memory effects are also evident in models investigating stimulus repetitions. Initial presentations of line drawings of common objects, but not novel stimuli, were shown to elicit increases of induced gamma-frequency activity and phase synchronization between distant electrode sites [48], probably reflecting the activation of an existing memory trace [1]. Whereas gamma-frequency activity was reduced following repeated presentations of familiar stimuli, possibly reflecting a more sparse representation by a sharpened network [49], novel objects elicited increases of gamma-frequency power and interelectrode phase-locking to repeated presentations, which would be consistent with the formation of a new object representation in the cortex. These studies demonstrate that successful encoding and recall of long-term memory are associated with increased gamma-frequency activity. During encoding, the increased gamma-frequency activity provides a phasic drive to neurons that are already active. This might serve to entrain the gamma activity in the receiving network and thus adjust the timing of neuronal firing to be optimal for

6 322 Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.30 No.7 Figure 2. Examples of gamma-frequency activity in humans during short-term memory tasks. (a) Gamma-band power modulations in Broca s area during encoding and maintenance of short-term memory. Activity from Broca s area was recorded in an epileptic patient using ieeg electrode location (marked with a white circle). (b) The patient was presented with ten letters, the last five of which had to be remembered (as instructed by a coloured cue preceding each letter: red, you do not have to remember the next letter ; green, you have to remember the next letter ). The display shows (blue) the time-evolution of power in the Hz gamma band while a subject progressively stores the five letters and maintains them in short-term memory for 3 s. The red curve corresponds to a putative model dissociating gamma-frequency power modulations, which could be explained by the encoding of the individual letters and the maintenance of the items, respectively. Adapted, with permission, from Ref. [25]. (c) Gamma-band activity was increased during an auditory spatial short-term memory task compared with a non-memory control condition [42]. The memory task required the maintenance of the lateralization angle in space of a 200 ms noise (S1) in short-term memory during an 800 ms delay phase. Subsequently, a second lateralized noise (S2) was presented and subjects had to decide whether S1 and S2 had the same lateralization angle. Statistical probability mapping was used to compare the induced spectral activity in this task and a control condition, requiring the detection of possible changes in a background sound. Increased gamma-band activity (57 62 Hz) during the delay phase was identified by sensors over the posterior parietal cortex. MEG sensors with increased gamma-frequency power are symbolized as filled circles on a schematic cortical-surface map (dorsal view, nose up), with the circle size corresponding to the statistical significance of the difference between memory and control conditions. The topography of the gamma-frequency power increase is consistent with the hypothesis that auditory spatial information is processed along a dorsal pathway involving the posterior temporo-parietal cortex. (d) Time course of filtered and amplitude-demodulated gamma-frequency signals at Hz. The statistically most robust increase was found at a left posterior temporo-parietal sensor (large grey circle) during the delay period between the presentation of the sample stimulus (0 0.2 s) and the probe sound (1 1.2 s). (e) A time-frequency representation of power differences between memory and control tasks at the large grey sensor depicted in (c). The colour scale reflects the statistical power of spectral activity differences between memory and control tasks (with warm and cold colours symbolizing memory-related spectral power increases and decreases, respectively). Adapted, with permission, from Ref. [42]. Copyright 2002 by the Society for Neuroscience. synaptic plasticity (Box 2, Figure Id). During recall, the increased gamma-frequency activity might reflect reactivation of synaptically encoded representations. These hypotheses are consistent with the suggestion that gamma-frequency activity supports maintenance of working memory. It has been proposed that sustained representation in a working-memory buffer facilitates synaptic encoding in long-term memory [50]. Concluding remarks Although the studies reviewed here point to an important role of activity in the gamma band, it should be emphasized that oscillatory brain activity is produced in other frequency bands too. In particular, theta-band activity is modulated by both working [51,52] and long-term memory tasks [44,45,53]. Interestingly, recent ieeg studies reported that gamma-frequency power is modulated by the phase of theta-frequency oscillations [54,55]. This finding is consistent with theoretical work proposing that nested theta- and gamma-frequency oscillations have an important role in memory processing [50,56,57]. Extensive empirical work is required in the future to better understand the interactions between brain oscillations in various frequency bands. Another issue is that gamma-band modulations from Hz are considered here. This is a broad frequency range and future work might reveal that specific cognitive effects are reflected in more well-defined sub-bands in the gamma-frequency range. Several conclusions can be drawn from this review. First, the hypothesis that oscillatory activity in the gamma-frequency band is related to object representations in humans is supported by a range of studies using EEG and MEG recordings. The human studies go beyond object representation and have demonstrated that directed attention and maintenance of working memory are reflected by increases in gamma-band synchronization. These modulations are observed not only in early sensory areas, but also in association areas and the medial temporal lobe.

7 Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.30 No main advantage of studying gamma-frequency activity in humans is the possibility of employing complex paradigms. This is exemplified by the paradigms of long-term memory addressed above; similar experiments could only be performed in animals with great difficulty. Finally, gamma-frequency activity in humans provides physiological markers of human attention and memory processes that can be applied to the diagnosis of neurological and psychiatric disorders [5,58]. Animal studies and human research should go hand-in-hand in the future to further elucidate the precise neural basis of those functions. In particular, human findings could guide microelectrode explorations in animals to investigate the neural basis of functional gamma-band effects observed at the macroscopic level. Acknowledgements This work was funded by the Volkswagen Foundation Grant I/79876 and Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research Innovative Research Incentive Schemes All authors contributed equally to this paper. Figure 3. An MEG study demonstrating that gamma-frequency activity predicts successful encoding of declarative memories. In the encoding session, subjects were presented with a list of 240 figures of landscapes and buildings. In a later recognition session, subjects were presented the 240 old pictures randomly mixed with 240 new pictures. Each picture was displayed for 1 s. Subjects were then asked if they had seen a specific picture in the encoding session. The trials during encoding were sorted according to whether the pictures were later remembered (LR) or forgotten (LF) and the data analysed accordingly. (a) The topographies of the Hz gamma-frequency activity revealed stronger posterior gamma-frequency power for LR, compared with LF, pictures. (b) Timefrequency representations of power show that the gamma-frequency activity was constrained to Hz. The main difference in gamma-frequency power distinguishing LR from LF occurred 0.2 s after presentation of the pictures. (d) The sources accounting for the effect in the gamma-frequency band were localized in the occipital lobe. Adapted, with permission, from Ref. [44]. Copyright 2006 by the Society for Neuroscience. Whereas most studies have employed visual paradigms, somatosensory and auditory tasks have also been shown to modulate gamma-band activity during directed attention and maintenance of working memory. Although human studies have typically applied more complex paradigms than those used in animals, there seem to be no obvious contradictions between human and animal findings. Thus, it seems justified to use the knowledge of single-cell firing in relation to the gamma-frequency activity obtained from animal recordings to interpret the human findings. The References 1 Herrmann, C.S. et al. (2004) Cognitive functions of gamma-band activity: memory match and utilization. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, Kaiser, J. and Lutzenberger, W. (2003) Induced gamma-band activity and human brain function. Neuroscientist 9, Tallon-Baudry, C. and Bertrand, O. (1999) Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, Le Van Quyen, M. et al. (2006) The dark side of high-frequency oscillations in the developing brain. Trends Neurosci. 29, Uhlhaas, P.J. and Singer, W. (2006) Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron 52, Salinas, E. and Sejnowski, T.J. (2001) Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, Williams, S.H. and Johnston, D. (1991) Kinetic properties of two anatomically distinct excitatory synapses in hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 66, Bressler, S.L. (1995) Large-scale cortical networks and cognition. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 20, Fries, P. (2005) A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, Varela, F. et al. (2001) The brainweb: phase synchronization and largescale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, von der Malsburg, C. and Schneider, W. (1986) A neural cocktail-party processor. Biol. Cybern. 54, Kaiser, J. et al. (2004) Magnetoencephalographic gamma-band responses to illusory triangles in humans. Neuroimage 23, Keil, A. et al. (1999) Human gamma band activity and perception of a gestalt. J. Neurosci. 19, Lutzenberger, W. et al. (1995) Visual stimulation alters local 40-Hz responses in humans: an EEG-study. Neurosci. Lett. 183, Muller, M.M. et al. (1996) Visually induced gamma-band responses in human electroencephalographic activity a link to animal studies. Exp. Brain Res. 112, Rodriguez, E. et al. (1999) Perception s shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity. Nature 397, Rose, M. and Buchel, C. (2005) Neural coupling binds visual tokens to moving stimuli. J. Neurosci. 25, Rose, M. et al. (2006) Integration of local features to a global percept by neural coupling. Cereb. Cortex 16, Eckhorn, R. et al. (1988) Coherent oscillations: a mechanism of feature linking in the visual cortex? Multiple electrode and correlation analyses in the cat. Biol. Cybern. 60, Gray, C.M. et al. (1989) Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature 338,

8 324 Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.30 No.7 21 Stopfer, M. et al. (1997) Impaired odour discrimination on desynchronization of odour-encoding neural assemblies. Nature 390, Pesaran, B. et al. (2002) Temporal structure in neuronal activity during working memory in macaque parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 5, Howard, M.W. et al. (2003) Gamma oscillations correlate with working memory load in humans. Cereb. Cortex 13, Kaiser, J. et al. (2003) Dynamics of gamma-band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory. Neuroimage 20, Mainy, N. et al. (2007) Neural correlates of consolidation in working memory. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, Tallon-Baudry, C. et al. (1998) Induced gamma-band activity during the delay of a visual short-term memory task in humans. J. Neurosci. 18, Tallon-Baudry, C. et al. (1999) Sustained and transient oscillatory responses in the gamma and beta bands in a visual short-term memory task in humans. Vis. Neurosci. 16, Jokisch, D. and Jensen, O. (2007) Modulation of gamma and alpha activity during a working memory task engaging the dorsal or ventral stream. J. Neurosci. 27, Wespatat, V. et al. (2004) Phase sensitivity of synaptic modifications in oscillating cells of rat visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 24, Treue, S. (2001) Neural correlates of attention in primate visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 24, Bichot, N.P. et al. (2005) Parallel and serial neural mechanisms for visual search in macaque area V4. Science 308, Fries, P. et al. (2001) Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291, Steinmetz, P.N. et al. (2000) Attention modulates synchronized neuronal firing in primate somatosensory cortex. Nature 404, Bauer, M. et al. (2006) Tactile spatial attention enhances gamma-band activity in somatosensory cortex and reduces low-frequency activity in parieto-occipital areas. J. Neurosci. 26, Gruber, T. et al. (1999) Selective visual-spatial attention alters induced gamma band responses in the human EEG. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, Sokolov, A. et al. (2004) Reciprocal modulation of neuromagnetic induced gamma activity by attention in the human visual and auditory cortex. Neuroimage 22, Brovelli, A. et al. (2005) High gamma frequency oscillatory activity dissociates attention from intention in the human premotor cortex. Neuroimage 28, Treisman, A.M. and Gelade, G. (1980) A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognit. Psychol. 12, Hebb, D.O. ed. (1949) The Organization of Behavior. A Neuropsychological Theory, John Wiley & Sons 40 Tallon-Baudry, C. et al. (2005) Attention modulates gamma-band oscillations differently in the human lateral occipital cortex and fusiform gyrus. Cereb. Cortex 15, Jung, J. et al. (2006) Functional significance of olfactory-induced oscillations in the human amygdala. Cereb. Cortex 16, Lutzenberger, W. et al. (2002) Dynamics of gamma-band activity during an audiospatial working memory task in humans. J. Neurosci. 22, Gruber, T. et al. (2004) Induced gamma band responses: an early marker of memory encoding and retrieval. Neuroreport 15, Osipova, D. et al. (2006) Theta and gamma oscillations predict encoding and retrieval of declarative memory. J. Neurosci. 26, Sederberg, P.B. et al. (2003) Theta and gamma oscillations during encoding predict subsequent recall. J. Neurosci. 23, Sederberg, P.B. et al. (2007) Hippocampal and neocortical gamma oscillations predict memory formation in humans. Cereb. Cortex 17, Fell, J. et al. (2001) Human memory formation is accompanied by rhinal-hippocampal coupling and decoupling. Nat. Neurosci. 4, Gruber, T. and Muller, M.M. (2002) Effects of picture repetition on induced gamma band responses, evoked potentials, and phase synchrony in the human EEG. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, Gruber, T. and Muller, M.M. (2005) Oscillatory brain activity dissociates between associative stimulus content in a repetition priming task in the human EEG. Cereb. Cortex 15, Jensen, O. and Lisman, J.E. (2005) Hippocampal sequence-encoding driven by a cortical multi-item working memory buffer. Trends Neurosci. 28, Lee, H. et al. (2005) Phase locking of single neuron activity to theta oscillations during working memory in monkey extrastriate visual cortex. Neuron 45, Raghavachari, S. et al. (2006) Theta oscillations in human cortex during a working-memory task: evidence for local generators. J. Neurophysiol. 95, Klimesch, W. et al. (1997) Theta synchronization and alpha desynchronization in a memory task. Psychophysiology 34, Mormann, F. et al. (2005) Phase/amplitude reset and theta-gamma interaction in the human medial temporal lobe during a continuous word recognition memory task. Hippocampus 15, Canolty, R.T. et al. (2006) High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science 313, Lisman, J.E. and Idiart, M.A. (1995) Storage of 7+/ 2 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science 267, Jensen, O. (2006) Maintenance of multiple working memory items by temporal segmentation. Neuroscience 139, Spencer, K.M. et al. (2004) Neural synchrony indexes disordered perception and cognition in schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, Maldonado, P.E. et al. (2000) Dynamics of striate cortical activity in the alert macaque: II. Fast time scale synchronization. Cereb. Cortex 10, Siegel, M. and Konig, P. (2003) A functional gamma-band defined by stimulus-dependent synchronization in area 18 of awake behaving cats. J. Neurosci. 23, Lachaux, J.P. et al. (2003) Intracranial EEG and human brain mapping. J. Physiol. 97, Lachaux, J.P. et al. (2005) The many faces of the gamma band response to complex visual stimuli. Neuroimage 25, Lachaux, J.P. et al. A simultaneous MEG and intracranial EEG study of task-related brain oscillations. In Human Brain Mapping (Series, I.C., ed.), International Congress Series. ICS 6940, Elsevier (in press) 64 Hoogenboom, N. et al. (2006) Localizing human visual gamma-band activity in frequency, time and space. Neuroimage 29, Endeavour Coming soon in the quarterly magazine for the history and philosophy of science: Earthquake theories in the early modern period by F. Willmoth Science in fiction - attempts to make a science out of literary criticism by J. Adams The birth of botanical Drosophila by S. Leonelli Endeavour is available on ScienceDirect,

The role of phase synchronization in memory processes

The role of phase synchronization in memory processes The role of phase synchronization in memory processes Juergen Fell and Nikolai Axmacher Abstract In recent years, studies ranging from single-unit recordings in animals to electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography

More information

A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal

A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal Published in final edited form as: Trends in Cognitive Science 9(10), 474-480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011 A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence Pascal

More information

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG Educational Symposium, MEG UK 2014, Nottingham, Jan 8th 2014 Krish Singh CUBRIC, School of Psychology Cardiff University What are we trying to achieve? Bridge the gap from

More information

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition Chair: Jason Samaha, University of Wisconsin-Madison Co-Chair: Ali Mazaheri, University of Birmingham

More information

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J. The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem By Andrew Laguna, S.J. Outline I. Introduction II. The Visual System III. What is the Binding Problem? IV. Possible Theoretical Solutions

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) Introduction 17 subjects were tested to measure effects of a Brain.fm focus session on cognition. With 4 additional subjects, we recorded EEG data during baseline and while

More information

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings J. Besle, P. Lakatos, C.A. Schevon, R.R. Goodman, G.M. McKhann, A. Mehta, R.G. Emerson, C.E.

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Assignment 5: Neural mechanisms of object-based attention Due: Friday, April 14 This Assignment is a guided reading of the 2014 paper, Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based

More information

Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions

Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions Nature Reviews Neuroscience AOP, published online 11 January 212; doi:1.138/nrn3137 REVIEWS Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions Markus Siegel 1 *, Tobias H. Donner 2 * and Andreas

More information

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements Supplementary Material Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements Xiaomo Chen, Katherine Wilson Scangos 2 and Veit Stuphorn,2 Department of Psychological and Brain

More information

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics Scott Makeig Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla CA sccn.ucsd.edu Talk given at the EEG/MEG course

More information

From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics

From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics a Delorme A., a Makeig, S., b Fabre-Thorpe, M., a Sejnowski, T. a The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, 10010 N. Torey Pines Road, La Jolla, CA92109, USA

More information

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS Franziska Kopp* #, Erich Schröger* and Sigrid Lipka # *University of Leipzig, Institute of General Psychology # University of Leipzig,

More information

Synchrony and the attentional state

Synchrony and the attentional state Synchrony and the attentional state Ernst Niebur, Dept. of Neuroscience & Krieger Mind/Brain Institute Johns Hopkins University niebur@jhu.edu Collaborators Arup Roy monkey monkey Peter monkey Steven monkey

More information

The Nonhuman Primate as Model System for Mechanistic Studies of Glutamate System Function and Dysfunction

The Nonhuman Primate as Model System for Mechanistic Studies of Glutamate System Function and Dysfunction The Nonhuman Primate as Model System for Mechanistic Studies of Glutamate System Function and Dysfunction FORUM ON NEUROSCIENCE AND NERVOUS SYSTEM DISORDERS Board on Health Sciences Policy Glutamate-related

More information

'Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes' by Lawrence M. Ward, 2003 [1].

'Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes' by Lawrence M. Ward, 2003 [1]. By: Lior Golgher Chosen Review Article Weizmann Institute of Science - Feinberg Graduate School The Cognitive Neuroscience of Human Memory Synchronous Neural Oscillations and Working Memory in Light of

More information

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature,

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature, Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature, Vol. 435, pp. 1102-7. Sander Vaus 22.04.2015 The study

More information

Thalamocortical Synchronization and Cognition: Implications for Schizophrenia?

Thalamocortical Synchronization and Cognition: Implications for Schizophrenia? Published in final edited form as: Neuron 77(6), 997 999. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.033 Thalamocortical Synchronization and Cognition: Implications for Schizophrenia? Peter J. Uhlhaas

More information

Electrophysiological and firing properties of neurons: categorizing soloists and choristers in primary visual cortex

Electrophysiological and firing properties of neurons: categorizing soloists and choristers in primary visual cortex *Manuscript Click here to download Manuscript: Manuscript revised.docx Click here to view linked Referenc Electrophysiological and firing properties of neurons: categorizing soloists and choristers in

More information

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems Compare Chap 31 of Purves et al., 5e Chap 24 of Bear et al., 3e Larry Wittie Computer Science, StonyBrook University http://www.cs.sunysb.edu/~cse511

More information

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Goals for the lecture Be able to: Conceptually understand some of the analysis and jargon encountered in a typical (sensory)

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Types and Structure of Memory Types of Memory Type of Memory Time Course Capacity Conscious Awareness Mechanism of Loss Sensory Short-Term and Working Long-Term Nondeclarative

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may have as many as 200,000

More information

The role of theta oscillations in memory and decision making

The role of theta oscillations in memory and decision making The role of theta oscillations in memory and decision making Thesis by Marijke Beulen Neuroscience and Cognition, Utrecht University July 2011 Supervisor: Dr. Marieke van Vugt Department of Artificial

More information

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons Yoonsuck Choe Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin Austin, TX 78712 USA Risto Miikkulainen Department

More information

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves SICE Annual Conference 27 Sept. 17-2, 27, Kagawa University, Japan Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves Seiji Nishifuji 1, Kentaro Fujisaki 1 and Shogo Tanaka 1 1

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

From single-trial EEG to brain area dynamics

From single-trial EEG to brain area dynamics Neurocomputing 44 46 (2002) 1057 1064 www.elsevier.com/locate/neucom From single-trial EEG to brain area dynamics A. Delorme a;, S. Makeig a, M. Fabre-Thorpe b, T. Sejnowski a a The Salk Institute for

More information

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight? How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? What happens to visual-devoted brain structure in individuals who are born deprived of sight?

More information

Visual Memory Any neural or behavioural phenomenon implying storage of a past visual experience. E n c o d i n g. Individual exemplars:

Visual Memory Any neural or behavioural phenomenon implying storage of a past visual experience. E n c o d i n g. Individual exemplars: Long-term Memory Short-term Memory Unconscious / Procedural Conscious / Declarative Working Memory Iconic Memory Visual Memory Any neural or behavioural phenomenon implying storage of a past visual experience.

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT 1 Courtesy of Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.

More information

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14 Motor Systems I Cortex Reading: BCP Chapter 14 Principles of Sensorimotor Function Hierarchical Organization association cortex at the highest level, muscles at the lowest signals flow between levels over

More information

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück The Binding Problem This lecture is based on following articles: Adina L. Roskies: The Binding Problem; Neuron 1999 24: 7 Charles M. Gray: The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration:

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

Simultaneous MEG-intracranial EEG: New insights into the ability of MEG to capture oscillatory modulations in the neocortex and the hippocampus

Simultaneous MEG-intracranial EEG: New insights into the ability of MEG to capture oscillatory modulations in the neocortex and the hippocampus Simultaneous MEG-intracranial EEG: New insights into the ability of MEG to capture oscillatory modulations in the neocortex and the hippocampus Sarang Dalal, Karim Jerbi, Olivier Bertrand, Claude Adam,

More information

The Nervous System. Neuron 01/12/2011. The Synapse: The Processor

The Nervous System. Neuron 01/12/2011. The Synapse: The Processor The Nervous System Neuron Nucleus Cell body Dendrites they are part of the cell body of a neuron that collect chemical and electrical signals from other neurons at synapses and convert them into electrical

More information

Consciousness as representation formation from a neural Darwinian perspective *

Consciousness as representation formation from a neural Darwinian perspective * Consciousness as representation formation from a neural Darwinian perspective * Anna Kocsis, mag.phil. Institute of Philosophy Zagreb, Croatia Vjeran Kerić, mag.phil. Department of Psychology and Cognitive

More information

Brain and Cognition. Cognitive Neuroscience. If the brain were simple enough to understand, we would be too stupid to understand it

Brain and Cognition. Cognitive Neuroscience. If the brain were simple enough to understand, we would be too stupid to understand it Brain and Cognition Cognitive Neuroscience If the brain were simple enough to understand, we would be too stupid to understand it 1 The Chemical Synapse 2 Chemical Neurotransmission At rest, the synapse

More information

Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex

Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex Thomas Klausberger Dept. Cognitive Neurobiology, Center for Brain Research, Med. Uni. Vienna The hippocampus is a key brain circuit for certain forms of memory

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html Memory Working Memory Long-term Memory Declarative Memory Procedural Memory

More information

Computational cognitive neuroscience: 2. Neuron. Lubica Beňušková Centre for Cognitive Science, FMFI Comenius University in Bratislava

Computational cognitive neuroscience: 2. Neuron. Lubica Beňušková Centre for Cognitive Science, FMFI Comenius University in Bratislava 1 Computational cognitive neuroscience: 2. Neuron Lubica Beňušková Centre for Cognitive Science, FMFI Comenius University in Bratislava 2 Neurons communicate via electric signals In neurons it is important

More information

Resonant synchronization of heterogeneous inhibitory networks

Resonant synchronization of heterogeneous inhibitory networks Cerebellar oscillations: Anesthetized rats Transgenic animals Recurrent model Review of literature: γ Network resonance Life simulations Resonance frequency Conclusion Resonant synchronization of heterogeneous

More information

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Yingxue Wang, Sandro Romani, Brian Lustig, Anthony Leonardo, Eva Pastalkova Supplementary Materials Supplementary Modeling

More information

Introduction to Electrophysiology

Introduction to Electrophysiology Introduction to Electrophysiology Dr. Kwangyeol Baek Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging Massachusetts General Hospital Harvard Medical School 2018-05-31s Contents Principles in Electrophysiology Techniques

More information

Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses

Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses NOTE Communicated by Laurence Abbott Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses W. Senn Department of Neurobiology, Hebrew University, Jerusalem 4, Israel, Department of Physiology, University

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

TEMPORAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE MEMORY CODE

TEMPORAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE MEMORY CODE TEMPORAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE MEMORY CODE Irina V. Maltseva 1, Yuri Masloboev 2 Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences Moscow, Russia 1 Moscow Institute of Electronic Engineering Moscow,

More information

Distinct Gamma-Band Components Reflect the Short-Term Memory Maintenance of Different Sound Lateralization Angles

Distinct Gamma-Band Components Reflect the Short-Term Memory Maintenance of Different Sound Lateralization Angles Cerebral Cortex October 2008;18:2286--2295 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm251 Advance Access publication February 5, 2008 Distinct Gamma-Band Components Reflect the Short-Term Memory Maintenance of Different Sound

More information

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 Psycholinguistics / Neurolinguistics Happy Hour!!! Tuesdays 7/11, 7/18, 7/25 5:30-6:30 PM @ the Boone Center

More information

Physiological and Physical Basis of Functional Brain Imaging 6. EEG/MEG. Kâmil Uludağ, 20. November 2007

Physiological and Physical Basis of Functional Brain Imaging 6. EEG/MEG. Kâmil Uludağ, 20. November 2007 Physiological and Physical Basis of Functional Brain Imaging 6. EEG/MEG Kâmil Uludağ, 20. November 2007 Course schedule 1. Overview 2. fmri (Spin dynamics, Image formation) 3. fmri (physiology) 4. fmri

More information

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Alexa Morcom Edinburgh SPM course 2017 Thanks to Rik Henson, Thomas Wolbers, Jody Culham, and the SPM authors for slides Overview Categorical designs Factorial designs

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing

More information

Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization

Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization Panagiotis Ioannou, Matthew Casey and André Grüning Department of Computing, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, GU2 7XH, UK

More information

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition Chapter 2 The Neural Basis of Cognition Capgras Syndrome Alzheimer s patients & others delusion that significant others are robots or impersonators - paranoia Two brain systems for facial recognition -

More information

Bursting dynamics in the brain. Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST

Bursting dynamics in the brain. Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST Bursting dynamics in the brain Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST Tonic and phasic activity A neuron is said to exhibit a tonic activity when it fires a series of single action potentials

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Section 4

Cognitive Neuroscience Section 4 Perceptual categorization Cognitive Neuroscience Section 4 Perception, attention, and memory are all interrelated. From the perspective of memory, perception is seen as memory updating by new sensory experience.

More information

Geography of the Forehead

Geography of the Forehead 5. Brain Areas Geography of the Forehead Everyone thinks the brain is so complicated, but let s look at the facts. The frontal lobe, for example, is located in the front! And the temporal lobe is where

More information

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST 722 - November 27 Jennifer Melcher Forward suppression of unit activity in auditory cortex Brosch and Schreiner (1997) J Neurophysiol 77: 923-943. Forward

More information

Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation

Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation So far we have talked about the hippocampus and: -coding of spatial locations in rats -declarative (explicit) memory -experimental evidence

More information

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Experimental Design Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Overview Design of functional neuroimaging studies Categorical designs Factorial designs Parametric

More information

Multiscale Evidence of Multiscale Brain Communication

Multiscale Evidence of Multiscale Brain Communication Multiscale Evidence of Multiscale Brain Communication Scott Makeig Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla CA Talk given

More information

Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators

Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators Balaji Sriram March 23, 2009 Abstract Feedback systems are ubiquitous in neural systems and are a subject of intense theoretical and experimental analysis.

More information

Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes

Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes Review Vol.7 No.12 December 2003 553 Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes Lawrence M. Ward Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, B.C. V6T

More information

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Theories of Sensory, short-term & long-term memories Memory & brain Cellular bases

More information

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25 Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP Reading: BCP Chapter 25 Memory Systems Learning is the acquisition of new knowledge or skills. Memory is the retention of learned information. Many different

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Key questions about attention

Key questions about attention Key questions about attention How does attention affect behavioral performance? Can attention affect the appearance of things? How does spatial and feature-based attention affect neuronal responses in

More information

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Human Brain Mapping 6:373 377(1998) Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Randy L. Buckner 1,2,3 * 1 Departments of Psychology, Anatomy and Neurobiology, and Radiology, Washington University,

More information

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131 ! Chapter 5 Summary and Conclusions! 131 Chapter 5!!!! Summary of the main findings The present thesis investigated the sensory representation of natural sounds in the human auditory cortex. Specifically,

More information

Prior Knowledge and Memory Consolidation Expanding Competitive Trace Theory

Prior Knowledge and Memory Consolidation Expanding Competitive Trace Theory Prior Knowledge and Memory Consolidation Expanding Competitive Trace Theory Anna Smith Outline 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Background in Memory Models Models of Consolidation The Hippocampus Competitive Trace Theory

More information

Attentional Cues Affect Accuracy and Reaction Time via Different Cognitive and Neural Processes

Attentional Cues Affect Accuracy and Reaction Time via Different Cognitive and Neural Processes 10408 The Journal of Neuroscience, July 25, 2012 32(30):10408 10412 Brief Communications Attentional Cues Affect Accuracy and Reaction Time via Different Cognitive and Neural Processes Freek van Ede, Floris

More information

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Name: Student #: BEFORE YOU BEGIN!!! 1) Count the number of pages in your exam. The exam is 8 pages long; if you do not

More information

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) What happens beyond the retina? What happens in Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)- 90% flow Visual cortex Information Flow Superior colliculus 10% flow Slide 2 Information

More information

The Role of Oscillations and Synchrony in Cortical Networks and Their Putative Relevance for the Pathophysiology of Schizophrenia

The Role of Oscillations and Synchrony in Cortical Networks and Their Putative Relevance for the Pathophysiology of Schizophrenia Schizophrenia Bulletin doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn062 Schizophrenia Bulletin Advance Access published June 17, 2008 The Role of Oscillations and Synchrony in Cortical Networks and Their Putative Relevance for

More information

Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli

Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli Neuroscience Letters 424 (2007) 55 60 Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli Udo Will, Eric Berg School of Music, Cognitive Ethnomusicology, Ohio State University, 110

More information

Precise Spike Timing and Reliability in Neural Encoding of Low-Level Sensory Stimuli and Sequences

Precise Spike Timing and Reliability in Neural Encoding of Low-Level Sensory Stimuli and Sequences Precise Spike Timing and Reliability in Neural Encoding of Low-Level Sensory Stimuli and Sequences Temporal Structure In the World Representation in the Brain Project 1.1.2 Feldman and Harris Labs Temporal

More information

1 Introduction Synchronous ring of action potentials amongst multiple neurons is a phenomenon that has been observed in a wide range of neural systems

1 Introduction Synchronous ring of action potentials amongst multiple neurons is a phenomenon that has been observed in a wide range of neural systems Model-free detection of synchrony in neuronal spike trains, with an application to primate somatosensory cortex A. Roy a, P. N. Steinmetz a 1, K. O. Johnson a, E. Niebur a 2 a Krieger Mind/Brain Institute,

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

Event-related fields (ERFs) measured by using magnetoencephalography

Event-related fields (ERFs) measured by using magnetoencephalography Posterior activity is not phase-reset by visual stimuli Ali Mazaheri and Ole Jensen* F. C. Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, P.O. Box 9101, NL-6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands Edited by Nancy

More information

Biomarkers in Schizophrenia

Biomarkers in Schizophrenia Biomarkers in Schizophrenia David A. Lewis, MD Translational Neuroscience Program Department of Psychiatry NIMH Conte Center for the Neuroscience of Mental Disorders University of Pittsburgh Disease Process

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature10776 Supplementary Information 1: Influence of inhibition among blns on STDP of KC-bLN synapses (simulations and schematics). Unconstrained STDP drives network activity to saturation

More information

Est-ce que l'eeg a toujours sa place en 2019?

Est-ce que l'eeg a toujours sa place en 2019? Est-ce que l'eeg a toujours sa place en 2019? Thomas Bast Epilepsy Center Kork, Germany Does EEG still play a role in 2019? What a question 7T-MRI, fmri, DTI, MEG, SISCOM, Of ieeg course! /HFO, Genetics

More information

Adventures into terra incognita

Adventures into terra incognita BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter VI. Adventures into terra incognita In primary visual cortex there are neurons

More information

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception EDGE DETECTION Edge Detectors Slide 2 Convolution & Feature Detection Slide 3 Finds the slope First derivative Direction dependent Need many edge detectors for all orientation Second order derivatives

More information

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Fani Deligianni B. A. Wandell, et al. Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex, Neuron, 56(2):366-383, 2007 Retinotopy Visual Cortex organised in visual field maps: Nearby neurons have

More information

Rolls,E.T. (2016) Cerebral Cortex: Principles of Operation. Oxford University Press.

Rolls,E.T. (2016) Cerebral Cortex: Principles of Operation. Oxford University Press. Digital Signal Processing and the Brain Is the brain a digital signal processor? Digital vs continuous signals Digital signals involve streams of binary encoded numbers The brain uses digital, all or none,

More information

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE BRAIN The central nervous system (CNS), consisting of the brain and spinal cord, receives input from sensory neurons and directs

More information

Areas of the visual cortex are arranged hierarchically, with

Areas of the visual cortex are arranged hierarchically, with Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex Timo van Kerkoerle a,1, Matthew W. Self a, Bruno Dagnino a, Marie-Alice Gariel-Mathis a, Jasper Poort

More information

Supplemental Information. Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking. Activity in Early Visual Cortex. Supplemental Information Inventory:

Supplemental Information. Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking. Activity in Early Visual Cortex. Supplemental Information Inventory: Neuron, Volume 77 Supplemental Information Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking Activity in Early Visual Cortex Xiaoxuan Jia, Seiji Tanabe, and Adam Kohn Supplemental Information Inventory: Supplementary

More information

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

A model of the interaction between mood and memory INSTITUTE OF PHYSICS PUBLISHING NETWORK: COMPUTATION IN NEURAL SYSTEMS Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 12 (2001) 89 109 www.iop.org/journals/ne PII: S0954-898X(01)22487-7 A model of the interaction between

More information

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Asymmetry in learning in the reverse direction Full recovery from UP using DOWN: initial return to naïve values within 10 minutes,

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. Authors Year Patients Male gender (%) Mean age (range) Adults/ Children

More information

Nature Methods: doi: /nmeth Supplementary Figure 1. Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse.

Nature Methods: doi: /nmeth Supplementary Figure 1. Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse. Supplementary Figure 1 Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse. a. Probability distribution of firing rates across the population (notice log scale) in our data. The range of firing rates is wide but

More information

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg The concept that different parts of the brain did different things started with Spurzheim and Gall, whose phrenology became quite

More information