HIPPOCAMPO CEREBELLAR THETA BAND PHASE SYNCHRONY IN RABBITS

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "HIPPOCAMPO CEREBELLAR THETA BAND PHASE SYNCHRONY IN RABBITS"

Transcription

1 Neuroscience 165 (2010) HIPPOCAMPO CEREBELLAR THETA BAND PHASE SYNCHRONY IN RABBITS J. WIKGREN,* 1 M. S. NOKIA 1 AND M. PENTTONEN Department of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 35, FIN 40014, Jyväskylä, Finland Abstract Hippocampal functioning, in the form of theta band oscillation, has been shown to modulate and predict cerebellar learning of which rabbit eyeblink conditioning is perhaps the most well-known example. The contribution of hippocampal neural activity to cerebellar learning is only possible if there is a functional connection between the two structures. Here, in the context of trace eyeblink conditioning, we show (1) that, in addition to the hippocampus, prominent theta oscillation also occurs in the cerebellum, and (2) that cerebellar theta oscillation is synchronized with that in the hippocampus. Further, the degree of phase synchrony (PS) increased both as a response to the conditioning stimuli and as a function of the relative power of hippocampal theta oscillation. However, the degree of PS did not change as a function of either training or learning nor did it predict learning rate as the hippocampal theta ratio did. Nevertheless, theta band synchronization might reflect the formation of transient neural assemblies between the hippocampus and the cerebellum. These findings help us understand how hippocampal function can affect eyeblink conditioning, during which the critical plasticity occurs in the cerebellum. Future studies should examine cerebellar unit activity in relation to hippocampal theta oscillations in order to discover the detailed mechanisms of theta-paced neural activity IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: hippocampus, cerebellum, theta, oscillation, eyeblink conditioning, phase synchrony. Common to all biologically meaningful learning is the engagement of multiple distinct phases and sub-processes that are governed by different, sometimes widely distributed, brain structures. Rabbit eyeblink conditioning (Gormezano et al., 1962), where a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS, e.g. a tone) is repeatedly paired with a reflex-eliciting unconditioned stimulus (US, e.g. a corneal airpuff) provides an example of a model system of learning which could be modified to tap into different aspects of the learning process. In this paradigm, the primary memory trace of the motor conditioned response (CR) is thought to be 1 J. Wikgren and M. Nokia both contributed equally. *Corresponding author. Tel: ; fax: address: jan.wikgren@jyu.fi (J. Wikgren). Abbreviations: ANOVA, analysis of variance; CS, conditioned stimulus; CR, conditioned response; EMG, electromyogram; FFT, fast fourier transform; HPC, hippocampus; HVI, cerebellar cortical area HVI; LFP, local-field potential; LTP/LTD, long-term potentiation/depression; mpfc, medial prefrontal cortex; PS, phase synchrony; US, unconditioned stimulus /10 $ - see front matter 2010 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi: /j.neuroscience stored within the cerebellar deep nuclei and/or the cerebellar cortical area HVI (Thompson and Steinmetz, 2009; De Zeeuw and Yeo, 2005), whereas various cerebral areas can modulate the learning process. For instance, when the cognitive demands of the task increase, hippocampal contribution becomes critical (Beylin et al., 2001; Moyer et al., 1990; Woodruff-Pak and Disterhoft, 2008). Hippocampal function, especially in the form of thetaband oscillation ( 6 Hz; for a review see, e.g., Bland, 1986 or Buzsáki, 2002), has been linked to a host of cognitive processes, most notably to learning and memory (Buzsáki, 2005; Hasselmo, 2005; Kahana, 2006). The relative power of the hippocampal theta activity (theta ratio) recorded before learning correlates strongly with the learning rate during subsequent eyeblink conditioning in rabbits, as shown both using the simple delay paradigm and using the trace paradigm, during which learning is hippocampally mediated (Berry and Thompson, 1978; Berry and Seager, 2001; Griffin et al., 2004; Nokia et al., 2008, 2009; Nokia and Wikgren, 2009; Seager et al., 2002). Moreover, blocking hippocampal theta oscillation with, for example, scopolamine injections before eyeblink conditioning retards behavioral learning and virtually abolishes any learningrelated unit responses in the hippocampus (Salvatierra and Berry, 1989). Previous research begs the questions, how do the hippocampus and the cerebellum interact, and what is the special role of the theta oscillation? In order for the hippocampus to modulate memory trace formation in the cerebellum we have to assume interaction between these areas. One indication of interaction between two brain areas is synchronized oscillation (e.g. Fries, 2005; Singer, 1999). By synchronizing and desynchronizing their activity, brain structures can select which inputs arrive and are sent during a time window of maximal effect, i.e. cause a change in the desired direction in the excitability of the receiving group of neurons (Fries, 2005). We recorded LFPs simultaneously from the rabbit hippocampus, cerebellar cortex, and medial prefrontal cortex (mpfc) during trace eyeblink conditioning and assessed the degree of phase synchrony (PS) (Palva et al., 2005; Lachaux et al., 1998) between these structures. Since synchronous oscillatory activity supposedly indicates co-operation, and because hippocampal theta oscillation has been associated with the learning rate during cerebellum-dependent eyeblink conditioning, we expected to observe (1) theta oscillation also in the cerebellum and (2) PS between the hippocampus and the cerebellum at the theta band. In addition, we aimed to determine whether the degree of PS changes as a function of training and/or learning and

2 J. Wikgren et al. / Neuroscience 165 (2010) whether, like the hippocampal theta ratio does, it predicts learning rate. EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES Subjects and surgery The subjects were 27 adult male New Zealand white rabbits (Harlan, Netherlands, BV, Horst, Netherlands) aged 4 months and weighing 2.7 kg at the time of surgery. The rabbits were housed in individual metal cages on the premises of the animal research unit of the University of Jyväskylä. Food and water were freely available, and room temperature and humidity were controlled. All procedures were conducted during the light portion of the 12/12 hour light/dark cycle and implemented in accordance with the European Communities Council Directive (86/609/EEC, uri CELEX: 31986L0609:EN:HTML) on the care and use of animals for research purposes. Every effort to minimize the number of animals and suffering caused to them was undertaken. For a detailed description of the surgery see Nokia et al. (2009). Two or three monopolar recording electrodes were chronically implanted into the right hippocampus 5 mm posterior and 4 6 mm lateral to the bregma and one electrode into the right cerebellar cortex lobule HVI 1 mm anterior and 5 mm lateral to the lambda. In addition, two electrodes were implanted into the left mpfc 4 5 mm anterior and 0.8 mm lateral to the bregma. The electrodes were attached to a pin connector and the whole construction cemented in place with dental acrylic. At least 1 week was allowed for postsurgical recovery. Stimuli and procedure Prior to the experiments, the rabbits were placed (for approximately 20 min) in a Plexiglas restraining box located in a ventilated, electrically insulated, and sound-attenuated conditioning chamber to familiarize them with the experimental situation, and to ensure the functioning of the implanted electrodes. Thereafter, sessions were conducted once per day on consecutive days. The CS was a 2 khz, 85 db, 200 ms tone and the US was a 100 ms corneal airpuff (0.35 bar source pressure, sound pressure level 64 db) delivered through a nozzle (inner diameter 2 mm) placed approximately 1 cm away from the eye. A fan located inside the conditioning chamber behind the rabbit created a steady background noise of approximately 65 db. E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA) was used to control the presentation of stimuli. The rabbits were randomly assigned to unpaired (n 10) and paired (n 17) groups. Both received 10 daily sessions of either trace eyeblink conditioning (paired) or explicitly unpaired training (unpaired). The unpaired sessions consisted of 70 CS-alone and 70 US-alone trials presented in a random order with an intertrial interval averaging out at 20 s (range, s). The conditioning sessions consisted of 80 trials: 60 conditioning, 10 CS-alone, and 10 US-alone trials were presented in a pseudorandom order with an average intertrial interval of 40 s (range, s). During the conditioning trials, CS onset preceded US onset by 700 ms, thus creating a 500 ms trace period. Recordings Eyeblinks were measured using stainless steel wire hooks placed around the upper and lower eyelids for the duration of the training session. To acquire neural measures, a low-noise pre-amplifier (MPA8I, Multi Channel Systems, Reutlingen, Germany) was directly attached to the electrode coupler anchored with dental acrylic to the rabbit s head. A flexible, insulated cable was used to connect the animal to the amplifier (Axon Cyberamp 380, Molecular Devices Corporation, Union City, CA, USA). Both the neural data and the bipolar electromyogram (EMG) were recorded with AxoScope (Molecular Devices Corporation) software and digitized (Digidata 1322A, Molecular Devices Corporation) using a khz sampling rate. Before digitization, the LFPs were band-passfiltered between 0.1 and 4000 Hz, and the EMG was filtered between 30 and 300 Hz. Data analyses Clampfit (Molecular Devices Corporation), MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA) and SPSS (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) were used for the data analyses. Eyeblinks. The EMG signal was further high-pass filtered over 100 Hz and Hilbert-transformed. Following this, an envelope curve following the peaks of the signal was calculated using the real and imaginary parts of the Hilbert transformation. Baseline EMG activity was calculated for each animal and session as the mean of the maximum EMG amplitude during a 500 ms prestimulus period (MAXpre). In addition, the mean of the standard deviation of the EMG activity during the 500 ms pre-stimulus period (SDpre) was determined. Eyeblinks were defined as EMG activity exceeding a threshold of [MAXpre 4 SDpre]. Blinks occurring during the 500-ms period following the offset of the tone (trace period) were counted as CRs. The learning rate was quantified as the number of trials needed to reach the 5th CR and the learning criterion. Phase synchrony calculations. To obtain comparable data from the control and experimental groups, only CS-alone and US-alone trials were included in the analyses. Phase synchrony (PS) between two signals was calculated as described by Palva et al. (2005). First, the LFP signals were band-pass filtered (delta 2 Hz, theta 6 Hz, alpha 12 Hz, beta 20 Hz, and gamma 60 Hz). Next, the filtered signals were transformed into a complex form using the Hilbert transform. Following this, the amplitudes of the signals were normalized to 1 (one) by dividing each data point by its absolute value. Then, the phase difference of the two signals in comparison was calculated by multiplying the first signal with the complex conjugate of the second signal (each data point of each trial). Finally, the PS was derived by averaging the phase difference matrix over trials, taking the absolute value. In order to standardize the PS values, 100 surrogate datasets per each real PS were created by shuffling the real trials. The PS was then determined for each surrogate dataset and the mean and the 95th percentile of the surrogate PSs derived. The mean of the surrogate PS values was subtracted from the real PS and the outcome divided point-by-point by the difference between the 95th percentile and the mean of the surrogate PSs ([realps surrps]/ [surr95th percentile surrps]) yielding a standardized PS. Thus standardized PS values exceeding 1 (one) represent statistically significant synchrony at the level of P.05. The group level statistical significance for the PS values was calculated using binomial statistics. Theta ratio. The FFT was calculated from the 1-s pre-stimulus period data, with a resolution of 0.5 Hz. The theta ratio was then calculated using (theta/[delta theta]). The delta and theta frequencies were used as the sole reference for theta, firstly, because in absolute power, delta and theta frequencies are fairly comparable and, secondly, because the absolute power of the higher frequencies (8 Hz ) is considerably lower than that of theta. To compare PS during periods of a high versus low hippocampal theta ratio, the tone-alone (10) and airpuff-alone (10) trials from each session (10) and each animal were sorted according to the hippocampal theta ratio calculated from the pre-stimulus period. Then, the trials with the lowest and highest theta ratio were selected to form sets of 10 trials per animal, pre-stimulus theta ratio level and stimulus type.

3 1540 J. Wikgren et al. / Neuroscience 165 (2010) Fig. 1. Recording electrodes were placed in the dorsal hippocampus, the cerebellar cortex lobule HVI, and in the medial prefrontal cortex (mpfc). All of the rabbits had at least one electrode in the hippocampal fissure or in the dentate gyrus/hilus and in the cerebellar cortex (A). In addition, 23 subjects had a recording electrode also in the mpfc (B). The open triangles refer to the subjects in the unpaired group and the filled circles to the subjects in the paired group. Statistical analyses. A paired samples t-test was used to compare pre- and post-stimulus PS values. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measures was used for the assessment of training and group effects. Pearson correlation coefficient was used when examining the connection between PS and learning rate. Histology After the experiments, the rabbits were anesthetized with an i.m. injection of ketamine xylazine cocktail and then overdosed with an i.v. injection of pentobarbital (Mebunat vet, Orion-Yhtymä Oyj, Espoo, Finland). A detailed description of the perfusion and histology procedure can be found in Nokia et al. (2009). The electrode-tip locations were determined from the stained slides with the help of a microscope and stereotaxic atlases (Bures et al., 1967; Lavond and Steinmetz, 2003). Histology RESULTS All 27 subjects had recording electrodes both in the hippocampus and in the cerebellar cortex lobule HVI (Fig. 1), and 23 also had an electrode in the mpfc (Fig. 1B). was low (Fig. 3B, D), the cerebellar theta oscillation was also weak. Phase synchrony is specific to hippocampo cerebellar theta band oscillation To assess the degree of PS between the hippocampus and the cerebellum and between the hippocampus and the mpfc without contamination from the possible effects of learning, the combined paired and unpaired group data from only the first session was analyzed. Binomial test (P.01) showed significant PS only in the theta band ( 6 Hz) oscillation occurring simultaneously in the hippocampus and in the cerebellar cortex (Fig. 4A, C). In response to external stimulation, hippocampo cerebellar theta band PS was further increased (tone: t(26) 5.67, P.001; airpuff: t(26) 5.47, P.001; binomial test P.001) (Fig. 4A). The explicit lack of hippocampo cerebellar PS was evident in all the other frequency bands (delta, alpha, beta, and gamma) and during all three conditions (pre-stimulus, Behavioral results The percentage of CRs (Fig. 2) increased across paired treatment but remained stable across unpaired treatment (block: F [4,100] 17.71, P.001; group: F [1,25] 11.25, P.01; block group: F [4,100] 4.91, P.01; paired group, block: F [4,64] 22.45, P.001; unpaired group, block: F [4,36] 3.27, ns). Theta ( 6 Hz) oscillation in the rabbit cerebellum A clear theta band oscillation visible to the naked eye and verified by FFT was found in the cerebellar cortex (lobule HVI, Fig. 3). FFT showed that when the hippocampal theta ratio was high (Fig. 3A, C), the cerebellar theta oscillation was also strong, and when the hippocampal theta ratio Fig. 2. Conditioned responding increased in the paired but not in the unpaired group.

4 J. Wikgren et al. / Neuroscience 165 (2010) Fig. 3. The power of the theta-band activity was high/low in the cerebellum (HVI) when it was high/low in the hippocampus (HPC). The results of fast fourier transform (FFT, upper panels) run on hippocampal and cerebellar LFPs (lower panels, representative examples) during periods of high relative power of hippocampal theta activity (theta/[delta theta]) are shown in A (C) and during low relative power of hippocampal theta activity in B (D). tone, and airpuff), as shown by binomial test (P.001) (Fig. 4A). No statistically significant PS was found between the hippocampus and the mpfc (Fig. 4B). In fact, the binomial test showed a significant lack of PS in all bands (P.05/.001). Hippocampo cerebellar theta band PS remains statistically significant across training, but decreases slightly Significant main effects of training block (5) and condition (3) were found when the hippocampo cerebellar PS during the pre-stimulus period and in response to the tone and airpuff across training was examined in both the unpaired group (block: F [4,108] 7.85, P.001; condition: F [1,27] 4.36, P.05) and paired group (block: F [4,192] 7.51, P.001; condition: F [1,48] 16.24, P.001) (Fig. 5). In both groups, hippocampo cerebellar PS was higher in response to stimuli compared to the pre-stimulus period, but decreased linearly across the training. The effect of training type (unpaired vs. paired) and learning (fast vs. slow learners) was also tested, but no connections were found. In addition, no significant correlations were found between hippocampo cerebellar PS and learning rate (trials to 5th CR and trials to criterion) in the paired group (n 17), although a clear predictive connection was found between the hippocampal theta ratio recorded during the first session and the number of trials needed to reach the 5th CR (pre-cs theta ratio 5th CR: r.48, P.05 and post-cs theta ratio 5th CR: r.63, P.01). Based on the lack of significant group differences, data from the paired (n 17) and unpaired (n 10) groups were again combined (n 27) for further analyses. Phase synchrony is stronger during episodes of a high hippocampal theta ratio but increases in response to external stimuli irrespective of the on-going theta state When the trials were divided into two classes based on the degree of the hippocampal theta ratio (high vs. low) during the absence of stimulation (pre-stimulus period), the PS between the hippocampus and cerebellar cortex was statistically significant only when the hippocampal theta ratio was high (binomial test P.001) (Fig. 4D, E). However, regardless of whether the hippocampal theta ratio was high or low (i.e. the PS during the pre-stimulus period was significant or not), the presentation of the tone or the airpuff always elicited significant theta band PS between the hip-

5 1542 J. Wikgren et al. / Neuroscience 165 (2010) Fig. 4. Statistically significant phase synchrony (PS) was found only between the hippocampus and the cerebellar cortex and only in the theta-band ( 6 Hz). Binomial statistics showed significant PS only in the theta oscillation occurring simultaneously in the hippocampus and in the cerebellar cortex (A). In response to external stimulation, this hippocampo cerebellar theta-band PS was increased. No statistically significant PS was found between the hippocampus and the mpfc (B). The hippocampo cerebellar theta-band PS increased in response to both stimuli (tone and airpuff, A and C), irrespective of the relative power of the hippocampal theta oscillation (low vs. high theta) immediately preceding stimulus onset (D and E). The PS calculated from the real data are shown in the uppermost panel of each subplot, the PS calculated from the surrogate data obtained by shuffling real data are shown in the middle panel, and the standardized PS is shown in the lowest panel. The vertical lines indicate the onset of the stimulus. The asterisk reflects statistical significance of paired samples t-tests: *** P.001. The horizontal dotted line shows the threshold value of 1 (one) corresponding to statistical significance at the level of P.05. pocampus and the cerebellar cortex (binomial test P.001) (Fig. 4D, E). DISCUSSION The two main findings of this study were (1) a prominent theta band oscillation in the cerebellar cortex of the rabbit and (2) an increased and high degree of PS between the theta band oscillation in the cerebellum and that in the hippocampus during the presentation of external stimuli. Hippocampo cerebellar PS was especially strong when the hippocampal theta ratio was high, but presenting the animal with external stimuli increased the theta band PS regardless of the ongoing theta ratio immediately preceding the stimulus presentation. However, we did not observe any changes in the hippocampo cerebellar theta band PS specific to paired training or learning, nor did we detect any correlation between the level of spontaneous hippocampo cerebellar theta-band PS and learning rate. To our knowledge, this is the first report on theta oscillation ( 6 Hz) in the rabbit cerebellum. In rats, D Angelo et al. (2001) have shown that granule cells in the cerebellar cortex exhibit bursting and resonance at the theta frequency range (3 12 Hz). Such rhythmicity might play an important role in cerebellum-mediated learning. Indeed, they have also shown that long-term potentiation/depression (LTP/LTD) is present in the mossy fiber-granule cell synapses in the cerebellum when high-frequency stimulation of the former is paired with membrane depolarization in the latter (D Angelo et al., 1999; Armano et al., 2000; Mapelli and D Angelo, 2007). Whereas the relative magni-

6 J. Wikgren et al. / Neuroscience 165 (2010) Fig. 5. The overall level of hippocampo cerebellar PS decreased across training, but remained higher in response to external stimuli compared to the pre-stimulus period at all times in both the unpaired (A) and paired (B) group. tude of the hippocampal theta band oscillation affects the behavioral learning rate and learning-related unit activity during eyeblink conditioning (see, for example, Nokia et al., 2008), the phase of hippocampal theta oscillation affects synaptic plasticity when LTP procedures are used (Huerta and Lisman, 1995; Hölscher et al., 1997). Stimulating the hippocampus via the Schaffer collaterals during the positive phase of hippocampal CA1 theta oscillation produced LTP, whereas stimulation given during the negative phase reversed the previously established LTP (Huerta and Lisman, 1995; Hölscher et al., 1997). In sum, the responsiveness and plasticity of hippocampal output, as well as that of the cerebellar input neurons, is synchronized by theta oscillation, now also shown to exist in the cerebellar LFPs. Not only did our present study show a theta oscillation in the cerebellar LFPs but it also demonstrated that the theta oscillation in the cerebellar cortex is in concert with that in the hippocampus, as indicated by the significant PS. Synchronous activity is thought to reflect the formation of transient functional assemblies by anatomically distant brain structures (Fries, 2005). Related to the role of the hippocampal theta oscillation in learning and memory, in healthy humans undergoing tasks requiring working memory, coherent activity at the theta-band between different cortical sites has been observed by recording electroencephalogram (EEG; for a review, see Sauseng and Klimesch, 2008). In addition, in an early experiment by Livanov (1977), visual stimulation caused limb movements at a probability increasing along with the degree of coherence between EEG signals measured above the visual and the motor cortex in rabbits. Similarly, Schoffelen et al. (2005) showed that subjects readiness to respond in a simple reaction-time task closely correlated with the degree of gamma-band coherence between neurons in the motor cortex and spinal cord. Given that neural plasticity in both the output area of the hippocampus (Huerta and Lisman, 1995; Hölscher et al., 1997) and in the input area of the cerebellar cortex (D Angelo et al., 1999; Armano et al., 2000; Mapelli and D Angelo, 2007) are related to the phase of the theta cycle, and that theta oscillations in both areas occur in a fixed temporal relation to each other, it is likely that hippocampo cerebellar theta band PS is relevant to neural communication between these structures. It is also to be noted that presenting a stimulus to the animal actually decreased the amount of theta band synchrony for a brief time period. This observation conforms to the theory of large-scale integration of neural activity (Rodriguez et al., 1999) which states that transiently formed cognitive states are separated by brief moments of desynchronization, i.e. active uncoupling of neural activity. The hippocampo cerebellar theta band PS observed in our study suggests the formation of transient neural assemblies between the structures, but, in the absence of accompanying single-unit analyses, unfortunately is not able to demonstrate it. However, evidence from other lines of research indicates that hippocampal theta oscillation might be the pacemaker for neuronal activity in learningrelevant regions. For instance, neuronal activity in prefrontal regions becomes synchronized with hippocampal theta in the early phase of appetitively motivated trace conditioning (Paz et al., 2008). Similarly, Berke et al. (2004) found coherent theta oscillations in the hippocampal CA1 and the ventral/medial striatum, and showed that the striatal neurons tended to fire more frequently during the descending phase of hippocampal theta oscillation, thus manifesting a link between hippocampal theta and motor areas relevant in consummatory behavior. Furthermore, Sirota et al. (2008; see also Sirota and Buzsáki (2005) recently suggested that oscillatory synchrony might be the mechanism through which the information flow between the hippocampus and the neocortex is controlled. There is also evidence in anesthetized animals that cells in the neocortex are synchronized with cells in the cerebellum, albeit not at the theta band (Ros et al., 2009). Thus, further studies are needed to investigate the possible connections between the hippocampo cerebellar theta oscillations and unit-activity in the cerebellar cortex/deep nuclei. Much to our surprise, we found no connection between hippocampo-cerebellar theta band PS and learning. This is at odds with previous studies, which have shown a clear predictive connection between the relative magnitude of hippocampal theta oscillation and learning rate, as well as changes across training/learning during trace eyeblink

7 1544 J. Wikgren et al. / Neuroscience 165 (2010) conditioning (Griffin et al., 2004; Nokia et al., 2009). This difference in results could be explained by the difference in the nature of the analyses used: in the present study the phase of theta oscillation was examined whereas in previous studies analyses were targeted at the relative amplitude of theta oscillation. It is possible that hippocampo cerebellar theta band PS is a fundamental and rather stable mediator of communication between the two structures, and thus does not show changes across training/ learning. In other words, the pattern of activation leading to synchronized activity and mediating efficient communication remains stable, whereas learning-related changes occur, for instance in the number of cerebellar neurons firing in relation to the theta band oscillation. It could well be that cerebellar unit activity either increases during periods of strong hippocampal theta oscillation, or becomes more sharply synchronized with it. Further investigation of this matter is needed. Another explanation for the lack of learning-related changes in PS between the cerebellar cortex and hippocampus in the present study could be that the cerebellar cortex actually does not critically contribute to this form of learning. For instance, it has been shown that impaired cerebellar cortical function has a detrimental effect specifically on delay but not trace eyeblink conditioning in mice (Kishimoto et al., 2001; Woodruff-Pak et al., 2006). As hippocampal function modulates learning also during delay conditioning, replicating the present study but using the delay conditioning paradigm instead might, therefore, result in changes in PS as a function of training or learning. In relation to more technical issues, it might be argued that the hippocampo cerebellar theta band PS observed in this study resulted from volume conduction. However, PS was seen only in the theta band, and only between the hippocampus and the cerebellum, and not between the hippocampus and the mpfc (which resides spatially closer to the hippocampus), making it unlikely that volume conduction could explain the results. It is more probable, since the rhythm of hippocampal theta oscillation is in part generated in the medial septum-diagonal band of Broca (see Buzsáki, 2002), that it also plays a part in pacing cerebellar theta oscillation. In the knowledge that both the hippocampus and the cerebellum are connected with the neocortex, it might be suggested that it provides a link between them. Whether this is the case and, if so, through which pathways, remains, however, yet to be determined. CONCLUSION In conclusion, the theta band PS found here between the hippocampus and the cerebellum might reflect a mechanism by which these two structures can interact efficiently. Via theta band PS the hippocampus might transiently form neural assemblies with its target area in the cerebellum, possibly enhancing synaptic modification and thus learning. In future studies it would be of importance to investigate whether firing in the cerebellar neurons is also synchronized with the phase of hippocampal theta oscillation. Showing synchrony between cerebellar plasticity and hippocampal theta oscillation would provide strong evidence for the functional significance of cerebellar theta oscillation. Acknowledgments Supported by grants from the Academy of Finland to JW ( and ) and from the Emil Aaltonen foundation to MSN (Young researcher s grant). The authors wish to thank Matias Palva for advice on phase synchrony calculation and Michael Dutton and Michael Freeman for language checking. REFERENCES Armano S, Rossi P, Taglietti V, D Angelo E (2000) Long-term potentiation of intrinsic excitability at the mossy fiber granule cell synapse of rat cerebellum. J Neurosci 20: Berke JD, Okatan M, Skurski J, Eichenbaum HB (2004) Oscillatory entrainment of striatal neurons in freely moving rats. Neuron 43: Berry SD, Seager MA (2001) Hippocampal theta oscillations and classical conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 76: Berry SD, Thompson RF (1978) Prediction of learning rate from the hippocampal electroencephalogram. Science 200: Beylin AV, Gandhi CC, Wood GE, Talk AC, Matzel LD, Shors TJ (2001) The role of the hippocampus in trace conditioning: temporal discontinuity or task difficulty? Neurobiol Learn Mem 76: Bland BH (1986) The physiology and pharmacology of hippocampal formation theta rhythms. Prog Neurobiol 26:1 54. Bures J, Petran M, Zachar J (1967) Electrophysiological methods in biological research. New York: Academic Press. Buzsáki G (2002) Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron 33: Buzsáki G (2005) Theta rhythm of navigation: link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus 15: D Angelo E, Rossi P, Armano S, Taglietti V (1999) Evidence for NMDA and mglu receptor-dependent long-term potentiation of mossy fiber granule cell transmission in rat cerebellum. J Neurophysiol 81: D Angelo E, Nieus T, Maffei A, Armano S, Rossi P, Taglietti V, Fontana A, Naldi G (2001) Theta-frequency bursting and resonance in cerebellar granule cells: experimental evidence and modeling of a slow K -dependent mechanism. J Neurosci 21: De Zeeuw CI, Yeo CH (2005) Time and tide in cerebellar memory trace formation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15: Fries P (2005) A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci 9: Gormezano I, Schneiderman N, Deaux E, Fuentes I (1962) Nictitating membrane: classical conditioning and extinction in the albino rabbit. Science 138: Griffin AL, Asaka Y, Darling RD, Berry SD (2004) Theta-contingent trial presentation accelerates learning rate and enhances hippocampal plasticity during trace eyeblink conditioning. Behav Neurosci 118: Hasselmo ME (2005) What is the function of hippocampal theta rhythm? linking behavioral data to phasic properties of field potential and unit recording data. Hippocampus 15: Hölscher C, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ (1997) Stimulation on the positive phase of the hippocampal theta rhythm induces long-term potentiation that can be depotentiated by stimulation on the negative phase in area CA1 in vivo. J Neurosci 17: Huerta PT, Lisman JE (1995) Bidirectional synaptic plasticity induced by a single burst during cholinergic theta oscillation in CA1 in vitro. Neuron 15: Kahana MJ (2006) The cognitive correlates of human brain oscillations. J Neurosci 26: Kishimoto Y, Hirono M, Sugiyama T, Kawahara S, Nakao K, Kishio M, Katsuki M, Yoshioka T, Kirino Y (2001) Impaired delay but normal

8 J. Wikgren et al. / Neuroscience 165 (2010) trace eyeblink conditioning in PLCbeta4 mutant mice. Neuroreport 12: Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J, Varela FJ (1998) Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp 8: Lavond DG, Steinmetz JE (2003) Handbook of classical conditioning. London: Springer. Livanov MN (1977) Spatial organization of cerebral processes. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Mapelli J, D Angelo E (2007) The spatial organization of long-term synaptic plasticity at the input stage of the cerebellum. J Neurosci 27: Moyer JR Jr, Deyo RA, Disterhoft JF (1990) Hippocampectomy disrupts trace eye-blink conditioning in rabbits. Behav Neurosci 104: Nokia MS, Penttonen M, Korhonen T, Wikgren J (2008) Hippocampal theta (3 8 Hz) activity during classical eyeblink conditioning in rabbits. Neurobiol Learn Mem 90: Nokia MS, Penttonen M, Korhonen T, Wikgren J (2009) Hippocampal theta band activity and trace eyeblink conditioning in rabbits. Behav Neurosci 123: Nokia MS, Wikgren J (in press) Hippocampal theta activity is selectively associated with contingency detection but not discrimination in rabbit discrimination-reversal eyeblink conditioning. Hippocampus. Palva JM, Palva S, Kaila K (2005) Phase synchrony among neuronal oscillations in the human cortex. J Neurosci 25: Paz R, Bauer EP, Paré D (2008) Theta synchronizes the activity of medial prefrontal neurons during learning. Learn Mem 15: Rodriguez E, George N, Lachaux JP, Martinerie J, Renault B, Varela FJ (1999) Perception s shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity. Nature 397: Ros H, Sachdev RN, Yu Y, Sestan N, MacCormick DA (2009) Neocortical networks entrain neuronal circuits in cerebellar cortex. J Neurosci 29: Salvatierra AT, Berry SD (1989) Scopolamine disruption of septohippocampal activity and classical conditioning. Behav Neurosci 103: Sauseng P, Klimesch W (2008) What does phase information of oscillatory brain activity tell us about cognitive processes? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32: Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Fries P (2005) Neuronal coherence as a mechanism of effective corticospinal interaction. Science 308: Seager MA, Johnson LD, Chabot ES, Asaka Y, Berry SD (2002) Oscillatory brain states and learning: impact of hippocampal thetacontingent training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99: Singer W (1999) Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron 24: Sirota A, Buzsáki G (2005) Interaction between neocortical and hippocampal networks via slow oscillations. Thalamus Relat Syst 3: Sirota A, Montgomery S, Fujisawa S, Isomura Y, Zugaro M, Buzsáki G (2008) Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm. Neuron 60: Thompson RF, Steinmetz JE (2009) The role of the cerebellum in classical conditioning of discrete behavioral responses. Neuroscience 162: Woodruff-Pak DS, Disterhoft JF (2008) Where is the trace in trace conditioning? Trends Neurosci 31: Woodruff-Pak DS, Green JT, Levin ST, Meisler MH (2006) Inactivation of sodium channel Scn8A (Na-sub(v)1.6) in Purkinje neurons impairs learning in Morris water maze and delay but not trace eyeblink classical conditioning. Behav Neurosci 120: (Accepted 18 November 2009) (Available online 27 November 2009)

Disrupting neural activity related to awake-state sharp wave-ripple complexes prevents hippocampal learning

Disrupting neural activity related to awake-state sharp wave-ripple complexes prevents hippocampal learning BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 04 December 2012 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00084 Disrupting neural activity related to awake-state sharp wave-ripple complexes prevents hippocampal

More information

The Role of the Hippocampal Theta Activity in Classical Eyeblink Conditioning in Rabbits

The Role of the Hippocampal Theta Activity in Classical Eyeblink Conditioning in Rabbits JYVÄSKYLÄ STUDIES IN EDUCATION, PSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL RESEARCH 371 Miriam Nokia The Role of the Hippocampal Theta Activity in Classical Eyeblink Conditioning in Rabbits JYVÄSKYLÄ STUDIES IN EDUCATION,

More information

Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Grid Cell The Hippocampus Serves a Role in Multimodal Information Processing and Memory

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Parallel Acquisition of Awareness and Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning

Parallel Acquisition of Awareness and Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Research Parallel Acquisition of Awareness and Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Joseph R. Manns, 1 Robert E. Clark, 2 and Larry R. Squire 3,4,5 1 Department of Psychology, University of California,

More information

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction Instructor: Paul Wolfgang Faculty sponsor: Yuan Shi, Ph.D. Andrey Mavrichev CIS 4339 Project in Computer Science May 7, 2009 Research work was completed in collaboration with Michael Tobia, Kevin L. Brown,

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

Following the seminal proposal of Hebb1 and many others, acquired learning

Following the seminal proposal of Hebb1 and many others, acquired learning Associative Learning and Long-Term Potentiation in Rodents: José M. Delgado-, MD, PhD Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) and Learning-Dependent Changes in Synaptic Strength Following the seminal proposal of

More information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Table 1. Quantitative features of EC neuron dendrites

Supplementary Information Supplementary Table 1. Quantitative features of EC neuron dendrites Supplementary Information Supplementary Table 1. Quantitative features of EC neuron dendrites Supplementary Table 2. Quantitative features of EC neuron axons 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Layer distribution

More information

ABSTRACT CEREBELLAR THETA OSCILLATIONS ARE SYNCHRONIZED DURING HIPPOCAMPAL THETA-CONTINGENT TRACE CONDITIONING. by Loren C.

ABSTRACT CEREBELLAR THETA OSCILLATIONS ARE SYNCHRONIZED DURING HIPPOCAMPAL THETA-CONTINGENT TRACE CONDITIONING. by Loren C. ABSTRACT CEREBELLAR THETA OSCILLATIONS ARE SYNCHRONIZED DURING HIPPOCAMPAL THETA-CONTINGENT TRACE CONDITIONING by Loren C. Hoffmann The hippocampus and cerebellum are critically involved in trace eyeblink

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing

More information

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) Introduction 17 subjects were tested to measure effects of a Brain.fm focus session on cognition. With 4 additional subjects, we recorded EEG data during baseline and while

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival Threats are much better reinforcers Fear is a prime

More information

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories? CASE 49 A 43-year-old woman is brought to her primary care physician by her family because of concerns about her forgetfulness. The patient has a history of Down syndrome but no other medical problems.

More information

A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal

A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal Published in final edited form as: Trends in Cognitive Science 9(10), 474-480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011 A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence Pascal

More information

Classical conditioning using stimulation of the inferior olive as the unconditioned stimulus

Classical conditioning using stimulation of the inferior olive as the unconditioned stimulus Proc. Nati. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 83, pp. 5349-5353, July 1986 Psychology Classical conditioning using stimulation of the inferior olive as the unconditioned stimulus (learning/neural plasticity/acquisition/extinction/rabbit)

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1 SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Figure 1 The supralinear events evoked in CA3 pyramidal cells fulfill the criteria for NMDA spikes, exhibiting a threshold, sensitivity to NMDAR blockade, and all-or-none

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

Supplementary Figure 1

Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Miniature microdrive, spike sorting and sleep stage detection. a, A movable recording probe with 8-tetrodes (32-channels). It weighs ~1g. b, A mouse implanted with 8 tetrodes in

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may have as many as 200,000

More information

Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals

Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals Supplementary Figure S1: Histological analysis of kainate-treated animals Nissl stained coronal or horizontal sections were made from kainate injected (right) and saline injected (left) animals at different

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics

From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics a Delorme A., a Makeig, S., b Fabre-Thorpe, M., a Sejnowski, T. a The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, 10010 N. Torey Pines Road, La Jolla, CA92109, USA

More information

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25 Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP Reading: BCP Chapter 25 Memory Systems Learning is the acquisition of new knowledge or skills. Memory is the retention of learned information. Many different

More information

Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms. Reading: BCP Chapter 19

Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms. Reading: BCP Chapter 19 Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms Reading: BCP Chapter 19 Brain Rhythms and Sleep Earth has a rhythmic environment. For example, day and night cycle back and forth, tides ebb and flow and temperature varies

More information

The role of phase synchronization in memory processes

The role of phase synchronization in memory processes The role of phase synchronization in memory processes Juergen Fell and Nikolai Axmacher Abstract In recent years, studies ranging from single-unit recordings in animals to electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography

More information

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition Chair: Jason Samaha, University of Wisconsin-Madison Co-Chair: Ali Mazaheri, University of Birmingham

More information

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Christopher J. Rozell, Don. H. Johnson and Raymon M. Glantz Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering Department of

More information

Introduction to Electrophysiology

Introduction to Electrophysiology Introduction to Electrophysiology Dr. Kwangyeol Baek Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging Massachusetts General Hospital Harvard Medical School 2018-05-31s Contents Principles in Electrophysiology Techniques

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Large-scale calcium imaging in vivo.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Large-scale calcium imaging in vivo. Supplementary Figure 1 Large-scale calcium imaging in vivo. (a) Schematic illustration of the in vivo camera imaging set-up for large-scale calcium imaging. (b) High-magnification two-photon image from

More information

In review. Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm

In review. Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm Tracy L. Greer 1*, Lucien T. Thompson 2* 1 Department of Psychiatry, UT Southwestern Medical Center,

More information

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG Educational Symposium, MEG UK 2014, Nottingham, Jan 8th 2014 Krish Singh CUBRIC, School of Psychology Cardiff University What are we trying to achieve? Bridge the gap from

More information

Theta Rhythmic Stimulation of Stratum Lacunosum-Moleculare in Rat Hippocampus Contributes to Associative LTP at a Phase Offset in Stratum Radiatum

Theta Rhythmic Stimulation of Stratum Lacunosum-Moleculare in Rat Hippocampus Contributes to Associative LTP at a Phase Offset in Stratum Radiatum J Neurophysiol 92: 1615 1624, 2004. First published May 5, 2004; 10.1152/jn.00848.2003. Theta Rhythmic Stimulation of Stratum Lacunosum-Moleculare in Rat Hippocampus Contributes to Associative LTP at a

More information

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings J. Besle, P. Lakatos, C.A. Schevon, R.R. Goodman, G.M. McKhann, A. Mehta, R.G. Emerson, C.E.

More information

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Asymmetry in learning in the reverse direction Full recovery from UP using DOWN: initial return to naïve values within 10 minutes,

More information

Cerebellar Cortex Lesions Prevent Acquisition of Conditioned Eyelid Responses

Cerebellar Cortex Lesions Prevent Acquisition of Conditioned Eyelid Responses The Journal of Neuroscience, December 15, 1999, 19(24):10940 10947 Cerebellar Cortex Lesions Prevent Acquisition of Conditioned Eyelid Responses Keith S. Garcia, Philip M. Steele, and Michael D. Mauk Department

More information

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Yingxue Wang, Sandro Romani, Brian Lustig, Anthony Leonardo, Eva Pastalkova Supplementary Materials Supplementary Modeling

More information

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves SICE Annual Conference 27 Sept. 17-2, 27, Kagawa University, Japan Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves Seiji Nishifuji 1, Kentaro Fujisaki 1 and Shogo Tanaka 1 1

More information

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT 1 Courtesy of Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.

More information

What do you notice?

What do you notice? What do you notice? https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=5v5ebf2kwf8&t=143s Models for communication between neuron groups Communication through neuronal coherence Neuron Groups 1 and 3 send projections to

More information

Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex

Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex Koji Yashiro, Thorfinn T. Riday, Kathryn H. Condon, Adam C. Roberts, Danilo R. Bernardo, Rohit Prakash, Richard J. Weinberg, Michael

More information

Bursting dynamics in the brain. Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST

Bursting dynamics in the brain. Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST Bursting dynamics in the brain Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST Tonic and phasic activity A neuron is said to exhibit a tonic activity when it fires a series of single action potentials

More information

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala 11 December 1997 Nature 390, 604-607 (1997) Macmillan Publishers Ltd. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala MICHAEL T. ROGAN, URSULA V. STÄUBLI & JOSEPH E. LEDOUX

More information

Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo

Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo Population coding in the motor cortex Overview and structure of cerebellum Microcircuitry of cerebellum Function of cerebellum -- vestibulo-ocular

More information

Aging and Learning-Specific Changes in Single-Neuron Activity in CA1 Hippocampus During Rabbit Trace Eyeblink Conditioning

Aging and Learning-Specific Changes in Single-Neuron Activity in CA1 Hippocampus During Rabbit Trace Eyeblink Conditioning Aging and Learning-Specific Changes in Single-Neuron Activity in CA1 Hippocampus During Rabbit Trace Eyeblink Conditioning MATTHEW D. MCECHRON, ALDIS P. WEIBLE, AND JOHN F. DISTERHOFT Department of Cell

More information

Supplemental Information. A Visual-Cue-Dependent Memory Circuit. for Place Navigation

Supplemental Information. A Visual-Cue-Dependent Memory Circuit. for Place Navigation Neuron, Volume 99 Supplemental Information A Visual-Cue-Dependent Memory Circuit for Place Navigation Han Qin, Ling Fu, Bo Hu, Xiang Liao, Jian Lu, Wenjing He, Shanshan Liang, Kuan Zhang, Ruijie Li, Jiwei

More information

Theta-Contingent Trial Presentation Accelerates Learning Rate and Enhances Hippocampal Plasticity During Trace Eyeblink Conditioning

Theta-Contingent Trial Presentation Accelerates Learning Rate and Enhances Hippocampal Plasticity During Trace Eyeblink Conditioning Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 118, No. 2, 403 411 0735-7044/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.403 Theta-Contingent Trial Presentation

More information

Physiology & Behavior

Physiology & Behavior Physiology & Behavior 96 (2009) 399 411 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Physiology & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/phb Associative and non-associative blinking in classically

More information

Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior

Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior Diana Ponce-Morado, Venmathi Gunasekaran and Varsha Vijayan Abstract The hippocampus is identified as an important structure in the cerebral cortex of mammals for forming

More information

THE EFFECT OF TETANIC STIMULATION ON FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY

THE EFFECT OF TETANIC STIMULATION ON FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY REPORT THE EFFECT OF TETANIC STIMULATION ON FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY Tim Witteveen FACULTY OF ELECTRICAL ENGINEERING, MATHEMATICS AND COMPUTER SCIENCE BIOMEDICAL SIGNALS AND SYSTEMS EXAMINATION COMMITTEE

More information

Spectral Analysis of EEG Patterns in Normal Adults

Spectral Analysis of EEG Patterns in Normal Adults Spectral Analysis of EEG Patterns in Normal Adults Kyoung Gyu Choi, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Neurology, Ewha Medical Research Center, Ewha Womans University Medical College, Background: Recently, the

More information

Thalamocortical Synchronization and Cognition: Implications for Schizophrenia?

Thalamocortical Synchronization and Cognition: Implications for Schizophrenia? Published in final edited form as: Neuron 77(6), 997 999. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.033 Thalamocortical Synchronization and Cognition: Implications for Schizophrenia? Peter J. Uhlhaas

More information

File name: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Table and Supplementary References

File name: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Table and Supplementary References File name: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Table and Supplementary References File name: Supplementary Data 1 Description: Summary datasheets showing the spatial

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) Hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Resonant synchronization of heterogeneous inhibitory networks

Resonant synchronization of heterogeneous inhibitory networks Cerebellar oscillations: Anesthetized rats Transgenic animals Recurrent model Review of literature: γ Network resonance Life simulations Resonance frequency Conclusion Resonant synchronization of heterogeneous

More information

Strick Lecture 3 March 22, 2017 Page 1

Strick Lecture 3 March 22, 2017 Page 1 Strick Lecture 3 March 22, 2017 Page 1 Cerebellum OUTLINE I. External structure- Inputs and Outputs Cerebellum - (summary diagram) 2 components (cortex and deep nuclei)- (diagram) 3 Sagittal zones (vermal,

More information

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING Pergamon Neuroscience Vol. 83, No. 3, pp. 837 841, 1998 Copyright 1998 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved PII: S0306-4522(97)00457-0 0306 4522/98 $19.00+0.00

More information

Temporal patterning of neural synchrony in the basal ganglia in Parkinson s disease

Temporal patterning of neural synchrony in the basal ganglia in Parkinson s disease Temporal patterning of neural synchrony in the basal ganglia in Parkinson s disease Shivakeshavan Ratnadurai-Giridharan 1, S. Elizabeth Zauber 2, Robert M. Worth 1,3, Thomas Witt 3, Sungwoo Ahn 1,5, Leonid

More information

Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses

Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses NOTE Communicated by Laurence Abbott Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses W. Senn Department of Neurobiology, Hebrew University, Jerusalem 4, Israel, Department of Physiology, University

More information

Modeling the Impact of Recurrent Collaterals In CA3 on Downstream CA1 Firing Frequency

Modeling the Impact of Recurrent Collaterals In CA3 on Downstream CA1 Firing Frequency Modeling the Impact of Recurrent Collaterals In CA3 on Downstream CA1 Firing Frequency 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Teryn Johnson Gladys Ornelas Srihita Rudraraju t8johnso@eng.ucsd.edu glornela@eng.ucsd.edu srrudrar@eng.ucsd.edu

More information

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013 Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013 Summary of EEG findings The intent of the EEG monitoring was to see which

More information

Links Between Single-Trial Changes and Learning Rate in Eyelid Conditioning

Links Between Single-Trial Changes and Learning Rate in Eyelid Conditioning Cerebellum () : DOI.7/s--9-8 ORIGINAL PAPER Links Between Single-Trial Changes and Learning Rate in Eyelid Conditioning Andrei Khilkevich & Hunter E. Halverson & Jose Ernesto Canton-Josh & Michael D. Mauk,

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 7: Network models Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis II 6 Single neuron

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) The hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Eyeblink Conditioning As Models of Declarative and Non-declarative Memories

Eyeblink Conditioning As Models of Declarative and Non-declarative Memories Review Eyeblink Conditioning As Models of Declarative and Non-declarative Memories Narawut Pakaprot Abstract Learning and memory are the crucial cognitive functions, which equip animals for the ability

More information

Supplementary Methods

Supplementary Methods 1 Supplementary Methods Social Preference Test Subjects Seventy-four Long-Evans, male rats served as subjects (S-rats) in the foodpreference test, with 40 assigned to the CXT-Same (CXT-S) Condition and

More information

Long-Term Plasticity Is Proportional to Theta-Activity

Long-Term Plasticity Is Proportional to Theta-Activity Long-Term Plasticity Is Proportional to Theta-Activity Marian Tsanov 1,2, Denise Manahan-Vaughan 1,2 * 1 Department of Experimental Neurophysiology, Medical Faculty, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany,

More information

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE BRAIN The central nervous system (CNS), consisting of the brain and spinal cord, receives input from sensory neurons and directs

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more strongly when an image was negative than when the same

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

11/2/2011. Basic circuit anatomy (the circuit is the same in all parts of the cerebellum)

11/2/2011. Basic circuit anatomy (the circuit is the same in all parts of the cerebellum) 11/2/2011 Neuroscientists have been attracted to the puzzle of the Cerebellum ever since Cajal. The orderly structure, the size of the cerebellum and the regularity of the neural elements demands explanation.

More information

Relationships between Spike Train Spatiotemporal Structure and Cortical Synchronization

Relationships between Spike Train Spatiotemporal Structure and Cortical Synchronization Relationships between Spike Train Spatiotemporal Structure and Cortical Synchronization Jason M. Samonds 1, Heather A. Brown 1, A.B. Bonds 1,2 Vanderbilt University, Nashville TN, USA 37235 1 Department

More information

Objectives. brain pacemaker circuits role of inhibition

Objectives. brain pacemaker circuits role of inhibition Brain Rhythms Michael O. Poulter, Ph.D. Professor, Molecular Brain Research Group Robarts Research Institute Depts of Physiology & Pharmacology, Clinical Neurological Sciences Schulich School of Medicine

More information

Protocol for Rat Sleep EEG

Protocol for Rat Sleep EEG Protocol for Rat Sleep EEG Subjects Male Spraue Dawley rats weihin 250-300 rams at the time of surery are used. Food and water are available ad libitum throuhout the experiment. Rats are roup housed prior

More information

Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex

Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex The Journal of Neuroscience, January 15, 2001, 21(2):682 690 Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex Tatsuya Ohyama and Michael D. Mauk Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, University

More information

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 1st Midterm Exam Ready for Pickup By the elevator on the 3 rd Floor of Pacific Hall (waiver) Exam Depot Window at the north entrance to Pacific Hall (no waiver) Mon-Fri, 10:00

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

SOMATIC SENSATION PART I: ALS ANTEROLATERAL SYSTEM (or SPINOTHALAMIC SYSTEM) FOR PAIN AND TEMPERATURE

SOMATIC SENSATION PART I: ALS ANTEROLATERAL SYSTEM (or SPINOTHALAMIC SYSTEM) FOR PAIN AND TEMPERATURE Dental Neuroanatomy Thursday, February 3, 2011 Suzanne S. Stensaas, PhD SOMATIC SENSATION PART I: ALS ANTEROLATERAL SYSTEM (or SPINOTHALAMIC SYSTEM) FOR PAIN AND TEMPERATURE Reading: Waxman 26 th ed, :

More information

For more information about how to cite these materials visit

For more information about how to cite these materials visit Author(s): Peter Hitchcock, PH.D., 2009 License: Unless otherwise noted, this material is made available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-commercial Share Alike 3.0 License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0/

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Anxiolytic Drugs and Altered Hippocampal Theta Rhythms: The Quantitative Systems Pharmacological Approach

Anxiolytic Drugs and Altered Hippocampal Theta Rhythms: The Quantitative Systems Pharmacological Approach Anxiolytic Drugs and Altered Hippocampal Theta Rhythms: The Quantitative Systems Pharmacological Approach Péter Érdi perdi@kzoo.edu Henry R. Luce Professor Center for Complex Systems Studies Kalamazoo

More information

Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function

Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function Behavioural Brain Research 89 (1997) 1 34 Review article Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function Michael E. Hasselmo

More information

EXTRACELLULAR RECORDINGS OF SPIKES

EXTRACELLULAR RECORDINGS OF SPIKES EXTRACELLULAR RECORDINGS OF SPIKES Information about spiking is typically extracted from the high frequency band (>300-500Hz) of extracellular potentials. Since these high-frequency signals generally stem

More information

Consciousness as representation formation from a neural Darwinian perspective *

Consciousness as representation formation from a neural Darwinian perspective * Consciousness as representation formation from a neural Darwinian perspective * Anna Kocsis, mag.phil. Institute of Philosophy Zagreb, Croatia Vjeran Kerić, mag.phil. Department of Psychology and Cognitive

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information ATP from synaptic terminals and astrocytes regulates NMDA receptors and synaptic plasticity through PSD- 95 multi- protein complex U.Lalo, O.Palygin, A.Verkhratsky, S.G.N. Grant and Y. Pankratov Supporting

More information

The Cerebellum. Outline. Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley. Overview Structure Micro-circuitry of the cerebellum The cerebellum and motor learning

The Cerebellum. Outline. Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley. Overview Structure Micro-circuitry of the cerebellum The cerebellum and motor learning The Cerebellum Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley 1 Outline Overview Structure Micro-circuitry of the cerebellum The cerebellum and motor learning 2 Overview Little brain 10% of the total volume of the brain,

More information

Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements

Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements Y. Isomura et al. 1 Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements Yoshikazu Isomura, Rie Harukuni, Takashi Takekawa, Hidenori Aizawa & Tomoki Fukai

More information

Characterization of Sleep Spindles

Characterization of Sleep Spindles Characterization of Sleep Spindles Simon Freedman Illinois Institute of Technology and W.M. Keck Center for Neurophysics, UCLA (Dated: September 5, 2011) Local Field Potential (LFP) measurements from sleep

More information

DOMINIQUE DEBANNE*, BEAT H. GÄHWILER, AND SCOTT M. THOMPSON MATERIALS AND METHODS

DOMINIQUE DEBANNE*, BEAT H. GÄHWILER, AND SCOTT M. THOMPSON MATERIALS AND METHODS Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 11225 11230, October 1996 Neurobiology Cooperative interactions in the induction of long-term potentiation and depression of synaptic excitation between hippocampal

More information

Multiscale Evidence of Multiscale Brain Communication

Multiscale Evidence of Multiscale Brain Communication Multiscale Evidence of Multiscale Brain Communication Scott Makeig Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla CA Talk given

More information

Classical conditioning, awareness, and brain systems

Classical conditioning, awareness, and brain systems 524 Review Vol.6 No.12 December 2 Classical conditioning, awareness, and brain systems Robert E. Clark, Joseph R. Manns and Larry R. Squire Memory is composed of several different abilities that are supported

More information

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A Hippocampus Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D. Common (Distributed) Model of Memory Processes Time Course of Memory Processes Long Term Memory DECLARATIVE NON-DECLARATIVE Semantic Episodic Skills

More information

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements Supplementary Material Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements Xiaomo Chen, Katherine Wilson Scangos 2 and Veit Stuphorn,2 Department of Psychological and Brain

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature,

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature, Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature, Vol. 435, pp. 1102-7. Sander Vaus 22.04.2015 The study

More information

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu 9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms. Working memory short-term

More information

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception Overview of Questions What is it that makes sounds high pitched or low pitched? How do sound vibrations inside the ear lead to the perception

More information

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA 1 Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA CONTENTS I Personal and clinical data II Conclusion. III Recommendations for therapy IV Report. 1. Procedures of EEG recording and analysis 2. Search for

More information