Dopamine receptor DOP-4 modulates habituation to repetitive photoactivation of a C. elegans polymodal nociceptor

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Dopamine receptor DOP-4 modulates habituation to repetitive photoactivation of a C. elegans polymodal nociceptor"

Transcription

1 Research Dopamine receptor to repetitive photoactivation of a C. elegans polymodal nociceptor Evan L. Ardiel, 1,4 Andrew C. Giles, 1,4 Alex J. Yu, 1 Theodore H. Lindsay, 3 Shawn R. Lockery, 3 and Catharine H. Rankin 1,2 1 DM Centre for Brain Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver V6T 2B5, Canada; 2 Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver V6T 1Z4, Canada; 3 Institute of Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403, USA Habituation is a highly conserved phenomenon that remains poorly understood at the molecular level. Invertebrate model systems, like Caenorhabditis elegans, can be a powerful tool for investigating this fundamental process. Here we established a high-throughput learning assay that used real-time computer vision software for behavioral tracking and optogenetics for stimulation of the C. elegans polymodal nociceptor, ASH. Photoactivation of ASH with ChR2 elicited backward locomotion and repetitive stimulation altered aspects of the response in a manner consistent with habituation. Recording photocurrents in ASH, we observed no evidence for light adaptation of ChR2. Furthermore, we ruled out fatigue by demonstrating that sensory input from the touch cells could dishabituate the ASH avoidance circuit. Food and dopamine signaling slowed habituation downstream from ASH excitation via D1-like dopamine receptor, DOP-4. This assay allows for large-scale genetic and drug screens investigating mechanisms of nociception modulation. Habituation is a nonassociative form of learning characterized by a decremented response to repeated stimulation. It can be observed in organisms across phylogeny and is often considered a cognitive building-block (Rankin et al. 2009). Consistent with this role, deficits in habituation are associated with a variety of neuropsychiatric disorders, including autism and schizophrenia (Braff et al. 1992; Kleinhans et al. 2009). Despite its omnipresence and conservation, the cellular mechanisms underlying habituation are poorly understood. Researchers have therefore turned to a diverse array of model organisms for mechanistic insights (Kandel 2004; Bozorgmehr et al. 2013; Roberts et al. 2013; Twick et al. 2014). C. elegans is a free-living nematode with a 302-cell nervous system of known connectivity (White et al. 1986). Its neurons are optically accessible through a transparent cuticle and genetically accessible with a sophisticated molecular biology toolkit. Despite its reproducible wiring diagram, C. elegans show a remarkable capacity for behavioral plasticity, including habituation (Rankin et al. 1990) and a variety of forms of associative learning (Ardiel and Rankin 2010). The best-characterized habituating behavior of C. elegans is the tap-withdrawal response, wherein the animal reverses following nonlocalized mechanosensory input a tap to the side of the Petri plate (Rankin et al. 1990; Bozorgmehr et al. 2013). This behavior arises from an integration of two antagonistic reflexes: backward locomotion and forward acceleration (Wicks and Rankin 1995). In this study, we examined habituation of reversal behavior in the absence of competing forward locomotion by stimulating ASH, paired polymodal nociceptors at the worm s nose. Activation of ASH neurons promotes backward locomotion away from the stimulus using many of the same interneurons as the tap-withdrawal response (Wicks and Rankin 1995; Guo et al. 2009; Piggott et al. 2011). 4 These authors contributed equally to this work. Corresponding author: crankin@psych.ubc.ca Article is online at ASH neurons are especially interesting in the context of habituation because of the diversity and salience of the cues they detect: toxic volatile and nonvolatile repellents, as well as osmotic pressure and physical contact (Bargmann et al. 1990; Kaplan and Horvitz 1993; Hilliard et al. 2002, 2005). Although an organism might not be expected to habituate to potentially lethal stimuli, repeated or prolonged exposure to naturally occurring ASHsensed stimuli leads to a decreased likelihood of responding that is mostly stimulus specific (Hart et al. 1999; Ezcurra et al. 2011; Hilliard et al. 2005; Lindy et al. 2014). Monitoring calcium transients in ASH, Hilliard et al. (2005) showed that the behavioral decrement correlates with a decrease in ASH cellular responsiveness and that both are at least partially dependent on GPC-1, a G-protein g- subunit. More recently, Lindy et al. (2014) found that reduced responding associated with prolonged exposure to hypertonicity depends on calcium flux through the TRPV-like channel OSM-9. As with tap habituation (Kindt et al. 2007), the decrement to ASH-sensed stimuli is affected by feeding status, with the presence of food promoting responding to repeated or prolonged exposure to the water-soluble repellent CuCl 2 (Ezcurra et al. 2011). Exogenous dopamine mimics the presence of food and a dopamine-deficient cat-2 mutant lacking tyrosine hydroxylase behaves on food as if tested in its absence, suggesting dopamine mediates the food effect; however, the key dopamine receptor has not been identified. Decreased cellular responsivity to specific stimuli (Hart et al. 1999; Hilliard et al. 2005) suggests that ASH may be adapting at the level of sensory transduction (e.g., by receptor internalization); however, habituation may also be occurring in parallel downstream. The molecular underpinnings of habituation are # 2016 Ardiel et al. This article is distributed exclusively by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press for the first 12 months after the full-issue publication date (see After 12 months, it is available under a Creative Commons License (Attribution- NonCommercial 4.0 International), as described at org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/. 23: ; Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press ISSN /16; Learning & Memory

2 poorly understood, but a key distinction from adaptation is that habituation is readily reversible, whereas recovery from adaptation requires time away from the stimulus. To better understand the response decrement associated with ASH, we established a high-throughput training assay that used real-time computer vision software for detailed behavioral tracking and optogenetic activation of ASH for tight temporal and intensity regulation. In addition to facilitating stimulus delivery, photoactivation prevented sensory adaptation by bypassing sensory transduction, thus allowing us to probe downstream loci of plasticity. By dissecting the response into multiple metrics we identified behavioral plasticity consistent with habituation. Through mutant analysis we found that GPC-1 and OSM-9 do not play a role in habituation and that dopamine signaling modulates it via D1-like dopamine receptor, DOP-4. Materials and Methods Strains Animals were maintained on nematode growth medium (NGM) seeded with Escherichia coli (OP50) as described in Brenner (1974). Two integrated Channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) transgenes were used for ASH photoactivation. Initial experiments were conducted using a strain with ChR2 under control of the sra-6 promoter (AQ2026 ljis105[sra-6p::chr2::yfp + unc-122p::gfp]), which expresses strongly in ASH and more weakly in a pair of sensory neurons (ASI) and interneurons (PVQ) (Troemel et al. 1995). To rule out a contribution of the off-target cells, we took advantage of a second strain that used intersecting promoters and FLP recombinase to specifically target ChR2 to ASH (AQ2235 lite-1(ce314) ljis114[gpa-13p::flpase + sra-6p::ftf::chr2::yfp]; Ezcurra et al. 2011; Schmitt et al. 2012). AQ2026 showed robust responding at irradiances below the threshold for the C. elegans innate response to blue light (we used 70 mw/mm 2 ); however, reduced ChR2 expression in AQ2235 necessitated increased irradiance (we used 250 mw/mm 2 ) and therefore experiments with this strain were performed in a background lacking LITE-1, a native C. elegans short-wavelength light receptor (Edwards et al. 2008). The transgenic background used is specified in the figure captions. The ljis105 and ljis114 ChR2 transgenes were crossed with available mutants to generate the following: VG53 glr-1(ky176); ljis105 VG54 mec-4(u253); ljis105 VG152 eat-4(ky5); ljis105 VG154 nmr-1(ak4); ljis105 VG155 glr-1(ky176); nmr-1(ak4); ljis105 VG177 cat-2(e1112); ljis105 VG224 cat-2(e1112); lite-1(ce314) ljis114 VG236 dop-3(ok295); ljis105 VG237 dop-1(vs101); ljis105 VG238 dop-1(ok398); ljis105 VG240 dop-2(vs105); lite-1(ce314) ljis114 VG241 dop-5(ok568); lite-1(ce314) ljis114 VG248 dop-4(ok1321); ljis105 VG249 dop-6(ok2090); ljis105 VG284 bas-1(ad446); cat-4(e1141); lite-1(ce314) ljis114 VG388 trp-4(sy695); lite-1(ce314) ljis114 VG389 trp-4(sy695); lite-1(ce314) ljis114; xuex584 [dat-1p::trp-4-sl2-yfp + unc-122p::gfp] VG515 osm-9(ky10); ljis105 VG524 gpc-1(pk298); ljis105 The dop-4 rescue plasmids (a gift from William Schafer, MRC Laboratory of Molecular Biology) (Ezcurra et al. 2011) were injected into the gonad of VG248 at 35 ng/ml with pcfj90 (myo- 2p::mCherry; 3ng/mL) (Frøkjaer-Jensen et al. 2008) for use as a visible marker and pbluescript to make the total DNA concentration 100 ng/ml (Mello et al. 1991). The following strains were generated by microinjection: VG394 dop-4(ok1321); ljis105; yvex[dop-4p::dop-4 + myo-2p::mcherry] VG384, VG385 dop-4(ok1321); ljis105; yvex[gpa-13p:: dop-4 + myo-2p::mcherry] VG566, VG567, VG568 dop-4(ok1321); ljis105; yvex [sra-6p::dop-4 + myo-2p::mcherry] Behavioral tracking NGM plates were spread with ml OP50 liquid culture mixed with all-trans-retinal (ATR; or equal volume of ethanol vehicle) for a final plate concentration of 5 mm ATR. Plates were stored at room temperature in the dark for h before use. For age-synchronized colonies, gravid adults were left 3 6 h to lay eggs before being removed from the plate. Animals were reared at 20 C and tested as 3 or 4 d olds. Behavioral tracking occurred directly on the rearing plates, except for experiments evaluating the effect of food or exogenous dopamine or those utilizing strains with extra-chromosomal arrays. For food + conditions, animals were transferred min before testing to plates streaked at most 2 h earlier with OP50 liquid culture (food+) or just the LB (food2). For dopamine treatment, animals were transferred to freshly made 10 mm dopamine (Sigma- Aldrich) plates min before testing. Animals with extrachromosomal arrays were picked based on expression of a fluorescent coinjection marker to ATR-containing food plates 24 h before testing, with control strains similarly handled. Multi-Worm Tracker software (version ) was used for stimulus delivery and image acquisition (Swierczek et al. 2011). Following a 3 5 min acclimatization phase, stimuli were presented using custom-built LED rings (Luxeon Star LEDs) capable of illuminating 60 or 35 mm (diameter) Petri plates with uniform blue light (max ¼ 70 or 250 mw/mm 2, respectively). An orange filter prevented the blue light from entering the camera. For dishabituation, taps were delivered by an electromagnetic tubular push solenoid (# , Ledex, Vandalia, OH, USA). Offline behavioral quantification with Choreography software (version 1.3.0_r1035; Swierczek et al. 2011) used shadowless, minimum-move-body 2, and minimum-time 20 filters to restrict the analysis to animals that moved at least two body lengths and were tracked for at least 20 sec. The MeasureReversal plugin was used to identify reversals occurring within 3 sec (dt ¼ 3) of the light pulse onset. Custom MatLab scripts organized and summarized Choreography output files. Naturally sensed stimuli The nose touch assay was conducted on rearing plates with the experimenter blind to strain (glr-1+ or 2) or treatment (ATR+ or 2). Nose touch sensitivity was assayed within 30 sec of the last light pulse. Positive responses were defined as reversals elicited by contact with an eyebrow hair placed perpendicular to the direction of forward motion (Kaplan and Horvitz 1993). For octanol exposure, four 3 ml drops of 30% 1-octanol or the ethanol vehicle were placed on the Petri plate lid of rearing plates. The plates were sealed with parafilm and the Multi-Worm Tracker was used to monitor responses elicited by ChR2 activation. Electrophysiology Whole-cell recordings followed procedures described in Lindsay et al. (2011). Briefly, worms were immobilized with cyanoacralate glue on an agarose pad and immersed in recording saline consisting of (in mm): 5 KCl, 10 HEPES, 8 CaCl 2, 143 NaCl, 30 glucose. ASH was exposed by cutting a small hole in the cuticle using a sharpened glass needle and patch clamped using an electrode filled with (in mm): 125 K-gluconate, 1 CaCl 2, 18 KCl, 1 MgCl 2, 10 HEPES. Neurons were voltage clamped at 275 mv and light pulses were delivered using a computer controlled LED Learning & Memory

3 illumination system (Rapp optoelectronics). Holding current was filtered at 2 khz and acquired at 10 khz. Charge transfer associated with each stimulus was calculated by integrating the holding current evoked during the stimulus epoch. Statistics One-way ANOVAs and Tukey s honestly significant difference criterion were used to compare responses between strains and treatments. For response probability, tests compared the mean from the proportion of worms responding on each plate (n ¼ number of plates tested). For duration and latency metrics, data were combined across plates and collective means were compared (n ¼ number of animals tested). For all statistical tests, a was Results Population assay for repeated ASH activation We adapted the Multi-Worm Tracker (Swierczek et al. 2011) for optogenetic experiments, allowing for simultaneous stimulation of ASH neurons with precise temporal and intensity regulation in an entire population of animals. Using a custom built LED light ring, reversal responses to blue light pulses were detected in the majority of animals with ChR2 driven by the sra-6 promoter (expresses in ASH, ASI, and PVQ; see below for data from a strain expressing ChR2 in ASH alone) and fed the essential opsin cofactor, ATR (Fig. 1A). Although the probability of an animal responding was relatively stable over a wide range of irradiance intensities and durations, the magnitude of the response was dictated by the illumination parameters, with response duration increasing with illumination irradiance and duration and response latency decreasing with increased illumination irradiance (Fig. 1B,C). This finding was similar to earlier reports using single-worm assays and is consistent with graded synaptic output of ASH (Lindsay et al. 2011; Husson et al. 2012). Avoidance responses to the various aversive stimuli detected by ASH can be genetically dissociated both at the levels of sensory transduction and signal propagation. Although photoactivation with ChR2 bypasses native sensory transduction machinery, deficits associated with synaptic transmission should generalize to stimuli simulated by photocurrents. For example, mutants lacking the AMPA-type glutamate receptor subunit GLR-1 are unresponsive to nose touch and delayed in responding to osmotic shock (Hart et al. 1995; Maricq et al. 1995; Mellem et al. 2002). To assess which types of stimuli the photoactivation parameters used in these studies mimicked the ChR2 transgene was crossed into several glutamate transmission mutants. Loss of the vesicular glutamate transporter, EAT-4, decreased the proportion of animals reversing, as well as the duration and latency of responses elicited by a 2-sec light pulse at 70 mw/mm 2 (Fig. 1D). As with EAT-4, loss of GLR-1 delayed the initiation of the reversal; however, the phenotype was less severe and neither the probability nor duration of the response was distinguishable from control (Fig. 1D), suggesting that additional glutamate receptor subunits play a role in the photoactivated response. The NMDA glutamate receptor subunit was dispensable for normal responding; however simultaneous loss of NMR-1 and GLR-1 recapitulated the extent of the eat-4 mutant s latency phenotype (Fig. 1D). This pattern of deficits was similar to those observed for osmotic shock (Mellem et al. 2002), suggesting that the 2-sec photostimulation was simulating an intense aversive stimulus and not nose touch. To examine the plasticity of the response, the 2-sec blue light pulse was administered every 10 sec for 5 min (i.e., 30 stimuli at 0.1 Hz). Repeated stimulation had a variety of behavioral consequences that were quantified with several metrics: proportion Figure 1. Activation of ChR2 in sra-6 expressing neurons elicits backward locomotion. (A) Raster plots of behavioral state in the presence (ATR+) or absence (ATR2) of all-trans retinal. Each line represents an animal, with black pixels depicting backward locomotion and remaining pixels (white) depicting forward locomotion or no movement. The blue bar represents whole-plate illumination with blue light at 0.07 mw/ mm 2. Scale bar corresponds to 1 sec (horizontal) and 30 animals (vertical). (B) Three response metrics (proportion, duration, and latency) for a 0.07 mw/mm 2 light pulse of several durations (left) and a 2-sec light pulse at several intensities (right). Circles are plate means, crosses are population means + SEM (n ¼ 2 4 plates). (C) Representative raster plots of behavior in response to a 0.07 mw/mm light pulse of 0.5, 1.5, and 2.5 sec (left)ora 2-sec light pulse at 0.004, 0.02, and 0.07 mw/mm 2 (right). (D) Proportion, duration and latency response metrics for glutamate transmission mutants stimulated with a 2-sec light pulse at 0.07 mw/mm 2. Circles are plate means, crosses are population means + SEM (n ¼ 4 plates), and asterisks denote statistically distinguishable groups. All data collected using the ljis105 ChR2 transgene. of the population reversing, response latency (time to initiate reversal), and reversal duration (Fig. 2A). With repeated stimulation the majority of animals reversed to each stimulus; however, the duration of the reversal responses decreased, and the latency increased. The 1-sec increase in response latency by the end of 30 stimuli could not fully account for the 2.5-sec decrease in response duration, suggesting that repetitive stimulation results in both delayed initiation and earlier termination of responses. Behavioral plasticity is specific to ASH photoactivation The experiments described above were conducted with a strain using the sra-6 promoter to drive ChR2 expression in ASH, ASI, and PVQ (Troemel et al. 1995). Neither ASI nor PVQ are thought to elicit rapid reversal responses, but chemosensory ASI neurons have been implicated in the inhibition of reversals (Gray et al Learning & Memory

4 Figure 2. Plasticity of reversal responses elicited by repeated photoactivation of sra-6 expressing cells (ASH, ASI, and PVQ; A) or just ASH (B). Three different response metrics: proportion responding with a reversal, duration of any reversals that occurred, and latency to initiate those reversal for thirty 2-sec light pulses administered at 0.1 Hz. Gray lines correspond to the individual plates (n ¼ 6 plates) comprising the mean (black line). 2005; Guo et al. 2015). To rule out a potential contribution from activating these off-target cells, we tested another strain constructed with intersecting promoters and FLP recombinase to specifically target ChR2 to only ASH (Ezcurra et al. 2011). Repeated photoactivation of ASH alone with thirty 2-sec light pulses resulted in a similar pattern of plasticity to that described above (Fig. 2A), i.e., the majority of animals responded to the final stimulus, but with a decreased reversal duration and increased reversal latency compared with the first stimulus (Fig. 2B). Photocurrents do not change with repeated stimulation Photocurrents in ASH were measured during stimulation to determine whether the behavioral decrement associated with repeated illumination (Fig. 2) was caused by light adaptation of ChR2. As expected, a 2-sec blue light pulse evoked inward currents in ASH neurons expressing functional ChR2. Importantly, the waveform of these currents did not change noticeably over 30 stimuli administered at 0.1 Hz (Fig. 3A) and there was no decrease in the magnitude of negative charge transfer (Fig. 3B). Although the precise cellular conditions in ASH differed between intact animals and those prepared for electrophysiological recordings, these data suggest that the behavioral decrement in response to repeated photoactivation was not caused by light adaptation of ChR2, but by downstream cellular or synaptic mechanisms. after a head-on collision with an eyelash than unstimulated controls, or stimulated animals reared in the absence of ATR (Fig. 4A). The experimental group s level of responding to nose touch was indistinguishable from the nose touch-defective glr-1 mutant. This is likely a function of the increased response latency associated with repeated photoactivation, as control animals immediately initiate backward locomotion upon contact with the eyelash. Prior exposure to a stimulus naturally sensed by ASH should have a similar behavioral consequence as repeated ASH photoactivation. Indeed, we found that a 3-min exposure to the volatile repellent 1-octanol decreased the duration and increased the latency of reversal responses elicited by ChR2 photocurrents (Fig. 4B). The decrease in response duration and increase in response latency were dependent on ASH activation, as the 3-min preexposure had no effect on a mutant lacking OSM-9, the TRPV-like channel required for initiating cell excitation to most, if not all, ASH-sensed stimuli (Fig. 4B; Colbert et al. 1997; Tobin et al. 2002; Hilliard et al. 2005). These experiments demonstrate that prior exposure to simulated stimuli affect responses to naturally sensed cues and vice versa. This bidirectional generalization confirms that the behavioral decrement observed in Figure 2 was not simply light adaptation of ChR2 and supports the validity of using optogenetics to understand the nature of response decrement to repeated stimulation. Although ASH senses a variety of cues, none are easily administered discretely to a large number of animals simultaneously. Optogenetics allows for consistent repetitive stimulation, which is important for habituation assays where the intensity and timing of stimuli determine the extent of the response decrement (Rankin et al. 2009). In addition, photoactivation bypasses sensory transduction and allows for the investigation of downstream loci of plasticity independent of adaptation at the level of sensory transduction. Generalization of simulated and naturally sensed stimuli If the observed decrements in responding (Fig. 2) were caused by processes downstream from ChR2-mediated photocurrents, then the behavioral consequence of repeated photoactivation would be expected to affect responding to naturally occurring cues. We tested this hypothesis by delivering twenty 2-sec light pulses at 0.1 Hz prior to assessing nose touch sensitivity. Stimulated ATR-fed ChR2 transgenic animals were indeed less likely to reverse Figure 3. Photocurrents are unaffected by training. (A) Membrane current in ASH for thirty 2-sec light pulses administered at 0.1 Hz. (B) For each stimulus, photocurrents were quantified by calculating the total charge transfer during 2 sec of illumination. Mean + SEM (n ¼ 11 animals). All data collected using the ljis105 ChR2 transgene Learning & Memory

5 Dishabituation In contrast to sensory adaptation or motor fatigue, habituation in many animals is an attentional process that can be readily reversed by presenting a novel or noxious dishabituating stimulus (Thompson and Spencer 1966). We tested whether a tap to the side of the Petri plate could dishabituate responses to repeated photoactivation of ASH. Tap was chosen because it could be discretely applied to the entire population simultaneously. Furthermore, the interneurons and motor neurons mediating the tap-withdrawal response are mostly overlapping with those required for ASH-mediated reversals (Wicks and Rankin 1995; Guo et al. 2009; Piggott et al. 2011). To test whether the response decrement associated with ASH photostimulation could be dishabituated, two groups of animals were given 2-sec light pulses at 0.1 Hz for 5 min, with the second to last stimulus (i.e., the 29th stimulus) either omitted or replaced by a tap. The results showed that tapped animals responded to the final light pulse with longer reversals (Fig. 5A) and shorter latencies than untapped controls (Fig. 5B). The untapped controls showed no evidence for spontaneous recovery over the 20 sec (Fig. 5A,B). Thus the tap dishabituated the reversal response to the light pulse. Since the behavioral decrement associated with repeated ASH photoactivation could be at least partially reversed by novel sensory input (i.e., a tap), sensory adaptation or motor fatigue could not fully account for the plasticity. The tap withdrawal response is mediated by five body touch receptor neurons located along the length of the animal (Wicks and Rankin 1995). Although ASH is not involved in the tap withdrawal response (Wicks and Rankin 1995), it is a mechanosensor and could potentially detect agar vibrations in response to tap. To test whether the body touch receptor neurons mediate dishabituation, a mec-4 loss-of-function allele was used to specifically render them insensitive to mechanical stimuli (O Hagan et al. 2005). Although worms carrying the mec-4 mutation responded and habituated to repeated ASH photoactivations, the habituated responses were not dishabituated by tap (Fig. 5C,D), indicating that activation of the touch receptor neurons mediates dishabituation in this assay. Figure 4. Generalization of natural and simulated stimuli. (A) Nose touch responding was affected by simulated stimuli (stim ¼ 2-sec light pulse 20 at 0.1 Hz), as the probability of crawling backward after a head-on collision with an eyelash was significantly reduced by light pulses if animals were reared on the essential opsin cofactor, ATR. # and & denote statistically distinguishable groups (n ¼ 41, 56, 46, 52 animals per group). (B) For control, but not for an osm-9 mutant, preexposure to octanol (oct) decremented the duration and increased the latency of reversals elicited by ChR2 photocurrents, when compared with preexposure to the ethanol vehicle (veh). Circles are plate means, crosses are population means + SEM (n ¼ 4 plates), asterisks denote statistically distinguishable groups and (n.s.) no significant difference. All data collected using the ljis105 ChR2 transgene. Recapitulating the cat-2 mutant phenotype In previous research, the G-protein g-subunit GPC-1, the TRPVlike channel OSM-9, and dopamine signaling have all been implicated in the behavioral decrement associated with repeated or prolonged exposure to stimuli naturally detected by ASH (Hilliard et al. 2005; Ezcurra et al. 2011; Lindy et al. 2014). We investigated whether mutations in genes encoding GPC-1, OSM-9, or CAT-2 (tyrosine hydroxylase, the rate limiting enzyme in dopamine biosynthesis) would similarly alter the decrement in response to repeated ASH photoactivation. We found no evidence for involvement of OSM-9 or GPC-1, as the corresponding loss of function mutants were indistinguishable from control in terms of the probability (Fig. 6A), duration (Fig. 6B), and latency (Fig. Figure 5. Sensory input from body touch receptors acts as a dishabituating cue. (A) Tap after training reversed the change in response (A) duration and (B) latency associated with repeated ASH activation. The dash-dot line is the initial response level and the dashed line is the habituated response level. (C,D) Habituated responding of a touch insensitive mec-4 mutant was not reversed by tap. Duration (C) and latency (D) of the reversal response elicited by the final 2-sec light pulse of training (hab) and after a dishabituating tap (dishab). Circles are plate means, crosses are population means + SEM (n ¼ 4 plates), asterisks denote statistically distinguishable groups, and (n.s.) no significant difference. All data collected using the ljis105 ChR2 transgene Learning & Memory

6 Figure 6. Analysis of mutations in genes previously implicated in repeated responding to naturally sensed ASH stimuli. Response probability (A), duration (B), and latency (C) for reversals elicited by thirty 2-sec light pulses administered at 0.1 Hz (n ¼ 4 plates). # and & denote statistically distinguishable groups based on the response to the final stimulus. All data collected using the ljis105 ChR2 transgene. 6C) of the first and final responses of training. The lack of a habituation phenotype in the optogenetic assay may be explained by stimulus-specific roles for GPC-1 and OSM-9 in ASH; however, we hypothesize that both of these molecules are functioning upstream of cell excitation to mediate adaptation at the level of sensory transduction and are therefore not relevant when ASH is directly depolarized with ChR2 photocurrents. In contrast, the dopamine deficient cat-2 mutant showed habituation deficits. Of the three response metrics, loss of cat-2 had the clearest effect on the proportion of worms responding, fewer mutants than control animals reversed after just a few trials and this continued to decline with repeated stimulation (Fig. 6A). This rapid decrement in proportion of animals reversing recapitulates the cat-2 mutant deficit associated with repeated or prolonged exposure to a naturally sensed stimulus, CuCl 2 (Ezcurra et al. 2011). For the cat-2 mutant it is difficult to interpret the duration and latency metrics because there were so few reversals at the end of the 30 stimuli; however restricting our analysis to the first few responses, we observed more rapid increase in reversal latency for the cat-2 mutant than for control (Fig. 6C), but no obvious difference for reversal duration after the initial stimulus (Fig. 6B). Because of the paucity of responses from the cat-2 mutant toward the end of the assay, the magnitude metrics (i.e., duration and latency) were not used in subsequent analyses. Food texture slows habituation via dopamine Because of its specificity to ASH, the ljis114 ChR2 transgene was preferentially used for subsequent mutant analysis, except when the gene of interest was located on chromosome X, where the ljis114 ChR2 transgene was integrated. As with the ljis105 ChR2 transgene, we found that disrupting dopamine biosynthesis in the ljis114 background resulted in a decreased likelihood of responding at the end of the habituation assay (Fig. 7A), thereby ruling out a necessary contribution from ASI and PVQ. This was true for both the monoamine deficient cat-4;bas-1 double mutant, as well as the dopamine deficient cat-2 mutant. C. elegans has eight dopaminergic neurons in three classes (CEP, ADE, and PDE). These cells are thought to directly detect the texture of the bacterial lawn (Sawin et al. 2000). To evaluate the role of food, we transferred animals to unseeded or freshly seeded plates immediately before testing. Recapitulating the observations of Ezcurra et al. (2011) for a naturally sensed cue (CuCl 2 ), we found that populations tested in the absence of food habituated faster in response to repeated ASH activation than populations tested on a bacterial lawn (Fig. 7B). Detection of the texture of the bacterial lawn requires the mechanosensitive TRPN channel, TRP-4 in the dopamine neurons (Li et al. 2006; Kindt et al. 2007). To confirm the link between food and dopamine signaling, we evaluated the loss-of-function phenotype for trp-4. Consistent with an inability to detect the bacterial lawn, the trp-4 mutant expressing ChR2 in ASH displayed a more rapid decrement in responding than control (Fig. 7C). In addition to the dopaminergic neurons, TRP-4 is expressed in at least two classes of interneurons, including DVA, where it functions as a stretch receptor for proprioceptive feedback (Li et al. 2006). To localize the TRP-4 site of action we rescued it in dopaminergic neurons by expressing TRP-4 cdna with a dat-1 promoter. The behavior of the trp-4 rescue strain was indistinguishable from control, with rescued animals being more responsive at the end of the assay than the trp-4 mutant (Fig. 7C). This indicates TRP-4 function in dopaminergic neurons is sufficient for food dependent suppression of habituation. Collectively, these results suggest a model where the texture of the bacterial food source stimulates dopamine release, which promotes persistent responses to aversive stimuli (i.e., slows habituation of response probability) by modulating the avoidance circuit downstream from ASH excitation. DOP-4 slows habituation To identify the receptor by which food and dopamine slowed habituation, we evaluated loss-of-function phenotypes for confirmed (DOP-1, DOP-2, DOP-3, and DOP-4) and potential (DOP-5 and DOP-6) dopamine GPCRs. The initial response probability of all mutant strains was indistinguishable from control animals (Fig. 8A). However, when evaluated at the final stimulus presentation, loss of DOP-4 an invertebrate-specific D1-like receptor (Sugiura et al. 2005) recapitulated the rapid habituation phenotype of the dopamine-deficient cat-2 mutant (Fig. 8B). To confirm the dop-4 mutation as the causative allele, we reintroduced dop-4 genomic DNA with its promoter. This fully rescued the deficit of the dop-4 mutant (Fig. 8C). Ezcurra et al. (2011) demonstrated that DOP-4 signaling increases ASH excitability in the presence of food. However, our data suggested that for repetitive stimulation, dopamine signaling modulated responses downstream from ASH excitation. In contrast to the naïve response described by Ezcurra et al. (2011), we found that DOP-4 expression in Learning & Memory

7 Figure 7. Dopamine signaling promotes responding during habituation training. (A) Loss of monoamine biosynthetic enzymes (CAT-2, BAS-1, CAT-4) altered the probability of responding relative to control (+; n ¼ 6 plates). (B) Comparing populations off of food (food2) with those tested on a bacterial lawn (food+; n ¼ 6 plates). Although all other experiments were conducted in the presence of a bacterial lawn, the food+ condition for Figure 7B refers to a very thin bacterial lawn that was spread with liquid culture at most 2 h before testing (compared with the usual 24 h or more before testing), thus the more rapid decline in responding than was observed for other trials with food (n ¼ 6 plates). (C) Loss of trp-4 recapitulated the dopamine deficient cat-2 mutant phenotype. Loss of trp-4 could be compensated for by restoring trp-4 expression to dopaminergic neurons (n ¼ 8 plates). Mean + SEM. # and & denote statistically distinguishable groups based on the likelihood of responding to the final stimulus. All data collected using the ljis114 ChR2 transgene. ASH did not rescue the habituation deficit of the dop-4 mutant as determined by five independent lines each expressing one of two rescue constructs used by Ezcurra et al. (2011) (gpa-13p::dop-4 or sra-6p::dop-4). To confirm DOP-4 as the receptor by which dopamine signaling promotes responding to repetitive ASH activation, we exposed animals to exogenous dopamine during habituation training. This treatment compensated for loss of tyrosine hydroxylase, but not loss of DOP-4, as the cat-2 mutant was indistinguishable from control, while the dop-4 mutant was less responsive than both strains (Fig. 8D). Thus, the D1-like receptor DOP-4 is essential for dopamine to exert its influence on habituation of the ASH avoidance circuit. Discussion Population assay for habituation of ASH-mediated reversals We have established a new C. elegans learning assay, combining optogenetics with real-time computer vision software (the Multi-Worm Tracker) (Swierczek et al. 2011) to enable both highthroughput and highly detailed behavioral analysis of responses elicited by the polymodal nociceptor, ASH. Although population assays can be used to obtain avoidance indices for naturally sensed stimuli, probing the dynamics of the reversal response itself requires discrete stimulus presentation. This has traditionally been accomplished with single worm assays (Kaplan and Horvitz 1993; Hilliard et al. 2002; Chao et al. 2004), although recently developed microfluidic devices could be used for stimulating multiple animals with watersoluble repellents (Larsch et al. 2013). In addition to increasing throughput, optogenetics allowed us to specifically target ASH and bypass sensory transduction to investigate downstream loci of plasticity. Repeated photoactivation of ASH induced a decrement in responding consistent with the characteristics of habituation, as electrophysiological and behavioral data ruled out sensory adaptation or motor fatigue. Most notably, mechanosensory input from the touch receptor neurons could reverse the habituated response (Fig. 5). The behavioral plasticity was observed using two different ChR2 transgenes and various stimulation protocols. Photoactivation with ChR2 bypassed sensory transduction, suggesting that at least one locus of plasticity lies downstream from cell excitation. Earlier work using naturally sensed cues implicated the G-protein g- subunit GPC-1, the TRPV-like channel OSM-9, and dopamine signaling in behavioral decrement associated with repeated or prolonged ASH activation (Hilliard et al. 2005; Ezcurra et al. 2011; Lindy et al. 2014). In our optogeneticbased assay, dopamine deficient animals showed altered habituation; however loss of GPC-1 or OSM-9 had no effect on the ASH-mediated response or plasticity (Fig. 6). We propose that sensory adaptation at the level of stimulus detection is mediated by GPC-1 and OSM-9 and occurs in parallel with more downstream mechanisms of habituation. Consistent with this model, previous assays (Hilliard et al. 2005; Ezcurra et al. 2011) found more rapid decrements in the probability of responding than those observed here. Independence of response measures A number of results demonstrate that different aspects of a response (e.g., probability, duration, and latency) can be independently controlled. For example, loss of the vesicular glutamate transporter EAT-4 decreased the likelihood of responding to ASH activation and delayed and shortened the responses that did occur, but loss of GLR-1 affected only response latency, a deficit exacerbated by simultaneous loss of NMR-1. These data show that multiple glutamate receptors shape the response elicited by ASH activation. Furthermore, both the response duration and latency habituated, but the 1 sec increase in response latency by the end of 30 stimuli could not fully account for the 2.5 sec decrease in response duration, suggesting they are independent metrics. Finally, mutations in genes altering dopamine transmission had the largest effect on habituation of response probability, but also altered response latency early in habituation training. In contrast, the dopamine deficit had little effect on habituation of response duration. Taken together, these data support the hypothesis that the proportion of worms reversing and the duration and latency of the response are mediated by different mechanisms Learning & Memory

8 Figure 8. Loss of dop-4 recapitulates the phenotype of a dopamine deficient cat-2 mutant. Proportion of the population reversing relative to control for the initial (A) and 30th (B) 2-sec light pulse delivered at 0.1 Hz. Circles are plate means, crosses are population means+ SEM (n ¼ 4 plates), and the asterisks denote statistically distinguishable groups. (C) Proportion of animals responding to each stimulus of habituation training, with reintroduction of dop-4 (dop-4p::dop-4) rescuing the rapid response decrement of the dop-4 mutant (n ¼ 4 plates). Mean + SEM. # and & denote statistically distinguishable groups based on the likelihood of responding to the final stimulus. (D) Proportion of animals responding on 10 mm exogenous dopamine (n ¼ 6 plates). Mean+ SEM. # and & denote statistically distinguishable groups based on the likelihood of responding to the final stimulus. All data collected using the ljis105 ChR2 transgene, except for dop-2 and dop-5 mutants, which had the ljis114 ChR2 transgene. Dopamine modulates habituation Dopamine is a neuromodulator frequently implicated in neural and behavioral plasticity in a wide range of organisms. A role for dopamine signaling has also been proposed for habituation in mammals (Lloyd et al. 2014), for which it is well-positioned based on responding of mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurons to unconditioned salient and arousing sensory input (Horvitz 2000). In C. elegans, dopamine influences a variety of behaviors, including locomotion (Sawin et al. 2000; Hills et al. 2004; Vidal-Gadea et al. 2011) and learning (Bettinger and McIntire 2004; Sanyal et al. 2004; Kindt et al. 2007; Hukema et al. 2008; Voglis and Tavernarakis 2008; Kimura et al. 2010). Previous work has demonstrated that dopamine signaling modulates naïve responding to ASH-sensed cues, increasing or decreasing sensitivity depending on the stimulus (Ferkey et al. 2007; Wragg et al. 2007; Ezcurra et al. 2011). However, unlike habituation, this modulation is thought to occur upstream of cell excitation. Here we showed that loss of dopamine signaling led to a rapid decrease in the proportion of worms responding to repeated ASH photoactivation and identified the dopamine receptor, DOP-4, as a key mediator. Using CuCl 2 as a stimulus, Ezcurra et al. (2011) also found that loss of dopamine signaling resulted in a more rapid decrement in responding; however, in their assay the dop-4 mutant did not have a phenotype. It is possible that DOP-4 signaling modulates habituation to some, but not all ASH-sensed stimuli. Alternatively, a role for DOP-4 may have been masked by sensory adaptation to CuCl 2. Our optogenetic approach removed the contribution of adaptation to specifically evaluate habituation, which may better reveal the dop-4 mutant phenotype. For habituation to nonlocalized mechanosensory input (taps), the D1-like DOP-1 dopamine receptor functions in the body touch receptor neurons to promote cell excitability (Kindt et al. 2007). Although DOP-1 and DOP-4 are both D1-like receptors, DOP-4 belongs to an invertebrate specific subfamily that includes the Drosophila receptor, DAMB, and the Apis mellifera receptor, DOP-2 (Sugiura et al. 2005). The deficits associated with each dopamine receptor appear to be assay-specific, as loss of DOP-4 did not decrease the probability of response in the tap habituation assay (data not shown) and loss of DOP-1 did not affect habituation of ASH-mediated reversals (Fig. 8A). Thus habituation of these avoidance circuits is similarly affected at the behavioral level by the same neuromodulator, but via distinct receptors. It remains to be determined whether the cellular signals are also divergent: one difference appears to be their site of action, with DOP-1 functioning in the sensory cells to promote excitability and DOP-4 signaling in some as-yet-unidentified downstream cell or cells. Further comparisons between habituation to ASH stimulation and habituation to tap will be useful for evaluating the generalizability of findings between circuits. As mechanistic insights emerge, it is apparent that multiple cellular processes can underlie habituation, for example the depression of excitatory synapses described for the gill and siphon withdrawal reflex of Aplysia (Kandel et al. 2004) and the recurrent inhibition motif of the Drosophila olfactory system (Twick et al. 2014). The best prospect for a complete characterization of habituation is the continued use of model systems with tractable nervous systems. Combining optogenetics with the Multi-Worm Tracker has removed the bottleneck of data collection and analysis, making it feasible to conduct large parametric studies or drug or genetic screens targeting habituation of the ASH avoidance circuit. Acknowledgments Strains were provided by the CGC (funding from NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs P40 OD010440), as well as the laboratories of William Schafer (MRC Laboratory of Molecular Biology) and Shawn Xu (University of Michigan). Andrew Lee, Myron Huen, and Alex Liang helped cross strains and run preliminary experiments. Research was funded with a NSERC CGSD3 to E.L.A. and a NSERC Discovery grant (NSERC RGPIN ) to C.H.R. References Ardiel EL, Rankin CH An elegant mind: learning and memory in Caenorhabditis elegans. Learn Mem 17: Bargmann CI, Thomas JH, Horvitz HR Chemosensory cell function in the behavior and development of Caenorhabditis elegans. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 55: Bettinger JC, McIntire SL State-dependency in C. elegans. Genes Brain Behav 3: Bozorgmehr T, Ardiel EL, McEwan AH, Rankin CH Mechanisms of plasticity in a Caenorhabditis elegans mechanosensory circuit. Front Physiol 4: 88. Braff DL, Grillon C, Geyer MA Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49: Learning & Memory

9 Brenner S The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 77: Chao MY, Komatsu H, Fukuto HS, Dionne HM, Hart AC Feeding status and serotonin rapidly and reversibly modulate a Caenorhabditis elegans chemosensory circuit. Proc Natl Acad Sci 101: Colbert HA, Smith TL, Bargmann CI OSM-9, a novel protein with structural similarity to channels, is required for olfaction, mechanosensation, and olfactory adaptation in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci 17: Edwards SL, Charlie NK, Milfort MC, Brown BS, Gravlin CN, Knecht JE, Miller KG A novel molecular solution for ultraviolet light detection in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS Biol 6(8): e198. Ezcurra M, Tanizawa Y, Swoboda P, Schafer WR Food sensitizes C. elegans avoidance behaviours through acute dopamine signalling. EMBO J 30: Ferkey DM, Hyde R, Haspel G, Dionne HM, Hess HA, Suzuki H, Schafer WR, Koelle MR, Hart AC C. elegans G protein regulator RGS-3 controls sensitivity to sensory stimuli. Neuron 53: Frøkjaer-Jensen C, Davis MW, Hopkins CE, Newman BJ, Thummel JM, Olesen SP, Grunnet M, Jorgensen EM Single-copy insertion of transgenes in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Genet 40: Gray JM, Hill JJ, Bargmann CI A circuit for navigation in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 102: Guo ZV, Hart AC, Ramanathan S Optical interrogation of neural circuits in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Methods 6: Guo M, Wu TH, Song YX, Ge MH, Su CM, Niu WP, Li L-L, Xu Z-J, Ge C-L, Al-Mhanawi MTH, et al Reciprocal inhibition between sensory ASH and ASI neurons modulates nociception and avoidance in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Commun 6: Hart AC, Sims S, Kaplan JM Synaptic code for sensory modalities revealed by C. elegans GLR-1 glutamate receptor. Nature 378: Hart AC, Kass J, Shapiro JE, Kaplan JM Distinct signaling pathways mediate touch and osmosensory responses in a polymodal sensory neuron. J Neurosci 19: Hilliard MA, Bargmann CI, Bazzicalupo P C. elegans responds to chemical repellents by integrating sensory inputs from the head and the tail. Curr Biol 12: Hilliard MA, Apicella AJ, Kerr R, Suzuki H, Bazzicalupo P, Schafer WR In vivo imaging of C. elegans ASH neurons: cellular response and adaptation to chemical repellents. EMBO J 24: Hills T, Brockie PJ, Maricq AV Dopamine and glutamate control area-restricted search behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci 24: Horvitz JC Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient non-reward events. Neuroscience 96: Hukema RK, Rademakers S, Jansen G Gustatory plasticity in C. elegans involves integration of negative cues and NaCl taste mediated by serotonin, dopamine, and glutamate. Learn Mem 15: Husson SJ, Liewald JF, Schultheis C, Stirman JN, Lu H, Gottschalk A Microbial light-activatable proton pumps as neuronal inhibitors to functionally dissect neuronal networks in C. elegans. PLoS One 7: e Kandel ER The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialog between genes and synapses. Biosci Rep 24: Kaplan JM, Horvitz HR A dual mechanosensory and chemosensory neuron in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 90: Kimura KD, Fujita K, Katsura I Enhancement of odor avoidance regulated by dopamine signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci 30: Kindt KS, Quast KB, Giles AC, De S, Hendrey D, Nicastro I, Rankin CH, Schafer WR Dopamine mediates context-dependent modulation of sensory plasticity in C. elegans. Neuron 55: Kleinhans NM, Johnson LC, Richards T, Mahurin R, Greenson J, Dawson G, Aylward E Reduced neural habituation in the amygdala and social impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Psychiatry 166: Larsch J, Ventimiglia D, Bargmann CI, Albrecht DR High-throughput imaging of neuronal activity in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 110: E4266 E4273. Li W, Feng Z, Sternberg PW, Xu XZ A C. elegans stretch receptor neuron revealed by a mechanosensitive TRP channel homologue. Nature 440: Lindsay TH, Thiele TR, Lockery SR Optogenetic analysis of synaptic transmission in the central nervous system of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Commun 2: 306. Lindy AS, Parekh PK, Zhu R, Kanju P, Chintapalli SV, Tsvilovskyy V, et al TRPV channel-mediated calcium transients in nociceptor neurons are dispensable for avoidance behaviour. Nat Commun 5: Lloyd DR, Medina DJ, Hawk LW, Fosco WD, Richards JB Habituation of reinforcer effectiveness. Front Integr Neurosci 7: 107. Maricq AV, Peckol E, Driscoll M, Bargmann CI Mechanosensory signalling in C. elegans mediated by the GLR-1 glutamate receptor. Nature 378: Mellem JE, Brockie PJ, Zheng Y, Madsen DM, Maricq AV Decoding of polymodal sensory stimuli by postsynaptic glutamate receptors in C. elegans. Neuron 36: Mello CC, Kramer JM, Stinchcomb D, Ambros V Efficient gene transfer in C.elegans: extrachromosomal maintenance and integration of transforming sequences. EMBO J 10: O Hagan R, Chalfie M, Goodman MB The MEC-4 DEG/ENaC channel of Caenorhabditis elegans touch receptor neurons transduces mechanical signals. Nat Neurosci 8: Piggott BJ, Liu J, Feng Z, Wescott SA, Xu XZ The neural circuits and synaptic mechanisms underlying motor initiation in C. elegans. Cell 147: Rankin CH, Beck CD, Chiba CM Caenorhabditis elegans: a new model system for the study of learning and memory. Behav Brain Res 37: Rankin CH, Abrams T, Barry RJ, Bhatnagar S, Clayton DF, Colombo J, Coppola G, Geyer MA, Glanzman DL, Marsland S, et al Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92: Roberts AC, Bill BR, Glanzman DL Learning and memory in zebrafish larvae. Front Neural Circuits 7: 126. Sanyal S, Wintle RF, Kindt KS, Nuttley WM, Arvan R, Fitzmaurice P, et al Dopamine modulates the plasticity of mechanosensory responses in Caenorhabditis elegans. EMBO J 23: Sawin ER, Ranganathan R, Horvitz HR C. elegans locomotory rate is modulated by the environment through a dopaminergic pathway and by experience through a serotonergic pathway. Neuron 26: Schmitt C, Schultheis C, Husson SJ, Liewald JF, Gottschalk A Specific expression of channelrhodopsin-2 in single neurons of Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS One 7: e Sugiura M, Fuke S, Suo S, Sasagawa N, Van Tol HH, Ishiura S Characterization of a novel D2-like dopamine receptor with a truncated splice variant and a D1-like dopamine receptor unique to invertebrates from Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurochem 94: Swierczek NA, Giles AC, Rankin CH, Kerr RA High-throughput behavioral analysis in C. elegans. Nat Methods 8: Thompson RF, Spencer WA Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychol Rev 73: Tobin D, Madsen D, Kahn-Kirby A, Peckol E, Moulder G, Barstead R, et al Combinatorial expression of TRPV channel proteins defines their sensory functions and subcellular localization in C. elegans neurons. Neuron 35: Troemel ER, Chou JH, Dwyer ND, Colbert HA, Bargmann CI Divergent seven transmembrane receptors are candidate chemosensory receptors in C. elegans. Cell 83: Twick I, Lee JA, Ramaswami M Olfactory habituation in Drosophila-odor encoding and its plasticity in the antennal lobe. Prog Brain Res 208: Vidal-Gadea A, Topper S, Young L, Crisp A, Kressin L, Elbel E, Maples T, Brauner M, Erbguth K, Axelrod A, et al Caenorhabditis elegans selects distinct crawling and swimming gaits via dopamine and serotonin. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108: Voglis G, Tavernarakis N A synaptic DEG/ENaC ion channel mediates learning in C. elegans by facilitating dopamine signalling. EMBO J 27: White JG, Southgate E, Thomson JN, Brenner S The structure of the nervous system of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 314: Wicks SR, Rankin CH Integration of mechanosensory stimuli in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci 15: Wragg RT, Hapiak V, Miller SB, Harris GP, Gray J, Komuniecki PR, Komuniecki RW Tyramine and octopamine independently inhibit serotonin-stimulated aversive behaviors in Caenorhabditis elegans through two novel amine receptors. J Neurosci 27: Received January 26, 2016; accepted in revised form April 27, Learning & Memory

10 Dopamine receptor to repetitive photoactivation of a C. elegans polymodal nociceptor Evan L. Ardiel, Andrew C. Giles, Alex J. Yu, et al. Learn. Mem. 2016, 23: Access the most recent version at doi: /lm References Creative Commons License Alerting Service This article cites 55 articles, 20 of which can be accessed free at: This article is distributed exclusively by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press for the first 12 months after the full-issue publication date (see After 12 months, it is available under a Creative Commons License (Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International), as described at Receive free alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top right corner of the article or click here Ardiel et al.; Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

GAP-FREE NEURAL CIRCUITS CLASS #3: C. elegans touch-induced locomotion

GAP-FREE NEURAL CIRCUITS CLASS #3: C. elegans touch-induced locomotion GAP-FREE NEURAL CIRCUITS CLASS #3: C. elegans touch-induced locomotion OUTLINE: C. elegans background o General background o Neural signaling: no action potentials Excitatory circuits: Touch-induced locomotion

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature10643 Supplementary Table 1. Identification of hecw-1 coding polymorphisms at amino acid positions 322 and 325 in 162 strains of C. elegans. WWW.NATURE.COM/NATURE 1 Supplementary Figure

More information

Chemotaxis Response in Caenorhabditis elegans to an Olfactory Repellent Paired with Nicotine

Chemotaxis Response in Caenorhabditis elegans to an Olfactory Repellent Paired with Nicotine Chemotaxis Response in Caenorhabditis elegans to an Olfactory Repellent Paired with Nicotine Amanda Bigness and Dr. Robert Grammer Belmont University Recent studies in C. elegans, a model organism with

More information

Reviewers' comments: Reviewer #1 (Remarks to the Author):

Reviewers' comments: Reviewer #1 (Remarks to the Author): Reviewers' comments: Reviewer #1 (Remarks to the Author): Overall, this is an interesting study. The authors used a custom microfluidic device to deliver precise concentrations of bacterial food to worms

More information

Nonassociative Learning

Nonassociative Learning Nonassociative Learning The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation As Published Publisher Kirchkamp, Oliver et al. Nonassociative

More information

CHAPTER 4: Multilevel Modulation in C.elegans Sleep

CHAPTER 4: Multilevel Modulation in C.elegans Sleep CHAPTER 4: Multilevel Modulation in C.elegans Sleep 28 Submitted for publication as Julie Cho & Paul Sternberg. Multilevel Modulation of a Sensory Motor Circuit in C. elegans Sleep 4.1 ABSTRACT 29 Sleep

More information

Concentration memory-dependent synaptic plasticity of a taste circuit regulates salt chemotaxis in Caenorhabditis elegans

Concentration memory-dependent synaptic plasticity of a taste circuit regulates salt chemotaxis in Caenorhabditis elegans Supplementary Information for: Concentration memory-dependent synaptic plasticity of a taste circuit regulates salt chemotaxis in Caenorhabditis elegans Hirofumi Kunitomo, Hirofumi Sato, Ryo Iwata, Yohsuke

More information

Shadowing and Blocking as Learning Interference Models

Shadowing and Blocking as Learning Interference Models Shadowing and Blocking as Learning Interference Models Espoir Kyubwa Dilip Sunder Raj Department of Bioengineering Department of Neuroscience University of California San Diego University of California

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Hyperpolarization-activated cation channels inhibit EPSPs by interactions with M-type K + channels Meena S. George, L.F. Abbott, Steven A. Siegelbaum Supplementary Information Part 1: Supplementary Figures

More information

Behavioral Neurobiology

Behavioral Neurobiology Behavioral Neurobiology The Cellular Organization of Natural Behavior THOMAS J. CAREW University of California, Irvine Sinauer Associates, Inc. Publishers Sunderland, Massachusetts PART I: Introduction

More information

Introduction to Neurobiology

Introduction to Neurobiology Biology 240 General Zoology Introduction to Neurobiology Nervous System functions: communication of information via nerve signals integration and processing of information control of physiological and

More information

Questions Addressed Through Study of Behavioral Mechanisms (Proximate Causes)

Questions Addressed Through Study of Behavioral Mechanisms (Proximate Causes) Jan 28: Neural Mechanisms--intro Questions Addressed Through Study of Behavioral Mechanisms (Proximate Causes) Control of behavior in response to stimuli in environment Diversity of behavior: explain the

More information

Dopamine Receptors Antagonistically Regulate Behavioral Choice between Conflicting Alternatives in C. elegans

Dopamine Receptors Antagonistically Regulate Behavioral Choice between Conflicting Alternatives in C. elegans RESEARCH ARTICLE Dopamine Receptors Antagonistically Regulate Behavioral Choice between Conflicting Alternatives in C. elegans Daoyong Wang, Yonglin Yu, Yinxia Li, Yang Wang, Dayong Wang* Key Laboratory

More information

A Dynamic Neural Network Model of Sensorimotor Transformations in the Leech

A Dynamic Neural Network Model of Sensorimotor Transformations in the Leech Communicated by Richard Andersen 1 A Dynamic Neural Network Model of Sensorimotor Transformations in the Leech Shawn R. Lockery Yan Fang Terrence J. Sejnowski Computational Neurobiological Laboratory,

More information

Sample Lab Report 1 from 1. Measuring and Manipulating Passive Membrane Properties

Sample Lab Report 1 from  1. Measuring and Manipulating Passive Membrane Properties Sample Lab Report 1 from http://www.bio365l.net 1 Abstract Measuring and Manipulating Passive Membrane Properties Biological membranes exhibit the properties of capacitance and resistance, which allow

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/317/5841/183/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Astrocytes Potentiate Transmitter Release at Single Hippocampal Synapses Gertrudis Perea and Alfonso Araque* *To whom

More information

How we study the brain: a survey of methods used in neuroscience

How we study the brain: a survey of methods used in neuroscience How we study the brain: a survey of methods used in neuroscience Preparing living neurons for recording Large identifiable neurons in a leech Rohon-Beard neurons in a frog spinal cord Living slice of a

More information

STRUCTURAL ELEMENTS OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

STRUCTURAL ELEMENTS OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ELEMENTS OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURE AND MAINTENANCE OF NEURONS (a) (b) Dendrites Cell body Initial segment collateral terminals (a) Diagrammatic representation of a neuron. The break in

More information

Applied Neuroscience. Conclusion of Science Honors Program Spring 2017

Applied Neuroscience. Conclusion of Science Honors Program Spring 2017 Applied Neuroscience Conclusion of Science Honors Program Spring 2017 Review Circle whichever is greater, A or B. If A = B, circle both: I. A. permeability of a neuronal membrane to Na + during the rise

More information

Psychophysical laws. Legge di Fechner: I=K*log(S/S 0 )

Psychophysical laws. Legge di Fechner: I=K*log(S/S 0 ) Psychophysical laws Legge di Weber: ΔS=K*S Legge di Fechner: I=K*log(S/S 0 ) Sensory receptors Vision Smell Taste Touch Thermal senses Pain Hearing Balance Proprioception Sensory receptors Table 21-1 Classification

More information

Neurons of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis (BNST)

Neurons of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis (BNST) Neurons of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis (BNST) Electrophysiological Properties and Their Response to Serotonin DONALD G. RAINNIE a Harvard Medical School and Department of Psychiatry, Brockton

More information

Enhancement of Odor Avoidance Regulated by Dopamine Signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans

Enhancement of Odor Avoidance Regulated by Dopamine Signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1, 2010 30(48):16365 16375 16365 Development/Plasticity/Repair Enhancement of Odor Avoidance Regulated by Dopamine Signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans Koutarou D.

More information

TMC9 as a novel mechanosensitive ion channel

TMC9 as a novel mechanosensitive ion channel TMC9 as a novel mechanosensitive ion channel Mechanical forces play numerous roles in physiology. When an object contacts our skin, it exerts a force that is encoded as touch or pain depending on its intensity.

More information

Part 11: Mechanisms of Learning

Part 11: Mechanisms of Learning Neurophysiology and Information: Theory of Brain Function Christopher Fiorillo BiS 527, Spring 2012 042 350 4326, fiorillo@kaist.ac.kr Part 11: Mechanisms of Learning Reading: Bear, Connors, and Paradiso,

More information

Neural Mechanisms for Evaluating Environmental Variability in Caenorhabditis elegans

Neural Mechanisms for Evaluating Environmental Variability in Caenorhabditis elegans Article Neural Mechanisms for Evaluating Environmental Variability in Caenorhabditis elegans Highlights d d d d C. elegans learn variability in their food environment and modify future behavior The authors

More information

Lecture 22: A little Neurobiology

Lecture 22: A little Neurobiology BIO 5099: Molecular Biology for Computer Scientists (et al) Lecture 22: A little Neurobiology http://compbio.uchsc.edu/hunter/bio5099 Larry.Hunter@uchsc.edu Nervous system development Part of the ectoderm

More information

THE NERVOUS SYSTEM. Homeostasis Strand

THE NERVOUS SYSTEM. Homeostasis Strand THE NERVOUS SYSTEM Homeostasis Strand Introduction In general, a nervous system has three overlapping functions : 1. Sensory input conduction of signals from sensory receptors to integration centres 2.

More information

Supplementary Figure 1

Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Miniature microdrive, spike sorting and sleep stage detection. a, A movable recording probe with 8-tetrodes (32-channels). It weighs ~1g. b, A mouse implanted with 8 tetrodes in

More information

Jonathan Kindberg BNF Molecular Biology Through Discovery. Research Proposal

Jonathan Kindberg BNF Molecular Biology Through Discovery. Research Proposal Jonathan Kindberg BNF0 491 Molecular Biology Through Discovery Research Proposal The effect of a single nucleotide polymorphism in the NPY protein and ethanol consumption in mice (08 December 2012) I.

More information

Spiking Inputs to a Winner-take-all Network

Spiking Inputs to a Winner-take-all Network Spiking Inputs to a Winner-take-all Network Matthias Oster and Shih-Chii Liu Institute of Neuroinformatics University of Zurich and ETH Zurich Winterthurerstrasse 9 CH-857 Zurich, Switzerland {mao,shih}@ini.phys.ethz.ch

More information

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25 Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP Reading: BCP Chapter 25 Memory Systems Learning is the acquisition of new knowledge or skills. Memory is the retention of learned information. Many different

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) The hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Trial structure for go/no-go behavior

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Trial structure for go/no-go behavior Supplementary Figure 1 Trial structure for go/no-go behavior a, Overall timeline of experiments. Day 1: A1 mapping, injection of AAV1-SYN-GCAMP6s, cranial window and headpost implantation. Water restriction

More information

A conserved dopamine-cholecystokinin signaling pathway shapes context-dependent Caenorhabditis elegans behavior

A conserved dopamine-cholecystokinin signaling pathway shapes context-dependent Caenorhabditis elegans behavior University of Massachusetts Medical School escholarship@umms Open Access Articles Open Access Publications by UMMS Authors 8-28-2014 A conserved dopamine-cholecystokinin signaling pathway shapes context-dependent

More information

Implantable Microelectronic Devices

Implantable Microelectronic Devices ECE 8803/4803 Implantable Microelectronic Devices Fall - 2015 Maysam Ghovanloo (mgh@gatech.edu) School of Electrical and Computer Engineering Georgia Institute of Technology 2015 Maysam Ghovanloo 1 Outline

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature12024 entary Figure 1. Distribution of the number of earned cocaine Supplementary Figure 1. Distribution of the number of earned cocaine infusions in Shock-sensitive

More information

Electrical Properties of Neurons. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota

Electrical Properties of Neurons. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota Electrical Properties of Neurons Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota 1 Neuronal Communication Neurons communicate with other cells, often over long distances. The electrical

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) Hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Notes: Synapse. Overview. PSYC Summer Professor Claffey PDF. Conversion from an signal to a signal - electrical signal is the

Notes: Synapse. Overview. PSYC Summer Professor Claffey PDF. Conversion from an signal to a signal - electrical signal is the PSYC 170 - Summer 2013 - Professor Claffey Notes: Synapse PDF Overview Conversion from an signal to a signal - electrical signal is the - chemical signal is the Presynaptic - refers to that sends/receives

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature10776 Supplementary Information 1: Influence of inhibition among blns on STDP of KC-bLN synapses (simulations and schematics). Unconstrained STDP drives network activity to saturation

More information

ANATOMY AND PHYSIOLOGY OF NEURONS. AP Biology Chapter 48

ANATOMY AND PHYSIOLOGY OF NEURONS. AP Biology Chapter 48 ANATOMY AND PHYSIOLOGY OF NEURONS AP Biology Chapter 48 Objectives Describe the different types of neurons Describe the structure and function of dendrites, axons, a synapse, types of ion channels, and

More information

Synapse. Structure & Function. Neurotransmitter Sequence. Integration. History: 10/4/12 original version

Synapse. Structure & Function. Neurotransmitter Sequence. Integration. History: 10/4/12 original version Synapse History: 10/4/12 original version Structure & Function (This content is covered in Sinjin's presentation, see link in calendar) Neurotransmitters Synaptic cleft Post-synaptic potential Excitation

More information

-51mV 30s 3mV. n=14 n=4 p=0.4. Depolarization (mv) 3

-51mV 30s 3mV. n=14 n=4 p=0.4. Depolarization (mv) 3 Supplementary Figure 1 a optoβ 2 -AR b ChR2-51mV 30s 3mV -50mV 30s 3mV c 4 n=14 n=4 p=0.4 Depolarization (mv) 3 2 1 0 Both optogenetic actuators, optoβ 2 AR and ChR2, were effective in stimulating astrocytes

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1 SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Figure 1 The supralinear events evoked in CA3 pyramidal cells fulfill the criteria for NMDA spikes, exhibiting a threshold, sensitivity to NMDAR blockade, and all-or-none

More information

Why am I working in a Drosophila lab? Dr. Stefan Pulver, Brandeis University

Why am I working in a Drosophila lab? Dr. Stefan Pulver, Brandeis University Why am I working in a Drosophila lab? Dr. Stefan Pulver, Brandeis University !Short life span The advantages of Drosophila as a model organism in neurocience!genetic manipulations are relatively easy!forward

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Recording sites.

Supplementary Figure 1. Recording sites. Supplementary Figure 1 Recording sites. (a, b) Schematic of recording locations for mice used in the variable-reward task (a, n = 5) and the variable-expectation task (b, n = 5). RN, red nucleus. SNc,

More information

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu 9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms. Working memory short-term

More information

The Role of Mitral Cells in State Dependent Olfactory Responses. Trygve Bakken & Gunnar Poplawski

The Role of Mitral Cells in State Dependent Olfactory Responses. Trygve Bakken & Gunnar Poplawski The Role of Mitral Cells in State Dependent Olfactory Responses Trygve akken & Gunnar Poplawski GGN 260 Neurodynamics Winter 2008 bstract Many behavioral studies have shown a reduced responsiveness to

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Diverse anorexigenic signals induce c-fos expression in CEl PKC-δ + neurons

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Diverse anorexigenic signals induce c-fos expression in CEl PKC-δ + neurons Supplementary Figure 1 Diverse anorexigenic signals induce c-fos expression in CEl PKC-δ + neurons a-c. Quantification of CEl c-fos expression in mice intraperitoneal injected with anorexigenic drugs (a),

More information

Supplementary figure 1: LII/III GIN-cells show morphological characteristics of MC

Supplementary figure 1: LII/III GIN-cells show morphological characteristics of MC 1 2 1 3 Supplementary figure 1: LII/III GIN-cells show morphological characteristics of MC 4 5 6 7 (a) Reconstructions of LII/III GIN-cells with somato-dendritic compartments in orange and axonal arborizations

More information

Is action potential threshold lowest in the axon?

Is action potential threshold lowest in the axon? Supplementary information to: Is action potential threshold lowest in the axon? Maarten H. P. Kole & Greg J. Stuart Supplementary Fig. 1 Analysis of action potential (AP) threshold criteria. (a) Example

More information

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 1st Midterm Exam Ready for Pickup By the elevator on the 3 rd Floor of Pacific Hall (waiver) Exam Depot Window at the north entrance to Pacific Hall (no waiver) Mon-Fri, 10:00

More information

Physiology of Tactile Sensation

Physiology of Tactile Sensation Physiology of Tactile Sensation Objectives: 1. Describe the general structural features of tactile sensory receptors how are first order nerve fibers specialized to receive tactile stimuli? 2. Understand

More information

Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain, 3e. Chapter 4: The action potential

Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain, 3e. Chapter 4: The action potential Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain, 3e Chapter 4: The action potential Introduction Action Potential in the Nervous System Conveys information over long distances Action potential Initiated in the axon

More information

What is Anatomy and Physiology?

What is Anatomy and Physiology? Introduction BI 212 BI 213 BI 211 Ecosystems Organs / organ systems Cells Organelles Communities Tissues Molecules Populations Organisms Campbell et al. Figure 1.4 Introduction What is Anatomy and Physiology?

More information

NEURONS COMMUNICATE WITH OTHER CELLS AT SYNAPSES 34.3

NEURONS COMMUNICATE WITH OTHER CELLS AT SYNAPSES 34.3 NEURONS COMMUNICATE WITH OTHER CELLS AT SYNAPSES 34.3 NEURONS COMMUNICATE WITH OTHER CELLS AT SYNAPSES Neurons communicate with other neurons or target cells at synapses. Chemical synapse: a very narrow

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information ATP from synaptic terminals and astrocytes regulates NMDA receptors and synaptic plasticity through PSD- 95 multi- protein complex U.Lalo, O.Palygin, A.Verkhratsky, S.G.N. Grant and Y. Pankratov Supporting

More information

Coding of Sensory Information

Coding of Sensory Information Coding of Sensory Information 22 November, 2016 Touqeer Ahmed PhD Atta-ur-Rahman School of Applied Biosciences National University of Sciences and Technology Sensory Systems Mediate Four Attributes of

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival Threats are much better reinforcers Fear is a prime

More information

Optogenetics. how to use light to manipulate neuronal networks. Alexandra Götz

Optogenetics. how to use light to manipulate neuronal networks. Alexandra Götz Optogenetics how to use light to manipulate neuronal networks Alexandra Götz 1 http://www.stanford.edu/group/dlab/optogenetics/ 2 Outline Background Neuron and ion channels Action potential Light sensitive

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. SybII and Ceb are sorted to distinct vesicle populations in astrocytes. Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn.

Supplementary Figure 1. SybII and Ceb are sorted to distinct vesicle populations in astrocytes. Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn. Supplementary Figure 1 SybII and Ceb are sorted to distinct vesicle populations in astrocytes. (a) Exemplary images for cultured astrocytes co-immunolabeled with SybII and Ceb antibodies. SybII accumulates

More information

1. (1 pt) At the equilibrium potential of an ion, what two things are equal? Electrical potential (voltage) and chemical potential (concentration)

1. (1 pt) At the equilibrium potential of an ion, what two things are equal? Electrical potential (voltage) and chemical potential (concentration) MIDERM REVIEW QUESIONS: IO 3411 (hese are questions from 3 of the previous years midterms) 1. (1 pt) t the equilibrium potential of an ion, what two things are equal? Electrical potential (voltage) and

More information

Brief Communications. Shekib A. Jami, 1 William G. Wright, 1,2 and David L. Glanzman 1,3 1

Brief Communications. Shekib A. Jami, 1 William G. Wright, 1,2 and David L. Glanzman 1,3 1 3064 The Journal of Neuroscience, March 21, 2007 27(12):3064 3068 Brief Communications Differential Classical Conditioning of the Gill-Withdrawal Reflex in Aplysia Recruits Both NMDA Receptor-Dependent

More information

Chapter 11 Introduction to the Nervous System and Nervous Tissue Chapter Outline

Chapter 11 Introduction to the Nervous System and Nervous Tissue Chapter Outline Chapter 11 Introduction to the Nervous System and Nervous Tissue Chapter Outline Module 11.1 Overview of the Nervous System (Figures 11.1-11.3) A. The nervous system controls our perception and experience

More information

HUMAN MOTOR CONTROL. Emmanuel Guigon

HUMAN MOTOR CONTROL. Emmanuel Guigon HUMAN MOTOR CONTROL Emmanuel Guigon Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique Université Pierre et Marie Curie CNRS / UMR 7222 Paris, France emmanuel.guigon@upmc.fr e.guigon.free.fr/teaching.html

More information

Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex

Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex Koji Yashiro, Thorfinn T. Riday, Kathryn H. Condon, Adam C. Roberts, Danilo R. Bernardo, Rohit Prakash, Richard J. Weinberg, Michael

More information

Principles of Anatomy and Physiology

Principles of Anatomy and Physiology Principles of Anatomy and Physiology 14 th Edition CHAPTER 12 Nervous Tissue Introduction The purpose of the chapter is to: 1. Understand how the nervous system helps to keep controlled conditions within

More information

Sensitivity and Adaptation in the Retina

Sensitivity and Adaptation in the Retina Sensitivity and Adaptation in the Retina Visual transduction single photon sensitivity dark current rhodopsin Ca ++ vs cgmp as the messenger amplification Operating range of vision saturation, threshold,

More information

Presynaptic NMDA receptor control of spontaneous and evoked activity By: Sally Si Ying Li Supervisor: Jesper Sjöström

Presynaptic NMDA receptor control of spontaneous and evoked activity By: Sally Si Ying Li Supervisor: Jesper Sjöström Presynaptic NMDA receptor control of spontaneous and evoked activity By: Sally Si Ying Li Supervisor: Jesper Sjöström NMDA receptors are traditionally known to function as post-synaptic coincidence detectors.

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 7: Network models Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis II 6 Single neuron

More information

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus Central Visual Pathways V1/2 NEUR 3001 dvanced Visual Neuroscience The Lateral Geniculate Nucleus () is more than a relay station LP SC Professor Tom Salt UCL Institute of Ophthalmology Retina t.salt@ucl.ac.uk

More information

The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 2000

The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 2000 The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 2000 Press Release NOBELFÖRSAMLINGEN KAROLINSKA INSTITUTET THE NOBEL ASSEMBLY AT THE KAROLINSKA INSTITUTE 9 October 2000 The Nobel Assembly at Karolinska Institutet

More information

PSY 215 Lecture 13 (3/7/11) Learning & Memory Dr. Achtman PSY 215. Lecture 13 Topic: Mechanisms of Learning and Memory Chapter 13, section 13.

PSY 215 Lecture 13 (3/7/11) Learning & Memory Dr. Achtman PSY 215. Lecture 13 Topic: Mechanisms of Learning and Memory Chapter 13, section 13. PSY 215 Lecture 13 Topic: Mechanisms of Learning and Memory Chapter 13, section 13.2 Corrections: No corrections Announcements: Question # 37 was thrown out on the last test because it was confusing the

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

The control of spiking by synaptic input in striatal and pallidal neurons

The control of spiking by synaptic input in striatal and pallidal neurons The control of spiking by synaptic input in striatal and pallidal neurons Dieter Jaeger Department of Biology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322 Key words: Abstract: rat, slice, whole cell, dynamic current

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more strongly when an image was negative than when the same

More information

Chapter 11: Nervous System and Nervous Tissue

Chapter 11: Nervous System and Nervous Tissue Chapter 11: Nervous System and Nervous Tissue I. Functions and divisions of the nervous system A. Sensory input: monitor changes in internal and external environment B. Integrations: make decisions about

More information

Nervous System. 2. Receives information from the environment from CNS to organs and glands. 1. Relays messages, processes info, analyzes data

Nervous System. 2. Receives information from the environment from CNS to organs and glands. 1. Relays messages, processes info, analyzes data Nervous System 1. Relays messages, processes info, analyzes data 2. Receives information from the environment from CNS to organs and glands 3. Transmits impulses from CNS to muscles and glands 4. Transmits

More information

Jana F Liewald, Martin Brauner, Gregory J Stephens, Magali Bouhours, Christian Schultheis, Mei Zhen & Alexander Gottschalk

Jana F Liewald, Martin Brauner, Gregory J Stephens, Magali Bouhours, Christian Schultheis, Mei Zhen & Alexander Gottschalk nature methods Optogenetic analysis of synaptic function Jana F Liewald, Martin Brauner, Gregory J Stephens, Magali Bouhours, Christian Schultheis, Mei Zhen & Alexander Gottschalk Supplementary figures

More information

Light-evoked hyperpolarization and silencing of neurons by conjugated polymers

Light-evoked hyperpolarization and silencing of neurons by conjugated polymers Light-evoked hyperpolarization and silencing of neurons by conjugated polymers Paul Feyen 1,, Elisabetta Colombo 1,2,, Duco Endeman 1, Mattia Nova 1, Lucia Laudato 2, Nicola Martino 2,3, Maria Rosa Antognazza

More information

File name: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Table and Supplementary References

File name: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Table and Supplementary References File name: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Table and Supplementary References File name: Supplementary Data 1 Description: Summary datasheets showing the spatial

More information

Chapter 2: Cellular Mechanisms and Cognition

Chapter 2: Cellular Mechanisms and Cognition Chapter 2: Cellular Mechanisms and Cognition MULTIPLE CHOICE 1. Two principles about neurons were defined by Ramón y Cajal. The principle of connectional specificity states that, whereas the principle

More information

MCB MIDTERM EXAM #1 MONDAY MARCH 3, 2008 ANSWER KEY

MCB MIDTERM EXAM #1 MONDAY MARCH 3, 2008 ANSWER KEY MCB 160 - MIDTERM EXAM #1 MONDAY MARCH 3, 2008 ANSWER KEY Name ID# Instructions: -Only tests written in pen will be regarded -Please submit a written request indicating where and why you deserve more points

More information

Supplemental Information. Memory-Relevant Mushroom Body. Output Synapses Are Cholinergic

Supplemental Information. Memory-Relevant Mushroom Body. Output Synapses Are Cholinergic Neuron, Volume 89 Supplemental Information Memory-Relevant Mushroom Body Output Synapses Are Cholinergic Oliver Barnstedt, David Owald, Johannes Felsenberg, Ruth Brain, John-Paul Moszynski, Clifford B.

More information

Receptors and Neurotransmitters: It Sounds Greek to Me. Agenda. What We Know About Pain 9/7/2012

Receptors and Neurotransmitters: It Sounds Greek to Me. Agenda. What We Know About Pain 9/7/2012 Receptors and Neurotransmitters: It Sounds Greek to Me Cathy Carlson, PhD, RN Northern Illinois University Agenda We will be going through this lecture on basic pain physiology using analogies, mnemonics,

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Basic properties of compound EPSPs at

Supplementary Figure 1. Basic properties of compound EPSPs at Supplementary Figure 1. Basic properties of compound EPSPs at hippocampal CA3 CA3 cell synapses. (a) EPSPs were evoked by extracellular stimulation of the recurrent collaterals and pharmacologically isolated

More information

Active Control of Spike-Timing Dependent Synaptic Plasticity in an Electrosensory System

Active Control of Spike-Timing Dependent Synaptic Plasticity in an Electrosensory System Active Control of Spike-Timing Dependent Synaptic Plasticity in an Electrosensory System Patrick D. Roberts and Curtis C. Bell Neurological Sciences Institute, OHSU 505 N.W. 185 th Avenue, Beaverton, OR

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Drd1a-Cre driven ChR2 expression in the SCN. (a) Low-magnification image of a representative Drd1a-ChR2 coronal brain section (n = 2) showing endogenous tdtomato fluorescence (magenta).

More information

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Christopher J. Rozell, Don. H. Johnson and Raymon M. Glantz Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering Department of

More information

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück The Binding Problem This lecture is based on following articles: Adina L. Roskies: The Binding Problem; Neuron 1999 24: 7 Charles M. Gray: The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration:

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Figure 1. Normal AMPAR-mediated fepsp input-output curve in CA3-Psen cdko mice. Input-output curves, which are plotted initial slopes of the evoked fepsp as function of the amplitude of the

More information

POSITION TITLE: Professor of Biology; Director, Center for Cell and Genome Science

POSITION TITLE: Professor of Biology; Director, Center for Cell and Genome Science NAME: Andres Villu Maricq BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Provide the following information for the Senior/key personnel and other significant contributors. Follow this format for each person. DO NOT EXCEED FIVE PAGES.

More information

Supplementary Materials for

Supplementary Materials for advances.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/3/3/e1600955/dc1 Supplementary Materials for Flexible and stretchable nanowire-coated fibers for optoelectronic probing of spinal cord circuits Chi Lu, Seongjun

More information

Supplementary Figure 1

Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Arcuate ChIEF-tdTomato neurons expressed TH These micrographs show that TH-Cre-ChIEF-tdTomato (magenta), expressed by AAV in a TH-Cre mouse, were immunostained with TH (green) in

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Confirmation that optogenetic inhibition of dopaminergic neurons affects choice

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Confirmation that optogenetic inhibition of dopaminergic neurons affects choice Supplementary Figure 1 Confirmation that optogenetic inhibition of dopaminergic neurons affects choice (a) Sample behavioral trace as in Figure 1d, but with NpHR stimulation trials depicted as green blocks

More information

Guided Reading Activities

Guided Reading Activities Name Period Chapter 28: Nervous Systems Guided Reading Activities Big idea: Nervous system structure and function Answer the following questions as you read modules 28.1 28.2: 1. Your taste receptors for

More information

Modulation of Local Reflexes During Centrally Commanded Movements

Modulation of Local Reflexes During Centrally Commanded Movements Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Biology Theses Department of Biology Spring 4-26-2013 Modulation of Local Reflexes During Centrally Commanded Movements Uzma H. Tahir Georgia

More information

Neuroscience 201A (2016) - Problems in Synaptic Physiology

Neuroscience 201A (2016) - Problems in Synaptic Physiology Question 1: The record below in A shows an EPSC recorded from a cerebellar granule cell following stimulation (at the gap in the record) of a mossy fiber input. These responses are, then, evoked by stimulation.

More information

Synfire chains with conductance-based neurons: internal timing and coordination with timed input

Synfire chains with conductance-based neurons: internal timing and coordination with timed input Neurocomputing 5 (5) 9 5 www.elsevier.com/locate/neucom Synfire chains with conductance-based neurons: internal timing and coordination with timed input Friedrich T. Sommer a,, Thomas Wennekers b a Redwood

More information