Social Influence and Persuasion: Recent Theoretical Developments and Integrative Attempts

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Social Influence and Persuasion: Recent Theoretical Developments and Integrative Attempts"

Transcription

1 Social Influence and Persuasion 1 Social Influence and Persuasion: Recent Theoretical Developments and Integrative Attempts Hans-Peter Erb University of Chemnitz and Gerd Bohner University of Bielefeld Final draft of Erb, H.-P., & Bohner, G. (2007). Social influence and persuasion: Recent theoretical developments and integrative attempts. In K. Fiedler (Ed.), Social communication (pp ). New York: Psychology Press.

2 Social Influence and Persuasion 2 Author Note Hans-Peter Erb, University of Chemnitz, Economical, Organizational and Social Psychology, Wilhelm-Raabe-Str. 43, D Chemnitz, Germany. Gerd Bohner, University of Bielefeld, Social Psychology, PO-Box , D Bielefeld, Germany. Electronic mail may be sent to hans-peter.erb@phil.tu-chemnitz.de or gerd.bohner@uni-bielefeld.de. We would like to thank Gerald Echterhoff and Justin Preston for their helpful comments on a previous draft. Keywords (19): Social Influence, Persuasion, Normative Influence, Informational Influence, Minority Influence, Majority Influence, Elaboration Likelihood Model, Heuristic-Systematic Model, Unimodel, Conversion Theory, Leniency Contract Model, Consensus, Objective Consensus Approach, Mere Consensus Approach, Social Identity, Deindividuation, Subjective Relevance, Processing Effort, Motivational Bias

3 Social Influence and Persuasion 3 INTRODUCTION The study of social influence phenomena lies at the very heart of social psychology. Allport s (1935) classical definition of social psychology as the study of how individuals are influenced by the real or imagined presence of others may be indicative of how crucial social psychologists have regarded influence for all aspects of social life. In fact, social influence occurs constantly. Consider our daily exposure to radio and television commercials, newspaper ads, direct requests, influence attempts by salespersons, politicians, and so forth. In a broader sense, influence by other individuals operates in many other forms of social interaction as well, including helping behavior, aggression, social loafing, social facilitation, leadership, obedience, prejudice, and many others. In the present chapter we start with a classic distinction between two types of social influence: normative and informational. We will then use this distinction as a guiding principle for reviewing developments in two major areas of research: Social influence in groups, which is mostly captured in terms of minority and majority influence, and persuasion. This focus, necessary with regard to space constraints, has resulted in the omission of a number of other, equally important, approaches to social influence that are less congenial to the structure we decided to present cf., Cialdini s (1994) work on influence tactics (see also Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004), or Latané and Nowak's (1994) application of catastrophe theory to social influence processes. We will conclude this chapter by arguing that abstractions from surface characteristics in social influence and persuasion and a focus on underlying principles can provide new insights and perspectives on the phenomena reported here. NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE Research on how the social context may influence subjective beliefs and attitudes has by and large centered on two domains: Attitude change in response to persuasive communication as opposed to influence in social groups. Persuasion research has typically focused on the cognitive mechanisms underlying judgment in response to communication while tending to neglect the

4 Social Influence and Persuasion 4 social context in which such communication takes place. Research more sensitive to interpersonal and group dynamics has predominantly focused on the social contexts of judgment formation, but tended to be inarticulate regarding the underlying cognitive mechanisms. At surface level, the distinction between those two domains resembles the separation between normative and informational influence introduced by Deutsch and Gerard (1955). Since its inception, Deutsch and Gerard s (1955) dual-process distinction has been one of the most important schemes to guide research on social influence. Normative influence has been assumed to be associated with little or no issue-related processing. As the social relationship between the influence source and its target becomes the focal concern, influence results in mere conforming (Deutsch & Gerard, 1955) or compliance (Moscovici, 1980) at the expense of concerns with the true value of the attitude object. Surveillance of the target by the source has been conceptualized as the critical momentum producing normative influence. As long as the socially valued influence source is present, the target will comply in public. As soon as surveillance ends, for example when the target is allowed to express its attitude in private, attitudes will fall back to their pre-influence value, because the initial change to the publicly expressed opinion has not carried over to a change in the target s true beliefs on the attitude issue. In informational influence, on the other hand, the target is assumed to be free to comply or not because the social implications of her or his attitudes are less relevant. If influence occurs, then, it is supposed to be based on the processing of information related to the issue under consideration. Such agreeing (Deutsch & Gerard, 1955) or conversion (Moscovici, 1980) has often been regarded as true attitude change. This change could be found in private attitude judgments and even when the influence source was no longer present; it persisted over time, resisted counter-argumentation, and often generalized to issues that were not identical but somehow related to the focal issue of influence. Attitude change on the basis of issue-related information (such as arguments) typically

5 Social Influence and Persuasion 5 has been the major concern of persuasion research, which we will review in more detail below. Presently it suffices to recall that dual-process models of persuasion (Chaiken, Liberman, & Eagly, 1989; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a, 1986b) distinguish between two qualitatively different processes of attitude formation and change. In a rather simple and effortless process, the targets of persuasive appeals may use shortcuts and simple heuristics (e.g., experts can be trusted, high consensus implies correctness ) to form their attitude judgments. Only when motivation and ability to process information are high, recipients engage in a more effortful process and consider mainly issue-related information such as the arguments presented. Such processing of the true merits (Petty & Wegener, 1999) of the issue is mostly concerned with what was called informational influence. On the other hand, influence based on the normative value of a given influence group, often in the absence of any issue-related processing, was treated as a peripheral or heuristic process (e.g., Baker & Petty, 1994) and was regarded as no different from other peripheral mechanisms such as the use of source expertise or one s current mood for judgment formation. SOCIAL INFLUENCE IN GROUPS Seminal Studies For a long time, social influence in groups was understood as the psychology of conformity, often represented in the influence that the majority exerts on an individual. In Sherif s (1935) studies, for example, individuals were exposed to an ambiguous stimulus situation, and they took into account the judgments of others as a source of information about the true value of the object to be judged (the assumed movement of a light spot). Other than Sherif (1935), Asch (1956) used fully unambiguous stimuli in his line-judgment experiments. Influence still occurred, presumably because individuals were concerned with the impression others might form of them. This (rather simplified) distinction between influence in Sherif s versus Asch s

6 Social Influence and Persuasion 6 paradigms may serve as a prime example of informational versus normative influence (Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). In informational influence, individuals use the information provided by others as a source of the true value of the object under consideration. Normative influence, on the other hand, refers to the individual s assumed need to align their attitude with that of some valued other(s), be it a single person (e.g., one s spouse) or a reference group. From this perspective, the Sherif paradigm reflects informational influence: The informational basis for the judgment is obscure, and others judgments will be regarded as information on the true value of the light s movement. In the Asch paradigm, however, information on the true value is straightforward (one s visual perception provides a usually valid source of information), but others judgments are in contradiction to the informational basis. It may seem that whenever others judgments prevail over the informational basis, normative influence is at operation. Normative Influence and Social Identity In more recent theorizing, normative influence in Deutsch and Gerard s sense is closely related to Social Identity Theory (SIT; e.g., Tajfel, 1981; Tajfel & Turner, 1986) and its central concept, self-categorization (e.g., Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987). According to SIT, group membership is of fundamental concern to the individual because it determines the individual s self-definition and social identity. Thus, group membership has important implications. Relevant to the present discussion, opinions held by an individual do not only reflect knowledge about an issue, they also reflect something about the individual s self, as conformity with a group s opinion means adopting the group s identity. Dissent with a selfrelevant group s attitude will urge the individual to resolve the disagreement, especially if shared attitudes are a group-defining feature (Turner, 1991). This is not to say that any selfdefining group is monolithic in its demands for within-group consensus on every issue. Still, it seems straightforward to predict that identity-defining in-groups exert social influence on the individual, whereas influence by out-groups can more easily be refuted (e.g., Crano, 2001;

7 Social Influence and Persuasion 7 David & Turner, 2001). Thus, identification is assumed to be a prerequisite of social influence. Yet SIT and its derivatives (e.g., Spears, Postmes, Lea, & Watt, 2001) do not ignore the role of issue-related information as an informational basis of social influence. In terms of SIT, the group is particularly important because it provides social validation of individual attitudes. It is the recipient s relation to the source that defines how the message is perceived. From the perspective of SIT, intra- and inter-group relations thus cannot be relegated to the status of a peripheral variable, but lie at the heart of any influence situation. The distinction between normative and informational influence can thus be regarded as a guiding principle in research into social influence and persuasion. Persuasion researchers theoretical focus was on informational influence, specifically message learning (Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953) or cognitive responding to persuasive messages (Petty, Ostrom, & Brock, 1981), whereas source variables were assigned peripheral status. Research on group-based social influence, on the other hand, has been mainly concerned with the psychological consequences of being in a group and its effects on judgment formation, with the meaning of issue-related information playing the less important role and being seen as dependent on whether its source was a self-defining group or not. Conversion Theory We now turn to a model that integrates normative and informational influence by distinctly allocating informational influence to social minorities, and normative influence to social majorities. Moscovici s (e.g., Moscovici, 1980) conversion theory (CT) has been one of the most important models in social influence during the last decades. It would be impossible to understand the most recent advances in research on social influence without discussing how they relate to Moscovici s seminal work (see also Martin & Hewstone, 2001). Research in the tradition of Asch (1956) and Sherif (1935) focused on the effect that a majority of others has on individual judgments. The question of what conditions foster

8 Social Influence and Persuasion 8 conformity thus dominated research into social influence for a long time (see Jones & Gerard, 1967, for a review). Despite the immense power of social consensus, however, history provides many examples of the opposite, namely, opinion minorities that exerted considerable influence, for example the civil rights movement in the U.S. or the environmentalist movement in Europe. Moscovici (e.g., Moscovici, Lage, & Naffrechoux, 1969) was the first researcher to acknowledge that at times minorities, who by definition hold positions of low consensus, can be influential. In CT, Moscovici (1980) distinguished two qualitatively different processes associated with majority and minority influence. He incorporated the distinction between normative and informational influence, and made predictions with regard to cognitive effort in processing issue-relevant information that mapped on the distinction between systematic or central route processing and heuristic or peripheral route processing that were developed in research on persuasion at roughly the same time. According to CT, minorities and majorities exert influence in qualitatively distinct ways. When confronted with majority influence, the individual experiences a social conflict. This conflict revolves around the question of why she or he sees things differently than most others. Reactions to this conflict focus on the social relationship between the individual and the influence group at the expense of issue-related processing. This is because the power of a majority resides in its ability to punish and reward group members. Deviance is regarded as leading to negative consequences, and the individual in disagreement with the majority s position is likely to experience discomfort. To solve this stressful social conflict, the individual agrees with the majority position in public. But given that such compliance is not backed up with issue-related processing, attitude change in the case of majority influence is superficial and short-lived, does not generalize to related issues, and vanishes as soon as the source of influence is no longer present. Majority influence according to CT is clearly an example of normative influence in Deutsch and Gerard s (1955) sense, and also an example of attitude change on the

9 Social Influence and Persuasion 9 basis of low-effort processing when viewed from dual-process models of persuasion. Minorities, on the other hand, lack the power to reward or punish. The target who is in conflict with a minority source, therefore, does not experience discomfort. The minority lacks the ability to exert influence based on sheer power. Its only chance of exerting influence is by making a virtue out of necessity and trying to convince others based on content related arguments. To do so, it has to display a consistent behavioral style. That is, to repeat its position unanimously as often as possible. Such consistency has two consequences: first, conflict produces a climate for social change. Other than in majority influence, such conflict is not social in nature but cognitive, as it relates to the controversial issue and the question of what is wrong and what is right. Second, the target ascribes confidence in their position to the minority members. In turn, the target engages in a validation process by which she or he considers the minority position carefully. Often, validation will not lead to direct, immediate, or publicly announced change. Because of the majority s power, the target is reluctant to align in public with the minority. In private, however, the target attempts to understand the deviating position and reevaluates her or his initial attitude. Validation results in conversion, a slow but constant change towards the minority position based on issue-related considerations. As such, conversion reflects informational influence in Deutsch and Gerard s (1955) sense, and high effort processing of issue-related information when viewed in terms of dual-process models of persuasion. Moscovici s (1980) CT had an extremely energizing effect on research into social influence phenomena. Empirical tests of CT, however, yielded mixed results. In a meta-analysis, Wood, Lundgren, Ouellette, Busceme, and Blackstone (1994) found greater influence of majority than minority sources on both public and private direct measures. In addition, minorities did not exert greater impact in private than in public. In favor of CT, however, minority influence was greater when assessed indirectly on issues related to the focal issue as

10 Social Influence and Persuasion 10 compared with direct measures of attitude change. Yet, even on indirect measures minorities did not exert reliably greater influence than did majorities. Overall, then, minorities were found to exert less influence than majorities (Wood et al., 1994). The status of CT s hypothesis that minorities instigate more effort in the processing of their messages than do majorities is not much clearer. By contrast, Mackie (1987) found that participants invested more effort in processing majority than minority messages. According to her objective consensus approach, the majority s high consensus implies correctness and therefore directs attention to the majority s message. Baker and Petty (1994) demonstrated that both minorities and majorities may instigate careful processing of their messages if the position they favor is unexpected: If a majority of students favored, or a minority opposed, a mandatory social service for students, this obvious source-position imbalance generated surprise which in turn increased processing effort. Erb, Bohner, Schmälzle, and Rank (1998) were the first to employ control conditions where no source information was provided and found that targets invested less effort in processing both minority and majority messages compared with the control condition. In this study, the influence group was socially irrelevant to recipients who held no prior attitudes, so that conflict in Moscovici s sense was not in operation. Under these conditions recipients simplified judgment formation by using consensus information as an indicator of message validity. Hence, influence targets were inclined to invest more effort in messages not attached to consensus information, where such a pre-judgment could not be generated. Despite the overall mixed empirical support for CT, many recent developments in the field refer to CT in one way or another. For example, work by Nemeth and her colleagues (e.g., Nemeth, 1986; Peterson & Nemeth, 1996) alludes to Moscovici s idea of differential conflict under minority versus majority influence. According to Nemeth, dissent with a majority induces pressure to comply, whereas dissent with a minority does not. It follows that, under majority

11 Social Influence and Persuasion 11 influence, individuals focus their attention on the information provided in the situation, reflecting convergent thinking. Under minority influence, on the other hand, the individual is free to deal with the issue under consideration in her or his own terms. This results in divergent thinking, a processing mode in which the target of influence considers self-generated novel information in a creative manner. Drawing on the heuristic-systematic model of persuasion (Chaiken et al., 1989) Moskowitz (1996; Moskowitz & Chaiken, 2001) proposed that positive attributions toward the minority or other conditions such as surprise motivate recipients to systematically process the minority s arguments (see also Baker & Petty, 1994; De Vries, De Dreu, Gordijn, & Schuurman, 1996). These recent approaches relate to Moscovici s idea that minority influence rests on the careful processing of issue-related information. Yet, they deny that a source s minority status in itself suffices for motivating targets to process their message thoroughly. In addition, they hold that mere processing of minority arguments does not necessarily lead to attitude change. Indeed, attitude change depends on the quality of the arguments presented: Only strong and convincing arguments, when processed, will produce attitude change in the intended direction, whereas weak arguments will not (see Bohner, Moskowitz, & Chaiken, 1995). Integrative Attempts The Leniency Contract Model A recently developed integrative theory is the leniency contract model (Alvaro & Crano, 1997; Crano, 2001; Crano & Chen, 1998). It is of particular interest here because Crano and his colleagues expanded CT by integrating assumptions of SIT (Tajfel, 1981) and dual-process theories of persuasion (Chaiken et al., 1989; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a, 1986b). As a result, their model contains aspects of both informational and normative influence. A major stipulation imported from SIT-based research on social influence (e.g., David & Turner, 2001) is that majority or minority messages impact hinges on the identity-relevance of both the source (is it

12 Social Influence and Persuasion 12 in-group or out-group?) and the topic (is it group-defining?). Only in-group sources are considered as relevant influence agents, because accepting or rejecting their message may have consequences for the individual s standing within the group. If an influence target perceives a counterattitudinal position within its in-group, the following chain of events is set in motion. If the source is a majority, the target will check if the source s position is legitimate, that is, whether it is central to group concerns: If it is not, the target may ignore or dismiss the majority message. If, however, the message pertains to a central, group-defining topic and is counterattitudinal, the target will experience threat and will be motivated to process the message with high effort. Depending on the quality of message arguments, this effortful processing may lead to either short-term change (if arguments are weak) or long-term change on the focal issue (if arguments are strong). The latter prediction is partly in line with dual-process models of persuasion; viewed from their perspective, perceived identity-relevance of the majority message increases the motivation to process that message with high effort. In this respect, the leniency model prediction deviates from conversion theory, which does not provide for content elaboration or long-term attitude change in response to majority influence. In another respect, however, the leniency contract model converges with conversion theory: Majority influence is not assumed to generalize to related issues. This is because elaboration of the majority message is seen as motivated by the need to fit in, not to extract the gist of the message (Crano, 2001, p. 137). A different situation arises if the source is an ingroup minority. Whether legitimate or not, a minority message will always be perceived as novel and unexpected. If its position is seen as threatening the existence of the group, the minority is likely to be ostracized and thus become viewed as an out-group; its message may then be ignored or derogated (cf. Festinger, 1950). If the minority s position does not threaten group concerns, however, majority members will be open to considering the minority s arguments. This is where the leniency contract comes into

13 Social Influence and Persuasion 13 play. Majority members will elaborate the minority s arguments with leniency and refrain from counterarguing. In exchange for this tolerant treatment of the minority, it is tacitly understood that the majority will not change its position on the focal issue. The leniency contract thus provides a mechanism by which groups may permit some degree of variance in expressed opinions while maintaining coherence and positive relationships within the group. At the cognitive level, however, the open-minded elaboration of the minority message has an unintended effect: It creates pressure toward change, not in the focal attitude (which is contractually protected from change), but in other, related attitudes, which are linked to the focal attitude through the spreading of activation in a cognitive network (Anderson & Bower, 1973). The result is indirect attitude change, which may further lead to delayed focal attitude change, especially if the message arguments are strong. The complexity of these assumed mechanisms may explain why findings in the minority influence literature are relatively mixed in terms of the levels and time delays at which attitude change has been observed (Wood et al., 1994). Empirical evidence for the leniency contract model mainly comes from two publications: The first is a paper by Alvaro and Crano (1997) which identified two attitude issues that were related according to a multidimensional scaling of attitude judgments: gay men in the military and gun control. Importantly, however, participants did not perceive the two issues to be related if asked whether change in one attitude might lead to change in the other. Students exposed to an in-group minority message arguing against gay men in the military showed no focal change on this attitude object; they did, however, become more opposed to gun control, thereby showing indirect attitude change in line with the conservative appeal of the anti-gay message. In a follow-up study, Alvaro and Crano reversed the roles of focal and related topic and found comparable results. In neither of these studies did in-group majority messages have any impact on participants attitudes, presumably because the message topics were not identity-relevant to university students as a group.

14 Social Influence and Persuasion 14 Crano and Chen (1998, Study 3) further provided some correlational evidence for the assumption that indirect attitude change may be followed by focal change after some delay. In their study, students read a minority or majority message arguing for a mandatory, unpaid university service for students (obviously a counterattitudinal position). Later, the students attitudes toward this focal issue and a related issue (tuition increase) were assessed both immediately and after one week. Analyses within the recipients who had been exposed to a minority position revealed that those students who showed greater immediate indirect change also showed greater delayed focal change. Overall, however, focal posttest attitudes did not differ from pretest levels for minority influence. In this study, unlike in Alvaro and Crano (1997), majority messages did affect students attitudes at both immediate and delayed tests. This is in line with the leniency model s hypothesis that majorities will exert substantial impact if the issue is group-defining. Although empirical evidence for the model s predictions is somewhat limited, the leniency contract model provides an interesting extension of conversion theory. It integrates the largely disparate theoretical traditions of social influence and persuasion research and specifies the interplay of normative and informational aspects, thereby generating a number of new predictions. Crano and his colleagues further introduced a novel empirical paradigm, including a quantitative technique, based on multidimensional scaling, for deriving well-defined indirect attitude objects, thus opening an avenue for greater precision in studying social influence at the private, indirect level an issue of some concern in previous literature reviews (e.g., Wood et al., 1994). Other aspects of the leniency contract model, however, seem to warrant critical investigation. First, is it plausible to assume that a social agreement as complex as the leniency contract would operate at a fully unconscious level? To further support this idea, it would be useful if specific evidence for the presence of leniency on the one hand and the proposed lack of

15 Social Influence and Persuasion 15 couterarguing on the other could be provided. It might also be useful to explore potential links to other theories of implicit social cognition (e.g., Greenwald et al., 2002). Secondly, some of the stages in the chain of events that is assumed to be set in motion by encountering a counterattitudinal in-group message have not been empirically tested yet. For example, would in-group minorities that present group-threatening positions indeed routinely be excluded? According to Festinger s (1950) theory of informal social communication, pressure toward communicating with the deviant group members would be an alternative result of encountering a threatening minority, whereas the expulsion of deviant group members might constitute only the ultima ratio if communication failed to restore group consensus. Finally, in attempting to subsume a broad range of phenomena under a common theoretical umbrella, Crano and his colleagues have specified a rather complex array of conditions, such that their theory may lack the parsimony needed for a general model of social influence. In the next section, we present our own attempt at formulating a more parsimonious general model. The Mere Consensus Approach We propose to reduce scientific explanation of the phenomena reported in social influence research to the operation of a single variable, namely consensus. Thus, our mere consensus approach (Erb & Bohner, 2001, 2006) often provides a more parsimonious explanation for these phenomena than do other models. It builds on the assumption that consensus is the defining feature of minorities and majorities in any context (see also Kruglanski & Mackie, 1990). A majority necessarily reflects higher consensus on a given issue than does a minority. Defining minorities and majorities by consensus alone, we deliberately exclude other variables that have been treated as explanatory constructs in other models. In empirical research, we found that consensus had a number of effects on information processing and judgment formation, even under the impoverished conditions used in our studies, where recipients were provided with consensus information independent of any effect of social conflict, tendencies to

16 Social Influence and Persuasion 16 identify with the influence group, social power, and so forth (e.g., Erb et al., 1998). Consensus information biased the processing of issue-related information, causing more favorable thoughts under high consensus and less favorable thoughts under low consensus, compared with control conditions where no consensus information was provided. Moreover, mere consensus influenced the divergence versus convergence of cognitive responding (cf. Nemeth, 1986). Under low consensus, respondents came up with new and original ideas about the issue that diverged from the information presented, whereas under high consensus, their processing focused more narrowly on the information presented in the influence setting. Going beyond Nemeth s theorizing, where dissent between recipients attitudes and the influence group s position was assigned major explanatory relevance, our studies showed that consensus that was completely detached from prior attitudes proved to be sufficient to affect the divergence of thought. In sum, this research demonstrated that consensus itself detached from other variables formerly proposed to shape minority and majority impact produces evaluative and cognitive consequences. As such, mere consensus effects can be used as a starting point to analyze the impact of other variables previously examined in the realm of minority and majority influence. One such variable is social conflict, defined as an opinion discrepancy between the position held by the target of influence and the position proposed by the influence source. Social conflict has been accorded major explanatory relevance in Moscovici s (1980) CT (see above). Still, conflict may be present in a social influence situation to a higher or lower degree and at times may be completely absent. We (Erb, Bohner, Rank & Einwiller, 2002) hypothesized that the absence of conflict may be the reason why Mackie (1987, see above) consistently found that majorities instigated targets to invest more processing effort than did minorities. According to Mackie s objective consensus approach, the majority position is accepted as reflecting objective reality (Mackie, 1987, p. 42) and is much more likely than the minority position to be translated from mere opinion into social reality. Consequently, a recipient s attention will be directed to the

17 Social Influence and Persuasion 17 majority message, which results in extensive processing of that message. A minority, on the other hand, lacks objective consensus. Hence, as it appears to make no sense to waste cognitive energy on processing a position which is likely to be incorrect, the minority message will be processed less extensively. Seemingly, the objective consensus approach is difficult to reconcile with Moscovici s CT where minority influence was expected to produce more extensive message processing. The contradiction between Moscovici s (1980) and Mackie s (1987) findings points to the possibility that moderating factors need to be taken into account (Wood, et al., 1994, p. 337). In doing so, we speculated that the attitudes used in Mackie s experiments were probably not related to strong convictions on the part of recipients. The issues (e.g., the U.S. should act to ensure a military balance in the Western hemisphere ) were relatively unfamiliar to participants; the situation was therefore not likely to induce strong conflict when compared with situations where one s own perceptual ability or deep concern with environmental issues are challenged (cf. Moscovici, 1980). In other words, we predicted consensus to interact with recipients prior attitudes in its effect on the level of message processing. Our studies showed that recipients who held a strong attitude that was in conflict with the forwarded position dedicated more processing effort to minority than to majority messages. Conversely, participants whose moderate prior attitudes were not in strong conflict with the influence source s position processed the majority message more extensively than the minority message (Erb, Bohner et al., 2002). We were thus able to reconcile seemingly contradictory findings in the literature by Moscovici (1980) and Mackie (1987) by studying the effect of consensus in interaction with conflict. In a related study, Bohner, Dykema-Engblade, Tindale, and Meisenhelder (2006) showed that salient situational concerns with accuracy versus social comparison may determine whether majority or minority positions on a novel issue are processed more or less extensively. Participants for whom social comparison concerns had been primed processed minority

18 Social Influence and Persuasion 18 arguments more extensively than majority arguments a finding consistent with Moscovici s (1980) idea that social concerns decrease the processing of majority arguments. Participants for whom accuracy concerns had been primed were generally sensitive to the presented arguments, independent of variations in consensus a finding inconsistent with Moscovici s theory. Taken together, these findings encourage to search for other variables that interact with consensus in order to clarify contradictory theorizing and empirical findings in the literature. Mere consensus effects can also be used as a starting point to study minority influence. Following Moscovici, researchers have generally treated low consensus as a negative characteristic: What the minority does is bad because there are few who do it (Moscovici, 1980, p. 210). Accordingly, research efforts were devoted to identifying conditions that promote minority influence despite the drawback that the minority s position lacks high consensus. A full discussion of all pertinent models is beyond the present scope, but some examples may illustrate this concern with overcoming seemingly inevitable disadvantages inherent in minority status. According to CT itself, the key to minority influence lies in the consistent behavioral style and the positive attributions such behavior elicits; inconsistent minorities, on the other hand, would not be influential. In the framework of self-categorization theory (e.g., David & Turner, 2001; Turner, 1991), minority influence rests on the fact that minorities are (still) part of the in-group, thus taking advantage of the majority members' interest in maintaining group coherence and an overall positive group evaluation. In a similar vein, the leniency-contract model (Crano, 2001) holds that minority influence hinges on the leniency toward deviant in-group members. In Latané's social impact theory (Latané & Wolf, 1981; Nowak, Szamrej, & Latané, 1990), the minority can surmount the disadvantage of being small in number by the immediacy (e.g., physical closeness) and strength (e.g., social power) of their appeal. In sum, research was mainly concerned with the discovery of conditions where minorities are influential in spite of the fact that their position enjoys only little social support. Moreover, much of the minority influence

19 Social Influence and Persuasion 19 studied referred to the hidden impact of minorities (Maass & Clark, 1984), that is indirect influence on issues related to the focal issue of the influence attempt, rather than direct influence on the focal issue (for reviews, see Tanford & Penrod, 1984; Wood et al., 1994). By contrast, instead of asking under what conditions a minority may overcome the obstacle of being small in number, we take our consensus perspective as a starting point for examining conditions that make the low consensus attached to a minority itself appear attractive to influence targets. Under such conditions a minority is expected to exert greater influence than a majority, even on the focal issue of influence. What might such conditions be? There is a considerable amount of research suggesting that events, characteristics, and objects are evaluated more extremely the lower their perceived prevalence. For example, success will have more positive consequences, and failure more negative consequences, if consensus among actors is low, whereas consequences will be less extreme when consensus is high (e.g., Kelley, 1967). Research on the scarcity principle has shown that identical characteristics were evaluated more extremely the lower their perceived prevalence (e.g., Ditto & Jemmott, 1989). Research on commodity theory (e.g., Brock & Brannon, 1992) and self-categorization (e.g., Simon & Hamilton, 1994) also attest to the idea that evaluative judgments are more extreme when the object or characteristic is scarce. As minorities hold positions of low consensus, it is only a small step to assume that minority positions represent an object of evaluative extremity. If a certain position is correct, it appears more positive and appealing to share it with a minority, but less positive to share it with a majority. If a position is incorrect, however, it appears more negative and less appealing to share it with a minority of others, but not quite as negative to share it with a majority. We propose that this asymmetry in valence has an important consequence: Evaluative extremity renders the minority position a risk-inclined option in the sense that consequences are either very positive or very negative (i.e., one appears foolish if one is wrong, but brilliant if right). By

20 Social Influence and Persuasion 20 the same token, there is safety in numbers, as the old adage goes. It is safe to yield to majority influence, because any consequence is less extreme if shared with many others. It does not hurt much to be wrong if most others were wrong as well, but neither is it very rewarding to be right if that was true of most others as well. Accordingly, targets of influence will tend toward the minority position if they are inclined to make a risky decision. In our research (Erb, Bohner, Hilton, Krings, & Büscher, 2006), participants indeed judged an actor s decision to follow a minority (vs. a majority) as more rash, risk-loving, adventurous, risky, but less security-oriented and over-anxious. These results support the prediction that others judgments and decisions are perceived as more risky when representing an object of low consensus. But would participants themselves yield to minority influence under preference for risk, when their own decisions and attitudes were at stake? We explored this possibility in three studies. To induce preference for risky versus cautious judgments, we used a priming procedure introduced by Erb, Bioy, and Hilton (2002): Participants were asked to rank-order adjectives according to their frequency of use. In the riskseeking condition, the set of adjectives included primes with positive connotations for riskseeking (e.g., enterprising ) and with negative connotations for risk-avoidance (e.g., overanxious ). In the risk-avoidance condition, these were replaced by primes with negative connotations for risk-seeking (e.g., rash ) and positive connotations for risk-avoidance (e.g., conscientious ). After the priming, in an ostensibly unrelated second study, participants received a persuasive message that was said to come from either a minority or a majority of others. The basic finding was that recipients in the risk-seeking conditions were more influenced by the minority than by the majority, whereas recipients in the risk-avoiding conditions showed an opposite pattern. The effect was independent of whether the influence group represented an in-group or an out-group. However, the effect of priming on preference for the low or high consensus position

21 Social Influence and Persuasion 21 should disappear when concerns about accuracy are no longer relevant to the judgmental process assumed to be underlying the effect. Empirically, we found that the effect of priming disappeared when the influence group was portrayed to represent a minority or majority of experts on the topic under consideration. Obviously, inferences drawn from the assumption that experts are correct canceled out the effect of the risk priming. This result speaks to the idea that accuracy concerns are driving the observed effects. If an individual follows the minority, he or she will feel foolish his or her decision if the minority position turns out to be incorrect but will brilliant if this position turns out to be correct. To share a position with a majority of others would be safe, however, because there are many who are wrong or right. This research provides a unique example for a condition under which the minority position in itself appears more attractive to targets than does the majority position. Minority influence exceeded majority influence on the focal issue. Most recently, Imhoff and Erb (2006) started researching another factor that may render low consensus attractive to targets of influence, namely deindividuation. Deindividuation is a state in which an individual feels indistinguishable from other individuals (e.g., Zimbardo, 1969). Research suggests that individuals at times strive to avoid deindividuation (e.g., Snyder & Fromkin, 1980) and seek optimal distinctiveness (e.g., Brewer, 1991). Accordingly, a majority position would be less attractive than a minority position for recipients who feel deindividuated, because sharing a high consensus position would magnify the feeling of being a non-distinct member of a silent majority. On the other hand, a low consensus position represents something special, something of particular distinction, and thus allows one to regain their individual status. In a first study linking deindividuation and minority influence, Imhoff and Erb (2006) used the test feedback method (Snyder & Fromkin, 1980) to induce deindividuation. Specifically, participants received bogus personality feedback suggesting that their personality

22 Social Influence and Persuasion 22 was either average (deindividuation condition) or specific (control condition). Imhoff and Erb predicted that participants in the average condition would experience deindividuation. In an ostensibly unrelated second experiment, participants were exposed to a persuasive message on a topic promoted by either a minority or a majority of others. Attitude judgments in the deindividuation condition were indeed more favorable under minority influence than under majority influence, whereas an opposite pattern was found in the control condition. In a second study, deindividuated participants were given the opportunity to regain individuation by expressing personality characteristics that would make them unique as compared to other individuals. In this condition minority influence did not occur. Research on minority influence under risk preference and deindividuation goes beyond previous work on minority influence in that minority status is not regarded as a negative feature of the influence source to begin with. Other models of minority influence discuss conditions that foster minority influence despite the drawback that minorities represent positions of low consensus. If minority status is regarded as negative by default it makes little sense to inquire into conditions where minority status appears attractive to recipients. We found the notion that minority status inevitably denotes a negative feature suspect, chose a different approach, and asked whether there exist conditions that render the minority s low consensus appealing. And indeed, conditions exist that render minorities influential because they represent sources of low consensus. Such conditions increase the influence of low consensus sources on the focal issue (and not only on related issues) and, at the same time, reduce the influence of high consensus sources, thus interacting with consensus information. Guided by the fundamental insight that consensus is the only variable that necessarily covaries with minority and majority status, research within the mere consensus approach thus yielded three conclusions: (1) Consensus proved sufficient to produce effects on judgment formation and processing that in other models were less parsimoniously explained with

23 Social Influence and Persuasion 23 variables like social conflict, identification, dissent and so forth. (2) Beyond mere consensus effects, we found variables that interact with consensus in predictable ways (e.g., conflict with prior attitudes, risk-seeking, deindividuation). (3) Variables that produce mere main effects for both minorities and majorities (and presumably other influence sources as well) do not represent viable explanations for minority and majority influence for example, behavioral consistency (Moscovici, 1980), or tendencies to identify with the influence group (or any other source; e.g., David & Turner, 2001) likely promote the impact of any source (for further discussion, see Erb & Bohner, 2006). The distinction between normative and informational influence is thus irrelevant to the mere consensus approach. Recipients respond to consensus information, a variable to which the human mind may be specifically tuned (Erb & Bohner, 2001; Prislin & Wood, 2005), and other variables may interact with consensus in its effect on judgments, but there is no need to classify these variables in terms of whether they relate to the issue under consideration or some property of the influence group that would elicit tendencies towards identification. Instead, the mere consensus approach classifies variables in terms of whether they do or do not interact with consensus in their effects on processing and judgment formation in minority and majority influence contexts. PERSUASION So far, we have reviewed theories in the tradition of Asch s (1956) research on interpersonal and group-based influence. The social context in which such influence occurs was the main concern of this tradition, though more recent developments have incorporated assumptions introduced by models of persuasion into their theorizing. We now turn to the discussion of persuasion, and provide a review of dual-process models that have dominated persuasion research for a long time, as well as a recently developed single-process alternative. Dual-Process Models

24 Social Influence and Persuasion 24 Persuasion research since the mid-1980s has mainly been based on dual-process models: the elaboration likelihood model (ELM; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a, 1986b; Petty & Wegener, 1999) and the heuristic-systematic model (HSM; Bohner et al.,1995; Chaiken, 1987; Chaiken et al., 1989; Chen & Chaiken, 1999). Both distinguish two qualitatively distinct modes of persuasion that form the endpoints of a continuum of processing effort. We will briefly outline their basic assumptions (see also Bohner & Wänke, 2002) and relate these assumptions to the distinction between informational and normative influence. The two modes of the ELM are called the central route, where persuasion is the result of targets' effortful scrutiny of message arguments and other issue-relevant information, and the peripheral route, where persuasion is mainly affected by peripheral cues (e.g., source likability, length of a message) through a variety of less effortful processing mechanisms (e.g., evaluative conditioning, mere exposure, heuristic processing). The HSM distinguishes between high-effort systematic processing, a mode largely identical to the central route, and low-effort heuristic processing, the specific application of highly accessible simple rules (e.g., "experts' statements are valid") in combination with salient and applicable cues in the persuasion setting (e.g., a communicator's academic degree). The ELM posits that "people are motivated to hold correct attitudes" (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a, p. 6, emphasis ours). Similarly, accuracy motivation, meaning the objective of holding valid attitudes, features prominently in the HSM (e.g., Chaiken et al., 1989). This processing goal sets the stage for informational influence, and each of the two processing modes just outlined is seen as a viable means for obtaining this goal. As such, scrutiny of complex content arguments and reliance on a heuristic such as "high consensus implies correctness" would be seen as functionally equivalent. However, this is not the whole story. As the dual-processing models developed further, other processing objectives were explicitly included. Even in the earliest renditions of the ELM,

Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives. Chapter 3 Outline -Attitude Change

Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives. Chapter 3 Outline -Attitude Change Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives 1) An understanding of how both internal mental processes and external influences lead to attitude change 2) An understanding of when and how behavior which is inconsistent

More information

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1 INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1 5.1 Clinical Interviews: Background Information The clinical interview is a technique pioneered by Jean Piaget, in 1975,

More information

Chapter 13. Social Psychology

Chapter 13. Social Psychology Social Psychology Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? Social Psychology Interpreting the Behavior of Others Behaving in the Presence of Others Establishing Relations With Others Social

More information

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT Van Der Velde / Guide to Business Research Methods First Proof 6.11.2003 4:53pm page 1 Part I PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT Van Der Velde / Guide to Business Research Methods First Proof 6.11.2003 4:53pm

More information

Resisting Persuasion by Illegitimate Means: A Metacognitive Perspective on Minority Influence

Resisting Persuasion by Illegitimate Means: A Metacognitive Perspective on Minority Influence Resisting Persuasion by Illegitimate Means: A Metacognitive Perspective on Minority Influence Zakary L. Tormala Victoria L. DeSensi Indiana University Richard E. Petty Ohio State University The present

More information

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies Emma ter Mors 1,2, Mieneke Weenig 1, Naomi Ellemers 1, Dancker Daamen 1 1 Leiden University,

More information

Groups, norms, and conformity. What s it about?

Groups, norms, and conformity. What s it about? 9 Groups, norms, and conformity What s it about? (Social Psychology pp. 310 350) This chapter is about how, when, and why people conform to group norms. People conform to group norms because of their need

More information

De invloed van overredende boodschappen met meerderheids- of minderheidssteun Schuurman, Mieke Karin

De invloed van overredende boodschappen met meerderheids- of minderheidssteun Schuurman, Mieke Karin University of Groningen De invloed van overredende boodschappen met meerderheids- of minderheidssteun Schuurman, Mieke Karin IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's

More information

SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY. Social Influences on the Self. Self Concept. How do we see ourselves? How do we see others?

SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY. Social Influences on the Self. Self Concept. How do we see ourselves? How do we see others? SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Social Cognition and Influence (how we think about ourselves) Social Influences on the Self How do we see ourselves? How do we see others? How do we compare ourselves with others? Self

More information

Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams

Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams The current plan is that there are 5 exams with 50 multiple choice items that will cover two chapters. Each chapter is planned to have 25 multiple choice items.

More information

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists 4,000 116,000 120M Open access books available International authors and editors Downloads Our

More information

CHAPTER 15. Social Psychology. Lecture Overview. Introductory Definition PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY. Social Cognition.

CHAPTER 15. Social Psychology. Lecture Overview. Introductory Definition PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY. Social Cognition. Social Psychology CHAPTER 15 Social Cognition Lecture Overview Social Influence Social Relations Applying Social Psychology to Social Problems Introductory Definition Social Psychology: scientific study

More information

Kantor Behavioral Profiles

Kantor Behavioral Profiles Kantor Behavioral Profiles baseline name: date: Kantor Behavioral Profiles baseline INTRODUCTION Individual Behavioral Profile In our earliest social system the family individuals explore a range of behavioral

More information

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d.

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d. Chapter 10: Cognitive Processing of Attitudes 1. McGuire s (1969) model of information processing can be best described as which of the following? a. Sequential b. Parallel c. Automatic 2. Which of the

More information

Factors that affect interpersonal attraction:

Factors that affect interpersonal attraction: Introduction: You are a member of a social world on a planet containing about 7 billion people. Our behaviors differ in different settings (like if we are in the college, market, or at home). Human behavior

More information

How do Minorities influence Majorities? Critically examine with reference to relevant Theories.

How do Minorities influence Majorities? Critically examine with reference to relevant Theories. How do Minorities influence Majorities? Critically examine with reference to relevant Theories. There are a variety of factors that can influence how a minority group is able to influence a majority group.

More information

Do Attitudes Affect Memory? Tests of the Congeniality Hypothesis

Do Attitudes Affect Memory? Tests of the Congeniality Hypothesis 5 Do Attitudes Affect Memory? Tests of the Congeniality Hypothesis Alice H. Eagly, 1 Patrick Kulesa, Serena Chen, and Shelly Chaiken Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois

More information

Hans-Joachim Mosler: Better Be Convincing or Better Be Stylish?

Hans-Joachim Mosler: Better Be Convincing or Better Be Stylish? Copyright JASSS Hans-Joachim Mosler (2006) Better Be Convincing or Better Be Stylish? a Theory Based Multi- Agent Simulation to Explain Minority Influence in Groups Via Arguments or Via Peripheral Cues

More information

A Connectionist Model of Attitude Strength and Change

A Connectionist Model of Attitude Strength and Change A Connectionist Model of Attitude Strength and Change Brian M. Monroe (monroe@usc.edu) Department of Psychology, University of Southern California Los Angeles, CA 90089 USA Abstract A localist constraint

More information

CHAPTER 1 SYNOPSIS OF THE THESIS 1.1 MOTIVATION AND STRUCTURE OF THE THESIS 1 SYNOPSIS OF THE THESIS MOTIVATION OF THE THESIS

CHAPTER 1 SYNOPSIS OF THE THESIS 1.1 MOTIVATION AND STRUCTURE OF THE THESIS 1 SYNOPSIS OF THE THESIS MOTIVATION OF THE THESIS 1 CHAPTER 1 SYNOPSIS OF THE THESIS 1.1 MOTIVATION AND STRUCTURE OF THE THESIS 1.1.1 MOTIVATION OF THE THESIS The social identity approach, comprising the theories of social identity and its cognitive derivate

More information

Energizing Behavior at Work: Expectancy Theory Revisited

Energizing Behavior at Work: Expectancy Theory Revisited In M. G. Husain (Ed.), Psychology and society in an Islamic perspective (pp. 55-63). New Delhi: Institute of Objective Studies, 1996. Energizing Behavior at Work: Expectancy Theory Revisited Mahfooz A.

More information

reward based power have ability to give you what you want. coercive have power to punish

reward based power have ability to give you what you want. coercive have power to punish Chapter 7 Finding and Using Negotiation Power Why Power Important to Negotiators? Seeking power in negotiations from 1 of 2 perceptions: 1. Negotiator believes he has less power than other party 2. Negotiator

More information

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution 1 Robert D. Mather University of Central Oklahoma Charles M. Mather University of Science

More information

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology:

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Defining Psychology Behaviorism: The scientific study of how rewards and punishment in the environment affect human and non-human behavior Empirical approach: vary contingencies of

More information

6. A theory that has been substantially verified is sometimes called a a. law. b. model.

6. A theory that has been substantially verified is sometimes called a a. law. b. model. Chapter 2 Multiple Choice Questions 1. A theory is a(n) a. a plausible or scientifically acceptable, well-substantiated explanation of some aspect of the natural world. b. a well-substantiated explanation

More information

Chapter 14. Social Psychology. How Does the Social Situation Affect our Behavior? Social Psychology

Chapter 14. Social Psychology. How Does the Social Situation Affect our Behavior? Social Psychology Chapter 14 Social Psychology This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited by law: Any public performance or display, including transmission of

More information

Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior

Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior Multiple Choice Questions 1. A theory is a(n): A. plausible or scientifically acceptable, well-substantiated explanation of some aspect of the

More information

Note:- Receptors are the person who receives any images from outer environment.

Note:- Receptors are the person who receives any images from outer environment. Concept According to Oxford Advanced Learner s Dictionary, Perception means the way you notice things especially with the senses. Perception is the process of organizing and attempting to understand the

More information

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about?

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about? 2 Asking and answering research questions What s it about? (Social Psychology pp. 24 54) Social psychologists strive to reach general conclusions by developing scientific theories about why people behave

More information

Defining Social Psychology. informal definition: the study of how people think about, influence, and relate to other people

Defining Social Psychology. informal definition: the study of how people think about, influence, and relate to other people Social Psychology Defining Social Psychology informal definition: the study of how people think about, influence, and relate to other people formal definition: the study of how a person s thoughts, feelings,

More information

Evaluation Models STUDIES OF DIAGNOSTIC EFFICIENCY

Evaluation Models STUDIES OF DIAGNOSTIC EFFICIENCY 2. Evaluation Model 2 Evaluation Models To understand the strengths and weaknesses of evaluation, one must keep in mind its fundamental purpose: to inform those who make decisions. The inferences drawn

More information

PSYC 210 Social Psychology

PSYC 210 Social Psychology South Central College PSYC 210 Social Psychology Course Information Description Total Credits 4.00 Pre/Corequisites PSYC100 or consent of instructor. Course Competencies Social Psychology introduces the

More information

attitude the theory that we explain someone's behavior by crediting the situation or the person's disposition attribution theory

attitude the theory that we explain someone's behavior by crediting the situation or the person's disposition attribution theory attitude feelings, often influenced by our beliefs, that predispose us to respond in a particular way to objects, people, and events attribution theory the theory that we explain someone's behavior by

More information

Communicating Market Scarcity: The Role of Information Congruity in Shaping the Persuasiveness of Time Restriction

Communicating Market Scarcity: The Role of Information Congruity in Shaping the Persuasiveness of Time Restriction International Journal of Business and Social Science Vol. 2 No. 23 [Special Issue December 2011] Communicating Market Scarcity: The Role of Information Congruity in Shaping the Persuasiveness of Time Restriction

More information

33 Multiple choice questions

33 Multiple choice questions NAME 33 Multiple choice questions 1. central route a. Aspect of persuasion, emotional appeals are quite powerful. Negative appeals play to the audience's b. Aspect of persuasion, whether or not someone

More information

A B C. Copyright Allyn and Bacon 2005

A B C. Copyright Allyn and Bacon 2005 A B C 1 A B C 2 A B C 3 A B C 4 Asch, 1951 How many of you own something from one of these companies? 5 Fads 6 Lecture 10 Social Influence 7 Social Influence Have you ever: Asked a friend what was appropriate

More information

43. Can subliminal messages affect behavior? o Subliminal messages have NO effect on behavior - but people perceive that their behavior changed.

43. Can subliminal messages affect behavior? o Subliminal messages have NO effect on behavior - but people perceive that their behavior changed. 34. How does the idea of mindlessness relate to persuasion? What is the Elaboration Likelihood Model? What are two routes of the ELM? What is the differences between the two routes? Many decisions are

More information

Non-smokers rights movement begins Per capita cigarette consumption First medical reports linking smoking and cancer.

Non-smokers rights movement begins Per capita cigarette consumption First medical reports linking smoking and cancer. 5 Non-smokers rights movement begins Per capita cigarette consumption 4 3 2 1 First medical reports linking smoking and cancer End of WW II Great Depression 19 191 192 193 194 195 Years US Surgeon General

More information

Social Psychology. What We Will Cover in This Section. Roles. PDF Created with deskpdf PDF Writer - Trial ::

Social Psychology. What We Will Cover in This Section. Roles. PDF Created with deskpdf PDF Writer - Trial :: Social Psychology Dr. Dennis C. Sweeney What We Will Cover in This Section Conformity Attitudes Group Behavior Prejudice Roles Socially defined expectations that we expect individuals to fulfill in a given

More information

Chapter Seven Attitude Change

Chapter Seven Attitude Change Chapter Seven Attitude Change Rationalizing Our Own Behaviour: Cognitive Dissonance Theory Feeling Bad About Irrational Behaviour: The Arousal Dissonance Cognitive dissonance theory proposed by Leon Festinger

More information

5. is the process of moving from the specific to the general. a. Deduction

5. is the process of moving from the specific to the general. a. Deduction Applied Social Psychology Understanding and Addressing Social and Practical Problems 3rd Edition Gruman Test Bank Full Download: https://testbanklive.com/download/applied-social-psychology-understanding-and-addressing-social-and-practical-p

More information

To conclude, a theory of error must be a theory of the interaction between human performance variability and the situational constraints.

To conclude, a theory of error must be a theory of the interaction between human performance variability and the situational constraints. The organisers have provided us with a both stimulating and irritating list of questions relating to the topic of the conference: Human Error. My first intention was to try to answer the questions one

More information

Effects of task ambiguity and other contextual factors on majority and minority influence

Effects of task ambiguity and other contextual factors on majority and minority influence Lehigh University Lehigh Preserve Theses and Dissertations 1992 Effects of task ambiguity and other contextual factors on majority and minority influence Kerry Koenemund Lehigh University Follow this and

More information

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Copyright 2015 McGraw-Hill Education. All rights reserved. No reproduction or distribution without the prior written consent of McGraw-Hill Education. Learning

More information

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology Preface p. xv Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology p. 3 Sociology, Psychology, or Both? p.

More information

SEMINAR ON SERVICE MARKETING

SEMINAR ON SERVICE MARKETING SEMINAR ON SERVICE MARKETING Tracy Mary - Nancy LOGO John O. Summers Indiana University Guidelines for Conducting Research and Publishing in Marketing: From Conceptualization through the Review Process

More information

9/5/ Research Hazards Awareness Training

9/5/ Research Hazards Awareness Training https://www.citiprogram.org/ Research Hazards Awareness Training luongo@oakland.edu https://www.oakland.edu/labsafety/training/ Committee Formed Sept. 27 Paper 20% Oct. 11 Exam 20% Nov. 13 Exam 20% Nov.

More information

-Attitude- Abdullah Nimer

-Attitude- Abdullah Nimer -Attitude- Abdullah Nimer Attitude refers to evaluation of things. The things can be concrete objects like cars or ideas like Marxism. Attitudes have: an affective component a cognitive component a behavioral

More information

The Good, the Bad and the Blameworthy: Understanding the Role of Evaluative Reasoning in Folk Psychology. Princeton University

The Good, the Bad and the Blameworthy: Understanding the Role of Evaluative Reasoning in Folk Psychology. Princeton University The Good, the Bad and the Blameworthy: Understanding the Role of Evaluative Reasoning in Folk Psychology Joshua Knobe Gabriel Mendlow Princeton University People ordinarily make sense of their own behavior

More information

A Model of Unethical Usage of Information Technology

A Model of Unethical Usage of Information Technology Association for Information Systems AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) AMCIS 2005 Proceedings Americas Conference on Information Systems (AMCIS) 2005 A Model of Unethical Usage of Information Technology Sutirtha

More information

Groups. Giovanni Masino - University of Ferrara

Groups. Giovanni Masino - University of Ferrara Groups 1 S T R U C T U R E A N D I N F L U E N C E Issues Norms and Roles Influence Performance and Decisions Leadership 2 Definitions 3 a general definition: Definitions a group is a set of two or more

More information

E G I H G T T AP A PROA O C A HES E T O T O AT A T T I T T I U T DE D

E G I H G T T AP A PROA O C A HES E T O T O AT A T T I T T I U T DE D Midterm followed by lecture. The lectures on attitudes are intended to provide you with guidance in conducting the team project. The next few lectures will not start or end as may be indicated by these

More information

The Influence of Spokesperson Trustworthiness on Message Elaboration, Attitude Strength, and Advertising Effectiveness

The Influence of Spokesperson Trustworthiness on Message Elaboration, Attitude Strength, and Advertising Effectiveness PRIESTER ENDORSER AND TRUSTWORTHINESS PETTY JOURNAL OF CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY, 13(4), 408 421 Copyright 2003, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Influence of Spokesperson Trustworthiness on Message Elaboration,

More information

Comparing Direct and Indirect Measures of Just Rewards: What Have We Learned?

Comparing Direct and Indirect Measures of Just Rewards: What Have We Learned? Comparing Direct and Indirect Measures of Just Rewards: What Have We Learned? BARRY MARKOVSKY University of South Carolina KIMMO ERIKSSON Mälardalen University We appreciate the opportunity to comment

More information

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration Dr. Chan-Chien Chiu, Associate Professor of Taiwan Hospitality & Tourism College, Taiwan ABSTRACT This

More information

More on Methodological Issues in Free-Response Psi Experiments

More on Methodological Issues in Free-Response Psi Experiments Other Methodology Articles More on Methodological Issues in Free-Response Psi Experiments J. E. KENNEDY (Original publication and copyright: Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 1979,

More information

EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY. Pages Social Identity 4:22

EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY. Pages Social Identity 4:22 EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY Pages 106 108 Social Identity 4:22 HENRI TAJFEL S SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY Individuals strive to improve their self image by trying to enhance their selfesteem, based on:

More information

Opinion Leaders or Opinion Followers? The Effect of Centrality on Susceptibility to Group Influence

Opinion Leaders or Opinion Followers? The Effect of Centrality on Susceptibility to Group Influence Opinion Leaders or Opinion Followers? The Effect of Centrality on Susceptibility to Group Influence Edith Shalev, Technion Israel Institute of Technology Rom Y. Schrift, The Wharton School, University

More information

Chapters 4 & 9: Dual-Process Theories of Behavior and Persuasion

Chapters 4 & 9: Dual-Process Theories of Behavior and Persuasion Chapters 4 & 9: Dual-Process Theories of Behavior and Persuasion Judgment & Behavior What s the relationship between attitudes and behavior? K-A-B Hierarchy (Knowledge-Attitude-Behavior) Wicker (1969)

More information

8 Attitudes and behavior. What s it about?

8 Attitudes and behavior. What s it about? 8 Attitudes and behavior What s it about? (Social Psychology pp. 274 309) Attitudes and actions are very closely related, and are often consistent, because they influence each other in both superficial

More information

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu Person Perception Forming Impressions of Others Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu Person Perception person perception: how we come to know about others temporary states, emotions, intentions and desires impression

More information

Persuasive Speech. Persuasive Speaking: Reasoning with Your Audience

Persuasive Speech. Persuasive Speaking: Reasoning with Your Audience Persuasive Speaking: Reasoning with Your Audience Persuasive Speech A speech whose goal is to influence the attitudes, beliefs, values, or behavior of audience members 1 Elaboration Likelihood Model People

More information

Models of Information Retrieval

Models of Information Retrieval Models of Information Retrieval Introduction By information behaviour is meant those activities a person may engage in when identifying their own needs for information, searching for such information in

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSCIOUSNESS AND PHENOMENAL CONSCIOUSNESS. Overview

PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSCIOUSNESS AND PHENOMENAL CONSCIOUSNESS. Overview Lecture 28-29 PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSCIOUSNESS AND PHENOMENAL CONSCIOUSNESS Overview David J. Chalmers in his famous book The Conscious Mind 1 tries to establish that the problem of consciousness as the hard

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

PSYC2600 Lecture One Attitudes

PSYC2600 Lecture One Attitudes PSYC2600 Lecture One Attitudes 16/07/14 7:01 AM Motivation (Lei, 2010) Intrinsic motivation have an internal motivation, you are interested. Extrinsic motivation have no specific interest, but are motivated

More information

2015 NADTA Conference Pre-Education Committee Book Club Everyday Bias, Howard J. Ross, Suggested Group Discussion Questions

2015 NADTA Conference Pre-Education Committee Book Club Everyday Bias, Howard J. Ross, Suggested Group Discussion Questions 2015 NADTA Conference Pre-Education Committee Book Club Everyday Bias, Howard J. Ross, Suggested Group Discussion Questions 1. After reading this book, which research examples stand out the most in your

More information

Chapter 15 Learning Objectives with SubQuestions PERSON PERCEPTION: FORMING IMPRESSIONS OF OTHERS

Chapter 15 Learning Objectives with SubQuestions PERSON PERCEPTION: FORMING IMPRESSIONS OF OTHERS Chapter 15 Learning Objectives with SubQuestions PERSON PERCEPTION: FORMING IMPRESSIONS OF OTHERS #1) Explain how schemas, stereotypes, and other factors contribute to subjectivity in person perception

More information

Exploring the reference point in prospect theory

Exploring the reference point in prospect theory 3 Exploring the reference point in prospect theory Gambles for length of life Exploring the reference point in prospect theory: Gambles for length of life. S.M.C. van Osch, W.B. van den Hout, A.M. Stiggelbout

More information

Social Psychology 10 (14)

Social Psychology 10 (14) Social Psychology 10 (14) KEY TERMS Attitude False-consensus effect Pluralistic ignorance Mere exposure effect Self-serving bias Attraction research Central versus peripheral Just-world bias Social facilitation

More information

PELLISSIPPI STATE COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER SYLLABUS SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY PSYC 2120

PELLISSIPPI STATE COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER SYLLABUS SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY PSYC 2120 PELLISSIPPI STATE COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER SYLLABUS SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY PSYC 2120 Class Hours: 3.0 Credit Hours: 3.0 Laboratory Hours: 0.0 Date Revised: Fall 2016 Catalog Course Description: Theories, methods

More information

Role of Health Education and Communication Part 3

Role of Health Education and Communication Part 3 Role of Health Education and Communication Part 3 Content Part 1 1. Introduction 2. Health 3. Health Education introduction 4. Health Promotion 5. Perception Part 2 6. Health Behaviour 7. Learning 8. Attribution

More information

QUESTIONING THE MENTAL HEALTH EXPERT S CUSTODY REPORT

QUESTIONING THE MENTAL HEALTH EXPERT S CUSTODY REPORT QUESTIONING THE MENTAL HEALTH EXPERT S CUSTODY REPORT by IRA DANIEL TURKAT, PH.D. Venice, Florida from AMERICAN JOURNAL OF FAMILY LAW, Vol 7, 175-179 (1993) There are few activities in which a mental health

More information

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz MULTIPLE CHOICE: 1. We usually adapt our behavior to the demands of the social situation, and in ambiguous situations: A) We take our cues from the behavior of others. B) We will do the same thing that

More information

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科目簡介

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科目簡介 COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科目簡介 COURSES FOR 4-YEAR UNDERGRADUATE PROGRAMMES PSY2101 Introduction to Psychology (3 credits) The purpose of this course is to introduce fundamental concepts and theories in psychology

More information

Choose an approach for your research problem

Choose an approach for your research problem Choose an approach for your research problem This course is about doing empirical research with experiments, so your general approach to research has already been chosen by your professor. It s important

More information

SOCI 323 Social Psychology

SOCI 323 Social Psychology SOCI 323 Session 9 SOCIAL PERCEPTION Lecturer: Dr. Peace Mamle Tetteh, Department of Sociology Contact Information: ptetteh@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education 2014/2015

More information

Conformity By CommonLit Staff 2014

Conformity By CommonLit Staff 2014 Name: Class: Conformity By CommonLit Staff 2014 Conformity is the act of matching one s beliefs or actions to the norms of a larger social group. Norms are the usually unspoken rules that govern the attitudes

More information

Recent theorizing in dual process models has focused

Recent theorizing in dual process models has focused Motivated Reasoning in Outcome-Bias Effects NIDHI AGRAWAL DURAIRAJ MAHESWARAN* In this research, we examine the conditions under which s bias judgments, and we provide insights on the processes underlying

More information

Psych 1Chapter 2 Overview

Psych 1Chapter 2 Overview Psych 1Chapter 2 Overview After studying this chapter, you should be able to answer the following questions: 1) What are five characteristics of an ideal scientist? 2) What are the defining elements of

More information

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments Consciousness and Cognition 9, 215 219 (2000) doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0438, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

More information

Chapter 3: Perception and the Self in IPC 01/24/2012

Chapter 3: Perception and the Self in IPC 01/24/2012 Chapter 3: Perception and the Self in IPC 01/24/2012 Section 1: The Self in Interpersonal Communication Self Concept Your self concept develops from at least four sources: Others Images: Cooley s concept

More information

Perception and Processing of Safe Driving Messages: A Multi-Method Approach

Perception and Processing of Safe Driving Messages: A Multi-Method Approach Perception and Processing of Safe Driving Messages: A Multi-Method Approach Nancy Rhodes University of Alabama June 4, 2008 General Approach Health beliefs Message processing Dual process approach Social

More information

Conformity ASCH S STUDY 12/8/2010 ASCH S STUDY. Social Psychology (581, ) Others Focus on Social Influences on Our Behavior

Conformity ASCH S STUDY 12/8/2010 ASCH S STUDY. Social Psychology (581, ) Others Focus on Social Influences on Our Behavior Social Psychology (581, 592-598) Study of how people behave, think, and feel in social situations Relatively recent addition to psychology Research influenced by current social problems Researchers in

More information

Economic incentives in household waste management: just a waste? A relational approach to agents and structures in household waste sorting

Economic incentives in household waste management: just a waste? A relational approach to agents and structures in household waste sorting Economic incentives in household waste management: just a waste? A relational approach to agents and structures in household waste sorting Økonomiske insentiver i forvaltningen av husstanders avfallssortering:

More information

This article, the last in a 4-part series on philosophical problems

This article, the last in a 4-part series on philosophical problems GUEST ARTICLE Philosophical Issues in Medicine and Psychiatry, Part IV James Lake, MD This article, the last in a 4-part series on philosophical problems in conventional and integrative medicine, focuses

More information

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Modified from Organizing Scientific Thinking Using the QuALMRI Framework Written by Kevin Ochsner and modified by others. Based on a scheme devised by

More information

Kahneman, Daniel. Thinking Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux, 2011.

Kahneman, Daniel. Thinking Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux, 2011. The accumulating research indicates that individuals cognitive and behavioral orientations to objects (their thoughts and actions) are frequently based on rapid shortcuts or heuristics. The past few decades

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

SOCIAL INFLUENCE: CONFORMITY

SOCIAL INFLUENCE: CONFORMITY SOCIAL INFLUENCE: CONFORMITY IB SYLLABUS SAYS: Evaluate research on conformity to group norms. Discuss factors influencing conformity. Textbook pages: 119-123 WHAT IS CONFORMITY? One of the key ways that

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM participant client HFM 03/13/201 This report was generated by the HFMtalentindex Online Assessment system. The data in this report are based on the answers given by the participant on one or more psychological

More information

SAMPLE. Social Psychology. Minority Influence & Social Change.

SAMPLE. Social Psychology. Minority Influence & Social Change. Social Psychology Minority Influence & Social Change Social Psychology Lesson Objectives: To outline and evaluate three aspects of minority influence, including: Consistency Commitment Flexibility Five,

More information

Running Head: NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 1

Running Head: NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 1 Running Head: NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 1 Coherence and Fidelity in Fisher s Narrative Paradigm Amy Kuhlman Wheaton College 13 November 2013 NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 2 Abstract This paper

More information

The Return of the One-Track Mind

The Return of the One-Track Mind The Return of the One-Track Mind Wolfgang Stroebe Department of Social and Organizational Psychology Utrecht University In their thoughtful and carefully argued target article, Kruglanski and Thompson

More information

Answers to end of chapter questions

Answers to end of chapter questions Answers to end of chapter questions Chapter 1 What are the three most important characteristics of QCA as a method of data analysis? QCA is (1) systematic, (2) flexible, and (3) it reduces data. What are

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH The following document provides background information on the research and development of the Emergenetics Profile instrument. Emergenetics Defined 1. Emergenetics

More information

Myers Psychology for AP*

Myers Psychology for AP* Myers Psychology for AP* David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2010 *AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board, which

More information