Timing and Force Components in Bilateral Transfer of Learning

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Timing and Force Components in Bilateral Transfer of Learning"

Transcription

1 Brain and Cognition 44, (2000) doi: /brcg , available online at on Timing and Force Components in Bilateral Transfer of Learning Luis Augusto Teixeira School of Physical Education and Sport, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil Published online August 18, 2000 Bilateral transfer of perceptual and motor components in movement control was investigated through two experiments. In Experiment 1 a simple anticipatory timing task was practiced with either the preferred or the nonpreferred hand. After a short resting interval an additional set of trials was performed with the contralateral hand. In Experiment 2, the same experimental design was used to investigate bilateral transfer of fine force control in a wrist-flexion movement. Analysis of the results showed that bilateral transfer of learning took place for both anticipatory timing and force control, with more noticeable transfer of training for the former. Asymmetry in transfer was found for force control, with significant transfer only in the preferredto-nonpreferred direction. Transfer of anticipatory timing occurred similarly in both directions. These results indicated anticipatory timing as a powerful component for bilateral transfer, while force control showed to be more dependent on practice with the specific muscular system Academic Press INTRODUCTION Acquisition of motor skills involves two complimentary sides that determine the extent to which new movements can be performed proficiently: the specificity and the generalizability of learning. Experimental evidence has pointed out that some conditions of practice lead to specificity of learning so that remarkable decrease in performance is seen when the sensory information available during acquisition is modified (Proteau, Marteniuk, & Lévesque, 1992; Teixeira, 1995). On the other hand, different kinds of transfer of learning are known to occur after the acquisition of a motor task, ranging from transfer of relational to specific parameters of control (see Young & Schmidt, 1987, for review). Completely specific acquisition of motor skills or perfect transfer of learning, however, rarely are observed, as in most instances there are some components that are incorporated by a new control assembly and other ones that need to be specifically learned from the require- Address correspondence and reprint requests to Luis Augusto Teixeira, Escola de Educação Física e Esporte, Av. Prof. Mello Moraes, 65, Universidade de São Paulo, Butantã, S.P., Brazil lateixei@usp.br /00 $35.00 Copyright 2000 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

2 456 LUIS AUGUSTO TEIXEIRA ments of the new task. Studies on bilateral transfer of learning have usually shown an advantage of previous practice with the homologous contralateral limb in tasks such as drawing/writing (Hicks, 1974; Parlow & Kinsbourne, 1989; Thut et al., 1996), tactual recognition (Parlow & Kinsbourne, 1990; Sathian & Zangaladze, 1998), pointing under displaced vision (Choe & Welsh, 1974; Elliott & Roy, 1981; Kalil & Freedman, 1966), pursuit tracking (Hicks, Gualtieri, & Schroeder, 1983), mirror tracing (Cook, 1933a, 1933b), and maze tracking (Milisen & Riper, 1939; Wieg, 1932). These investigations have revealed an increased proficiency in performance with a resting body segment after practice with the contralateral homologous limb, but few investigations have provided information about what control parameters are effectively transferred to the contralateral side of the body. Considering transfer of learning in general, temporization of movements has been shown to be relatively independent of the effector system used to produce the response. Franz, Zelaznik, and Smith (1992) showed that intraindividual variability in repetitive timing tasks is significantly correlated between movements made with the index finger, forearm, and jaw, pointing out a common element responsible for movement synchronization (see also Keele, Pokorni, Corcos, & Ivry, 1985). Additionally, Matos, Teixeira, Lomônaco, Lima, and Sañudo (1997) have shown the transfer of anticipatory timing between tasks involving different levels of movement complexity. Because transfer of learning was observed to take place from a simple (button pressing) to a complex (hitting a foam ball with a racquet) anticipatory timing task as well as in the opposite direction, this study suggested anticipatory timing to be a generalizable element in movement control. On the other hand, transfer of learning in tasks characterized by reduced perceptual and strong motor components has shown to be less pronounced than in tasks that require greater perceptual resources. Karni et al. (1995) analyzed cortical and behavioral modifications during learning a serial fingertapping task and in subsequent transfer-tapping tasks performed with either the same or the contralateral hand. The results indicated that learning led to an increased cortical map only for the practiced motor task, and neither transfer to different tapping sequences performed with the same hand nor transfer between hands for the same sequence were found. Similar specificity of learning has been found by Teixeira (1992, 1993) in investigations on bilateral transfer of learning when a task requiring fine force control was used. In these studies, Teixeira employed a task of propelling a small plastic disk with the index finger from a home position to a horizontal target on a smooth surface and analyzed the gain in performance with the contralateral hand after practice. The results indicated no significant improvement for the contralateral hand in comparison to a condition without any previous practice. In general, these results are indicative that bilateral transfer of learning is task specific, with potential of transfer apparently being a function of the main control components required for proficient performance. Another as-

3 TRANSFER OF TIMING AND FORCE 457 pect of bilateral transfer of learning related to the specificity of the task is the direction of transfer. In the cross-activation model (Parlow & Kinsbourne, 1989), direction of bilateral transfer of learning has been proposed to be related to relative cerebral specialization of function. When the dominant neuromotor system is used, engrams are formed in both cerebral hemispheres, while the use of the nondominant neuromotor system leads to the formation of a single-sided engram in the nondominant hemisphere. As a corollary, bilateral transfer is hypothesized to take place from the dominant to the nondominant system but not in the opposite direction. Because the left cerebral hemisphere is supposed to play the main role in the sequential organization of movements and the right hemisphere to be the main responsible for visuospatial analysis (Goodale, 1990), the direction of transfer would be determined by the main component required for the control of action. In the callosal access model (Taylor & Heilman, 1980), on the other hand, it has been proposed the formation of a neural representation only in the dominant left hemisphere. When the dominant side of the body is used there is a direct access to this representation. When a nondominant limb is used, control activities of the nondominant hemisphere are carried out with the leading participation of the dominant contralateral hemisphere through the corpus callosum. Thus, as a function of bilateral cerebral activation in the performance with the nondominant (but not with the dominant) limb, transfer of learning from the nondominant to dominant hand is expected to take place. The research strategy employed here to investigate the issues raised above was to select a task composed of strong perceptual and weak effector components (Experiment 1) and then of the opposite characteristics, i.e., strong effector and weak perceptual components (Experiment 2) and to analyze the amount of bilateral transfer of learning after practicing one of these tasks with either the preferred or the nonpreferred hand. EXPERIMENT 1 In Experiment 1 a strong perceptual/weak effector component task was used to assess the bilateral transferability of learning. The task selected to represent this category involved pressing a switch coincidentally with the end of the displacement of a luminous stimulus. The main skill developed during the acquisition of this task was to make a simple movement in a narrow temporal window; that is, to learn to anticipate the correct time to produce the response to make two events occur simultaneously. Utilization of the contralateral hand after the task acquisition was expected to lead to a similar level of performance in relation to the practiced hand, since this short and simple movement requires a minimum of both spatial and force accuracy. As the perceptual component was maintained unchanged in the transfer condition, one could expect to find a high index of bilateral transfer to the untrained hand.

4 458 LUIS AUGUSTO TEIXEIRA Method Subjects. University students (n 40), ages 18 to 33 years old (M 22 years), volunteered to participate in this study. Thirty-seven subjects declared right-handedness for fine motor skills and three of them declared left-handedness. Apparatus and task. A commercial Bassin Anticipation Timer (Lafayette Instrument) was used to produce the displacement of a luminous signal in the direction of the subject. This apparatus is made up of a metallic runway 152 cm long, which holds 32 light-emitting diodes (LEDs) arranged in line on its mid-longitudinal axis. Adjacent LEDs are spaced by intervals of 4.5 cm, which gives rise to a displacement distance of 144 cm. The sequenced lightening of the LEDs produce the perception of a single stimulus moving from one end of the runway to the other. The runway was placed on the mid-sagittal plane in a horizontal position a few centimeters from the subject, with the stimulus moving toward the subject at a constant velocity of 4.4 m/s. At the end of the runway there was a manual switch, which was to be pressed with the thumb coincidentally with the lightening of the last LED of the sequence. The performance was analyzed as a function of the unsigned temporal difference between the synchronizatory movement and the actual arrival of the luminous stimulus at the criterion position (absolute error) and consistency of response (variable error). Design and procedures. Subjects sat facing the apparatus, which was positioned on a workbench, aligning the mid-sagittal axis of their bodies with the line formed by LEDs on the runway. After assessing subjects preferred hand by verbally inquiring about the preferred hand used to perform fine motor tasks such as writing and painting, they were familiarized with the task and experimental procedures by performing three trials in the condition of the following practice. Immediately after familiarization, 60 practice trials were performed in a single series, with intertrial intervals of approximately 7 s. On each trial a verbal warning signal was followed by the lightening of the first LED of the runway (for 1 s), and then the luminous stimulus began to move toward the subject. The subject s practice hand was kept in a constant place, positioned on the workbench beside the last LED of the sequence. Subjects were pseudorandomly assigned to one of two groups, maintaining the number of subjects and the number of males and females equivalent between them. One group carried out practice trials with their preferred hand. After this acquisition phase, they remained engaged on a cognitive task for 10 min and then performed five trials with the nonpreferred hand (P-NP group). The other group underwent the opposite treatment, practicing with the nonpreferred hand and being transferred to the preferred hand (NP-P group). Knowledge of results was provided both during acquisition and transfer by verbally informing the subject with the directional timing error (in milliseconds) after every trial. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Absolute error in the acquisition were grouped in blocks of six trials and analyzed through a two-way 2 (Group) 10 (Block) ANOVA with repeated measures on the second factor. The results showed only a significant main effect for Block [F(9, 342) 7.73, p.0001], indicating improvement in accuracy during acquisition. Absence of a significant main effect for Group [F(1, 38).35, p.1] and interaction [F(9, 342) 1.13, p.1] showed equivalent levels of accuracy and homogeneous improvement throughout acquisition in the performance with the preferred and nonpreferred hand (Fig. 1). In the comparison between transfer and control trials, a descriptive analysis was made by calculating the index of transfer, based on the proportional

5 TRANSFER OF TIMING AND FORCE 459 FIG. 1. Temporal absolute error for blocks of six trials during the acquisition phase for the preferred (P) and nonpreferred (NP) hands. gain in performance in the transfer condition in relation to the opposite group s initial five acquisition trials (same hand, intergroup comparison) and the index of performance decline, representing the proportional deterioration of performance from the last five acquisition trials in relation to transfer (different hand, intragroup comparison). For the preferred hand, the experimental condition had a 36% gain in performance, while its index of performance decline was 27% (Fig. 2a). A similar index of transfer was achieved with the nonpreferred hand, in which there was a 41% gain in relation to the control condition and an index of decline equal to 29% (Fig. 2b). Further inferential analysis was conducted for accuracy scores through a three-way 2 (Group) 3 (Phase: beginning of acquisition end of acquisition transfer) 5 (Trial) ANOVA with repeated measures on the last two factors. The results indicated significant main effects for Phase [F(2, 76) 30.61, p.001] and Trial [F(4, 152) 6.81, p.001]. Significant interactions were observed for Group Phase [F(2, 76) 4.08, p.05], Group Trial [F(4, 152) 2.67, p.05], and Phase Trial [F(8, 304) 3.07, p.005]. Newman Keuls post hoc procedures pointed out significant superior performance of the experimental conditions over their controls, i.e., in the comparison between transfer trials of one group with initial trials of the other group, except for the last trial when similar performance was observed. These results were due to a significant improvement of performance throughout trials in the beginning of acquisition while no significant modifications in temporal accuracy were observed for transfer trials. Additionally, in the analysis of the transition from acquisition to transfer no significant differences were found, which indicates no disruption in performance by the transfer task. Analysis of variable error was conducted through a two-way 2 (Group) 3 (Phase) ANOVA with repeated measures on the second factor. The results

6 460 LUIS AUGUSTO TEIXEIRA FIG. 2. Temporal absolute error for the initial trials of the P-NP group (P-Bgn), final (NP-End) and transfer (P-Exp) trials of the NP-P group (a), initial trials of the NP-P group (NP- Bgn), and final (P-End) and transfer (NP-Exp) trials of the P-NP group (b). indicated only significant main effects for Phase [F(2, 76) 24.61, p.001]. Post hoc comparisons pointed out significant differences between beginning of acquisition and the other two phases, with no difference between end of acquisition and transfer. These results showed that consistency developed by both groups during practice was maintained in the transfer trials, resulting in a reduced variability of performance in relation to the beginning of acquisition (Fig. 3). Overall, these results indicated that anticipatory timing performance transfers bilaterally in a symmetric way. Comparing the gain of the contralateral nonpracticed hand over the control condition without previous practice, both P-NP and NP-P groups showed a significant improved performance. Considering the intergroup index of transfer, the similarity between the proportional gain in relation to initial trials for both groups indicates that bilateral transfer was rather symmetric, with the same amount of transfer from the preferred to nonpreferred hand as in the opposite direction. Low intragroup indexes of performance decline and absence of significant differences in the transition from acquisition to transfer, both in accuracy and consistency, pointed

7 TRANSFER OF TIMING AND FORCE 461 FIG. 3. trials. Temporal variable error for P-NP and NP-P groups in the initial, final, and transfer out a powerful bilateral transfer. Such findings reveal the subjects capacity to maintain similar levels of performance in the transfer to the contralateral homologous effector system in relation to the end of acquisition. In agreement with previous findings (Franz et al., 1992; Keele et al., 1985; Matos et al., 1997), the results presented here support the proposition of timing as a motor function relatively independent of the effector system, which can be incorporated into different sensorimotor maps without necessity of specific practice with a particular limb in order to maintain the level of performance achieved in the acquisition of a primary timing task. Additionally, such bilateral transfer achievement was observed on the very first trials, indicating that incorporation of the timing component by the contralateral hand structure of control was remarkably efficient. EXPERIMENT 2 Experiment 2 was designed to test transferability of learning in a task with strong effector and weak perceptual components. As tasks involving control of force in a stable environment are representative of this category of skills, the task consisted of launching a cursor through a linear runway to a specified target position. Because the launching of the cursor was made with a wristflexion movement, good performance at this task required refined control over the force produced to propel the cursor with the preferred or nonpreferred hand, without considerable load on perceptual abilities. As the main requirement for accurate performance at this task is controlled application of force with manual muscular groups, a limited bilateral transfer of learning was expected to be found.

8 462 LUIS AUGUSTO TEIXEIRA Method Subjects. University students (n 40), ages 18 to 28 years old (M 21 years), participated as subjects in this experiment. Thirty-seven subjects asserted preference for the right hand to perform fine motor skills and three used the left hand. None of these subjects participated in Experiment 1. Apparatus and task. A commercial Linear Positioning Apparatus (Lafayette Instrument) was used to produce a force-control task. This instrument is composed of a 1-m-long tubular runway supported by a wooden frame, with a parallel scale in millimeters. A low-friction moving cursor can be displaced along this runway. Its position is shown on a digital display, indicating the discrepancy between the intended and the actual end position. At both ends of this runway there are cursor displacement limiters, which can be fixed at desired points along the runway to determine the start position of the movement. To yield a task requiring force control, subjects were asked to launch the cursor to a target 50 cm away from the initial position, using a wrist/fingers-flexion movement. Right-hand movements were made from the right to the left side, while left-hand movements were performed in the opposite direction. The cursor was contacted with the index and middle fingers, and the movement was initiated with the elbow and wrist lined up with the shoulder on the sagittal plane. In this initial position, the wrist and fingers were kept hyperextended in order to assume a favorable angle to propel the cursor. During the movement the elbow was maintained static and flexed at approximately 120 and the propelling of the cursor was performed by flexing in a continuous way the wrist and the two fingers contacting the cursor. Design and procedures. The experimental design was similar to that used in Experiment 1. Subjects were pseudorandomly assigned either to the P-NP group (i.e., practicing the task with the preferred hand and transferring to the nonpreferred hand) or to the NP-P group (practicing with the nonpreferred hand and transferring to the preferred hand). After determining subjects preferred hand for fine motor skills through verbal inquiry, they sat facing the apparatus. They had their shoulder, corresponding to their practice hand, aligned with the start position of the cursor. A limiter was used to establish the initial position for a distance of 50 cm from the target place. The target was indicated by a vertical arrow right under the criterion position along the runway. To prevent a whole-arm movement, a vertical paddle was positioned close to the wrist, limiting the movement in the direction of the target. Subjects were given three familiarization trials to assess their comprehension of the task and procedures. Afterward they began the acquisition phase, in which after every trial the experimenter recorded the discrepancy between the criterion and the actual final position of the propelled cursor and signaled to the subject to return the cursor to the initial position using their opposite hand. In order to prevent fatigue in the proximal muscles of the arm, the subjects were instructed to uphold their practicing arm on the table supporting the apparatus in the intertrial intervals. Because intrinsic knowledge of results was available from the apparatus scale during movement execution and cursor displacement, no augmented feedback was given. The 60 acquisition trials were performed in sequence with intertrial intervals of 7 8 s. After an active resting interval (the same activity as for Experiment 1), subjects changed their position in relation to the apparatus to perform five transfer trials. In the transfer task they had their shoulder, elbow, and hand of the resting arm lined up with the contralateral initial position and performed the 50-cm launching task with the opposite hand, also with the displacement of their wrist in the direction of the target limited by a paddle. Results and Discussion The results were analyzed in the same way as for Experiment 1, using spatial accuracy (absolute error) and consistency (variable error) as depen-

9 TRANSFER OF TIMING AND FORCE 463 FIG. 4. Spatial absolute error for blocks of six trials during the acquisition phase for the preferred (P) and nonpreferred (NP) hands. dent variables. The two-way 2 (Group) 10 (Block) ANOVA with repeated measures on the second factor, employed for analysis of absolute error in the acquisition phase, revealed only a significant main effect for Block [F(9, 342) 27.44, p.0001], indicating a homogeneous improvement in accuracy for both P-NP and NP-P groups (Fig. 4). The index of transfer showed an advantage of 11% for the experimental preferred hand in relation to its control, and an index of decline of 115% in relation to the performance with the contralateral practice hand in the end of acquisition (Fig. 5a). The experimental nonpreferred hand achieved an index of transfer equal to 35% and an index of performance decline of 65% (Fig. 5b). The three-way 2 (Group) 3 (Phase) x 5 (Trial) ANOVA with repeated measures on the last two factors for absolute error indicated significant main effects for Phase [F(2, 76) 33.35, p.001] and Trial [F(4, 152) 8.76, p.001]. Significant interactions were found for Group Phase [F(2, 76) 4.08, p.05], Phase Trial [F(8, 304) 2.86, p.05], and Group Phase Trial [F(8, 304) 2.21, p.05]. Newman Keuls procedures indicated that a significant gain in transfer compared to initial acquisition trials took place only on the first trial for the P-NP condition, while no significant gain was found for the NP-P group. In the comparison between end of acquisition and transfer, significant decline of performance was observed only on the first trial in the NP-P condition, while no significant differences were observed in the opposite direction of transfer. The combined effect of transfer for the P-NP group and decline of performance for the NP-P group on the first trial of transfer led to a significantly superior performance with the nonpreferred hand on this trial, while no significant between-hand differences were found afterward. Additionally, there were no between-trial significant differences in the performance with the nonpreferred hand in the

10 464 LUIS AUGUSTO TEIXEIRA FIG. 5. Spatial absolute error for the initial trials of the P-NP group (P-Bgn), final (NP- End) and transfer (P-Exp) trials of the NP-P group (a), initial trials of the NP-P group (NP- Bgn), and final (P-End) and transfer (NP-Exp) trials of the P-NP group (b). transfer phase, which indicates stability of performance. Differently, the experimental preferred hand showed a similar pattern in relation to beginning of acquisition, with significant improvement of performance throughout trials. Analysis of variable error, conducted through a two-way 2 (Group) 3 (Phase) ANOVA with repeated measures on the second factor, indicated only a significant main effect for Phase [F(2, 76) 24.61, p.0001]. Posthoc contrasts indicated that all comparisons were significant, revealing an increased consistency of performance in transfer compared to initial trials in spite of the decline in the transition from acquisition to transfer (Fig.6). Previous studies on bilateral transfer of force control have failed to show significant effects in tasks involving fine manual musculature (Teixeira, 1992, 1993). Lack of bilateral transfer of control between distal body segments has been explained by absence of neural interhemispheric projections between homotopic primary motor and somatosensory areas representing the hands in the cerebral cortex (Thut et al., 1996). The present results, though, indicated significant improvement of the nonpreferred hand as a result of practice with the preferred hand, while the opposite direction of transfer did not show similar transfer effects. The inferential analysis was complemented

11 TRANSFER OF TIMING AND FORCE 465 FIG. 6. trials. Spatial variable error for P-NP and NP-P groups in the initial, final, and transfer by indexes of transfer and performance decline by showing that transfer from the preferred to nonpreferred hand produced a threefold higher index of transfer and a twofold lower index of performance decline in comparison with the opposite direction of transfer. Thus, bilateral transfer of force control was found to be asymmetric, with the main transfer from the preferred to the nonpreferred hand. GENERAL DISCUSSION In principle, bilateral transfer of learning may take place in different ways. It may occur through transfer of all relevant components for proficient performance or through transfer of only more generalizable, effector independent, control components. Yet, transfer may be partial, with decline in performance of one or more components in the transfer task, or it may be complete, so that no alteration in the performance is observed in the transfer to the contralateral limb for some components. Previous studies have shown that there is a third element to be considered, i.e., symmetry of transfer, with several findings of unidirectional transfer of learning either from the dominant to nondominant limb (e.g., Parlow & Kinsbourne, 1989) or in the opposite direction (e.g., Taylor & Heilman, 1980). The present results showed that both anticipatory timing and force control are transferable components in motor behavior. Comparison of the results of Experiment 1 and Experiment 2, though, reveals that bilateral transfer of learning occurred in qualitatively different ways for these two components in sensorimotor control. Transfer of force control between hands was found to be asymmetrical, reaching a significant level only from the preferred to nonpreferred hand direction. These results are in accordance with Parlow and Kinsbourne s (1989) proposition that bilateral transfer of learning occurs from the dominant to nondominant neuromotor system. Differently, transfer of anticipatory

12 466 LUIS AUGUSTO TEIXEIRA timing control showed a symmetric relationship, with both hands benefiting similarly from previous practice with the contralateral hand, as pointed out by the index of transfer, as well as demonstrated the same capacity to maintain approximately 70% of the level of performance achieved with the contralateral practiced hand, as was shown by the index of performance decline. Considered as a whole, these findings demonstrate that amount and symmetry of bilateral transfer of learning are dependent on the main components involved in movement control for a given task. Most studies on bilateral transfer of learning have used motor tasks in which there exists a complex combination of perceptual and motor components, or that even require different parameters of movement control, so that it is difficult to determine what control components were in fact transferred from one limb to the other. Previous findings by Teixeira (1992, 1993), employing a force-control task, have shown no significant gain in performance for the contralateral nonpracticed hand, which suggests acquisition of fine force control to be dependent on practice specifically with a particular neuromotor system. Somewhat different from these findings, the present results showed that for the wrist-flexion movement task used here the nonpreferred resting limb benefited from practice with the contralateral homologous limb. Scarce significant differences in relation to controls and high indexes of performance decline, however, indicate that the amount of bilateral transfer of force control was limited in relation to that observed for anticipatory timing. Such weak transfer of learning of force control may be related to the lack of neural interhemispheric projections between hand areas in the primary motor and somatosensory areas in the cerebral cortex (Pandya & Vignolo, 1971), which requires indirect neural pathways for interhemispheric communication between the main cortical areas responsible for controlling distal muscular systems. This characteristic of the brain architecture may dictate the increased dependence on specific practice for the acquisition of force control in manual movements, as observed here. Apparently contradictory to this is the fact that the anticipatory timing task involved a distal thumb movement and even so the transfer was rather robust in either direction. The anticipatory timing task, however, required neither accurate force nor another form of complex spatial specification, so that the critical aspect for proficient performance was initiating the movement at the right time. Under such circumstances, alteration of the effector system seems to lead to minimal disturbance on performance. These conclusions are in accordance with previous findings of significant correlations (Franz et al., 1992; Keele et al., 1985) and transfer of learning (Matos et al., 1997) between timing tasks of different levels of complexity in terms of the effector system used, shown by a timing capacity that can be used bilaterally, independent of the specific limb employed to perform the task. Previous conceptual models on bilateral transfer of learning have proposed the formation of single-sided engrams in the dominant cerebral hemi-

13 TRANSFER OF TIMING AND FORCE 467 sphere independent of the limb used during practice (Taylor & Heilman, 1980) or in the nondominant hemisphere when the nondominant hand is employed in the acquisition (Parlow & Kinsbourne, 1989). Additionally, Laszlo, Baguley, and Bairstow (1970) have proposed the formation of unilateral engrams on the basis of practice with the contralateral limb. All of these models propose a single direction of bilateral transfer of learning, which varies as a function of particular explanations for interhemispheric flow of information. In light of the present findings, no single model seems to provide a good fit for all the data. Symmetry between the preferred and nonpreferred hand observed in the performance on the anticipatory timing task suggests that the two cerebral hemispheres worked in a quite similar way, without asymmetries in the mode in which the task was acquired or in the amount of contralateral transfer of learning. Thus, formation of unilateral engrams, or asymmetrical transfer as a function of hemispheric dominance, turns out to be an inappropriate general conclusion. A conceptualization put forth to explain current and previous experimental findings is based on the conception of cerebral hemispheres as composed of a number of neural populations, which are responsible for specific functions in sensorimotor control. The assembly and activation of a group of such control units to perform a motor task in the hemisphere contralateral to the limb employed in the acquisition leads to activation of homologous units in the opposite hemisphere (cf. Tanji, Okano, & Sato, 1988). Power of interhemispheric coactivation is thought to depend on variables like the main sensorimotor components involved in the control of the movement and the strength of the connections among components of a neural population responsible for controlling a given sensorimotor operation. As a function of particular structural and anatomical characteristics of homologous limbs acquired throughout life-span development, motor components used in force control are dependent to some extent on specific practice with the criterion limb (Teixeira, 1992, 1993). Such practice with a particular body segment seems to hold an important function in proficient performance of strengthening the connections between control units and in molding them to conform with the idiosyncrasies of this particular effector system. Perceptual components like anticipatory timing, on the other hand, are capable of achieving better bilateral transfer. The fact that this component in a global sensorimotor map is a behavioral function that results from the collective work of a set of processing units, which are widely distributed throughout large regions in the associative area in both cerebral hemispheres, renders its connection to different effector systems easier to be implemented. This feature prevents greater disturbances in the response when transfer to a different action system is required. Bilateral transfer of learning, hence, is proposed to be a function of utilization of general-purpose control units and of well-defined neural groups of sensorimotor control. Similarity of performance between the preferred and

14 468 LUIS AUGUSTO TEIXEIRA nonpreferred hand throughout the acquisition phase shows that both hands have the same potential for achieving refined control over these kinds of movement. It is hypothesized that asymmetry in bilateral transfer of force control observed here is due to asymmetric neural architectures established before the beginning of learning as a result of previous differential usage of the two hands. As a well-defined global map is expected to produce stronger coactivation signals between the two cerebral hemispheres, the transfer effect in tasks requiring refined movement control should be higher from the preferred (more used) to the nonpreferred system. In tasks in which general purpose units play the main role, like in simple anticipatory timing tasks, transfer to the homologous contralateral limb can be expected to be stronger and symmetric, since these functions are relatively independent of a specific effector system. REFERENCES Choe, C. S., & Welsh, R. B. (1974). Variables affecting the intermanual transfer and decay after prism adaptation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, Cook, T. W. (1933a). Studies in cross education: I. Mirror tracing the star-shaped maze. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16, Cook, T. W. (1933b). Studies in cross education: II. Further experiments in mirror tracing the star-shaped maze. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16, Elliott, D., & Roy, E. A. (1981). Interlimb transfer after adaptation to visual displacement: Patterns predicted from the functional closeness of limb neural control centres. Perception, 10, Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N., & Smith, A. (1992). Evidence of common timing processes in the control of manual, orofacial, and speech movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 24, Goodale, M. A. (1990). Brain asymmetries in the control of reaching. In M. A. Goodale (Ed.), Vision and action: The control of grasping. Norwood, NJ, Ablex. Pp Hicks, R. E. (1974). Asymmetry of bilateral transfer. American Journal of Psychology, 87, Hicks, R. E., Gualtieri, C. T., & Schroeder, S. R. (1983). Cognitive and motor components of bilateral transfer. American Journal of Psychology, 96, Kalil, R. E., & Freedman, S. J. (1966). Intermanual transfer of compensation for displaced vision. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 22, Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during skill learning. Nature, 377, Keele, S. W., Pokorny, R. A., Corcos, D. M., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do perception and motor production share common time mechanisms? A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, Laszlo, J. I., Baguley, R. A., & Bairstow, P. J. (1970). Bilateral transfer in tapping skill in the absence of peripheral information. Journal of Motor Behavior, 2, Matos, T. C. S., Teixeira, L. A., Lomônaco, F. B., Lima, A. C. P., & Sañudo, A. (1997). Transfer of learning between anticipatory timing movements with different levels of mo-

15 TRANSFER OF TIMING AND FORCE 469 tor complexity. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Canadian Society for Psychomotor Learning and Sport Psychology, 28, 56. Milisen, R., & Riper, C. van (1939). Differential transfer of training in a rotary activity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, Pandya, D. N., & Vignolo, L. A. (1971). Intra- and interhemispheric projections of the precentral, premotor and arcuate areas in the rhesus monkey. Brain Research, 26, Parlow, S. E., & Kinsbourne, M. (1989). Asymmetrical transfer of training between hands: Implications for interhemisferic communication in normal brain. Brain and Cognition, 11, Parlow, S. E., & Kinsbourne, M. (1990). Asymmetrical transfer of braille acquisition between hands. Brain and Language, 39, Proteau, L., Marteniuk, R. G., & Lévesque, L. (1992). A sensorimotor basis for motor learning: Evidence indicating specificity of practice. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 44, Sathian K., & Zangaladze, A. (1998). Perceptual learning in tactile hyperacuity: Complete intermanual transfer but limited retention. Experimental Brain Research, 118, Tanji, J., Okano, K., & Sato, K. C. (1988). Neuronal activity in cortical motor areas related to ipsilateral, contralateral, and bilateral digit movements of monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 60, Taylor, H. G., & Heilman, K. M. (1980). Left-hemisphere motor dominance in right-handers. Cortex, 16, Teixeira, L. A. (1992). Transferência de aprendizagem inter-membros: O que é transferido? [Interlimb transfer of learning: What is transferred?]. Revista Paulista de Educação Física, 6, Teixeira, L. A. (1993). Bilateral transfer of learning: The effector side in focus. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 25, Teixeira, L.A. (1995). Integração visomotora em tarefas sincronizatórias [Visuomotor integration in anticipatory timing tasks]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, S.P., Brazil. Thut, G., Cook, N., Regard, M., Leenders, K., Halsband, U., & Landis, T. (1996). Intermanual transfer of proximal and distal motor engrams in humans. Experimental Brain Research, 108, Wieg, E. L. (1932). Bi-lateral transfer in the motor learning of young children and adults. Child Development, 3, Young, D. E., & Schmidt, R. A. (1987). Transfer of movement control in motor skill learning. In S. M. Cornier, & J. D. Hagman (Eds.), Transfer of learning: Contemporary research and applications. San Diego, Academic Press. Pp

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical Short Communication TheScientificWorldJOURNAL (2006) 6, 1805 1809 ISSN 1537-744X; DOI 10.1100/tsw.2006.291 Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical V. Frak 1,2, *, D. Bourbonnais 2, I. Croteau

More information

Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback

Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback Exp Brain Res (2006) 168: 307 311 DOI 10.1007/s00221-005-0280-y RESEARCH NOTE Daniel J. Goble Æ Colleen A. Lewis Æ Susan H. Brown Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback Received:

More information

Asymmetrical intermanual transfer of learning in a sensorimotor task

Asymmetrical intermanual transfer of learning in a sensorimotor task Exp Brain Res (2010) 202:927 934 DOI 10.1007/s00221-010-2184-8 RESEARCH NOTE Asymmetrical intermanual transfer of learning in a sensorimotor task Waldemar Kirsch Joachim HoVmann Received: 15 September

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS.

ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS. ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS. Alan M. Wing Sensory Motor Neuroscience Centre, The University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, B15 2TT, UK. a.m.wing@bham.ac.uk J. Randall Flanagan

More information

Sequential effects after practice with the dominant and non-dominant hand on the acquisition of a sliding task in schoolchildren

Sequential effects after practice with the dominant and non-dominant hand on the acquisition of a sliding task in schoolchildren This article was downloaded by: [Tel Aviv University] On: 01 March 2015, At: 02:41 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Laboratory Equipment & Test Procedures. Motor learning and control historically has built its discipline through laboratory research. This has led to the

More information

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Exp Brain Res (1998) 123:346±350 Springer-Verlag 1998 RESEARCH NOTE Jinsung Wang George E. Stelmach Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Received: 1 April

More information

Peripheral facial paralysis (right side). The patient is asked to close her eyes and to retract their mouth (From Heimer) Hemiplegia of the left side. Note the characteristic position of the arm with

More information

Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection

Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection Vijay Laxmi, J. N. Carter and R. I. Damper Image, Speech and Intelligent Systems (ISIS) Research Group Department of Electronics and Computer Science University

More information

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg The concept that different parts of the brain did different things started with Spurzheim and Gall, whose phrenology became quite

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

The origins of localization

The origins of localization Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. The origins of localization The concept that different parts of the brain did different

More information

Multi-joint Mechanics Dr. Ted Milner (KIN 416)

Multi-joint Mechanics Dr. Ted Milner (KIN 416) Multi-joint Mechanics Dr. Ted Milner (KIN 416) Muscle Function and Activation It is not a straightforward matter to predict the activation pattern of a set of muscles when these muscles act on multiple

More information

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D.

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions Michael E. Goldberg, M.D. The origins of localization The concept that different parts of the brain did different

More information

Higher Cortical Function

Higher Cortical Function Emilie O Neill, class of 2016 Higher Cortical Function Objectives Describe the association cortical areas processing sensory, motor, executive, language, and emotion/memory information (know general location

More information

NOTE INTERMANUAL TRANSFER IN A SIMPLE MOTOR TASK

NOTE INTERMANUAL TRANSFER IN A SIMPLE MOTOR TASK NOTE INTERMANUAL TRANSFER IN A SIMPLE MOTOR TASK Katrin Schulze 1, Eileen Lüders 1 and Lutz Jäncke 2 ( 1 Otto-von-Guericke-University Magdeburg, Department of Experimental and General Psychology, Magdeburg,

More information

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Overview Explain the significance of the turn to the brain in cognitive science Explain Mishkin and Ungerleider s hypothesis that there are two distinct visual systems Outline

More information

Neurophysiology of systems

Neurophysiology of systems Neurophysiology of systems Motor cortex (voluntary movements) Dana Cohen, Room 410, tel: 7138 danacoh@gmail.com Voluntary movements vs. reflexes Same stimulus yields a different movement depending on context

More information

Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill

Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 1979, Vol. S, No. 2, 179-187 Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill John B. Shea and

More information

Right hemisphere. Left hemisphere. Demonstration: Contralateral processing. Motor functions on left side of body Perceives left side of space

Right hemisphere. Left hemisphere. Demonstration: Contralateral processing. Motor functions on left side of body Perceives left side of space Week 6 Lab: Hemispheric Functions - Contralateral processing Back to Index Right hemisphere Motor functions on left side of body Perceives left side of space Left hemisphere Motor functions on right side

More information

The Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on the Kinematics of Reach-to-Pinch Function

The Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on the Kinematics of Reach-to-Pinch Function The Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on the Kinematics of Reach-to-Pinch Function Raviraj Nataraj, Peter J. Evans, MD, PhD, William H. Seitz, MD, Zong-Ming Li. Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH, USA. Disclosures:

More information

The Effects of Vision and Internal and External Focus of Attention on Balance in a Yoga Pose

The Effects of Vision and Internal and External Focus of Attention on Balance in a Yoga Pose University of Colorado, Boulder CU Scholar Undergraduate Honors Theses Honors Program Spring 017 The Effects of Vision and Internal and External Focus of Attention on Balance in a Yoga Pose Eun Kim Eun.B.Kim@Colorado.EDU

More information

Therapy Manual DO NOT PRINT

Therapy Manual DO NOT PRINT Therapy Manual Contents 1. Shoulder 2. Shoulder and elbow a. Protraction: 1 DoF 1 b. Flexion: 1 DoF 1-6 c. Extension: 1 DoF 1-2 d. Abduction: 1 DoF 1-4 e. External rotation: 1 DoF 1-14 a. Combined shoulder

More information

TRANSFER EFFECTS OF EYE-HAND CO-ORDINATION SKILLS FROM THE RIGHT TO THE LEFT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES IN SOUTH AFRICAN SCHOOLBOY RUGBY PLAYERS

TRANSFER EFFECTS OF EYE-HAND CO-ORDINATION SKILLS FROM THE RIGHT TO THE LEFT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES IN SOUTH AFRICAN SCHOOLBOY RUGBY PLAYERS African Journal for Physical, Health Education, Recreation and Dance (AJPHERD) Vol. 12, No. 1 (March) 2006, pp. 41-49. TRANSFER EFFECTS OF EYE-HAND CO-ORDINATION SKILLS FROM THE RIGHT TO THE LEFT CEREBRAL

More information

The Motor Systems. What s the motor system? Plan

The Motor Systems. What s the motor system? Plan The Motor Systems What s the motor system? Parts of CNS and PNS specialized for control of limb, trunk, and eye movements Also holds us together From simple reflexes (knee jerk) to voluntary movements

More information

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory TENNET XI 135 Results Allocentric WM. There was no difference between lesioned and control rats; i.e., there was an equal proportion of rats from the two groups in the target quadrant (saline, 100%; lesioned,

More information

Left hand, but not right hand, reaching is sensitive to visual context

Left hand, but not right hand, reaching is sensitive to visual context Exp Brain Res (010) 03:7 3 DOI 10.1007/s001-010-14-6 RESEARCH NOTE Left hand, but not right hand, reaching is sensitive to visual context Jos J. Adam Rick Müskens Susan Hoonhorst Jay Pratt Martin H. Fischer

More information

The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology?

The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology? ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 13(2), 117 122 Copyright 2001, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology? Patrick R. Green Department

More information

Neuroplasticity. Jake Kurczek 9/19/11. Cognitive Communication Disorders

Neuroplasticity. Jake Kurczek 9/19/11. Cognitive Communication Disorders Jake Kurczek 9/19/11 Activity Therapy Be creative Try new things Be prepared to fail Learn from past experiences Be flexible Participants begin working/communicating not good As they work together more

More information

Interlimb coupling in the proximal and distal joints : a comparison

Interlimb coupling in the proximal and distal joints : a comparison The University of Toledo The University of Toledo Digital Repository Master s and Doctoral Projects Interlimb coupling in the proximal and distal joints : a comparison Michael D. Iott The University of

More information

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m. Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Wednesday, 16 March

More information

Learning Objectives.

Learning Objectives. Emilie O Neill, class of 2016 Learning Objectives 1. Describe the types of deficits that occur with lesions in association areas including: prosopagnosia, neglect, aphasias, agnosia, apraxia 2. Discuss

More information

The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense

The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense www.tutis.ca/senses/ Contents Objectives... 1 Introduction... 2 Muscle Spindles and Golgi Tendon Organs... 3 Gamma Drive... 5 Three Spinal Reflexes...

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Transfer of Motor Learning across Arm Configurations

Transfer of Motor Learning across Arm Configurations The Journal of Neuroscience, November 15, 2002, 22(22):9656 9660 Brief Communication Transfer of Motor Learning across Arm Configurations Nicole Malfait, 1 Douglas M. Shiller, 1 and David J. Ostry 1,2

More information

Timing Variability in Circle Drawing and Tapping: Probing the Relationship Between Event and Emergent Timing

Timing Variability in Circle Drawing and Tapping: Probing the Relationship Between Event and Emergent Timing Journal of Motor Behavior, 2005, Vol. 37, No. 5, 395 403 Timing Variability in Circle Drawing and Tapping: Probing the Relationship Between Event and Emergent Timing Howard N. Zelaznik Department of Health

More information

Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning

Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning Introduction Learning new motor skills Variability in sensors and task Tennis: Velocity of ball Not all are equally probable over time Increased uncertainty:

More information

Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres. Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University

Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres. Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University The largest part of the brain. Ovoid shape. Two incompletely separated cerebral hemispheres. The outer surface of the cerebral

More information

Online Journal Club-Article Review

Online Journal Club-Article Review Online Journal Club-Article Review Article Citation Study Objective/Purpose (hypothesis) Brief Background (why issue is important; summary of previous literature) Study Design (type of trial, randomization,

More information

LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS

LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS WHAT WE DO Present studies and future directions Our laboratory is currently involved in two major areas of research. The first

More information

Voluntary Movements. Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley. Outline. Organization of the motor cortex (somatotopic) Corticospinal projection

Voluntary Movements. Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley. Outline. Organization of the motor cortex (somatotopic) Corticospinal projection Voluntary Movements Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley 1 Outline Organization of the motor cortex (somatotopic) Corticospinal projection Physiology of motor neurons Direction representation, population coding

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

Ipsilateral Motor Cortex Activity During Unimanual Hand Movements Relates to Task Complexity

Ipsilateral Motor Cortex Activity During Unimanual Hand Movements Relates to Task Complexity Ipsilateral Motor Cortex Activity During Unimanual Hand Movements Relates to Task Complexity Timothy Verstynen, Jörn Diedrichsen, Neil Albert, Paul Aparicio and Richard B. Ivry J Neurophysiol 93:1209-1222,

More information

and the surrounding annuli. Effects on grasp scaling be-

and the surrounding annuli. Effects on grasp scaling be- Brief Communication 177 The dissociation between perception and action in the Ebbinghaus illusion: Nonillusory effects of pictorial cues on grasp Angela M. Haffenden, Karen C. Schiff and Melvyn A. Goodale

More information

Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control

Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control These chapters come from Magill s (2010), Motor Learning and Control: Concepts and Application(9 th ) edition textbook. Additional materials regarding these key concepts

More information

What is Kinesiology? Basic Biomechanics. Mechanics

What is Kinesiology? Basic Biomechanics. Mechanics What is Kinesiology? The study of movement, but this definition is too broad Brings together anatomy, physiology, physics, geometry and relates them to human movement Lippert pg 3 Basic Biomechanics the

More information

TEMPORAL CHANGE IN RESPONSE BIAS OBSERVED IN EXPERT ANTICIPATION OF VOLLEYBALL SPIKES

TEMPORAL CHANGE IN RESPONSE BIAS OBSERVED IN EXPERT ANTICIPATION OF VOLLEYBALL SPIKES TEMPORAL CHANGE IN RESPONSE BIAS OBSERVED IN ANTICIPATION OF VOLLEYBALL SPIKES Tomoko Takeyama, Nobuyuki Hirose 2, and Shuji Mori 2 Department of Informatics, Graduate School of Information Science and

More information

SPECIFICITY OF BILATERAL TRANSFER EFFECT IN MIRROR DRAWING

SPECIFICITY OF BILATERAL TRANSFER EFFECT IN MIRROR DRAWING SPECIFICITY OF BILATERAL TRANSFER EFFECT IN MIRROR DRAWING By TOSHITERU HAT A YAM A (mul(~: li!) (Tohoku-Gakuin Univer8ity, Sendai) MASAYOSHI YO S H I I Z U M I (a~ie~) (Sendai City) and TOSHIRO SAT 0

More information

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above.

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above. Section: Chapter 14: Multiple Choice 1. Alex the parrot: pp.529-530 shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all

More information

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE Isabelle Guay and Simon Grondin Université Laval, Québec, Canada Email: simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca ABSTRACT

More information

A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Theoretical mechanisms and processes underlying motor skill acquisition by Sharon Brooks Dorfman A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE in PSYCHOLOGY

More information

What utility is there in distinguishing between active and passive touch? Jack M. Loomis and Susan J. Lederman

What utility is there in distinguishing between active and passive touch? Jack M. Loomis and Susan J. Lederman What utility is there in distinguishing between active and passive touch? Jack M. Loomis and Susan J. Lederman University of California, Santa Barbara and Queen's University Paper presented at the Psychonomic

More information

Evaluating Movement Posture Disorganization

Evaluating Movement Posture Disorganization Evaluating Movement Posture Disorganization A Criteria-Based Reference Format for Observing & Analyzing Motor Behavior in Children with Learning Disabilities By W. Michael Magrun, MS, OTR 3 R D E D I T

More information

Memory trace strength and response biasing in short-term motor memory*

Memory trace strength and response biasing in short-term motor memory* Memory & Cognition 1975, Vol. 3 (1), 58-62 Memory trace strength and response biasing in short-term motor memory* GEORGE E. STELMACH and J. A. SCOTT KELSO University ofwisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin 53706

More information

Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning

Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (3), 449 456 Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning Natacha Deroost and Eric Soetens Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels,

More information

LAB 4 BALANCE, PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT, AND FINE MOTOR SKILLS DEVELOPMENT

LAB 4 BALANCE, PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT, AND FINE MOTOR SKILLS DEVELOPMENT Introduction LAB 4 BALANCE, PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT, AND FINE MOTOR SKILLS DEVELOPMENT This lab consists of a series of experiments that explore various perceptual skills that help us understand how we perform

More information

Brain Structure and Function in Nephropathic Cystinosis

Brain Structure and Function in Nephropathic Cystinosis Brain Structure and Function in Nephropathic Cystinosis Doris A. Trauner M.D. Professor, Depts. of Neurosciences and Pediatrics University of California San Diego School of Medicine La Jolla, CA USA Cystinosis

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

The Language of Anatomy. (Anatomical Terminology)

The Language of Anatomy. (Anatomical Terminology) The Language of Anatomy (Anatomical Terminology) Terms of Position The anatomical position is a fixed position of the body (cadaver) taken as if the body is standing (erect) looking forward with the upper

More information

Exam 1. Mean 78.0% Median 80% Mode 86% Min 26% Max 98% Std Dev 12.6%

Exam 1. Mean 78.0% Median 80% Mode 86% Min 26% Max 98% Std Dev 12.6% Exam 1 Mean 78.0% Median 80% Mode 86% Min 26% Max 98% Std Dev 12.6% None at the moment! Announcements VII. Imaging techniques of the brain A. CT: anatomical B. MRI: anatomical C. fmri: functional D. SPECT

More information

passive arm movements*

passive arm movements* Arm-body adaptation with passive arm movements* JOHN S. BAILY Laboratory of Experimental Psychology University of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton, England Two experiments investigated the aftereffects of pointing

More information

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of Health and Human Development

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of Health and Human Development The Pennsylvania State University The Graduate School College of Health and Human Development INTERLIMB DIFFERENCES IN WRIST DIADOCHOKINESIS ARE MOST PRONOUNCED PERPENDICULAR TO THE INSTRUCTED MOTION,

More information

Vibrotactile masking:,a comparison of psychophysical procedures

Vibrotactile masking:,a comparison of psychophysical procedures Perception & Psychophysics 1977, Vol. 22 (5), 471-475 Vibrotactile masking:,a comparison of psychophysical procedures RITA E. SNYDER Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana 47401 The amounts of ipsilateral

More information

Spatial deficits in ideomotor limb apraxia A kinematic analysis of aiming movements

Spatial deficits in ideomotor limb apraxia A kinematic analysis of aiming movements Spatial deficits in ideomotor limb apraxia A kinematic analysis of aiming movements Kathleen Y. Haaland, 1 Deborah L. Harrington 1 and Robert T. Knight 2,3 Brain (1999), 122, 1169 1182 1 Psychology and

More information

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES Journal of Experimental Vol. 51, No. 4, 1956 Psychology CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES KENNETH H. KURTZ AND CARL I. HOVLAND Under conditions where several concepts are learned concurrently

More information

Takwa Adly Gabr Assistant lecturer of Audiology

Takwa Adly Gabr Assistant lecturer of Audiology Mismatch Negativity as an Objective Tool for the Assessment of Cognitive Function in Subjects with Unilateral Severe to Profound Sensorineural Hearing Loss Takwa Adly Gabr Assistant lecturer of Audiology

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Terms of Movements by Prof. Dr. Muhammad Imran Qureshi

Terms of Movements by Prof. Dr. Muhammad Imran Qureshi Terms of Movements by Prof. Dr. Muhammad Imran Qureshi Three systems of the body work in coordination to perform various movements of the body. These are: A System of Bones (Osteology), A System of Muscles

More information

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14 Motor Systems I Cortex Reading: BCP Chapter 14 Principles of Sensorimotor Function Hierarchical Organization association cortex at the highest level, muscles at the lowest signals flow between levels over

More information

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight? How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? What happens to visual-devoted brain structure in individuals who are born deprived of sight?

More information

PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS AND ERGONOMICS SOCIETY 48th ANNUAL MEETING

PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS AND ERGONOMICS SOCIETY 48th ANNUAL MEETING PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS AND ERGONOMICS SOCIETY 48th ANNUAL MEETING 2004 1464 EFFECT OF HANDLE DESIGN AND TARGET LOCATION ON WRIST POSTURE DURING AIMING WITH A LAPAROSCOPIC TOOL Judkins, T.N. 1,

More information

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses? Mental Imagery What is Imagery? What is nature of the images? Exact copy of original images? Represented in terms of meaning? If so, then how does the subjective sensation of an image arise? What is the

More information

Carl Wernicke s Contribution to Theories of Conceptual Representation in the Cerebral Cortex. Nicole Gage and Gregory Hickok Irvine, California

Carl Wernicke s Contribution to Theories of Conceptual Representation in the Cerebral Cortex. Nicole Gage and Gregory Hickok Irvine, California Carl Wernicke s Contribution to Theories of Conceptual Representation in the Cerebral Cortex Nicole Gage and Gregory Hickok Irvine, California Acknowledgments Christian Sekirnjak, Ph.D. San Diego, CA Heidi

More information

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of Health and Human Development

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of Health and Human Development The Pennsylvania State University The Graduate School College of Health and Human Development INTERLIMB DIFFERENCES IN COORDINATION OF UNSUPPORTED REACHING MOVEMENTS A Thesis in Kinesiology by Jacob E.

More information

Multi-joint limbs permit a flexible response to unpredictable events

Multi-joint limbs permit a flexible response to unpredictable events Exp Brain Res (1997) 117:148±152 Springer-Verlag 1997 RESEARCH NOTE E.M. Robertson R.C. Miall Multi-joint limbs permit a flexible response to unpredictable events Received: 24 March 1997 / Accepted: 7

More information

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Hearing-aids Induce Plasticity in the Auditory System: Perspectives From Three Research Designs and Personal Speculations About the

More information

MOTOR EVALUATION SCALE FOR UPPER EXTREMITY IN STROKE PATIENTS (MESUPES-arm and MESUPES-hand)

MOTOR EVALUATION SCALE FOR UPPER EXTREMITY IN STROKE PATIENTS (MESUPES-arm and MESUPES-hand) MOTOR EVALUATION SCALE FOR UPPER EXTREMITY IN STROKE PATIENTS (MESUPES-arm and MESUPES-hand) Name patient: Test date - hour: Name examiner: Duration of the test: min Handedness: right/left Support sitting

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) FOCUSED QUESTION For stroke patients, in what ways does robot-assisted therapy improve upper extremity performance in the areas of motor impairment, muscle power, and strength?

More information

Development of goal-directed gaze shift based on predictive learning

Development of goal-directed gaze shift based on predictive learning 4th International Conference on Development and Learning and on Epigenetic Robotics October 13-16, 2014. Palazzo Ducale, Genoa, Italy WePP.1 Development of goal-directed gaze shift based on predictive

More information

functional MRI everything you always wanted to know, but never dared to MD PhD

functional MRI everything you always wanted to know, but never dared to MD PhD functional MRI everything you always wanted to know, but never dared to ask @MarionSmits, MD PhD Associate Professor of Neuroradiology Dept. of Radiology, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam (NL) Honorary Consultant

More information

Announcements. Exam 1. VII. Imaging techniques of the brain. Anatomical/Structural Scans. Structural Scans: CT. Structural Scans: CT 2/17/2014

Announcements. Exam 1. VII. Imaging techniques of the brain. Anatomical/Structural Scans. Structural Scans: CT. Structural Scans: CT 2/17/2014 Exam 1 None at the moment! Announcements Mean 78.0% Median 80% Mode 86% Min 26% Max 98% Std Dev 12.6% VII. Imaging techniques of the brain A. CT: anatomical B. MRI: anatomical C. fmri: functional D. SPECT

More information

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. Cortical Organization Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. 2. Secondary cortex: located immediately adjacent to primary cortical areas,

More information

The effects of sensory deprivation on sensory processing. Ione Fine, University of Washington

The effects of sensory deprivation on sensory processing. Ione Fine, University of Washington The effects of sensory deprivation on sensory processing Ione Fine, University of Washington Molyneux s question "Suppose a man born blind, and now adult, and taught by his touch to distinguish between

More information

Configural Response Learning: The Acquisition of a Nonpredictive Motor Skill

Configural Response Learning: The Acquisition of a Nonpredictive Motor Skill Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2007, Vol. 33, No. 6, 1451 1467 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1451

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Based on: Chapter 18 and 19, Breedlove, Watson, Rosenzweig, 6e/7e. Cerebral Cortex Brain s most complex area with billions of neurons and

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Fani Deligianni B. A. Wandell, et al. Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex, Neuron, 56(2):366-383, 2007 Retinotopy Visual Cortex organised in visual field maps: Nearby neurons have

More information

The role of visual feedback of hand position in the control of manual prehension

The role of visual feedback of hand position in the control of manual prehension Exp Brain Res (1999) 125:281 286 Springer-Verlag 1999 RESEARCH ARTICLE J.D. Connolly M.A. Goodale The role of visual feedback of hand position in the control of manual prehension Received: 3 July 1998

More information

NATURAL DEVELOPMENT AND TRAINABILITY OF PLYOMETRIC ABILITY DURING CHILDHOOD BY KIRSTY QUERL SPORT SCIENTIST STRENGTH AND CONDITIONING COACH

NATURAL DEVELOPMENT AND TRAINABILITY OF PLYOMETRIC ABILITY DURING CHILDHOOD BY KIRSTY QUERL SPORT SCIENTIST STRENGTH AND CONDITIONING COACH NATURAL DEVELOPMENT AND TRAINABILITY OF PLYOMETRIC ABILITY DURING CHILDHOOD BY KIRSTY QUERL SPORT SCIENTIST STRENGTH AND CONDITIONING COACH The truth... Youth are not as active as they used to be, Decline

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) What happens beyond the retina? What happens in Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)- 90% flow Visual cortex Information Flow Superior colliculus 10% flow Slide 2 Information

More information

Principals of Object Perception

Principals of Object Perception Principals of Object Perception Elizabeth S. Spelke COGNITIVE SCIENCE 14, 29-56 (1990) Cornell University Summary Infants perceive object by analyzing tree-dimensional surface arrangements and motions.

More information

Wetware: The Biological Basis of Intellectual Giftedness

Wetware: The Biological Basis of Intellectual Giftedness Wetware: The Biological Basis of Intellectual Giftedness Why is "giftedness" such a puzzle for parents? Why is there so much confusion? The most common plea heard on TAGFAM is "my child is different; please

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information