The Organization of Connections between Areas V5 and V2 in Macaque Monkey Visual Cortex

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Organization of Connections between Areas V5 and V2 in Macaque Monkey Visual Cortex"

Transcription

1 European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 1, No. 4 0 European Neuroscience Association W89 $3.00 The Organization of Connections between Areas V5 and V2 in Macaque Monkey Visual Cortex S. Shipp and S. Zeki Department of Anatomy and Developmental Biology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1 E 6BT Key words: owl monkey, stripes, re-entry, integration Abstract Area V2 of the cerebral cortex of higher primates has a complex cytochrome oxidase architecture whose most characteristic element is a set of stripes running orihogonal to its long axis. These stripes can be related to the segregation between the various pathways in which V2 participates. In the macaque monkey the more metabolically active stripes are alternately thick and thin and only one set, the thick stripes, is found to possess clusters of labelled cells following injections of horseradish peroxidase - wheatgerm agglutinin into area V5. Some of these clusters, but not all, coincide with substructures inside the thick stripes. V2 of the owl monkey has a similar organization except that the diversification into thick and thin stripes is less prominent, both in terms of their appearance and in that more than every alternate stripe is connected to area MT, the likely homologue of V5. The return projection from V5 to V2 is more widespread than the origin of the forward projection. It extends not only between the clusters of V5-efferent cells within the thick stripes but also across the intervening thin stripes and less active interstripes. Because the latter subserve functions different from those of the thick stripes it would seem that their receipt of a back projection from an area to which they do not project, V5, may be relevant to the process of intergration of signals relating to different attributes of vision. Introduction The territory adjoining the primary visual cortex (V 1) of higher primates was divided, historically, into just two concentric regions, areas 18 and 19 (Brodmann, 1905). The sharp architectonic discontinuity between V1 (area 17) and these areas led Brodmann to believe that the latter were non-visual in function (Brodmann, 1909, in von Bonin, 1960). But evidence to the contrary (e.g. Campbell, 1905) promoted the subsequent growth of hierarchical ideas of visual function in the cortex. Only relatively recently has the complementary parallel element in cortical information processing gained a wider acceptance. This is due, in a large part, to the demonstration that Brodmann s areas 18 and 19 are in fact composed of a much larger number of areas, most of which receive independent inputs from V1 and show some form of functional specialization (Zeki, 1975, 1978a; Baker et al., 1981). Thus V1, as the gateway to the cortex, distributes a number of different signals for further analysis. The first of the receiving areas, V2, however, shares a number of characteristics with V1. V2 was initially identified on the basis of the topographically organized projections that it receives from V 1 (Cragg, 1969; Zeki, 1969) and was later shown to project to all the same specialized areas as V1, including areas V3, V3A, V4 and V5 (Zeki, 1971, 1978b; Rockland and Pandya, 1979). Furthermore it possesses an equally heterogeneous population of functional cell types, including cells selective for wavelength, orientation, retinal disparity, or direction of movement (Baizer et al., 1977; Poggio and Fischer, 1977; Zeki, 1978d; Burkhalter and Van Essen, 1986). And thirdly it has a modular substructure made evident by staining it for the metabolic enzyme cytochrome oxidase (Livingstone and Hubel, 1982; Tootell et al., 1983). As with V1 groupings of cells with common preferences are found to coincide with the cytochrome oxidase architecture (Shipp and Zeki, 1985; Hubel and Livingstone, 1985, 1987; DeYoe and Van Essen, 1985). In V2 this takes the form of alternate thick and thin dark stripes separated by paler staining interstripes. The functional segregation in both V1 and V2 is also reflected in the specific pattern of connections between them, the thin stripes of V2 receiving from the blobs of Vl, the interstripes from the interblobs and the thick stripes from layer 4B (Livingstone and Hubel, 1984, 1987a). In the companion paper (Shipp and Zeki, 1989) we show that V5 makes connections with punctate patches of layer 4B whose disposition strongly resembles that of the blobs. But despite this superficial similarity the two systems bear no fixed relationship to each other. For V2, however, the connections made with V5 are related to the cytochrome oxidase pattern and are found to be concentrated in one set of stripes, the thick stripes; similarly the connections between V2 and V4 are concentrated within the thin stripes and interstripes (Shipp and Zeki, 1985; DeYoe and Van Essen, 1985). Here we present in greater detail the anatomy of the connections of the thick stripes with Correspondence to: S. Shipp and S. Zeki, as above Received 9 June 1988, revised 9 December 1988, accepted 24 January 1989

2 334 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 V5 and examine the variability of the patterns of connection and of the cytochrome oxidase stripes themselves. In addition we have studied the connections of owl monkey area MT with V2 and show that, on an anatomical basis at least, alternation of character of the darkly staining stripes in owl monkey V2 is a much less regular feature than it is in the macaque. Thus the segregation between the pathways ascending through V:! to diverge to different destinations suggests that V2, like Vl, plays a role in the early distributive functions of the cortical visual system. But the segregation between the return projections to V2, like that from V5 which we describe here, does not appear to be as strict. Though concentrated in the thick stripes the return projection from V5 als, invades the thin stripes and interstripes, hence departing from a strict1 y reciprocal arrangement. And, in forming a link between different1 y specialized pathways, one of the functions of this reciprocal asymmetry may be integrative in character-one of a wider set of systems fcr assembling signals relating to different attributes (Zeki and Shipp, 1988). FIG. I. Lateral view of macaque cerebral cortex after removal of the occipital operculum. The dissection is performed by cutting through the fundi of the hate (LS), inferior occipital (10.3) and calcarine (CS) sulci and by making short cuts across the cortex between these sulci (shown in etched lines). The exterior surface of the dissected operculum consists mostly of V 1, apart from a narrow strip of V2 along its dorsal rim. The interior surface (illustrated) is composed of a triangular portion of V1 from the 'roof' of the CS, flanked by V2 from the posterior banks of the LS and 10s. Materials and Methods This report is based on the same set of injections of horseradish peroxidase - wheatgerm agglutinin (HRP-WGA) into area V5 of the macaque and area MT of the owl monkey described in the preceding paper (Shipp and Zeki, 1989) and involves similar techniques. In brief, the brains were sectioned in the horizontal plane and at least 1 in 6 sections were stained for HRP by the method of Mesulam (1982) or for cytochrome oxidase by the method of Wong-Riley (1979). In tuo FIG. 2. The pattern of activity of cytochrome oxidase in area V2 of the macaque as visualized in tangential sections through the flatmounted occipital operculum. A-D are from the inferior occipital sulcus and E-H from the lunate sulcus. A-D have been rotated through 180" such that the fundus lies to the bottom, medial to the left and lateral to the right. For E-H medial is rightward and lateral leftward. All are to the same scale; the scale bar. in A. is 5 rnm.

3 macaques the occipital operculum was removed prior to cutting the rest of the brain. It was then flattened and sectioned in the tangential plane (see Fig. I). Of the three owl monkey brains, one was sectioned horizontally and one in a plane intermediate between horizontal and coronal (roughly parallel to the line of the sylvian sulcus). The occipital and temporal cortex of the third brain was flatmounted and sectioned tangentially. Some owl monkey sections were also stained for myelin by the method of Gallyas (1979). The relationship between the patterns of cytochrome oxidase activity and HRP labelling was studied by making superimposed diagrams of adjacent sections. Normally the outline and blood vessels of the cytochrome oxidase section were drawn first (from the image provided by a conventional slide projector) and labelled cells and terminals superimposed via the drawing tube of the microscope, numerous small adjustments in the relative alignment being made so as to obtain the best fit between the blood vessels in each vicinity (the method of local adjustment ). Diagrams of horizontal sections were further used to obtain an impression of the patterns of distribution in the tangential plane by means of computer-aided reconstructions. Results Cortical connections between V5 and V2 335 Connections between V5 and V2 in the nurcaque The cytochrome oxidase architecture of V2. The pattern revealed by staining for cytochrome oxidase in V2 consists essentially of a series of stripes which run in a direction orthogonal to the VI/V2 border. The striped pattern can vary substantially from animal to animal but normally the cytochrome oxidase dense stripes are alternately thick and thin and separated by paler zones, interstripes, of more regular width (Tootell er al., 1983, 1985; Livingstone and Hubel, 1982, 1984; Horton, 1984). Previous detailed descriptions of the stripes have concentrated on the New World owl and squirrel monkeys. In the macaque the pattern is qualitatively similar but the widths of the stripes, and consequently the overall periodicity, are both greater. The flatmounted operculum preparation (Fig. 1) proved especially suitable for studying the stripes in more detail, giving, as it does, sections which pass tangentially through the cortex of V2 buried within the posterior banks of both the hate sulcus (LS) and inferior occipital sulcus (10s). Figure 2 shows a number of examples of the classic pattern and FIG. 3. The radial organization of labelled cells and terminals in macaque area V2, following an injection of HRP-WGA into area V5, in relation to the cytoarchitecture and cytochrome oxidase architecture of V2. (A)Cross-polarized photomicrograph illustrating a typical cluster of labelled cells and a typical column of labelled axonal terminals. (B) Bright-field photomicrograph of a column of label similar to A taken from a section counterstained for Nissl substance. Note that the majority of labelled cells occur in layer 3, just above the granular layer. (C) Photomicrograph of the section adjacent to B stained for cytochrome oxidase and counterstained for Nissl substance. Note that the region staining most densely for cytochrome oxidase (part of a thick stripe.) is mainly in layer 3 but just invades layer 4, the granular layer. The scale bar is 200 pm.

4 336 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 variations which involve branching or other irregularities. One cycle of stripes consists of a thick stripe, a thin stripe and two interstripes. The alternation of thick and thin proved sufficiently regular to extend uninterrupted for at least three cycles and sometimes up to five (e.g Fig. 2B) within both the LS and 10s of most cases examined. Elements of the striped pattern not readily identifiable in one section were often easier to identify in the adjacent section or one nearby. To estimate the mean length of a cycle (the perpendicular distance from one thin stripe to the next, or one thick stripe to the next) we measured 47 cycles from 15 hemispheres in the LS and 30 cycles from 12 hemispheres in the 10s. Cycles were measured in groups of 2 to 4 in regions where the pattern of alternation was clear and regular and all the stripes near parallel to each other. All the animals weighed between 3 and 4 kg. The result obtained was a cycle length of 4.0 rnm (s.d. = 0.3 mm) in the LS and 3.8 mm (s.d. = 0.5) in the 10s. Assuming a shrinkage of 16% during perfusion and histological treatment, the mean cycle length in the brain in vivo is about 4.7 mm The relative widths of the stripes also vary somewhat and are not simple to determine because the exact placement of their borders is a little arbitrary. An approximate mean ratio for the widths of the stripes is 2: 1: 1.5 (thick:thin:inter). The identification of thick and thin stripe!, becomes questionable where they assume a more uniform width, though a provisional identification, based on the principle that thick and thin stripes alternate, is often possible provided that there are at least somc: stripes present which can be identified with confidence. Irregularities in the striped pattern more severe than variation in strip: width are relatively common. The medial end of the LS is especially prone to disruption: all four examples in Figure 2 (E-H), for instance, show some form of irregularity. Irregularities are most commonly duc: to stripes of the same or different type bifurcating, or coalescing diffusely, though more than one interpretation is often possible. The least ambiguous examples of branching we have observed are illustrated in Figure 2C and D where, in both cases, the most laterally located thin stripe within the 10s bifurcates in passing from the fundus to thl: lip of the sulcus. A similar pattern may be present in Figure 2G; but in this case it looks as if a thick stripe is generated in between the fork of the thin stripe, thus restoring the regular pattern of alternation. Less frequently, two adjacent stripes are of the same type and run parallel to each other without joining, the remainder of the pattern on either side showing the normal regular sequence of alternation: the two clearest examples we have observed both involved a pair of thin stripes in the 10s. Also notable is the variability in the substructure of thc stripes. Some are relatively uniform in their staining density (e.g. Fig. 2B and D) whereas in others heavily staining regions alternat? with regions where the staining density is no greater than that of th: interstripe-which itself is not always uniform (e.g. Fig. 2F, G and H). Finally, in the most irregular cases, the whole striped organization can break down, leaving an array of densely staining blotches several mm wide which do not align in any notable fashion. Correlation of the VS-efferent zones with cytochrome oxidase architecture. (i) Radial organization. Following injections of HRP-WGA into V5, transported label in V2 appears in isolated dense columns, normally spaced 3-4 mm apart but occasionally in pairs within several FIG. 4. The disposition of regions of V2 connected to V5, in relation to the cytochrome oxidase architecture of V2, as seen at low power in horizontal sections passing through the medial part of the 10s. (A) Dark-field photomicrograph of a section processed for HRP. There art: five major groups of labelled cells and terminals, three of which clearly possess subclusters. (B) The adjacent section stained for cytochrome oxidase. Striate cortex, on the external surface, is distinguished by the densely staining layer 4C and thc remainder of the section that is shown is all V2. The plane of section is near radial, which is not advantageous for revealing a regular set of stripes of alternating thickness as smaller scale fluctuations in staining density tend to become more prominent. (C) Adjusted superimposition of A and B. The heavy continuous and heavy dashed lines are respectively the outlines of the pial surface and the base of layer 6 from the cytochrome oxidase section. Lighter lines represent the outlines of relatively densely staining regions; broken outlines indicate regions whos,: contrast with respect to the background was only very slight. Coarse and fine stipple represent labelled cells and terminals repectively. A few of the bloai vessels by which the two sections were aligned (as usual with adjustment from region to region to achieve optimal registration) are also included; those with continuous outline are from the cytochrome oxidase section and those with dotted outline are from the HRP section. The scale bar is 1 mm.

5 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 337 B FIG. 5 (pan I). (A) Posterior view of a right cerebral hemisphere from which the occipital operculum has been removed. The dashed line follows the course of the lip of the hate sulcus. The rectangular outline indicates the approximate limits of the region reconstructed within the anterior bank of the 10s; this region was inverted to form the top half of the reconstruction. (B) The interior surface of an occipital operculum from a right hemisphere, viewed posteriorly as if the external surface were transparent. The rectangular outline indicates the approximate limits of the reconstructed region within the posterior bank of the IOS, which forms the lower half of the reconstruction * _ _- * _ FIG. 5 (pan 2). Reconstruction, from 34 diagrams such as Figure 4C, of the joint distribution of cytochrome oxidase activity and of labelled layer 3 cells in macaque area V2 following an injection of HRP-WGA into area V5 (case LV73). The reconstruction is roughly symmetrical about the fundus of the 10s with the lower part representing the posterior bank and the upper part the anterior bank; a fold through its centre parallel to the straightened sections would allow the two banks to be placed in apposition roughly restoring their arrangement in the intact brain. Cytochrome oxidase is represented by broken lines and labelled cells by rows of dots (with each dot corresponding to many cells). The dots derived from each HRP section have been duplicated on either side of the contour line of the adjacent cytochrome oxidase section for greater clarity of appearance. Where the dots are more closely spaced labelled cells were present in greater numbers; four different density levels are shown but the variation in density was greater than the reconstruction conveys. No attempt has been made to represent more than two levels of cytochrome oxidase activity. A reasonably coherent pattern of stripes cmcrges from the somewhat fragmentary distribution of cytochrome oxidase activity seen in the individual sections. Some stripes are obviously thicker than others and over most of the reconstruction a regular sequence of alternation can be discerned. Within this region almost all labelled cells coincide with thick stripes. The stxiped organization is least clear in the most lateral portion of the reconstruction (rightward on the figure), where the stripes appear to be branching, but there are few labelled cells in this vicinity. The sections are spaced vertically at the same scale as their horizontal magnification; the scale bar is 2 mm.

6 338 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 hundred microns of each other (Figs. 3A and 4A). In agreement with previous reports (Lund et al., 1981; Maunsell and Van Essen, 1983a; Ungerleider and Desimone, 1986b) labelled cells occur principally in layer 3B, with a few in layers 2, 3A and 5. Most labelled cells, in the upper layers at least, are identifiably pyramidal. Terminal label is heaviest in layers 1 and 6, and diminishes in density towards layer 4 (Maunsell and Van Essen, 1983a; Ungerleider and Desimone, 1986b). Layer 4 itself is normally free from terminal label but not invariably so. The basic laminar organization of cytochrome oxidase activity is simply stated: layers 2, 3 and the upper part of layer 4 stain moderately densely, and the remainder more lightly. This is evident in Figures 3C and 4B and confirms previous reports (Tootell et al., 1983; Wong- Riley and Carroll, 1984). Added to this basic pattern there is a tendency for all layers within thick or thin stripes to stain more densely. The contrast between stripe and interstripe is most marked in layers 3 and 4 and least, if detectable at all, in layer 1. Thick stripes and thin stripes commonly stain equally heavily, except in some instances where the thin stripes are notably more dense in the upper layers (e.g. Fig. 7C). Superimposition of sections such as those illustrated in Figure 3A and B shows that the columns of HRP label coincide with regions staining densely for cytochrome oxidase (Fig. 3C). The supragranular labelled cells fall within the most heavily stained laminae, the deepest still slightly superficial to the base of this zone. Labelled terminals by contrast are most prevalent in layers 1, 5 and 6, the very layers where cytochrome oxidase activity is weakest. (ii) Tangential organization. The conventional horizonal sections illustrated in Figure 4 give only a poor impression of the alternation between thick and thin stripes in the cytochrome oxidase architecture. Indeed it is not always possible to distinguish stripe from interstripe in such sections. In order to establish the two-dimensional relationship between the stripes and the V5-efferent zones we reconstructed their appearance within the tangential plane from a series of superimposed diagrams like that of Figure 4C and sought to confirm the outcome by injecting the CentraVparacentral visual field representation in V5 with the intention of flatmounting the occipital operculum. A number of features emerge from the reconstruction (Fig. 5) which are not evident in its component sections. Firstly the labelled cells coincide, on a regular basis, only with every alternate stripe. Secondly, these cells are separated into clusters along the length of the stripe, labelled zones alternating with sparsely labelled or unlabelled zones. Thirdly, on closer inspection, there is an alternating sequence of thick and thin stripes, with the great majority of labelled cells occurring within thick stripes. These features were confirmed by the two flatmounted cases illustrated in Figures 6 and 7. Figure 6 shows two neighbouring thick stripes in the LS, each of which contains several distinct clusters of labelled cells. Figure 7 shows a series of four bands of label from the 10s of the other case; each of these clearly coincides with a thick stripe, though a number of the more minor details of the cytochrome oxidase pattern in this sulcus are open to interpretation (see legend to Fig. 7). Several patches of label seem to extend beyond the boundaries of the stripes in the adjacent section, though this may merely reflect the difficulty in determining the stripe boundaries with an accuracy better than a few hundred microns. Indeed it is possible that, functionally, the boundaries of the stripes are as diffuse as they appear to be in the cytochrome oxidase stain. It is perhaps more important to note that we have yet to observe a cluster of V5-efferent cells that is centred on an interstripe. We have, by contrast, observed occasional small clusters of cells situated in thin stripes (e.g. Fig. 5); these were not present in all animals, were infrequent when present, and probably involved far less than 1% of all labelled cells in V2. An alternative source of the label in the thin stripes would be V4 (Shipp and Zeki, 1985; DeYoe and Van Essen, 1985). But only one V5 injection was situated sufficiently laterally within the STS for involvement of the V4 complex to be at all likely, and it produced no trace of labelled cells in the thin stripes (Fig. 6). One or two of the other injections may have exceeded the limits of V5 more medially (see the accompanying paper, Shipp and Zeki, 1989). But V2 may not be connected with this region of cortex (Ungerleider and Desimone, 1986a) and, if it is, the source of the output must be similar to that leading to V5 itself. (iii) Clustering within thick stripes. The two neighbouring thick stripe!, shown in Figure 6 contained patterns of clusters of labelled cells anc terminals that were remarkably similar. Superimposition of severa. sections revealed that one pattern consisted of five elements and the other of four, lacking a corresponding cluster of label in just onr: location. The clusters were separated by unlabelled or at best sparsel), labelled zones both across the width of the stripe as well as along its length. Multiple clusters can also be noted in the reconstructed cast: (Fig. 5), though nowhere is there such a regular repetition of the samr: substructure within a stripe. Only one thick stripe, in the posterior bank of the 10s (lower half of the reconstruction as shown in Fig. 5). containi a pair of elements separated across the width of the stripe (the same pair of clusters can be seen in Fig. 4A). One of these is at least 3 mni in length. All the other labelled stripes show signs of clustering along their lengths, the best example being a stripe in the anterior bank (top half of Fig. 5 reconstruction) in which there are four separate clusters. The distance along the stripe from the centre of the first cluster to the fourth is 6 mm, giving a repeat distance of about 2 mm. This is slightly greater than the repeat distance obtainable from Figure 6, which is about 1.5 mm. Further examples of clustering, at various spacings, can be seen in Figure 8, again from the 10s. In a number of instances (described in more detail in the legends to Figs. 6 and 7) the individual clusters of HRP label coincide with regions within the thick stripes that stain relatively heavily for cytochrome oxidase. We believe that these coincidences are not due to reaction of the HRP with the cytochrome oxidase incubation medium because not all areas with HRP activity stain heavily for cytochrome oxidase. Such areas include patches of label in layer 1 and the injection site itself. Also the outlines of the regions of high HRP and high cytochrome oxidase activity often fail to match each other exactly in regions of close correspondence. There might thus be a correlation between cytochrome oxidase activity and the pattern of cortico-cortical connectivity at the substrip? level. But the variation between individuals or between different parts of V2 in the same hemisphere (e.g. LS vs. IOS), both in the pattern of connections and in cytochrome oxidase architecture, precludes 3 more definitive statement about the substructure of thick stripes. Reciprocal and non-reciprocal features of the back projection from V5 to V2. Following all our injections of HRP-WGA into V5 the heavie,;t deposits of terminal label were consistently found within thick stripe:;. As with V1, however, the back projection is more diffusely spread than the origins of the forward projection in both topographical and functional terms, a departure from strict reciprocity which we refer to as reciprocal asymmetry (Shipp and Zeki, 1989). Collateral termind

7 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 339

8 340 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 label from retrogradely filed cells can potentially intermingle with the direct orthograde terminal label of the back projection, but two considerations can afford some distinction between them. Firstly, intrinsic connections in V2 have a limited range, which has not been demonstrated to exceed 3-4 mm in the macaque (Livingstone and Hubel, 1984; Rockland, 1985), though a range of up to 6 nun has been found in the squirrel monkey (Tigges et a/., 1974; Colonnier and Sas, 1978; Cusick and Kaas, 1988). Secondly, the laminar distribution of intrinsic connections is rather diffuse, perhaps heaviest in layers 3 and 5, but involving all layers (Valverde, 1978; Colonnier and Sas, 1978; Lund et al., 1981; Rockland, 1985). Towards the fringes of the labelled region in V2 we commonly observed patches of terminal label in the upper layers, which were accompanied by relatively few labelled cells or by none at all. A good example is provided by the sequence of five labelled thick stripes illustrated in Figure 7. All of these stripes contained moderately dense patches of terminal label but only the two most medial thick stripes contained large numbers of labelled cells; the remainder displayed little or no cell labelling. Hence, by the arguments above, the majority of this terminal label in the lateral three thick stripes must represent the back projection from V5. The importance of this observation is that in contacting the lateral three thick stripes the back projection from the injected portion of V5 is essentially non-reciprocal, since these three stripes make little or no contribution to the forward projection to this part of v5. In terms of visual topography the distribution of labelled cells in this case was much as expected given the distribution observed in VI (see Fig. 1, case SP3, of Shipp and Zeki, 1989)-a region straddling the horizontal meridian representation between eccentricities of approximately 4" and 10". That is, because of the nature of the split representation of the horizontal meridian in V2 (Cragg, 1969; Zeki, 1969; Allman and Kaas, 1974a; Gattass et a/., 1981), labelled cells were found toward the medial ends of both the LS and IOS, but only close to the fundus of the sulci, nearby the horizontal meridian representation forming the border with V3. The back projection from V5, in extending (a) more laterally and (b) further towards the lip of the IOS, succeeds in influencing cortex concerned with visual locations closer to the fovea and closer to the vertical meridian respectively. The back projection is not only more widespread than the origin of the forward projection in visuotopic terms but also in functional terms: though concentrated within the thick stripes it also contacts stripes of other types lying in between. In our more sensitively stained HRP material we observed, in the upper layers, occasional faint patches of labelled terminals in the thin stripes but not in the interstripes (Fig. 7D). These patches were too faint to be visible in a low power darkfield micrograph. In layers 5 and 6, and sometimes layer 1, labelled terminals were more diffusely distributed, encompassing both thin stripes and interstripes such that parts of these layers were continuously labelled throughout a cycle of stripes. An example is shown in Figure 8, a reconstruction of the distribution of labelled cells in layer 3 superimposed upon the distribution of terminals in layer 6, taken from a series of conventional horizontal sections through the posterior bank of the 10s. The reconstruction involves six (presumed) thick stripes; between three of these there are two relatively fainter concentrations of terminal label which might coincide with thin stripes. We are uncertain because the adjacent cytochrome oxidase stained sections were not easily plotted for the purpose of reconstruction. Figure 9 shows part of one of the sections from which the reconstruction is derived. Examination of sections from an animal with an injection of 3H proline and 3H leucine into V5 provided a rather different picture, illustrated in Figure 10. Terminal label in V2 is much more diffuse in comparison with the HRP material, being continuously distributed in all layers except layer 4 where it is more or less absent. Heavier patches of label, doubtless corresponding to thick stripes, are evident but the difference between thick stripes and the others is less marked. Although the autoradiographic example involved the posterior bank of the parieto-occipital sulcus (POS), a different region of V2 to that illustrated in Figures 8 and 9, we doubt that the difference in labelling reflects correspondingly different patterns of connectivity of V2 in the 10s and POS. This is because all of our HRP material has yielded qualitatively similar distributions of terminal label, including one case which also involved V2 in the POS. Since the autoradiographic label in layer 4B of V1 is also more diffuse than that produced by HRP- WGA (Shipp and Zeki, 1989) we suspect that certain technical factors. yet to be resolved, are more likely to be responsible for the dissimilarity Hence the autoradiographic case serves to emphasize a non-reciprocal characteristic that is apparent, if less obvious, in the HRP material too: that the back projection from an area specifically sensitive to motion (V5) can influence subregions of a second area (V2) which arc: differently specialized from the first and which do not provide it with an input. Connections of area MT with extrastriate cortex in the owl monkey We carried out a parallel investigation of the connections of MT in the owl monkey. Two animals were given single injections of HRP- WGA into MT and sectioned conventionally and in a third, MT received multiple injections, following which the occipital cortex was dissected for flatmounting. Though many features of the organization of th,? connections with V2 were similar to those observed in the macaque, we were unable to arrive at an equally succinct conclusion concernin:: the relationship of MT-efferent regions to the cytochrome oxidase architecture of V2, principally because the latter is frequently irregular. Due to the largely lissencephalic nature of the cerebral cortex of the owl monkey, the reconstructions and flatmounts presented to show the distribution of label in V2 also extend further to reveal connections FIG 6. The distibution of cytochrome oxidase activity and of HRP label following an injection into V5, in macaque area V2 as visualized in tangential section:. through the flatmounted occipital operculum (case LV97). (A) Section through the entire operculum to show the location of the enlarged region, the central and medial portions of the LS. (B) Dark-field photomicrograph of a section processed for HRP. (C) The adjacent section stained for cytochrome oxidase. (D) Adjusted superimposition of B and C. N signifies a thin stripe and K a thick stripe; other conventions are as for Figure 4C. All the clusters of labelled cells and terminals clearly fall within thick stripes and, in this case, there is no discernible label in the intervening thin stripes or interstripes. There is a further degree of correspondence between these clusters and the more densely staining elements within the thick stripes. This is notable for all the clusters within the more medial of the two heavily labelled thick stripes (the leftward of the pair in the figure) and for at least two clusters in the more lateral thick stripe, though the activity of cytochrome oxidase in this stripe is more uniformly dense. In further sections two more thick stripes, lying on either side, were also found to contain patches of labelled cells and terminals. A faint patch of terminals corresponding to one of the latter is just visible to the right in B, once more coincidin;; with a relatively dense constituent of the stripe. The scale bar is 2 mm.

9 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 341

10 342 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 made by MT with a number of extrastriate areas beyond V2. In general we found that the areas involved, and the topographic and laminar distribution of label between them, conformed to the description previously given by Weller et al. (1984). Most of these connections were also patchily organized, and in all instances the patchiness was more evident in the upper layers than in the lower layers. However a detailed description of the tangential and laminar organization of the connections outside V2 is beyond the scope of this paper. Cytochrome oxidase architecture. The cytochrome oxidase architecture of owl monkey V2 has previously been described by Horton (1984) and Tootell et al. (1985). From these examples it is clear that the striped structure of V2 is considerably less regular than that observed in the macaque or even the more closely related squirrel monkey (Tootell et a[., 1983; Livingstone and Hubel, 1984). Individual stripes are less distinct and tend to fuse with a neighbour more frequently such that the intervening interstripe is vestigial in its appearance. This can be seen in the tangentially sectioned preparation in Figure 11 (from an animal which did not receive an injection of HRP-WGA). But in this example there is a more obvious sequence of alternation of thickness of the stripes than has previously been described in the owl monkey. The alternation of thick and thin stripes is here most evident in ventral V2, but disappears centrally only to re-emerge in dorsal V2, though not with equal clarity. The length of one cycle of stripes is 2.4 mm in ventral V2 and 2.8 mm in dorsal V2, agreeing with the figure of mm for the spacing between individual stripes reported by Tootell et al. (1985). Correcting for shrinkage, the mean length of a cycle of stripes in the brain in vivo is thus about 3 mm. The connections of MT with V2. Following injections of HRP-WGA into area MT labelled cells are distributed in V2 in much the same way as they are in V2 of the macaque following a V5 injection. Dense clusters are found at intervals in layer 3, with a few cells located more superficially in layer 2 and fewer still in layer 5 (Weller et al., 1984). Relative to the macaque, however, the clusters are more closely spaced and a little broader, such that a greater overall proportion of V2 is occupied by labelled cells. Labelled terminals were heaviest in layer 1, diminished in density toward layer 4, and were moderately dense in layers 5 and 6. The terminal label was densest immediately above and below the clusters of cells in layer 3, but the gaps between these columns were spanned by lighter concentrations of label. This was most obvious in layers 5 and 6 but could also be observed superficially, especially in layer 1. On reconstruction, the clusters of labelled cells were found to be aligned into bands running near perpendicular to the V1/V2 border. The example illustrated in Figure 12 shows five bands within V2 on the dorsal surface and medial convexity of the hemisphere. The densib of cell labelling along each band was not entirely uniform, bul longitudinal clustering was not as evident as in the similar reconstructiori carried out on the macaque (cf. Fig. 5). Some degree of clustering. across the width of the band was also seen in a few instances. More ventrally in V2 there were further sporadic groupings of cells which were too sparse or isolated to give any impression of a band-like structure. Sections from a 1 in 12 set stained for cytochrome oxidase wen: superimposed with those treated for HRP, as before. This revealed that every cluster of label coincided with a cytochrome oxidase density though, at this frequency, the reconstniction of entire stripes was 3 little incomplete. Somewhat surprisingly, however, there was littl: indication that only every alternate dense stripe is connected to MT: the two most dorsal bands of cells in Figure 12 appeared to be separated by an unlabelled stripe, but not the others. There was also no reliable sign that the stripes alternated in thickness. To assess the tangential organization of MT-efferent regions more directly we prepared an animal with three injections of HRP-WGA into roughly the central portion of MT and flatmounted its occipitid cortex. The flatmount preparation was carried out by a relatively simple method. Briefly the occipital and temporal lobes were dissected away from the remainder of the cortex and then divided transversely, one part containing cortex on the lateral surface, the other including cortex of the medial and ventral surfaces and all the cortex buried within the calcarine sulcus. Both were flattened and sectioned tangentially, alternate sections being stained for HRP, cytochrome oxidase, and myelin. Figure 13 summarizes the overall distribution of label that u'e obtained in this case. Perhaps the most prominent feature was the presence of at least seven irregularly shaped but distinct bands of labelled cells and terminals within V2, arranged over the medial, lateral, and ventral surfaces. Further clusters of label situated ventrally on the FIG. 7 The distribution of cytochrome oxidase activity and of HRP label following an injection into V5, in macaque area V2 as visualized in tangential sectiors through the flatmounted occipital operculum (case SP3). (B) Section through the entire operculum stained for cytochrome oxidase. (A and C-F) are enlargements of the IOS, rotated through nearly 180" such that the fundus lies towards the bottom of the picture with medial to the left and lateral to the right. (A and C) are adjacent sections predominantly through layer 2 processed for HRP and cytochrome oxidase respectively. (D) Is an adjusted superimposition of A and C. (E) Is a section stained for cytochrome oxidase predominantly through layers 3 and 4 where the demarcation of stripes is clearer. The majority of patches of HRP label in a section adjacent to E were too faint to register in a dark-field photomicrograph but their locations are marked in the superimposition F. Tt.e conventions in D and F are as for Figure 4. Both scale bars represent 2 mm. From F it is evident that at least four thick stripes contain patches of labelled terminals. One further stripe, lying at the lateral extremity of the section (rightmost in E) and beyond what appears to be an adjacent pair of thin stripes, is a probable fifth. This stripe is partially obliterated in C, but is coincident with a patch of labelled terminals in section A, shown in the superimposition D but too faint to register in a photomicrograph. More doubt concerns the extent of the most medial thick stripe (leftmost in the figure) which can only be traced two-thirds of the way from the lip of the sulcus to its fundus before seeming to fade awall. Slightly medial to this thick stripe and located at the fundus is a dense cluster of cells which, while not clearly belonging to the thick stripe, is neither clearly identifiable with a thin stripe nor an interstripe. The labelled terminals surrounding this cluster of cells just fail to be continuous with the cloud of terminals within the nearby thick stripe, being interrupted by a discontinuity due to curvature at the fundus (see A and D). One possible explanation of these observations is that some kind of stripe branching is taking place, distorting the normal regular pattern. Four of the labelled thick stripes contain at least two separate patches of labelled terminals. Some of these patches coincide with more densely staining elemerts within the cytochrome oxidase stripes (e.g. E and F, the top patch of terminals in the thick stripe second from the left) but others do not. Most impressive is the coincidence of a patch of terminals with a small densely staining zone that lies at the fundus between two thick stripes, perhaps belonging to the thin stripe above it (see E and F). Additional fainter patches of labelled terminals, not visible in the photomicrograph A but depicted in the superimposition D, also coincided with thin stripes.

11 Cortical connections between V5 and V FIG. 8. Reconstruction from serial horizontal sections of the distribution of labelled cells in layer 3 and of labelled terminals in layer 6 of macaque area V2 in the posterior bank of the 10s following an injection of HRP-WGA into area V5 (case SP19). Labelled cells are represented by large dots (each dot corresponding to a number of cells) and labelled terminals by fine stipple. Both are shown at four levels of density. The reconstruction is based on a I in 3 series of sections which run horizontally across the reconstruction; they are spaced vertically at the same scale as their horizontal magnification. The scale bar is 2 mm. The label derived from each section is expanded vertically so as to fill in the space corresponding to the 2 out of 3 sections omitted from the reconstruction. Medial is to the left, lateral to the right. FIG. 9. Dark-field photomicrographs of one of the sections reconstructed in Figure 8. (A) Shows three separate columns of label which correspond to the three bands lying to the left of Figure 8; the central column also appears in Figure 3A. (B) Is an enlargement of the rectangular region outlined in A. Although lying outside the thick stripes it still contains terminal label its upper- and lowermost layers. This distribution conforms to the pattern of a reciprocal projection from V5 but is unlike the pattern of intrinsic collateral label, which should also involve layers 2 to 5. The scale bar is 200 pm. lateral surface were also probably within V2 but some doubt must remain because the location of the anterior border of V2 could not be ascertained exactly. The appearance of the posterior parts of the lateral and ventro-medial surfaces under darkfield illumination (mainly reflecting the distribution of terminal label) is illustrated in Figure 14. Figure 15A shows the distribution of labelled cells in the same section as Figure 14A and Figure 15B a section 150 pm more superficial, in which the distribution of cells amongst two of the bands was a little more widespread. Part of the irregularity in the shape of the bands derives from the way they appear to deform around the outlines of more evenly shaped non-labelled zones, leading to the formation of occasional cross-links. The distribution of labelled cells within the bands

12 344 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 FIG. 10. The continuous distribution of labelled axonal terminals in V2, following an injection of tritiated proline and leucine into V5, as visualized in a coron:il section through the parieto-occipital sulcus (see Acknowledgements). No direct comparison with cytochrome oxidase stained tissue was available, but the ~ UO heavier concentrations of terminals very probably coincide with thick stripes, and the terminals distributed continuously around them must therefore invoh e both thin stripes and interstripes. Neighbouring sections showed a similar pattern over a range of several mm ruling out the unlikely possibility that all the lab.21 in this section corresponds to a single thick stripe running parallel to the plane of section. The scale bar is I mm. is relatively uniform; some signs of clustering can be detected, but again the phenomenom is much less prominent than in the macaque (cf. Fig. 6B). The large size of the injection in this animal made it possible that the crescent-shaped area surrounding MT, area DL (Allman and Kaas, 1974b), was also involved to some extent. Though we were unable to identify a reliable boundary for area MT from the myelin strains, the presence of weak labelling in inferotemporal cortex (see Fig. 13), known to be connected to DL (Weller and Kaas, 1985), also indicated that this was likely. A small fraction of the label in V2 may thus also have derived from DL (Kaas and Lin, 1977; Weller and Kaas, 1984). But, as demonstrated by the extent of labelling in V 1, the involvement of MT was massive by comparison and the label derived from DL could not have significantly influenced the overall distribution in V2. Similar considerations apply to the previous case, which also has a small amount of label in inferotemporal cortex (not shown in Fig. 12). The pattern of cytochrome oxidase staining in dorsal V2 of the flatmounted case was very irregular and 'blotchy' in appearance, in that the contrast in staining density between light and dark patches varied substantially from region to region. This was partly because the large cortical exposure made during injection impaired the perfusion of the cortex in its vicinity. Yet the pattern of staining on the ventral surface was also irregular, so we assumed that the smaller scale fluctuations of cytochrome oxidase activity revealed in the stain were of natural origin. Figure 16 affords a direct comparison between the patterns of HRP labelling and cytochrome staining on both the lateral and ventrill surfaces. Though neither has a pattern of cytochrome oxidase activity immediately suggestive of a set of stripes, on closer inspection it IS possible to discern a periodic variation in staining density along the axis parallel to the border with V1. Each of the four HRP bands cn the lateral surface and the two bands on the ventral surface seem to coincide, roughly, with zones staining relatively densely for cytochrome oxidase. Hence there is some likelihood that these zones do indeed correspond to the striped architecture that is normally more evident in cytochrome oxidase stained material. But it is less clear whethcr some of the smaller dense patches which lie between the bands of HRP label (indicated by arrowheads in Fig. 16) should also be regarded ;IS separate stripes, or perhaps as rogue, densely staining elements within interstripes. We had difficulty in identifying densities in the cytochrome oxidase stain corresponding to clusters of labelled cells in the third animal, but these bands of cells occurred in the monocular representation #of far peripheral visual field within the calcarine sulcus where cytochronie oxidase stripes may not be present (Tootell et ul., 1983, 1985). This was also true of one macaque where the involvement of V2 was very peripheral. The presence of bands of labelled cells in both cases would nonetheless suggest that some kind of segregation of function remains even in the absence of stripes. To summarize, our evidence shows that the regions of V2 connected with MT are distributed in a band-like fashion that correlates with the

13 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 345

14 346 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 cytochrome oxidase architecture of V2. Stripes with relatively high cytochrome oxidase activity are the major source of output to MT, but we cannot be certain whether this involves every dense stripe or every alternate dense stripe. By comparison with the macaque we would have expected the latter, but our evidence for the presence of alternate unlabelled dense stripes was weak. Isolated cytochrome oxidase dense patches which lacked label were observed in a number of instances, but in none of the animals was the overall pattern of cytochrome oxidase activity sufficiently resolved to allow positive identification of these patches as separate stripes in their own right. An alternative and more indirect line of reasoning is to consider the various periodicities we have observed. The four adjacent bands of MT-efferent cells in dorsal V2 of Figure 12 had a periodicity (mean centre-to-centre interband interval) of 1.5 mm and those of dorsal V2 in Figure 14A 1.7 mm. (Based on measurements in adjacent cytochrome oxidase stained sections, since the HRP sections undergo substantially greater shrinkage.) In dorsal V2 of a third animal (Fig. 11) the cytochrome oxidase stripes had a periodicity of 1.4 mm and alternation of thickness was also evident, such that one cycle of stripes occupied 2.8 mm. In the absence of more detailed knowledge of the variation associated with stripe-like periodicity in V2 of the owl monkey we note that, faced with a strict choice between the every stripe and every alternate stripe hypotheses, these figures would favour the former. Discussion Our chief finding is that the VS-efferent cells of V2 are mostly restricted to just one set of stripes, the thick stripes, whereas the back projection to V2 from V5 is more widely distributed and thus has the potential to influence not only the VS-efferent cells but also cells projecting elsewhere. We interpret these observations in the light of two contrasting ideas: the segregation and reintegration of information within the visual system. The former, which might equally be referred to as functional specialization (Zeki, 1978a), is a well attested phenomenon for which much evidence - anatomical, physiological, psychophysical, and clinical -has been amassed and whose value can be understood in terms of concepts such as efficiency of engineering. The concepts underlying the latter are less well founded. But the impetus for discussing intergration is a strong one deriving from everyday experience-that our visual perceptions bear little or no trace of the internal subdivisions of function from which they are derived. Hence some mechanism must ensure that the various attributes of a visual scene, such as its forms, colours, and motions, are correctly referred to each other in order to give a coherent and unified percept. Here it is our aim simply to present the idea that asymmetrically organized reciprocal connections, such as those we have observed between V5 and V2, might be a part of this mechanism. We begin, however, with a discussion of the more firmly rooted evidence supporting the ideas of segregation and specialization. Tangential segregation in V2 Like V1, V2 is involved with several visual attributes, but unlike VI the pathways subserving different attributes do not involve separate layers. Layer 3B of V2, for instance, in which most VS-efferent cells reside, is much like layer 4B of V1 in that it lies just above the granular layer and possesses a plexus of horizontally running fibres (Lund et al ). But layer 3B is not restricted to the motion submodality since it also houses cells projecting to V4 and V3 and perhaps elsewhere (Rockland and Pandya, 1979, 1981; Lund et al ; Fries and Zeki. 1983). Rather, groups of cells with different prestriate targets arc: segregated tangentially from each other and in a way which coincides with the striped cytochrome oxidase architecture of V2 (Shipp and Zeki. 1985; DeYoe and Van Essen, 1985). According to our past and present observations the great majority of VS-efferent cells are located within the thick stripes, whereas V4-efferent cells mostly avoid the thick stripe:; and occur in bands or clusters centred on thin stripes or interstripes. Our evidence so far also points to the thick stripes as being the major source of the output to V3, but we are as yet uncertain about the origin of the projection of V2 to a fourth prestriate area, V3A (Zeki, 1978b). Most instances where a departure from this scheme seems possible involve cases of irregularity in the pattern of cytochrome oxidase stripes making identification of a particular stripe uncertain. We havt: occasionally observed small clusters of VS-efferent cells situated within thin stripes and examples of bands of V4-efferent cells stretching across thick stripes, but in general, using HRP-WGA as a tracer of neural connectivity, V2 appears to maintain a high degree of segregation between its output to V4 and to V5. De Yoe and Van Essen (1985), while essentially in agreement with our results, report rather larger numbers of VS-efferent cells between the thick stripes. Some, at least, of this variance may be due to the different technical characteristics of the fluorescent dyes they employed as neuronal tracers. The three types of stripe defined by the cytochrome oxidase architecture of V2 show distinct differences in their physiologicel response selectivities, most of which correlate with what is known of the physiology of their immediate targets in prestriate cortex, areas V3, V4 and V5. We have found directionally selective cells only within the thick stripes and units with frank colour-opponent properties, all of which lacked orientation selectivity, only within thin stripes (Shipp FIG. 11. A tangential section through flatmounted occipital cortex of the owl monkey stained for cytochrome oxidase. The flatmount includes all of areas V1 and V2 lying on the dorsal and ventral surface of the hemisphere. Dorsal is to the top of the figure and posterior to the right. Most of the section passes througi layer 3 or 4. Three areas can be identified on the basis of their distinctive cytochrome oxidase architecture: V1 (lying posteriorly, with blobs); V2 (immediately anterior, with stripes); and MT (the oval-shaped area to the left with a reticular substructure). The remainder of the cortex anterior to V2 is far from uniform in its staining pattern, but there is no simple correspondence between the pattern and the outlines of areas determined by physiological studies. The anterior border of V2 itself is also none too clearly defined, though it can be placed to within a mm or so if taken as the anterior limit of the stripes running near orthogonal to the VllV2 border. So defined, V2, in this animal at least, is clearly narrower centrally than it is dorsally or ventrally, the width being about 2 mm centrally (immediately opposite MT) and up to 5 nun at the broadest point ventrally. The regularity of the striped architecture also varics substantially along the length of V2. Ventrally at least two cycles of an alternating sequence of thick and thin stripes are evident, but more centrally the pattern becomes disorganized with individual stripes running obliquely with respect to the V1/V2 border and being of indeterminate character. In dorsal cortex a plausible sequence of alternating thick and thin stripes re-emerges. The stripes are labelled K (for thick) and N (for thin) with lower case letters indicating that the identification is made purely according to the principle of alternation. One stripe is labelled nk to indicate that it may be a product of two stripes having fused; the pair of stripes labelled k and N immediately ventral to it also appear to be fused at the V1/V2 border, only to diverge as they run anteriorly. Measurement of the length of cortex associated with one cycle of stripes is made more difficult by the fact that the stripes do not run perfectly parallel to each other. But taking their perpendicular separation at the level of the VllV2 border yields a length of 4.8 mm for two cycles ventrally and 5.6 mm for two cycles dorsally.

15 ~ A Cortical connections between V5 and V ,- 361 / h/... / ::.: \ ts :i / FIG. 12. Reconstruction of the distribution of labelled cells in visual cortex on the lateral surface of the owl monkey brain following an injection of HRP-WGA into area MT (case FP1). (A) Lateral view of the cerebral cortex showing the plane of section and location of six illustrative sections. The reconstructed region was the lateral surface of the hemisphere, from the medial convexity up to the dotted line marked on the temporal lobe and from its posterior extremity to an anterior limit at section 361. In the reconstruction itself each horizontal level of dashes represents one section, of which there are 88 in total. Slightly longer dashes in V1 (at lower left) indicate a zone of relatively greater labelled cell density, but elsewhere differences in cell density are not shown. Continuous lines represent the full extent of the visible injection site and diamond symbols the location of the V1/V2 border taken from a 1 in 12 series of sections stained for cytochrome oxidase. The outlines at left, right and lower right represent the limits of the reconstructed area; the location of the fundus of the temporal sulcus (ts), just above the right-most patch of label, is also indicated. The numbers and arrowheads at far left and right indicate the levels of the 6 sections illustrated in A. The horizontal scale is appropriate for the cytochrome oxidase sections (which undergo less shrinkage than sections stained for HRP) and is at the same magnification as the vertical scale. The scale bar at lower right represents 2 mm in both axes. The sections were aligned with respect to each other by reference to their general outline and to subcortical features, since at 150 pm apart they could not be reliably aligned by matching blood vessels. Further small adjustments were made to ensure that the VI/V2 border was roughly linear. This procedure produced an approximately correct outline for the reconstruction but it is possible that some distortion in the form of shear is still present. The reconstruction reveals that at least five separate bands of labelled cells are present in V2, oriented roughly perpendicularly to the border with V1. Signs of patchy connections are also present in other regions, most notably in the ventral bank of the temporal sulcus. Not incorporated in the reconstruction are a few patches of label which occurred on the ventral surface of the temporal lobe, where the plane of section was tangential. and Zeki, 1985). The frequencies of these responses in the two types of stripe were approximately 20% and 50% respectively. Correspondingly, directional selectivity is very frequent, perhaps universal, in V5 (Zeki, 1974; Maunsell and Van Essen, 1983b; Albright, 1984) and substantial (40%) in V3 (Zek, 1978c; Felleman and Van Essen, 1987), areas to which the thick stripes project, whereas only about 10% of V4 cells are directionally selective (Zeki, 1978d; Desimone and Schein, 1987). Similarly V4, the target of the thin stripes, contains a much higher proportion of wavelength or colour selective units than either V3 or V5 (Zeki, 3978d; Desimone et al., 1985). We refer here only to V3 in the dorsal part of the hemisphere: lower V3 has been reported to differ significantly from upper V3 in its properties (Burkhalter and Van Essen, 1986) and we do not know the source of its input from V2. The results of Hubel and Livingstone (1985, 1987) are in accord with ours concerning the coincidence of wavelength selective cells, and cells lacking orientation selectivity, within the thin stripes. The study of DeYoe and Van Essen (1985) reported selectivity for wavelength and non-selectivity for orientation to be near equally prevalent in both thin stripes and interstripes; note, however, that they employed different criteria for assessing selectivity. The distribution of directionally selective units was not reported by Hubel and Livingstone, but DeYoe and Van Essen found them more frequently in thick stripes than thin stripes and most frequently amongst clusters of V5-efferent cells, again largely in accord with our own findings. The three types of stripe in V2 also appear to be distinguished by features in addition to those related to the detection of motion and colour. Both the reports of Hubel and Livingstone and of DeYoe and Van Essen agree, for instance, that cells selective for binocular disparity (a feature we ourselves did not examine) are relatively concentrated within the thick stripes. Furthermore Hubel and Livingstone report that the interstripes are characterized by an abundance of end-stopped

16 348 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 FIG. 13. Diagrams to illustrate the distribution of HRP label observed in flatmounts of the visual cortex from owl monkey FP6, following multiple injections into area MT. The upper diagram shows a tangential view of cortex on the lateral surface. and the middle diagram a tangential view of cortex on the medial antl ventral surfaces; the lower diagram is a section through the inner part of the medial flatmount which passes semi-tangentially through both banks of the calcarine sulcus (CS). WM is white matter. Cortex lying within the temporal sulcus (ts) and sylvian sulcus (ss) is indicated by a dashed outline; the VI/V2 border is marked by dashed lines, determined from adjacent sections stained for myelin or cytochrome oxidase: and the three injection sites and the limit of diffusion of HRP-WGA are shown by dark rings surrounded by a continuous border. Arrowheads indicate the approximate location of the anterior border of V2. assuming that V2 is about 4 mm wide (Allman and Kaas, 1974a). The distribution of both retrograde and orthograde label is depicted by stipple of variable density, antl represents a combination of the patterns observed in all sections passing through the supragranular layers. The label in V1 was distributed continuously betweeii the dorsal and ventro-medial surfaces but did not extend quite up to the posterior margins of the sections owing to some residual curvature of the cortical laminat: in this vicinity. It also extended into the upper bank of calcarine sulcus. The label in V2 occurred in seven bands, four on the lateral surface. one on the medial surface and two on the ventral surface. On the lateral surface these bands extended up to the anterior border of V2 and were continuous with the patchily distributed label occupying the areas beyond. DM and D1 and possibly DL. On the ventral surface the bands probably extend out of V2 into area VP. No architectural features could be found to reliably identify the borders of these areas, even within the myelin stained sections. Further patches of label were found between MT and the sylvian sulcus, nearby and inside the caudal end of the temporal sulcus. and more anteriorly on the ventral surface.

17 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 349 FIG. 14. Dark-field photomicrographs showing the distribution of HRP label in tangential sections through owl monkey visual cortex following multiple injections into area MT (case FP6). (A) Lateral occipital cortex. (B) Medial and ventral occipital cortex. Both sections pass through layer 4B in VI, where the distribution of label is more continuous than elsewhere but still displays signs of patchiness. Scale bars are each 2mm (measured from adjacent cytochrome oxidase sections). cells. But neither of these characteristics has been examined sufficiently extensively in V3, V4 and V5 to permit an effective comparison with these areas. Subdivision offinction within stripes. There are a number of indications that different functions are segregated within stripes as well as between them. Firstly the pattern of cytochrome oxidase activity within all three types of stripe is itself frequently blob-like or patchy, though it is also sufficiently irregular from individual to individual to discourage any attempt to generalize about the size or periodicities of the blobswithin-stripes (see Fig. 2). Secondly the cells efferent to V3, V4 and V5 all tend to fall into clusters along the lengths of their respective stripes and we have observed that at least some of the V5-efferent clusters coincide with periodic densities within the thick stripes. Also in the case of the thick stripes there is the possibility that zones connected to V3 might be segregated from zones connected to V5, a question to be resolved by a double label experiment. A third indication of functional heterogeneity derives from experiments utilizing 2-deoxyglucose. Tootell et al. (1983) demonstrated that low and high spatial frequency gratings (1 and 7 cycles per degree, respectively) presented at all orientations, gave rise to distinctly different patterns of activity. In particular the high frequency stimulus produced multiple isolated spots of activity, in between more elongated strip-like regions of activity whose periodicity suggested that they coincided either with thick stripes alone or thin stripes alone (Fig. 3B of Tootell et al., 1983). The spots must therefore have coincided with the interstripes and perhaps with the other set of dark stripes, suggesting the presence in them of clusters of cells selective for high spatial frequencies. Segregation offunction in owl monkey V2. The definition of stripes in the cytochrome oxidase architecture of V2 in the owl monkey is distinctly poorer than in other primates and the example we illustrate in Figure 11 is exceptional in the clarity with which alternate thick and thin stripes can be discerned. Like the macaque, MT-efferent cells in the owl monkey reside within the darkly staining stripes, but we

18 350 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 FIG 15 Diagrams illustrating the distribution of individual labelled cells in tangential sections through occipital cortex on the lateral surface following multipls injections of HRP-WGA into area MT of the owl monkey (case FP6). The diagram on the left is of the same section that is shown in Figure 14A; the sectioi illustrated to the right was located 150 pm more superficially (the two sections intervening were not treated for HRP). Arrowheads indicate the approximat: location of the anterior border of V2, assuming a width of about 4 mm (Allman and Kaas, 1974a). The scale is adjusted to the level of shrinkage in the accompanyin: cytochrome oxidase stained section. have no evidence that these are alternate thicker ones. Instead it seemed that most dark stripes within the labelled region of V2 contained labelled cells, perhaps with an occasional unlabelled stripe breaking the sequence. Hence segregation of different functions may be a less prominent feature in owl monkey V2, with alternate dark stripes possessing little or no diversification. Or, possibly, diversification takes place to differing extents in different individuals, perhaps even to different extents at different locations within V2 of a single individual. Owl monkeys possess rather poor colour vision (Jacobs, 1977) and perhaps the loss, effectively, of one of the specialist functions that in the macaque helps to differentiate between the stripes diminishes the developmental forces responsible for diversification. Yet the cytochrome oxidase blobs of V 1 are prominent and well defined in the owl monkey and if not involved with colour, the blob-to-stripe pathway may retain an involvement in greyness (lightness) constancy and possiby other tasks too (Allman, 1988). It would thus be useful to know whether the patterns of connection (a) between blobs and stripes and (b) between V2 and area DL, the likely homologue of V4 in the macaque (Weller and Kaas, 1985), also differ from those of the macaque. It may be noted that bands of labelled cells have been observed in V2 following an injection into the caudal part of area D1, (Weller and Kaas, 1985). No comparison with cytochrome oxidase stained tissue was available but, judging from their Figure 17, the bands of cells had a periodicity of just over 1 mm at the level of the V1/V2 border. This corresponds to the presence of at least two bands of labelled cells per cycle of stripes, implying that the bands could thus have coincided with every dark stripe or with every interstripe. Alternatively the rearrangement of the connections of V2 may not be consequent upon the loss of colour vision or unique to the owl monkey. Recent reports suggest that in the squirrel monkey, too, area MT receives input from every dark stripe (Krubitzer and Kaas, 198") and area DL mainly from the interstripes (Cusick and Kaas,

19 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 351 FIG 16. The distribution of cytochrome oxidase activity and of HRP label following multiple injections into area MT, in owl monkey area V2 as visualized in tangential sections through flatmounted occipital cortex (case FP6). (A) Darkfield view of a section through layer 3 of dorsal V2 on the lateral surface. The border with V1, marked by a linear, narrow concentration of label, is evident to the right. There is a clear series of 4 bands of label in V2, though anteriorly and especially within the lower half of the frame, the pattern becomes less regular. The exact location of the anterior border of V2 is not readily determinable. (B) The identical part of an adjacent section stained for cytochrome oxidase. An outline of the major concentrations of HRP label seen in A has been superimposed, but note that fainter traces of labelling are also present beyond this outline. The pattern of cytochrome oxidase activity in this case is very irregular, but a periodic variation in staining density along the axis parallel to the V1N2 border, roughly matching the periodicity of the HRP bands, is still apparent. Arrowheads point to smaller cytochrome oxidase densities lying between the HRP bands whose status is more equivocal. The scale bar in A, applicable to both A and B, is 2mm. (C) Dark-field view of an HRP section through V2 on the ventral surface. The plane of section is not perfectly tangential, as seen by the presence of white matter (WM) towards the top of the section. (D) The corresponding part of an adjacent section stained for cytochrome oxidase. The border between V1 and V2 is clearest where it passes through layer 4C of V 1, indicated by an arrow. The dotted outlines again indicate the location of the HRP label visible in C. The arrowhead points to an unlabelled cytochrome density lying between the two HRP bands. The scale bar in C, applicable to both C and D, is 2 mm. This is more surprising, since colour vision is better developed in this species (Jacobs, 1983) and a clear diversification of function between thick and thin stripes has been demonstrated physiologically (Hubel and Livingstone, 1985, 1987). Overall there is the possibilty that the segregation of pathways through V2 of New World monkeys shows some significant differences from that found in the Old World macaque. Integration Viewed within the context of the visual system as a whole the segregated connections of V2 we discuss above have been regarded (a) as extensions of the magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) divisions of the retino-geniculo-cortical pathway and (b) as the antecedents of two distinct streams of cortical areas leading respectively to the parietal and temporal lobes where the visual functions of motion/spatial awareness and object recognition respectively are emphasized (Mishkin et al., 1983; Van Essen and Manusell, 1983; Desimone et al., 1985; Livingstone and Hubel, 1987a,b). The essential tenet of this view is that these two pathways, though each themselves possessing elements

20 352 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 of a parallel organization, are two largely independent subdivisions of the visual system, segregated physically from each other from the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) onwards and specialized to provide different attributes of visual perception. However, in tracing the connections more centrally and in greater detail, one uncovers a substantial anatomical potential for interactions at all levels both within and between the broad M and P systems. Rather than interpret these cross-connections in a negative fashion as evidence for a blurring of specialization, it is preferable to consider specialization as first and foremost a property of an individual area (or other structure). The cross-connections can then be considered to be playing a significant integrative role, in one or both of the following respects: (a) in bringing together information of diverse types to be used for a common purpose and thus making, in fact, a useful contribution to specialization; (b) as providing the intercommunication necessary for a unified and coherent percept to arise out of the activities of separate specialized areas (Zeki and Shipp, 1988). Ascending pathways. Given that cross-connections can be identified in forward, backward, and lateral connections between cortical areas (Zeki and Shipp, 1988), can functions such as (a) and (b) above be associated with any type of connection in particular? Despite knowing little about the systematic physiological differences between these types of connection, some inference might be drawn from the fact that forward connections normally give rise to larger receptive fields whereas backward connections do not succeed in enlarging the excitatory (or classical) receptive fields of their target cells. Because the cells with larger receptive fields are normally capable of responding to stimulation within any small part of their field it would see that they are capable of being activated by a small proportion of their afferents active in isolation. If the same is true of afferents representing different visual attributes, as opposed to different regions in space, then the cell will be able to detect a feature by any one of a number of alternative means. To cast the argument in perceptual terms, take the example of the analysis of visual form. Cortical areas involved in this task could make use of information about contours and shapes derived from either luminance, colour or textural changes, retinal disparity, or relative motion. Information about structure in three dimensions is provided not only by disparity but also by velocity gradients and a variety of textural, perspective and shading cues (e.g. Cavanagh, 1987). Undoubtedly varieties of form processing are carried out by both the M and P systems and convergent forward projections, operating both within and between them, could allow an area to use diverse types of information in performing a particular role. Lateral interconnections between areas and intrinsic connections may also bring about this type of integration. But if the same is to be achieved by backward projections, which we infer to be incapable of exciting cells on their own, then the mechanism involved must be more subtle. Descendingpathways. We have described here how the back projection from V5 to V2 invades not only the thick stripes but also the thin stripes and interstripes, so forming a continuous distribution. Similarly we have observed that V4 projects back to the thick stripes as well as the thin stripes and interstripes from which it receives its output (Zeki and Shipp, 1989). Both are examples of a descending cross-connection. Since the functional role of descending connections within a specialized pathway is not particularly clear, it is even less obvious what role is played by the asymmetric component in extending between specialized pathways. One possibility we have considered, however, is that the overlapping projections of V4 and V5 within V2 may bc involved with the mechanism whereby sensations of colour and motioii are perceived as belonging to each other and to the appropriate object. so as to yield a coherent and unified visual image. We have no concrete evidence to support this proposition, but can offer a number of inferential arguments. Firstly there is the idea, however, simplistic, that attributes such as colour and motion are associated with each other by virtue of occupying identical parts of the visual field. Since topographic organization appears to be progressively eroded in higher visual areas, the projection of a focus of activity in V5 or V4 back to V2 where topographic order is less eroded could constitute a means of re-establishing or maintaining information as to the location in space of the stimuli eliciting that activity. This is a view elaborated b.y Edelman (1987) and is one of the roles he proposes for re-entram connections - the preservation of spatio-temporal continuity in the representation of an object across separate maps. Secondly the overall architecture of V2 may itself be of significance. Note that the segregation afforded between the pathways through the thick, thin and interstripes has already been achieved at the level clf V 1, in the segregation of blobs and interblobs in layers 2 and 3 and of these from layer 4B. The gross anatomical reorganization in V2 is simply to place all the segregations within the tangential plane and at a larger scale. Since, a priori, we might expect that the functions of the thick stripes could have been embodied in V3 or V5 and those of the thin stripes and interstripes in V4, the suspicion arises that the major utility to the visual system of a second visual area that retains an involvement in all visual atributes, organized so as to remain in close association, is that it facilitates interactions between them. Theie interactions could be achieved both by intrinsic connections within V2 stretching between the different types of stripe (Rockland, 1985) and by diffuse descending projections of the type we have described. It is interesting to observe in this context that the descending projection from V5 to V1 does not appear to invade layers 2 and 3 and is potentially re-entrant only with respect to the V5- and V3-efferent cells of layer 4B within the M pathway (Shipp and Zeki, 1989). Thus the first cortic;tl site for re-entrant connections between the M and P pathways seem to be V2 rather than V1. The striped architecture of V2 may also be of significance in determining the size and configuration of the focus of attention -the limited part of the field of view upon which attention is concentrated at any one moment and which may be shifted to a new location independently of eye movements (e.g. Bergen and Julesz, 1983). We introduce this idea here because the feature integration theory of Triesman (Triesman and Gelade, 1980; Triesman and Gormican, 1988) suggests that it is only within the focus of attention that the features of an object within separately coded dimensions such as colour, shape and motion are reliably combined so as to yield an accurate percept; forcible stretching of the focus of attention over a number of 0bjeci.s appears to generate errors in the recombination process (Triesman and Schmidt, 1982). Similarly, in visual search tasks, a target item defined only by a conjunction of two features-such as its colour and shape:, or colour and direction of motion - is not immediately detectable (does not pop out ) and can be found only by a serial scan of focal attention from one item to the next (Triesman and Gelade, 1980; Nakayarr,a and Silverman, 1986). Sagi and Julesz (1986) devised a means of charting the size and configuration of the focus of attention when it was directed outside foveal vision. Surprisingly it was found to tie asymmetrical in shape and elongated along an isoeccentricity meridi2.n - extending over an arc of almost 90 at an eccentricity of 4. This immediately suggests the possibility of an association with V2 since

21 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 353 each stripe describes a similar arc within the visual field, passing from the vertical meridian at the border with VI to the horizontal meridian at the border with V3 (Zeki, 1969; Cragg, 1969; Gattass et al., 1981). Furthermore the extent of the focus in the radial direction (along a meridian perpendicularly intersecting lines of isoeccentricity) was, at just over 2, very similar to the meridional shift in receptive field location that we have recorded in passing through one cycle of stripes in V2 at a comparable eccentricity (Shipp and Zeki, in preparation). Thus, though the mechanisms of attention doubtless involve many separate areas, some of their spatial properties may be determined by the nature of the modular architecture of V2. We note that Anderson and Van Essen (1987) have also made a similar proposal. The assumption is made, of course, that human V2 has a cytochrome oxidase architecture not too dissimilar from that of the macaque, which, so far as we know, has yet to be confirmed. The above observations constitute, we believe, reasonable grounds for supposing that the descending projections to V2 may be involved in the process of perceptual unification, though many conceptual difficulties remain. A useful experimental approach is to consider situations in which the mechanisms responsible for unification break down. One such demonstration, not dependent on lapses of attention, is described by Ramachandran (1987) and also discussed in relation to V2. It involves the adherence of colour and motion, a pair of attributes whose initial computation is probably performed entirely independently (e.g. Carney et al., 1987). A field of dots drifts uniformly over a stationary red square in a green surround. As long as there is a luminance difference between the red and the green the square appears stable, but at isoluminance it appears to adopt the motion of the dots, oscillating convincingly to and fro as the direction of drift of the dots is reversed. Other demonstrations of motion capture (Ramachandran and Inada, 1985; Ramachandran and Cavanagh, 1987) indicate that within a given region of the visual field there is a tendency for a single motion signal to prevail over others which are weaker, so in this instance it might be surmised that at isoluminance the representation of a stationary contour is sufficiently attenuated to allow it to undergo capture by the motion of the dots. But the neural mechanisms underlying this effect are far from clear. Is there a specific class of neurons which become active around the point of isoluminance and whose activity is responsible for the change in perception? Or is the illusory motion of the colour contour due to a correlated change in the firing patterns of separate direction and colour neurons which otherwise remain active throughout the variations in luminance? Finally is it possible to account for either of these changes in terms of the modulating influences normally associated with back projections? It goes without saying that answers to these questions would substantially enhance our understanding of the process of perceptual unification. Acknowledgements The authors thank Ian Wilson for doing the histology and John Romaya and Dr Phil Cope for assistance with computer programming. The material illustrated in Figure 10 was kindly provided by M. G. Maioli, C. Galletti, S. Squatrito and P. P. Battaglini of the Physiological Institute of Bologna University, Italy. A description of its preparation can be found in Maioli et af. (1983). This work was supported by grants from the SERC and the Wellcome Trust. Abbreviations DL I 10s K dorsolateral visual area interstripe inferior occipital sulcus thick cytochrome oxidase stripe LS M MT N P POS HRP- WGA, HRP lunate sulcus magnocellular middle temporal visual area thin cytochrome oxidase stripe parvocellular parieto-occipital sulcus horseradish peroxidase- wheatgerm agglutinin References Albright, T. D. (1984) Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 52: Allman, J. M. and Kaas, J. H. (1974a) The organization of the second visual area (VII) in the owl monkey: a second order transformation of the visual hemifield. Brain Res. 76: Allman, J. M. and Kaas, J. H. (1974b) A crescent-shaped cortical visual area surrounding the middle temporal area (MT) in the owl monkey (Aorus trivirgarus). Brain Res. 81: Allman, J. M. (1988) Variations in visual cortex organization in primates. In: Rakic, P. and Singer, W. (eds), Neurobiology of Neocortex, pp John Wiley and Sons, New York. Anderson, C. H. and Van Essen, D. C. (1987) Shifter circruits: a computational strategy for dynamic aspects of visual processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 84: 6297:6301. Baizer, J. S., Robinson, D. L. and Dow, B. M. (1977) Visual responses of area 18 neurons in awake, behaving monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 40: Baizer, J. S. (1982) Receptive field properties of V3 neurons in monkey. Invest. Ophthal. 23: Baker, J. F., Petersen, S. E., Newsome, W. T. and Allman, J. M. (1981) Visual response properties of neurons in four extrastriate areas of the owl monkey (Aorus trivirgarus): a quantitative comparison of medial, dorsomedial, dorsolateral and middle temporal areas. J. Neurophysiol. 45: Bergen, J. R. and Julesz, B. (1983) Parallel versus serial processing in rapid pattern discrimination. Nature 303: von Bonin, G. (1960) The Cerebral Cortex. C. C. Thomas, Springfield, Illinois, USA. Brodmann, K. (1905) Beitrage zur histologischen lokalisation der grosshirnrinde. J. Psychol. Neurol. 4: Burkhalter, A. and Van Essen, D. C. (1986) Processing of color, form and disparity information in visual areas VP and V2 of ventral extrastriate cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 6: Campbell, A. W. (1905) Histological Studies on the Localisation of Cerebral Function. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Carney, T., Shadlen, M. and Switkes, E. (1987) Parallel processing of motion and colour information. Nature 328: Cavanagh, P. (1987) Reconstructing the third dimension: interactions between color, texture, motion, binocular disparity, and shape. Comput. Vision Graphics Image Process. 32: Colonnier, M. and Sas, E. (1978) An anterograde degeneration study of the tangential spread of axons in cortical areas 17 and 18 of the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). J. Comp. Neurol. 179: Cragg, B. G. (1969) The topography of the afferent projections in the circumstriate visual cortex of the monkey studied by the Nauta method. Vision Res. 9: Cusick, C. G. and Kaas, J. H. (1988) Cortical connections of area 18 and dorsolateral visual cortex in squirrel monkeys. Vis. Neurosci. 1: Desimone, R., Schein, S. J., Moran, J. and Ungerleider, L. G. (1985) Contour, color and shape analysis beyond the striate cortex. Vision Res. 25: Desimone, R. and Schein, S. J. (1987) Visual properties of neurons in area V4 of the macaque: sensitivity to stimulus form. J. Neurophysiol. 57: DeYoe, E. A. and Van Essen, D. C. (1985) Segregation of efferent connections and receptive field properties in visual area V2 of the macaque. Nature 317: Edelman, G. M. (1987) Neural Darwinism. Basic Books, New York. Felleman, D. J. and Van Essen, D. C. (1987) Receptive field properties of neurons in area V3 of macaque monkey extrastriate cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 57: Fries, W. and Zeki, S. (1983) The laminar origin of the inputs to the fourth visual complex of macaque monkey cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 340: 51P. Gallyas, F. (1979) Silver staining of myelin by means of physical development. Neurol. Res. 1:

22 354 Cortical connections between V5 and V2 Gattass, R., Gross, C. G. and Sandell, J. H. (1981) Visual topography of V2 in the macaque. J. Comp. Neurol. 201: Horton, J. C. and Hubel, D. M. (1981) Regular patchy distribution of cytochrome oxidase staining in primary visual cortex of macaque monkey. Nature 292: Horton, J. C. (1984) Cytochrome oxidase patches: a new cytoarchitectonic feature of monkey visual cortex. Phd. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 304: Hubel, D. M. and Livingstone, M. S. (1985) Complex-unoriented cells in a subregion of primate area 18. Nature 315: Hubel, D. H. and Livingstone, M. S. (1987) Segregation of form, color and stereopsis in primate area 18. J. Neurosci. 7: Jacobs, G. H. (1977) Visual capacities of the owl monkey (Aorus rrivirgarus): I spectral sensitivity and color vision. Vision Res. 16: Jacobs, G. M. (1983) Variations in color vision among non-human primates. In: Mollon, J. P. and Sharpe, L. T. Colour Vision, pp Academic Press, London. Kaas, J. H. and Lin, C. S. (1971) Cortical projections of area 18 in owl monkeys. Vision Res. 16: Krubitzer, L. A. and Kaas, J. H. (1987) Connections of modular subdivisions of cortical visual areas 17 and 18 with the middle temporal area, MT, in squirrel monkeys. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 13: 3. Livingstone, M. S. and Hubel, D. H. (1982) Thalamic inputs to cytochrome oxidase-rich regions in monkey visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 79: Livingstone, M. S. and Hubel, D. H. (1984) Anatomy and physiology of a colour system in the primate visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 4: Livingstone, M. S. and Hubel, D. H. (1987a) Connections between layer 48 of area 17 and the thick cytochrome oxidase stripes of area 18 in the squirrel monkey. J. Neurosci. 7: Livingstone, M. S. and Hubel, D. H. (1987b) Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement and depth. J. Neurosci. 7: Lund, J.S., Hendrickson, A. E., Ogren, M.P. and Tobin, E. A. (1981) Anatomical organization of primate visual cortex area VII. J. Comp. Neurol. 202: Maioli, M. G,. Squatrito, S., Galleti, C., Battaglini, P. P. and Sanseverino, E. R. (1983) Cortico-cortical connections from the visual region of the superior temporal sulcus to frontal eye field in the macaque. Brain Res. 265: Maunsell, J. H. R. and Van Essen, D. C. (l983a) The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 3: Maunsell, J. H. R. and Van Essen, D. C. (1983b) Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed and orientation. J. Neurophysiol. 49: Mesulam, M. M. (1982) Tracing Neural Connections with Horseradish Peroxidase. Wiley, New York. Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G. and Macko, K. A. (1983) Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. Trends Neurosci. 6: Nakayama, K. and Silverman, G. H. (1986) Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions. Nature 320: Poggio, G. F. and Fischer, B. (1977) Binocular interactions and depth sensitivity in striate and prestriate cortex of behaving rhesus monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 40: Ramachandran, V. S. and Inada, V. (1985) Spatial phase and frequency in motion capture of random dot patterns. Spatial Vision 1: Ramachandran, V. S. and Cavanagh, P. C. (1987) Motion capture anisotropy. Vision Res. 27: Ramachandran, V. S. (1987) Interaction between colour and motion in human vision. Nature 328: Rockland, K. S. and Pandya, D. N. (1979) Laminar origins and terminations of cortical connections of the occipital lobe in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 179: Rockland, K. S. and Pandya, D. N. (1981) Cortical connections of the occipital lobe in the rhesus monkey: interconnections between areas 17, 18, 19 and the superior temporal sulcus. Brain Res. 212: Rockland, K. S. (1985) A reticular pattern of intrinsic connections in primale area V2 (area 18). J. Comp. Neurol. 235: Sagi, D. and Julesz, B. (1986) Enhanced detection in the aperture of foc.11 attention during simple discrimination tasks. Nature 321: Shipp, S. and Zeki, S. (1985) Segregation of pathways leading from area V2 to areas V4 and V5 of macaque monkey visual cortex. Nature. 315: Shipp, S. and Zeki, S. (1989) The organization of connections between area V5 and V1 in macaque monkey visual cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1: I. Tootell, R. B. H., Silverman, M. S., DeValois, R. L. andjacobs, G. H. (1983) Functional organization of the second cortical visual area in primates. Science 220: Tootell, R. B. H., Hamilton, S. L. and Silverman, M. S. (1985) Topography of cytochrome oxidase activity in owl monkey cortex. J. Neurosci. 5: Triesman, A. and Gelade, G. (1980) A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology 12: Triesman, A. and Schmidt, H. (1982) Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cognitive Psychology 14: Triesman, A. and Gormican, S. (1988) Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. Psychol. Rev. 95: Ungerleider, L. G. and Desimone, R. (1986a) Projections to the superior temporal sulcus from the central and peripheral field representations of V1 and V2. J. Comp. Neurol. 248: Ungerleider, L. G. and Desimone, R. (1986b) Cortical connections of visual area MT in the macaque. J. Comp. Neurol. 248: Valverde, F. (1978) The organization of area 18 in the monkey. A Golgi study. Anat. Embryol. 154: Van Essen, D. C. and Maunsell, J. H. R. (1983) Hierarchical organization and functional streams in the visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 6: Weller, R. E., Wall, J. T. and Kaas, J. H. (1984) Cortical connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and the superior temporal cortex in ow1 monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 228: Weller, R. E. and Kaas, J. H. (1985) Cortical projections of the dorsolateral visual area in owl monkeys: the prestriate relay to inferior temporal cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 234: Wong-Riley, M. T. T. (1979) Changes in the visual system of monocularly sutured or enucleated cats demonstrable with cytochrome oxidase histochemistry. Brain Res. 171: , Wong-Riley, M. T. T. and Carroll, E. W. (1984) Quantitative light and electron microscopic analysis of cytochrome oxidase-rich zones in Vn prestriate cortex of the squirrel monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 222: Zeki, S. (1969) Representation of central fields in prestriate cortex of monkey. Brain Res. 19: Zeki, S. (1971) Cortical projections from two prestriate areas in the monkey. Brain Res. 34: Zeki, S. (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey. J. Physiol. 236: Zeki, S. (1975) The functional organization of projections from striate to prestriate visual cortex in the rhesus monkey. Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. 15: Zeki, S. (1978a) Functional specialisation in the visual cortex of the rhesus monkey. Nature 274: Zeki, S. (1978b) The cortical projections of foveal striate cortex in the rhesus monkey. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 277: Zeki, S. (1978~) The third visual complex of rhesus monkey prestriate cortex. J. Physiol. 277: Zeki, S. (1978d) Uniformity and diversity of structure and function in rhesus monkey prestriate cortex. J. Physiol. 277: Zeki, S. and Shipp, S. (1988) The functional logic of cortical connections. Nanire 355: Zeki, S. and Shipp, S. (1989) Modular connections between areas V2 and V4 in macaque monkey visual cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. (in press).

The Visual System. Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004

The Visual System. Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004 The Visual System Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004 Phone: 343-4538 Email: vivien.casagrande@mcmail.vanderbilt.edu Office: T2302 MCN Required Reading Adler s Physiology of the Eye Chapters

More information

Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the Macaque

Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the Macaque Cerebral Cortex March 2008;18:477-499 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm061 Advance Access publication June 4, 2007 Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the Macaque Leslie G. Ungerleider 1, Thelma W. Galkin 2, Robert

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization THE VISUAL SYSTEM Higher Visual Processing Martin Paré Assistant Professor Physiology & Psychology Most of the cortex that covers the cerebral hemispheres is

More information

London CYVlE 6BT. animals, by plotting receptive field positions for different recording sites. Results

London CYVlE 6BT. animals, by plotting receptive field positions for different recording sites. Results J. Physiol. (1978), 277, pp. 193-226 193 With 3 plates and 14 text-figures Printed in Great Britain THE TOPOGRAPHIC ORGANIZATION OF RHESUS MONKEY PRESTRIATE CORTEX BY D. C. VAN ESSEN* AND S. M. ZEKIt From

More information

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye: Problems confronting the visual system: Visual Physiology image contains a huge amount of information which must be processed quickly. image is dim, blurry and distorted. Light levels vary enormously.

More information

Multiple Visual Areas in the Caudal Superior Temporal Sulcus of the Macaque

Multiple Visual Areas in the Caudal Superior Temporal Sulcus of the Macaque THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 248:164-189 (1986) Multiple Visual Areas in the Caudal Superior Temporal Sulcus of the Macaque ROBERT DESIMONE AND LESLIE G. UNGERLEIDER Laboratory of Neuropsychology,

More information

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m. Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Wednesday, 16 March

More information

Multiple Circuits Relaying Primate Parallel Visual Pathways

Multiple Circuits Relaying Primate Parallel Visual Pathways The Journal of Neuroscience, December 6, 2006 26(49):12789 12798 12789 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Multiple Circuits Relaying Primate Parallel Visual Pathways to the Middle Temporal Area Jonathan J. Nassi

More information

Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe

Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe How many extrastriate areas are there? LOTS!!! Macaque monkey flattened cortex Why? How do we know this? Topography Functional properties Connections

More information

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling. Lect21-22: Vision Central Pathways

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling. Lect21-22: Vision Central Pathways CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect02: BOSS Discrete Event Simulator Lect21-22: Vision Central Pathways Chapter 12 of Purves et al., 4e Larry Wittie Computer Science, StonyBrook

More information

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Fani Deligianni B. A. Wandell, et al. Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex, Neuron, 56(2):366-383, 2007 Retinotopy Visual Cortex organised in visual field maps: Nearby neurons have

More information

(Received 31 March 1977)

(Received 31 March 1977) J. Phyeiol. (1978), 277, pp. 227-244 227 With 1 plate and 10 text-ftgure8 Printed in Great Britain THE CORTICAL PROJECTIONS OF FOVEAL STRIATE CORTEX IN THE RHESUS MONKEY BY S. M. ZEKI* From the Department

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

Functional Demarcation of a Border Between Areas V6 and V6A in the Superior Parietal Gyrus of the Macaque Monkey

Functional Demarcation of a Border Between Areas V6 and V6A in the Superior Parietal Gyrus of the Macaque Monkey European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 8, pp. 30-52, 1996 0 European Neuroscience Association Functional Demarcation of a Border Between Areas V6 and V6A in the Superior Parietal Gyrus of the Macaque Monkey

More information

BINOCULAR VISUAL FIELD. fixation point. fovea

BINOCULAR VISUAL FIELD. fixation point. fovea BINOCULAR VISUAL FIELD fixation point fovea left hemisphere right hemisphere VISUAL HEMIFIELD monocular crescent temporal hemi-retina nasal hemi-retina left hemisphere right hemisphere VISUAL PATHWAYS

More information

Third tier ventral extrastriate cortex in the New World monkey, Cebus apella

Third tier ventral extrastriate cortex in the New World monkey, Cebus apella Exp Brain Res (2000) 132:287 305 Digital Object Identifier (DOI) 10.1007/s002210000344 RESEARCH ARTICLE Marcello G.P. Rosa Maria Carmen Piñon Ricardo Gattass Aglai P. B. Sousa Third tier ventral extrastriate

More information

Reappraisal of DL/V4 Boundaries Based on Connectivity Patterns of Dorsolateral Visual Cortex in Macaques

Reappraisal of DL/V4 Boundaries Based on Connectivity Patterns of Dorsolateral Visual Cortex in Macaques Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published September 30, 2004 Reappraisal of DL/V4 Boundaries Based on Connectivity Patterns of Dorsolateral Visual Cortex in Macaques Iwona Stepniewska, Christine E. Collins

More information

A visuo-somatomotor pathway through superior parietal cortex in the macaque monkey: cortical connections of areas V6 and V6A

A visuo-somatomotor pathway through superior parietal cortex in the macaque monkey: cortical connections of areas V6 and V6A European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 10, pp. 3171 3193, 1998 European Neuroscience Association A visuo-somatomotor pathway through superior parietal cortex in the macaque monkey: cortical connections

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

Adventures into terra incognita

Adventures into terra incognita BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter VI. Adventures into terra incognita In primary visual cortex there are neurons

More information

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision 1. Explain what a hierarchically organised neural system is, in terms of physiological response properties of its neurones.

More information

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I Monocular Sensory Processes of Vision: Color Vision Mechanisms of Color Processing . Neural Mechanisms of Color Processing A. Parallel processing - M- & P- pathways B. Second

More information

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner Cerebral Cortex 1 Sarah Heilbronner heilb028@umn.edu Want to meet? Coffee hour 10-11am Tuesday 11/27 Surdyk s Overview and organization of the cerebral cortex What is the cerebral cortex? Where is each

More information

Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres. Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University

Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres. Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University The largest part of the brain. Ovoid shape. Two incompletely separated cerebral hemispheres. The outer surface of the cerebral

More information

The arteries of the human kidney

The arteries of the human kidney J. Anat. (1966), 100, 4, pp. 881-894 881 With 8 figures Printed in Great Britain The arteries of the human kidney BY H. FINE AND E. N. KEEN Department of Anatomy, University of Natal INTRODUCTION A study

More information

Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Verse 1: Neocortex, frontal lobe, Brain stem, brain stem, Hippocampus, neural node, Right hemisphere, Pons and cortex visual, Brain stem, brain stem, Sylvian fissure,

More information

Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published October 8, 2008

Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published October 8, 2008 Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published October 8, 2008 Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn178 The Organization of Orientation-Selective, Luminance-Change and Binocular- Preference Domains in the Second

More information

Nature Neuroscience doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of viral injections.

Nature Neuroscience doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of viral injections. Supplementary Figure 1 Characterization of viral injections. (a) Dorsal view of a mouse brain (dashed white outline) after receiving a large, unilateral thalamic injection (~100 nl); demonstrating that

More information

Fig. 2), i.e. a brain containing only the fissures present in every brain of. posterior composite convolutions, cannot be represented.

Fig. 2), i.e. a brain containing only the fissures present in every brain of. posterior composite convolutions, cannot be represented. 310 E. KLEN. REPORT ON THE PARTS DESTROYED ON THE LEFT SDE OF THE BRAN OF THE DOG OPERATED ON BY PROF. GOLTZ. BY E. KLEN, M.D., F.R.S. Plate X. THE injury on the left side of the brain, as it presents

More information

The Distribution of Retino-Collicular Axon Terminals in Rhesus Monkey

The Distribution of Retino-Collicular Axon Terminals in Rhesus Monkey The Distribution of Retino-Collicular Axon Terminals in Rhesus Monkey J. G. POLLACK 'School of Optometry/The Medical Center and Neuroscience Program, Uniuersity of Alabama in Birmingham, Birmingham, Alabama

More information

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes Finding the Central Sulcus MAP TO NEUROSCIENCE CORE CONCEPTS 1 NCC1. The brain is the body's most complex organ. LEARNING OBJECTIVES After study of the assigned learning

More information

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

A model of the interaction between mood and memory INSTITUTE OF PHYSICS PUBLISHING NETWORK: COMPUTATION IN NEURAL SYSTEMS Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 12 (2001) 89 109 www.iop.org/journals/ne PII: S0954-898X(01)22487-7 A model of the interaction between

More information

Any of the vertebra in the cervical (neck) region of the spinal column. The cervical vertebra are the smallest vertebra in the spine, reflective of th

Any of the vertebra in the cervical (neck) region of the spinal column. The cervical vertebra are the smallest vertebra in the spine, reflective of th Any of the vertebra in the cervical (neck) region of the spinal column. The cervical vertebra are the smallest vertebra in the spine, reflective of the fact that they support the least load. In humans,

More information

20 2 Stomach Fig. 2.1 An illustration showing different patterns of the myenteric plexus peculiar to the regions in the guinea-pig stomach stained wit

20 2 Stomach Fig. 2.1 An illustration showing different patterns of the myenteric plexus peculiar to the regions in the guinea-pig stomach stained wit Stomach 2 The stomach is unique in that ICC have a different distribution in proximal and distal regions of the same organ. ICC-CM and ICC-LM are densely distributed throughout the thick circular and longitudinal

More information

LOCAL CIRCUITS IN PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX OF THE MACAQUE MONKEY

LOCAL CIRCUITS IN PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX OF THE MACAQUE MONKEY Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1998. 21:47 74 Copyright c 1998 by Annual Reviews Inc. All rights reserved 48 CALLAWAY LOCAL CIRCUITS IN PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX OF THE MACAQUE MONKEY Edward M. Callaway Systems Neurobiology

More information

Organization of Intrinsic Connections in Owl Monkey Area MT

Organization of Intrinsic Connections in Owl Monkey Area MT Organization of Intrinsic Connections in Owl Monkey Area MT R. Malach, T. D. Schirman, M. Harel, R. B. H. Tootell 1 and D. Malonek Department of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel

More information

Vision II. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota

Vision II. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota Vision II Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota 1 Ganglion Cells The axons of the retinal ganglion cells form the optic nerve and carry visual information into the brain. 2 Optic

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

COMPUTATIONAL NEUROIMAGING OF HUMAN VISUAL CORTEX

COMPUTATIONAL NEUROIMAGING OF HUMAN VISUAL CORTEX Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1999. 22:145 73 Copyright c 1999 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved COMPUTATIONAL NEUROIMAGING OF HUMAN VISUAL CORTEX Brian A. Wandell Neuroscience Program and Department of Psychology,

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Modular Complexity of Area V2 in the Macaque Monkey

Modular Complexity of Area V2 in the Macaque Monkey 1243_book.fm Page 109 Thursday, May 22, 2003 10:45 AM 5 Modular Complexity of Area V2 in the Macaque Monkey Anna W. Roe CONTENTS 5.1 Introduction...109 5.1.1 Optical Imaging of V2...110 5.2 Redefining

More information

Muscle Tissue. General concepts. Classification of muscle. I. Functional classification is based on the type of neural control.

Muscle Tissue. General concepts. Classification of muscle. I. Functional classification is based on the type of neural control. Muscle Tissue LEARNING OBJECTIVES 1. Identify the three types of muscle tissue at the light microscopic level. 2. List and compare the structural and functional features of each of the three muscle fiber

More information

Microscopic Anatomy of Inferior Medullary Velum Of Cerebellum

Microscopic Anatomy of Inferior Medullary Velum Of Cerebellum 32 J Anat. Soc. India 51(1) 32-34 (2002) Microscopic Anatomy of Of Cerebellum Arora, N.K. Department of Anatomy, Government Medical College, Chandigarh INDIA. Abstract. A study of the inferior medullary

More information

Connections between Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex and Superior Temporal Sulcus Regions in the Macaque Monkey

Connections between Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex and Superior Temporal Sulcus Regions in the Macaque Monkey The Journal of Neuroscience, July 1, 2000, 20(13):5083 5101 Connections between Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex and Superior Temporal Sulcus Regions in the Macaque Monkey K. S. Saleem, 1,2 W. Suzuki, 1,3

More information

CORTICAL CONNECTIONS OF THE OCCIPITAL LOBE IN THE RHESUS MONKEY: INTERCONNECTIONS BETWEEN AREAS 17, 18, 19 AND THE SUPERIOR TEMPORAL SULCUS*

CORTICAL CONNECTIONS OF THE OCCIPITAL LOBE IN THE RHESUS MONKEY: INTERCONNECTIONS BETWEEN AREAS 17, 18, 19 AND THE SUPERIOR TEMPORAL SULCUS* Brain Research, 212 (1981) 249-270 249 Elsevier/North-Holland Biomedical Press Research Reports CORTICAL CONNECTIONS OF THE OCCIPITAL LOBE IN THE RHESUS MONKEY: INTERCONNECTIONS BETWEEN AREAS 17, 18, 19

More information

THE sebaceous glands of the rabbit consist of clusters of about ten cells

THE sebaceous glands of the rabbit consist of clusters of about ten cells 79 On the Relationship between Mammary, Sweat, and Sebaceous Glands By D. B. CARLISLE (From the Department of Zoology and Comparative Anatomy, Oxford, and the Plymouth Laboratory of the Marine Biological

More information

The Eye. Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Today s goals. 5 From eye to brain. Today s reading

The Eye. Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Today s goals. 5 From eye to brain. Today s reading Cognitive Neuroscience of Language 5 From eye to brain Today s goals Look at the pathways that conduct the visual information from the eye to the visual cortex Marielle Lange http://homepages.inf.ed.ac.uk/mlange/teaching/cnl/

More information

The cortical visual area V6: brain location and visual topography

The cortical visual area V6: brain location and visual topography European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol.11, pp.3922±3936, 1999 Ó European Neuroscience Association The cortical visual area V6: brain location and visual topography Claudio Galletti, Patrizia Fattori, Michela

More information

Neuroanatomy, Text and Atlas (J. H. Martin), 3 rd Edition Chapter 7, The Visual System, pp ,

Neuroanatomy, Text and Atlas (J. H. Martin), 3 rd Edition Chapter 7, The Visual System, pp , Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: RETINA and CENTRAL VISUAL PATHWAYS P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Friday, 20

More information

Convergence Principles: Information in the Answer

Convergence Principles: Information in the Answer Convergence Principles: Information in the Answer Sets of Some Multiple-Choice Intelligence Tests A. P. White and J. E. Zammarelli University of Durham It is hypothesized that some common multiplechoice

More information

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Frank Tong Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Office: Room 3-N-2B Telephone: 609-258-2652 Fax: 609-258-1113 Email: ftong@princeton.edu Graduate School Applicants

More information

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision Biological Bases of Behavior 6: Vision Sensory Systems The brain detects events in the external environment and directs the contractions of the muscles Afferent neurons carry sensory messages to brain

More information

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Baran Çürüklü Department of Computer Science and Engineering Mälardalen University Västerås S-721 23, Sweden Abstract. In this paper

More information

Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system

Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system Parallel pathways from the retina to the cortex Visual input is initially encoded in the retina as a 2D distribution of intensity. The retinal

More information

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Limits to Iconic Memory 0 Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Ronald A. Rensink Departments of Psychology and Computer Science University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4 Canada Running Head: Limits

More information

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience Levels of organization Central Nervous System 1m 10 11 neurons Neural systems and neuroanatomy Systems 10cm Networks 1mm Neurons 100μm 10 8 neurons Professor Daniel Leeds

More information

Neuronal responses to plaids

Neuronal responses to plaids Vision Research 39 (1999) 2151 2156 Neuronal responses to plaids Bernt Christian Skottun * Skottun Research, 273 Mather Street, Piedmont, CA 94611-5154, USA Received 30 June 1998; received in revised form

More information

Essentials of Human Anatomy & Physiology. Seventh Edition. The Nervous System. Copyright 2003 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Benjamin Cummings

Essentials of Human Anatomy & Physiology. Seventh Edition. The Nervous System. Copyright 2003 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Benjamin Cummings Essentials of Human Anatomy & Physiology Seventh Edition The Nervous System Copyright 2003 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Benjamin Cummings Functions of the Nervous System 1. Sensory input gathering

More information

triplelayered unit for the plasma cell membrane component; two such Model parameters are assigned to three varieties of peripheral nerve

triplelayered unit for the plasma cell membrane component; two such Model parameters are assigned to three varieties of peripheral nerve STRUCTURAL PARAMETERS OF NERVE MYELIN* BY C. R. WORTHINGTON DEPARTMENT OF PHYSICS AND BIOPHYSICS RESEARCH DIVISION, UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN, ANN ARBOR Communicated by J. L. Oncley, April 7, 1969 Abstract.-Low-angle

More information

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones Photoreceptors Rods Cones 120 000 000 Dim light Prefer wavelength of 505 nm Monochromatic Mainly in periphery of the eye 6 000 000 More light Different spectral sensitivities!long-wave receptors (558 nm)

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

Sulcus Classification Rating Protocol: Anterior Temporobasal Sulci (SCRaP:aTB)

Sulcus Classification Rating Protocol: Anterior Temporobasal Sulci (SCRaP:aTB) Sulcus Classification Rating Protocol: Anterior Temporobasal Sulci (SCRaP:aTB) 2012 Reckess, Dunn, Bauer & Leonard Address correspondence to: Gila Z. Reckess, Ph.D. reckessg@gmail.com Anterior Temporobasal

More information

The human brain weighs roughly 1.5 kg and has an average volume of 1130 cm 3. A sheep s brain weighs in however at kg.

The human brain weighs roughly 1.5 kg and has an average volume of 1130 cm 3. A sheep s brain weighs in however at kg. Sheep Brain Dissection Objectives: 1. List and describe the principal structures of the sheep brain 2. Identify important parts of the sheep brain in a preserved specimen Materials: Dissection tools, lab

More information

Neuroscience Tutorial

Neuroscience Tutorial Neuroscience Tutorial Brain Organization : cortex, basal ganglia, limbic lobe : thalamus, hypothal., pituitary gland : medulla oblongata, midbrain, pons, cerebellum Cortical Organization Cortical Organization

More information

Independent Projection Streams from Macaque Striate Cortex to the Second Visual Area and Middle Temporal Area

Independent Projection Streams from Macaque Striate Cortex to the Second Visual Area and Middle Temporal Area 5684 The Journal of Neuroscience, July 2, 2003 23(13):5684 5692 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Independent Projection Streams from Macaque Striate Cortex to the Second Visual Area and Middle Temporal Area

More information

Anatomy of the Heart

Anatomy of the Heart Biology 212: Anatomy and Physiology II Anatomy of the Heart References: Saladin, KS: Anatomy and Physiology, The Unity of Form and Function 8 th (2018). Required reading before beginning this lab: Chapter

More information

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information Peter Kalocsai and Irving Biederman Department of Psychology and Computer Science University of Southern California Los

More information

C:\Documents and Settings\sstensaas\Desktop\dental visual 2010\VisualPath dental 2010.docVisualPath dental 2010.doc

C:\Documents and Settings\sstensaas\Desktop\dental visual 2010\VisualPath dental 2010.docVisualPath dental 2010.doc Neuroanatomy Suzanne Stensaas April 8, 2010, 10:00-12:00 p.m. Reading: Waxman Ch. 15, Computer Resources: HyperBrain Ch 7 THE VISUAL PATHWAY Objectives: 1. Describe the pathway of visual information from

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 250~ (1986)

THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 250~ (1986) THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 250~403-430 (1986) Microelectrode Maps, Myeloarchitecture, and Cortical Connections of Three Somatotopically Organized Representations of the Body Surface in the Parietal

More information

Regional and Lobe Parcellation Rhesus Monkey Brain Atlas. Manual Tracing for Parcellation Template

Regional and Lobe Parcellation Rhesus Monkey Brain Atlas. Manual Tracing for Parcellation Template Regional and Lobe Parcellation Rhesus Monkey Brain Atlas Manual Tracing for Parcellation Template Overview of Tracing Guidelines A) Traces are performed in a systematic order they, allowing the more easily

More information

Does History Repeat Itself? The case of cortical columns. Those who fail to learn the lessons of history are condemned to repeat it George Santayana

Does History Repeat Itself? The case of cortical columns. Those who fail to learn the lessons of history are condemned to repeat it George Santayana Does History Repeat Itself? The case of cortical columns Those who fail to learn the lessons of history are condemned to repeat it George Santayana Stripe of Gennari Gennari says that he first saw his

More information

ANATOMY & PHYSIOLOGY DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP BRAIN LAB GROUP:

ANATOMY & PHYSIOLOGY DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP BRAIN LAB GROUP: ANATOMY & PHYSIOLOGY DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP BRAIN LAB GROUP: Introduction The purpose of the sheep brain dissection is to familiarize you with the three dimensional structure of the brain and teach you

More information

GENERAL SCOPE AND USES OF PHYSICAL/BIOLOGICAL ANTHROPOLOGY. Paper No. & Title: B.A./B.Sc. (Honours) 2 dn semester. (Practical)

GENERAL SCOPE AND USES OF PHYSICAL/BIOLOGICAL ANTHROPOLOGY. Paper No. & Title: B.A./B.Sc. (Honours) 2 dn semester. (Practical) GENERAL SCOPE AND USES OF PHYSICAL/BIOLOGICAL ANTHROPOLOGY Course name: Physical Anthropology Paper No. & Title: B.A./B.Sc. (Honours) 2 dn semester (Practical) Topic No. & Title: 5/12 (Part-I) Drawing

More information

Sheep Brain Dissection

Sheep Brain Dissection Sheep Brain Dissection Mammalian brains have many features in common. Human brains may not be available, so sheep brains often are dissected as an aid to understanding the mammalian brain since he general

More information

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex I: To list the functions of different cortical laminae II: To describe the four motor areas of the cerebral cortex. III: To discuss the functions and dysfunctions of

More information

LIMBIC SYSTEM. Dr. Amani A. Elfaki Associate Professor Department of Anatomy

LIMBIC SYSTEM. Dr. Amani A. Elfaki Associate Professor Department of Anatomy LIMBIC SYSTEM Dr. Amani A. Elfaki Associate Professor Department of Anatomy Learning Objectives Define the limbic system Identify the parts of the limbic system Describe the circulation of the limbic system

More information

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!)

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!) Introduction to Physiological Psychology Vision ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ /~ksweeney/psy260.html Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy (the part that s visible to us!)

More information

A hierarchy of the functional organization for color, form and disparity in primate visual area V2

A hierarchy of the functional organization for color, form and disparity in primate visual area V2 Vision Research 41 (2001) 1333 1349 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres A hierarchy of the functional organization for color, form and disparity in primate visual area V2 Daniel Y. Ts o a, *, Anna Wang Roe

More information

Blood vessels of the ciliary ganglion

Blood vessels of the ciliary ganglion Brit. J. Ophthal. (I973) 57, 766 Blood vessels of the ciliary ganglion in man M. ELIJKOVA Institute of Anatomy, Faculty of Medicine, Charles University, Prague, Czechoslovakia Many authors, including Egorov

More information

******************************************************************************************************* MUSCLE CYTOLOGY AND HISTOLOGY

******************************************************************************************************* MUSCLE CYTOLOGY AND HISTOLOGY BIOLOGY 211: HUMAN ANATOMY & PHYSIOLOGY ******************************************************************************************************* MUSCLE CYTOLOGY AND HISTOLOGY *******************************************************************************************************

More information

The Fine Structure of the Epithelial Cells of the Mouse Prostate* II. Ventral Lobe Epithelium

The Fine Structure of the Epithelial Cells of the Mouse Prostate* II. Ventral Lobe Epithelium Published Online: 1 June, 1960 Supp Info: http://doi.org/10.1083/jcb.7.3.511 Downloaded from jcb.rupress.org on September 28, 2018 The Fine Structure of the Epithelial Cells of the Mouse Prostate* II.

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Gross Organization I The Brain. Reading: BCP Chapter 7

Gross Organization I The Brain. Reading: BCP Chapter 7 Gross Organization I The Brain Reading: BCP Chapter 7 Layout of the Nervous System Central Nervous System (CNS) Located inside of bone Includes the brain (in the skull) and the spinal cord (in the backbone)

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Chapter 1. Abstraction

Chapter 1. Abstraction Chapter 1 Abstraction There are many functions that we can assign to the brain, and more specifically to the cerebral cortex, that thin sheet containing billions of cells that envelopes the cerebral hemispheres.

More information

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes Lateral Surface of the Brain MAP TO NEUROSCIENCE CORE CONCEPTS 1 NCC1. The brain is the body's most complex organ. LEARNING OBJECTIVES After study of the assigned learning

More information

Visually Driven Activation in Macaque Areas V2 and V3 without Input from the Primary Visual Cortex

Visually Driven Activation in Macaque Areas V2 and V3 without Input from the Primary Visual Cortex Visually Driven Activation in Macaque Areas V2 and V3 without Input from the Primary Visual Cortex Michael C. Schmid 1 *, Theofanis Panagiotaropoulos 1, Mark A. Augath 1, Nikos K. Logothetis 1,2, Stelios

More information

Chapter IV. Angionephrography in Simple Renal Cysts

Chapter IV. Angionephrography in Simple Renal Cysts Acta Radiologica ISSN: 0001-6926 (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iaro20 Chapter IV. Angionephrography in Simple Renal Cysts To cite this article: (1957) Chapter IV. Angionephrography

More information

V4 lesions in macaques affect both single- and multiple-viewpoint shape discriminations

V4 lesions in macaques affect both single- and multiple-viewpoint shape discriminations Visual Neuroscience (1998), 15, 359 367. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1998 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238098 $12.50 V4 lesions in macaques affect both single- and multiple-viewpoint shape discriminations

More information

Leah Militello, class of 2018

Leah Militello, class of 2018 Leah Militello, class of 2018 Objectives 1. Describe the general organization of cerebral hemispheres. 2. Describe the locations and features of the different functional areas of cortex. 3. Understand

More information

Sound waves from the auditory environment all combine in the ear canal to form a complex waveform. This waveform is deconstructed by the cochlea with

Sound waves from the auditory environment all combine in the ear canal to form a complex waveform. This waveform is deconstructed by the cochlea with 1 Sound waves from the auditory environment all combine in the ear canal to form a complex waveform. This waveform is deconstructed by the cochlea with respect to time, loudness, and frequency and neural

More information

1. General characteristics of muscle tissues: 2. A. Skeletal muscle tissue ("striated muscle tissue")

1. General characteristics of muscle tissues: 2. A. Skeletal muscle tissue (striated muscle tissue) 1. General characteristics of muscle tissues: Muscle fibers, AKA, muscle cells Vascularized. Other tissues dense and loose C.T. nerves and nerve fibers Muscle fibers (muscle cells) close together. From

More information

Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system

Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system Jonathan J. Nassi* and Edward M. Callaway Abstract Incoming sensory information is sent to the brain along modality-specific channels corresponding

More information

Morphometric analysis of the cerebral cortex in the developing baboon

Morphometric analysis of the cerebral cortex in the developing baboon Eur J Anat, 8 (1): 29-34 (2004) Morphometric analysis of the cerebral cortex in the developing baboon H. Dossajee 1 and J. Hassanali 2 1- Faculty of Medicine, University of Nairobi, Kenya 2- Department

More information

Announcement. Danny to schedule a time if you are interested.

Announcement.  Danny to schedule a time if you are interested. Announcement If you need more experiments to participate in, contact Danny Sanchez (dsanchez@ucsd.edu) make sure to tell him that you are from LIGN171, so he will let me know about your credit (1 point).

More information

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. Cortical Organization Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. 2. Secondary cortex: located immediately adjacent to primary cortical areas,

More information