NOTE INTERMANUAL TRANSFER IN A SIMPLE MOTOR TASK

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "NOTE INTERMANUAL TRANSFER IN A SIMPLE MOTOR TASK"

Transcription

1 NOTE INTERMANUAL TRANSFER IN A SIMPLE MOTOR TASK Katrin Schulze 1, Eileen Lüders 1 and Lutz Jäncke 2 ( 1 Otto-von-Guericke-University Magdeburg, Department of Experimental and General Psychology, Magdeburg, Germany; 2 University Zürich, Department of Neuropsychology, Zürich, Switzerland) ABSTRACT The present study examines the effects of a four-week training session in a pegboard task on uni- and bimanual performance. Of particular interest was whether practice transfer from the trained to the untrained hand takes place. Twenty-five consistently right-handed subjects were trained to perform the task with the dominant hand, or the subdominant hand, or with both hands. After this training, the learning effects for the trained and untrained hands were analysed. To summarise, we obtained the following findings: (1) After training, movement times were considerably reduced for all hands and for all training conditions (practice effects); (2) practice effects were found for the hand trained and also for the untrained hand; (3) there was not a great difference in the size of the practice effects for the right hand after left hand training or for the left hand after right hand training; (4) task difficulty had no clear influence on the practice effect; (5) and finally, we discovered that bimanual movements not only profit from bimanual training but also from unimanual training and conversely unimanual movements benefit from bimanual training. These findings are discussed in the context of different motor control models and in the light of recent brain imaging findings. INTRODUCTION A wealth of studies have demonstrated that there is information transfer between both cortical hand motor areas. A classical paradigm used to study intermanual information transfer is to practice a specific motor task with one hand, and to examine whether the opposite untrained hand benefits from this training. In general it is found that not only the trained hand but also the untrained hand shows performance improvements in the practiced task. Although these findings have largely been replicated, there is some inconsistency as to whether this information transfer is symmetrical or asymmetrical. For example, several studies have demonstrated that the subdominant hand benefits more from dominant hand training than does the dominant hand from subdominant hand training (Halsband, 1992; Laszlo et al., 1970; Milisen and Riper, 1939; Parlow and Kinsbourne, 1989), while other studies found the opposite effect (Hicks, 1974; Taylor and Heilman, 1980). Some studies examined intermanual training effects in subjects of different ages and found that four-year-olds showed poor transfer from the dominant to the nondominant hand, whereas older subjects showed transfer effects in both directions (Uehara, 1998). Intermanual transfer effects are explained in the context of three different models (Thut et al., 1996): (i) the callosal access model, (ii) the proficiency model, and (iii) the crossactivation model. The callosal access model assumes that motor programs are stored in the dominant (mainly left) hemisphere, irrespective of the hand used for hand skill training. As a consequence, the right hand will have direct access to these motor programs while the left hand will only have indirect access via the corpus callosum (Taylor and Heilman, 1980). According to this model the dominant (usually right) hand will benefit more from subdominant hand training than vice versa. Cortex, (2002) 38,

2 806 Katrin Schulze and Others The two other models are based on those studies which revealed a greater benefit for the subdominant (usually left) hand after dominant hand training. The proficiency model (Laszlo et al., 1970) postulates the formation of unilateral engrams which are stored in hand motor areas contralateral to the trained hand. Because of the general advantage of the left-hemispheric motor area controlling the right hand, improving right hand skill through training will improve the superior left-sided motor control centre, leading to optimal information transfer to the hand motor centres located in the opposite hemisphere. In contrast, training with the subdominant hand will only improve the inferior motor programs. Therefore, the dominant hand motor area cannot benefit from this inferior motor control information. The cross activation model (Parlow and Kinsbourne, 1989) suggests the generation of two motor programs, one located in each hemisphere. However, both programs are thought to operate in a coupled manner. When subjects learn with their dominant hand, the two motor programs are stored independently in the dominant and non dominant motor cortex. Because the dominant motor cortex has access to superior motor programs, learning with the dominant hand will rely on these superior motor programs. Thus, the nondominant motor cortex will receive a copy of this updated motor program which will work independently from the dominant motor cortex when the nondominant hand is required to perform the practiced task. Therefore, learning with the dominant hand will facilitate subsequent performance of the nondominant hand. Conversely, when learning with the subdominant hand, formation of the motor program should rely on the inferior motor program located in the subdominant motor cortex, resulting in inferior performance of the dominant hand. Currently, there is no consensus as to which of these models might explain intermanual transfer, because of conflicting findings in the literature. These conflicting findings may be due to several factors. Among them are heterogenous samples, different cognitive requirements necessary to perform the motor tasks, or different durations of the training sessions. The present study was designed to re-examine the question of whether there is symmetrical or asymmetrical intermanual transfer of information during the course of unimanual motor learning. Because most of the aforementioned studies applied relatively short training sessions which might only have moderate or even transient practice effects, we introduced a long and relatively intensive training session lasting about four weeks. A second aspect of our study concerns the difficulty of the motor task, a factor which has not been controlled for in recent experiments of this type. In our study we applied four different levels which are quantified according to the formula given by Fitt and Seeger (1954). In so doing, we were in the position to study intermanual practice effects in the context of different difficulty levels. We then asked the following questions: (1) Are there practice effects contralateral to the trained hand? (2) Which of the above-mentioned models is supported? (3) Do the practice effects for unimanual movements after bimanual training differ from practice effects after unimanual training? (4) Does task difficulty influence the practice effects? MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects The study was carried out with 25 female neurologically intact students (mean age = 22 years, range years). We have examined only female subjects in order to exclude possible between-gender differences. Handedness Measurements All subjects were strongly right-handed (no subject with a history of familial lefthandedness), as determined by two preference questionnaires and one performance test. One preference test was the Annett-handedness-questionnaire (AHQ) with 12 items used to

3 Intermanual transfer 807 detect consistent right-handed (CRH) subjects. Consistent right-handedness was determined when all six primary tasks were carried out with the right hand, in accordance with Annett (1972, 1996). All subjects were classified as CRH. For the second preference test, a hand preference test based on the Bryden-Steenhuis handedness test (Steenhuis et al., 1990) with eight preference questions was used. All subjects indicated right-hand preference on these items. Finally, the performance test Handdominanztest HDT, originally designed by Steingrüber (1971) for the assessment of children, was used. For the purpose of studying adults we applied appropriate time limits (Jäncke, 1996). This test consists of three dexterity tasks (tracing lines, dotting circles and tapping on squares), each of which has to be performed with maximum speed and precision for 15 secs. Laterality coefficients (R L)/(R+L) were determined for each test and rounded to two decimal places. Negative total scores indicate left-handedness and positive values right-handedness. Subjects with scores < 0.05 were excluded, because a previous study has shown that scores lower than 0.05 are typical of consistent left-handers or mixed-handers (Jancke, 1996). The Motor Task We used a pegboard apparatus consisting of 2 bars (bar length 140mm) each containing eight holes oriented parallel to each other (Figure 1). The centres of the holes were separated by 15 mm. The bars were fixed at a distance of 205 mm from each other. We used four different sets of bars containing holes of different diameter (1.5, 3, 6, or 12 mm). The subjects were required to place pegs of appropriate diameters into the holes as quickly as possible. The dependent variable was the time it took to place the peg successively into all eight holes (timing was not interrupted in the event of a dropped peg). The subjects had to start the movements from a predetermined starting position in front of the apparatus (Figure 1). The movement distance from this starting position to each hole varied from 150 mm to 245 mm. Because we used four different sets of bars with different hole diameters, we had motor tasks with different levels of movement difficulty. We estimated movement difficulty according to the formula given by Fitts and Peterson (1964). That is, movement difficulty Fig. 1 Schematic drawing of the peg-board task used in this study.

4 808 Katrin Schulze and Others was estimated by the width of the holes (W) and the movement distances (D) [log(2) (2*(D/W)]. The obtained average estimates for movement difficulty were 1.17 (12 mm diameter), 1.86 (6 mm diameter), 2.56 (3 mm diameter), and 3.25 (1.5 mm diameter) for the four different holes ranked from small to large diameter. This resulted in considerable differences in movement difficulty between these subtasks. Motor Training Before the procedure started, subjects were randomly assigned to three groups: the first group was required to practice only with the dominant right hand (right hand practice: RHP, n = 9); the second group was instructed to practice only with the subdominant left hand (left hand practice: LHP, n = 9); and the third group practiced with both hands simultaneously (both hand practice: BHP, n = 7). Practicing consisted of eight sessions (each session lasting 15 minutes, 2 sessions a week, resulting in a four-week practicing period with a total of 120 minutes). During each training session the order of the different difficulty levels was arranged randomly. Before the first and after the last practice session, we measured dominant and subdominant hand performance (in the time it took to complete the task in seconds) as well as bimanual performance in this pegboard task. Statistical Analysis The basic design was four factorial with one between-subjects factor (group: RHP, LHP, and BHP) and three within-subjects factors (task difficulty: four levels; hand with three levels: right, left, and both; and training: pre vs. post). The dependent variable was the time it took to complete the motor tasks. Since the data did not entirely fulfil the prerequisites for conventional linear ANOVA analysis (normal distribution, homogeneity of variances, reasonable number of subjects), distribution-free statistical models were applied (Krauth, 1988). The results of these analyses are not interpreted in terms of statistical significance, but are interpreted using p-values as a measure of effect. The p-value is defined as the lowest significance level at which one would still have obtained a significant result for a given data set, a given significance test, and a given test problem. This has the advantage that other researchers can decide for themselves whether the results are significant at the significance level they find acceptable. Since we have to take into consideration the fact that p-values depend on sample size we also calculated effect sizes according to Cohen (1969). Here, the d-value was used, which is the difference between two means divided by the accompanying standard deviation. A d > 0.5 is considered as being moderate, while a d > 0.8 is considered as being large (Cohen, 1969). All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS for Windows version We will only comment on effects associated with a p 0.05 or a d > 0.5 (moderate effect size). In order to handle the different conditions and the possible interactions between them, as well as to avoid an unnecessarily large number of statistical tests, we performed a series of statistical tests which were strongly guided by a priori hypotheses. (1) To answer our first question we first tested for the existence of significant practice effects. The averaged performance (averaged across all difficulty levels) for the pre- and post-training sessions were compared separately for each practice group and for each hand by applying the Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test. Based on published data (Peters, 1981), we predicted that our training would cause a reduction in the time spent for each task, and so all tests were one-tailed tests. (2) In a second step, we calculated the time difference (in secs) between the movement time for the pre- and post-training conditions (this difference measure will be designated the practice effect). This measure was subjected to betweengroups comparisons by applying Kruskal-Wallis tests. In case of a p-value 0.05 for the Kruskal-Wallis analysis, subsequent Mann-Whitney U-tests were performed. (3) In a third step, the influence of task difficulty on the practice effects was examined. For this the practice effect was subjected to Friedman s tests separately for each training group and hand used. In case of a p-value 0.05 for these analyses, subsequent Wilcoxon-tests were applied. (4) General influences of task difficulty and hands used were described only at a general level and not separately for each training group or task difficulty level, because the

5 Intermanual transfer 809 influence of task difficulty and hand used on movement time has already been described in numerous papers (Annett, 1992a; Doyen and Carlier, 2002; Rigal, 1992). RESULTS As expected, task difficulty had a strong influence on movement time in the predicted direction with the shortest movement times for the easiest task and the longest times for the most difficult task. The movement times were ranked from easiest to the most difficult task (14.9 secs, 18.8 secs, 21.6 secs, 28.6 secs; Friedman s test comparing the four difficulty levels, p < 0.001; effect sizes obtained for the comparisons between the mean movement times under each task difficulty ranged between 1.4 to 6.1 altogether indicating strong effects). In addition, movement times were fastest for the dominant right hand (mean movement times for the right and left hand: 18.2 secs, 20.1 secs; mean effect size for the difference between right and left hand = 2.4, p < 0.001) and slowest for bimanual movements (mean movement time for bimanual movement: 29.4 secs, mean effect size for the difference between bimanual and unimanual movements = 5.5, p < 0.001). Furthermore, movement times were similar across groups before the training (Kruskal-Wallis analysis separately for right hand, left hand, and bimanual movements revealed no significant difference, all p-values 0.72). 1. Test for General Practice Effects The mean pre- and post-training scores for each practice group and for each hand (calculated across all task difficulty conditions, see Table I) were subjected to a series of Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank tests. These tests revealed very low p-values with eight practice effects associated with a p-value One test (for right hand performance of the left-hand practice group) revealed a p-value of However, this practice effect is associated with an effect size of d = 0.88 indicating a strong effect. As can be seen in Table 1, the remaining effect sizes are larger than 1.45, indicating very strong effects. Thus, we can conclude that training results in strong practice effects even for the untrained hand (left hand for the right hand training group and the right hand for the left hand training group). 2. Test for Between- and Within-Group Differences for the Practice Effects The between-group analysis for the practice effects revealed a clear between-group difference only for bimanual movements (p = 0.006). For right and left hand movements there were only weak or moderate between-group differences (see Table 1). Bimanual performance improved more after bimanual training than after unimanual training (effect size for bimanual vs. the averaged left and right hand training d = 2.01, p = 0.002). The moderate differences for right and left hand performance were due to the slightly larger practice effects for the trained hand compared to the untrained hand (right hand: RHP vs. LHP: d = 0.47, p = 0.04; left hand: RHP vs. LHP: d = 0.7, p = 0.03). The difference between the practice effects of the untrained hands was small and in the range of a small effect (d = 0.27, p = 0.13, see also Figure 2). 3. Test for the Influence of Task Difficulty The practice effects obtained for the different difficulty levels, training groups, and hands used are depicted in Table 2. As one can see in this table, the practice effects are mostly strong and exceed the effect size criterion of d = 0.8, therefore indicating strong effects. The only exception was found for the practice effects of the left hand for the right hand training group. In the easiest task ( 12 mm), movement times basically did not change after training (d = 0.04), while for the second most difficult task ( 3 mm) there was a moderate effect size (d = 0.69) for the practice effect. By subjecting these practice effects (differences between pre- and post-training scores) to a series of Friedman s tests separately for each training group

6 810 Katrin Schulze and Others TABLE I Mean Movement Times and Standard Deviations (in brackets) before (pre) and after (post) Training, as well as pre-post Differences, Effect Sizes (d), and p-values Broken down for the Three Training Groups (RHP: Right Hand Practice, LHP: Left Hand Practice, BHP: Bimanual Practice) pre post pre-post d P Right hand RHP (1.50) (1.45) 2.88 (0.6) LHP (1.88) (1.03) 2.38 (1.4) BHP (2.36) (1.65) 2.90 (1.2) K-W result p = 0.72 p = 0.15 p = 0.13 Left hand RHP (2.78) (1.46) 1.86 (2.7) LHP (1.86) (1.03) 3.42 (1.7) BHP (2.46) (1.80) 3.90 (2.7) K-W result p = 0.72 p = p = 0.08 Bimanual RHP (1.42) (1.52) 2.74 (1.6) LHP (3.11) (1.41) 3.29 (2.3) BHP (3.18) ( (1.5) K-W result p = 0.78 p = p = Fig. 2 Mean practice effects and standard errors of the mean (as vertical bars) for the untrained hands obtained in the present study (LH-RHP: left hand after right hand practice; RH- LHP: right hand after left hand practice). In addition, predicted practice effects according to the callosal access model and the other two models (see Introduction) are also presented. and hand used (nine tests, see also Table II), it was revealed that the practice effects were generally not influenced by task difficulty. The only exception was found for the bimanual training group performing bimanual movements (p = 0.04). In terms of effect sizes there were strong differences between the easiest task and the other difficulty levels (d-values ranging between 1.4 and 1.53, P-values from p = 0.05 to p = 0.01). DISCUSSION The main purpose of this study was to examine whether intensive and long lasting training (which was longer than in any previously published study of this kind) of the

7 Intermanual transfer 811 TABLE II Mean Movement Times and Standard Deviations (in brackets) before (pre) and after (post) Training, as well as pre-post Differences, and Effect Sizes (d) Broken down for the Three Training Groups [(a) RHP: Right Hand Practice, (b) LHP: Left hand Practice, (c) BHP: Bimanual Practice], the Hand Used (Right Hand, Left Hand, and Bimanual: RH, LH, BH), and Movement Difficulty Indexed as the Diameter of the Holes (a) Difficulty pre post pre-post d RHP RH 12 mm 13.4 (1.9) 11.1 (1.6) 2.3 (1.6) mm 17.5 (2.2) 14.8 (1.8) 2.7 (1.6) mm 18.4 (2.1) 15.3 (1.3) 3.1 (1.9) mm 23.6 (2.1) 20.2 (2.6) 3.4 (3.1) 1.43 p-values* p = 0.76 LH 12 mm 14.6 (1.8) 14.6 (0.9) 0.06 (1.1) mm 18.3 (1.8) 16.3 (1.7) 2.0 (2.1) mm 20.8 (2.6) 19.2 (1.9) 1.6 (2.5) mm 13.4 (1.9) 11.1 (1.5) 2.3 (1.6) 1.33 p-values p = 0.16 BH 12 mm 20.9 (1.6) 18.9 (1.8) 1.9 (1.9) mm 26.1 (1.4) 23.0 (1.7) 3.1 (2.1) mm 30.7 (2.3) 27.8 (2.3) 2.8 (2.8) mm 40.4 (2.2) 37.4 (4.0) 3.1 (3.9) 0.99 p-values p = 0.60 (b) Difficulty pre post pre-post d LHP RH 12 mm 13.4 (2.0) 12.1 (0.9) 1.4 (2.1) mm 17.3 (2.2) 15.6 (1.5) 1.8 (2.9) mm 19.2 (2.5) 16.8 (0.9) 2.4 (2.6) mm 24.9 (2.9) 21.0 (2.4) 3.9 (2.6) 1.47 p-values p = 0.18 LH 12 mm 14.5 (1.3) 12.1 (1.4) 2.3 (2.1) mm 18.0 (1.9) 14.6 (1.7) 3.5 (1.7) mm 19.9 (2.1) (.7) 3.9 (1.9) mm 13.4 (2.0) 12.1 (0.9) 1.4 (2.1) 0.93 p-values p = 0.35 BH 12 mm 19.8 (1.9) 17.1 (1.4) 2.7 (1.7) mm 26.6 (2.9) 22.3 (1.9) 4.3 (3.0) mm 30.8 (3.3) 28.1 (1.9) 2.7 (2.1) mm 39.5 (4.7) 36.1 (2.3) 3.4 (3.9) 0.98 p-values p = 0.51 (c) Difficulty pre post pre-post d BHP RH 12 mm 13.6 (2.3) 11.3 (1.3) 2.3 (1.6) mm 16.2 (2.1) 13.9 (1.5) 2.3 (1.2) mm 18.2 (2.4) 15.1 (1.5) 3.1 (2.3) mm 23.1 (3.2) 19.2 (3.0) 3.9 (2.5) 1.25 p-values p = 0.54 LH 12 mm 14.7 (1.8) 12.6 (1.6) 2.1 (1.1) mm 18.2 (2.7) 14.8 (1.7) 3.6 (2.2) mm 20.4 (2.2) 15.9 (3.8) 4.5 (5.1) mm 13.6 (2.3) 11.3 (1.3) 2.3 (1.6) 1.28 p-values p = 0.48 BH 12 mm 19.3 (1.8) 14.9 (1.6) 4.4 (1.1) mm 25.3 (4.3) 18.9 (2.2) 6.5 (2.9) mm 30.9 (4.2) 23.6 (1.6) 7.4 (2.9) mm 42.2 (5.4) 34.2 (2.8) 8.0 (3.6) 1.95 p-values p = 0.04 *p-values are from Friedman-ANOVAs testing for a general difference between the different movement difficulty levels.

8 812 Katrin Schulze and Others Fig. 3 Mean practice effects and standard errors of the mean (as vertical bars) for the untrained hands obtained in the present study broken down for the different difficulty levels. (no difference between the size of the practice effects for LH-RHP p = 0.15 and RH-LHP p = 0.17). pegboard task results in clear practice effects (reduced movement time to perform the task). Apart from wanting to study practice effects, we were also interested in studying the possible benefits gained by the untrained hands as a result of training the opposite hands in the context of different task difficulty levels. A further question was whether bimanual movements might benefit from unimanual training and vice versa. To summarise, we obtained the following findings: (1) After training, movement times were considerably reduced for all hands and for all training conditions, i.e. there were strong practice effects. (2) Practice effects were found for the hand trained and also for the untrained hand. Therefore, some kind of interhemispheric information transfer might have occurred, which may have positively influenced the untrained hand. (3) Interestingly, there was no clear sign of an asymmetric information transfer as predicted by the models discussed in the introduction. However, there was a weak trend for the right hand to benefit a little bit more from left hand training than the left hand from right hand training (effect size d = 0.27, p = 0.13), thus tentatively supporting the callosal access model. (4) Task difficulty had no clear influence on the practice effect. (5) Finally, we found that bimanual movements not only profit from bimanual training but also from unimanual training, and conversely unimanual movements benefit from bimanual training. In the following section we will firstly discuss the motor paradigm before we go on with the discussion of the behavioural findings and how they relate to published findings in the neuropsychological literature. The Motor Task In this study we used a variant of the pegboard task which is widely used in neuropsychology for the examination of complex uni- and bimanual motor functions (Annett, 1985; Doyen and Carlier, 2002). Although this task had originally been designed for the examination of motor functions, several authors emphasize that the obtained measures correlate with various normal and deviant cognitive functions and with an anatomical marker related to language processing (Annett, 1992b, 1994, 1996; Bouquet et al., 1999; Bowler et al., 2001; Rosselli et al., 2001). Although this task is associated with a variety of functions, it is definitely a specific kind of sensorimotor task in which particular visual stimuli require a specific response. Previous neuroimaging studies have shown that motor tasks triggered or guided by visual cues evoked strong cortical activations bilaterally

9 Intermanual transfer 813 in the ventral and dorsal premotor cortex (vpmc and dpmc), in the left sensorimotor cortex (M1 and S1), in the left supplementary motor cortex (SMA), bilaterally in the inferior parietal lobe (IPL), in the right inferior cerebellum, and in the left ventro-lateral thalamus (Iacoboni et al., 1998; Jäncke et al., 2000a; Lutz et al., 2000; Penhune et al., 1998). We will not interpret or explain any of these activations because that has already been done in the above-mentioned papers. We would simply like to draw attention to the fact that the pegboard task is most likely accompanied by distributed activations in a bilateral frontoparietal network, the cerebellum, the basal ganglia, and the thalamus. This distributed activation requires time-consuming intrahemispheric and interhemispheric transfer of information and also time-consuming processing within each node of this network. The Practice Effect There were strong practice effects for each hand and for each training condition. But most interestingly for the present paper is the fact that there were practice effects for the untrained hands. Therefore, one might assume that some kind of interhemispheric transfer had occurred. There was however no clear sign of asymmetric information transfer. Both untrained hands (the dominant and the subdominant hand) benefited from training of the opposite hands. The reason for the lack of asymmetric information transfer might be the nature of the task used and the neural structures involved. As mentioned, the pegboard task is a typical visuo-motor task most likely accompanied by neural activations bilaterally in the vpmc and dpmc. The most critical part of this task is not the movement itself, which is controlled by the primary motor cortex, but rather the adjustment of movement parameters (e.g., the diameter of the hole) by means of visual information. According to the abovementioned brain imaging data, we know that visual information associated with motor functions is transferred from the visual cortex bilaterally via the dorsal stream to the dpmc. Thus, even when the pegboard task is performed unimanually with the right hand (causing strong activations within the left dpmc and the left M1), the right dpmc and possibly the adjacent right M1 is also activated. It may be the case that there is no interhemispheric transfer, but rather that bilateral adjustment of visuo-motor associations occurs during unimanual movements. Learning to perform the pegboard task with the right hand will therefore automatically lead to an improvement of visuo-motor associations in both hemispheres, in which case performing the motor task with the untrained hand will rely on improved visuo-motor associations. This line of argument does not exclude the possibility that the improvement of visuo-motor association within the PMC is the only mechanism stimulated by our training. There may be further processes such as the training of motor programs located in M1 or PMC, or the interhemispheric exchange of information between both PMCs which are also stimulated. However, the present study design (and most of the previous studies of this kind) does not allow us to disentangle the contribution of the different processes to motor learning. Although task difficulty had the predicted influence on movement time with faster movements for easy tasks and slower movements for difficult tasks, we could not identify a consistent and clear influence of task difficulty on the practice effects. As can be seen in Table 2, there is a large variability in the practice effects across the different task difficulty levels, which are indexed as standard deviations. The reasons for this variability are difficult to explain and might include different skill levels or different strategies of the subjects. A further interesting finding from our study was that bimanual training resulted in strong practice effects for the unimanual tasks. Conversely, unimanual training resulted in strong practice effects for the bimanual tasks. The practice effect of bimanual training on unimanual training was similar to the effect obtained when the subjects had been practising unimanually. However, the practice effect of unimanual training on bimanual movements was smaller than the practice effect of bimanual training on bimanual movements. These findings might point to the fact that several mechanisms operative during unimanual training are also active during bimanual training. For example, during bimanual training there is simultaneous training of the visuo-motor associations and motor programs located on the dominant left (for right hand movements) and non-dominant right hemisphere (for left hand movements). Bimanual training however resulted in very strong practice effects for the

10 814 Katrin Schulze and Others bimanual task, which were about twice as large as the practice effects for the unimanual tasks were. This finding may indicate that bimanual movements are controlled in a different way than unimanual movements. Recent neuroimaging studies have shown that bimanual movements require more processing demands bilaterally in the primary motor areas but also in the lateral and mesial premotor areas, suggesting that additional motor control processes are involved (Jäncke et al., 1998, 2000a, 2000b; Stephan et al., 1999). Thus, there are more degrees of freedom to control in bimanual movements. Intensive bimanual practice might help to adjust and synchronize these different motor control processes. In contrast, relatively easy unimanual movements, as were used in this study, are controlled by relatively small neural assemblies within M1 and the lateral premotor areas, therefore resulting in fewer degrees of freedom than in bimanual movements. In addition, unimanual movements are more often performed in everyday-life than synchronous bimanual movements like those used in our study, suggesting that unimanual performance has already reached a relatively high skill level which can only be furthered a small amount. Nevertheless, we are facing conflicting results in this research area. While our study does not support any of the models already mentioned, other studies report data which support the callosal access, the proficiency, or the cross-activation models. These different findings are difficult to explain because the studies differ with respect to the motor tasks used, sample size, or heterogeneity of the studied sample, and with respect to various aspects such as age, sex, and motor proficiency. A further important difference between these studies and our study is the duration and intensity of the applied motor training. In our study we used a relatively long training period of four weeks, which is longer than any other training period used in previous experiments of the same type. It is known from previous studies that variability of motor performance decreases with increasing duration and intensity of movement training. Therefore, we might have measured stable motor performance dependent upon cortical reorganization of the hand motor areas while previous studies measured increased motor proficiency primarily due to attentional effects. Acknowledgements. We thank Dr. Alan Beaton and an anonymous reviewer for most helpful suggestions. REFERENCES ANNETT M. The distribution of manual asymmetry. British Journal of Psychology, 63: , ANNETT M. Left, Right, Hand and Brain: The Right Shift Theory. London: Erlbaum, ANNETT M. Five tests of hand skill. Cortex, 28: , 1992a. ANNETT M. Parallels between asymmetries of planum temporale and of hand skill. Neuropsychologia, 30: , 1992b. ANNETT M. Handedness as a continuous variable with dextral shift: Sex, generation, and family handedness in subgroups of left- and right- handers. Behavioral and Genetics, 24: 51-63, ANNETT M. The right shift theory of a genetic balanced polymorphism for cerebral dominance and cognitive processing. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 14: , BOUQUET CA, GARDETTE B, GORTAN C and ABRAINI JH. Psychomotor skills learning under chronic hypoxia. Neuroreport, 10: , BOWLER RM, LEZAK M, BOOTY A, HARTNEY C, MERGLER D, LEVIN J and ZISMAN F. Neuropsychological dysfunction, mood disturbance, and emotional status of munitions workers. Applied Neuropsychology, 8: 74-90, COHEN J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Academic Press, DOYEN AL and CARLIER M. Measuring handedness: A validation study of Bishop s reaching card test. Laterality, 7: , FITTS PM and PETERSON JR. Information capacity of discrete motor responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, FITTS PM and SEEGER CM. SR compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46: , HALSBAND U. Left hemisphere preponderance in trajectorial learning. Neuroreport, 3: , HICKS RE. Asymmetry of bilateral transfer. American Journal of Psychology, 87: , IACOBONI M, WOODS RP and MAZZIOTTA JC. Bimodal (auditory and visual) left frontoparietal circuitry for sensorimotor integration and sensorimotor learning. Brain, 121: , JÄNCKE L. The hand performance test with a modified time limit instruction enables the examination of hand performance asymmetries in adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82: , 1996.

11 Intermanual transfer 815 JÄNCKE L, LOOSE R, LUTZ K, SPECHT K and SHAH NJ. Cortical activations during paced finger-tapping applying visual and auditory pacing stimuli. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 10: 51-66, JÄNCKE L, PETERS M, HIMMELBACH M, NOSSELT T, SHAH J and STEINMETZ H. fmri study of bimanual coordination. Neuropsychologia, 38: , 2000a. JÄNCKE L, PETERS M, SCHLAUG G, POSSE S, STEINMETZ H and LLER G. Differential magnetic resonance signal change in human sensorimotor cortex to finger movements of different rate of the dominant and subdominant hand. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 6: , JÄNCKE L, SHAH NJ and PETERS M. Cortical activations in primary and secondary motor areas for complex bimanual movements in professional pianists. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research., 10: , 2000b. KRAUTH J. Distribution-free Statistics. An Application-oriented Approach. Amsterdam, New York, Oxford: Elsevier, LASZLO JL, BAGULEY RA and BAIRSTOW PJ. Bilateral transfer in tapping skill in the absence of peripheral information. Journal of Motor Behavior, 2: 271, LUTZ K, SPECHT K, SHAH NJ and JÄNCKE L. Tapping movements according to regular and irregular visual timing signals investigated with fmri. Neuroreport, 11: , MILISEN R and RIPER CV. Differential transfer of training in a rotary activity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24: , PARLOW SE and KINSBOURNE M. Asymmetrical transfer of training between hands: Implications for interhemispheric communication in normal brain. Brain and Cognition, 11: , PENHUNE VB, ZATTORE RJ and EVANS AC. Cerebellar contributions to motor timing: a PET study of auditory and visual rhythm reproduction. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences, 10: , PETERS M. Handedness: effects of prolonged practise on between hand performance differences. Neuropsychologia, 19: , RIGAL RA. Which handedness: Preference or performance? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75: , ROSSELLI M, ARDILA A, BATEMAN JR and GUZMAN M. Neuropsychological test scores, academic performance, and developmental disorders in Spanish-speaking children. Developments in Neuropsychology, 20: , STEENHUIS RE, BRYDEN MP, SCHWARTZ M and LAWSON S. Reliability of hand preference items and factors. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12: , STEINGRÜBER HJ. Zur Messung der Händigkeit. Zur Messung der Händigkeit. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 18: , STEPHAN KM, BINKOFSKI F, HALSBAND U, DOHLE C, WUNDERLICH G, SCHNITZLER A, TASS P, POSSE S, HERZOG H, STURM V, ZILLES K, SEITZ RJ and FREUND HJ. The role of ventral medial wall motor areas in bimanual co-ordination. A combined lesion and activation study. Brain, 122: , TAYLOR HG and HEILMAN KM. Left-hemisphere motor dominance in righthanders. Cortex, 16: , THUT G, COOK ND, REGARD M, LEENDERS KL, HALSBAND U and LANDIS T. Intermanual transfer of proximal and distal motor engrams in humans. Experimental Brain Research, 108: , UEHARA I. No transfer of visuomotor learning of button-pressing from right to left hands in right-handed four-year-olds. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87: , Lutz Jäncke, Department of Neuropsychology, University Zürich, Zürichbergstr. 43, 8044 Zürich, Switzerland. jaencke@web.de (Received 16 November 2001; reviewed 20 February 2002; revised 21 March 2002; accepted 17 April 2002; Action Editor: Alan Beaton)

Timing and Force Components in Bilateral Transfer of Learning

Timing and Force Components in Bilateral Transfer of Learning Brain and Cognition 44, 455 469 (2000) doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1205, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Timing and Force Components in Bilateral Transfer of Learning Luis Augusto Teixeira

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Dominant Limb Motor Impersistence Associated with Anterior Callosal Disconnection

Dominant Limb Motor Impersistence Associated with Anterior Callosal Disconnection Dominant Limb Motor Impersistence Associated with Anterior Callosal Disconnection S. W. Seo, MD, 1 K. Jung, MS, 2 H. You, PhD 2, E. J. Kim, MD, PhD 1, B. H. Lee, MA, 1 D. L. Na, MD 1 1 Department of Neurology,

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg The concept that different parts of the brain did different things started with Spurzheim and Gall, whose phrenology became quite

More information

Asymmetrical intermanual transfer of learning in a sensorimotor task

Asymmetrical intermanual transfer of learning in a sensorimotor task Exp Brain Res (2010) 202:927 934 DOI 10.1007/s00221-010-2184-8 RESEARCH NOTE Asymmetrical intermanual transfer of learning in a sensorimotor task Waldemar Kirsch Joachim HoVmann Received: 15 September

More information

Brain, language, and handedness: a family affair

Brain, language, and handedness: a family affair Brain, language, and handedness: a family affair Bernard Mazoyer 1,2,3, Gregory Simon 1,2, Fabrice Crivello 1, Laure Zago, Laurent Petit, Emmanuel Mellet, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer 1* 1 CI-NAPS, CNRS, CEA,

More information

Peripheral facial paralysis (right side). The patient is asked to close her eyes and to retract their mouth (From Heimer) Hemiplegia of the left side. Note the characteristic position of the arm with

More information

Sensorimotor Functioning. Sensory and Motor Systems. Functional Anatomy of Brain- Behavioral Relationships

Sensorimotor Functioning. Sensory and Motor Systems. Functional Anatomy of Brain- Behavioral Relationships Sensorimotor Functioning Sensory and Motor Systems Understanding brain-behavior relationships requires knowledge of sensory and motor systems. Sensory System = Input Neural Processing Motor System = Output

More information

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Sensory to Motor Transformation Sensory information (visual,

More information

Neurophysiology of systems

Neurophysiology of systems Neurophysiology of systems Motor cortex (voluntary movements) Dana Cohen, Room 410, tel: 7138 danacoh@gmail.com Voluntary movements vs. reflexes Same stimulus yields a different movement depending on context

More information

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN Vs. 2 Background 3 There are different types of research methods to study behaviour: Descriptive: observations,

More information

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Author's response to reviews Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Authors: Julia Miro (juliamirollado@gmail.com) Pablo Ripollès (pablo.ripolles.vidal@gmail.com)

More information

The origins of localization

The origins of localization Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. The origins of localization The concept that different parts of the brain did different

More information

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Overview Explain the significance of the turn to the brain in cognitive science Explain Mishkin and Ungerleider s hypothesis that there are two distinct visual systems Outline

More information

Intermanual differences on neuropsychological motor tasks in a Japanese university student sample 1

Intermanual differences on neuropsychological motor tasks in a Japanese university student sample 1 bs_bs_banner Japanese Psychological Research 2014, Volume 56, No. 2, 103 113 doi: 10.1111/jpr.12039 Intermanual differences on neuropsychological motor tasks in a Japanese university student sample 1 HIKARI

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D.

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. The origins of localization The concept that different parts of the brain did different

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 284 (2015) 167 178 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report How coordinate and categorical

More information

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. Cortical Organization Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. 2. Secondary cortex: located immediately adjacent to primary cortical areas,

More information

Hemispheric interaction in simple reaction time as a function of handedness

Hemispheric interaction in simple reaction time as a function of handedness Hemispheric interaction in simple reaction time as a function of handedness Emily Howe 2009 Project Advisor: Matt Roser School of Biological Sciences, University of Plymouth, Drake Circus, Plymouth, PL4

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Key questions about attention

Key questions about attention Key questions about attention How does attention affect behavioral performance? Can attention affect the appearance of things? How does spatial and feature-based attention affect neuronal responses in

More information

Measuring sensorimotor synchronization abilities

Measuring sensorimotor synchronization abilities TIMELY School on «Timing and Time Perception: Procedures, Measures, & Applications» Corfu, February 4-8, 2013 Measuring sensorimotor synchronization abilities Simone Dalla Bella EuroMov, M2H Laboratory,

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Based on: Chapter 18 and 19, Breedlove, Watson, Rosenzweig, 6e/7e. Cerebral Cortex Brain s most complex area with billions of neurons and

More information

Which Factors Affect Midline Crossing in Adult: Combined Effect of Task Complexity and Object Location

Which Factors Affect Midline Crossing in Adult: Combined Effect of Task Complexity and Object Location ISSN 1750-9823 (print) International Journal of Sports Science and Engineering Vol. 04 (2010) No. 04, pp. 251-256 Which Factors Affect Midline Crossing in Adult: Combined Effect of Task Complexity and

More information

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical Short Communication TheScientificWorldJOURNAL (2006) 6, 1805 1809 ISSN 1537-744X; DOI 10.1100/tsw.2006.291 Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical V. Frak 1,2, *, D. Bourbonnais 2, I. Croteau

More information

TRANSFER EFFECTS OF EYE-HAND CO-ORDINATION SKILLS FROM THE RIGHT TO THE LEFT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES IN SOUTH AFRICAN SCHOOLBOY RUGBY PLAYERS

TRANSFER EFFECTS OF EYE-HAND CO-ORDINATION SKILLS FROM THE RIGHT TO THE LEFT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES IN SOUTH AFRICAN SCHOOLBOY RUGBY PLAYERS African Journal for Physical, Health Education, Recreation and Dance (AJPHERD) Vol. 12, No. 1 (March) 2006, pp. 41-49. TRANSFER EFFECTS OF EYE-HAND CO-ORDINATION SKILLS FROM THE RIGHT TO THE LEFT CEREBRAL

More information

Online Journal Club-Article Review

Online Journal Club-Article Review Online Journal Club-Article Review Article Citation Study Objective/Purpose (hypothesis) Brief Background (why issue is important; summary of previous literature) Study Design (type of trial, randomization,

More information

On the nature of Rhythm, Time & Memory. Sundeep Teki Auditory Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London

On the nature of Rhythm, Time & Memory. Sundeep Teki Auditory Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London On the nature of Rhythm, Time & Memory Sundeep Teki Auditory Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London Timing substrates Timing mechanisms Rhythm and Timing Unified timing

More information

The Motor Systems. What s the motor system? Plan

The Motor Systems. What s the motor system? Plan The Motor Systems What s the motor system? Parts of CNS and PNS specialized for control of limb, trunk, and eye movements Also holds us together From simple reflexes (knee jerk) to voluntary movements

More information

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles?

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles? Carnegie Mellon University Research Showcase @ CMU Dietrich College Honors Theses Dietrich College of Humanities and Social Sciences 2006 Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize

More information

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Online supplementary information for: Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Joel L. Voss, Brian D. Gonsalves, Kara D. Federmeier,

More information

Higher Cortical Function

Higher Cortical Function Emilie O Neill, class of 2016 Higher Cortical Function Objectives Describe the association cortical areas processing sensory, motor, executive, language, and emotion/memory information (know general location

More information

Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control

Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control These chapters come from Magill s (2010), Motor Learning and Control: Concepts and Application(9 th ) edition textbook. Additional materials regarding these key concepts

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

Optimization of Computer presented Information for left-handed Observer

Optimization of Computer presented Information for left-handed Observer PsychNology Journal, 2003 Volume 1, Number 2, 151-163 Optimization of Computer presented Information for left-handed Observer Cecília Sikné Lányi Laboratory of Colour and Multimedia, Department of Image

More information

Supplementary Material for

Supplementary Material for Supplementary Material for Selective neuronal lapses precede human cognitive lapses following sleep deprivation Supplementary Table 1. Data acquisition details Session Patient Brain regions monitored Time

More information

Title: Quality of life in childhood epilepsy with lateralized focus

Title: Quality of life in childhood epilepsy with lateralized focus Author's response to reviews Title: Quality of life in childhood epilepsy with lateralized focus Authors: Krystyna A. Mathiak (krystyna.mathiak@psych.uw.edu.pl) Malgorzata Luba (malgosia.luba@gmail.com)

More information

Hemispheric Lateralization of Event-Related Brain Potentials in Different Processing Phases during Unimanual Finger Movements

Hemispheric Lateralization of Event-Related Brain Potentials in Different Processing Phases during Unimanual Finger Movements Sensors 2008, 8, 2900-2912 sensors ISSN 1424-8220 2008 by MDPI www.mdpi.org/sensors Full Research Paper Hemispheric Lateralization of Event-Related Brain Potentials in Different Processing Phases during

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Lingnau et al. 10.1073/pnas.0902262106 Fig. S1. Material presented during motor act observation (A) and execution (B). Each row shows one of the 8 different motor acts. Columns in

More information

Anatomic, functional and cognitive asymmetries in monozygotic twins with discordant handedness

Anatomic, functional and cognitive asymmetries in monozygotic twins with discordant handedness Eastern Journal of Medicine 19 (2014) 185-190 Original Article Anatomic, functional and cognitive asymmetries in monozygotic twins with discordant handedness Ozlem Ergul Erkec *, Yalçın Yetkin Department

More information

Interhemispheric Stroop-Like Interference in Number Comparison: Evidence for Strong Interhemispheric Integration of Semantic Number Information

Interhemispheric Stroop-Like Interference in Number Comparison: Evidence for Strong Interhemispheric Integration of Semantic Number Information Neuropsychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 16, No. 2, 217 229 0894-4105/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0894-4105.16.2.217 Interhemispheric Stroop-Like Interference

More information

Learning Objectives.

Learning Objectives. Emilie O Neill, class of 2016 Learning Objectives 1. Describe the types of deficits that occur with lesions in association areas including: prosopagnosia, neglect, aphasias, agnosia, apraxia 2. Discuss

More information

Layered organization of cortex: Paleocortex 3 layers hippocampal formation / ventral & medial cortex closest to brainstem

Layered organization of cortex: Paleocortex 3 layers hippocampal formation / ventral & medial cortex closest to brainstem Layered organization of cortex: Paleocortex 3 layers hippocampal formation / ventral & medial cortex closest to brainstem Archicortex 3-4 layers hippocampal formation / amygdala Neocortex 6 layers more

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

Speeded right-to-left information transfer: the result of speeded transmission in right-hemisphere axons?

Speeded right-to-left information transfer: the result of speeded transmission in right-hemisphere axons? Neuroscience Letters 380 (2005) 88 92 Speeded right-to-left information transfer: the result of speeded transmission in right-hemisphere axons? Kylie J. Barnett, Michael C. Corballis Research Centre for

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy

Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy Eliot Hazeltine 1, Andrea Weinstein 2, and Richard B. Ivry 2 Abstract & Two studies [Ivry, R. B., Franz, E. A., Kingstone, A., & Johnston, J. C. The psychological

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Predictive Model for Group Selection Performance on Touch Devices

Predictive Model for Group Selection Performance on Touch Devices Predictive Model for Group Selection Performance on Touch Devices Per Bjerre 1, Allan Christensen 1, Andreas K. Pedersen 1, Simon A. Pedersen 1, Wolfgang Stuerzlinger 2, and Rasmus Stenholt 1 1 Aalborg

More information

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above.

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above. Section: Chapter 14: Multiple Choice 1. Alex the parrot: pp.529-530 shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all

More information

STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN

STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN Statistics 1 STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN These are subjects that are frequently confused. Both subjects often evoke student anxiety and avoidance. To further complicate matters, both areas appear have

More information

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex I: To list the functions of different cortical laminae II: To describe the four motor areas of the cerebral cortex. III: To discuss the functions and dysfunctions of

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Exam 1 PSYC Fall 1998

Exam 1 PSYC Fall 1998 Exam 1 PSYC 2022 Fall 1998 (2 points) Briefly describe the difference between a dualistic and a materialistic explanation of brain-mind relationships. (1 point) True or False. George Berkely was a monist.

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuropsychologia. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 January 31.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuropsychologia. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 January 31. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Neuropsychologia. 2008 January 31; 46(2): 659 664. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.008. The Role of Task History in Simple

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Classification of Inhibitory Function

Classification of Inhibitory Function Classification of Inhibitory Function Noriaki Tsuchida Behavioral regulation functions can intentionally initiate or terminate behavior (Luria, ). This study investigated the inhibitory function, a behavioral

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Moriguchi and Hiraki 10.1073/pnas.0809747106 SI Text Differences in Brain Activation Between Preswitch and Postswitch Phases. The paired t test was used to compare the brain activation

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. Authors Year Patients Male gender (%) Mean age (range) Adults/ Children

More information

The Dutch Handedness Questionnaire

The Dutch Handedness Questionnaire The Dutch Handedness Questionnaire Jan W. Van Strien FSW, Department of Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam December 2002 Handedness questionnaires typically include an item on preference for writing.

More information

Hemispheric Specialization (lateralization) Each lobe of the brain has specialized functions (Have to be careful with this one.)

Hemispheric Specialization (lateralization) Each lobe of the brain has specialized functions (Have to be careful with this one.) Cerebral Cortex Principles contralaterality the right half of your brain controls the left half of your body and vice versa. (contralateral control.) Localization of function Specific mental processes

More information

LAB 1: MOTOR LEARNING & DEVELOPMENT REACTION TIME AND MEASUREMENT OF SKILLED PERFORMANCE. Name: Score:

LAB 1: MOTOR LEARNING & DEVELOPMENT REACTION TIME AND MEASUREMENT OF SKILLED PERFORMANCE. Name: Score: LAB 1: MOTOR LEARNING & DEVELOPMENT REACTION TIME AND MEASUREMENT OF SKILLED PERFORMANCE Name: Score: Part I: Reaction Time Environments Introduction: Reaction time is a measure of how long it takes a

More information

Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Stroke Patients After Upper Limb Robot-Assisted Therapy: A Pilot Study

Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Stroke Patients After Upper Limb Robot-Assisted Therapy: A Pilot Study Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Stroke Patients After Upper Limb Robot-Assisted Therapy: A Pilot Study N. Kinany 1,3,4(&), C. Pierella 1, E. Pirondini 3,4, M. Coscia 2, J. Miehlbradt 1, C. Magnin

More information

Unilateral Spatial Neglect USN

Unilateral Spatial Neglect USN 86 214 28 2 Behavioural Inattention Test Catherine Bergego Scale 28 2 214 223 2008 Key Words unilateral spatial neglect language disturbance mechanism priming rehabilitation Unilateral Spatial Neglect

More information

MOTOR LEARNING AND ITS TRANSFER DURING BILATERAL ARM REACHING

MOTOR LEARNING AND ITS TRANSFER DURING BILATERAL ARM REACHING MOTOR LEARNING AND ITS TRANSFER DURING BILATERAL ARM REACHING A Thesis Presented to The Academic Faculty by Linda Rosemary Harley In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy

More information

Ben Cipollini & Garrison Cottrell

Ben Cipollini & Garrison Cottrell Ben Cipollini & Garrison Cottrell NCPW 2014 Lancashire, UK A developmental approach to interhemispheric communication Ben Cipollini & Garrison Cottrell NCPW 2014 Lancashire, UK Lateralization Fundamental

More information

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 J'OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 446-452 (1963) Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 A visual configuration, or pattern, has qualities over and above those which can be specified

More information

Behavioral Task Performance

Behavioral Task Performance Zacks 1 Supplementary content for: Functional Reorganization of Spatial Transformations After a Parietal Lesion Jeffrey M. Zacks, PhD *, Pascale Michelon, PhD *, Jean M. Vettel, BA *, and Jeffrey G. Ojemann,

More information

Cerebral asymmetry in twins: Predictions of the right shift theory

Cerebral asymmetry in twins: Predictions of the right shift theory Cerebral asymmetry in twins: Predictions of the right shift theory Marian Annett School of Psychology, University of Leicester. Affiliation: Dr. Marian Annett, Emeritus Reader, School of Psychology, University

More information

Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in

Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 1972, 35, 545-550 Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions G. GAINOTTI, P. MESSERLI, AND R. TISSOT

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

Screening Tests. alertness, which can be described as the improvement of the response time through an

Screening Tests. alertness, which can be described as the improvement of the response time through an Screening Tests RehaCom is a therapy tool. Its focus stays on the fact that it fulfils the cognitive therapy. To use its application more effectively, now it offers the possibility of a screening. In contrast

More information

Caring Sheet #1: The Healthy Brain and Cognition By Shelly E. Weaverdyck, PhD

Caring Sheet #1: The Healthy Brain and Cognition By Shelly E. Weaverdyck, PhD : By Shelly E. Weaverdyck, PhD Introduction This caring sheet outlines the organization of the healthy brain and its cognitive functions. Caring sheet #2 describes the brain changes in dementia and the

More information

Sex differences and cerebral asymmetry facial affect perception as a function of depressed mood

Sex differences and cerebral asymmetry facial affect perception as a function of depressed mood Psychobiology 1994. 22 (2). 112-116 Sex differences and cerebral asymmetry facial affect perception as a function of depressed mood. In W. DAVID CREWS, JR. and DAVID W. HARRISON Virginia Polytechnic Institute

More information

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia In general, sensory processing as well as selective attention impairments are common amongst people with schizophrenia. It is important to note that experts have in

More information

Chapter 9. HANDEDNESS IN CHILDREN Elke H. Kraus CHAPTER OUTLINE. Assessment Intervention Theory Concluding Remarks SUMMARY

Chapter 9. HANDEDNESS IN CHILDREN Elke H. Kraus CHAPTER OUTLINE. Assessment Intervention Theory Concluding Remarks SUMMARY Chapter 9 HANDEDNESS IN CHILDREN Elke H. Kraus CHAPTER OUTLINE DEFINITION AND CLASSIFICATION OF HANDEDNESS Defining Handedness in Terms of Handedness Dimensions Classifying Handedness into Categories Description

More information

Identify these objects

Identify these objects Pattern Recognition The Amazing Flexibility of Human PR. What is PR and What Problems does it Solve? Three Heuristic Distinctions for Understanding PR. Top-down vs. Bottom-up Processing. Semantic Priming.

More information

Optimal exploration strategies in haptic search

Optimal exploration strategies in haptic search Optimal exploration strategies in haptic search Vonne van Polanen, Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest, Noortje Creemers, Merel J. Verbeek, and Astrid M. L. Kappers MOVE Research Institute, Faculty of Human Movement

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation. REVIEW OF FRONTAL LOBE STRUCTURES Main organization of frontal cortex: 1. Motor area (precentral gyrus). 2. Premotor & supplementary motor areas (immediately anterior to motor area). Includes premotor,

More information

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY Thomas Lachmann *, Gunjan Khera * and Cees van Leeuwen # * Psychology II, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany # Laboratory

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University; Graduate School of Economics, Osaka University syamane@kindai.ac.jp Abstract We examined

More information

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner Cerebral Cortex 1 Sarah Heilbronner heilb028@umn.edu Want to meet? Coffee hour 10-11am Tuesday 11/27 Surdyk s Overview and organization of the cerebral cortex What is the cerebral cortex? Where is each

More information

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Selective Attention Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form,

More information

Wetware: The Biological Basis of Intellectual Giftedness

Wetware: The Biological Basis of Intellectual Giftedness Wetware: The Biological Basis of Intellectual Giftedness Why is "giftedness" such a puzzle for parents? Why is there so much confusion? The most common plea heard on TAGFAM is "my child is different; please

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

Stuttering Research. Vincent Gracco, PhD Haskins Laboratories

Stuttering Research. Vincent Gracco, PhD Haskins Laboratories Stuttering Research Vincent Gracco, PhD Haskins Laboratories Stuttering Developmental disorder occurs in 5% of children Spontaneous remission in approximately 70% of cases Approximately 1% of adults with

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shikakeology: Designing Triggers for Behavior Change: Papers from the 2013 AAAI Spring Symposium The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University;

More information

Florian A. Kagerer Page 1 of 5 CURRICULUM VITAE POSITION TITLE. Assistant Professor

Florian A. Kagerer Page 1 of 5 CURRICULUM VITAE POSITION TITLE. Assistant Professor Florian A. Kagerer Page 1 of 5 CURRICULUM VITAE NAME Florian A. Kagerer ADDRESS Michigan State University Department of Kinesiology 27Q IM Sports Circle East Lansing, MI 48824 POSITION TITLE Assistant

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception

Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception Sundeep Teki Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception Auditory Cognition Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging SENSORY CORTEX SMA/PRE-SMA HIPPOCAMPUS BASAL

More information

Lesson 14. The Nervous System. Introduction to Life Processes - SCI 102 1

Lesson 14. The Nervous System. Introduction to Life Processes - SCI 102 1 Lesson 14 The Nervous System Introduction to Life Processes - SCI 102 1 Structures and Functions of Nerve Cells The nervous system has two principal cell types: Neurons (nerve cells) Glia The functions

More information

Mirror Writing: An Advantage for the Left-Handed?

Mirror Writing: An Advantage for the Left-Handed? BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 37, 616-627 (1989) Mirror Writing: An Advantage for the Left-Handed? JY~TSNA VAID Texas A & M University AND JOAN STILES-DAVIS University of California, San Diego It has been claimed

More information