Subregion-specific p300 conditional knock-out mice exhibit long-term memory impairments

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Subregion-specific p300 conditional knock-out mice exhibit long-term memory impairments"

Transcription

1 Research Subregion-specific p300 conditional knock-out mice exhibit long-term memory impairments Ana M.M. Oliveira, 1 Marcel A. Estévez, Joshua D. Hawk, Shannon Grimes, Paul K. Brindle, 2 and Ted Abel 3 Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104, USA Histone acetylation plays a critical role during long-term memory formation. Several studies have demonstrated that the histone acetyltransferase (HAT) CBP is required during long-term memory formation, but the involvement of other HAT proteins has not been extensively investigated. The HATs CBP and p300 have at least 400 described interacting proteins including transcription factors known to play a role in long-term memory formation. Thus, CBP and p300 constitute likely candidates for transcriptional coactivators in memory formation. In this study, we took a loss-of-function approach to evaluate the role of p300 in long-term memory formation. We used conditional knock-out mice in which the deletion of p300 is restricted to the postnatal phase and to subregions of the forebrain. We found that p300 is required for the formation of long-term recognition memory and long-term contextual fear memory in the CA1 area of the hippocampus and cortical areas. It is now well established that long-term memory formation requires activation of transcription (Alberini 2009). Recently, the focus of many studies has been on the role of epigenetic mechanisms that control chromatin remodeling and regulate gene expression in synaptic plasticity and memory formation (Duman and Newton 2007; Sweatt 2009). In particular, histone acetylation is thought to play a critical role in this process (Barrett and Wood 2008; Sharma 2010). Protein acetylation is catalyzed by histone acetyltransferase (HAT) proteins and involves the addition of an acetyl group to lysines of histone N-terminal tails and other proteins. For histones, acetylation is modeled to facilitate transcription by reducing the repression imposed by chromatin structure and by influencing the binding of transcriptional proteins recruited to chromatin. Studies have shown that memory consolidation correlates with increases in histone acetylation (Levenson et al. 2004; Fontan-Lozano et al. 2008) and that inhibition of histone deacetylases (HDACs) facilitates learning and synaptic plasticity (Fischer et al. 2007; Vecsey et al. 2007; Stefanko et al. 2009; Roozendaal et al. 2010). Consistent with these findings, genetically modified mice expressing mutant HAT proteins have memory impairments. There are four main families of mammalian HATs: GCN5 and PCAF, the MYST family, the nuclear receptor coactivator family, and the CBP and p300 family (Allis et al. 2007). Several studies have demonstrated that impaired CBP activity leads to memory impairments (Alarcon et al. 2004; Korzus et al. 2004; Wood et al. 2005, 2006), but the involvement of other HAT proteins has not been extensively investigated. The HATs CBP and p300 have at least 400 described interacting proteins (Kasper et al. 2006; Bedford et al. 2010) including transcription factors known to play a role in long-term memory formation. Thus, CBP and p300 act as pivotal molecules Present addresses: 1 Department of Neurobiology, Interdisciplinary Center for Neurosciences (IZN), University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany; 2 Department of Biochemistry, St. Jude Children s Research Hospital, Memphis, TN 38105, USA 3 Corresponding author. abele@sas.upenn.edu; fax (215) Article is online at in gene regulation and constitute primordial candidates as transcriptional coactivators in memory formation. The first suggestion for a role for CBP in cognitive function came from a study in which mutations in the CBP gene (CREBBP) were identified in patients with Rubinstein-Taybi Syndrome (RTS) (Petrij et al. 1995). Later, studies with animal models for RTS carrying mutations in the Cbp gene showed cognitive impairments (Oike et al. 1999). However a clear demonstration for a role for CBP in memory formation was only achieved when the cognitive function of CBP conditional mutants, allowing restriction of CBP mutations to the forebrain and postdevelopmental phases, was evaluated (Korzus et al. 2004; Wood et al. 2005, 2006). Recently, Viosca and colleagues evaluated the cognitive function of p300 heterozygous knock-out mice and reported largely normal performance in learning and memory tasks (Viosca et al. 2010). However, the developmental defects exhibited by those mice may confound the interpretation of the behavioral phenotypes and the study of a role for p300 in memory formation in the adult brain. Moreover, p300 heterozygous knock-out mice still have 50% of p300 protein levels. We have recently demonstrated that transgenic mice expressing a truncated form of p300 postnatally exhibit long-term memory deficits (Oliveira et al. 2007). Transgenic approaches involve the overexpression of wild type or mutated forms of a gene. The risks associated with overexpression studies include nonspecific interactions, gene dosage effects, and compensatory alterations. CBP has been extensively implicated in long-term memory formation; therefore, one concern is that the dominant negative form of p300 (p300d1) (Oliveira et al. 2007) might interact with molecules that normally recruit CBP, due to the high degree of homology between CBP and p300. In a loss-of-function approach, non-specific interactions are not a concern. Thus, in this study a loss-of-function approach was taken to study the role of p300 in long-term memory formation. Conditional knock-out mice were generated using the Cre/LoxP system to allow for regional control of the deletion of the p300 gene. Cre recombinase expression was driven by the calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II a (CaMKIIa) promoter, which drives expression postnatally in excitatory neurons within the hippocampus, striatum, amygdala, and 18: # 2011 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press ISSN /11; Learning & Memory

2 cortex (Burgin et al. 1990; Mayford et al. 1996). The postnatal expression of Cre recombinase restricts the deletion of the p300 gene to adulthood allowing us to evaluate the role of p300 in memory formation without potential developmental confounds. The p300 conditional knock-out mice used in this study lack p300 expression mainly in superficial layers of the cortex and CA1. Behaviorally, our mutant mice showed impairments in long-term recognition memory and longterm contextual fear memory. These results indicate that p300 function in the subregions targeted in our mutant mice plays a role in the formation of object recognition and contextual fear memories. Results In this study, we used p300 conditional knock-out mice to investigate the role of p300 in long-term memory formation. These mice were created using the Cre/LoxP system, which allows spatial and temporal control of the deletion of genes. This approach avoids potential developmental defects and allows deletion to be restricted to brain areas and cell types known to be important for learning and memory. Furthermore, a loss of function approach was chosen because in this type of approach nonspecific interactions of a mutant transgenic protein are not a concern. To inactivate p300 in specific brain regions, we used p300 flox/flox mice (Kasper et al. 2006), carrying a CaMKIIa- Cre transgene (R4ag#11, Dragatsis and Zeitlin 2000). The CaMKIIa promoter is only activated postnatally in excitatory neurons of the forebrain (Burgin et al. 1990; Mayford et al. 1996), thus restricting the deletion of the p300 flox gene to adulthood and to excitatory neurons of the amygdala, hippocampus, and cortex (Fig. 1A). The CaMKIIa-Cre transgenic mice (Dragatsis and Zeitlin 2000) were crossed to the p300 flox/flox mice (Kasper et al. 2006) to generate Cre + ;p300 flox/flox and Cre 2 ;p300 flox/flox mice. Cre 2 ; p300 flox/flox mice were used as control animals (see Materials and Methods for mating details). Cre + ;p300 flox/flox mice do not show gross morphological abnormalities or reduced size compared to control littermates, and the expected Mendelian ratio of genotypes was observed suggesting no developmental defects in these mice (data not shown). Importantly, it has previously been shown that mice expressing Cre recombinase in the forebrain do not exhibit learning and memory deficits (Chen et al. 2006). We next characterized the p300 deletion pattern in p300 conditional knock-out mice. Using immunohistochemistry and Western blot analysis (Fig. 1A,B), we observed a reduction in Figure 1. p300 conditional knock-out mice show reduced expression of p300 in the forebrain. (A) Immunohistochemical labeling of p300 in brain coronal sections from control and p300 conditional knock-out mice. (B) Western blot analysis of the levels of p300 in the cerebellum, hippocampus, and cortex of p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates (n ¼ 5 per group); P, Picture shows a representative blot from hippocampal tissue. (C) Immunohistochemical labeling of CBP in brain coronal sections from control and p300 conditional knock-out mice. (D) Western blot analysis of CBP protein expression in the cerebellum, hippocampus, and cortex of p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates (n ¼ 5 per group). Picture shows a representative blot from hippocampal tissue. Abbreviations: cko, p300 conditional knock-out; Ct, control littermate; PRh, Perirhinal cortex; Ent, Entorhinal cortex; I, Insular cortex; CA1, CA1 area of the hippocampus; CA3, CA3 area of the hippocampus; DG, dentate gyrus area of the hippocampus; Amy, amygdala; Hipp, Hippocampus; Cb, Cerebelum; Cx, Cortex. p300 expression in CA1, the dentate gyrus, and superficial layers of the cortex. Cells lacking p300 expression were also observed within the amygdala (Fig. 1A). In Cre 2 ;p300 flox/flox control mice, p300 is broadly expressed, reflecting the wild type expression of p300 (Oliveira et al. 2007), which demonstrates that the loxp sites do not interfere with gene function as found by Kasper et al. (2006). The Western blot analysis showed that levels of p300 in the hippocampus and cortex of p300 conditional knock-out mice are significantly reduced compared to their control Learning & Memory

3 We next investigated whether p300 conditional knock-out mice exhibit locomotor or anxiety-like phenotypes. Such phenotypes could potentially confound the interpretation of the animals performance in memory tasks. We observed that p300 conditional knock-out mice exhibit normal locomotor activity, motor skill learning, and anxiety-related responses (Fig. 3). We tested locomotor activity and motor skill learning in the open field and accelerating rotarod, respectively (Fig. 3A,B). Spontaneous activity, expressed as total distance traveled and velocity in an open field, is not different between p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates (Fig. 3A). In the rotarod, both p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates exhibit a significant improvement in time spent on the rotating rod across trials (effect of trial F (8,192) ¼ 11.64, P, ). No effect of genotype (F (1,192) ¼ 0.145, P ¼ NS) or genotype trial interaction (F (8,192) ¼ 0.59, P ¼ NS) was observed (Fig. 3B). The identical performance between control littermates and p300 conditional knock-out mice in the first trials and similar improvement across trials indicates that p300 conditional knockout mice have no impairment in locomotor activity or motor skill learning, respectively. We assessed anxiety-like behavior in the elevated zero-maze. In this task, mice face the conflict between their tendency to explore new environments and the innate aversion to open and bright spaces. Knock-out and control mice spent similar time in the closed arms when tested in the elevated zero-maze (Fig. 3C), Figure 2. Global histone acetylation levels in p300 conditional knock-out mice in CA1 and cortex. (A) Quantification of the global level of acetylation of histones H3 and H4 in the CA1 and perirhinal cortex of p300 conditional knock-out mice (n ¼ 5) and control littermates (n ¼ 7) assessed by immunohistochemistry. Reduced AcH3 is observed in caudal perirhinal cortex of p300 conditional knock-out mice; P, (B) Representative images of the rostral and caudal perirhinal cortex of p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates. Coronal brain sections were stained with an antibody against the acetylated form of histone H3. cko, p300 conditional knock-out; Ct, control littermate; PRh, Perirhinal cortex; CA1, CA1 area of the hippocampus. littermates (hippocampus: t (6) ¼ 24.12, P, 0.05; cortex: t (7) ¼ 23.12, P, 0.05) (Fig. 1B). Levels of p300 in the cerebellum, where Cre recombinase is not expressed, are not significantly different between p300 conditional knock-out mice and control animals (Fig. 1B). We also examined CBP expression pattern and levels in the same brain structures (Fig. 1C,D). No significant differences between knock-out mice and control mice were observed, suggesting that CBP is not being upregulated to compensate for the reduction of p300 and that CBP protein levels are normal in neurons lacking p300. p300 is a histone acetyltransferase; thus we hypothesized that histone acetylation would be changed in cells lacking p300. We assessed the global acetylation levels of histones H3 and H4 by immunohistochemistry in the brain regions where p300 deletion is clear (CA1 and the medial temporal cortex) (Fig. 2). Interestingly, we observed a selective reduction in histone H3 acetylation in the caudal perirhinal cortex (Fig. 2A,B). No changes were observed in the levels of acetylation of histone H4 in the brain regions analyzed or histone H3 in CA1 or the rostral perirhinal cortex. Figure 3. p300 conditional knock-out mice show normal spontaneous activity, motor coordination, motor skill learning, and anxiety-related behavior. (A) p300 conditional knock-out mice (n ¼ 5) and control littermates (n ¼ 5) show similar distance traveled and velocity in an open field. (B) p300 conditional knock-out mice (n ¼ 13) and control littermates (n ¼ 13) show similar time on the rotarod across trials. (C) p300 conditional knock-out mice (n ¼ 6) and control littermates (n ¼ 6) spent similar time in the closed arms of the elevated zero-maze. cko, p300 conditional knock-out; Ct, control littermate Learning & Memory

4 showing that p300 conditional knock-out mice exhibit normal anxiety-like behavior. To assess spatial memory, p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates were tested in the hidden platform version of the Morris water maze task (Fig. 4). The ability to acquire this task has been shown to depend on an intact hippocampus (Morris et al. 1982; Logue et al. 1997). Knock-out mice and control littermates showed a significant improvement across the acquisition phase (effect of training day F (7,154) ¼ 11.12, P, ), showing that both groups acquire the task. No effect of genotype (F (1,22) ¼ 3.39, P ¼ NS) or genotype day interaction (F (7,154) ¼ 1.20, P ¼ NS) was observed. Overall, p300 conditional knock-out mice exhibit similar latencies to find the hidden platform as their control littermates (Fig. 4A). To assess spatial memory, two probe trials were performed: the first, one day after session four, and the second, one day after day eight of training. No differences were observed between p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates in the time spent in the target quadrant or number of crossings over the area where the platform was located during training (Fig. 4B,C). Swim speed, thigmotaxis, and total swimming distance were not different between the two groups (data not shown). We have also tested the performance of p300 Figure 4. p300 conditional knock-out mice show normal spatial memory when tested in the Morris water maze. (A) Control (n ¼ 12) and p300 conditional knock-out (n ¼ 12) mice do not show a significant difference in the latency to find the hidden platform during the acquisition phase of the task. (B) During the probe trial performed after session 8, p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates showed similar percentage of time spent swimming in the target quadrant. (C) During the probe trial performed after session 8, the number of crossings over the area where the platform was located during acquisition was not different between control and p300 conditional knockout mice. cko, p300 conditional knock-out; Ct, control littermate. Figure 5. p300 conditional knock-out mice show impaired long-term recognition memory. p300 conditional knock-out mice (n ¼ 7) and control littermates (n ¼ 7) show similar preference for the novel object when tested 30 min after training. However, when tested 24 h after training, p300 conditional knock-out mice (n ¼ 6) show a significant lower preference for the novel object than their control littermates (n ¼ 6); P, cko, p300 conditional knock-out; Ct, control littermate. conditional knock-out mice in a reversal learning task. In this task, the mice learn a new platform location to assess learning flexibility. No differences were found between p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates (data not shown). We next tested the performance of p300 conditional knockout mice in a novel object recognition task (Fig. 5), a task that is based on the animals natural tendency to explore novel environments and objects. We observed that p300 conditional knock-out mice had significantly lower preference for a novel object when tested 24 h after training (t (10) ¼ 4.49, P, 0.05). In contrast, object recognition memory tested 30 min after training was not different between p300 conditional knock-out mice and control littermates. Different batches of animals were used to perform short-term and long-term memory experiments. These results indicate that p300 conditional knock-out mice display normal short-term recognition memory but impaired long-term recognition memory. Importantly, the total time spent exploring both objects during training and testing phases among the different conditions tested was not different between the two genotypes (Table 1). Finally, we assessed the performance of p300 conditional knock-out mice in an associative form of memory using fear conditioning (Fig. 6). In contextual fear conditioning, mice learn the association between a specific environmental context and a foot shock. This task is known to require the function of the hippocampus and the amygdala (Phillips and LeDoux 1992; Maren and Quirk 2004). When p300 conditional knock-out mice were re-exposed to the same context 24 h after training, they showed significantly lower freezing compared with their control littermates (t (20) ¼ 2.21, P, 0.05) (Fig. 6B). However, when testing occurred 1 h after training, no significant difference was observed between control and p300 conditional knock-out mice, suggesting that short-term memory is not affected in these mice (Fig. 6A). In cued fear conditioning, mice learn the association between a tone and a foot shock. This memory depends on the function of the amygdala but not the hippocampus (Phillips and LeDoux 1992; Maren and Quirk 2004). p300 conditional knock-out mice showed normal long-term cued fear memory as assessed by re-exposing the mice to the same cue (tone) 24 h after training in a novel context (Fig. 6C). Discussion In this study, we demonstrate that p300 is required for long-term memory formation. We used a loss of function approach to show that p300 conditional knock-out mice exhibit impairments in long-term recognition memory and long-term memory for contextual fear conditioning, with no deficits in spatial memory Learning & Memory

5 Table 1. Time exploring objects (sec) during the training session of novel object recognition tests (Fig. 5) Short-term memory Long-term memory Wild type p300 conditional knock-out Values correspond to average + standard error of the mean (SEM). tested in the Morris water maze and motor skill learning. Our previous study, in which we used transgenic mice expressing a truncated form of p300, already suggested a role for p300 in long-term memory (Oliveira et al. 2007). In the present study, we confirmed that p300 plays a critical role during memory formation, and the restriction of the p300 gene deletion to subregions of the hippocampus and cortex allowed us to begin to define the cells in which p300 activity is required. Importantly, p300 conditional knockout mice exhibited reduced acetylation of histone H3 specifically in the caudal perirhinal cortex, suggesting that p300 activity is critical in this brain region. Recently, Viosca and colleagues studied the cognitive function of p300 heterozygous knock-out mice and found no impairments Figure 6. p300 conditional knock-out mice show impaired long-term contextual fear memory and normal cued fear memory. (A) p300 conditional knock-out mice (n ¼ 10) show identical percentage of freezing compared to their control littermates (n ¼ 10) when tested 1 h after training in the cued fear conditioning. (B) p300 conditional knock-out mice (n ¼ 12) show a significant lower percentage of freezing compared to their control littermates (n ¼ 12) when tested 24 h after training in the contextual fear conditioning; P, (C) p300 conditional knockout mice (n ¼ 8) show an identical percentage of freezing compared to their control littermates (n ¼ 10) when tested 24 h after training in the cued fear conditioning. cko, p300 conditional knock-out; Ct, control littermate. in memory tests; however, the mice exhibited reduced size and increased anxiety (Viosca et al. 2010). Reduced weight and size is consistent with developmental defects in p300 knock-out mice which could potentially confound the interpretation of memory deficits due to potential compensatory mechanisms. Furthermore, only heterozygous mice were used in that study which opens the possibility that monoallelic expression of p300 is sufficient for memory formation. p300 conditional knock-out mice show normal anxiety and locomotor activity suggesting that basal behavioral responses are not altered in these animals. Furthermore, the deletion of the p300 gene should be restricted to the postnatal phase, due to the fact that the expression of Cre recombinase is under the control of the CaMKIIa promoter. This minimizes the likelihood of developmental defects. In the case of the study of the role of p300 in memory formation, this is a critical aspect as p300 is known to be involved in embryonic development (Yao et al. 1998). Furthermore, deletion is restricted to neurons, assuring that the phenotypes observed are due to the loss of activity of p300 in this cell type rather than in glial cells. Importantly, the levels of CBP in the brain of p300 conditional knock-out mice were not changed, suggesting no compensatory upregulation of the p300 homologue, CBP. We consistently observed that p300 mutant mice exhibit normal spatial memory in the Morris water maze (this study and Oliveira et al. 2007). Both p300d1 transgenic mice and p300 conditional knock-out mice lack p300 function in the hippocampus, and it is known that the function of the hippocampus is critical for spatial memory formation (Morris et al. 1982). Thus, our results suggest that p300 may not play a role in spatial memory formation. In contrast, this study shows that p300 plays a role in contextual fear memory, another hippocampus-dependent task. Other studies have also observed a molecular dissociation between distinct types of hippocampus-dependent long-term memory. In particular, differences in the molecular mechanisms underlying spatial and contextual memory formation (for review, see Mizuno and Giese 2005). Studies in which the role of CBP in spatial memory was addressed showed that CBP function is required for the Morris water maze task performance (Korzus et al. 2004; Wood et al. 2005). It is a possibility that CBP and p300 can function interchangeably during the formation of spatial memory, or that only the CBP function is required. Indeed, evidence for distinct functions for CBP and p300 has been shown (Tanaka et al. 1997; Yao et al. 1998; Kasper et al. 2002, 2006; Oliveira et al. 2006). However, these hypotheses remain to be tested. p300 conditional knock-out mice show impairments in longterm contextual fear conditioning but normal cued fear conditioning. Both contextual and cued fear conditioning require the activity of the amygdala, but only contextual fear conditioning requires the hippocampus (Maren and Quirk 2004). Therefore, the impairment in contextual fear conditioning suggests that the activity of p300 in the hippocampus is important for associative memory formation. In contrast, normal cued fear conditioning memory was observed in p300 conditional knock-out mice. This may indicate that the amygdala neurons in which the expression of p300 remains intact can support the formation of cued fear memory, or that p300 activity in the amygdala may not be required for this type of memory. Interestingly, p300d1 transgenic mice also show normal long-term cued fear memory (Oliveira et al. 2007). Thus, two distinct mouse models in which the activity of p300 is compromised exhibit unaltered cued fear conditioning, supporting the view that the activity of p300 in the amygdala is not necessary for this form of memory. The involvement of cortical areas, in particular, the perirhinal cortex, in contextual fear memory consolidation has been demonstrated (Sacchetti et al Learning & Memory

6 1999). Therefore, we should not exclude the possibility that the absence of p300 in cortical areas contributes to this phenotype. The role of the hippocampus in object recognition memory has been a matter of debate, whereas cortical regions have consistently been shown to be involved in recognition memory (for review, see Dere et al. 2007). Studies have suggested that deficits in object recognition memory are only evident after near complete hippocampus lesions. Another prevalent hypothesis is that the hippocampus is only required for object recognition memory when spatial or contextual cues are relevant during object encoding. We have recently demonstrated that the novel object recognition memory test established in our laboratory is hippocampus-independent (Oliveira et al. 2010). Thus, the impairment exhibited by p300 conditional knock-out mice in long-term recognition memory is likely due to p300 inactivation in cortical regions. However, it remains to be investigated whether full deletion of p300 in the hippocampus induces long-term object recognition memory deficits. Cortical regions such as the perirhinal and insular cortices have been implicated in the consolidation of object recognition memory (Bermudez-Rattoni et al. 2005; Winters and Bussey 2005; Barker et al. 2007; Balderas et al. 2008; Winters et al. 2008; Roozendaal et al. 2010) and p300 conditional knock-out mice lack p300 in cells within these regions. Importantly, acetylation of histone H3 is reduced in the caudal perirhinal cortex in p300 conditional knock-out mice, and lesions of this region have been correlated with recognition memory deficits (Albasser et al. 2009). Furthermore, recent studies have implicated histone acetylation as an important mechanism during recognition memory consolidation (Fontan- Lozano et al. 2008; Roozendaal et al. 2010). Thus, our study strongly suggests that p300 is a key player in this mechanism. Fontan-Lozano and colleagues showed that the levels of acetylated H3 are significantly increased in the perirhinal cortex after training in the novel object recognition task (Fontan-Lozano et al. 2008). Another study showed that infusion of a HDAC inhibitor into the insular cortex leads to enhanced novel object recognition (Roozendaal et al. 2010). Interestingly, Roozendaal and colleagues demonstrated a double dissociation between post-training HDAC inhibitor infusion into the insular cortex and the hippocampus on the enhancement of object recognition and object location memory respectively. This suggests that histone acetylation in the insular cortex, but not in the hippocampus, plays an important role during novel object memory formation. Both in the novel object recognition task and contextual fear conditioning, the memory impairment exhibited by p300 conditional knock-out mice was restricted to the 24-h delay test (longterm memory), whereas short-term memory was normal. This suggests that p300 plays a role in memory consolidation as a transcription regulator. p300 is likely to be recruited during the activation of gene expression induced by learning and to modulate transcription through its histone acetyltransferase (HAT) activity or by stabilizing multiprotein complexes at the promoter region of target genes. Besides participating in acetylation of histones, p300 can also acetylate non-histone transcription regulators (Kouzarides 2000). p300 and CBP have at least 400 described interacting proteins (Kasper et al. 2006; Bedford et al. 2010) including transcription factors known to play a role in long-term memory formation (e.g., CREB and family members, c-fos, Elk-1, C/EBP, NF-kB). Thus, CBP and p300 act as critical molecules in gene regulation, and involvement in memory formation should be highly expected. In p300 conditional knock-out mice, transcription regulators that require p300 activity will have their function compromised. These factors should include not only transcription factors that recruit p300 HAT activity to initiate transcription but also transcription factors that require acetylation for activation. One extensively studied example is the transcription factor p65, a member of the NF-kB family. Acetylation of p65 is required for p65-dependent activation of transcription (Chen and Greene 2004). Interestingly, p300 was shown to acetylate p65, and this acetylation resulted in activation of p65 and consequently enhanced expression of the NMDA receptor subunits, NR2A and NR2B (Tai et al. 2009), critical for synaptic plasticity and memory formation. At this point, it is becoming clear that p300 plays a critical role in long-term memory formation. To further understand the role of p300 in this process, it will be critical to identify the molecules that recruit p300 during learning-induced transcription activation and the genes targeted by p300-containing complexes. Materials and Methods Mice All mice were 2 4 mo old and had free access to food and water in their home cages. Lights were maintained on a 12-h light dark cycle, with all behavioral experiments carried out during the light portion of the cycle. CaMKIIa-Cre (line R4ag#11) mice were a kind gift from Dr. Ioannis Dragatsis (University of Tennessee). CaMKIIa-Cre (line R4ag#11) and p300 flox were generated as previously described (Dragatsis and Zeitlin 2000, Kasper et al. 2006). CaMKIIa-Cre and p300 flox were backcrossed to C57BL/6J mice at least 5 generations. To obtain p300 conditional knock-out mice, we first crossed p300 flox/flox mice with mice that contained one transgenic allele expressing Cre recombinase under the control of the CaMKIIa promoter (Cre + ). Cre + ;p300 flox/+ mice were crossed with p300 flox/flox to obtain mice that contain the CaMKIIa-Cre transgene and are p300 flox/flox homozygous (Cre + ;p300 flox/flox ). Cre 2 ;p300 flox/flox littermates were used as control mice. Genotyping was performed as previously described (Dietrich et al. 2000; Kasper et al. 2006). All experiments were carried out in accordance with National Institutes of Health Guidelines and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Pennsylvania. Western blotting Nuclear extracts from cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum were obtained. The tissue from one mouse was homogenized in solution A (10 mm 4-(2-hydroxyethyl)-1-piperazineethanesulfonic acid (HEPES) ph 7.9, 1 mm ethylenediamine tetraacetic acid (EDTA), 1.5 mm MgCl 2, 10 mm KCl, 1 mm dithiothreitol (DTT), 1% protease inhibitor cocktail (Sigma)). The nuclear pellet was resuspended in solution C (20 mm HEPES ph 7.9, 0.2 mm EDTA, 1.5 mm MgCl 2, 420 mm KCl, 25% glycerol, 1 mm DTT, 1% protease cocktail inhibitor (Sigma)) and the supernatant was used as nuclear extract. The protein quantification was done using the Bradford reagent (BioRad). Samples were loaded in a 4% 12% gradient acrylamide gel (Invitrogen) and blotted onto a nitrocellulose membrane (BioRad). The membrane was blocked in Tris-buffered saline (TBS) and 5% nonfat dry milk and probed with actin (1:2500, Chemicon), CBP and p300 (1:1000 and 1:500, respectively, Santa Cruz Biotechnology) antibodies diluted in TBS containing 0.05% Tween 20 (TBST) and 2% milk. Blots were then incubated with horseradish peroxidase (HRP)-conjugated anti-rabbit or anti-mouse secondary antibodies (1:5000 dilution in TBST and 2% milk, Chemicon). Finally, blots were incubated in enhanced chemiluminescence (ECL) plus substrate (Amersham) and exposed to BioMax MR film (Kodak). Immunohistochemistry Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and transcardially perfused with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS using a peristaltic perfusion pump. Fixed brains were dissected, post-fixed overnight, and then cryoprotected for 48 h in 30% sucrose at 48C. Brains were flash frozen in 2-methylbutane on dry ice and mounted on cryostat chucks using Learning & Memory

7 Tissue Freezing Medium (Electron Microscopy Sciences, Hatfield, PA). Coronal sections were cut at a thickness of 30 mm and collected in PBS containing 0.02% sodium azide and stored at 48C until further processing. The procedure used for immunohistochemistry was executed as described previously (Havekes et al. 2006; Isiegas et al. 2008). Floating sections were washed with PBS (5 min), incubated with 0.1% H 2 O 2 in PBS (30 min) followed by rinsing in PBS (3 times, 5 min). Sections were pre-incubated in 8% normal goat serum (Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories) in PBS containing 0.3% Triton X-100 for 1 h at room temperature. After pre-incubation, sections were incubated with either p300, CBP, AcH3 or AcH4 antibodies (p300: C-20 and N-15, 1:500 each, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, CBP: A-22 and C-20, 1:1000 each, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, AcH3: , 1:1000, Millipore, AcH4: , 1:1000, Millipore) overnight at 48C in 2% normal goat serum, 0.3% Triton X-100 in PBS. Sections were then rinsed in PBS (3 times, 5 min) and incubated for 2 h with biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgG (1:500; Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories) in PBS containing 0.3% Triton X-100 and 2% normal goat serum at 48C. After rinsing with PBS (3 times, 5 min), sections were incubated with the avidin-biotin-horseradish peroxidase complex (A and B components of ABC kit, 1:1000 each, Vector Laboratories) in PBS containing 0.3% Triton X-100 for 2 h at room temperature. Finally, after rinsing in PBS (2 times, 5 min, then overnight) sections were processed with diaminobenzidine (0.02% in PBS) with 0.003% H 2 O 2 as a reaction initiator. After 7 min (p300), 5.5 min (CBP, AcH3) or 4.5 min (AcH4), the reaction was stopped by washing the sections with PBS containing 0.02% sodium azide. Sections were mounted on Superfrost Plus microscope slides (Fisher Scientific) and dried for 24 h. The mounted sections were then dehydrated using ethanol and xylenes, coverslipped with Permount (Fisher Scientific), and dried for another 24 h. Images were taken using a Fisher Micromaster microscope with camera. Immunohistochemistry quantification Perirhinal cortex was identified in rostral (21.70 Bregma) and caudal (22.80 Bregma) sections. Quantification of signal was performed with Image J. The area of interest was identified, and the total background intensity was subtracted from total intensity to obtain the total intensity of the signal (I signal ¼ I total I background ). The mean intensity of the signal was calculated by dividing the total intensity of the signal by the area of the signal (m signal ¼ I signal /A signal ). To calculate the signal value, the mean intensity of the signal was inverted in the 255 scale and multiplied by the fraction of the area that the signal occupies (signal ¼ 2[m s - 255] [A signal /A total ]). Quantification of the CA1 area of the hippocampus was performed by selecting the area of interest, and the value was normalized with the signal of adjacent thalamic areas. Behavioral experiments Open-field The experimental apparatus consisted of a gray rectangular open field (38.5 cm 30.5 cm 26 cm). The mice were allowed to explore the open field for 6 min. Exploratory activity in the experimental arena was measured with the use of TopScan (Clever Systems Inc.). Accelerating rotarod The rotarod apparatus (Ugo Basile, Stoelting Co.) has a 3-cmdiameter rotating rod raised 16 cm above a platform and divided into five sections for testing multiple mice simultaneously. The rotarod gradually increases its rotation speed from 4 to 40 rpm over the course of 5 min. Latency to fall was recorded. Three trials a day were given during three consecutive days with an inter-trial interval of 1 h. Each trial started at the same time every day and ended when mice fell off the rod, or when mice ran for 300 sec. Elevated zero-maze The zero-maze apparatus consists of a raised circular track divided into two open and two closed quadrants. The track had an internal diameter of 40.5 cm and a width of 5.1 cm and was elevated off the floor at a height of 40 cm. The closed quadrants had walls 11-cm-high. Mice were placed at the entrance of a closed quadrant and observed for 5 min. The time spent in the closed and open quadrants was scored. Water maze The hidden platform experiment was performed using the method previously described (Wood et al. 2005; Oliveira et al. 2007). On the first day, mice were trained to sit on a submerged platform in a bucket for two 30-sec trials. Then mice were placed in the pool twice a day where a submerged platform was located in a fixed position (approximately 5- min inter-trial interval) for 4 d. Next, mice were given a probe trail on the fifth day and then four more days of training followed. A second probe trial was given on the tenth day. Each training trial lasted a maximum of 60 sec, and when mice did not find the platform, they were placed on it and allowed to sit for 20 sec. Each probe trial lasted 60 sec. For reversal training, the mice had four training trials per day, with an inter-trial interval of approximately 40 min. Also for reversal training, each training trial lasted a maximum of 120 sec. Mice were trained for 7 d in the original platform position, followed by 7 d in a new platform position in the opposite quadrant. The path of the mouse was recorded using a video tracking system (HVS Image, Water2020 version 1/2001). Fear conditioning The fear conditioning experiments were performed using the methods previously described (Wood et al. 2005; Oliveira et al. 2007). For contextual fear conditioning, mice were placed into the conditioning chamber and received a 2-sec 1.5-mA scrambled footshock 2.5 min after placement into the chamber. Mice were removed from the chamber after 3 min. During testing, mice received one 5-min exposure to the same context in the absence of footshock 1 h or 24 h after conditioning. Different sets of animals were used for 1 h and 24 h tests. For cued fear conditioning, mice were placed into the chamber and the cue (white noise) was activated from min after placement into the chamber with a 1.5-mA footshock delivered in the last 2 sec of the cue. Mice were in the chamber for a total of 3 min. On testing day, mice were exposed to a novel context for a total of 5 min (a different conditioning chamber with smooth flat floor, altered dimensions, and a novel odorant), the onset of the cue was 2 min after mice were placed in the chamber, and lasted 3 min. Freezing was scored at 5-sec intervals and the percentage of those intervals in which the mouse froze was calculated. Different sets of mice were used for contextual and cued conditioning experiments. Novel object recognition The novel object recognition experiments were performed using the methods described (Wood et al. 2006; Oliveira et al. 2007). The experimental apparatus consisted of a white rectangular open field (60 cm 50 cm 26 cm). Prior to training, mice were handled for 1 min a day for 2 d and habituated to the experimental apparatus for 5 min in the absence of objects. During the training phase, mice were placed in the experimental apparatus in the presence of two identical objects and allowed to explore for 15 min. After a retention interval of 30 min or 24 h, mice were placed again in the apparatus where, this time, one of the objects was replaced by a novel one. Mice were allowed to explore for 15 min. Objects were rinsed with ethanol between trials and before the first trial. All testing and training sessions were videotaped and analyzed by an experimenter blind to the genotype of the animals. It was considered exploration of the objects when mice were facing an object and/or touching it. Preference for the novel object was expressed as the percent time spent exploring the novel object relative to the total time spent exploring both Learning & Memory

8 objects. The identity of the objects which was novel or familiar as well as their spatial locations were balanced between subjects. Statistical analyses Statistical analyses were performed using Student s t-test, with the exception of the Morris water maze and rotarod experiments. In the Morris water maze and rotarod experiments, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA were performed using Statistica. To compare groups, we used the Student-Newman-Keuls multiple comparisons post-hoc test. All experiments were done by an individual blind to genotype. Acknowledgments We thank Dr. Ioannis Dragatsis (University of Tennessee) for kindly providing the CaMKIIa-Cre transgenic mice (line R4ag#11). This work was supported by grants from the Human Frontiers Science Program (to T.A.), National Institutes of Health (to T.A.), PENN-PORT (to M.A.E.), NIH Pre-doctoral Training Program in Genetics (to J.D.H.), and Foundation for Science and Technology, Portugal (to A.M.M.O.). References Alarcon JM, Malleret G, Touzani K, Vronskaya S, Ishii S, Kandel ER, Barco A Chromatin acetylation, memory, and LTP are impaired in CBP+/- mice: A model for the cognitive deficit in Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome and its amelioration. Neuron 42: Albasser MM, Davies M, Futter JE, Aggleton JP Magnitude of the object recognition deficit associated with perirhinal cortex damage in rats: Effects of varying the lesion extent and the duration of the sample period. Behav Neurosci 123: Alberini CM Transcription factors in long-term memory and synaptic plasticity. Physiol Rev 89: Allis CD, Berger SL, Cote J, Dent S, Jenuwien T, Kouzarides T, Pillus L, Reinberg D, Shi Y, Shiekhattar R, et al New nomenclature for chromatin-modifying enzymes. Cell 131: Balderas I, Rodriguez-Ortiz CJ, Salgado-Tonda P, Chavez-Hurtado J, McGaugh JL, Bermudez-Rattoni F The consolidation of object and context recognition memory involve different regions of the temporal lobe. Learn Mem 15: Barker GR, Bird F, Alexander V, Warburton EC Recognition memory for objects, place, and temporal order: A disconnection analysis of the role of the medial prefrontal cortex and perirhinal cortex. J Neurosci 27: Barrett RM, Wood MA Beyond transcription factors: The role of chromatin modifying enzymes in regulating transcription required for memory. Learn Mem 15: Bedford DC, Kasper LH, Fukuyama T, Brindle PK Target gene context influences the transcriptional requirement for the KAT3 family of CBP and p300 histone acetyltransferases. Epigenetics 5: Bermudez-Rattoni F, Okuda S, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL Insular cortex is involved in consolidation of object recognition memory. Learn Mem 12: Burgin KE, Waxham MN, Rickling S, Westgate SA, Mobley WC, Kelly PT In situ hybridization histochemistry of Ca2+/ calmodulin-dependent protein kinase in developing rat brain. J Neurosci 10: Chen LF, Greene WC Shaping the nuclear action of NF-kappaB. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol 5: Chen AP, Ohno M, Giese KP, Kuhn R, Chen RL, Silva AJ Forebrainspecific knockout of B-raf kinase leads to deficits in hippocampal longterm potentiation, learning, and memory. J Neurosci Res 83: Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA The pharmacology, neuroanatomy and neurogenetics of one-trial object recognition in rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31: Dietrich P, Dragatsis I, Xuan S, Zeitlin S, Efstratiadis A Conditional mutagenesis in mice with heat shock promoter-driven cre transgenes. Mamm Genome 11: Dragatsis I, Zeitlin S CaMKIIalpha-Cre transgene expression and recombination patterns in the mouse brain. Genesis 26: Duman RS, Newton SS Epigenetic marking and neuronal plasticity. Biol Psychiatry 62: 1 3. Fischer A, Sananbenesi F, Wang X, Dobbin M, Tsai LH Recovery of learning and memory is associated with chromatin remodelling. Nature 447: Fontan-Lozano A, Romero-Granados R, Troncoso J, Munera A, Delgado-Garcia JM, Carrion AM Histone deacetylase inhibitors improve learning consolidation in young and in KA-inducedneurodegeneration and SAMP-8-mutant mice. Mol Cell Neurosci 39: Havekes R, Nijholt IM, Luiten PG, Van der Zee EA Differential involvement of hippocampal calcineurin during learning and reversal learning in a Y-maze task. Learn Mem 13: Isiegas C, McDonough C, Huang T, Havekes R, Fabian S, Wu LJ, Xu H, Zhao MG, Kim JI, Lee YS, et al A novel conditional genetic system reveals that increasing neuronal camp enhances memory and retrieval. J Neurosci 28: Kasper LH, Boussouar F, Ney PA, Jackson CW, Rehg J, van Deursen JM, Brindle PK A transcription-factor-binding surface of coactivator p300 is required for haematopoiesis. Nature 419: Kasper LH, Fukuyama T, Biesen MA, Boussouar F, Tong C, de Pauw A, Murray PJ, van Deursen JM, Brindle PK Conditional knockout mice reveal distinct functions for the global transcriptional coactivators CBP and p300 in T-cell development. Mol Cell Biol 26: Korzus E, Rosenfeld MG, Mayford M CBP histone acetyltransferase activity is a critical component of memory consolidation. Neuron 42: Kouzarides T Acetylation: A regulatory modification to rival phosphorylation? Embo J 19: Levenson JM, O Riordan KJ, Brown KD, Trinh MA, Molfese DL, Sweatt JD Regulation of histone acetylation during memory formation in the hippocampus. J Biol Chem 279: Logue SF, Paylor R, Wehner JM Hippocampal lesions cause learning deficits in inbred mice in the Morris water maze and conditioned-fear task. Behav Neurosci 111: Maren S, Quirk GJ Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nat Rev 5: Mayford M, Bach ME, Huang YY, Wang L, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER Control of memory formation through regulated expression of a CaMKII transgene. Science 274: Mizuno K, Giese KP Hippocampus-dependent memory formation: Do memory type-specific mechanisms exist? J Pharmacol Sci 98: Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O Keefe J Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 297: Oike Y, Hata A, Mamiya T, Kaname T, Noda Y, Suzuki M, Yasue H, Nabeshima T, Araki K, Yamamura K Truncated CBP protein leads to classical Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome phenotypes in mice: Implications for a dominant-negative mechanism. Hum Mol Genet 8: Oliveira AM, Abel T, Brindle PK, Wood MA Differential role for CBP and p300 CREB-binding domain in motor skill learning. Behav Neurosci 120: Oliveira AM, Wood MA, McDonough CB, Abel T Transgenic mice expressing an inhibitory truncated form of p300 exhibit long-term memory deficits. Learn Mem 14: Oliveira AM, Hawk JD, Abel T, Havekes R Post-training reversible inactivation of the hippocampus enhances novel object recognition memory. Learn Mem 17: Petrij F, Giles RH, Dauwerse HG, Saris JJ, Hennekam RC, Masuno M, Tommerup N, van Ommen GJ, Goodman RH, Peters DJ, et al Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome caused by mutations in the transcriptional co-activator CBP. Nature 376: Phillips RG, LeDoux JE Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 106: Roozendaal B, Hernandez A, Cabrera SM, Hagewoud R, Malvaez M, Stefanko DP, Haettig J, Wood MA Membrane-associated glucocorticoid activity is necessary for modulation of longterm memory via chromatin modification. J Neurosci 30: Sacchetti B, Lorenzini CA, Baldi E, Tassoni G, Bucherelli C Auditory thalamus, dorsal hippocampus, basolateral amygdala, and perirhinal cortex role in the consolidation of conditioned freezing to context and to acoustic conditioned stimulus in the rat. J Neurosci 19: Sharma SK Protein acetylation in synaptic plasticity and memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34: Stefanko DP, Barrett RM, Ly AR, Reolon GK, Wood MA Modulation of long-term memory for object recognition via HDAC inhibition. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106: Sweatt JD Experience-dependent epigenetic modifications in the central nervous system. Biol Psychiatry 65: Tai DJ, Su CC, Ma YL, Lee EH SGK1 phosphorylation of IkappaB Kinase alpha and p300 up-regulates NF-kappaB activity and increases Learning & Memory

9 N-Methyl-D-aspartate receptor NR2A and NR2B expression. J Biol Chem 284: Tanaka Y, Naruse I, Maekawa T, Masuya H, Shiroishi T, Ishii S Abnormal skeletal patterning in embryos lacking a single Cbp allele: A partial similarity with Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci 94: Vecsey CG, Hawk JD, Lattal KM, Stein JM, Fabian SA, Attner MA, Cabrera SM, McDonough CB, Brindle PK, Abel T, et al Histone deacetylase inhibitors enhance memory and synaptic plasticity via CREB:CBP-dependent transcriptional activation. J Neurosci 27: Viosca J, Lopez-Atalaya JP, Olivares R, Eckner R, Barco A Syndromic features and mild cognitive impairment in mice with genetic reduction on p300 activity: Differential contribution of p300 and CBP to Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome etiology. Neurobiol Dis 37: Winters BD, Bussey TJ Transient inactivation of perirhinal cortex disrupts encoding, retrieval, and consolidation of object recognition memory. J Neurosci 25: Winters BD, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ Object recognition memory: Neurobiological mechanisms of encoding, consolidation and retrieval. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32: Wood MA, Kaplan MP, Park A, Blanchard EJ, Oliveira AM, Lombardi TL, Abel T Transgenic mice expressing a truncated form of CREB-binding protein (CBP) exhibit deficits in hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory storage. Learn Mem 12: Wood MA, Attner MA, Oliveira AM, Brindle PK, Abel T A transcription factor-binding domain of the coactivator CBP is essential for long-term memory and the expression of specific target genes. Learn Mem 13: Yao TP, Oh SP, Fuchs M, Zhou ND, Ch ng LE, Newsome D, Bronson RT, Li E, Livingston DM, Eckner R Gene dosage-dependent embryonic development and proliferation defects in mice lacking the transcriptional integrator p300. Cell 93: Received July 15, 2010; accepted in revised form January 5, Learning & Memory

10 Subregion-specific p300 conditional knock-out mice exhibit long-term memory impairments Ana M.M. Oliveira, Marcel A. Estévez, Joshua D. Hawk, et al. Learn. Mem. 2011, 18: Access the most recent version at doi: /lm References This article cites 50 articles, 22 of which can be accessed free at: License Alerting Service Receive free alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top right corner of the article or click here Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

DNA methylation and histone acetylation work in concert to regulate memory formation and synaptic plasticity

DNA methylation and histone acetylation work in concert to regulate memory formation and synaptic plasticity Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 89 (2008) 599 603 Brief Report DNA methylation and histone acetylation work in concert to regulate memory formation and synaptic

More information

DNA and Histone Methylation in Learning and Memory

DNA and Histone Methylation in Learning and Memory EXPERIMENTAL BIOLOGY ANNUAL MEETING April 2010 DNA and Histone Methylation in Learning and Memory J. David Sweatt Dept of Neurobiology McKnight Brain Institute UAB School of Medicine The Molecular Basis

More information

Dissected tissues were minced and lysed in lysis buffer (1x Tris buffered saline (TBS), 1% NP-40,

Dissected tissues were minced and lysed in lysis buffer (1x Tris buffered saline (TBS), 1% NP-40, Data Supplement for Dincheva et al., Effect of Early-Life Fluoxetine on Anxiety-Like Behaviors in BDNF Val66Met Mice. Am J Psychiatry (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.15121592) Contents Supplemental Methods

More information

Assessing mouse behaviors: Modeling pediatric traumatic brain injury

Assessing mouse behaviors: Modeling pediatric traumatic brain injury Assessing mouse behaviors: Modeling pediatric traumatic brain injury Bridgette Semple Ph.D. Postdoctoral Fellow, Noble Laboratory Department of Neurological Surgery, UCSF Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury

More information

Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates

Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates Wen Cai, MD, Ph.D. Chief Medical Officer--La Frontera Arizona Clinical Associate Professor--Psychiatry and Psychology University of Arizona College

More information

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal A. Project Description Objective of research: The objective of this study is to determine if hippocampal dopamine D1 receptors facilitate novel object

More information

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Hippocampal Pyramidal Neuron of Mice and Rats Figure 1 Open Field Apparatus Open Field Behavior

More information

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and Gallo Taste Learning and Memory in Aging Milagros Gallo, PhD Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and in humans involves conscious recall. 1 Visual recognition memory is

More information

International Journal of. Peptides. Hindawi Publishing Corporation. Submit your manuscripts at. Anatomy Research International

International Journal of. Peptides. Hindawi Publishing Corporation. Submit your manuscripts at.   Anatomy Research International Directions in Science TheScientificWorld (2001) 1, 530 533 ISSN 1532-2246; DOI 10.1100/tsw.2001.259 Calcineurin, Synaptic Plasticity, and Memory Carl Weitlauf and Danny Winder Vanderbilt University School

More information

Limbic system outline

Limbic system outline Limbic system outline 1 Introduction 4 The amygdala and emotion -history - theories of emotion - definition - fear and fear conditioning 2 Review of anatomy 5 The hippocampus - amygdaloid complex - septal

More information

Terminology. Terminology. Terminology. Terminology. Terminology. Bromodeoxyuridine

Terminology. Terminology. Terminology. Terminology. Terminology. Bromodeoxyuridine Kateřina Náměstková, Zuzana Šimonová, Eva Syková Behavioural Brain Research Bromodeoxyuridine : Doublecortin : DCX Glial Fibrillary Acidic Protein : GFAP Trace eye blink conditioning 1 Volume 163 : pp.

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi: 10.1038/nature05772 SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplemental figure 1. Enrichment facilitates learning. a. Images showing a home cage and a cage used for environmental enrichment (EE). For EE up to

More information

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear Historical views that thought and emotion were processed separately in the brain Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: So far, fear is the best understood What is fear? Dictionary: A feeling of agitation and

More information

Zhu et al, page 1. Supplementary Figures

Zhu et al, page 1. Supplementary Figures Zhu et al, page 1 Supplementary Figures Supplementary Figure 1: Visual behavior and avoidance behavioral response in EPM trials. (a) Measures of visual behavior that performed the light avoidance behavior

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) Hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival Threats are much better reinforcers Fear is a prime

More information

Protocol for Gene Transfection & Western Blotting

Protocol for Gene Transfection & Western Blotting The schedule and the manual of basic techniques for cell culture Advanced Protocol for Gene Transfection & Western Blotting Schedule Day 1 26/07/2008 Transfection Day 3 28/07/2008 Cell lysis Immunoprecipitation

More information

CREB Binding Protein Is Required for Both Short-Term and Long-Term Memory Formation

CREB Binding Protein Is Required for Both Short-Term and Long-Term Memory Formation 13066 The Journal of Neuroscience, September 29, 2010 30(39):13066 13077 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive CREB Binding Protein Is Required for Both Short-Term and Long-Term Memory Formation Guiquan Chen, 1

More information

Rescue of neurological deficits in a mouse model for Angelman Syndrome by reduction of αcamkii inhibitory phosphorylation

Rescue of neurological deficits in a mouse model for Angelman Syndrome by reduction of αcamkii inhibitory phosphorylation Rescue of neurological deficits in a mouse model for Angelman Syndrome by reduction of αcamkii inhibitory phosphorylation Geeske M van Woerden 1, Karen D Harris 2, Mohammad Reza Hojjati 1, Richard M Gustin

More information

Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949

Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949 Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras Lecture 5: Memory Consolidation Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949 McGaugh, 2 Squire: retention of TV shows after ECS At least 2 kinds

More information

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi LT Memory and the Amygdala 1 Running Head: Role of the amygdala in long term memory What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment University of Mississippi LT Memory and the Amygdala

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Rett Syndrome Mutation MeCP2 T158A Disrupts DNA Binding, Protein Stability and ERP Responses

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Rett Syndrome Mutation MeCP2 T158A Disrupts DNA Binding, Protein Stability and ERP Responses SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Rett Syndrome Mutation T158A Disrupts DNA Binding, Protein Stability and ERP Responses Darren Goffin, Megan Allen, Le Zhang, Maria Amorim, I-Ting Judy Wang, Arith-Ruth S. Reyes,

More information

The Schedule and the Manual of Basic Techniques for Cell Culture

The Schedule and the Manual of Basic Techniques for Cell Culture The Schedule and the Manual of Basic Techniques for Cell Culture 1 Materials Calcium Phosphate Transfection Kit: Invitrogen Cat.No.K2780-01 Falcon tube (Cat No.35-2054:12 x 75 mm, 5 ml tube) Cell: 293

More information

Supplementary Fig. 1: TBR2+ cells in different brain regions.

Supplementary Fig. 1: TBR2+ cells in different brain regions. Hip SVZ OB Cere Hypo Supplementary Fig. 1: TBR2 + cells in different brain regions. Three weeks after the last tamoxifen injection, TBR2 immunostaining images reveal a large reduction of TBR2 + cells in

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/330/6009/1408/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Paradoxical False Memory for Objects After Brain Damage Stephanie M. McTighe, Rosemary A. Cowell, Boyer D. Winters,

More information

Neuronal Plasticity, Learning and Memory. David Keays Institute of Molecular Pathology

Neuronal Plasticity, Learning and Memory. David Keays Institute of Molecular Pathology Neuronal Plasticity, Learning and Memory David Keays Institute of Molecular Pathology http://keayslab.org Structure 1. What is learning and memory? 2. Anatomical basis 3. Cellular basis 4. Molecular

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) The hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

MeCP2 and psychostimulantinduced behavioral adaptations. Anne E. West, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Neurobiology Duke University Medical Center

MeCP2 and psychostimulantinduced behavioral adaptations. Anne E. West, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Neurobiology Duke University Medical Center MeCP2 and psychostimulantinduced behavioral adaptations Anne E. West, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Neurobiology Duke University Medical Center Psychostimulants and antidepressants slowly change behavior Psychostimulants

More information

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 Second midterm is next Tuesday!! Covers lectures 7-12 (Synaptic transmission, NT & receptors, intracellular signaling & synaptic plasticity). Review session is on Monday (Nov

More information

1. Materials and Methods 1.1 Animals experiments process The experiments were approved by the Institution Animal Ethics Committee of Jilin University

1. Materials and Methods 1.1 Animals experiments process The experiments were approved by the Institution Animal Ethics Committee of Jilin University 1. Materials and Methods 1.1 Animals experiments process The experiments were approved by the Institution Animal Ethics Committee of Jilin University (Reference NO. 2015-003). 96 Kunming (KM) mice (8 weeks;

More information

TFEB-mediated increase in peripheral lysosomes regulates. Store Operated Calcium Entry

TFEB-mediated increase in peripheral lysosomes regulates. Store Operated Calcium Entry TFEB-mediated increase in peripheral lysosomes regulates Store Operated Calcium Entry Luigi Sbano, Massimo Bonora, Saverio Marchi, Federica Baldassari, Diego L. Medina, Andrea Ballabio, Carlotta Giorgi

More information

Supplemental Information. A Visual-Cue-Dependent Memory Circuit. for Place Navigation

Supplemental Information. A Visual-Cue-Dependent Memory Circuit. for Place Navigation Neuron, Volume 99 Supplemental Information A Visual-Cue-Dependent Memory Circuit for Place Navigation Han Qin, Ling Fu, Bo Hu, Xiang Liao, Jian Lu, Wenjing He, Shanshan Liang, Kuan Zhang, Ruijie Li, Jiwei

More information

SensoLyte 520 HDAC Activity Assay Kit *Fluorimetric*

SensoLyte 520 HDAC Activity Assay Kit *Fluorimetric* SensoLyte 520 HDAC Activity Assay Kit *Fluorimetric* Catalog # 72084 Kit Size 100 Assays (96-well plate) Optimized Performance: This kit is optimized to detect HDAC activity. Enhanced Value: It provides

More information

Cdk5 Is Required for Memory Function and Hippocampal Plasticity via the camp Signaling Pathway

Cdk5 Is Required for Memory Function and Hippocampal Plasticity via the camp Signaling Pathway Cdk5 Is Required for Memory Function and Hippocampal Plasticity via the camp Signaling Pathway Ji-Song Guan 1,2,3,4., Susan C. Su 1,2,3,4., Jun Gao 1,2,3,4, Nadine Joseph 1,2,3,4, Zhigang Xie 1,2, Ying

More information

Development of a Touch-Screen-Based Paradigm for Assessing Working Memory in the Mouse

Development of a Touch-Screen-Based Paradigm for Assessing Working Memory in the Mouse http://dx.doi.org/10.5607/en.2015.24.1.84 Exp Neurobiol. 2015 Mar;24(1):84-89. pissn 1226-2560 eissn 2093-8144 Original Article Development of a Touch-Screen-Based Paradigm for Assessing Working Memory

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Nat Neurosci. 2006 August ; 9(8): 1004 1006. Maternal presence serves as a switch between learning fear and attraction in infancy

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Figures Supplementary Figure S1. Binding of full-length OGT and deletion mutants to PIP strips (Echelon Biosciences). Supplementary Figure S2. Binding of the OGT (919-1036) fragments with

More information

Consolidation of CS and US Representations in Associative Fear Conditioning

Consolidation of CS and US Representations in Associative Fear Conditioning Consolidation of CS and US Representations in Associative Fear Conditioning HIPPOCAMPUS 14:557 569 (2004) Paul W. Frankland, Sheena A. Josselyn, Stephan G. Anagnostaras, Jeffrey H. Kogan, Eiki Takahashi,

More information

Bone marrow-derived mesenchymal stem cells improve diabetes-induced cognitive impairment by

Bone marrow-derived mesenchymal stem cells improve diabetes-induced cognitive impairment by Nakano et al. Supplementary information 1. Supplementary Figure 2. Methods 3. References Bone marrow-derived mesenchymal stem cells improve diabetes-induced cognitive impairment by exosome transfer into

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioural effects of ketamine in non-stressed and stressed mice. Naive C57BL/6 adult male mice (n=10/group) were given a single dose of saline vehicle or ketamine (3.0 mg/kg,

More information

Vol 6, Iss 9, May 5, 2016

Vol 6, Iss 9, May 5, 2016 Olfactory Recognition Memory Test in Mice Stephanie A. Jacobs 1*, Fengying Huang 1, Joe Z. Tsien 1 and Wei Wei 2* 1 Department of Neurology, Brain and Behavior Discovery Institute, Medical College of Georgia,

More information

Behavioral Neurobiology

Behavioral Neurobiology Behavioral Neurobiology The Cellular Organization of Natural Behavior THOMAS J. CAREW University of California, Irvine Sinauer Associates, Inc. Publishers Sunderland, Massachusetts PART I: Introduction

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Histone Deacetylase Inhibitors Enhance Memory and Synaptic Plasticity via CREB: CBP-Dependent Transcriptional Activation

Histone Deacetylase Inhibitors Enhance Memory and Synaptic Plasticity via CREB: CBP-Dependent Transcriptional Activation 6128 The Journal of Neuroscience, June 6, 2007 27(23):6128 6140 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Histone Deacetylase Inhibitors Enhance Memory and Synaptic Plasticity via CREB: CBP-Dependent Transcriptional

More information

How to measure rodent behavior and perform a neurological screen.

How to measure rodent behavior and perform a neurological screen. An Organ Systems Approach to Experimental Targeting of the Metabolic Syndrome How to measure rodent behavior and perform a neurological screen. Fiona Harrison, PhD Department of Medicine Vanderbilt University

More information

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu 9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms. Declarative memory conscious,

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Franco et al. 10.1073/pnas.1015557108 SI Materials and Methods Drug Administration. PD352901 was dissolved in 0.5% (wt/vol) hydroxyl-propyl-methylcellulose, 0.2% (vol/vol) Tween

More information

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION. Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition. A Senior Honors Thesis

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION. Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition. A Senior Honors Thesis Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION Alcohol on Object Recognition 1 Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition A Senior Honors Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the

More information

BMP6 treatment compensates for the molecular defect and ameliorates hemochromatosis in Hfe knockout mice

BMP6 treatment compensates for the molecular defect and ameliorates hemochromatosis in Hfe knockout mice SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS BMP6 treatment compensates for the molecular defect and ameliorates hemochromatosis in Hfe knockout mice Elena Corradini, Paul J. Schmidt, Delphine Meynard, Cinzia Garuti, Giuliana

More information

Persistent improvement in synaptic and cognitive functions in an Alzheimer mouse model after rolipram treatment

Persistent improvement in synaptic and cognitive functions in an Alzheimer mouse model after rolipram treatment Persistent improvement in synaptic and cognitive functions in an Alzheimer mouse model after rolipram treatment Bing Gong,, Michael Shelanski, Ottavio Arancio J Clin Invest. 2004;114(11):1624-1634. https://doi.org/10.1172/jci22831.

More information

SEX DIFFERENCES IN CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING ARE ASSOCIATED WITH DIFFERENTIAL VENTRAL HIPPOCAMPAL EXTRACELLULAR SIGNAL-REGULATED KINASE ACTIVATION

SEX DIFFERENCES IN CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING ARE ASSOCIATED WITH DIFFERENTIAL VENTRAL HIPPOCAMPAL EXTRACELLULAR SIGNAL-REGULATED KINASE ACTIVATION Neuroscience 159 (2009) 451 467 SEX DIFFERENCES IN CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING ARE ASSOCIATED WITH DIFFERENTIAL VENTRAL HIPPOCAMPAL EXTRACELLULAR SIGNAL-REGULATED KINASE ACTIVATION J. E. GRESACK, a G.

More information

Neural stem cells and the neurobiology of ageing. Chen Siyun 1, Dawe G.S. 2

Neural stem cells and the neurobiology of ageing. Chen Siyun 1, Dawe G.S. 2 ABSTRACT Neural stem cells and the neurobiology of ageing Chen Siyun 1, Dawe G.S. 2 Department of Physics, Faculty of Science, National University of Singapore 10 Kent Ridge Road, Singapore 117546 The

More information

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman BIOMED 509 Executive Control Cambridge University Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan UNM SOM JL Brigman 4-7-17 Symptoms and Assays of Cognitive Disorders Symptoms of Cognitive Disorders Learning & Memory

More information

mtorc2 controls actin polymerization required for consolidation of long-term memory

mtorc2 controls actin polymerization required for consolidation of long-term memory CORRECTION NOTICE Nat. Neurosci.; doi:1.138/nn.3351 mtorc2 controls actin polymerization required for consolidation of long-term memory Wei Huang, Ping Jun Zhu, Shixing Zhang, Hongyi Zhou, Loredana Stoica,

More information

Molecular and Circuit Mechanisms for Hippocampal Learning

Molecular and Circuit Mechanisms for Hippocampal Learning Molecular and Circuit Mechanisms for Hippocampal Learning Susumu Tonegawa 1 and Thomas J. McHugh 1 The hippocampus is crucial for the formation of memories of facts and episodes (Scoville and Milner 1957;

More information

Supplementary Methods

Supplementary Methods 1 Supplementary Methods Social Preference Test Subjects Seventy-four Long-Evans, male rats served as subjects (S-rats) in the foodpreference test, with 40 assigned to the CXT-Same (CXT-S) Condition and

More information

(A) PCR primers (arrows) designed to distinguish wild type (P1+P2), targeted (P1+P2) and excised (P1+P3)14-

(A) PCR primers (arrows) designed to distinguish wild type (P1+P2), targeted (P1+P2) and excised (P1+P3)14- 1 Supplemental Figure Legends Figure S1. Mammary tumors of ErbB2 KI mice with 14-3-3σ ablation have elevated ErbB2 transcript levels and cell proliferation (A) PCR primers (arrows) designed to distinguish

More information

Minute TM Plasma Membrane Protein Isolation and Cell Fractionation Kit User Manual (v5)

Minute TM Plasma Membrane Protein Isolation and Cell Fractionation Kit User Manual (v5) Minute TM Plasma Membrane Protein Isolation and Cell Fractionation Kit Catalog number: SM-005 Description Minute TM plasma membrane (PM) protein isolation kit is a novel and patented native PM protein

More information

Expression of acid base transporters in the kidney collecting duct in Slc2a7 -/-

Expression of acid base transporters in the kidney collecting duct in Slc2a7 -/- Supplemental Material Results. Expression of acid base transporters in the kidney collecting duct in Slc2a7 -/- and Slc2a7 -/- mice. The expression of AE1 in the kidney was examined in Slc26a7 KO mice.

More information

Brain Region-Specific Gene Expression Activation Required for Reconsolidation and Extinction of Contextual Fear Memory

Brain Region-Specific Gene Expression Activation Required for Reconsolidation and Extinction of Contextual Fear Memory 402 The Journal of Neuroscience, January 14, 2009 29(2):402 413 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Brain Region-Specific Gene Expression Activation Required for Reconsolidation and Extinction of Contextual Fear

More information

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety C82NAB Neuroscience and Behaviour Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 1 Outline Emotion I (first part) Studying brain substrates of

More information

Trident Membrane Protein Extraction Kit

Trident Membrane Protein Extraction Kit Cat. No. Size Shelf life GTX16373 5/ 20 tests 12 months at the appropriate storage temperatures (see below) Contents Component Storage Amount for 5 tests Amount for 20 tests Buffer A -20 o C 2.5 ml 10

More information

Suppl. Information Supplementary Figure 1. Strategy/latency analysis of individual mice during maze learning. a,

Suppl. Information Supplementary Figure 1. Strategy/latency analysis of individual mice during maze learning. a, Goal-oriented searching mediated by ventral hippocampus early in trial-and-error learning Ruediger, S, Spirig, D., Donato, F., Caroni, P. Suppl. Information Supplementary Figure 1. Strategy/latency analysis

More information

Chemical Chaperones Mitigate Experimental Asthma By Attenuating Endoplasmic

Chemical Chaperones Mitigate Experimental Asthma By Attenuating Endoplasmic Chemical Chaperones Mitigate Experimental Asthma By Attenuating Endoplasmic Reticulum Stress Lokesh Makhija, BE, Veda Krishnan, MSc, Rakhshinda Rehman, MTech, Samarpana Chakraborty, MSc, Shuvadeep Maity,

More information

Cognitive Function Test. NOR & spatial NOR task. Introduction. Novel Object Recognition (NOR) Estrogen Receptor (ER) in Brain

Cognitive Function Test. NOR & spatial NOR task. Introduction. Novel Object Recognition (NOR) Estrogen Receptor (ER) in Brain Cognitive Function Test Human Rodent Sutcliffe JS, Marshall KM, Neill JC. Behavioral Brain Research 177(2007) 117-125. Radial Arm maze Morris Water maze Siriporn Vongsaiyat 16 th Feb 2007 Novel Object

More information

TITLE: P11, a biomarker for memory retrieval: a possible role in traumatic stress

TITLE: P11, a biomarker for memory retrieval: a possible role in traumatic stress AD Award Number: W81XWH-08-2-0202 TITLE: P11, a biomarker for memory retrieval: a possible role in traumatic stress PRINCIPAL INVESTIGATOR: Lei Zhang, M.D. CONTRACTING ORGANIZATION: Henry M. Jackson Foundation

More information

Supplementary Figure 1

Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 The average sigmoid parametric curves of capillary dilation time courses and average time to 50% peak capillary diameter dilation computed from individual capillary responses averaged

More information

The Effects of Cytotoxic Perirhinal Cortex Lesions on Spatial Learning by Rats: A Comparison of the Dark Agouti and Sprague Dawley Strains

The Effects of Cytotoxic Perirhinal Cortex Lesions on Spatial Learning by Rats: A Comparison of the Dark Agouti and Sprague Dawley Strains Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 120, No. 1, 150 161 0735-7044/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.1.150 The Effects of Cytotoxic Perirhinal

More information

Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK),

Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK), Supplemental Material: Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK), weighing between 280 300 g at the beginning of the experiment, were housed singly in holding rooms

More information

EPIGENETICS: A POSSIBLE MECHANISM OF MEMORY Aliza Grossman Rubenstein

EPIGENETICS: A POSSIBLE MECHANISM OF MEMORY Aliza Grossman Rubenstein Epigenetics: a Possible Mechanism of Memory 43 EPIGENETICS: A POSSIBLE MECHANISM OF MEMORY Aliza Grossman Rubenstein INTRODUCTION The mind-body connection has fascinated philosophers and scientists for

More information

(Stratagene, La Jolla, CA) (Supplemental Fig. 1A). A 5.4-kb EcoRI fragment

(Stratagene, La Jolla, CA) (Supplemental Fig. 1A). A 5.4-kb EcoRI fragment SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION Supplemental Methods Generation of RyR2-S2808D Mice Murine genomic RyR2 clones were isolated from a 129/SvEvTacfBR λ-phage library (Stratagene, La Jolla, CA) (Supplemental Fig.

More information

The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory JERRY W. RUDY University of Colorado, Boulder Sinauer Associates, Inc. Publishers Sunderland, Massachusetts 01375 Table of Contents CHAPTER 1 Introduction: Fundamental

More information

Molecular Biology of Memory Storage: The Persistence of Memory

Molecular Biology of Memory Storage: The Persistence of Memory Molecular Biology of Memory Storage: The Persistence of Memory The Study of Memory Has Two Parts: (1) The Systems Problem of Memory: Where in the brain is memory stored? (2) The Molecular Problem of Memory:

More information

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala 11 December 1997 Nature 390, 604-607 (1997) Macmillan Publishers Ltd. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala MICHAEL T. ROGAN, URSULA V. STÄUBLI & JOSEPH E. LEDOUX

More information

ErbB4 migrazione I parte. 3- ErbB4- NRG1

ErbB4 migrazione I parte. 3- ErbB4- NRG1 ErbB4 migrazione I parte 3- ErbB4- NRG1 1 In rodent brains postnatal neuronal migration is evident in three main areas: the cerebellum (CB), the hippocampus (Hipp) and the rostral migratory stream (RMS).

More information

Deficits in amygdaloid camp-responsive element binding protein signaling play a role in genetic predisposition to anxiety and alcoholism

Deficits in amygdaloid camp-responsive element binding protein signaling play a role in genetic predisposition to anxiety and alcoholism Research article Related Commentary, page 2697 Deficits in amygdaloid camp-responsive element binding protein signaling play a role in genetic predisposition to anxiety and alcoholism Subhash C. Pandey,

More information

The Effects of Chemotherapy on Cognitive Behavior and Neurogenesis in an Animal Model of Pre- and Post- Menopausal Females

The Effects of Chemotherapy on Cognitive Behavior and Neurogenesis in an Animal Model of Pre- and Post- Menopausal Females The Effects of Chemotherapy on Cognitive Behavior and Neurogenesis in an Animal Model of Pre- and Post- Menopausal Females Samantha Pavlock (Medical Student) Pradeep Bhide and Deirdre McCarthy (Faculty

More information

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials Supplementary Materials Fig. S1. Weights of full-dose treatment groups comparing 1 st, 2 nd, and 3 rd generation gene replacement therapy. Mice were treated at p1 with 4x10 11 GC of the three different

More information

The Regulation of Gene Expression During Memory Consolidation in the Hippocampus

The Regulation of Gene Expression During Memory Consolidation in the Hippocampus University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations 1-1-2014 The Regulation of Gene Expression During Memory Consolidation in the Hippocampus Shane Gary Poplawski University

More information

Cognitive Enhancement Strategies. Florian Plattner, James A. Bibb

Cognitive Enhancement Strategies. Florian Plattner, James A. Bibb Cognitive Enhancement Strategies Florian Plattner, James A. Bibb A decline in memory and cognitive function is a natural aspect of aging. In addition, cognitive deficits are comorbid with many mental disorders

More information

Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma

Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma Paper: Abraham, Wickliffe C., and Anthony Robins. "Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma." Trends in neurosciences

More information

Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning Lee, Jonathan

Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning Lee, Jonathan Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning Lee, Jonathan DOI: 10.1038/nn.2205 Document Version Peer reviewed version Citation for published version (Harvard):

More information

Insular Cortex-Amygdala Dialogue During Taste Recognition Memory Formation

Insular Cortex-Amygdala Dialogue During Taste Recognition Memory Formation The highlight for April is by Dr. Federico Bermudez-Rattoni from the Institute of Cellular Physiology, National University of Mexico in Mexico City, Mexico. Dr. Bermudez-Rattoni s work over the past 25

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURES

SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURES SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURES 1 Supplementary Figure 1, Adult hippocampal QNPs and TAPs uniformly express REST a-b) Confocal images of adult hippocampal mouse sections showing GFAP (green), Sox2 (red), and REST

More information

Inducible and Selective Erasure of Memories in the Mouse Brain via Chemical-Genetic Manipulation

Inducible and Selective Erasure of Memories in the Mouse Brain via Chemical-Genetic Manipulation Article Inducible and Selective Erasure of Memories in the Mouse Brain via Chemical-Genetic Manipulation Xiaohua Cao, 1,3 Huimin Wang, 2,3 Bing Mei, 1,2,3 Shuming An, 1 Liang Yin, 1 L. Phillip Wang, 2

More information

GENETIC APPROACHES TO MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR COGNITION: A Focus on LTP and Learning and Memory

GENETIC APPROACHES TO MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR COGNITION: A Focus on LTP and Learning and Memory Annu. Rev. Genet. 2002. 36:687 720 doi: 10.1146/annurev.genet.36.062802.091007 Copyright c 2002 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved First published online as a Review in Advance on September 12, 2002

More information

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 1st Midterm Exam Ready for Pickup By the elevator on the 3 rd Floor of Pacific Hall (waiver) Exam Depot Window at the north entrance to Pacific Hall (no waiver) Mon-Fri, 10:00

More information

Instructions for Use. APO-AB Annexin V-Biotin Apoptosis Detection Kit 100 tests

Instructions for Use. APO-AB Annexin V-Biotin Apoptosis Detection Kit 100 tests 3URGXFW,QIRUPDWLRQ Sigma TACS Annexin V Apoptosis Detection Kits Instructions for Use APO-AB Annexin V-Biotin Apoptosis Detection Kit 100 tests For Research Use Only. Not for use in diagnostic procedures.

More information

What to watch for when analyzing mouse behavior

What to watch for when analyzing mouse behavior NEWS What to watch for when analyzing mouse behavior BY ALLA KATSNELSON 21 MARCH 2018 Tests for unusual behavior in mice are notoriously prone to operator error. Many scientists conduct or interpret them

More information

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories? CASE 49 A 43-year-old woman is brought to her primary care physician by her family because of concerns about her forgetfulness. The patient has a history of Down syndrome but no other medical problems.

More information

Protein MultiColor Stable, Low Range

Protein MultiColor Stable, Low Range Product Name: DynaMarker Protein MultiColor Stable, Low Range Code No: DM670L Lot No: ******* Size: 200 μl x 3 (DM670 x 3) (120 mini-gel lanes) Storage: 4 C Stability: 12 months at 4 C Storage Buffer:

More information

Dynamic expression of SynDIG1 mrna in cerebellar Purkinje neurons

Dynamic expression of SynDIG1 mrna in cerebellar Purkinje neurons Current Neurobiology 2010; 1 (1): 77-81 Dynamic expression of SynDIG1 mrna in cerebellar Purkinje neurons Elva Díaz* Department of Pharmacology, UC Davis School of Medicine, Davis, CA 95616, USA Abstract

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html Memory Working Memory Long-term Memory Declarative Memory Procedural Memory

More information

Supplementary Figure S1. Effect of stress during withdrawal on expression of sensitization to repeated cocaine exposure in WT and D2R / mice.

Supplementary Figure S1. Effect of stress during withdrawal on expression of sensitization to repeated cocaine exposure in WT and D2R / mice. Supplementary Figure S1. Effect of stress during withdrawal on expression of sensitization to repeated cocaine exposure in WT and D2R / mice. The time course of locomotor activity for WT (a, b) or D2R

More information

Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Grid Cell The Hippocampus Serves a Role in Multimodal Information Processing and Memory

More information

The levels of mrna expression in the mouse brain were measured at 52 dpi after the brains were divided into five regions: cortex, hippocampus,

The levels of mrna expression in the mouse brain were measured at 52 dpi after the brains were divided into five regions: cortex, hippocampus, Supplemental material Supplemental method RNA extraction, reverse transcription, and real-time PCR The levels of mrna expression in the mouse brain were measured at 52 dpi after the brains were divided

More information

Increased serotonin transporter expression reduces fear and recruitment of. parvalbumin interneurons of the amygdala

Increased serotonin transporter expression reduces fear and recruitment of. parvalbumin interneurons of the amygdala Increased serotonin transporter expression reduces fear and recruitment of parvalbumin interneurons of the amygdala Marco Bocchio, Giulia Fucsina, Lydia Oikonomidis, Stephen B McHugh, David M Bannerman,

More information

PERSPECTIVES. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and anxiety

PERSPECTIVES. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and anxiety OPINION Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and anxiety David M. Bannerman, Rolf Sprengel, David J. Sanderson, Stephen B. McHugh, J. Nicholas P. Rawlins, Hannah Monyer and Peter H. Seeburg

More information

Anti-Lamin B1/LMNB1 Picoband Antibody

Anti-Lamin B1/LMNB1 Picoband Antibody Anti-Lamin B1/LMNB1 Picoband Antibody Catalog Number:PB9611 About LMNB1 Lamin-B1 is a protein that in humans is encoded by the LMNB1 gene. The nuclear lamina consists of a two-dimensional matrix of proteins

More information