ABSTRACT. BRATTON, KEVIN M. Thought Suppression: Divergent Implications for Explicit and Implicit Memory. (Under the direction of Katherine W. Klein.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "ABSTRACT. BRATTON, KEVIN M. Thought Suppression: Divergent Implications for Explicit and Implicit Memory. (Under the direction of Katherine W. Klein."

Transcription

1 ABSTRACT BRATTON, KEVIN M. Thought Suppression: Divergent Implications for Explicit and Implicit Memory. (Under the direction of Katherine W. Klein.) The purpose of this research was to examine the relationship between suppression of a personal negative experience and performance on a task that included an explicit or recollection component, and an implicit or familiarity memory component. The study was a 2 (study; no suppression or suppression) X 2 (test; no suppression or suppression) X 2 (type; recollection or familiarity) X 2 (time; session one or session two) experimental design with study and test conditions as between groups variables and type and time as repeated within participants variables. Participants completed the first session of Jacoby s process dissociation task to obtain baseline scores. Participants were then randomly assigned to groups which differed on when they would be asked to engage in concurrent suppression. Control participants were asked to express and then suppress a telephone pole. The other three groups were asked to nominate a personal negative experience from their own lives. After practicing expressing and suppressing their target thought, all participants completed the second session of the memory task, with groups two through four continuing to suppress their target thought during either the study phase (condition 2), the test phase (condition 3), or during both study and test phases (condition 4). Participants in the control condition experienced an improvement on the explicit portion of the memory task, and a decline on the implicit portion of the memory task. Participants in conditions three and four experienced a decline in the explicit portion of the memory task, while their implicit scores remained unchanged. Findings are discussed in terms of Wegner's Ironic Processing model.

2 THOUGHT SUPPRESSION: DIVERGENT IMPLICATIONS FOR EXPLICIT AND IMPLICIT MEMORY by KEVIN MICHAEL BRATTON A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty of North Carolina State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy PSYCHOLOGY Raleigh, North Carolina 2007 APPROVED BY: Dr. Douglas Gillan Dr. Denis Gray Dr. Kitty Klein Chair of Advisory Committee Dr. Chris Mayhorn

3 ii BIOGRAPHY Kevin Michael Bratton was born in Champagne-Urbana, Illinois on August 12, He graduated from Northmont Senior High School in 1987 and entered the University of Toledo in the fall of He graduated in the fall of 1999 with a Bachelor of Arts in Psychology. Realizing his enjoyment of research, he entered the graduate program in Social Psychology at North Carolina State University in the fall of He earned his Masters of Science in Psychology in the fall of Kevin subsequently continued his education in the field of social psychology by attending the doctoral program at North Carolina State University. Throughout his graduate career, he has been active in research and teaching in the undergraduate program at North Carolina State University as well as at Central Carolina Community College. Kevin s post-graduation plan is to work as a faculty member at a university in the United States while continuing his research.

4 iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This research has been a lengthy collaborative experience including many faculty, graduate, and undergraduate students. There have been a number of people who assisted in developing the design, collecting the data, and statistical analysis. This research is a product of these very talented individuals and I wish to express my gratitude. First, I would like to thank my advisor, Dr. Katherine Klein. Dr. Klein was essential for the completion of this study. She was intimately involved with every phase of this research, and made herself tremendously available to me for the numerous glitches that occur when conducting research. She gave me access to the laboratory and the tools I would need to complete this research. Moreover, her gentle prodding was very helpful in motivating me to complete this work. I would like to thank Dr. Lynne Baker-Ward for always making time for me. I spoke with her early in my graduate career about my research and she showed great interest. She continually encouraged me in my research. Dr. Denis Gray was a key component in this research. I counted on him to analyze my proposal and find any design errors that could contaminate the research. He was also occasionally sought for critical statistical information. I would also like to thank Elwyn Martin, Christina Costanzo-Mendat, Phillip Braddy, Sara Wren, and Kyle Huff for their contributions to conversations about the design and the interpretation of the results. Lastly, I would like to thank my wife, Susan, and my child, Andrew, who motivated me to complete my degree.

5 iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES vi LIST OF FIGURES..vii INTRODUCTION... 1 Cost of Suppression on Cognitive Tasks... 5 Individual Differences in Working Memory (WM); Influence on Cognitive Tasks...17 Individual Differences in Suppression Ability; Relationship with WM...24 METHOD...31 Participants...31 Materials...31 OSPAN...32 Process Dissociation Task...33 Thought Control Ability Questionnaire...35 Impact of Events Scale...36 Apparatus...36 Procedure...36 RESULTS...38 DISCUSSION...48 REFERENCES...60 APPENDICES...67 APPENDIX A: WORDS, STEMS, BASERATES, FREQUENCIES, AND SET SIZES...68 APPENDIX B: WORDS, STEMS, BASERATES, FREQUENCIES,

6 v AND SET SIZES...69 APPENDIX C: STUDY LISTS...70 APPENDIX D: STEM COMPLETION LIST ONE...71 APPENDIX E: STEM COMPLETION LIST TWO...72 APPENDIX F: STEM COMPLETION LIST THREE...73 APPENDIX G: STEM COMPLETION LIST FOUR...74 APPENDIX H: STEM COMPLETION LIST FIVE...75 APPENDIX I: STEM COMPLETION LIST SIX...76 APPENDIX J: STEM COMPLETION LIST SEVEN...77 APPENDIX K: STEM COMPLETION LIST EIGHT...78 APPENDIX L: THOUGHT CONTROL ABILITY QUESTIONNAIRE...79 APPENDIX M: IMPACT OF EVENTS SCALE...80 APPENDIX M: IMPACT OF EVENTS SCALE (cont.)...81 APPENDIX N: NEGATIVE EXPERIENCE NOMINATION REQUEST FORM...82

7 vi LIST OF TABLES 1. Mean Suppression Failures, OSPAN Scores, and Baseline Scores of the PD Task Repeated Measures ANOVA with Recollection and Familiarity Scores as a Function of Study and Test Conditions Intercorrelations Between OSPAN Scores and Baseline Estimates of the PD Task Intercorrelations Between OSPAN Scores and Suppression Failures Intercorrelations Between Suppression Failures in all Periods Intercorrelations Between the IES Sub-scales and Total IES

8 vii LIST OF FIGURES 1. Mean Recollection Scores by Study and Test Conditions across Time Mean Familiarity Scores by Study and Test Conditions across Time..43

9 1 Introduction Stressful experiences tax an individual s ability to adapt to their environment. McKinnon, Baum, and Morokoff, (1988) conclude that, stressful events include the threat of being harmed, strong environmental demands or loss of something valued (p. 43). Experiencing these stressors often leads to intrusive thoughts. Intrusive thoughts are thoughts that are brought into conscious awareness without being consciously requested. These intrusive thoughts are often unwanted for two reasons. First, they may interfere with the task the person is consciously engaged in at the time. This interference may cause deficits in sustaining controlled attention, or may impair one s ability to inhibit stimuli that are unrelated to the task at hand. Second, intrusive thoughts are associated with increased negative affect (Wenzlaff, 1991, cited in Wenzlaff & Wagner, 1998). Because intrusive thoughts about stressful experiences are unwanted, we try to avoid them (Manne, 1999). People use a variety of techniques to avoid intrusive thoughts. They may attempt to replace the unwanted thought, distract themselves, or rely on drugs or alcohol to alleviate the negative feelings associated with these intrusive thoughts. Due to the recurrent and unwanted nature of intrusive thoughts, people often engage in these strategies to regain mental control. The most common method used to remove unwanted intrusive thoughts is intentional thought suppression. Thought suppression is defined as the intentional and controlled removal of a thought from conscious awareness. Freud thought that some thoughts were so disturbing or unacceptable that we unconsciously eject them from conscious awareness. This unconscious, unplanned ejection of thoughts from conscious awareness is termed repression. I will deal exclusively with suppression

10 2 of unwanted intrusive thoughts, and it will become important to distinguish the characteristics of this particular strategy of dealing with intrusive thoughts. How suppression is accomplished may be more complex than it first appears. Wegner (1994) proposed an Ironic Process of Mental Control that described the structural components and processes involved with intentional thought suppression. Wegner s theory states that there are two processes that work together to suppress an unwanted thought: an operating system and an ironic monitor. The operating system is a controlled process, which endeavors to create the desired state of mind (Wegner 1994, p.37). The operating system is used to bring items into conscious awareness, such as a controlled distracter, that will temporarily replace the unwanted intrusive thought. The controlled characteristic of the operating system brings with it limitations. It takes mental resources to initiate the system and therefore, when resources run low, the operating system s effectiveness declines. Time pressure (Wegner & Erber 1992, Exp. 1) and imposition of a cognitive load (Wegner & Erber, 1992 Exp. 2; Wegner, Erber, & Zanakos, 1993, Exp. 1 and 2) have been shown to decrease an individual s ability to suppress or cause impairment on another controlled concurrent task. The other process is an ironic monitor. The ironic monitor is an automatic, uncontrolled process that constantly browses through the conscious contents of the mind looking for anything that is not associated with the desired state. If the monitor finds items that imply failed mental control, it increases their activation. This heightened activation increases the likelihood that these items will return to conscious thought. The uncontrolled characteristic of the ironic monitor suggests that it does not consume mental resources and therefore, should not be affected by diminished resources.

11 3 The irony of this theory involves the interplay between these two processes. The monitor will keep looking for anything not associated with the desired state. Because of the increased activation mentioned earlier, and the fact that this process does not use mental resources, the monitor will eventually find an item in consciousness that is associated with the thought to be suppressed. The monitor will then alert the operating system to run again to find a new suitable distracter. Once the operating system has found a new distracter, the monitor begins searching again. In the absence of a cognitive load, people are capable of suppressing unwanted intrusive thoughts by temporarily distracting themselves. Wegner s research suggests that while suppression may not be effective over long periods of time, it may be successful temporarily (Wegner 1994; Wegner, Schneider, Carter, & White, 1987). Suppression of unwanted intrusive thoughts may be successful and may lead to alleviation of the negative feelings associated with them. However, when suppression fails, the unwanted thought will return to conscious awareness along with the negative feelings associated with them. Wegner s ironic process model shares several key features with current models of working memory. Working memory is a limited capacity system allowing controlled sustained attention in the face of distractions or interference. This memory system is seen as that part of short-term memory that is actively rotating, computing, or otherwise mentally manipulating inner representations of the world. Controlled attention is required to selectively attend to only those pieces of information that are relevant to the task at hand, and to inhibit all other non-relevant information (Kane & Engle, 2000). Intrusive thoughts are examples of non-relevant information that need to be inhibited so as not to

12 4 infringe on the resources needed to complete the task at hand. Thought intrusions are believed to occur automatically. Interestingly, once the intrusive thought has entered consciousness, intentionally controlled thought suppression may temporarily remove it from conscious awareness at the expense of reducing the available resources for completing the task. Working memory includes both aspects of controlled attention and inhibition. Inhibition requires attentional resources as well (Hasher & Zacks, 1998). Considering the limited capacity of working memory, the amount of mental resources available for controlled, sustained attention is negatively related to the amount of mental resources available for inhibition. When the pool of mental resources runs low, there is likely to be a failure in either controlled attention or inhibition, with both resulting in the interruption of the task at hand. While some working memory theorists only consider the ability to sustain controlled attention (Conway & Engle, 1994), the consequences of diminished resources will be the same. Suppression of stressful intrusive thoughts may drain the resource pool and increase the probability of intrusions due to a lack of controlled attention, resulting in a failure of inhibition, or may cause impairments on other consciously controlled mental tasks. The purpose of this study is to investigate individual differences in suppression ability and working memory, and the effects suppression may have on other concurrent cognitive tasks. Wegner s directed suppression methodology is the most common way of empirically investigating thought suppression (Wegner, Schneider, Carter, & White, 1987). After practicing how to report their stream of consciousness, participants are instructed to either express or suppress a white bear for the first five-minute period. Next,

13 5 instructions are reversed for both groups. They are also instructed to ring a bell on the table whenever a white bear comes to mind in both the express and suppress conditions. The results indicate that the numbers of thoughts about a white bear are more prevalent in the expression condition, although thoughts of the white bear are more frequent in the expression period following initial suppression than in the initial expression period. A similar effect is not found in the suppression period following initial expression. The authors conclude that initial suppression appears to produce a rebound effect. A negative cue hypothesis has been developed to explain this phenomenon. The negative cue hypothesis suggests that thinking without focus is difficult and proposes that participants attempt to distract themselves by using cues in the environment. However, in the following expression period, these negative cues constantly remind one of what was previously forbidden to think about. While the rebound effect helps us to understand the persistence of unwanted intrusive thoughts, the consequences of suppression on other cognitive tasks need to be further examined. I will begin by examining the empirical research related to the differential cost of suppression on other non-related tasks. Next, I will review the literature concerning individual differences in working memory and the influence working memory has on different cognitive tasks. Finally, I will discuss the research on individual differences in people s ability to suppress unwanted thoughts and how this difference is related to working memory. Cost of Suppression on Cognitive Tasks Wegner, Erber and Zanakos (1993) used the Stroop color-naming task with and without a cognitive load to measure the ironic effects of suppression. Participants were

14 6 asked to suppress or think about a recent personal success or failure. The Stroop words were either related or unrelated to the personal experiences. Under low cognitive load, participants speed at naming the color of the words on the task was unaffected by suppression. Under high cognitive load, participants who had been asked to suppress their personal experiences were slower to name the color of the experience-related words compared to participants asked to think about their personal experiences. These results suggest that suppression of a success or failure under cognitive load makes that success or failure more accessible than other irrelevant thoughts, and resulted in slower task performance. Hodges and Wegner (1997) investigated the effects of suppression on an explicit memory task. The authors completed a study in which participants were asked to assume or reject the perspective of another person. Participants read a story in which two boys played in a house that is described in detail. The experimental conditions included a control group who simply read the story, a title group (who were given the story to read with the title, Would you rob this house? ), a think like a burglar group and a not think like a burglar group. All participants were assigned to either a low or high cognitive load (remember a one-digit or six-digit number while reading the story). Under low cognitive load, the think like a burglar group recalled more burglar relevant information compared to the control and the not think like a burglar groups. The not think like a burglar group under high cognitive load recalled more burglar relevant information than the not think like a burglar group under low cognitive load. Moreover, under high cognitive load, the not think like a burglar group recalled more burglar relevant information than the think like a burglar group or the control group. These results suggest that suppression

15 7 conducted under cognitive load may cause the suppressed information to become hyperaccesible mirroring the results of Wegner and Erber, (1992). Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne and Wheeler (1996) investigated the relationship between stereotype suppression and a probe reaction task. The authors asked participants not to suppress descriptions of a skinhead, suppress a less stereotypic description of a skinhead, or suppress a highly stereotypic description of a skinhead. The group who was suppressing a highly stereotypic description of a skinhead had slower response times on a concurrent probe reaction task compared to the other two groups. Unfortunately, suppression failures of the skinhead were not measured and the slower response times could be due to the distraction of the intrusions or to the cognitive cost of suppressing stereotypes. Nevertheless, participants attempting to suppress a highly stereotypical description of a skinhead experienced either unwanted intrusive thoughts or impairment on another controlled cognitive task. Wegner, Quillian and Houston (1996) predicted that one of the effects of intentional suppression is the disruption of episodic memory for extended event sequences. First, participants were shown a film clip of a coal miners strike that had a clear beginning, middle and end. Participants were then assigned to a control condition with no instructions, a suppression condition with the instructions, Do not think about the film clip today, or an expression group with the instructions, Think about the film clip all day. Five hours later, participants were asked to complete a number of memory measures. There were no group differences in overall memory or reports of the vividness of the memory of the film. Interestingly, participants who were asked to suppress thoughts about the film during the five-hour delay were less accurate in recalling the

16 8 sequence of events depicted in the film compared to participants told to think about the film all day or to the control group who received no instructions. These results suggest that thought suppression may also interfere with the temporal order of memories. Richards and Gross (2000) investigated the cost of suppressing emotional reactions to a film (Exp. 1) and to a slide show (Exp. 2). In experiment one, participants were assigned to a watch condition (where they were asked to watch and listen to the film clip carefully) or an expressive suppression condition (where they were asked to hide their emotional reactions to the film clip). They were then asked to watch a film clip that depicted an argument between a husband and wife about an extramarital affair in front of their children. Participants in the expressive suppression condition showed poorer memory for the visual and auditory details compared with participants asked to merely watch the film clip. Moreover, participants asked to suppress their emotional reactions to the film clip reported less confidence in their memory compared to those asked to watch the film clip. Apparently suppressing emotional reactions produces a cognitive impairment in memory and a reduction in confidence for that memory. To address a few methodological concerns from the previous study, Richards and Gross (2000) included a reappraisal condition and a second level of emotional intensity in experiment two. Participants were assigned to a watch condition, an expressive suppression condition or a reappraisal condition (where they were asked to try their best to adopt a neutral attitude toward the slides). The slides contained verbal information (name, occupation, and injury) and non-verbal information (the actual image of the injury). The injury images were of a high emotional intensity (people who appeared gravely injured) or low emotional intensity (people who appeared healthy because they

17 9 had healed). Participants in the expressive suppression condition showed poorer memory for verbally encoded information presented during the high emotion slides compared to participants in the watch condition. Interestingly, the reappraisal condition enhanced nonverbal memory. These results also suggest that suppression of emotions may drain resources available for ongoing processing. In a study by Klein and Bratton (in press) participants were randomly assigned to a neutral personal experience, a non-personally relevant negative experience, a personally relevant negative experience thought condition, or a non-suppression control condition. The control group simply completed the dependent measures which included a sentence verification task; a modified version of the Speed and Capacity of Language Processing Test (Baddeley, Emslie, & Nimmo-Smith, 1992). After practicing the sentence verification task and intentional suppression, the remaining participants were asked to spend three minutes expressing their target experience followed by three minutes suppressing their target experience. Next, the participants were asked to suppress their target experience while concurrently completing the sentence verification task. Initial analysis revealed that those who had the hardest time practicing suppression also had more failures while suppressing their personal experience. Results from the dual task period revealed that participants suppressing a personal negative experience had slower response times on the sentences compared to the control group and the group suppressing a non-emotional experience, but did not differ from participants suppressing a nonpersonal negative experience. Furthermore, these differences were exacerbated on the more complex sentences. Overall, these results lend support to the hypotheses that

18 10 suppression may have a cognitive cost on other controlled mental tasks, and that these costs increase as task difficulty increases. Harvey and Bryant (1999) investigated the role of anxiety in attempted thought suppression. The authors categorized participants as high or low in anxiousness. Participants were then assigned to a neutral or distressing film condition. After watching the film clip, participants were assigned to a think about anything group or a suppress thoughts about the film group. Next, the participants were asked to monitor their thoughts for any film-related thought while completing a lexical decision task. There were no differences between suppressors or non-suppressors in the number of film related intrusions for those in the high anxiousness group during the concurrent task period. However, those given suppression instructions in the low anxiousness group during the concurrent task period experienced significantly more film-related intrusions compared to those given no such instructions. The authors concluded that participants who were categorized as highly anxious engaged in defensive suppression, which reduced the paradoxical effects seen in other studies. While there were no significant group differences in the lexical decision task, the cost of suppression in this study apparently resulted in the increased number of film-related thoughts during the concurrent task. While there are some questions about the nature of suppression effects and which cognitive tasks may be affected by this intentional process, thought suppression appears to have a negative effect on other controlled concurrent mental tasks. The key hypothesis of the proposed study is that thought suppression will cause impairment on the explicit portion of a process dissociation task as both are controlled, effortful processes but should have little to no effect on the implicit portion of the process dissociation task.

19 11 Most previous research examining the cognitive costs of thought suppression has used explicit memory tasks. Performance on explicit memory tasks refers to those mental processes involved in controlled cognition. Controlled cognition implies that the information is available to conscious awareness, and can be purposely retrieved from memory if there are adequate resources available. In contrast, performance on implicit memory tasks refers to those mental processes involved in uncontrolled cognition. This uncontrolled (automatic) cognition implies that the information is not available to conscious awareness and therefore, is not subject to conscious retrieval. Because uncontrolled cognition does not draw from the resource pool, performance on the implicit task should not be affected by suppression attempts. Current theories of working memory and competition for resources suggest that suppression uses cognitive resources and that this diminished resource pool has effects on other consciously controlled mental processes, but should leave the more automatic processes unhampered. To the extent that these premises are true, directed suppression should have effects on the performance of explicit memory tasks, but performance on implicit tasks should remain intact. I will now cover previous research that has investigated separating the influences of controlled versus automatic processing and research that has employed memory tasks that include both explicit and implicit components. Jacoby, Woloshyn & Kelley (1989) sought to separate the independent influences of conscious recollection versus familiarity in memory tasks. The participants were first asked to read a list of non-famous names with full or divided attention. Next, the participants were asked whether names shown to them were famous or non-famous. They were told that all of the names from the previous list were not famous. If the participants

20 12 recognized a name from the first list it must be non-famous. Those who studied the list under full attention were later less likely to call an old non-famous name famous than new non-famous names. This was due to the fact that the participants presumably remembered some of the old names from the first list. On the other hand, those who studied the list under divided attention, were more likely to call an old non-famous name famous than new non-famous names. The divided attention condition interfered with the level of processing which lowered the probability of remembering the old names from the first list. The divided attention condition caused the participants to call old names famous due to familiarity and not recollection. However, placing recollection and familiarity in opposition to each other may cause one to underestimate the process of familiarity. Results suggest that it is possible to separate the independent contributions of recollection and familiarity in memory tasks. To investigate the possibility that placing recollection and familiarity in opposition to each other causes an underestimation of familiarity, Jacoby, Toth, and Yonelinas (1993) discussed a general approach to separating the influences of controlled and automatic cognition on laboratory tasks. The process-dissociation procedure defines conscious control as the difference in performance between conditions where one is trying to do something versus trying not to do something (Kelley and Jacoby, 1998 p.131). The authors suggest using both an exclusion condition to estimate familiarity and an inclusion condition to estimate recollection. The inclusion score is the probability of recollection (R) plus the probability of the word automatically coming to mind (A) when there is a failure of recollection (1-R): Inclusion = R + A (1-R). (p. 141)

21 13 The exclusion score involves the probability that the word automatically comes to mind (A) and a failure to recollect that it was seen earlier (1-R): Exclusion = A (1-R). The probability of recollection is equal to the inclusion score minus the exclusion score: R = I-E. The probability of familiarity is equal to the exclusion score divided by the equation for when there is a failure to recollect: A = Exclusion/ (1-R). The previous equation estimate reflects both the automatic influences of memory (M) and the baseline probability of responding with a particular word: A = M + B. Given that these premises are true, one can calculate the independent influences of automatic memory by subtracting the baseline probability of responding with a particular word from the estimate of A: M = A B. Original attempts to estimate recollection and familiarity used two separate tests. This relied on the assumption that the two processes were relatively pure. In contrast, the process dissociation procedure allows one to estimate both the independent influences of recollection and familiarity within a single task. If these two processes have independent effects on a task, researchers should be able to create manipulations that either only affect one of these processes, or even better, have opposite effects on these processes. For obvious reasons, there is a definite advantage to using the process dissociation procedure

22 14 to estimate the independent contributions of recollection and familiarity in laboratory memory tasks. Jacoby (1998) discussed the general approach to separating the influences of controlled and automatic cognition on laboratory tasks in recent studies. In three experiments the authors manipulated instructions (Direct-retrieval versus generaterecognize), attention during study (full versus divided), presentation duration during study (one versus ten seconds) and presentation of items (separate versus intermixed). In experiment one, the authors manipulated instructions and investigated the effects of full versus divided attention during study. Half of the participants were assigned to the directretrieval condition (DR) and half were assigned to the generate-recognize condition (GR). Direct-retrieval instructions required participants to avoid words specifically on the basis of recollection. Exclusions in the generate-recognize instructions would result from an old word automatically coming to mind, without being recollected which would then be checked for recognition and would be avoided because it was recognized. Half of the participants from each group were assigned to the full attention study condition and half of the participants were assigned to the divided attention study condition. Participants were asked to read 40 study words aloud that were presented on a computer screen one at a time and to remember them for a future memory test. The divided attention groups were asked to listen to a recorded list of digits and to detect any sequences of three odd digits in a row while reading the study words aloud. In the final phase of the study, participants were presented with word stems, consisting of the first three letters of a five-letter word on a computer screen one at a time. For participants in the DR condition, each word stem was presented with a prompt (NEW or OLD) and the inclusion and exclusion tests were

23 15 intermixed. Participants were instructed that if the prompt was OLD, their job was to use a recalled word as a completion. If the prompt was NEW, their job was to not use a recalled word as a completion, and they were further instructed to use a different word to complete the stem. For participants in the GR condition, word stems were presented without prompts, and the inclusion and exclusion tests were blocked. Participants in this condition were asked to check each completion word that came to mind and make sure it was not presented earlier for the exclusion condition. For the inclusion condition, participants were asked to complete the word stem with the first five-letter word that came to mind that fit the stem. Inclusion test scores were higher for the DR condition compared to the GR condition, and were higher under full attention compared to divided attention during study. Exclusion test scores were higher for the GR condition compared to the DR condition, and were higher under full attention compared to divided attention during study. Of specific interest, analysis of A revealed a process dissociation. Results from the DR instructions revealed that dividing attention during study produced a decrease in R, but left A unhampered. An opposite dissociation was found from the GR instructions. Results revealed a decrease in R, but an increase in A. For words studied under full attention, A was significantly below baseline (p.8), which is a sign that the assumptions of independence and equality of R had been violated. The authors concluded that direct-retrieval instructions were the appropriate choice when conducting research on process dissociations. In his second study, Jacoby manipulated presentation duration during study (one versus ten seconds) as a within subjects variable. Half of the participants received the short duration study list first and half of the participants received the long duration study

24 16 list first. After the first study list was presented the participants were reminded to read the words aloud and try to remember them for a future memory test. Jacoby also included the remember-know procedure during the stem completion phase. As word stems were presented to the participant, they were instructed to use the stem as a cue to recall a word from the study list. If the participant could not recall a word from the study list, they were to complete the stem with the first five-letter word that came to mind and fit the stem. Once a stem was completed, the participant evaluated their response as remember, know or new. The participant would respond with remember if they could consciously remember details of the word from one of the study lists. A know response was to be given if the participant knew that the word had been presented in one of the study lists but could not remember any details of the word from the study list. If the participant believed that the word had not been presented on either of the study lists they were asked to respond, New. Combining know and new responses (direct-retrieval), reducing study time decreased R, but left A invariant. Combining know and remember responses (generate-recognize), reducing study time decreased R, and increased A. Jacoby concluded that the difference in outcomes due to the different procedures used to estimate R and A explains the disagreement in the current research literature; combining know and remember responses instead of know and new response violates the assumption of the process dissociation procedure. Taken together, these results suggests that researchers should consider carefully the instructions they give to participants and the procedures they use to estimate R and A. Jacoby concluded that, To meet the assumptions underlying the process-dissociation

25 17 procedure, it is important that instructions encourage participants to exclude items only because they recollect (remember) earlier studying the items (p.17). Individual Differences in Working Memory (WM); Influence on Cognitive Tasks The next issue involves investigating individual differences in working memory. There are individual differences in people s capacity to control attention (Engle, Kane & Tuholski, 1999) and this difference manifests itself on laboratory tasks. Fink and Neubauer (2004) examined the relationship between working memory and subjective time estimations and found that individuals with higher WMC made more accurate time estimations on a task than those with lower WMC. It is not known if people with higher working memory will experience lower dual task costs arising from efforts to suppress a personal memory. Oberauer, Lange, and Engle (2004) examined the relationship between WMC and the ability to resist interference. Participants working memory capacity was assessed with four standard measures in a previous study. Participants were asked to complete a number of verbal and spatial tasks, which included either a single or dual task component. In the dual tasks, the researchers varied the phonological and semantic similarity to investigate their relationship with WMC. Dual task costs (DTC) were modestly correlated with WMC, although the authors concluded that, the correlations between DTC and WMC are artifacts from non-linear transformations on the differences between single- and dual-task performances (p.91). The authors suggest that dual task interference had an additive effect (or better: subtractive) effect on performance, whether measured as span or percentage correct, independent of a person s working memory capacity (p. 92). Semantic and phonological similarity had little, if any effect

26 18 on dual task costs. More importantly, there were no significant correlations between dual tasks costs across the domains. The researchers suggest that these results lend no support for the idea of a domain-general central executive. However, the researchers used content from the same domain for the secondary task. The authors propose that the difference between simple and complex span tasks cannot be explained by the added component of a central executive or general executive attention. Results suggest that there is a relationship between WMC and the ability to resist interference. Rosen and Engle, (1998) investigated individual differences in working memory capacity and how they are related to people s ability to inhibit information in two experiments. In experiment one, participants were prescreened with a modified version of Operation Span Task (OSPAN; Turner & Engle, 1989). Next, the participants were assigned to an interference condition or a non-interference condition for the paired associate task. The paired associate task involved participants learning three lists of twelve word pairs. Participants were instructed that speed was crucial and that their goal was to say the correct response before the deadline. High span participants needed fewer trials to reach the criterion level, produced fewer total intrusions, and produced fewer intrusions per trial on the second-list compared to low spans. Results from experiment one imply that there is a negative relationship between WMC and vulnerability to intrusions. In Rosen and Engle s (1998) second experiment they investigated the relationship between individuals WMC and their ability to suppress intrusions. The procedure was the same as in experiment one except that instead of speed, participants were instructed that accuracy was crucial and that their goal was to really try hard to come up with the

27 19 correct response. High span participants in the interference condition took longer to retrieve response items from the first-list on the first trial of the third-list compared to their control. Low span participants were faster than their control. Moreover, high span participants in the interference condition took longer to retrieve response items from the first-list on the first trial of the third-list compared to when they first retrieved the same response items on trial one of the first-list. Low span participants in the interference condition showed little difference in response times between the first and third-lists. The authors concluded that, high span participants in the interference condition had suppressed first-list response items during second-list learning, whereas the low span participants had not (p.430). Taken together, Rosen & Engle s (1998) two experiments provide strong support for the notion that WMC has a negative relationship with vulnerability to intrusions and a positive relationship with the ability to inhibit intrusions. Beilock and Carr (2005) investigated individual differences in working memory and the effects of psychological pressure on a mathematical problem-solving task. The participants first completed two working memory measures: Turner and Engle s (1989) Operation Span and Daneman and Carpenter s (1980) Reading Span. The participants were then asked to complete Gauss (1801) modular arithmetic task (cited in Bogomolny, 1996). The task involved solving a number of mathematical equation and deciding if the answers were whole numbers or not. There were an equal number of high and low difficulty equations. In the low pressure condition, there were no significant differences between the high and low working memory capacity groups. Under the high pressure condition, those in the high working memory capacity group showed significantly diminished scores on the high difficulty equations, whereas the low working memory

28 20 group s scores slightly improved on the high difficulty equations. The authors suggest that those with higher working memory may be disadvantaged when high difficulty levels and high pressure demands are concurrent, but that lower working memory individuals may not suffer the same diminishment in performance due to their scores already being near floor effects. These findings suggest that when high span participants working memory resources are depleted due to psychological pressure that performance on a controlled task with a high cognitive load may suffer. Unsworth and Engle (2005) conducted a study to examine individual differences in working memory and their relationship to both incidental and intentional learning. The participants were pre-screened with the OSPAN for working memory capacity. Participants were then asked to practice a serial reaction time task involving locating an asterisk on the computer screen in one of four locations. Next, participants completed twelve blocks of trials with five blocks consisting of random sequences of asterisks locations and seven blocks consisting of repeating sequences of asterisks locations intermixed. Results of the practice trials showed that those in the high working memory condition were faster than those in the low working memory condition. During the repeating sequences, the high span group was faster, but both of the groups performances increased at the same rate. During the random sequences, the high span group performed significantly faster than the low span group. Results suggest that high and low spans do not differ in performing the unintentional task, but do on the intentional explicit task. Results suggest that individual differences in working memory manifest themselves on tasks that require controlled processing, but have little or no implications for automatic processing.

29 21 Pulos and Denzine (2005) investigated individual differences in working memory and planning behavior using a computerized version of the Tower of London task (Welsh, Satterlee-Cartmell, & Stine, 1999). The participants completed a visuo-spatial span task, a verbal-span task, a trail making task, a standardized version of the Stroop task (Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe, & Leber, 1989), and the Tower of London (TOL) task. The average solution time of the TOL was significantly correlated with both the visuospatial-span task and the verbal-span task (.41 and.26 respectively). The number of excess moves was not significantly related to any of the working memory tasks. The authors suggest that individuals with higher working memory scores develop more efficient online planning strategies by making larger partial plans and therefore do not need as many iterations of the partial plan-execution cycle. Those with lower working memory tend to make smaller partial plans and thus must engage in more cycles to attain the desired outcome. The authors also point out that the correlation between the visual and verbal WMC tasks was only.07 suggesting that these make independent contributions to the TOL task. Apparently, individuals working memory capacity is also significantly related to planning behavior. Bleckley, Durso, Crutchfield, Engle and Khanna (2003) investigated individual differences in working memory capacity and the ability to allocate visual attention. The authors used the operation-word-span task (OSPAN) to measure working memory capacity (WMC). Using a modified visual attention allocation task, participants were asked to identify a letter flashed briefly in the middle of a three concentric octagonal pattern and to locate a letter presented outside of the fixation point. Trials were uncued, validly cued or invalidly cued for the letter outside of the fixation point (close, medium or

30 22 distant). On the invalidly cued trials, individuals with high span compared to low span condition showed poorer location of the displaced letter when it was presented on a ring closer to the fixation point than the one cued. The authors concluded that the low span WMC participants used their attention much like Posner et al. s (1980) spotlight metaphor, and that high span WMC participants were able to inhibit the non-cued rings. Low span WMC participants were less able to ignore the non-cued rings, which allowed the participants to correctly locate the letter when it was presented on a ring closer to the fixation point. However, the design of this study did not allow the researchers to specify whether the high span WMC participants inhibited information on the uncued rings, focused their attention on the cued rings or some combination of both explanations. Nevertheless, results are consistent with previous research that suggests WMC is related to controlled attention and inhibition of non-goal relevant information. Unsworth, Schrock, and Engle (2004) investigated the relationship between working memory capacity and inhibition ability with a prosaccade and antisaccade task. The authors pre-screened participants using the OSPAN. In the prosaccade task, participants were asked to make their first eye movement in the direction of the flashing square. In the antisaccade task, participants were asked to make their first eye movement in the opposite direction of the flashing square. On the prosaccade task, working memory span was not related to speed or accuracy during the task. Moreover, both groups were faster on the prosaccade versus the antisaccade tasks. On the antisaccade tasks, participants in the high span condition were faster and more accurate compared to participants in the low span condition. Apparently, participants with low spans

31 23 experienced more difficulty inhibiting the natural response to move their eyes in the direction of the flashing square. In their second experiment, the researchers presented the participants with trials of prosaccade and antisaccade tasks intermixed to increase the demands on attention on both the antisaccade and prosaccade tasks. The procedure was similar to experiment one except that the white plus sign was replaced with either a white circle or a white diamond to indicate if the trial was prosaccade or antisaccade, and after 80 trials the symbol meaning was switched. Results replicated those in study one as prosaccade trials were completed faster and more accurately than antisaccade trials for both groups. More importantly, low span participants made more errors on both prosaccade and antisaccade tasks compared to the high spans. There was only a marginal difference in speed between the high and low spans. These two studies suggest that low span individuals are poorer at controlling attention to directions and inhibiting natural response to stimuli that are nongoal relevant compared to high span individuals. Conway, Cowan, and Bunting (2001) examined the relationship between participants working memory spans and their ability to inhibit irrelevant, non-goal directed information using a dichotic listening task. The participants were given headsets and were then asked to listen to the message in their right ear and ignore the message in their left ear, and to shadow each word out loud that was presented in their right ear. Participants were randomly assigned to a condition where the authors inserted their first name in their left ear after either four or five minutes of shadowing. Immediate retrospective reports showed that only 20% of the high span participants heard their first name in the irrelevant message compared to 65% of the low span participants. The

32 24 authors propose that the low span individuals were less able to block information from the irrelevant message compared to high spans. Interestingly, shadowing errors before presentation of their names were equal between the groups, but errors were significantly higher after the presentation of their names for both groups relative to those who had not heard their names. The authors concluded that previous research supports the interpretation that high span individuals are better at inhibiting distracting information. Individual Differences in Suppression Ability; Relationship with WM I have presented evidence that thought suppression can impair performance on concurrent cognitive tasks, that a dissociation paradigm can be used to separate deliberate and automatic influences in memory, and that differences in working memory capacity are related to individuals performance on laboratory tasks, their ability to attend to laboratory tasks and their ability to inhibit irrelevant information while completing a task. There is also evidence that differences in individual s working memory capacity are linked to the ability to suppress unwanted thoughts. It is reasonable to believe that some people have either a larger resource pool (Just & Carpenter, 1992) or they use the resource pool more efficiently, and this should be related to an individual s ability to suppress unwanted thoughts. Brewin and Beaton (2002) investigated the relationship between working memory, intelligence, and suppression ability. The authors measured fluid intelligence using Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven, Court, & Raven, 1977), crystallized intelligence using the National Adult Reading Test (NART-2; Nelson & Willison, 1991), and assessed working memory capacity using OSPAN. During a first five-minute period participants were asked to report their stream of consciousness

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 699-703 Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening GREGORY J. H. COLFLESH University of Illinois, Chicago,

More information

Individual differences in working memory capacity predict visual attention allocation

Individual differences in working memory capacity predict visual attention allocation Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2003, 10 (4), 884-889 Individual differences in working memory capacity predict visual attention allocation M. KATHRYN BLECKLEY and FRANCIS T. DURSO Texas Tech University,

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

Unexpected costs of high working memory capacity following directed forgetting and contextual change manipulations

Unexpected costs of high working memory capacity following directed forgetting and contextual change manipulations Unexpected costs of high working memory capacity following directed forgetting and contextual change manipulations By: PETER F. DELANEY AND LILI SAHAKYAN Delaney, P. F., & Sahakyan, L. (2007). Unexpected

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

Working Memory Capacity and the Antisaccade Task: Individual Differences in Voluntary Saccade Control

Working Memory Capacity and the Antisaccade Task: Individual Differences in Voluntary Saccade Control Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2004, Vol. 30, No. 6, 1302 1321 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.6.1302

More information

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention Chapter 6 Attention Attention William James, in 1890, wrote Everyone knows what attention is. Attention is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously

More information

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 4, 354-373 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association Inc 0096-3445/94/S3.00 Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent

More information

Psycholinguistics Psychological Mechanisms

Psycholinguistics Psychological Mechanisms Psycholinguistics Psychological Mechanisms Psychological Mechanisms Information processing system Processing Serial and parallel processing Top-down and bottom-up processing Automatic and controlled processes

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

Dual n-back training increases the capacity of the focus of attention

Dual n-back training increases the capacity of the focus of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:135 141 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0335-6 BRIEF REPORT Dual n-back training increases the capacity of the focus of attention Lindsey Lilienthal & Elaine Tamez & Jill Talley Shelton

More information

Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control

Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2011, 64 (6), 1048 1055 Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control Thomas S. Redick and Randall

More information

2/27/2017. Modal Model of Memory. Executive Attention & Working Memory. Some Questions to Consider (over the next few weeks)

2/27/2017. Modal Model of Memory. Executive Attention & Working Memory. Some Questions to Consider (over the next few weeks) Executive Attention & Working Memory Memory 1 Some Questions to Consider (over the next few weeks) Why can we remember a telephone number long enough to place a call, but then we forget it almost immediately?

More information

Yuka Kotozaki Cognitive Psychology, Graduate School of Information Sciences Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan

Yuka Kotozaki Cognitive Psychology, Graduate School of Information Sciences Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan Working Memory and Persistent Inhibitory Set: An Individual Differences Investigation Syoichi Iwasaki (siwasaki@cog.is.tohoku.ac.jp) Cognitive Psychology, Graduate School of Information Sciences Tohoku

More information

The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity

The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2001, 8 (2), 331-335 The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity ANDREW R. A. CONWAY University of Illinois, Chicago, Illinois NELSON

More information

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (3), 490-494 Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness GINO CAMP, DIANE PECHER, and HENK G. SCHMIDT Erasmus University Rotterdam,

More information

Perceptual Processes II: Attention and Consciousness

Perceptual Processes II: Attention and Consciousness Perceptual Processes II: Attention and Consciousness Chapter 3 1 Attention [Attention] is taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible

More information

ABSTRACT. The Mechanisms of Proactive Interference and Their Relationship with Working Memory. Yi Guo Glaser

ABSTRACT. The Mechanisms of Proactive Interference and Their Relationship with Working Memory. Yi Guo Glaser ABSTRACT The Mechanisms of Proactive Interference and Their Relationship with Working Memory by Yi Guo Glaser Working memory (WM) capacity the capacity to maintain and manipulate information in mind plays

More information

The Role of Individual Differences in Working Memory in the Encoding and Retrieval of Information. Michael J. Montaño

The Role of Individual Differences in Working Memory in the Encoding and Retrieval of Information. Michael J. Montaño The Role of Individual Differences in Working Memory in the Encoding and Retrieval of Information by Michael J. Montaño A thesis submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Auburn University in partial fulfillment

More information

Attention. What is attention? Attention metaphors. Definitions of attention. Chapter 6. Attention as a mental process

Attention. What is attention? Attention metaphors. Definitions of attention. Chapter 6. Attention as a mental process What is attention? Attention Chapter 6 To drive a car you Use effort Sustain attention Orient to several locations Restrict attention Select particular objects Search for particular objects Respond with

More information

Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Episodic Retrieval: Examining the Dynamics of Delayed and Continuous Distractor Free Recall

Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Episodic Retrieval: Examining the Dynamics of Delayed and Continuous Distractor Free Recall Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 6, 1020 1034 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.6.1020

More information

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Unsworth, Nash] On: 13 January 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 918847458] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Attention. Concentrating and focusing of mental effort that is:

Attention. Concentrating and focusing of mental effort that is: What is attention? Concentrating and focusing of mental effort that is: Page 1 o Selective--focus on some things while excluding others o Divisible--able to focus on more than one thing at the same time

More information

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005,?? 12 (?), (5),???-??? 852-857 BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval LARRY L. JACOBY, YUJIRO SHIMIZU,

More information

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (6), 1067-1073 Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources MOSHE NAVEH-BENJAMIN University of Missouri,

More information

The impact of evaluative pressure and higher working memory capacity on sensorimotor skill performance.

The impact of evaluative pressure and higher working memory capacity on sensorimotor skill performance. University of Louisville ThinkIR: The University of Louisville's Institutional Repository College of Arts & Sciences Senior Honors Theses College of Arts & Sciences 5-2015 The impact of evaluative pressure

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN WORKING MEMORY AND INTELLIGENCE: DECONSTRUCTING THE WORKING MEMORY TASK

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN WORKING MEMORY AND INTELLIGENCE: DECONSTRUCTING THE WORKING MEMORY TASK THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN WORKING MEMORY AND INTELLIGENCE: DECONSTRUCTING THE WORKING MEMORY TASK By YE WANG A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Psychology Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d e. 2. a b c d e 14. a b c d e

Psychology Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d e. 2. a b c d e 14. a b c d e 1 7 Psychology 3450 - Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A Name: MUN ID: Indicate your answer by crossing out the appropriate letter. Answer 20 questions only. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d

More information

Proactive interference and practice effects in visuospatial working memory span task performance

Proactive interference and practice effects in visuospatial working memory span task performance MEMORY, 2011, 19 (1), 8391 Proactive interference and practice effects in visuospatial working memory span task performance Lisa Durrance Blalock 1 and David P. McCabe 2 1 School of Psychological and Behavioral

More information

Sperling conducted experiments on An experiment was conducted by Sperling in the field of visual sensory memory.

Sperling conducted experiments on An experiment was conducted by Sperling in the field of visual sensory memory. Levels of category Basic Level Category: Subordinate Category: Superordinate Category: Stages of development of Piaget 1. Sensorimotor stage 0-2 2. Preoperational stage 2-7 3. Concrete operational stage

More information

Separating Habit and Recollection: Memory Slips, Process Dissociations, and Probability Matching

Separating Habit and Recollection: Memory Slips, Process Dissociations, and Probability Matching Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1996, Vol. 22, No. 6,1323-1335 Copyright 19% by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/96/$3.00 Separating Habit and

More information

INTRODUCTION METHODS

INTRODUCTION METHODS INTRODUCTION Deficits in working memory (WM) and attention have been associated with aphasia (Heuer & Hallowell, 2009; Hula & McNeil, 2008; Ivanova & Hallowell, 2011; Murray, 1999; Wright & Shisler, 2005).

More information

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Attentional Blink Paradigm Attentional Blink Paradigm ATTENTIONAL BLINK 83 ms stimulus onset asychrony between all stimuli B T D A 3 N P Z F R K M R N Lag 3 Target 1 Target 2 After detection of a target in a rapid stream of visual

More information

Working Memory and Mental Health: A Primer for the Mental Health Professional

Working Memory and Mental Health: A Primer for the Mental Health Professional Abstract Working Memory and Mental Health: A Primer for the Mental Health Professional By Stephen Morgan, Ph.D. & Jerrod Brown, Ph.D. Working memory is an active and integrative stage in the human memory

More information

Cognition. Mid-term 1. Top topics for Mid Term 1. Heads up! Mid-term exam next week

Cognition. Mid-term 1. Top topics for Mid Term 1. Heads up! Mid-term exam next week Cognition Prof. Mike Dillinger Mid-term 1 Heads up! Mid-term exam next week Chapters 2, 3, of the textbook Perception, Attention, Short-term memory The lectures are to help you digest the chapters; exams

More information

Dream Rebound: The Return of Suppressed Thoughts in Dreams. Adya, Kennie, Madison, Makala, and Nathalie

Dream Rebound: The Return of Suppressed Thoughts in Dreams. Adya, Kennie, Madison, Makala, and Nathalie Dream Rebound: The Return of Suppressed Thoughts in Dreams Adya, Kennie, Madison, Makala, and Nathalie Reference Wegner, D., Wenzlaff, R., & Kozak, M. (2004). Dream Rebound: The Return of Suppressed Thoughts

More information

Executive Control, Working Memory, and Action Planning: An Individual Differences Approach

Executive Control, Working Memory, and Action Planning: An Individual Differences Approach Lehigh University Lehigh Preserve Theses and Dissertations 2012 Executive Control, Working Memory, and Action Planning: An Individual Differences Approach Kaitlin Reiman Lehigh University Follow this and

More information

Effects of Cognitive Load on Processing and Performance. Amy B. Adcock. The University of Memphis

Effects of Cognitive Load on Processing and Performance. Amy B. Adcock. The University of Memphis Effects of Cognitive Load 1 Running Head: Effects of Cognitive Load Effects of Cognitive Load on Processing and Performance Amy B. Adcock The University of Memphis Effects of Cognitive Load 2 Effects of

More information

How To Optimize Your Training For i3 Mindware v.4 And Why 2G N-Back Brain Training Works

How To Optimize Your Training For i3 Mindware v.4 And Why 2G N-Back Brain Training Works How To Optimize Your Training For i3 Mindware v.4 And Why 2G N-Back Brain Training Works Mark Ashton Smith, Ph.D. CONTENTS I. KEY SCIENTIFIC BACKGROUND 3 II. PRACTICE: WHAT STRATEGIES? 10 III. SCHEDULING

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Intelligence 38 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Intelligence

Intelligence 38 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Intelligence Intelligence 38 (2010) 255 267 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence Interference control, working memory capacity, and cognitive abilities: A latent variable analysis Nash Unsworth University

More information

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist M------- P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION Name: Date of Birth: Date of Evaluation: 05-28-2015 Tests Administered: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth

More information

Cognitive control, automaticity, and working memory: A dual-process analysis. Karen A. Daniels. Washington University in St. Louis

Cognitive control, automaticity, and working memory: A dual-process analysis. Karen A. Daniels. Washington University in St. Louis Running title: Cognitive control and working memory. Cognitive control, automaticity, and working memory: A dual-process analysis Karen A. Daniels Washington University in St. Louis Please do not quote

More information

Defining principles of Strategic family therapy

Defining principles of Strategic family therapy Roselyne Kattar Psychologist and coach http://www.coaching-kattar.com 1 Defining principles of Strategic family therapy Strategic therapy focuses on present observable behavioral interaction and uses deliberate

More information

Chapter 5. Memory Structures and Processes. Chapter 5 Memory Processes

Chapter 5. Memory Structures and Processes. Chapter 5 Memory Processes Chapter 5 Memory Structures and Processes Chapter 5 Memory Processes 1 Memory The Man with the 30 Second Memory Chapter 5 Memory Processes 2 Are Some things Easier (Harder) to remember than others? Chapter

More information

Tutorial: Cognition See Tutorials on Attention, Memory, Retrieval, Organization, Problem Solving, Reasoning, Self- Regulation/Executive Functions

Tutorial: Cognition See Tutorials on Attention, Memory, Retrieval, Organization, Problem Solving, Reasoning, Self- Regulation/Executive Functions Tutorial: Cognition See Tutorials on Attention, Memory, Retrieval, Organization, Problem Solving, Reasoning, Self- Regulation/Executive Functions WHAT IS COGNITION? comprehending it, storing it, retrieving

More information

MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0

MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0 MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0 Dawn M. McBride Methods for Measuring Conscious and Automatic Memory A Brief Review Abstract: Memory researchers have discussed the relationship between consciousness and memory

More information

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES Journal of Experimental Vol. 51, No. 4, 1956 Psychology CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES KENNETH H. KURTZ AND CARL I. HOVLAND Under conditions where several concepts are learned concurrently

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Spontaneous Trait Inferences Are Bound to Actors Faces: Evidence From a False Recognition Paradigm

Spontaneous Trait Inferences Are Bound to Actors Faces: Evidence From a False Recognition Paradigm Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 83, No. 5, 1051 1065 0022-3514/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.5.1051 Spontaneous

More information

Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk.

Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk. chunking Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk. clustering Organizing items into related groups during

More information

THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT

THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT Tajuk Bab/Chapter title 1 THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT Teo Sieak Ling and Yeo Kee Jiar shireleneteoh@hotmail.my kjyeo_utm@yahoo.com Abstract Extensive scientific studies in human

More information

How should you study for Friday's exam?

How should you study for Friday's exam? How should you study for Friday's exam? re-read textbook? re-read lecture slides? study guide? NO! these are passive. Use active study. Test yourself by Take the practice quizzes in Moodle Create your

More information

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about?

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about? 2 Asking and answering research questions What s it about? (Social Psychology pp. 24 54) Social psychologists strive to reach general conclusions by developing scientific theories about why people behave

More information

Working Memory, Attention Control, and the N-Back Task: A Question of Construct Validity. Michael J. Kane. University of North Carolina at Greensboro

Working Memory, Attention Control, and the N-Back Task: A Question of Construct Validity. Michael J. Kane. University of North Carolina at Greensboro Working Memory, Attention Control, and the N-Back Task: A Question of Construct Validity Michael J. Kane University of North Carolina at Greensboro Andrew R. A. Conway Princeton University Timothy K. Miura

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Mem Cogn (2011) 39:1222 1231 DOI 10.3758/s13421-011-0105-0 Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Dawn M. McBride & Jaclyn K. Beckner & Drew

More information

Modeling Unconscious Gender Bias in Fame Judgments: Finding the Proper Branch of the Correct (Multinomial) Tree

Modeling Unconscious Gender Bias in Fame Judgments: Finding the Proper Branch of the Correct (Multinomial) Tree CONSCIOUSNESS AND COGNITION 5, 221 225 (1996) ARTICLE NO. 0013 Modeling Unconscious Gender Bias in Fame Judgments: Finding the Proper Branch of the Correct (Multinomial) Tree SEAN C. DRAINE AND ANTHONY

More information

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI?

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI? Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI? Franklin P. Tamborello, II Phillip H. Chung Michael D. Byrne Rice University Department of Psychology 61 S. Main,

More information

Effects of Pressure-induced Worry and Negative Thoughts on Verbal and Visuospatial Working Memory Capacity

Effects of Pressure-induced Worry and Negative Thoughts on Verbal and Visuospatial Working Memory Capacity Kawasaki Journal of Medical Welfare Vol. 23, No. 2, 2018 43-50 Short Report Effects of Pressure-induced Worry and Negative Thoughts on Verbal and Visuospatial Working Memory Capacity Yoshihide NORITAKE

More information

ASHI 712. The Neuroscience of Human Memory. Dr. Olave E. Krigolson LECTURE 2: Short Term Memory and Sleep and Memory

ASHI 712. The Neuroscience of Human Memory. Dr. Olave E. Krigolson LECTURE 2: Short Term Memory and Sleep and Memory ASHI 712 The Neuroscience of Human Memory Dr. Olave E. Krigolson krigolson@uvic.ca LECTURE 2: Short Term Memory and Sleep and Memory Working / Short Term Memory Sunglasses Chair Dress Earrings Boots Bed

More information

Chapter 4. Two Types of Attention. Selective Listening 25/09/2012. Paying Attention. How does selective attention work?

Chapter 4. Two Types of Attention. Selective Listening 25/09/2012. Paying Attention. How does selective attention work? Chapter 4 Paying Attention Two Types of Attention How does selective attention work? How do we block out irrelevant information? What can t we block out? How much control do we have over our attention?

More information

There are often questions and, sometimes, confusion when looking at services to a child who is deaf or hard of hearing. Because very young children

There are often questions and, sometimes, confusion when looking at services to a child who is deaf or hard of hearing. Because very young children There are often questions and, sometimes, confusion when looking at services to a child who is deaf or hard of hearing. Because very young children are not yet ready to work on specific strategies for

More information

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses. Which of the following findings does NOT support the existence of task-specific mental resources? 1. a) It is more difficult to combine two verbal tasks than one verbal task and one spatial task. 2. b)

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Effects of working memory capacity on mental set due to domain knowledge

Effects of working memory capacity on mental set due to domain knowledge Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (6), 1456-1462 Effects of working memory capacity on mental set due to domain knowledge TRAVIS R. RICKS University of Illinois, Chicago, Illinois KANDI JO TURLEY-AMES Idaho

More information

Shayne Loft, Michael S. Humphreys, & Susannah J. Whitney. The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia

Shayne Loft, Michael S. Humphreys, & Susannah J. Whitney. The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia Memory Access Task Interference 1 Running Head: Memory Access Task Interference Control of Access to Memory: The use of Task Interference as a Behavioral Probe Shayne Loft, Michael S. Humphreys, & Susannah

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 2, Number 1 Submitted: September 12, 2006 Accepted: November 1, 2006 Publication Date: November 15, 2006 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Ironic Effects of Mental Control in Problem Solving:

More information

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Anna V. Fisher (fisher.449@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Matching and Recall: The Relationship with Awareness

Matching and Recall: The Relationship with Awareness Matching and Recall: The Relationship with Awareness ess Michael S. Humphreys The University of Queensland How Do We Know That Our Memories Are Correct? The recollection of details that support the inference

More information

ATTENTION! Learning Objective Topics. (Specifically Divided and Selective Attention) Chapter 4. Selective Attention

ATTENTION! Learning Objective Topics. (Specifically Divided and Selective Attention) Chapter 4. Selective Attention ATTENTION! (Specifically Divided and Selective Attention) Chapter 4 Learning Objective Topics Selective Attention Visual Tasks Auditory Tasks Early vs. Late Selection Models Visual Search Divided Attention

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Memory. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Memory. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin AQA A Level Psychology Topic Companion Memory Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin AQA A LEVEL Psychology topic companion: MEMORY Page 2 Contents Memory The multi-store model 3 Types of long-term memory 9 The working

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1995 The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the

More information

Selective Interference With the Maintenance of Location Information in Working Memory

Selective Interference With the Maintenance of Location Information in Working Memory Neuropsychology Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1996, Vol. I0, No. 2, 228-240 08944105/96/$3.00 Selective Interference With the Maintenance of Location Information in Working

More information

Probabilistic Retroactive Interference: The Role of Accessibility Bias in Interference Effects

Probabilistic Retroactive Interference: The Role of Accessibility Bias in Interference Effects Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 136, No. 2, 200 216 0096-3445/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.200 Probabilistic Retroactive

More information

Working Memory, Attention Control, and the N-Back Task: A Question of Construct Validity

Working Memory, Attention Control, and the N-Back Task: A Question of Construct Validity Working Memory, Attention Control, and the N-Back Task: A Question of Construct Validity By: Michael J. Kane, Andrew R. A. Conway, Timothy K. Miura and Gregory J. H. Colflesh Kane, M.J., Conway, A.R.A.,

More information

Keeping Autism in Mind

Keeping Autism in Mind Page 1 of 7 With a clear understanding of the nature of Autism there is much that can be done to help people with Autism through their everyday life. In highlighting some of the features of Autism this

More information

Chapter 3 What do Psychological Refractory Period and Attentional Blink have in Common?

Chapter 3 What do Psychological Refractory Period and Attentional Blink have in Common? What do Psychological Refractory Period and Attentional Blink have in Common? Merel M. Pannebakker, Lorenza S. Colzato, Guido P. H. Band, & Bernhard Hommel Manuscript submitted for publication 63 Abstract

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The spacing and lag effect in free recall The spacing and lag effect in free recall Michael J. Kahana, Bradley R. Wellington & Marc W. Howard Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology Brandeis University Send correspondence to: Michael

More information

The Role of Spontaneous Retrieval, Monitoring and Sustained Attention in Prospective Memory

The Role of Spontaneous Retrieval, Monitoring and Sustained Attention in Prospective Memory Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Dissertations Department of Psychology 12-18-2013 The Role of Spontaneous Retrieval, Monitoring and Sustained Attention in Prospective

More information

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS. Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS. Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney Sonification Lab, School of Psychology Georgia Institute of Technology, 654 Cherry

More information

Introduction to Long-Term Memory

Introduction to Long-Term Memory Introduction to Long-Term Memory Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/26/2018: Lecture 05-4 Note: This Powerpoint presentation may contain macros that I wrote to help me create

More information

Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction

Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction Global aging poses significant burdens as age-related impairments in cognitive function affect quality

More information

Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010.

Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010. Replacing the frontal lobes? 1 Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010. Ben R. Newell 1 Christopher

More information

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Selective Attention Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form,

More information

ENGAGE: Level of awareness activity

ENGAGE: Level of awareness activity ENGAGE: Level of awareness activity ENGAGE: Meditation instructions Step 1: Explain how people meditate for religious and/or relaxation reasons. Step 2: Describe three major characteristics of an altered

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience Working memory 1 What is working memory? Brief, immediate memory for information we are currently processing. Closely related to attention: attending to something is often

More information

Information is taken in by the senses (input) then encoded in the brain where it is processed; this processing is followed by an output.

Information is taken in by the senses (input) then encoded in the brain where it is processed; this processing is followed by an output. Define information processing Information is taken in by the senses (input) then encoded in the brain where it is processed; this processing is followed by an output. Define memory Memory is the encoding,

More information

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition 1 Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition Intro Memory Empirical Studies Interf MR Scan LTM Codes DCT Imagery & Spatial Cognition Rel Org Principles ImplEnc SpatEq Neuro Imaging Critique StruEq Prop

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

False Recognition: Words and Images

False Recognition: Words and Images The Huron University College Journal of Learning and Motivation Volume 51 Issue 1 Article 3 2013 False Recognition: Words and Images Tristan Bell Follow this and additional works at: http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/hucjlm

More information