Regional glucose metabolic changes after learning a complex visuospatial/motor task: a positron emission tomographic study

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Regional glucose metabolic changes after learning a complex visuospatial/motor task: a positron emission tomographic study"

Transcription

1 134 Brain Research, 570 (1992) Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. BRES Regional glucose metabolic changes after learning a complex visuospatial/motor task: a positron emission tomographic study Richard J. Haier, Benjamin V. Siegel Jr., Andrew MacLachlan, Eric Soderling, Stephen Lottenberg and Monte S. Buchsbaum Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA (U.S.A.) (Accepted 29 August 1991) Key words: Learning; Intelligence; Fluorodeoxyglucose; Positron emission tomography; Glucose; Brain efficiency Regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate (GMR) quantified with positron emission tomography (PET) with 18-fluoro-2-deoxyglucose (FDG) was measured twice in 8 young men performing a complex visuospatial/motor task (the computer game Tetris), before and after practice. After 4-8 weeks of daily practice on Tetris, GMR in cortical surface regions decreased despite a more than 7-fold increase in performance. Subjects who improved their Tetris performance the most after practice showed the largest glucose metabolic decreases after practice in several areas. These results suggest that learning may result in decreased use of extraneous or inefficient brain areas. Changes in regional subcortical glucose metabolic rate with practice may reflect changes in cognitive strategy that are a part of the learning process. INTRODUCTION Superior performance of complex cognitive tasks has been associated with lower overall brain glucose metabolic rate (GMR) in 3 positron emission tomographic (PET) studies 1'7'1 and lower cerebral blood flow (CBF) in one study (Berman, personal communication). The PET studies, measuring regional brain uptake of 18- fluoro-2-deoxyglucose (FDG) uptake during the performance of the Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM) 7, verbal fluency 1, or on a separate session from assessment of one subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) ~, showed statistically significant negative correlations between glucose use and task performance in normal adults. These negative correlations suggest that the brains of the subjects doing the tasks well might function more efficiently than those of the poor performers 6'7'H. Subjects performing a complex task well may use a limited group of brain circuits and/or fewer neurons, thus requiring minimal glucose use, while poor performers use more circuits and/or neurons, some of which are inessential or detrimental to task performance, and this is reflected in higher overall brain glucose metabolism. Or efficiency could be a characteristic of neurons throughout the cortex so that any task performed in any combination of areas might use less glucose in a more efficient brain. Both views are consistent with the pattern of inverse correlations reported between psychometric task performance and GMR across the whole cortex. In the present study, we looked for within-individual changes in brain glucose use when performing a visuospatial/motor task (the Tetris computer game) for the first time and then after practice. The task, described below, was chosen because it is difficult enough so that marked and continuous improvement is shown over many sessions so that brain activity during good and poor performance can be compared. Also, to determine which brain regions are important in performing this task, regional brain glucose metabolic rate was compared to that in subjects viewing visual stimuli passively. We hypothesized that, with improved performance of the task after practice, there would be overall decreased brain FDG uptake, consistent with the inverse correlations found between GMR and complex task performance. We also predicted negative correlations between task performance and absolute glucose metabolic rate (GMR). We expected relative changes in GMR after practice might occur in those brain regions that are used in the performance of this kind of task, those important in attention, planning, visuospatial manipulation, problem-solving and motor learning, motor coordination, and sensory and motor functions of the right hand. Correspondence: R. Haier, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of California, Irvine, Med. Sci. I, Rm. D404, Irvine, CA 92717, U.S.A.

2 135 lil I::IIIIIHII I:: I.',~11111::11111o Io Io I I.-k'lll;:l::lllllo I ",/.17".1~!~, I:: x'lllil:: IIIIO IO IO I--I.~1 Fig. 1. The Tetris game in progress from left to right. Subjects attempt to manipulate configurations of 4 square blocks falling from the top of the screen to produce solid rows of blocks. Once a solid row of blocks is completed, it disappears and is replaced by the row above it. Symbol markings show individual shapes which have already been placed. Note the bottom row of the middle pannel is complete; in the panel on the fight it has disappeared (one point is scored) and the rows above moved down. MATERIALS AND METHODS Twenty-four right-handed males volunteered to undergo PET scans. Eight performed the Tetris learning task, and 16 were in the Continuous Performance Task (CPT) no task condition during the 35-rain glucose uptake period. Both conditions are described below. The group performing the learning task were years old (mean + S.D ), and the CPT no task group were years old (mean S.D. = ). All were recruited by advertisements on the UCI campus, and were screened and found to be without any history of psychiatric, neurological, or major medical disorder. Each of the subjects who performed the Tetris learning task was scanned with FDG PET on two occasions 4-8 weeks (mean + S.D. = days) apart. On the first occasion (naive scan), each subject was injected with FDG after a maximum of 3 min of practice playing the video game, Tetris, and on the second occasion (practiced scan), after Tetris practice sessions of rain in duration, 5 times/week. During the 35-min FDG uptake period for both scans, the subjects played the Tetris game, which requires the subject, using a computer keyboard, to rotate and move objects, consisting of 4 square blocks in various configurations (see Fig. 1) in such a fashion as to try to create a solid row of blocks. The objects move downward from the top of the screen, and as the game progresses, if performance is good, their speed increases. When a solid row is complete, the row disappears, and all the blocks above it drop down to replace the completed row. The score was visible on the screen at all times and equals the number of lines of blocks that are created filling the entire horizontal aspect of the computer screen. Subjects were instructed to try to get the highest score possible. All subjects used their right hand during task performance. The number of button presses and the amount of hand movement were not recorded. The subjects in the CPT no task condition 7'9 passively viewed visual stimuli on a screen during the 35 min following FDG injection. On the screen, single digits (0-9) were projected for 40 ms at a rate of one every 2 s. Subjects were told to view the stimuli, but were not asked to respond to them in any way. After the 35-min FDG uptake period, subjects were moved to the PET scanner, a NeuroECAT. Resolution in plane is 7.6 mm and 10.9 mm in the Z-dimension (verified at time of acceptance). Scanner resolution of 7.6 mm FWHM (full width half maximum) governed the structures selected for analysis. Many structures, includ- ing the medial areas of the frontal lobe, cingulate, thalamus, pons, and cerebellum extend vertically more than one slice thickness, diminishing further the risks of partial voluming. Nine or 10 slice planes were obtained parallel to the canthomental (CM) line at 10 mm increments starting at 85% of head height (vertex to CM line, usually about cm). Patients were scanned for min. Scans were transformed to glucose metabolic rates (GMR) as elsewhere 2. The min resolution of the deoxyglucose method in this study may have advantages over the shorter time resolution of blood flow measures. Since the task requires continuous performance on a series of randomly varying stimuli, a short time interval for assessment might be more variable both in physiological and behavioral response; no clear advantage of examining only 40 s of behavior using a shorter half-life tracer is apparent. Cortical regions of interest were measured using our cortical peel technique3"4; subcortical and medial cortical structures were located using stereotaxic coordinates derived from a standard neuroanatomical atlas 8. Where slice levels are indicated, they are expressed as percentage of head height measured perpendicular to the canthomeatal line from the line to the highest point on the head. Greater percentages of head height indicate higher (more dorsal) regions. In addition to the analysis of the entire cortical surface, in order to measure activity in the motor and sensory cortical regions for the hand, we analyzed GMR for the precentral and postcentral cortical peel regions exactly as in our previous study 4, but included only the 80, 74, and 68% atlas levels. These areas approximate those characterized by Penfield and Rasmussen 12 in their classical intraoperative brain stimulation studies. In addition to the 63 bilateral (126 total) subcortical and medial cortical regions-of-interest (ROI) included in our standard template used in our previous studies, we added regions of interest in the pons and cerebellum (Fig. 5). The coordinates of these areas were developed from magnetic resonance images (MRI) of 10 normal subjects. The structures were analyzed at the 14% Matsui and Hirano atlas level, but the anterior/posterior and left/right brain dimensions were taken from a higher slice (28% level) to avoid potential variability in the frontal lobe edges on the PET scan from affecting the position of the template. Boxes, 3 by 3 pixels (57.8 mm 2) in size, were placed in the cerebellum and one by one pixel (6.3 mm 2) boxes were placed in the pons. The cerebellar peduncle boxes were placed immediately lateral and posterior to the lateral and posterior edges of the pons. In the anteroposterior dimension, the vermis boxes were placed anterior to the posterior edge of the cerebellum, at a distance of one third the maximum distance between anterior pons and the posterior edge of the cerebellum (MAP). In the left/right dimension, these boxes were placed in the midline, such that left and right vermis boxes overlapped. The anterior cerebellar cortex boxes were placed 15% of the maximum width of the cerebellum (MWC) away from the lateral edges of the cerebellum and at half the MAP from the posterior edge of the cerebellum. The posterior cerebellar cortex boxes were placed at 20% of MWC from the lateral edges and 20% of MAP from the posterior edge of the cerebellum. The middle cerebellar cortex box was placed exactly halfway between the anterior and posterior cerebellar boxes. Errors in ROI placement result primarily from two sources: failure to return the head to the same position in the second scan, and individual divergence from the proportional atlas coordinates. We measured variation in our headholder using MRI in a separate study in which a triangle of vaseline-filled tubes was placed on the thermoplastic mask. The transection of this set of tubing shows up as two dots and slices at different heights above the CM line will have varying distances between the two points. In 5 subjects scanned twice, we measured the distance between the two tubing marks on each occasion. Using the proportion of the triangle, we were then able to calculate the variation in vertical (axial) height. The mean of the absolute value of the axial height differences was 2.6 mm, S.D. = 2.3 mm. We also measured the anteroposterior distance

3 136 between a horizontal line drawn from one tube reference mark and the posterior tip of the caudate nucleus. The mean absolute value of this distance was 2.3 mm, S.D. = 1.7 mm. Thus, our repositioning error was about 2 mm on repeat MRI (small with respect to PET FWHM) and is expected to be in the same range for repeat PET on the same subject since the identical mask and headholder are used for both MRI and PET. Individual divergence from proportional atlas coordinates would affect the metabolic measurement, but would be the same from scan 1 to scan 2 and not produce spurious learning effects. However, we have measured the accuracy and limits of the stereotaxic method with MRI for our smaller structures, including the caudate nucleus and vermis. Comparisons for these brain areas show a good correspondence between our sterotaxic and our MRI templates. For example, the caudate (center of mass) is located at stereotaxic atlas proportional coordinates x = 59.8%, y = 30.9%; actual MRI measurement in 20 subjects selected at random showed the caudate center of mass at x = 58.6% (S.D. = 1.8) and y = 33.6% (S.D. = 2.0). The vermis was clearly visible on 18 of 20 subjects. We measured the width of the vermis at its widest point. The anteroposterior (y) position and left and right margins (x) were recorded and transformed to percentages as described above. The measurements showed a mean (y) position of 78.5% and left and right (x) positions of 45.1% and 54.1% Our atlas coordinates of 79.3% (y) and 49% thus fall well within the vermis. For individuals one S.D. below the mean width, our entire box falls inside the vermis. Further calculation shows that for 94.1% of individuals, the box is entirely within the vermis. Anxiety might decline from scan 1 to scan 2, and this decline could be associated with changes in metabolic rate. Therefore, we assessed state anxiety with the Spielberger scale t3 just before FDG injection and again just after the end of the uptake (before the scan). BMDP 5 was used for all statistical analyses including analysis of variance (ANOVA, BMDP 4V), t-tests (3D), and correlations (8D). For each region the Tetris subjects were compared to themselves in the two conditions (naive vs practiced), and each condition was compared to the CPT no task group. Because of the large intersubject variability of whole brain glucose metabolic rate, we routinely calculate a ratio of regional GMR to whole brain mean GMR (for surface cortical structures) or to whole scan slice mean GMR (for subcortical structures) for each region. This ratio, termed the relative glucose metabolic rate, controls for this source of variance. All statistical analyses were done for both GMR and relative GMR. For cortical regions, 3 4-way ANOVA's were done with condition (naive vs practiced conditions, and each of these conditions vs CPT no task), hemisphere (left, right), brain lobe (frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital), and anteroposterior lobar segment (4 gyral areas in each lobe). To generate new hypotheses of learning, exploratory t-tests were performed for all brain regions for both GMR and relative GMR (GMR divided by whole slice mean GMR). Because the same group of subjects was scanned twice, one-sample t-tests were performed for practiced minus naive GMR for each region for comparisons between naive and practiced conditions. For the Tetris subjects, for each scan and for scan 2 minus scan 1, correlations between GMR and task performance for GMR and relative metabolic rates were calculated and tested for 2-tailed significance. Only correlations for analogous score and GMR are reported, i.e. naive GMR vs naive score, practiced GMR vs practiced score, and practiced minus naive GMR vs practiced minus naive score; not naive GMR vs practiced score etc. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were applied post-hoc in any tests of significance where P < We are also reporting any P < 0.05 differences uncorrected for multiple tests, because the number of comparisons done in an exploratory PET study is necessarily quite large, making it extremely difficult for any one comparison to reach statistical significance by Bonferroni criteria. On the one hand, those regions reaching significance prior to c O 10'4 0 ~----~ I O- I -- ~0 215 I 4-- 4iO ---I 0 5, Doys of Pro(rice Fig. 2. Tetris learning curve. Plots average score (lines completed) at each practice session. Some later sessions included fewer subjects. Bonferroni correction may be useful for hypothesis generation and comparison to other studies. But, given the large number of comparisons, a P < 0.05 level of significance must be interpreted with caution. RESULTS Game performance All 8 Tetris subjects showed markedly improved scores from the first to the second scan. The average + S.D. number of lines completed during the first scan was with a range of , and during the second scan, average + S.D. was with a range of The correlation between practiced score and naive score was 0.23 (NS). A learning curve (Fig. 2) was generated by averaging the scores of all the subjects on their nth practice session. Some subjects had more prac- tice sessions than others, so fewer subjects were averaged for the later sessions. The learning curve demonstrates that performance improves at the most rapid rate during the first sessions and at a slower rate after that. Since the points in the curve for the later times represent fewer subjects, no accurate estimate of the time after which no improvement occurs can be made. Anxiety There were no differences in state anxiety score before and after either the naive or the practiced scans (mean for prenaive scan, 31.3; S.D. = 9.1 and postnaive, 33.4; S.D. = 6.8; mean for prepractice scan, 31.3; S.D. = 5.6 and postpractice, 34.8; S.D. = 7.0; all comparisons not significant by t-tests). Comparisons of the CPT no task group to the Tetris groups Tables I and II show GMR and relative GMR com- parisons, respectively, among the Tetris naive and prac- ticed conditions and the CPT no task group for cortical

4 137 TABLE I GMR in cortical lobes compared among CPT no task (n = 16), naive and practiced Tetris conditions (n = 8) Surface cortical GMR compared by 4-way ANOVA with repeated measures, condition (naive, praticed) x lobe (frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital) x segment (1-4) x hemisphere (left, right), shows trend for main effect for condition (F = 5.18, df 1,7; P = 0.057). See Tables III and IV for cortical segment differences. Same 4-way ANOVA (non-repeated measures) for naive vs CPT no task: group! lobe F = 4.66, Huynh-Feldt df 1.46, 32.08; P = 0.026; group! segment! hemisphere F = 2.78, Huynh-Feldt df 2.98, 65.49; P = 0.048; group! segment! hemisphere x lobe F = 2.18, Huynh-Feldt df 5.79, ; P = For practiced vs CPT no task: no significant effects. The analysis of the hand motor and sensory areas revealed a left greater than right asymmetry in GMR for the Tetris naive group compared to the opposite asymmetry in the CPT no task group. This was tested by ANOVA: group (Tetris, CPT) x level (top 3 brain slices) x structure (precentral, postcentral gyrus; see Fig. 3)! hemisphere (right, left); for GMR, level! hemisphere x group, F = 16.14, df 1,22; P = ; for relative GMR, hemisphere x group interaction, F = 14.43, df 1,22; P = Means showed the left greater than right asymmetry in levels 74 and 68% of head height for Naive Practiced CPT no task Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Frontal Parietal Temporal Occipital Whole cortex areas. GMR is most similar between the CPT no task group and the practiced Tetris condition. The naive Tetris condition shows the highest GMR for cortex. An ANOVA comparing naive Tetris to CPT no task (group! lobe! segment x hemisphere) showed significant interactions for group x lobe; group! segment x hemisphere; and group! lobe x segment x hemisphere (see legend Table I). These differences are illustrated in Fig. 3. For relative GMR, the same ANOVA showed significant effects for group and for group x lobe interaction (see legend Table II). There were no significant effects in the ANOVAs comparing the practiced Tetris condition with the CPT no task group. TABLE II Relative GMR in cortical lobes compared among CPT no task (n = 16), naive and practiced Tetris conditions (n = 18) Surface cortical relative GMR compared by 4-way ANOVA with repeated measures, condition (naive, practiced)! lobe (frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital)! segment (1-4) x hemisphere (left, right): no significant effects. Same 4-way ANOVA (nonrepeated measures) for naive vs CPT no task: group effect, F = 4.72, df 1,22; P = 0.04; group! lobe F = 5.03, Huynh-Feldt df 1.84, 40.37; P = For practiced vs CPT no task: no significant effects. Naive Practiced CPT no task Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Frontal Parietal Temporal Occipital Whole cortex Precentral / Postcentral.... / Supramarginal M,ddle r r n t. ~ e i. i o r ~ _ parietal Iobule Superior, I f l ~ / ~ ~,.Angular gyrus frontal \ J,,x~1111iii~i~i.:'.~i~J.liii:: ::~::i~::::::~//'~'~ll'~l Lateral occipital l I~!:.::i:.:.lvJ77/~i~i~i~T '.--~B--19 I'f:iiii~K/'///~ ~ilililiiii~////,i t::i~ ~7 kll:iiiilf///////lr ' / ~ 1 9 Inferior frontal / ~. ~. ~ ~ 7. / Middle tempora-'~ Infertor temporal Superior temporal Posterior temporal Fig. 3. Surface cortical regions differing in relative GMR between subjects in the Tetris naive condition and subjects in the CPT no task condition. The bottom illustration shows the position of the cortical gyri with individual gyri shaded to show anatomy. In the top illustrations (right and left hemispheres, respectively), areas where Tetris naive was higher than CPT no task are shaded black, indicating two-tailed P < , (P < 0.05 after Bonferroni correction). Diagonal lines indicate P < 0.05 (but NS after Bonferroni correction), and no shading indicates areas not significant. Speckling indicates the regions where CPT no task subjects had higher relative GMR than did Tetris subjects (P < 0.05, NS after Bonferroni correction).

5 138,,,~//,g///4 k' "~-'/////~ NI life I/ ~/4F//,A.,, L. _, ~ ~l ( I J ~////~ y ~ "X,N ~ ~, 4 / t_..4r_l~i JA" A I~ I'k-x-k M I...f~ V~:~..,el7 Fig. 4. Cortical regions showing decreases in GMR with practice (see Table IV). The name of each region is shown in Fig. 3 (bottom). All significant decreases (shading) were bilateral except for the posterior temporal gyrus (left only). Tetris but not for CPT no task. The 80% level was sym- metrical, suggesting a hand area localization. We ex- pected Tetris to activate the left motor strip more than CPT no task. The precentral gyrus had the expected asymmetrical motor activation -- asymmetry scores (right minus left/left plus right) were for Tetris and 0.01 for CPT no task (t = 2.43, P = 0.027). These results demonstrate that this sample size was sufficient to reveal regional task effects. Cortical glucose metabolic rate changes with practice A condition (naive vs practiced Tetris) by hemisphere by lobe by segment repeated measures ANOVA for cerebral cortical GMR showed a nearly significant condition effect (F = 5.18, df 1,7; P = 0.057). As predicted, this effect was a decrease in GMR with practice (whole cortex mean GMR naive, 39.2, S.D ; mean practiced, 28.8, S.D. = 7.1, see Table I). This condition effect was not significant for relative GMR. Since all higher order interactions with condition had trend level (<0.10) significance, we performed exploratory simple interaction analyses for each lobe (repeated measures, condition by hemisphere by segment). These showed a condition effect (F = 5.22, df 1,7; P = 0.056) and a condition! segment effect (F = 5.31, Huynh-Feldt df 1.95, 13.65; P = 0.02) for the frontal lobe, a condition effect for the parietal lobe (F = 5.51, df 1,7; P = 0.051), TABLE III GMR and relative GMR for each surface cortical region in naive and practiced conditions: left and right hemisphere values averaged Condition by lobe by segment by hemisphere ANOVA showed a nearly significant condition effect (F = 5.18, df 1,7; P = 0.057). Condition (naive vs practiced) by segment by hemisphere ANOVA's (simple interactions) for GMR for each lobe showed the following effects: a: frontal lobe: condition (F = 5.22, df 1,7; P = 0.056); condition by segment (F , Huynh-Feldt df 1.95, 13.65; P ). b: parietal lobe: condition (F = 5.51, df 1,7; P = 0.051). c: temporal lobe: none. d: occipital lobe: condition by segment (F = 5.86, Huyn- Feldt df 1.77, 12.38; P = 0.02); condition by segment by hemisphere (F = 4.12, Huynh-Feldt df 2.11, 14.78; P = 0.04). For relative GMR, the only significant simple interaction was condition by segment for the frontal lobe (F = 3.41, Huynh-Feldt df 2.56, 17.93; P = 0.046). Cortical regions GMR Relative GMR Frontal lobe gyri Superior frontal Middle frontal Inferior frontal Precentral 46.1" Parietal lobe gyri Postcentral 40.8* Supramarginal 41.9* Angular 45.3* Superior parietal Temporal lobe gyri Superior temporal Middle temporal Inferior temporal Posterior temporal Occipital lobe Brodmann's areas Area Area * Area 17, dorsal half Area 17, ventral half * Differs from practiced condition by one sample, 2-tailed t-test, P < Naive Practiced Naive Practiced Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D "

6 139 Sul~rlor F Svl~rlor From~ml G ~ v s ~ ~ Frlmtol Gyra Medial FrontIl N '~;..i~i ~ %1, ~+~ ~!i:~i White Matter " CinQulatl Gytul IdickJlll.+.+~+ L COl+pUS ~OIO414Ht Cingulota Gyrul ~ [i ~L... i, poltlrior CinlFltOll G),rul ~ V prtcuniul- PatallUl~ h'~ I~t "-.~P'_~,~ ~ 'j +-'-" - + ~ W~ Ant~rtm' ktol Gyrul Orblto4 G y r ~ PoItlriOr RIctol Gyrw - i 1 ~cl~ Pmllri0+ Cerebellum- br~-~' ++ +.!, i+ ram, I% pxllrkdr l~rlllllror 4Mlrltl Fig. 5. Subcortical regions-of-interest showing changes in GMR between naive and practiced Tetris scans, as detailed in Table V. Each slice is labeled with percent head height above the canthomeatal line. Black boxes are locations where P < 0.05.

7 140 and condition! segment (F = 5.86, Huynh-Feldt df 1.77, 12.38; P = 0.02) and condition! segment x hemi- sphere (F = 4.12, Huynh-Feldt df 2.11, 14.78; P = 0.04) effects for the occipital lobe (see Tables I and III). For relative GMR only the frontal lobe (condition! seg- ment, F = 3.41, Huynh-Feldt df 2.56, 17.93; P = 0.046) was significant (see Tables II and III). Two-tailed t-tests comparing each cortical region between the two condi- tions (see Table IV and Fig. 4) showed significant GMR decreases with practice for the left and right precentral cortex, the left and right postcentral cortex, the left an- gular gyrus (right angular gyrus P = 0.052), the left posterior temporal cortex, and the left area 17 of the occipital cortex (right area 17 P = 0.052). For relative GMR, significant decreases were in the left postcentral gyrus and the left angular gyrus. A significant GMR increase was in area 18 of the right occipital cortex. Since we noted the greatest relative activation be- tween Tetris and CPT no task in the left motor strip area at the 74% level, we examined metabolic change with practice in this area. A significant decrease was confirmed (-20.0, practice minus naive difference score, one sample t = 3.55, P = 0.009). Levels 80 and 68% were not significant, nor was the difference for the right side. Thus, the largest drop was for an area identified as the left hand area in the Tetris naive vs control task contrast. TABLE IV GMR and relative GMR in cortical segments compared between naive and practiced conditions (areas significant by t-test only) All these comparisons were non-significant after Bonferroni correction. Segment GMR Naive Practiced 2-tailed P level Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Left precentral Right precentral Left post. central Right post. central Left angular gyrus Right angular gyrus (0.052) 1 Left post. temp Left area Right area (0.052) 1 Relative GMR Left post. central Left angular gyrus Right area These nearly significant results are included to show bilaterality comparisons. TABLE V Subcortical and medial cortical regions showing significant differences (by t-test, 2-tailed) in GMR between naive and practiced conditions All these comparisons were non-significant after Bonferroni correction. Region Naive Practiced Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P Right paracentral 80% Left superior frontal 74% Right superior frontal 74% Left paracentral 74% Right paracentral 74% Left precuneus 74% Right precuneus 74% Left paracentral 68% Left superior frontal 61% Right ant. cingulate 54% Left post. cingulate 54% Right post. cingulate 54% Left precuneus 54% Right precuneus 54% Left mid. corpus coll. 47% Left caudate 41% Left ant. cingulate 34% Left frontal white 34% Left putamen 34% Right putamen 34% Right globus pallidus 34% Left superior coll. 34% Left fusiform 34% Right fusiform 34% Left ant. cingulate 28% Left putamen 28% Right inferior coll. 28% Left orbital gyrus 21% Left uncus 21% Right posterior pons 14% Right cereb. vermis 14% Left mid. cereb. cortex 14% Left ant. cereb. cortex 14% Right post. cereb. cortex 14% % indicates percent of head height from CM line of the slice in which the structure is measured according to Matsui and Hirano. 2 These nearly significant results are included to show bilaterality comparisons.

8 141 TABLE VI TABLE VII Subcortical and medial cortical regions showing significant differences (by t-test, 2-tailed) in relative GMR between naive and practiced conditions Regions with significant correlations between Tetris score and glucose metabolic rate in each condition All these comparisons were non-significant after Bonferroni correction. Region Right precuneus 74% 1 Left sup. frontal 61% Left cingulate 61% Right post. cingulate 54% Left ant. cingulate 28% Right hippocampus 28% Left mid. cereb, cort. 14% Left ant. cereb, cort. 14% Right post. cereb, cort. 14% Naive Practiced Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P ' % indicates percent of head height from CM line of the slice in which the structure is measured according to Matsui and Hirano. S u b c o r t i c a l a n d m e d i a l cortical g l u c o s e m e t a b o l i c rate comparisons between naive and practiced conditions E x p l o r a t o r y t w o - t a i l e d t-tests b e t w e e n the n a i v e and practiced conditions for G M R are shown in Table V (only areas w h e r e there is a significant difference are shown). G M R decreases significantly with practice in 7 areas bilaterally, 7 areas on the fight only and 13 areas on the left only. R e l a t i v e G M R (see Table VI) significant decreases are in t h e left s u p e r i o r frontal c o r t e x, the left a n t e r i o r cingulate, the right p o s t e r i o r cingulate gyrus, t h e left Naive condition r Practiced condition r GMR Relative GMR left caudate right superior colliculus right posterior rectal gyrus none GMR Relative GMR left superior frontal left putamen left inferior temporal right cerebellar vermis none Practiced minus naive condition GMR r Relative GMR r Left paracentral gyrus Right mid. corpus cal. Right post. corpus cal. Right frontal white Left globus pallidus Right globus pallidus Left putamen 28% Right inferior coll. Left ant. rectal gyrus Right ant. rectal gyrus Left post. rectal gyrus Left amygdala Left hippocampus Right hippocampus Right post. cerebellum Left anterior pons Right anterior pons Left posterior pons Right mid. cereb, cort Right frontal white Left anterior thai. Left med. thalamus Right lateral thai. Right caudate Left putamen 34% Right putamen 34% Left sup. coll. Right putamen 28% Fig. 6. PET images (41% atlas slice) of a subject in naive and practiced conditions, showing decreases in GMR.

9 142 anterior and middle cerebellar cortex, and right posterior cerebellar cortex. Significant relative GMR increases are in the right precentral frontal cortex, the right hippocampus, and the left cingulate gyrus. All these significant areas are shown in Fig. 5. Fig. 6 shows an example of a PET slice for one subject before and after practice. Correlations between GMR and task performance No correlations between GMR and Tetris score in either condition were significant (Table VII) in an exploratory analysis. For relative GMR, 3 areas showed correlations between metabolic rate and score in the naive condition and another 4 areas showed correlations in the practiced condition. The change in GMR (practiced minus naive), however, shows widespread correlations with the change in score (practiced minus naive). For GMR, all 19 significant correlations are negative whereas, for relative GMR, eight of nine significant correlations are positive. None of these areas (listed in Table VII) overlap between GMR and relative GMR with the exception of fight frontal white matter. No cortical areas showed significant correlations between GMR change and score change. DISCUSSION The major finding of this study was the decrease in overall brain glucose metabolism following practice. This is consistent with our previous study 6'7 which showed a correlation between better performance on a difficult test of abstract reasoning and lower glucose metabolism. The findings of the 1988 study generated the hypothesis that the brains of those who are better at a task are more efficient, i.e. use less energy during the task. The findings of the current study that, after learning, brain energy use is reduced during task performance is not inconsistent with our hypothesis. While it is possible that individual brain cells metabolize glucose more efficiently after learning (e.g. derive more phosphate bonds per mole of glucose), this explanation for our findings seems unlikely. We believe that during the first attempts at playing the game, the subjects are trying out many different cognitive strategies for the task, thus using many different brain circuits involving varied brain areas. After much practice, it is likely that subjects have developed a set strategy for performance of the task and thus use fewer brain circuits and/or fewer neurons per circuit with the resulting less overall brain activity. The correlation between improvement on the task and decreasing brain glucose use suggests then that those who honed their cognitive strategy to the fewest circuits improved the most. The absence of significant correlations between task performance and overall brain metabolism in both the practiced and naive conditions fails to support our 1988 findings. The Tetris task involves hand-eye coordination and spatial operations. It is therefore, interesting that Table III reveals that the cortical areas which change with practice involve motor and sensory strip, the primary visual region, and the angular gyrus. GMR in these areas decreases significantly after learning, as predicted. Additional regions in the medial temporal lobe, medial frontal lobe, basal ganglia, and cerebellum may be important both for attention and hand-eye coordination. No area showed an increase in GMR but some areas showed increases relative to the brain slice. These areas (Tables IV, VI), including the right area 18 and parts of the cingulate gyrus and hippocampus, may be involved in a shift of cognitive strategy to use these areas. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Our PET study of changes in regional glucose metabolic rate after practice of a complex visuospatial/motor task showed a significant decrease in whole brain glucose metabolism with learning, and showed changes in GMR in several medial cortical and subcortical regions, the precuneus, medial superior frontal gyrus, cingulate gyrus, hippocampus, and cerebellum. We believe that the decrease in overall brain glucose with practice reflects a more selective use of brain circuitry, reflecting a better-honed cognitive strategy which was formed during the learning process. Changes in metabolism in hippocampus and frontal cortex with practice may reflect the importance of those regions in learning and planning. REFERENCES 1 Berent, S., Giordani, B., Lehtinen, S., Markel, D., Penney, J.B., Buchtel, H.A., Starosta-Rubinstein, S., Hichwa, R. and Young, A.B., Positron emission tomographic scan investigations of Huntington's disease: cerebral metabolic correlates of cognitive function, Ann. Neurol., 23 (1988) Buchsbaum, M.S., Wu, J., Haier, R., Hazlett, E., Ball, R., Katz, M., Sokolski, K., Lagunas-Solar, M. and Langer, D., Positron emission tomography assessment of effects of benzodiazepines on regional glucose metabolic rate in patients with anxiety disorder, Life Sci., 40 (1987) Buchsbaum, M.S., DeLisi, L.E., Holcomb, H.H., Cappelletti, J., King, A.C., Johnson, J., Hazlett, E., Dowling-Zimmerman, S., Post, R.M., Morihisa, J., Carpenter, W., Cohen, R., Pickar, D., Weinberger, D.R., Margolin, R. and Kessler, R.M., Anteroposterior gradients in cerebral glucose use in schizophrenia and affective disorders, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 41 (1984) Buchsbaum, M.S., Gillin, J.C., Wu, J., Hazlett, E., Sicotte, N., Dupont, R.M. and Bunney Jr., W.E., Regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate in human sleep assessed by positron emission

10 143 tomography, Life Sci., 45 (1989) Dixon, W.J., BMD Biomedical Computer Programs, University of California Press, Berkeley, Haier, R.J., LaFalase, J., Katz, M. and Buchsbaum, M.S., Brain efficiency and intelligence: inverse correlations between cerebral glucose metabolic rate and abstract reasoning, submitted. 7 Haier, R.J., Siegel, B.V., Nuechterlein, K.H., Hazlett, E., Wu, J.C., Paek, J., Browning, H.L. and Buchsbaum, M.S., Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of abstract reasoning and attention studied with positron emission tomography, Intelligence, 12 (1988) Matsui, T. and Hirano, A., An Atlas of the Human Brain for Computerized Tomography, Igaku-Shoin, Tokyo, Nuechterlein, K.H., Parasuraman, R. and Jiang, Q., Visual sustained attention: image degradation produces rapid decrement over time, Science, 220 (1983) Parks, R.W., Loewenstein, D.A., Dodrill, K.L., Barker, W.W., Yoshii, F., Chang, J.Y., Emran, A., Apicella, A., Sheramata, W. and Duara, R., Cerebral metabolic effects of a verbal fluency test: a PET scan study, J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol., 10 (1988) Parks, R.W., Crockett, D.J., Tuokko, H., Beattie, B.L., Ashford, J.W., Coburn, K.L., Zec, R.E, Becker, R.E., McGeer, P.L. and MeGeer, E.G., Neuropsychological 'systems efficiency' and positron emission tomography, J. Neuropsychiatry, 1 (1989) Penfield, W. and Rasmussen, T., The Cerebral Cortex of Man: A Clinical Study of Localization of Function, MacMillan Company, New York, Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L. and Lushene, R.E., State- Trait Anxiety Inventory Manual, Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., Palo Alto, CA, 1970.

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Table S1: Brain regions involved in the adapted classification learning task Brain Regions x y z Z Anterior Cingulate

More information

Regional and Lobe Parcellation Rhesus Monkey Brain Atlas. Manual Tracing for Parcellation Template

Regional and Lobe Parcellation Rhesus Monkey Brain Atlas. Manual Tracing for Parcellation Template Regional and Lobe Parcellation Rhesus Monkey Brain Atlas Manual Tracing for Parcellation Template Overview of Tracing Guidelines A) Traces are performed in a systematic order they, allowing the more easily

More information

Gross Organization I The Brain. Reading: BCP Chapter 7

Gross Organization I The Brain. Reading: BCP Chapter 7 Gross Organization I The Brain Reading: BCP Chapter 7 Layout of the Nervous System Central Nervous System (CNS) Located inside of bone Includes the brain (in the skull) and the spinal cord (in the backbone)

More information

Announcement. Danny to schedule a time if you are interested.

Announcement.  Danny to schedule a time if you are interested. Announcement If you need more experiments to participate in, contact Danny Sanchez (dsanchez@ucsd.edu) make sure to tell him that you are from LIGN171, so he will let me know about your credit (1 point).

More information

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Online supplementary information for: Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Joel L. Voss, Brian D. Gonsalves, Kara D. Federmeier,

More information

Chapter 3. Structure and Function of the Nervous System. Copyright (c) Allyn and Bacon 2004

Chapter 3. Structure and Function of the Nervous System. Copyright (c) Allyn and Bacon 2004 Chapter 3 Structure and Function of the Nervous System 1 Basic Features of the Nervous System Neuraxis: An imaginary line drawn through the center of the length of the central nervous system, from the

More information

Telencephalon (Cerebral Hemisphere)

Telencephalon (Cerebral Hemisphere) Telencephalon (Cerebral Hemisphere) OUTLINE The Cortex - Lobes, Sulci & Gyri - Functional Subdivisions - Limbic Lobe & Limbic System The Subcortex - Basal Ganglia - White Matter (Internal Capsule) - Relations

More information

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner Cerebral Cortex 1 Sarah Heilbronner heilb028@umn.edu Want to meet? Coffee hour 10-11am Tuesday 11/27 Surdyk s Overview and organization of the cerebral cortex What is the cerebral cortex? Where is each

More information

Pediatric MS MRI Study Methodology

Pediatric MS MRI Study Methodology General Pediatric MS MRI Study Methodology SCAN PREPARATION axial T2-weighted scans and/or axial FLAIR scans were obtained for all subjects when available, both T2 and FLAIR scans were scored. In order

More information

The human brain. of cognition need to make sense gives the structure of the brain (duh). ! What is the basic physiology of this organ?

The human brain. of cognition need to make sense gives the structure of the brain (duh). ! What is the basic physiology of this organ? The human brain The human brain! What is the basic physiology of this organ?! Understanding the parts of this organ provides a hypothesis space for its function perhaps different parts perform different

More information

Structural brain variation and general intelligence

Structural brain variation and general intelligence Rapid Communication Structural brain variation and general intelligence www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 23 (2004) 425 433 Richard J. Haier, a, * Rex E. Jung, b Ronald A. Yeo, c Kevin Head, a and

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

PROPERTY OF ELSEVIER SAMPLE CONTENT - NOT FINAL. Gross Anatomy and General Organization of the Central Nervous System

PROPERTY OF ELSEVIER SAMPLE CONTENT - NOT FINAL. Gross Anatomy and General Organization of the Central Nervous System 3 Gross Anatomy and General Organization of the Central Nervous System C h a p t e r O u t l i n e The Long Axis of the CNS Bends at the Cephalic Flexure Hemisecting a Brain Reveals Parts of the Diencephalon,

More information

PARIETAL LOBE. Vasilios A. Zerris MD, MPH, MSc, FAANS

PARIETAL LOBE. Vasilios A. Zerris MD, MPH, MSc, FAANS PARIETAL LOBE Vasilios A. Zerris MD, MPH, MSc, FAANS Diplomate of the American Board of Neurological Surgery Fellow of the American Association of Neurological Surgeons Professor of Neurosurgery, European

More information

Classic Evidence: Raine, Buchsbaum and LaCasse, 1997 Brain Abnormalities in Murderers indicated by PET (pg 16-19)

Classic Evidence: Raine, Buchsbaum and LaCasse, 1997 Brain Abnormalities in Murderers indicated by PET (pg 16-19) Classic Evidence: Raine, Buchsbaum and LaCasse, 1997 Brain Abnormalities in Murderers indicated by PET (pg 16-19) Patrick has just been arrested for murder. He confesses to the crime of killing his next

More information

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Supplemental Information Table of Contents 2 Behavioral Data 2 Table S1. Participant demographics

More information

MITELMAN, SHIHABUDDIN, BRICKMAN, ET AL. basic necessities of life, including food, clothing, and shelter. Compared to patients with good-outcome schiz

MITELMAN, SHIHABUDDIN, BRICKMAN, ET AL. basic necessities of life, including food, clothing, and shelter. Compared to patients with good-outcome schiz Article MRI Assessment of Gray and White Matter Distribution in Brodmann s Areas of the Cortex in Patients With Schizophrenia With Good and Poor Outcomes Serge A. Mitelman, M.D. Lina Shihabuddin, M.D.

More information

Use of Multimodal Neuroimaging Techniques to Examine Age, Sex, and Alcohol-Related Changes in Brain Structure Through Adolescence and Young Adulthood

Use of Multimodal Neuroimaging Techniques to Examine Age, Sex, and Alcohol-Related Changes in Brain Structure Through Adolescence and Young Adulthood American Psychiatric Association San Diego, CA 24 May 2017 Use of Multimodal Neuroimaging Techniques to Examine Age, Sex, and Alcohol-Related Changes in Brain Structure Through Adolescence and Young Adulthood

More information

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Brain electrical activity methods description and explanation of method state effects trait effects Positron emission tomography

More information

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes Finding the Central Sulcus MAP TO NEUROSCIENCE CORE CONCEPTS 1 NCC1. The brain is the body's most complex organ. LEARNING OBJECTIVES After study of the assigned learning

More information

Chapter 3. Biological Processes

Chapter 3. Biological Processes Biological Processes Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? Biological Solutions Communicating internally Initiating and coordinating behavior Regulating growth and other internal functions

More information

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes Blood Supply to the Brain MAP TO NEUROSCIENCE CORE CONCEPTS 1 NCC1. The brain is the body's most complex organ. LEARNING OBJECTIVES After study of the assigned learning

More information

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Neuron, Volume 70 Supplemental Information Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Luke J. Chang, Alec Smith, Martin Dufwenberg, and Alan G. Sanfey Supplemental Information

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Nature Neuroscience doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of viral injections.

Nature Neuroscience doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of viral injections. Supplementary Figure 1 Characterization of viral injections. (a) Dorsal view of a mouse brain (dashed white outline) after receiving a large, unilateral thalamic injection (~100 nl); demonstrating that

More information

Accurate discrimination of Alzheimer s disease from other dementia and/or normal subjects using SPECT specific volume analysis

Accurate discrimination of Alzheimer s disease from other dementia and/or normal subjects using SPECT specific volume analysis Accurate discrimination of Alzheimer s disease from other dementia and/or normal subjects using SPECT specific volume analysis Hitoshi Iyatomi a, Jun Hashimoto b, Fumihito Yoshii c, Toshiki Kazama b, Shuichi

More information

CEREBRUM Dr. Jamila Elmedany Dr. Essam Eldin Salama

CEREBRUM Dr. Jamila Elmedany Dr. Essam Eldin Salama CEREBRUM Dr. Jamila Elmedany Dr. Essam Eldin Salama Objectives At the end of the lecture, the student should be able to: List the parts of the cerebral hemisphere (cortex, medulla, basal nuclei, lateral

More information

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain 1 Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain Emily Falk, Elliot Berkman, Traci Mann, Brittany Harrison, Matthew Lieberman This document contains: Example

More information

Neuroanatomy lecture (1)

Neuroanatomy lecture (1) Neuroanatomy lecture (1) Introduction: Neuroanatomy has two parts: the central and peripheral nervous system. The central nervous system is composed of brain and spinal cord. The brain has the following

More information

Biological Bases of Behavior. 3: Structure of the Nervous System

Biological Bases of Behavior. 3: Structure of the Nervous System Biological Bases of Behavior 3: Structure of the Nervous System Neuroanatomy Terms The neuraxis is an imaginary line drawn through the spinal cord up to the front of the brain Anatomical directions are

More information

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018)

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) 1 / 22 Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) Jérôme Dockès, ussel Poldrack, Demian Wassermann, Fabian Suchanek, Bertrand

More information

Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf

Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf Supplementary Material Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf Ella Striem-Amit 1*, Jorge Almeida 2,3, Mario Belledonne 1, Quanjing Chen 4, Yuxing Fang

More information

2017, Joule Inc. or its licensors Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors.

2017, Joule Inc. or its licensors Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors. Results Validation: Reproducibility Figure S1. Reproducibility of the results of small-world parameters. Differences in topological properties of functional brain networks between bulimia nervosa (BN)

More information

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults Supplementary Material Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance across cognitive domains in older adults Emily E. Shaw 1,2, Aaron P. Schultz 1,2,3, Reisa A.

More information

Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres. Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University

Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres. Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University The largest part of the brain. Ovoid shape. Two incompletely separated cerebral hemispheres. The outer surface of the cerebral

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Devenney E, Bartley L, Hoon C, et al. Progression in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a longitudinal study. JAMA Neurol. Published online October 26, 2015. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.2061.

More information

Course Booklet. We have felt the pain that Neuroscience is giving you.

Course Booklet. We have felt the pain that Neuroscience is giving you. Exams Stressing You Out? Take Action! Course Booklet NEUR 1202 Carleton University* *TranscendFinals is not affiliated with the university We have felt the pain that Neuroscience is giving you. Our mission

More information

DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP'S BRAIN

DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP'S BRAIN Sheep Brain Dissection Guide Page 1 DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP'S BRAIN Introduction The purpose of the sheep brain dissection is to familiarize you with the threedimensional structure of the brain and teach

More information

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience Levels of organization Central Nervous System 1m 10 11 neurons Neural systems and neuroanatomy Systems 10cm Networks 1mm Neurons 100μm 10 8 neurons Professor Daniel Leeds

More information

DIADEM Instructions for Use

DIADEM Instructions for Use Software Version: 1.0.0 Document Version: 3.0 July 2017 Contents 1 Intended Use... 4 2 Scanning... 5 2.1 Automatic Operation... 5 2.2 Suitable Scans... 5 2.2.1 Protocols... 5 2.2.2 Brain Coverage... 5

More information

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex in dual-task performance Fabienne Collette a,b, Laurence 01ivier b,c, Martial Van der Linden a,d, Steven Laureys b, Guy Delfiore b, André Luxen

More information

Relationship Between Cerebral Glucose Metabolic Disorder and Malignant Tumor Type

Relationship Between Cerebral Glucose Metabolic Disorder and Malignant Tumor Type American Journal of Clinical and Experimental Medicine 2018; 6(4): 94-98 http://www.sciencepublishinggroup.com/j/ajcem doi: 10.11648/j.ajcem.20180604.12 ISSN: 2330-8125 (Print); ISSN: 2330-8133 (Online)

More information

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Figure I. Changes in connectivity with the right anterior insular cortex. (A) wake > mild sedation, showing a reduction in connectivity between the anterior

More information

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition Chapter 2 The Neural Basis of Cognition Capgras Syndrome Alzheimer s patients & others delusion that significant others are robots or impersonators - paranoia Two brain systems for facial recognition -

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. Authors Year Patients Male gender (%) Mean age (range) Adults/ Children

More information

How to report my result using REST slice viewer?

How to report my result using REST slice viewer? How to report my result using REST slice viewer? Han Zhang Center for Cognition and Brain Disorders, Hangzhou Normal University napoleon1982@gmail.com 2013/12/30 Commonly, you got an activation for functional

More information

CEREBRUM. Dr. Jamila EL Medany

CEREBRUM. Dr. Jamila EL Medany CEREBRUM Dr. Jamila EL Medany Objectives At the end of the lecture, the student should be able to: List the parts of the cerebral hemisphere (cortex, medulla, basal nuclei, lateral ventricle). Describe

More information

Case Report of the PET Scan Changes and Clinical Effects of Testosterone in the Treatment of a Stroke Patient

Case Report of the PET Scan Changes and Clinical Effects of Testosterone in the Treatment of a Stroke Patient Send Orders of Reprints at reprints@benthamscience.org The Open Andrology Journal, 2012, 4, 21-25 21 Open Access Case Report of the PET Scan Changes and Clinical Effects of Testosterone in the Treatment

More information

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1428 1442 Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Alessandra Schiavetto a, Stefan Köhler a, Cheryl L. Grady a, Gordon Winocur a,c,

More information

Supplementary Digital Content

Supplementary Digital Content Supplementary Digital Content Contextual modulation of pain in masochists: involvement of the parietal operculum and insula Sandra Kamping a, Jamila Andoh a, Isabelle C. Bomba a, Martin Diers a,b, Eugen

More information

Anatomy and Physiology (Bio 220) The Brain Chapter 14 and select portions of Chapter 16

Anatomy and Physiology (Bio 220) The Brain Chapter 14 and select portions of Chapter 16 Anatomy and Physiology (Bio 220) The Brain Chapter 14 and select portions of Chapter 16 I. Introduction A. Appearance 1. physical 2. weight 3. relative weight B. Major parts of the brain 1. cerebrum 2.

More information

Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Verse 1: Neocortex, frontal lobe, Brain stem, brain stem, Hippocampus, neural node, Right hemisphere, Pons and cortex visual, Brain stem, brain stem, Sylvian fissure,

More information

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex I: To list the functions of different cortical laminae II: To describe the four motor areas of the cerebral cortex. III: To discuss the functions and dysfunctions of

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Van Laere K, Vanhee A, Verschueren J, et al. Value of 18 fluorodeoxyglucose positron-emission tomography in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. JAMA Neurol. Published online March

More information

Figure 1. Excerpt of stimulus presentation paradigm for Study I.

Figure 1. Excerpt of stimulus presentation paradigm for Study I. Transition Visual Auditory Tactile Time 14 s Figure 1. Excerpt of stimulus presentation paradigm for Study I. Visual, auditory, and tactile stimuli were presented to sujects simultaneously during imaging.

More information

10/3/2016. T1 Anatomical structures are clearly identified, white matter (which has a high fat content) appears bright.

10/3/2016. T1 Anatomical structures are clearly identified, white matter (which has a high fat content) appears bright. H2O -2 atoms of Hydrogen, 1 of Oxygen Hydrogen just has one single proton and orbited by one single electron Proton has a magnetic moment similar to the earths magnetic pole Also similar to earth in that

More information

Anatomy Lab (1) Theoretical Part. Page (2 A) Page (2B)

Anatomy Lab (1) Theoretical Part. Page (2 A) Page (2B) Anatomy Lab (1) This sheet only includes the extra notes for the lab handout regarding the theoretical part, as for the practical part it includes everything the doctor mentioned. Theoretical Part Page

More information

PSY 302: CHAPTER 3 NOTES THE BRAIN (PART II) - 9/5/17. By: Joseline

PSY 302: CHAPTER 3 NOTES THE BRAIN (PART II) - 9/5/17. By: Joseline PSY 302: CHAPTER 3 NOTES THE BRAIN (PART II) - 9/5/17 By: Joseline Left 3 MAJOR FISSURES : 2HEMISPHERES Right Lateral Ventricle Central Fissure Third Ventricle Sulcus Lateral Fissure Gyros Fissure- Fissures

More information

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m. Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Wednesday, 16 March

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Green SA, Hernandez L, Tottenham N, Krasileva K, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. The neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Published

More information

Prof. Saeed Abuel Makarem & Dr.Sanaa Alshaarawy

Prof. Saeed Abuel Makarem & Dr.Sanaa Alshaarawy Prof. Saeed Abuel Makarem & Dr.Sanaa Alshaarawy 1 Objectives By the end of the lecture, you should be able to: Describe the anatomy and main functions of the thalamus. Name and identify different nuclei

More information

Neocortex. Hemispheres 9/22/2010. Psychology 472 Pharmacology of Psychoactive Drugs. Structures are divided into several section or lobes.

Neocortex. Hemispheres 9/22/2010. Psychology 472 Pharmacology of Psychoactive Drugs. Structures are divided into several section or lobes. Neocortex Psychology 472 Pharmacology of Psychoactive Drugs 1 Is the most developed in Humans Has many folds and fissures The folds of tissue are called gyri or a gyrus (single) The fissures or valleys

More information

Sincerely, Ms. Paoloni and Mrs. Whitney

Sincerely, Ms. Paoloni and Mrs. Whitney Dear Students, Welcome to AP Psychology! We will begin our course of study focusing on the nervous system with a particular emphasis on how the brain and neurotransmitters influence our behaviors. In preparation

More information

Automated detection of abnormal changes in cortical thickness: A tool to help diagnosis in neocortical focal epilepsy

Automated detection of abnormal changes in cortical thickness: A tool to help diagnosis in neocortical focal epilepsy Automated detection of abnormal changes in cortical thickness: A tool to help diagnosis in neocortical focal epilepsy 1. Introduction Epilepsy is a common neurological disorder, which affects about 1 %

More information

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. Cortical Organization Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. 2. Secondary cortex: located immediately adjacent to primary cortical areas,

More information

M555 Medical Neuroscience Lab 1: Gross Anatomy of Brain, Crainal Nerves and Cerebral Blood Vessels

M555 Medical Neuroscience Lab 1: Gross Anatomy of Brain, Crainal Nerves and Cerebral Blood Vessels M555 Medical Neuroscience Lab 1: Gross Anatomy of Brain, Crainal Nerves and Cerebral Blood Vessels Anatomical Directions Terms like dorsal, ventral, and posterior provide a means of locating structures

More information

Neuroimaging for Diagnosis of Psychiatric Disorders

Neuroimaging for Diagnosis of Psychiatric Disorders Psychiatric Disorder Neuroimaging for Diagnosis of Psychiatric Disorders JMAJ 45(12): 538 544, 2002 Yoshio HIRAYASU Associate Professor, Department of Neuropsychiatry Kyorin University School of Medicine

More information

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA The Effect of Cognitive Functioning, Age, and Molecular Variables on Brain Structure Among Carriers of the Fragile X Premutation: Deformation Based Morphometry Study Naomi J. Goodrich-Hunsaker*, Ling M.

More information

A few notions of brain anatomy

A few notions of brain anatomy A few notions of brain anatomy Christophe Pallier CNRS, INSERM 562, Orsay, France Note some slides were taken from lectures available from the excellent web site 'fmri for dummies' by Jody Culham. Drawing

More information

FDG-PET e parkinsonismi

FDG-PET e parkinsonismi Parkinsonismi FDG-PET e parkinsonismi Valentina Berti Dipartimento di Scienze Biomediche, Sperimentali e Cliniche Sez. Medicina Nucleare Università degli Studi di Firenze History 140 PubMed: FDG AND parkinsonism

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Braver et al. 10.1073/pnas.0808187106 SI Methods Participants. Participants were neurologically normal, righthanded younger or older adults. The groups did not differ in gender breakdown

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

Lab 2. we will look into several angled horizontal sections ( orbitomeatal plane ) i.e passing from the orbit into the ear

Lab 2. we will look into several angled horizontal sections ( orbitomeatal plane ) i.e passing from the orbit into the ear we will look into several angled horizontal sections ( orbitomeatal plane ) i.e passing from the orbit into the ear Figure I page 76 : looking at the key on the left side this section passed through the

More information

Introduction to the Central Nervous System: Internal Structure

Introduction to the Central Nervous System: Internal Structure Introduction to the Central Nervous System: Internal Structure Objective To understand, in general terms, the internal organization of the brain and spinal cord. To understand the 3-dimensional organization

More information

Cerebellum. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota

Cerebellum. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota Cerebellum Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota 1 Anatomy of the Cerebellum The cerebellum has approximately half of all the neurons in the central nervous system. The cerebellum

More information

Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of

Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of Introduction Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of scientific inquiry for over 100 years. In particular, this topic has had significant interest in the past

More information

Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour

Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour Section 1: Communication in the Nervous System Section 2: Organization in the Nervous System Section 3: Researching the Brain Section 4: The Brain Section 5: Cerebral

More information

Systems Neuroscience Dan Kiper. Today: Wolfger von der Behrens

Systems Neuroscience Dan Kiper. Today: Wolfger von der Behrens Systems Neuroscience Dan Kiper Today: Wolfger von der Behrens wolfger@ini.ethz.ch 18.9.2018 Neurons Pyramidal neuron by Santiago Ramón y Cajal (1852-1934, Nobel prize with Camillo Golgi in 1906) Neurons

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

SPAMALIZE s Cerebellum Segmentation routine.

SPAMALIZE s Cerebellum Segmentation routine. SPAMALIZE s Cerebellum Segmentation routine. Outline: - Introduction - Data Inputs - Algorithm Steps - Display Notes - Example with menu selections Introduction: This program attempts to segment the cerebellum

More information

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Pain Pathways Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Pain Pathways MAP TO NEUROSCIENCE CORE CONCEPTS 1 NCC1. The brain is the body's most complex organ. NCC3. Genetically determined circuits are the foundation

More information

Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep wake cycle An H 2 15 O PET study

Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep wake cycle An H 2 15 O PET study braini0716 Brain (1997), 120, 1173 1197 Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep wake cycle An H 2 15 O PET study A. R. Braun, 1 T. J. Balkin, 3 N. J. Wesensten, 3 R. E. Carson, 2 M. Varga, 1

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Proc SPIE. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 February 07.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Proc SPIE. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 February 07. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Proc SPIE. 2007 March 5; 6512: 651236. doi:10.1117/12.708950. Semi-Automatic Parcellation of the Corpus Striatum Ramsey Al-Hakim a,

More information

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Supplemental informations Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Stéphanie Lefebvre, PhD^1,2, Guillaume Baille, MD^4, Renaud Jardri MD, PhD 1,2 Lucie Plomhause, PhD

More information

Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3. Tomography vs Topography. Computed Tomography (CT): A simplified Topographical review of the Brain. Learning Objective

Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3. Tomography vs Topography. Computed Tomography (CT): A simplified Topographical review of the Brain. Learning Objective Slide 1 Computed Tomography (CT): A simplified Topographical review of the Brain Jon Wheiler, ACNP-BC Slide 2 Tomography vs Topography Tomography: A technique for displaying a representation of a cross

More information

CEREBRUM & CEREBRAL CORTEX

CEREBRUM & CEREBRAL CORTEX CEREBRUM & CEREBRAL CORTEX Seonghan Kim Dept. of Anatomy Inje University, College of Medicine THE BRAIN ANATOMICAL REGIONS A. Cerebrum B. Diencephalon Thalamus Hypothalamus C. Brain Stem Midbrain Pons

More information

Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Psychiatry

Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Psychiatry 2003 KHBM DTI in Psychiatry Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Psychiatry KHBM 2003. 11. 21. 서울대학교 의과대학 정신과학교실 권준수 Neuropsychiatric conditions DTI has been studied in Alzheimer s disease Schizophrenia Alcoholism

More information

Supplementary Materials for

Supplementary Materials for Supplementary Materials for Folk Explanations of Behavior: A Specialized Use of a Domain-General Mechanism Robert P. Spunt & Ralph Adolphs California Institute of Technology Correspondence may be addressed

More information

Peripheral facial paralysis (right side). The patient is asked to close her eyes and to retract their mouth (From Heimer) Hemiplegia of the left side. Note the characteristic position of the arm with

More information

ANATOMY & PHYSIOLOGY DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP BRAIN LAB GROUP:

ANATOMY & PHYSIOLOGY DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP BRAIN LAB GROUP: ANATOMY & PHYSIOLOGY DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP BRAIN LAB GROUP: Introduction The purpose of the sheep brain dissection is to familiarize you with the three dimensional structure of the brain and teach you

More information

Biomedical Technology Research Center 2011 Workshop San Francisco, CA

Biomedical Technology Research Center 2011 Workshop San Francisco, CA Diffusion Tensor Imaging: Parkinson s Disease and Atypical Parkinsonism David E. Vaillancourt court1@uic.edu Associate Professor at UIC Departments t of Kinesiology i and Nutrition, Bioengineering, and

More information

NeuroImage 45 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 45 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 45 (2009) 614 626 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fmri

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Organization of the nervous system. The withdrawal reflex. The central nervous system. Structure of a neuron. Overview

Organization of the nervous system. The withdrawal reflex. The central nervous system. Structure of a neuron. Overview Overview The nervous system- central and peripheral The brain: The source of mind and self Neurons Neuron Communication Chemical messengers Inside the brain Parts of the brain Split Brain Patients Organization

More information

The Central Nervous System I. Chapter 12

The Central Nervous System I. Chapter 12 The Central Nervous System I Chapter 12 The Central Nervous System The Brain and Spinal Cord Contained within the Axial Skeleton Brain Regions and Organization Medical Scheme (4 regions) 1. Cerebral Hemispheres

More information

Ways we Study the Brain. Accidents Lesions CAT Scan PET Scan MRI Functional MRI

Ways we Study the Brain. Accidents Lesions CAT Scan PET Scan MRI Functional MRI The Brain Ways we Study the Brain Accidents Lesions CAT Scan PET Scan MRI Functional MRI Accidents Phineas Gage Story Personality changed after the accident. What this this tell us? That different part

More information

Brain anatomy tutorial. Dr. Michal Ben-Shachar 459 Neurolinguistics

Brain anatomy tutorial. Dr. Michal Ben-Shachar 459 Neurolinguistics Brain anatomy tutorial Dr. Michal Ben-Shachar 459 Neurolinguistics The human brain Left hemisphere Right hemisphere http://www.brainmuseum.org/ Zoom out Zoom in Types of Brain Tissue Gray Matter: Cell

More information

Frontal Lobe Metabolic Decreases with Sleep Deprivation not Totally Reversed by Recovery Sleep

Frontal Lobe Metabolic Decreases with Sleep Deprivation not Totally Reversed by Recovery Sleep (2006) 31, 2783 2792 & 2006 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/06 $30.00 www.neuropsychopharmacology.org Frontal Lobe Metabolic Decreases with Sleep Deprivation not Totally Reversed

More information

performance of verbal working memory tasks

performance of verbal working memory tasks Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 90, pp. 878-882, February 1993 Neurobiology Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks MICHAEL PETRIDES, BESSIE

More information